NATURAL INHERITANCE BY FRANCIS GALLON, F.R.S. AUTHOR OF “hereditary genius,” “inquiries into human faculty,” etc. ftonflon MACMILLAN AND CO. AND NEW YORK 1889 The Right of Translation and Reproduction is Reserved5 ' S»c* Richard Clay and Sons, Limited LONDON AND BUNGAY.CONTENTS. CHAPTER I. PAGE Introductory........................................ 1 CHAPTER IT. Processes in Heredity............................... 4 Natural and Acquired Peculiarities, 4.—Transmutation of Female into Male measures, 5.—Particulate Inheritance, 7.—Family-Likeness and Individual Variation, 9.—Latent Characteristics, 11.—Heritages that Blend, and those that are mutually Exclusive, 12.—Inheritance of Acquired Faculties, 14.—Variety of Petty Influences, 16. CHAPTER III. Organic Stability......................................... 18 Incipient Structure, 18.—Filial Relation, 19.—Stable Forms 20.— Subordinate Positions of Stability, 25.—Model, 27.—Stability of Sports, 30.—Infertility of Mixed Types, 31.—Evolution not by Minute Steps only, 32. CHAPTER IV. Schemes of Distribution and of Frequency ................... 35 Fraternities and Populations to be treated as units, 35.—Schemes of Distribution and their Grades, 37.—The Shape of Schemes is independent of the number of Observations, 44.—Data for eighteen Schemes, 46.—Application of Schemes to inexact measures, 47.—Schemes of Frequency, 49.VI CONTENTS. CHAPTER Y. PAGE Normal Variability.......................................... 51 Scheme of Deviations, 51.—Normal Curve of Distributions, 54.— Comparison of the Observed with the Normal Curve, 56.—The value of a single Deviation at a known Grade, determines a Normal Scheme of Deviations, 60.—Two Measures at known Grades, determine a Normal Scheme of Measures, 61. The Charms of Statistics, 62.—Mechanical Illustration of the Cause of Curve of Frequency, 63.—Order in Apparent Chaos, 66.— Problems in the Law of Error, 66. CHAPTER YI. Data................................................... 71 Records of Family Faculties, or R.F.F. data, 72.—Special Data, 78.—Measures at my Anthropometric Laboratory, 79.—Experiments in Sweet Peas, 79. CHAPTER VII. Discussion op the Data op Stature......................... 83 Stature as a Subject for Inquiry, 83.—Marriage Selection, 85.— Issue of Unlike Parents, 88.—Description of the Tables of Stature, 91.—Mid-Stature of the Population, 92.—Variability of the* Population, 93.—Variability of Mid-Parents, 93.— Variability in Co-Fraternities, 94.—Regression,^—a, Filial, 95 ; b, Mid-Parental, 99 ; c, Parental, 100 ; ¿7, Fraternal, 108.— Squadron of Statures, 110.—Successive Generations of a People, 115.—Natural Selection, 119.—Variability in Fraternities : First Method, 124 ; Second Method, 127; Third Method, 127 ; Fouith Method, 128 ; Trustworthiness of the Constants, 130 ; General view of Kinship, 132.—Separate Contribution of each Ancestor, 134.—Pedigree Moths, 136. CHAPTER VIII. Discussion op the Data op Eye Colour ......................138 Preliminary Remarks, 138.—Data, 139.—Persistence of Eye Colour in the Population, 140.—Fundamental Eye Colours, 142.—Principles of Calculation, 148.—Results, 152.CONTENTS. vii CHAPTER IX. PAGE The Artistic Faculty.......................................... 154 Data, 154.—Sexual Distribution, 156.—Marriage Selection, 157.— Regression, 158.—Effect of Bias in Marriage, 162. CHAPTER X. Disease.......................................................164 Preliminary Problem, 165.—Data, 167.—Trustworthiness of R.F.F. data, 167.—Mixture of Inheritances, 167.—Consumption : General remarks, 171 ; Distribution of Fraternities, 174 ; Severely Tainted Fraternities, 176 ; Consumptivity, 181. —Data for Hereditary Diseases, 185. CHAPTER XI. Latent Elements................................................... 187 Latent Elements not very numerous, 187 ; Pure Breed, 189.— Simplification of Hereditary Inquiry, 190. CHAPTER XII. Summary . . 192CONTENTS. viii TABLES. The words by which the various Tables are here described, have been chosen for the sake of quick reference ; they are often not identical with those used in their actual headings. Ta^ies Subjects of the Tables. page 1. Strengths of Pull arranged for drawing a Scheme...............199 2. Data for Schemes of Distribution for various Qualities and Faculties ..................................................200 3. Evidences of the general applicability of the Law of Frequency of Error . . . . ,..........................................201 4. Values for the Normal Curve of Frequency (extracted from the well-known Table)...................................... 202 5. Values of the Probability Integral (extracted from the well- known Table)............................................... 202 6. Values of the Probability Integral when the scale by which the Errors are measured has the Prob : Error for its Unit . 203 7. Ordinates to the Normal Curve of Distribution, when its 100 Grades run from — 50°, through 0°, to + 50°...............204 8. Ditto when the Grades run from 0° to 100°. (This Table is especially adapted for use with Schemes)....................205 9. Marriage Selection in respect to Stature........................206 9a. Marriage Selection in respect to Eye Colour....................206 9 b. Marriages of the Artistic and the Non Artistic.................207 10. Issue of Parents who are unlike in Stature......................207 11. Statures of adult children born to Mid Parents of Various Statures....................................................208 12. Statures of the Brothers of men of various Statures, from K.F.F. data 209CONTENTS. IX 'Tables Subjects of the Tables. page 13. Ditto from the Special data.......................................210 14. Deviations of Individual brothers from their common Mid- Fraternal Stature .......................................... 211 15. Frequency of the different Eye Colours in 4 successive Gene- rations .....................................................212 16. The Descent of Hazel-Eyed families................................213 17. Calculated contributions from Parents and from Grandparents, according as they are Light, Hazel, or Dark eyed . . . . 213 18. Examples of the application of Table 17........................214 19. Observed and calculated Eye Colours in 16 groups of families 215 20. Ditto in 78 separate families.....................................216 21. Amounts of error in the various calculations of anticipated Eye Colour...................................................218 22. Inheritance of the Artistic Faculty...............................218 APPENDICES. A. Memoirs and Books on Heredity by the Author.................219 B. Problems by J. D. Hamilton Dickson..........................221 C. Experiments on Sweet Peas, bearing on the law of Begression. . 225 D. Good and bad Temper in English Families.....................226 E. The Geometric Mean in Vital and Social Statistics...........238 F. Probable extinction of Families, 214 ; Discussion of the Problem by the Bev. H. W. Watson, D.Sc.............................241 G. Orderly arrangement of Hereditary Data......................248 Index....................................................... 257NATURAL INHERITANCE. CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTORY. I have long been engaged upon certain problems that lie at the base of the science of heredity, and during several years have published technical memoirs concerning them, a list of which is given in Appendix A. This volume contains the more important of the results, set forth in an orderly way, with more completeness than has hitherto been possible, together with a large amount of new matter. The inquiry relates to the inheritance of moderately exceptional qualities by brotherhoods and multitudes rather than by individuals, and it is carried on by more refined and searching methods than those usually employed in hereditary inquiries. One of the problems to be dealt with refers to the curious regularity commonly observed in the statistical peculiarities of great populations during a long series of B2 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. generations. The large do not always beget the large, nor thev&ínall the small, and yet the observed proportions between the large and the small in each degree of size and in every quality, hardly varies from one generation to another. A second problem regards the average share contributed to the personal features of the offspring by each ancestor severally. Though one half of every child may be said to be derived from either parent, yet he may receive a heritage from a distant progenitor that neither of his parents possessed as personal characteristics. Therefore the child does not on the average receive so much as one half of his personal qualities from each parent, but something less than a half. The question I have to solve, in a reasonable and not merely in a statistical way, is, how much less ? The last of the problems that I need mention now, concerns the nearness of kinship in different degrees. We are all agreed that a brother is nearer akin than a nephew, and a nephew than a cousin, and so on, but how much nearer are they in the precise language of numerical statement ? These and many other problems are all fundamentally connected, and I have worked them out to a first degree of approximation, with some completeness. The conclusions cannot however be intelligibly presented in an introductory chapter. They depend on ideas that must first be well comprehended, and which are now novel to the large majority of readers and unfamiliar to all. But those who care to brace themselves to aI.] INTRODUCTORY. 3 sustained effort, need not feel much regret that the road to be travelled over is indirect, and does not admit of being mapped beforehand in a way they can clearly understand. It is full of interest of its own. It familiarizes us with the measurement of variability, and with curious laws of chance that apply to a vast diversity of social subjects. This part of the inquiry may be said to run along a road on a high level, that affords wide views in unexpected directions, and from which easy descents may be made to totally different goals to those we have now to reach. I have a great subject to write upon, but feel keenly my literary incapacity to make it easily intelligible without sacrificing accuracy and thoroughness.chaptee.il PROCESSES IN' HEREDITY. Natural and Acquired Peculiarities.—Transmutation of Female into Male Measures.—Particulate Inheritance.—Family Likeness and Individual Variation.—Latent Characteristics.—Heritages that Blend and those that are Mutually Exclusive.—Inheritance of Acquired Faculties.— Variety of Petty Influences. A concise account of the chief processes in heredity will be given in this chapter, partly to serve as a reminder to those to whom the works of Darwin especially, and of other writers on the subject, are not familiar, but principally for the sake of presenting them under an aspect that best justifies the methods of investigation about to be employed. Natural and Acquired Peculiarities.—The peculiarities of men may be roughly sorted into those that are natural and those that are acquired. It is of the former that I am about to speak in this book. They are noticeable in every direction, but are nowhere so remarkable as in those twins1 who have been dissimilar 1 See Human Faculty, 237.CHAP. II.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 5 in features and disposition from their earliest years, though brought into the world under the same conditions and subsequently nurtured in an almost identical manner. It may be that some natural peculiarity does not appear till late in life, and yet may justly deserve to be considered natural, for if it is decidedly exceptional in its character its origin could hardly be ascribed to the effects of nurture. If it was also possessed by some ancestor, it must be considered to be hereditary as well. But “Natural’" is an unfortunate word for our. purpose; it implies that the moment of birth is the earliest date from which the effects of surrounding conditions are to be reckoned, although nurture begins much earlier than that. I therefore must ask that the word “Natural” should not be construed too literally, any more than the analogous phrases of inborn, congenital, and innate. This convenient laxity of expression for the sake of avoiding a pedantic periphrase need not be accompanied by any laxity of idea. Transmutation of Female into Male Measures.—We shall have to deal with the hereditary influence of parents over their offspring, although the characteristics of the two sexes are so different that it may seem impossible to speak of both in the same terms. The phrase of “ Average Stature ” may be applied to two men without fear of mistake in its interpretation ; neither can there be any mistake when it is applied to two women, but what meaning can we attach to the word “ Average ” when it is applied to the stature of two such differentG NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. beings as the Father and the Mother ? How can we appraise the hereditary contributions of different ancestors whether in this or in any other quality, unless we take into account the sex of each ancestor, in addition to his or her characteristics ? Again, the same group of progenitors transmits qualities in different measure to the sons and to the daughters; the sons being on the whole, by virtue of their sex, stronger, taller, hardier, less emotional, and so forth, than the daughters. A serious complexity due to sexual differences seems to await us at every step when, investigating the problems of heredity. Fortunately we are able to evade it altogether by using an artifice at the outset, else, looking back as I now can, from the stage which the reader will reach when he finishes this book, I hardly know how we should have succeeded in making a fair start. The artifice is never to deal with female measures as they are observed, but always to employ their male equivalents in the place of them. I transmute all the observations of females before taking them in hand, and thenceforward am able to deal with them on equal terms with the observed male values. For example : the statures of women bear to those of men the proportion of about twelve to thirteen. Consequently by adding to each observed female stature at the rate of one inch for every foot, we are enabled to compare their statures so increased and transmuted, with the observed statures of males, on equal terms. If the observed stature of a woman is 5 feet, it will count by this rule as 5 feet + 5 inches; if it beII.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 7 6 feet, as 6 feet + 6 inches ; if 5^ feet, as 5^ feet + 5^ inches ; that is to say, as 5 feet +11^ inches.1 Similarly as regards sons and daughters ; whatever may be observed or concluded concerning daughters will, if transmuted, be held true as regarding sons, and whatever is said concerning sons, will if retransmuted, be held true for daughters. We shall see further on that it is easy to apply this principle to all measurable qualities. Particulate Inheritance.—All living beings are individuals in one aspect and composite in another. They are stable fabrics of an inconceivably large number of cells, each of which has in some sense a separate life of its own, and which have been combined under influences that are the subjects of much speculation, but are as yet little understood. We seem to inherit bit by bit, this element from one progenitor that from another, under conditions that will be more clearly expressed as we proceed, while the several bits are themselves liable to some small change during the process of transmission. Inheritance may therefore be described as largely if not wholly “ particulate,” and as such it will be treated in these pages. Though this word is good English and accurately expresses its own meaning, the application 1 The proportion I use is as 100 to 108 ; that is, I multiply every female measure by 108, which is a very easy operation to those who possess that most useful book to statisticians, Crelle’s Tables (G. Reimer, Berlin, 1875). It gives the products of all numbers under 1000, each into each ; so bv referring to the column headed 108; the transmuted values of the female statures can be read off at once.8 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. now made of it will be better understood through an illustration. Thus, many of the modern buildings in Italy are historically known to have been built out of the pillaged structures of older days. Here we may observe a column or a lintel serving the same purpose for a second time, and perhaps bearing an inscription that testifies to its origin, while as to the other stones, though the mason may have chipped them here and there, and altered their shapes a little, few, if any, came direct from the quarry. This simile gives a rude though true idea of the exact meaning of Particulate Inheritance, namely, that each piece of the new structure is derived from a corresponding piece of some older one, as a lintel was derived from a lintel, a column from a column, a piece of wall from a piece of wall. I will pursue this rough simile just one step further, which is as much as it will bear. Suppose we were building a house with second-hand materials carted from a dealer’s yard, we should often find considerable portions of the same old houses to be still grouped together. Materials derived from various structures might have been moved and much shuffled together in the yard, yet pieces from the same source would frequently remain in juxtaposition and it may be entangled. They would lie side by side ready to be carted away at the same time and to be re-erected together anew. So in the process of transmission by inheritance, elements derived from the same ancestor are apt to appear in large groups, just as if they had clung together in the pre-embryonic stage, as perhapsil] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 9 they did. They form what is well expressed by the word “ traits,” traits of feature and character—that is to say, continuous features and not isolated points. We appear, then, to be severally built up out of a host of. minute particles of whose nature we know nothing, any one of which may be derived from any one progenitor, but which are usually transmitted in aggregates, considerable groups being derived from the same progenitor. It would seem that while the embryo is developing itself, the particles more or less qualified for each new post wait as it were in competition, to obtain it. Also that the particle that succeeds, must owe its success partly to accident of position and partly to being better qualified than any equally well placed competitor to gain a lodgment. Thus the step by step development of the embryo cannot fail to be influenced by an incalculable number of small and mostly unknown circumstances. Family Likeness and Individual Variation.—Natural peculiarities are apparently due to two broadly different causes, the one is Family Likeness and the other is Individual Variation. They seem to be fundamentally opposed, and to require independent discussion, but this is not the case altogether, nor indeed in the greater part. It will soon be understood how the conditions that produce a general resemblance between the offspring and their parents, must at the same time give rise to a considerable amount of individual differences. Therefore I need not discuss Family Likeness and Individual Varia-NATURAL INHERITANCE. 10 [chap. tion under separate heads, but as different effects of the same underlying causes. The origin of these and other prominent processes in heredity is best explained by illustrations. That which will be used was suggested by those miniature gardens, self-made and self-sown, that may be seen in crevices or other receptacles for drifted earth, on the otherwise bare faces of quarries and cliffs. I have frequently studied them through an opera glass, and have occasionally clambered up to compare more closely their respective vegetations. Let us then suppose the aspect of the vegetation, not of one of these detached little gardens, but of a particular island of substantial size, to represent the features, bodily and mental, of some particular parent. Imagine two such islands floated far away to a desolate sea, and anchored near together, to represent the two parents. Next imagine a number of islets, each constructed of earth that was wholly destitute of seeds, to be feared near to them. Seeds from both of the islands will gradually make their way to the islets through the agency of winds, currents, and birds. Vegetation will spring up, and when the islets are covered with it, their several aspects will represent the features of the several children. It is almost impossible that the seeds could ever be distributed equally among the islets, and there must be slight differences between them in exposure and other conditions, corresponding to differences in pre-natal circumstances. All of these would have some influence upon the vegetation; hence there would be a corre-IL] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 11 sponding variety in the results. In some islets one plant would prevail, in others another; nevertheless there would be many traits of family likeness in the vegetation of all of them, and no plant would be found that had not existed in one or other of the islands. Though family likeness and individual variations are largely due to a common cause, some variations are so large and otherwise remarkable, that they seem to belong to a different class. They are known among breeders as “ sports ” ; I will speak of these later on. Latent Characteristics.—Another fact in heredity may also be illustrated by the islands and islets ; namely, that the child often resembles an ancestor in some^ feature or character that neither of his parents personally possessed. We are told that buried seeds may lie dormant for many years, so that when a plot of ground that was formerly cultivated is again deeply dug into and upturned, plants that had not been known to grow on the spot within the memory of man, will frequently make their appearance. It is easy to imagine that some of these dormant seeds should find their way to an islet, through currents that undermined the island cliffs and drifted away their débris, after the cliffs had tumbled into the sea. Again, many plants on the islands may maintain an obscure existence, being hidden and half smothered by successful rivals ; but whenever their seeds happened to find their way to any one of the islets, while those of their rivals did not, they would sprout freely and assert themselves. This12 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. illustration partly covers the analogous fact of diseases and other inheritances skipping a generation, which by the way I find to be by no means so usual an occurrence as seems popularly to be imagined. Heritages that Blend and those that are Mutually Exclusive.—As regards heritages that blend in the offspring, let us take the case of human skin colour. The children of the white and the negro are of a blended tint ; they are neither wholly white nor wholly black, neither are they piebald, but of a fairly uniform mulatto brown. The quadroon child of the mulatto and the white.has a quarter tint; some of the children may be altogether darker or lighter than the rest, but they are not piebald. Skin-colour is therefore a good example of what I call blended inheritance. It need be none the less “ particulate ” in its origin, but the result may be regarded as a fine mosaic too minute for its elements to be distinguished in a general view. Next as regards heritages that come altogether from one progenitor to the exclusion of the rest. Eye-colour is a fairly good illustration of this, the children of a light-eyed and of a dark-eyed parent being much more apt to take their eye-colours after the one or the other than to have intermediate and blended tints. There are probably no heritages that perfectly blend or that absolutely exclude one another, but all heritages have a tendency in one or the other direction, and the tendency is often a very strong one. This is paralleledII.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 13 by what we may see in plots of wild vegetation, where two varieties of a plant mix freely, and the general aspect of the vegetation becomes a blend of the two, or where individuals of one variety congregate and take exclusive possession of one place, and those of another variety congregate in another. A peculiar interest attaches itself to mutually exclusive heritages, owing to the aid they must afford to the establishment of incipient races. A solitary peculiarity that blended freely with the characteristics of the parent stock, would disappear in hereditary transmission, as quickly as the white tint imported by a solitary European would disappear in a black population. If the European mated at all, his spouse must be black, and therefore in the very first generation the offspring would be mulattoes, and half of his whiteness would be lost to them. If these mulattoes did not interbreed, the whiteness would be reduced in the second generation to one quarter; in a very few more generations all recognizable trace of it would have gone. But if the whiteness refused to blend with the blackness, some of the offspring of the white man would be wholly white and the rest wholly black. The same event would occur in the grandchildren, mostly but not exclusively in the children of the white offspring, and so on in subsequent generations. Therefore, unless the white stock became wholly extinct, some undiluted specimens of it wTould make their appearance during an indefinite time, giving it repeated14 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. chances of holding its own in the struggle for existence, and of establishing itself if its qualities were superior to those of the black stock under any one of many different conditions. Inheritance of Acquired Faculties.—I am unprepared to say more than a few words on the obscure, unsettled, and much discussed subject of the possibility of transmitting acquired faculties. The main evidence in its favour is the gradual change of the instincts of races at large, in conformity with changed habits, and through their increased adaptation to their surroundings, otherwise apparently than through the influence of Natural Selection. There is very little direct evidence of its influence in the course of a single generation, if the phrase of Acquired Faculties is used in perfect strictness and all inheritance is excluded that could be referred to some form of Natural Selection, or of Infection before birth, or of peculiarities of Nurture and Rearing. Moreover, a large deduction from the collection of rare cases must be made on the ground of their being accidental coincidences. When this is done, the remaining instances of acquired disease or faculty, or of any mutilation being transmitted from parent to child, are very few. Some apparent evidence of a positive kind, that was formerly relied upon, has been since found capable of being interpreted in another way, and is no longer adduced. On the other hand there exists such a vast mass of distinctly negative evidence, that every instance offered to prove the transmissionII.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 15 of acquired faculties requires to be closely criticized. For example, a woman who was sober becomes a drunkard. Her children born during the period of her sobriety are said to be quite healthy ; her subsequent children are said to be neurotic. The objections to accepting this as a valid instance in point are many. The woman’s tissues must have been drenched with alcohol, and the unborn infant alcoholised during all its existence in that state. The quality of the mother’s milk would be bad. The surroundings of a home under the charge of a drunken woman would be prejudicial to the health of a growing child. No wonder that it became neurotic. Again, a large number of diseases are conveyed by germs capable of passing from the tissues of the mother into those of the unborn child otherwise than through the blood. Moreover it must be recollected that the connection fbetween the unborn child and the mother is hardly more intimate than that between some parasites and the animals on which they live. Not a single nerve has been traced between them, not a drop of blood1 has been found to pass from the mother to the child. The unborn child together with the growth to which it is attached, and which is afterwards thrown off, have their own vascular system to themselves, entirely independent of that of the mother. If in an anatomical preparation the veins of the mother are injected with a coloured fluid, none of it enters the veins of the child; conversely, if the veins of the child 1 See Lectures by William 0. Priestley, M.D. (Churchill, London, 1860), pp. 50, 52, 55, 59, and 64.16 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. are injected, none of the fluid enters those of the mother. Again, not only is the unborn child a separate animal from its mother, that obtains its air and nourishment from her purely through soakage, but its constituent elements are of very much less recent growth than is popularly supposed. The ovary of the mother is as old as the mother herself; it was well developed in her own embryonic state. The ova it contains in her adult life were actually or potentially present before she was born, and they grew as she grew. There is more reason to look on them as collateral with the mother, than as parts of the mother. The same may be said with little reservation concerning the male elements. It is therefore extremely difficult to see how acquired faculties can be inherited by the children. It would be less difficult to conceive of their inheritance by the grandchildren. Well devised experiment into the limits of the power of inheriting acquired faculties and mutilations, whether in plants or animals, is one of the present desiderata in hereditary science. Fortunately for us, our ignorance of the subject will not introduce any special difficulty in the inquiry on which we are now engaged. Variety of Petty Influences.—The incalculable number of petty accidents that concur to produce variability among brothers, make it impossible to predict the exact qualities of any individual from hereditary data. But we may predict average results with great certainty, as will be seen further on, and we can alson.] PROCESSES IN HEREDITY. 17 obtain precise information concerning the penumbra of uncertainty that attaches itself to single predictions. It would be premature to speak further of this at present; what has been said is enough to give a clue to the chief motive of this chapter. Its intention has been to show the large part that is always played by chance in the course of hereditary transmission, and to establish the importance of an intelligent use of the laws of chance and of the statistical methods that are based upon them, in expressing the conditions under which heredity acts. I may here point out that, as the processes of statistics are themselves processes of intimate blendings, their results are the same, whether the materials had been partially blended or not, before they were statistically taken in hand. cCHA.PTEB III. ORGANIC STABILITY. Incipient Structure.—Filial relation.—Stable Forms.—Subordinate positions of Stability.—Model.—Stability of Sports.—Infertility of mixed Types.—Evolution not by minute steps only. Incipient Structure.—The total heritage of each man must include a greater variety of material than was utilised informing his personal structure. The existence in some latent form of an unused portion is proved by his power, already alluded to, of transmitting ancestral characters that he did not personally exhibit. Therefore the organised structure of each individual should be viewed as the fulfilment of only one out of an indefinite number of mutually exclusive possibilities. His structure is the coherent and more or less stable development of what is no more than an imperfect sample of a large variety of elements. The precise conditions under which each several element or particle (whatever may be its nature) finds its way into the sample are, it is needless to repeat, unknown, but we may provisionally classify them under one or other of the following three categories, as theyCHAP. III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 19 apparently exhaust all reasonable possibilities : first, that in which each element selects its most suitable immediate neighbourhood, in accordance with the guiding idea in Darwin’s theory of Pangenesis ; secondly, that of more or less general co-ordination of the influences exerted on each element, not only by its immediate neighbours, but by many or most of the others as well; finally, that of accident or chance, under which name a group of agencies are to be comprehended, diverse in character and alike only in the fact that their influence on the settlement of each particle was not immediately directed towards that end. In philosophical language we say that such agencies are not purposive, or that they are not teleological; in popular language they are called accidents or chances. Filial Relation.—A conviction that inheritance is mainly particulate and much influenced by chance, greatly affects our idea of kinship and makes us consider the parental and filial relation to be curiously circuitous. It appears that there is no direct hereditary relation between the personal parents and the personal child, except perhaps through little-known channels of secondary importance, but that the main line of hereditary connection unites the sets of elements out of which the personal parents had been evolved with the set out of which the personal child was evolved. The main line may be rudely likened to the chain of a necklace, and the personalities to pendants attached to its links. We are unable to see the particles and C 220 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. watch their grouping, and we know nothing directly about them, but we may gain some idea of the various possible results by noting the differences between the brothers in any large- fraternity (as will be done further on with much minuteness), whose total heritages must have been much alike, but whose personal structures are often very dissimilar. This is why it is so important in hereditary inquiry to deal with \ fraternities rather than with individuals, and with large fraternities rather than small ones. We ought, for example, to compare the group containing both parents and all the uncles and aunts/ with that containing all the children. The relative weight to be assigned to the uncles and aunts is a question of detail to be discussed in its proper place further on (see Chap. XI.) Stable Forms.—The changes in the substance of the newly-fertilised ova of all animals, of which more is annually becoming known,1 indicate segregations as well as aggregations, and it is reasonable to suppose that repulsions concur with affinities in producing them. We know nothing as yet of the nature of these affinities and repulsions, but we may expect them to act in great numbers and on all sides in a space of three dimensions, just as the personal likings and dis- 1 A valuable memoir on the state of onr knowledge of these matters up to the end of 1887 is published in Yol. XIX. of the Proceedings of the Philosophical Society of Glasgow, and reprinted under the title of The Modern Cell Theory, and Theories as to the Physiological Basis of Heredity, by Prof. John Gray McKendrick, M.D., F.R.S., &c. (R. Anderson, Glasgow, 1888.)III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 21 likings of each individual insect in a flying swarm may be supposed to determine the position that he occupies in it. Every particle must have many immediate neighbours. Even a sphere surrounded by other spheres of equal sizes, like a cannon-ball in the middle of a heap, when they are piled in the most compact form, is in actual contact with no less than twelve others. We may therefore feel assured that the particles which are still unfixed must be affected by very numerous influences acting from all sides and varying with slight changes of place, and that they may occupy many positions of temporary and unsteady equilibrium, and be subject to repeated unsettlement, before they finally assume the positions in which they severally remain at rest. The whimsical effects of chance in producing stable results are common enough. Tangled strings variously twitched, soon get themselves into tight knots. Rubbish thrown down a sink is pretty sure in time to choke the pipe; no one bit may be so large as its bore, but several bits in their numerous chance encounters will at length so come into collision as to wedge themselves into a sort of arch across the tube, and effectually plug it. Many years ago there was a fall of large stones from the ruinous walls of Kenilworth Castle. Three of them, if I recollect rightly, or possibly four, fell into a very peculiar arrangement, and bridged the interval between the jambs of an old window. There they stuck fast, showing clearly against the sky. The oddity of the structure attracted continual attention, and its stability was much commented on. These hanging stones, as22 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [ciiap. they were called, remained quite firm for many years ; at length a storm shook them down. In every congregation of mutually reacting elements, some characteristic groupings are usually recognised that have become familiar through their frequent recurrence and jmrtial persistence. Being less evanescent than other combinations, they may be regarded as temporarily Stable Forms. No demonstration is needed to show that their ^number must be greatly smaller than that of all the possible combinations of the same elements. I will briefly give as great a diversity of instances as I can think of, taken from Governments, Crowds, Landscapes, and even from Cookery, and shall afterwards draw some illustrations from Mechanical Inventions, to illustrate what is meant by characteristic and stable groupings. From some of them it will also be gathered that secondary and other orders of stability exist besides the primary ones. In Governments, the primary varieties of stable forms are very few in number, being such as autocracies, constitutional monarchies, oligarchies, or republics. The secondary forms are far more numerous ; still it is hard to meet with an instance of one that cannot be pretty closely paralleled by another. A curious evidence of the small variety of possible governments is to be found in the constitutions of the governing bodies of the Scientific Societies of London and the Provinces, which are numerous and independent. Their development seems to follow a single course that has many stages,III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 23 and invariably tends to establish the following staff of officers : President, Vice-Presidents, a Council, Honorary Secretaries, a paid Secretary, Trustees, and a Treasurer. As Britons are not unfrequently servile to rank, some seek a purely ornamental Patron as well. Every variety of Crowd has its own characteristic features. At a national pageant, an evening party, a race-course, a marriage, or a funeral, the groupings in each case recur so habitually that it sometimes appears to me as if time had no existence, and that the ceremony in which I am taking part is identical with others at which I had been present one year, ten years, twenty years, or any other time ago. The frequent combination of the same features in Landscape Scenery, justifies the use of such expressions as “ true to nature,” when applied to a pictorial composition or to the descriptions of a novel writer. The experiences of travel in one part of the world may curiously resemble those in another. Thus the military expedition by boats up the Nile was planned from experiences gained on the Red River of North America, and was carried out with the aid of Canadian voyageurs. The snow mountains all over the world present the same peculiar difficulties to the climber, so that Swiss experiences and in many cases Swiss guides have been used for the exploration of the Himalayas, the Caucasus, the lofty mountains of New Zealand, the Andes, and Greenland. Whenever the general conditions of a new country resemble our own, we recognise characteristic and familiar features at every turn, whether we24 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. are walking by the brookside, along the seashore, in the woods, or on the hills. Even in Cookery it seems difficult to invent a new and good dish, though the current recipes are few, and the proportions of the flour, sugar, butter, eggs, &c., used in making them might be indefinitely varied and be still eatable. I consulted cookery books to learn the facts authoritatively, and found the following passage : “ I have constantly kept in view the leading principles of this work, namely, to give in these domestic recipes the most exact quantities. ... I maintain that one cannot be too careful; it is the only way to put an end to those approximations and doubts which will beset the steps of the inexperienced, and which account for so many people eating indifferent meals at home.”1 It is the triteness of these experiences that makes the most varied life monotonous after a time, and many old men as well as Solomon have frequent occasion to lament that there is nothing new under the sun. The object of these diverse illustrations is to impress the meaning I wish to convey, by the phrase of stable forms or groupings, which, however uncertain it may be in outline, is perfectly distinct in substance. Every one of the meanings that have been attached by writers to the vague but convenient word “ type ” has for its central idea the existence of a limited number 1 The Royal CooTcery Booh By Jules GoufF£, Chef de Cuisine of the Paris Jockey Club ; translated by Alphonse Gouffe, Head Pastry Cook to H.M. the Queen. Sampson Low. 1869. Introduction, p. 9.III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 25 of frequently recurrent forms. The word etymologically compares these forms to the identical medals that may be struck by one or other of a set of dies. The central idea on which the phrase “ stable forms ” is based is of the same kind, while the phrase further accounts for their origin, vaguely it may be, but still significantly, by showing that though we know little or nothing of details, the result of organic groupings is analogous to much that we notice elsewhere on every side. Subordinate positions of Stability.—Of course there are different degrees of stability. If the same structural form recurs in successively descending generations, its stability must be great, otherwise it could not have withstood the effects of the admixture of equal doses of alien elements in successive generations. Such a form well deserves to be called typical. A breeder would always be able to establish it. It tends of itself to become a new and stable variety; therefore all the breeder has to attend to is to give fair play to its tendency, by weeding out from among its offspring such reversions to other forms as may crop up from time to time, and by preserving the breed from rival admixtures until it has become confirmed, and adapted in every minute particular to its surroundings. Personal Forms may be compared to Human Inventions, as these also may be divided into types, sub-types, and deviations from them. Every important invention is a new type, and of such a definite kind as to admit of clear verbal description, and so of becoming20 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. the subject of patent rights ; at the same time it need not be so minutely defined as to exclude the possibility of small improvements or of deviations from the main design, any of which may be freely adopted by the inventor without losing the protection of his patent. But the range of protection is by no means sharply distinct, as most inventors know to their cost. Some other man, who may or may not be a plagiarist, applies for a separate patent for himself, on the ground that he has introduced modifications of a fundamental character; in other words, that he has created a fresh type. His application is opposed, and the question whether his plea be valid or not, becomes a subject for legal decision. Whenever a patent is granted subsidiary to another, and lawful to be used only by those who have acquired rights to work the primary invention, then we should rank the new patent as a secondary and not as a primary type. Thus we see that mechanical inventions offer good examples of types, sub-types, and mere deviations. The three kinds of public carriages that characterise the streets of London; namely, omnibuses, hansoms, and four-wheelers, are specific and excellent illustrations of what I wish to express by mechanical types, as distinguished from sub-types. Attempted improvements in each of them are yearly seen, but none have as yet superseded the old familiar patterns, which cannot, as it thus far appears, be changed with advantage, taking the circumstances of London as they are. Yet there have been numerous subsidiary and patented contriv-III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 27 ances, each a distinct step in the improvement of one or other of the three primary types, and there are or may be in each of the three an indefinite number of varieties in details, too unimportant to be subjects of patent rights. The broad classes, of primary or subordinate types, and of mere deviations from them, are separated by no well-defined frontiers. Still the distinction is very serviceable, so much so that the whole of the laws of patent and copyright depend upon it, and it forms the only foundation for the title to a vast amount of valuable property. Corresponding forms of classification must be equally appropriate to the organic structure of all living things. Model.—The distinction between primary and subordinate positions of stability will be made clearer by the help of Fig 1, which is drawn from a model I made. The model has more sides, but Fig. 1 suffices for illustration. It is a polygonal slab that can be made to stand on any one of its edges when set upon a level table, and is28 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. intended to illustrate the meaning of primary and subordinate stability in organic structures, although the conditions of these must be far more complex than anything we have wits to imagine. The model and the organic structure have the cardinal fact in common, that if either is disturbed without transgressing the range of its stability, it will tend to re-establish itself, but if the range is overpassed it will topple over into a new position; also that both of them are more likely to topple over towards the position of primary stability, than away from it. The ultimate point to be illustrated is this. Though a long established race habitually breeds true to its kind, subject to small unstable deviations, yet every now and then the offspring of these deviations do not tend to revert, but possess some small stability of their own. They therefore have the character of sub-types, always, however, with a reserved tendency under strained conditions, to revert to the earlier type. The model further illustrates the fact that sometimes a sport may occur of such marked peculiarity and stability as to rank as a new type, capable of becoming the origin of a new race with very little assistance on the part of natural selection. Also, that a new type may be reached without any large single stride, but through a fortunate and rapid succession of many small ones. The model is a polygonal slab, the polygon being one that might have been described within an oval, and it is so shaped as to stand on any one of its edges. When the slab rests as in Fig. 1, on the edge A B, corresponding toIII.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 29 the shorter diameter of the oval, it stands in its most stable position, and in one from which it is equally difficult to dislodge it by a tilt either forwards or backwards. So long as it is merely tilted it will fall back on being left alone, and its position when merely tilted corresponds to a simple deviation. But when it is pushed with sufficient force, it will tumble on to the next edge, b c, into a new position of stability. It will rest there, but less securely than in its first position; moreover its range of stability will no longer be disposed symmetrically. A comparatively slight push from the front will suffice to make it tumble back, a comparatively heavy push from behind is needed to make it tumble forward. If it be tumbled over into a third position (not shown in the Fig.), the process just described may recur with exaggerated effect, and similarly for many subsequent ones. If, however, the slab is at length brought to rest on the edge c D, most nearly corresponding to its longest diameter, the next onward push, which may be very slight, will suffice to topple it over into an entirely new system of stability; in other words, a “ sport ” comes suddenly into existence. Or the figure might have been drawn with its longest diameter passing into a projecting spur, so that a push of extreme strength would be required to topple it entirely over. If the first position, A b, is taken to represent a type, the other portions will represent sub-types. All the stable positions on the same side of the longer diameter are subordinate to the first position. On whichever of30 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of them the polygon may stand, its principal tendency on being seriously disturbed will be to fall back towards the first position; yet each position is stable within certain limits. Consequently the model illustrates how the following conditions may co-exist: (1) Variability within narrow limits without prejudice to the purity of the breed. (2) Partly stable sub-types. (3) Tendency, when much disturbed, to revert from a sub-type to an earlier form. (4) Occasional sports which may give rise to new types. Stability of Sports.—Experience does not show that those wide varieties which are called “ sports ” arc unstable. On the contrary, they are often transmitted to successive generations with curious persistence. Neither is there any reason for expecting otherwise. While we can well understand that a strained modification of a type would not be so stable as one that approximates more nearly to the typical centre, the variety may be so wide that it falls into different conditions of stability, and ceases to be a strained modification of the original type. The hansom cab was originally a marvellous novelty. In the language of breeders it was a sudden and remarkable “ sport,” yet the suddenness of its appearance has been no bar to its unchanging hold on popular favour. It is not a monstrous anomaly of incongruous parts, and therefore unstable, but quite the contrary. Many other instances of very novel and yet stable inventions could be quoted. One of the earliestm.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 31 electrical batteries was that which is still known as a Grove battery, being the invention of Sir William Grove. Its principle was quite new at the time, and it continues in use without alteration. The persistence in inheritance of trifling characteristics, such as a mole, a white tuft of hair, or multiple fingers,' has ofteu been remarked. The reason of it is, I presume, that such characteristics have inconsiderable influence upon the general organic stability; they are mere excrescences, that may be associated with very different types, and are therefore inheritable without let or hindrance. It seems to me that stability of type, about which we as yet know very little, must be an important factor in the general theory of heredity, when the theory is applied to cases of high breeding. It will be shown later on, at what point a separate allowance requires to be made for it. But in the earlier and principal part of the inquiry, which deals with the inheritance of qualities that are only exceptional in a small degree, a separate allowance does not appear to be required. Infertility of Mixed Types.—It is not difficult to see in a general way why very different types should refuse to coalesce, and it is scarcely possible to explain the reason why, more clearly than by an illustration. Thus a useful blend between a four-wheeler and a hansom would be impossible ; it would have to run on three wheels and the half-way position for the driver would be upon its roof. A blend would be equally impossibleNATURAL INHERITANCE. 32 [chap. between an omnibus and a hansom, and it would be difficult between an omnibus and a four-wheeler. Evolution not by Minute Steps Only.—The theory of Natural Selection might dispense with a restriction, for which it is difficult to see either the need or the justification, namely, that the course of evolution always proceeds by steps that are severally minute, and that become effective only through accumulation. That the steps may be small and that they must be small are very different views; it is only to the latter that I object, and only when the indefinite word “ small ” is used in the sense of “ barely discernible,” or as small compared with such large sports as are known to have been the origins of new races. An apparent ground for the common belief is founded on the fact that whenever search is made for intermediate forms between widely divergent varieties, whether they be of plants or of animals, of weapons or utensils, of customs, religion or language, or of any other product of evolution, a long and orderly series can usually be made out, each member of . which differs in an almost imperceptible degree from the adjacent specimens. But it does not at all follow because these intermediate forms have been found to exist, that they are the very stages that were passed through in the course of evolution. Counter evidence exists in abundance, not only of the appearance of considerable sports, but of their remarkable stability in hereditary transmission. Many of the specimens of intermediate forms may have been unstable varieties,III.] ORGANIC STABILITY. 33 whose descendants had reverted ; they might be looked upon as tentative and faltering steps taken along parallel courses of evolution, and afterwards retraced. Affiliation from each generation to the next requires to be proved before any apparent line of descent can be accepted as the true one. The history of inventions fully illustrates this view. It is a most common experience that what an inventor knew to be original, and believed to be new, had been invented independently by others many times before, but had never become established. Even when it has new features, the inventor usually finds, on consulting lists of patents, that other inventions closely border on his own. Yet we know that inventors often proceed by strides, their ideas originating in some sudden happy thought suggested by a chance occurrence, though their crude ideas may have to be laboriously worked out afterwards. If, however, all the varieties of any machine that had ever been invented, were collected and arranged in a Museum in the apparent order of their Evolution, each would differ so little from its neighbour as to suggest the fallacious inference that the successive inventors of that machine had progressed by means of a very large number of hardly discernible steps. The object of this and of the preceding chapter has been first to dwell on the fact of inheritance being “ particulate,” secondly to show how this fact is compatible with the existence of various types, some of which" are subordinate to others, and thirdly to argue D34 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. III. that Evolution need not proceed by small steps only. I have largely used metaphor and illustration to explain the facts, wishing to avoid entanglements with theory as far as possible, inasmuch as no complete theory of inheritance has yet been propounded that meets with general acceptation.CHAPTER IV. SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. Fraternities and Populations to be treated as Units.—Schemes of Distribution and tbeir Grades.—The Shape of Schemes is independent of the number of observations.—Data for Eighteen Schemes.—Application of the method of Schemes to inexact Measures.—Schemes of Frequency. Fraternities and Populations to be Treated as Units.— The science of heredity is concerned with Fraternities and large Populations rather than with individuals, and must treat them as units. A compendious method is therefore requisite by which we may express the distribution of each faculty among the members of any large group, whether it be a Fraternity or an entire Population. The knowledge of an average value is a meagre piece of information. How little is conveyed by the bald statement that the average income of English families is 100?. a year, compared with what we should learn if we were told how English incomes were distributed ; what proportion of our countrymen had just and only just enough means to ward off starvation, and what were the d 236 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. proportions of those who had incomes in each and every other degree, np to the huge annual receipts of a few great speculators, manufacturers, and landed proprietors. So in respect to the distribution of any human quality or faculty, a knowledge of mere averages tells but little ; we want to learn how the quality is distributed among the various members of the Fraternity or of the Population, and to express what we know in so compact a form that it can be easily grasped and dealt with. A parade of great accuracy is foolish, because precision is unattainable in biological and social statistics; their results being never strictly constant. Over-minuteness is mischievous, because it overwhelms the mind with more details than can be compressed into a single view. We require no more than a fairly just and comprehensive method of expressing the way in which each measurable quality is distributed among the members of any group, whether the group consists of brothers or of members of any particular social, local, or other body of persons, or whether it is coextensive with an entire nation or race. A knowledge of the distribution of any quality enables us to. ascertain the Rank that each man holds among his fellows, in respect to that quality. This is a valuable piece of knowledge in this struggling and competitive world, where success is to the foremost, and failure to the hindmost, irrespective of absolute efficiency. A blurred vision would be above all price to an individual man in a nation of blind men, though it would hardly enable him to earn his bread elsewhere. WhenIV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 37 the distribution of any faculty has been ascertained, we can tell from the measurement, say of our child, how he ranks among other children in respect to that faculty, whether it be a physical- gift, or one of health, or of intellect, or of morals. As the years go by, we may learn by the same means whether he is making his way towards the front, whether he just holds his place, or whether he is falling back towards the rear. Similarly as regards the position of our class, or of our nation, among other classes and other nations. Schemes of Distribution and their Grades.—I shall best explain my graphical method of expressing Distribution, which I like the more, the more I use it, and which I have latterly much developed, by showing how to determine the Grade of an individual among his fellows in respect to any particular faculty. Suppose that we have already put on record the measures of many men in respect to Strength, exerted as by an archer in pulling his bow, and tested by one of Salter’s well-known dial instruments with a movable index. Some men will have been found strong and others weak ; how can we picture in a compendious diagram, or how can we define by figures, the distribution of this faculty of Strength throughout the group ? How shall we determine and specify the Grade that any particular person would occupy in the group ? The first step is to marshal our measures in the orderly way familiar to statisticians, which is shown in Table I. I usually work to about twice its degree of minuteness, but enough38 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. has been entered in the Table for the purpose of illustration, while its small size makes it all the more intelligible. The fourth column of the Table headed “ Percentages ” of “ Bums from the beginning,” is pictorially translated into Fig. 2, and the third column headed “ Percentages ” of “ No. of cases observed,” into Fig. 3. The scale of lbs. is given at the side of both Figs. : and the compartments a to g, that are shaded with broken lines, have the same meaning in both, but they are differently disposed in the two Figs. We will now consider Fig. 2 only, which is the one that principally concerns us. The percentages in the last column of Table I. have been marked off on the bottom line of Fig. 2, where they are called (centesimal) Grades. The number of lbs. found in the first column of the Table determinesIV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 39 the height of the vertical lines to be erected at the corresponding Grades when we are engaged in constructing the Figure. Let us begin with the third line in the Table for illustration: it tells us that 37 per cent, of the group had Strengths less than 7 0 lbs. Therefore, when drawing the figure, a perpendicular must be raised at the 37 th grade to a height corresponding to that of 70 lbs. on the side scale. The fourth line in the Table tells us that 7 0 per cent, of the group had Strengths less than 80 lbs. ; therefore a perpendicular must be raised at the 70th Grade to a height corresponding to 80 lbs. We proceed in the same way with respect to the remaining figures, then we join the tops of these perpendiculars by straight lines. As these observations of Strength have been sorted into only 7 groups, the trace formed by the lines that connect the tops of the few perpendiculars differs sensibly from a flowing curve, but when working with double minuteness, as mentioned above, the connecting lines differ little to the eye from the dotted curve. The dotted curve may then be accepted as that which would result if a separate perpendicular had been drawn for every observation, and if permission had been given to slightly smooth their irregularities. I call the figure that is bounded by such a curve as this, a Scheme of Distribution; the perpendiculars that formed the scaffolding by which it was constructed having been first rubbed out. (See Fig 4, next page.) A Scheme enables us in a moment to find the Grade40 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of Rank (on a scale reckoned from 0° to 100°) of any person in the group to which he belongs. The measured strength of the person is to be looked for in the side scale of the Scheme ; a horizontal line is thence drawn until it meets the curve; from the point of meeting a perpendicular is dropped upon the scale of Grades at the base; then the Grade on which it falls is the one required. For example: let us suppose the Strength of Pull of a man to have been 74 lbs., and that we wish to determine his Rank in Strength among the large group of men who were measured at the Health Exhibition in 1884. We find by Fig. 4 that his centesimal Grade is 50°; in other words, that 50 per cent, of the group will be weaker than he is, and 50 per cent, will be stronger. HisIV.] SCHEMES OP DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 41 position will be exactly Middlemost, after the Strengths of all the men in the group have been marshalled in the order of their magnitudes.. In other words, he is of mediocre strength. . The accepted term to express the value that occupies the Middlemost position is “ Median,” which may be used either as an adjective or as a substantive, but it will be usually replaced in this book by the abbreviated form M. I also use the word “ Mid ” in a few combinations, such as “ Mid-Fraternity,” to express the same thing. The Median, M, has three properties. The first follows immediately from its construction, namely, that the chance is an equal one, of. any previously unknown measure in the group exceeding or falling short of M. The second is, that the most probable value of any previously unknown measure in the group is M. Thus if N be any one of the measures, and u be the value of the unit in which the measure is recorded, such as an inch, tenth of an inch, &c., then the number of measures that fall between (N — \v) and (N + \u), is greatest when N = M. Mediocrity is always the commonest condition, for reasons that will become apparent later on. The third property is that whenever the curve of the Scheme is symmetrically disposed on either side of M, except that one half of it is turned upwards, and the other half downwards, then M is identical with the ordinary Arithmetic Mean or Average. This is closely the condition of . all the curves I have to discuss. The reader may look on the Median and on the Mean as being practically the same things, throughout this book.42 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. It must be understood that M, like the Mean or the Average, is almost always an interpolated value, corresponding to no real measure. If the observations were infinitely numerous its position would not differ more than infinitesimally from that of some one of them; even in a series of one or two hundred in number, the difference is insignificant. Now let us make our Scheme answer another question. Suppose we want to know the percentage of men in the group of which we have been speaking, whose Strength lies between any two specified limits, as between 74 lbs. and 64 lbs. We draw horizontal lines (Fig. 4) from points on the side scale corresponding to either limit, and drop perpendiculars upon the base, from the points where those lines meet the curve. Then the number of Grades in the intercept is the answer. The Fig. shows that the number in the present case is 30 ; therefore 30 per cent, of the group have Strengths of Pull ranging between 74 and 64 lbs. We learn how to transmute female measures of any characteristic into male ones, by comparing their respective schemes, and devising a formula that will change the one into the other. In the case of Stature, the simple multiple of l'O 8 was found to do this with sufficient precision. If we wish to compare the average Strengths of two different groups of persons, say one consisting of men and the other of women, we have simply to compare the values at the 50th Grades in the two schemes. For even if the Medians differ considerably from the Means,IV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 43 both the ratios and the differences between either pair of values would be sensibly the same. A different way of comparing two Schemes is sometimes useful. It is to draw them in opposed directions, as in Fig. 5, p. 40. Their curves will then cut each other at some point, whose Grade when referred to either of the two Schemes (whichever of them may be preferred), determines the point at which the same values are to be found. In Fig. 5, the Grade in the one Scheme is 20°; therefore in the other Scheme it is 100°—20°, or 80°. In respect to the Strength of Pull of men and women, it appears that the woman who occupies the Grade of 96° in her Scheme, has the same strength as the man who occupies the Grade of 4° in his Scheme. I should add that this great inequality in Strength x between the sexes, is confirmed by other measurements made at the same time in respect to the Strength of their Squeeze, as tested by another of Salter’s instruments. Then the woman in the 93rd and the man in the 7th Grade of their resective Schemes, proved to be of equal strength. In my paper1 on the results obtained at the laboratory, I remarked: “Very powerful women exist, but happily perhaps for the repose of the other sex such gifted women are rare. Out of 1,657 adult women of all ages measured at the laboratory, the strongest could only exert a squeeze of 86 lbs., or about that of a medium man.” 1 Journ. Anthropol. Inst. 1885. Mem.: There is a blunder in the paragraph, p. 23, headed “Height Sitting and Standing.” The paragraph should be struck out.u NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. The Shape of Schemes is Independent of the Number of Observations.—When Schemes are drawn from different samples of the same large group of measurements, though the number in the several samples may differ greatly, we can always so adjust the horizontal scales that the breadth of the several Schemes shall be uniform. Then the shapes of the Schemes drawn from different samples will be little affected by the number of observations used in each, supposing of course that the numbers are never too small for ordinary statistical purposes. The only recognisable differences between the Schemes will be, that, if the number of observations in the sample is very large, the upper margin of the Scheme will fall into a more regular curve, especially towards either of its limits. Some irregularity will be found in the above curve of the Strength of Pull; but if the observations had been ten times more numerous, it is probable, judging from much experience of such curves, that the irregularity would have been less conspicuous, and perhaps would have disappeared altogether. However numerous the observations may be, the curve will always be uncertain and incomplete at its extreme ends, because the next value may happen to be greater or less than any one of those that preceded it. Again, the position of the first and the last observation, supposing each observation to have been laid down separately, can never coincide with the adjacent limit. The more numerous the observations, and therefore the closer the perpendiculars by which they are represented, the nearer will the two extreme perpendiculars approach theIV ] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 45 limits, but they will never actually touch them. A chess board has eight squares in a row, and eight pieces may be arranged in order on any one row, each piece occupying the centre of a square. Let the divisions in the row be graduated, calling the boundary to the extreme left, 0°. Then the successive divisions between the squares will be 1°, 2°, 3°, up to 7°, and the boundary to the extreme right will be 8°. It is clear that the position of the first piece lies half-way between the grades (in a scale of eight grades) of 0° and 1°; therefore the grade occupied by the first piece would be counted on that scale as OA0; also the grade of the last piece as 7'5°. Or again, if we had 800 pieces, and the same number of class-places, the grade of the first piece, in a scale of 800 grades, would exceed the grade 0°, by an amount equal to the width of one half-place on that scale, while the last of them would fall short of the 800th grade by an equal amount. This half-place has to be attended to and allowed for when schemes are constructed from comparatively few observations, and always when values that are very near to either of the centesimal grades 0° or 100° are under observation; but between the centesimal grades of 5° and 95° the influence of a half class-place upon the value of the corresponding observation is insignificant, and may be disregarded. It will not henceforth be necessary to repeat the word centesimal. It will be always implied when nothing is said to the contrary, and nothing henceforth will be said to the contrary. The word will be used for the last time in the next paragraph.46 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Data for Eighteen Schemes.—Sufficient data for reconstructing any Scheme, with much correctness, may he printed in a single line of a Table, and according to a uniform plan that is suitable for any kind of values. The measures to be recorded are those at a few definite Grades, beginning say at 5°, ending at 95°, and including every intermediate tenth Grade from 10° to 90°. It is convenient to add those at the Grades 25° and 75°, if space permits. The former values are given for eighteen different Schemes, in Table 2. In the memoir from which that table is reprinted, the values at what I now call (centesimal) Grades, were termed Percentiles. Thus the values at the Grades 5° and 10° would be respectively the 5th and the 10th percentile. It still seems to me that the word percentile is a useful and expressive abbreviation, but it will not be necessary to employ it in the present book. It is of course unadvisable to use more technical words than is absolutely necessary, and it will be possible to get on without it, by the help of the new and more important word “ Grade.” A series of Schemes that express the distribution of various faculties, is valuable in an anthropometric laboratory, for they enable every person who is measured to find his Bank or Grade in each of them. Diagrams may also be constructed by drawing parallel lines, each divided into 100 Grades, and entering each round number of inches, lbs., &c.,.at their proper places. A diagram of this kind is very convenient for reference, but it does not admit of being printed; it must be drawn or lithographed. I have constructed one of theseIV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 47 from the 18 Schemes, and find it is easily understood and much used at my laboratory. Application of Schemes to Inexact Measures.—Schemes of Distribution may be constructed from observations that are barely exact enough to deserve to be called measures. I will illustrate the method of doing so by marshalling the data contained in a singularly interesting little memoir written by Sir James Paget, into the form of such a Scheme. The memoir is published in vol. v. of St. Bartholomew’s Hospital Eeports, and is entitled “ What Becomes of Medical Students.” He traced with great painstaking the career of no less than 1,000 pupils who had attended his classes at that Hospital during various periods and up to a date 15 years previous to that at which his memoir was written. He thus did for St. Bartholomew’s Hospital what has never yet been done, so far as- I am aware, for any University or Public School, whose historians count the successes and are silent as to the failures, giving to inquirers no adequate data for ascertaining the real value of those institutions in English Education. Sir J. Paget divides the successes of his pupils in their profession into five grades, all of which he carefully defines; they are distinguished; considerable; moderate; very limited success; and failures. Several of the students had left the profession either before or after taking their degrees, usually owing to their unfitness to succeed, so after analysing the accounts of them given in the memoir, I drafted48 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. several into the list of failures and distributed the rest, with the result that the number of cases in the successive classes, amounting now to the full total of 1,000, became 28, 80, 616, 151, and 125. This differs, I should say, a little from the inferences of the author, but the matter is here of small importance, so I need not go further into details. If a Scheme is drawn from these figures, in the way described in page 39, it will be found to have the characteristic shape of our familiar curve of Distribution. If we wished to convey the utmost information that this Scheme is capable of giving, we might record in much detail the career of two or three of the men who are clustered about each of a few selected Grades, such as those that are used in Table II., or fewer of them. I adopted this method when estimating the variability of the Visualising Power (Inquiries into Human Faculty). My data were very lax, but this method of treatment got all the good out of them that they possessed. In the present case, it appears that towards the foremost of the successful men within fifteen years of taking their degrees, stood the three Professors of Anatomy at Oxford, Cambridge, and Edinburgh; that towards the bottom of the failures, lay two men who committed suicide under circumstances of great disgrace, and lowest of all Palmer, the Rugeley murderer, who was hanged. We are able to compare any two such Schemes as the above, with numerical precision. The want of exactness in the data from which they are drawn, will of course cling to the result, but no new error will be introducedIV.] SCHEMES OF DISTRIBUTION AND OF FREQUENCY. 49 by the process of comparison. Suppose the second Scheme to refer to the successes of students from another hospital, we should draw the two Schemes in opposed directions, just as was done in the Strength of Pull of Males and Females, Fig. 5, and determine the Grade in either of the Schemes at which success was equal. Schemes of Frequency.—The method of arranging observations in an orderly manner that is generally employed by statisticians, is shown in Fig. 3, page 38, which expresses the same facts as Fig. 2 under a different aspect, and so gives rise to the well-known Curve of “Frequency of Error,” though in Fig. 3 the curve is turned at right angles to the position in which it is usually drawn. It is so placed in order to show more clearly its- relation to the Cúrve of Distribution. The Curve of Frequency is far less convenient than that of Distribution, for the purposes just described and for most of those to be hereafter spoken of. But the Curve of Frequency has other uses, of which advantage will be taken later on, and to which it is unnecessary now to refei'. A Scheme as explained thus far, is nothing more than a compendium of a mass of observations which, on being marshalled in an orderly manner, fall into a diagram whose contour is so regular, simple, and bold, as to admit of being described by a few numerals (Table 2), from which it can at any time be drawn afresh. The regular distribution of the several faculties among a large population is little disturbed by the fact that its E50 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. IV. members are varieties of different types and sub-types. So the distribution of a heavy mass of foliage gives little indication of its growth from separate twigs, of separate branches, of separate trees. The application of theory to Schemes, their approximate description by only two values, and the properties of their bounding Curves, will be described in the next chapter.CHAPTER Y. NORMAL VARIABILITY. Schemes of Deviations.—Normal Curve of Distribution.—Comparison of the observed with the Normal Curve.—The value of a single Deviation at a known Grade determines a Normal Scheme of Deviations.— Two 'Measures at two known Grades determine a Normal Scheme of Measures.—The Charms of Statistics.—Mechanical illustration of the Cause of the Curve of Frequency.—Order in apparent Chaos.— Problems in the Law of Error. Schemes of Deviations.—We have now seen how easy it is to represent the distribution of any quality among a multitude of men, either by a simple diagram or by a line containing a few figures. In this chapter it will be shown that a considerably briefer description is approximately sufficient. Every measure in a Scheme is equal to its Middlemost, or Median value, or M, plus or minus a certain Deviation from M. The Deviation, or “ Error ” as it is technically called, is plus for all grades above 50°, zero for 50°, and minus for all grades below 50°. Thus if (±D) be the deviation from M in any particular case, every measure in a Scheme may be expressed in the E 252 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [CIIAF. form of M + (±D). If IVI = 0, or if it is subtracted from every measure, the residues which are the different values of (±D) will form a Scheme by themselves. Schemes may therefore be made of Deviations as well as of Measures, and one of the former is seen in the upper part of Fig. 6, page 40. It is merely the upper portion of the corresponding Scheme of Measures, in which the axis of the curve plays the part of the base. A strong family likeness runs between the 18 different Schemes of Deviations that may be respectively derived from the data in the 18 lines of Table 2. If the slope of the curve in one Scheme is steeper than that of another, we need only to fore-shorten the steeper Scheme, by inclining it away from the line of sight, in order to reduce its apparent steepness and to make it look almost identical with the other. Or, better still, we may select appropriate vertical scales that will enable all the Schemes to be drawn afresh with a uniform slope, and be made strictly comparable. Suppose that we have only two Schemes, a. and b., that we wish to compare. Let L.l5 L.2 be the lengths of the perpendiculars at two specified, grades in Scheme A., and K.j K.2 the lengths of those at the same grades in Scheme B. ; then if every one of the data from which Scheme B. was drawn be multiplied by L.t — L.2. K.a —K.2’ series of transmuted data will be obtained for drawing a new Scheme b'., on such a vertical scale that its general slope between the selected grades shall be the same as in Scheme A. For practical convenience theV.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 53 selected Grades will be always those of 25° and 75°. They stand at the first and third quarterly divisions of the base, and are therefore easily found by a pair of compasses. They are also well placed to afford a fair criterion of the general slope of the Curve. If we call the perpendicular at 25°, Q.x; and that at 75°, Q.2, then the unit by which every Scheme will be defined is its value of ^(Q.2 — Q-i), and will be called its Q. As the M measures the Average Height of the curved boundary of a Scheme, so the Q measures its general slope. When we wish to transform many different Schemes, numbered I., II., III., &c., whose respective values of Q are qu qi} qB, &c., to others whose values of Q are in each case equal to q0, then all the data from which Scheme I. was drawn, must be multiplied by 2®; those from which Scheme II. was drawn, by 2®, and so on, and new Schemes have to be constructed from these transmuted values. Our Q has the further merit of being practically the same as the value which mathematicians call the “ Probable Error,” of which we shall speak further on. Want of space in Table 2 prevented the insertion of the measures at the Grades 25° and 75°, but those at 20° and 30° are given on the one hand, and those at 70° and 80° on the other, whose respective averages differ but little from the values at 25° and 75°. I therefore will use those four measures to obtain a value for our unit, which we will call Q', to distinguish it from Q.54 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. These are not identical in value, because the outline of the Scheme is a curved and not a straight line, but the difference between them is small, and is approximately the same in all Schemes. It will shortly be seen that Q'=l,015xQ approximately; therefore a series of Deviations measured in terms of the large unit Q' are numerically smaller than if they had been measured in terms of the small unit (for the same reason that the numerals in 2, 3, &c.,feet are smaller than those in the corresponding values of 24, 36, &c., inches), and they must be multiplied by 1.015 when it is desired to change them into a series having the smaller value of Q for their unit. All the 18 Schemes of Deviation that can be derived from Table 2 have been treated on these principles, and the results are given in Table 3. Their general accordance with one another, and still more with the mean of all of them, is obvious. Normal Curve of Distribution.—The values in the bottom line of Table 3, which is headed “ Normal Values when Q = 1,” and which correspond with minute precision to those in the line immediately above them, are not derived from observations at all, but from the well-known Tables of the “ Probability Integral ” in a way that mathematicians will easily understand by comparing the Tables 4 to 8 inclusive. I need hardly remind the reader that the Law of Error upon which these Normal Values are based, was excogitated for the use of astronomers and others who are concerned with extremeV.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 55 accuracy of measurement, and without the slightest idea until the time of Quetelet that they might be applicable to human measures. But Errors, Differences, Deviations, Divergencies, Dispersions, and individual Variations, all spring from the same kind of causes. Objects that bear the same name, or can be described by the same phrase, are thereby acknowledged to have common points of resemblance, and to rank as members of the same species, class, or whatever else we may please to call the group. On the other hand, every object has Differences peculiar to itself, by which it is distinguished from others. This general statement is applicable to thousands of instances. The Law of Error finds a footing wherever the individual peculiarities are wholly due to the combined influence of a multitude of “accidents,” in the sense in which that word has already been defined. All persons conversant with statistics are aware that this supposition brings Variability within the grasp of the laws of Chance, with the result that the relative frequency of Deviations of different amounts admits of being calculated, when those amounts are measured in terms of any self-contained unit of variability, such as our Q. The Tables 4 to 8 give the results of these purely mathematical calculations, and the Curves based upon them may with propriety be distinguished as “Normal.” Tables 7 and 8 are based upon the familiar Table of the Probability Integral, given in Table 5, via that in Table 6, in which the unit of variability is taken to be the “ Probable Error ” or our Q, and not the “Modulus.” Then I turn Table 656 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. inside out, as it were, deriving the “ arguments ” for Tables 7 and 8 from the entries in the body of Table 6, and making other easily intelligible alterations. Comparison of the Observed with the Normal Curve. —I confess to having been amazed at the extraordinary coincidence between the two bottom lines of Table 3, considering the great variety of faculties contained in the 18 Schemes; namely, three kinds of linear measurement, besides one of weight, one of capacity, two of strength, one of vision, and one of swiftness. It is obvious that weight cannot really vary at the same rate as height, even allowing for the fact that tall men are often lanky, but the theoretical impossibility is of the less practical importance, as the variations in weight are small compared to the weight itself. Thus we see from the value of Q in the first column of Table 3, that half of the persons deviated from their M by no more than 10 or 11 lbs., which is about one-twelfth part of the value of M. Although the several series in Table 3 run fairly well together, I should not have dared to hope that their irregularities would have balanced one another so beautifully as they have done. It has been objected to some of my former work, especially in Hereditary Genius, that I pushed the applications of the Law of Frequency of Error somewhat too far. I may have done so, rather by incautious phrases than in reality; but I am sure that, with the evidence now before us, the applicability of that law is more than justified within the reasonable limits asked for in the present book. Iv.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 57 am satisfied to claim that the Normal Curve is a fair average representation of the Observed Curves during nine-tenths of their course ; that is, for so much of them as lies between the grades of 5° and 95°. In particular, the agreement of the Curve of Stature with the Normal Curve is very fair, and forms a mainstay of my inquiry into the laws of Natural Inheritance. It has already been said that mathematicians laboured at the law of Error for one set of purposes, and we are entering into the fruits of their labours for another. Hence there is no ground for surprise that their Nomenclature is often cumbrous and out of place, when applied to problems in heredity. This is especially the case with regard to their term of “ Probable Error,” by which they mean the value that one half of the Errors exceed and the other half fall short of. This is practically the same as our Q.1 It is strictly the same whenever the two halves of the Scheme of Deviations to which it applies are symmetrically disposed about their common axis. The term Probable Error, in its plain English interpretation of the most Probable Error, is quite misleading, for it is not that. The most Probable Error (as Dr. Yenn has pointed out, in his Logic of Chance) 1 The following little Table may be of service :— Values of the different Constants when the Prob. Error is taken as unity, and their corresponding Grades. Prob. Error.......... 1*000; corresponding Grades 25°*0, 75c,0 Modulus.............. 2 097 ; „ „ 7°*9, 92°*1 Mean Error...........1*183; „ „ 21°*2, 78°‘8 Error of Mean Squares 1*483 ; „ „ 16c0, 84c*058 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chai\ is zero. This results from what was said a few pages back about the most probable measure in a Scheme being its M. In a Scheme of Errors the (VI is equal to 0, therefore the most Probable Error in such a Scheme is 0 also. It is astonishing that mathematicians, who are the most precise and perspicacious of men, have not long since revolted against this cumbrous, slip-shod, and misleading phrase. They really mean what I should call the Mid-Error, but their phrase is too firmly established for me to uproot it. I shall however always write the word Probable when used in this sense, in the form of “ Prob. ”; thus “ Prob. Error,” as a continual protest against its illegitimate use, and as some slight safeguard against its misinterpretation. Moreover the term Probable Error is absurd when applied to the subjects now in hand, snch as Stature, Eye-colour, Artistic Faculty, or Disease. I shall therefore usually speak of Prob. Deviation. Though the value of our Q is the same as that of the Prob. Deviation, Q is not a convertible term with Prob. Deviation. We shall often have to speak of the one without immediate reference to the other, just as we speak of the diameter of the circle without reference to any of its properties, such as, if fines are drawn from its ends to any point in the circumference, they will meet at a right angle. The Q of a Scheme is as definite a phrase as the Diameter of a Circle, but we cannot replace Q in that phrase by the words Prob. Deviation, and speak of the Prob. Deviation of a Scheme, without doing some violence to language. We*.] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 59 should have to express ourselves from, another point of view, and at much greater length, and say “ the Prob. Deviation of any, as yet unknown measure in the Scheme, from the Mean of all the measures from which the Scheme was constructed.” The primary idea of Q has no reference to the existence of a Mean value from which Deviations take place. It is half the difference between the measures found at the 25th and 75th Centesimal Grades. In this definition, there is not the slightest allusion, direct or indirect, to the measure at the 50th Grade, which is the value of M. It is perfectly true that the measure at Grade 25° is M—Q, and that at Grade 75° is M + Q, but all this is superimposed upon the primary conception. Q stands essentially on its own basis, and has nothing to do with M. It will often happen that we shall have to deal with Prob : Deviations, but that is no reason why we should not use Q whenever it suits our purposes better, especially as statistical statements tend to be so cumbrous that every abbreviation is welcome. The stage to which we have now arrived is this. It has been shown that the distribution of very different human qualities and faculties is approximately Normal, and it is inferred that with reasonable precautions we may treat them as if they were wholly so, in order to obtain approximate results. We shall thus deal with an entire Scheme of Deviations in terms of its Q, and with an entire Scheme of Measures in terms of its M and Q, just as we deal with an entire Circle in terms of its60 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. radius, or with an entire Ellipse in terms of its major and minor axes. We can also apply the various beautiful properties of the Law of Frequency of Error to the observed values of Q. In doing so, we act like woodsmen who roughly calculate the cubic contents of the trunk of a tree, by measuring its length, and its girth at either end, and submitting their measures to formulae that have been deduced from the properties of ideally perfect straight lines and circles. Their results prove serviceable, although the trunk is only rudely straight and circular. I trust that my results will be yet closer approximations to the truth than those usually arrived at by the woodsmen. The value of a single Deviation at a known Grade determines a Normal Scheme of Deviations.—When Normal Curves of Distribution are drawn within the same limits, they differ from each other only in their general slope; and the slope is determined if the value of the Deviation is given at any one specified Grade. It must be borne in mind that the width of the limits between which the Scheme is drawn, has no influence on the values of the Deviations at the various Grades, because the latter are proportionate parts of the base. As the limits vary in width, so do the intervals between the Grades. When measuring the Deviation at a specified Grade for the purpose of determining the whole Curve, it is of course convenient to adhere to the same Grade in all cases. It will be recollected that when dealing with the observed curves a few pages back, IV] NORMAL VARIABILITY. 61 used not one Grade but two Grades for the purpose, namely 25° and 75° ; but in the Normal Curve, the plus and minus Deviations are equal in amount at all pairs of symmetrical distances on either side of grade 50°; therefore the Deviation at either of the Grades 25° or 7 5° is equal to Q, and suffices to define the entire Curve. The reason why a certain value Q' was stated a few pages back to be equal to 1'015 Q, is that the Normal Deviations at 20° and at 30°, (whose average we called Q') are found in Table 8, to be 1*25 and 0*78; and similarly those at 70° and 60°. The average of 1*25 and 0*78 is 1*015-, whereas the Deviation at 25° or at 75° is 1*000. Two Measures at known Grades determine a Normal Scheme of Measures.■—If we know the value of M as well as that of Q we know the entire Scheme. M expresses the mean value of all the objects contained in the group, and Q defines their variability. But if we know the Measures at any two specified Grades, we can deduce M and Q from them, and so determine the entire Scheme. The method of doing this is explained in the foot-note.1 1 The following is a fuller description of the propositions in this and in the preceding paragraph :— (1) In any Normal Scheme, and therefore approximately in an observed one, if the value of the Deviation is given at any one specified Grade the whole Curve is determined. Let D be the given Deviation, and d the tabular Deviation at the same Grade, as found in Table 8 ; then multiply every entry in Table 8 by^. As the tabular value of Q is 1, it will become changed into d62 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. The Charms of Statistics.—It is difficult to understand why statisticians commonly limit their inquiries to Averages, and do not revel in more comprehensive views. Their souls seem as dull to the charm of variety as that of the native of one of our flat English counties, whose retrospect of Switzerland was that, if its mountains could be thrown into its lakes, two nuisances would be got rid of at once. An Average is but a solitary fact, whereas if a single other fact be added to it, an entire Normal Scheme, which nearly corresponds to the observed one, starts potentially into existence. Some people hate the very name of statistics, but I find them full of beauty and interest. Whenever they are not brutalised, but delicately handled by the higher methods, and are warily interpreted, their power of dealing with complicated phenomena is extraordinary. They are the only tools by which an opening can be cut (2) If the Measures at any two specified Grades are given, the whole Scheme of Measures is thereby determined. Let A, B he the two given Measures of which A is the larger, and let a, b be the values of the tabular Deviations for the same Grades, as found in Table 8, not omitting their signs of plus or minus as the case may be. A —B Then the Q of the Scheme = ± ..—_ . (The sign of Q is not to be regarded ; it is merely a magnitude.) M *= <4 - a-Q; or IM = £ - & Q. Example : A, situated at Grade 55°, = 14*38 B, situated at Grade 5°, = 912 The corresponding tabular Deviations are :—a = + 019 ; b = 14*38 - 9*12 5*26 -2*44. Therefore Q = = 2*0 0*19 + 2*44 2*63 M = 14.38 - 0*19 X 2 = 14 0 or = 9*12 + 2*44 X 2 = 14*0NORMAL VARIABILITY. 03 v.] through the formidable thicket of difficulties that bars the path of those who pursue the Science of man. Mechanical Illustration of the Cause of the Curve of Frequency.—The Curve of Frequency, and that of Distribution, are convertible : therefore if the genesis of either of them can be made clear, that of the other becomes also intelligible. I shall now illustrate the origin of the Curve of Frequency, by means of an apparatus shown in Fig. 7, that mimics in a very pretty way the conditions A B on which Deviation depends. It is a frame glazed in front, leaving a depth of about a quarter of an inch behind the glass. Strips are placed in the upper part to act as a funnel. Below the outlet of the funnel stand a FIG .7.64 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [/— x -f 1 x-4, + \ x 2 X^ + 'X 4 x -i + &c. 13 22 94 26 9 2s \ ; = d[- + (12 (L V24 2i 3 x 25 32 x 26 + &C, -))-Dx(-n i2+ro)-Dx0'16- So that each grandparent contributes on the average 0T6 (more exactly 0T583) of his peculiarity to the heritage of S, and the four grandparents contribute between them 0'64, leaving 36 indeterminate, which when rateably assigned gives 0'25 to light and 0T1 to dark. A hazel-eyed grandparent contributes, according to the ratio described in the last paragraph, O’lO to light and O'06 to dark. All this is clearly expressed and employed in the columns II. of Tables 17 and 18. III. Eye-colours given of the two Parents and four Grand-Parents— Suppose F to possess D, then F taken alone, and not in connection with what his possession of D might imply concerning the contributions of the previous ancestry, will contribute an average of 0'25 to the heritage of S. Suppose Gi also to possess D, then his contribution together with what his possession of D may imply concerning the previous ancestry, was calculated in the last paragraph as D x ^ = D x 0'07 5. For the con- venience of using round numbers I take this as Dx0'08. So the two parents contribute between152 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. them 0’50 of the peculiarity of S, the four grandparents together with what they imply of the previous ancestry contribute 0‘32, being an aggregate of 0'82, leaving a residue of 0‘18 to be rateably assigned as 012 to light, and 0‘6 to dark. A hazel-eyed Parent is here reckoned as contributing 0‘16 to light and 0-9 to dark; a hazel-eyed Grand-Parent as contributing 0'5 to light and 0-3 to dark. All this is tabulated in Table 17, and its working explained by an example in the columns headed III. of Table 18. Results.—A mere glance at Tables 19 and 20 will show how surprisingly accurate the predictions are, and therefore how true the basis of the calculations must be. Their average correctness is shown best by the totals in Table 19, which give an aggregate of calculated numbers of light-eyed children under Groups 1., II., and III. as 623, 601, and 614 respectively, when the observed numbers were 629 ; that is to say, they are correct in the ratios of 99, 96, and 98 to 100. Their trustworthiness when applied to individual families is shown as strongly in Table 20 whose results are conveniently summarised in Table 21. I have there classified the amounts of error in the several calculations : thus if the estimate in any one family was 3 lighteyed children, and the observed number was 4, I should count the error as 1-0. I have worked to one place of decimals in this table, in order to bring out the different shades of trustworthiness in the three sets of calculations, which thus become very apparent. It will beVIII.] DISCUSSION OF THE DATA OF EYE COLOUR. 153 seen that the calculations in Class III. are by far the most precise. In more than one-half of those calculations the error does not exceed 0'5, whereas in more than three-quarters of those in I. and II. the error is at least of that amount. Only one-quarter of Class III., but somewhere about the half of Classes I. and II., are more than IT in error. In comparing I. with II., we find I. to he slightly but I think distinctly the superior estimate. The relative accuracy of III. as compared with I. and II., is what we should have expected, supposing the basis of the calculations to be true, because the additional knowledge utilised in III., over what is turned to account in I. and II., must be an advantage. My returns are insufficiently numerous and too subject to uncertainty of observation, to make it worth while to submit them to a more rigorous analysis, but the broad conclusion to which the present results irresistibly lead, is that the same peculiar hereditary relation that was shown to subsist between a man and each of his ancestors in respect to the quality of Stature, also subsists in respect to that of Eye-colour.CHAPTER IX THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. Data.—Sexual Distribution.—Marriage Selection.—Regression.—Effect of Bias in Marriage. Data.—It is many years since I described the family history of the great Painters and Musicians in Hereditary Genius. The inheritance of much less exceptional gifts of Artistic Faculty will be discussed in this chapter, and from an entirely different class of data. They are the answers in my R.F.F collection, to the question of “ Favourite pursuits and interests ? Artistic aptitudes ? ” The list of persons who were signalised as being especially fond of music and drawing, no doubt includes many who are artistic in a very moderate degree. Still they form a fairly well defined class, and one that is easy to discuss because their family history is complete. In this respect, they are much more suitable subjects for statistical inquiry than the great Painters and Musicians, whose biographers usually say little or nothing of their non-artistic relatives.CHAP. IX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 155 The object of the present chapter is not to give a reply to the simple question, whether or no the Artistic faculty tends to be inherited.' A man must be very crotchety or very ignorant, who nowadays seriously doubts the inheritance either of this or of any other faculty. The question is whether or no its inheritance follows a similar law to that which has been shown to govern Stature and Eye-colour, and which has been worked out with some completeness in the foregoing chapters. Before answering this question,, it will be convenient to compare the distribution of the Artistic faculty in the two sexes, and to learn the influence it may exercise on marriage selection. I began by dividing my data into four classes of aptitudes; the first was for music alone; the second for drawing alone; the third for both music and drawing ; and the fourth includes all those about whose artistic capacities a discreet silence was observed. After prefatory trials, I found it so difficult to separate aptitude for music from aptitude for drawing, that I determined to throw the three first classes into the single group of Artistic. This and the group of the Non-Artistic are the only two divisions now to be considered. A difficulty presented itself at the outset in respect to the families that included boys, girls, and young children, whose artistic tastes and capacities can seldom be fairly judged, while they are liable to be appraised too favourably by the compiler of the Family records, especially if he or she was one of their parents. As the practice of picking and choosing is very hazardous in156 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. statistical inquiries, however fair our intentions may he, and as it in justice always excites suspicion, I decided, though with much regret at their loss, to omit the whole of those who were not adult. Sexual Distribution.—Men and women, as classes, may differ little in their natural artistic capacity, but such difference as there is in adult life is somewhat in favour of the women. Table 9b contains 894 cases, 447 of men and 447 of women, divided into three groups according to the rank they hold in the pedigrees. These groups agree fairly well among themselves, and therefore their aggregate results may be freely accepted as trustworthy. They show that 28 per cent, of the males are Artistic and 72 are Not Artistic, and that there are 33 per cent. Artistic females to 67 who are Not Artistic. Part of this female superiority is doubtless to be ascribed to the large share that music and drawing occupy in the education of women, and to the greater leisure that most girls have, or take, for amusing themselves. If the artistic gifts of men and women are naturally the same, as the experience of schools where music and drawing are taught, apparently shows it to be, the small difference observed in favour of women in adult life would be a measure of the smallness of the effect of education compared to that of natural talent. Disregarding the distinction of sex, the figures in Table 9 b show that the number of Artistic to Non-Artistic persons in the general population is in the proportionIX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 157 of 30^ to 69-|. The data used in Table 22 refer to a considerably larger number of persons, and do not include more than two-thirds of those employed in Table 9b, and they make the proportion to be 31 to 69. So we shall be quite correct enough if we reckon that out of ten persons in the families of my E.F.F. correspondents, three on the average are artistic and seven are not. Marriage Selection.—Table 9b enables us to ascertain whether there is any tendency, or any disinclination among the Artistic and the Non-Artistic, to marry within their respective castes. It shows the observed frequency of their marriages in each of the three possible combinations ; namely, both husband and wife artistic ; one artistic and one not; and both not artistic. The Table also gives the calculated frequency of the three classes, supposing the pairings to be regulated by the laws of chance. There is I think trustworthy evidence of the existence of some slight disinclination to marry within the same caste, for signs of it appear in each of the three sets of families with which the Table deals. The total result is that there are only 36 per cent, of such marriages observed, whereas if there had been no disinclination but perfect indifference, the number would have been raised to 42. The difference is small and the figures are few, but for the above reasons it is not likely to be fallacious. I believe the facts to be, that highly artistic people keep pretty much to themselves, but that the very much larger body of158 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. moderately artistic people do not. A man of highly artistic temperament must look on those who are deficient in it, as barbarians; he would continually crave for a sympathy and response that such persons are incapable of giving. On the other hand, eyery quiet unmusical man must shrink a little from the idea of wedding himself to a grand piano in constant action, with its vocal and peculiar social accompaniments ; but he might anticipate great pleasure in having a wife of a moderately artistic temperament, who would give colour and variety to his prosaic life. On the other hand, a sensitive and imaginative wife would be conscious of needing the aid of a husband who had enough plain common-sense to restrain her too enthusiastic and frequently foolish projects. If wife is read for husband, and husband for wife, the same argument still holds true. Regression.—Having disposed of these preliminaries, we will now examine into the conditions of the inheritance of the Artistic Faculty. The data that bear upon it are summarised in Table 22, where I have not cared to separate the sexes, as my data are not numerous enough to allow of more subdivision than can be helped. Also, because from such calculations as I have made, the hereditary influences of the two sexes in respect to art appear to be pretty equal: as they are in respect to nearly every other characteristic, exclusive of diseases, that I have examined. It is perfectly conceivable that the Artistic FacultyIX.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 159 in any person might be somehow measured, and its amount determined; just as we may measure Strength, the power of Discrimination of Tints, or the tenacity of Memory. Let us then suppose the measurement of the Artistic Faculty to be feasible and to have been often performed, and that the measures of a large number of persons were thrown into a Scheme. It is reasonable to expect that the Scheme of the Artistic Faculty would be approximately Normal in its proportions, like those of the various Qualities and Faculties whose measures were given in Tables 2 and 3. It is also reasonable to expect that the same law of inheritance might hold good in the Artistic Faculty that was found to hold good both in Stature and in Eye colour; in other words, that the value of Filial Regression would in this case also be ■§. We have now to discover whether these assumptions are true without any help from direct measurement. The problem to be solved is a pretty one, and will illustrate the method by which many problems of a similar class have to be worked. Let the graduations of the scale by which the Artistic Faculty is supposed to be measured, be such that the unit of the scale shall be equal to the Q of the Art-Scheme of the general population. Call the unknown M of the Art-Scheme of the population, P. Then, as explained in page 52, the measure of any individual will be of the form P + (± D), where D is the deviation from P. The first fact we have to deal with is, that only 30 per cent, of the population1G0 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. are Artistic. Therefore no person whose Grade in the Art-Scheme does not exceed 70° can be reckoned as Artistic. Referring to Table 8 we see that the value of D for the Grade of 70° is + 0'78 ; consequently the art-measure of an Artistic person, when reckoned in units of the accepted scale, must exceed P + 0'78. The average art-measure of all persons whose Grade is higher than 70°, may be obtained with sufficient approximation by taking the average of all the values given in Table 8, for every Grade, or more simply for every odd Grade from 71° to 99° inclusive. It will be found to be l'7l. Therefore an artistic person has, on the average, an art-measure of P + 1*71. We will consider persons of this measure to be representatives of the whole of the artistic portion of the Population. It is not strictly correct to do, so, but for approximative purposes this rough and ready method will suffice, instead of the tedious process of making a separate calculation for each Grade. The M of the Co-Fraternity born of a group of Mid-Parents whose measure is P + 1*71 will be P + (f x 1 '71) or (P + 1'4). We will call this value C. The Q of this or any other Co-Fraternity may be expected to bear approximately the same ratio to the Q of the general population, that it did in the case of Stature, namely, that of 1'5 to 1'7. Therefore the Q of the Co-Fraternity who are born of Mid-Parents whose Art-measure is C, will be 0*88. The artistic members of this Co-Fraternity will be those whose measures exceed {P + 0'78}. We may write thisI*.] THE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 161 value in the form of {(P + 1 ’4) — 0'36}, or {C — 0*36}. Table 8 shows that the Deviation of— 0‘36 is found at the Grade of 40°. Consequently 40 per cent, of this Co-Fraternity will be Non-Artistic and 60 per cent, will be Artistic. Observation Table 23 shows the numbers to be 36 and 64, which is a very happy agreement. Next as regards the Non-Artistic Parents. The Non-Artistic portion of the Population occupy the 7 0 first Grades in the Art-Scheme, and may be divided into two groups; one consisting of 40 Grades, and standing between the Grades of 70° and 30°, or between the Grade of 50° and 20 Grades on either side of it, the average Art-measure of whom is P; the other group standing below 30°, whose average measure maybe taken to be P — 1 *71, for the same reason that the group above 70° was taken as P + 1 *71- Consequently the average measure of the entire Non-Artistic class is -^{(40 x P) + 30 (P - 1-71)} = P - ?{} x 1-71 = P - 0-73. Supposing Mid-Parents of this measure, to represent the entire Non-Artistic group, their offspring will be a Co-Fraternity having for their M the value of P — {-§- x 0 '7 3} or P — 0-49, which we will call C', and for their Q the value of 0'88 as before. Such among them as exceed {P — 0*78}, which we may write in the form of {(P — 0'49) + (l-27)}, or {C + 1 *27}, are Artistic, and they are those who, according to Table 8, rank higher than the Grade 83°. In other words, 83 per cent, of the children of Non- M162 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. Artistic parents will be Non-Artistic, and tbe remainder of 17 per cent, will be Artistic. Observation gives the values of 79 and 21, which is a very fair coincidence. When one parent is Artistic and the other Not, their joint hereditary influence would be the average of the above two cases; that is to say, ^ (40 + 83), or 6l| per cent, of their children would be Non-Artistic, and ^ (60 + 17), or 38^, would be Artistic. The observed numbers are 61 and 39, which agree excellently well. We may therefore conclude that the same law of Regression, and all that depends upon it, which governs the inheritance both of Stature and Eye-colour, applies equally to the Artistic Faculty. Effect of Bias in Marriage.—The slight apparent disinclination of the Artistic and the Non-Artistic to marry in their own caste, is hardly worth regarding, but it is right to clearly understand the extreme effect that might be occasioned by Bias in Marriage. Suppose the attraction of like to like to become paramount, so that each individual in a Scheme married his or her nearest available neighbour, then the Scheme of Mid-Parents would be practically identical with the Scheme drawn from the individual members of the population. In the case of Stature their Q would be 1’7 inch, instead of 1‘7 divided by \/2. The regression and subsequent dispersion remaining unchanged, the Q of the offspring would consequently be increased. On the other hand, suppose the attraction of contrastIX.] TIIE ARTISTIC FACULTY. 163 to become suddenly paramount, so that Grade 99° paired in an instant with Grade 1°; next 98° with 2°; and so on in order, until the' languid desires of 49° and 51° were satisfied last of all. Then every one of the Mid-Parents would be of precisely the same stature P. Consequently their Q would be zero; and that of the system of the Mid-Co-Fraternities would be zero also; hence the Q of the next generation would contract to the Q of a Co-Fraternity, that is to 1\5 inch. Whatever might be the character or strength of the bias in marriage selection, so long as it remains constant the Q of the population would tend to become constant also, and the statistical resemblance between successive generations of the future Population would be ensured. The stability of the balance between the opposed tendencies of Regression and of Co-Fraternal expansion is due to the Regression increasing with the Deviation. Its effect is like that of a spring acting against a weight; the spring stretches until its gradually increasing resilient force balances the steady pull of the weight, then the two forces of spring and weight are in stable equilibrium. For, if the weight be lifted by the hand, it will obviously fall down again as soon as the hand is withdrawn; or again, if it be depressed by the hand, the resilience of the spring will become increased, and the weight will rise up again when it is left free to do so.CHAPTER X. DISEASE. Preliminary Problem.—Data.—Trustworthiness of R.F.F. Data.—Mixture of Inheritances.—Consumption : General Remarks ; Parent to Child; Distribution of Fraternities; Severely Tainted Fraternities; Con-sump tivity.—Data for Hereditary Diseases. The vital statistics of a population are those of a vast army marching rank behind rank, across the treacherous table-land of life. Some of its members drop out of sight at every step, and a new rank is ever rising up to take the place vacated by the rank that preceded it, and which has already moved on. The population retains its peculiarities although the elements of which it is composed are never stationary, neither are the same individuals present at any two successive epochs. In these respects, a population may be compared to a cloud that seems to repose in calm upon a mountain plateau, while a gale of wind is blowing over it. The outline of the cloud remains unchanged, although its elements are in violent movement and in a condition of perpetual destruction and renewal. TheX.] DISEASE. 165 well understood cause of such clouds is the deflection of a wind laden with invisible vapour, by means of the sloping flanks ctf the mountain, up to a level at which the atmosphere is much colder and rarer than below. Part of the invisible vapour with wdiich the wind was charged, becomes thereby condensed into the minute particles of water of which clouds are formed. After a while the process is reversed. The particles of cloud having been carried by the wind across the plateau, are swept down the other side of it again to a lower level, and during their descent they return into invisible vapour. Both in the cloud and in the population, there is on the one hand a continual supply and inrush of new individuals from the unseen; they remain a while as visible objects, and then disappear. The cloud and the population are composed of elements that resemble each other in the brevity of their existence, while the general features of the cloud and of the population are alike in that they abide. Preliminary Problem.—The proportion of the population that dies at each age, is well known, and the diseases of which they die are also well known, but the statistics of hereditary disease are as yet for the most part contradictory and untrustworthy. It is most desirable as a preliminary to more minute inquiries, that the causes of death of a large number of persons should be traced during two successive generations in somewhat the same broad way that Stature and several other peculiarities were traced in the pre-166 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. ceding chapters. There are a certain number of recognized groups of disease, which we may call A, B, C, &c., and the proportion of persons who die of these diseases in each of the two generations is the same. The preliminary question to be determined is whether and to what extent those who die of A in the second generation, are more or less often descended from those who died of A in the first generation, than would have been the case if disease were neither hereditarily transmitted nor clung to the same families for any other reason. Similarly as regards B, C, D, and the rest. This inquiry would be more difficult than those hitherto attempted, because longevity and fertility are both affected by the state of health, and the circumstances of home life and occupation have a great effect in causing and in checking disease. Also because the father and mother are found in some notable cases to contribute disease in very different degrees to their male and female descendants. I had hoped even to the last moment, that my collection of Family Records would have contributed in some small degree towards answering this question, but after many attempts I find them too fragmentary for the purpose. It was a necessary condition of success to have the completed life-histories of many Fraternities who were born some seventy or more years ago, that is, during the earlier part of this century, as well as those of their parents and all their uncles and aunts. My Records contain excellent material of a later date, that will be valuable in future years; but they mustX.] DISEASE. 167 bide tbeir time ; they are insufficient for the period in question. By attempting to work with incompleted life histories the risk of serious error is incurred. Data.—The Schedule in Appendix Gr, which is illustrated in more detail by Tables A and B that follow it, shows the amount of information that I had hoped to obtain from those who were in a position to furnish complete returns. It relates to the “ Subject ” of the pedigree and to each of his 14 direct ancestors, up to the great-grandparents inclusive, making in all 15 persons. Also, to the Fraternities of which each of these 15 persons was a member. Beckoning the total average number of persons in each fraternity at 5, which is under the mark for my R.F.F collection, questions were thus asked concerning an average of 75 different persons in each family. . The total number of the Records that I am able to use, is about 160; so the aggregate of the returns of disease ought to have been about twelve thousand, and should have included the causes of death of perhaps 6,000 of them. As a matter of fact, I have only about one-third of the latter number. Trustworthiness of R.F.F. data.—The first object was to ascertain the trustworthiness of the medical information sent to me. There is usually much disinclination among families to allude to the serious diseases that they fear to inherit, and it was necessary to learn whether this tendency towards suppression notably vitiated the returns. The test applied was both simple and just.168 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. If consumption, cancer, drink and suicide, appear among the recorded cases of death less frequently than they do in ordinary tables of mortality, then a bias towards suppression could be proved and measured, and would have to be reckoned with ; otherwise the returns might be accepted as being on the whole honest and outspoken. I find the latter to be the case. Sixteen per cent, of the causes of death (or 1 in 6^) are ascribed to consumption, 5 per cent, to cancer, and nearly 2 per cent, to drink and to suicide respectively. Insanity was not specially asked about, as I did not think it wise to put too many disagreeable questions, however it is often mentioned. I dare say that it, or at least eccentricity, is not unfrequently passed over. Careful accuracy in framing the replies appears to have been the rule rather than the exception. In the preface to the blank forms of the Records of Family Faculties and elsewhere, I had explained my objects so fully and they were so reasonable in themselves, that my correspondents have evidently entered with interest into what was asked for, and shown themselves willing to trust me freely with their family histories. They seem generally to have given all that was known to them, after making much search and many inquiries, and after due references to registers of deaths. The insufficiency of their returns proceeds I feel sure, much less from a desire to suppress unpleasant truths than from pure ignorance, and the latter is in no small part due to the scientific ineptitude of the mass of the members of the medical profession two and more generations ago, when even the stetho-X] DISEASE. ICO scope was unknown. They were then incompetent to name diseases correctly. Mixture of Inheritances.—The first thing that struck me after methodically classifying the diseases of each family, in the form shown in the Schedule, wTas their great intermixture. The Tables A and B in Appendix Clare offered as ordinary specimens of what is everywhere to he found. They are actual cases, except that I have given fancy names and initials, and for further concealment, have partially transposed the sexes. Imagine an intermarriage between any two in the lower division of these tables, and then consider the variety of inheritable disease to wrhich their children would be liable ! The problem is rendered yet more complicated by the metamorphoses of disease. The disease A in the parent does not necessarily appear, even when inherited, as A in the children. We know very little indeed about the effect of a mixture of inheritable diseases, how far they are mutually exclusive and how far they blend ; or how far when they blend, they change into a third form. Owing to the habit of free inter-marriage no person can be exempt from the inheritance of a vast variety of diseases or of special tendencies to them. Deaths by mere old age and the accompanying failure of vital powers without any well defined malady, are very common in my collection, but I do not find as a rule, that the children of persons who die of old age have any marked immunity from specific diseases. There is a curious double appearance in the Records.170 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. the one of an obvious hereditary tendency to disease and the other of the reverse. There are far too many striking instances of coincidence between the diseases of the parents and of the children to admit of reasonable doubt of their being often inherited. On the other hand, when I hide with my hand the lower part of a page such as those in Tables A and B, and endeavour to make a forecast of what I shall find under my hand after studying the upper portion, I am sometimes greatly mistaken. Very unpromising marriages have occasionally led to good results, especially where the parental disease is one that usually breaks out late in life, as in the case of cancer. The children may then enjoy a fair length of days and die in the end of some other disease; although if that disease had been staved off it is quite possible that the cancer would ultimately have appeared. I have two remarkable instances of this. In one of them, three grandparents out of four died of cancer. In each .of the fraternities of which the father and mother were members, one and one person only, died of it. As to the children, although four of them have lived to past seventy years, not one has shown any sign of cancer. The other case differs in details, but is equally remarkable. However diseased the parents may be, it is of course possible that the children may inherit the healthier constitutions of their remoter ancestry. Promising looking marriages are occasionally found to lead to a sickly progeny, but my materials are too scanty to permit of a thorough investigation of these cases. The general conclusion thus far is, that owing toX.] DISEASE. 171 the hereditary tendencies in each person to disease being usually very various, it is by no means always that useful forecasts can be made concerning the health of the future issue of any couple. Consumption. General Remarks.—The frequency of consumption in England being so great that one in at least every six or seven persons dies of it, and the fact that it usually appears early in life, and is therefore the less likely to be forestalled by any other disease, render it an appropriate subject for statistics. The fact that it may be acquired, although there has been no decided hereditary tendency towards it, introduces no serious difficulty, being more or less balanced by the opposite fact that it may be withstood by sanitary precautions although a strong tendency exists. Neither does it seem worth while to be hypercritical and to dwell overmuch on the different opinions held by experts as to what constitutes consumption. The ordinary symptoms are patent enough, and are generally recognized; so we may be content at first with lax definitions. At the same time, no one can he more strongly impressed than myself with the view that in proportion as we desire to improve our statistical work, so we must be increasingly careful to divide our material into truly homogeneous groups, in order that all the cases contained in the same group shall be alike in every important particular, differing only in petty details. This is far more important than adding to the number172 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. of cases. My material admits of no such delicacy of division ; nevertheless it leads to some results worth mentioning. In sorting my cases, I included under the head of Consumption all the causes of death described by one or the other following epithets, attention being also paid to the context, and to the phraseology used elsewhere by the same writer:—Consumption ; Phthisis; Tubercular disease ; Tuberculosis; Decline; Pulmonary, or lung disease; Lost lung; Abscess on lung; Haemorrhage of lungs (fatal); Lungs affected (here especially the context was considered). All of these were reckoned as actual Consumption. In addition to these there were numerous phrases of doubtful import that excited more or less reasonable suspicion. It may be that the disease had not sufficiently declared itself to justify more definite language, or else that the phrase employed was a euphemism to veil a harsh truth. Paying still more attention to the context than before, I classed these doubtful cases under three heads:—(1) Highly suspicious; (2) Suspicious ; (3) Somewhat suspicious. They were so rated that four cases of the first should be reckoned equivalent to three cases of actual consumption, four cases of the second to two cases, and four of the third to one case. The following is a list of some of the phrases so dealt with. The occasional appearance of the same phrase under different headings is due to differences in the context:— 1. Highly suspicious :—Consumptive tendency, Con-X.] DISEASE 173 sumption feared, and died of bad chill. Chest colds with pleurisy and congestion of lungs. Died of an attack on the chest. Always delicate. Delicate lungs. Haemorrhage of lungs. Loss of part of lung. Severe pulmonary attacks and chest affections. 2. Suspicious :—Chest complaints. Delicate chest. Colds, cough and bronchitis. Delicate, and died of asthma. Scrofulous tendency. 3. Someivhat suspicious :—Asthma when young. Pulmonary congestion. Not strong; anaemic. Delicate. Colds, coughs. Debility ; general weakness. [The context was especially considered in this group.] Parent to Child.—I have only four cases in which both parents were consumptive; these will be omitted in the following remarks ; but whether included or not, the results would be unaltered, for they run parallel to the rest. There are 66 marriages in which one parent was consumptive; they produced between them 413 children, of whom 70 were actually consumptive, and others who were suspiciously so in various degrees. When reckoned according to the above method of computation, these amounted to 37 cases in addition, forming a total of 107. In other words, 26 per cent, of the children weiß, consumptive. Where neither parent was consumptive, the proportion in a small batch of well marked cases that I tried, was as high as 18 or 19 per cent., but this is clearly too much, as that of the general population is only 16 per cent. Again, by taking each fraternity separately and dividing the quantity of consumption in it by the number of its members, I obtained the average174 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. consumptive taint of each fraternity. For instance, if in a fraternity of 10 members there was one actually consumptive member and four “somewhat suspiciously ” so, it would count as a fraternity of ten members, of whom two were actually consumptive, and the average taint of the fraternity would be reckoned at one-fifth part of the whole or as 20 per cent. Treating each fraternity separately in this way, and then averaging the whole of them, the mean taint of the children of one consumptive parent was made out to be 28 per cent. Distribution of Fraternities.—Next I arranged the fraternities in such way as would show whether, if we reckoned each fraternity as a unit, their respective amounts of consumptive taint were distributed “normally ” or not. The results are contained in line A of the following table :— Percentage of Cases haying various Percentages of Taint. Percentages of Taint. 0 and under 9 10 and under 19 20 and under 29 30 and under 39 40 and above Total. A. 66 cases, one parent consumptive. 27 20 9 15 29 100 B. 84 cases, one brother consumptive. 49 14 10 13 14 100X.] DISEASE. 175 They struck me as so remarkable, in the way shortly to be explained, that I proceeded to verify them by as different a set of data as my Records could afford. I took every fraternity in which at least one member was consumptive, and treated them in a way that would answer the following question. “ One member of a fraternity, whose number is unknown, is consumptive; what is the chance that a named but otherwise unknown brother of that man will be consumptive also \ ” The fraternity that was taken above as an example, would be now reckoned as one of nine members, of whom one was actually consumptive. There were 84 fraternities available for the present purpose, and the results are given in the line B of the table. The data in A and B somewhat overlap, but for the most part they differ. They concur in telling the same tale, namely, that it is totally impossible to torture the figures so as to make them yield the single-humped “Curve of Frequency” (Fig. 3 p. 38). They make a distinctly double-humped curve, whose outline is no more like the normal curve than the back of a Bactrian camel is to that of an Arabian camel. Consumptive taints reckoned in this way are certainly not “ normally ” distributed. They depend mainly on one or other of two groups of causes, one of which tends to cause complete immunity and the other to cause severe disease, and these two groups do not blend freely together. Consumption tends to be transmitted strongly or not at all, and in this respect it resembles the baleful influence ascribed to cousin176 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. marriages, which appears to be very small when statistically discussed, but of whose occasional severity most persons have observed examples. I interpret these results as showing that consumption is largely acquired, and that the hereditary influence of an acquired attack is small when there is no accompanying “ malformation.” This last phrase is intended to cover not only a narrow chest and the like, but whatever other abnormal features may supply the physical basis upon which consumptive tendencies depend, and which I presume to be as hereditary as any other malformations. Severely-tainted Fraternities.—Pursuing the matter further, I selected those fraternities in which consumption was especially frequent, and in which the causes of the deaths both of the Father and of the Mother were given. They were 14 in number, and contained between them a total of 102 children, of whom rather more than half died before the age of 40. Though records of infant deaths were asked for, I doubt if they have been fully supplied. As 102 differs little from 100, the following figures will serve as percentages : 42 died of actual consumption and 11 others of lung disease variously described. Only one case was described as death from heart disease, but weakness of the heart during life was spoken of in a few cases. The remaining causes of death were mostly undescribed, and those that were named present no peculiarity worth notice. I then took out the causes of death of theDISEASE. 177 x.| Fathers and Mothers and their ages at death, and severally classified them as in the Table below. It must be understood that there is nothing in the Table to show how the persons were paired. The Fathers are treated as a group by themselves, and the Mothers as a separate group, also by themselves. Causes of Death of the Parents of those Fraternities in which Consumption Greatly Prevailed. Father. Age at death. Mother. Age at death. Order of ages at death. F. M. Asthma . . 70 Consumption . . . . . 40 51 40 Bronchitis Consumption . . . . . 43 52 42 Inf. kidneys and bronchitis . 73 Consumption . . . . . 47 59 43 Abscess of liver through lung(alive) Consumption . . . 62 44 Heart . . 68 Consumption . . . . . 65 68 47 Heart . . 74 Consumption . . . 70 50 Apoplexy Water on chest . •. . . 60 73 58 Apoplexy . . 75 Weak chest. . . . (alive) 74 60 Apoplexy. ...... (1 br. and 2 ss. d. of cons.) 74 65 Decay . . 74 Haemorrhage of lungs . 44 75 66 Cancer . . 52 Ossification of heart . . 50 76 73 Senile gangrene .... . . 76 N ose bleeding. . . 78 74 (2 bros. d. of cancer). Cancer . . 42 89 83 Mortification of toe , . . . 59 Atrophy . . 73 Accident . . 51 Age . . 74 (3 bros. and 2 ss. d. of cons.) Yery little account is given of the fraternities to which the fathers and mothers belong, and nothing of interest beyond what is included in the above. The contrast is here most striking between the tendencies of the Father and Mother to transmit a serious consumptive taint to their children. The cases were selected without the slightest bias in favour of showing this result; in fact, such is the incapacity to see statistical facts clearly until they are pointed out, that I had no idea of the extraordinary tendency on178 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. the part of the mother to transmit consumption, as shown in this Table, until I had selected the cases and nearly finished sorting them. Out of the fourteen families, the mother was described as actually dying of consumption in six cases, of lung complaints in two others, and of having highly consumptive tendencies in another, making a total of nine cases out of the fourteen. On the other hand the Fathers show hardly any consumptive taints. One was described as of a very consumptive fraternity, though he himself died of an accident; and another who was still alive had suffered from an abscess of the liver that broke through the lungs. Beyond these there is nothing to indicate consumption on the Fathers’ side. Another way of looking at the matter is to compare the ages at death of the Mothers and of the Fathers respectively, as has been done at the side of the Table, when we see a notable difference between them, the Mid-age of the Mothers being 58, as against 73 of the Fathers. The only other group of diseases in my collection, that affords a fair number of instances in which fraternities are greatly affected, are those of the Heart. The instances are only nine in number, but I give an analysis of them, not for any value of their own, but in order to bring the peculiarities of the consumptive fraternities more strongly into relief by means of comparison. In one of these there was no actual death from heart disease, though three had weak hearts and two others had rheumatic gout and fever. These nineX.] DISEASE. 179 families contained between them sixty-nine children, being at the rate of 7'7 to a family. The number of deaths from heart disease was 24; from ruptured blood vessels, 2 ; from consumption and lung disease, 8 ; from dropsy in various forms, 3 ; from apoplexy, paralysis, and epilepsy, 5; from suicide, 2; from Causes of Death of the Parents of those Fraternities in which Heart Disease Prevailed. Causes of death. Ages at death. Order of ages at death. Father. Mother. F. M. Heart 59, 70 61, 63, 74 53 61 Apoplexy and paralysis . 74, 78 62, 70, 72 55 62 Consumption 58 59 63 Asthma 70 70 70 Gout 55 70 72 Senile Gangrene .... 81 74 74 Tumour in liver .... 77 75 77 Cancer 75 78 81 Living ........ Unknown old. 85 2 bros. and 1 sis. d. of heart disease and 1 of paralysis mt. 40. old, 85 cancer, 1. There is no obvious difference between the diseases of their Fathers and Mothers as shown in the Table, other than the smallness of the number of cases would account for. Their mid-ages at death were closely the same, 70 and 72, and the ages in the two groups run alike. I must leave it to medical men to verify the amount of truth that may be contained in what I have deduced from these results concerning the distinctly superior N 2180 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. power of the mother over that of the father to produce a highly consumptive family. Any physician in large practice among consumptive cases could test the question easily by reference to his note-books. A “ highly consumptive” fraternity may conveniently be defined as one in which at least half of its members have actually died of consumption, or else are so stricken that their ultimate deaths from that disease may be reckoned upon. Also to avoid statistical accidents, the fraternities selected for the inquiry should be large, consisting say of six children and upwards. Of course the numerical proportions given by the above 14 fraternities are very rude indications indeed of the results to which a thorough inquiry might be expected to lead. Accepting the general truth of the observation that consumptive mothers produce highly consumptive families much more commonly than consumptive fathers, it is easy to offer what seems to be an adequate explanation. Consumption is partly acquired by some form of contagion or infection, and is partly an hereditary malformation. So far as it is due to the latter in the wide sense already given to the word “ malformation,” it may perhaps be transmissible equally by either parent. But so far as it is contagious or infectious, we must recollect that the child is peculiarly exposed during all the time of its existence before birth, to contagion from its mother. During infancy, it lies perhaps for hours daily in its mother’s arms, and afterwards lives much by her side, closely caressed, and breathing the tainted air of her shelteredX.] DISEASE. 181 rooms. The explanation of the fact that we have been discussing appears therefore to be summed up in the single word—Infection. Consumptivity. — Before abandoning the topic of hereditary consumption, it may be well to discuss it from the same point of view that was taken when discussing the artistic temperament. Consumption being so common in this country that fully one person out of every six or seven die of it, and all forms of hereditary disease being intermixed through marriage, it follows that the whole population must be more or less tainted with consumption. That a condition which we may call “ consumptivity,” for want of a better word, may exist without showing any outward sign, is proved by the fact that as sanitary conditions worsen by ever so little, more persons are affected by the disease. It seems a fair view to take, that when the* amount of consumptivity reaches a certain level, the symptoms of consumption declare themselves; that when it approaches but falls a little short of that level, there are threatening symptoms ; that when it falls far below the level, there is a fallacious appearance of perfect freedom from consumptivity. We may reasonably proceed on the hypothesis that consumptivity might somehow be measured, and that if its measurement was made in each of any large group of persons, the measures would be distributed “ normally.” So far we are on fairly safe ground, but now uncertainties begin upon which my data fail to throw182 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. sufficient light. Longevity, marriage, and fertility must all be affected by the amount of consumptivity, whereas in the case of the faculties hitherto discussed they are not affected to any sensible extent. It however happens that these influences tend to neutralize one another. It is true that consumptive persons die early, and many of them before a marriageable age. On the other hand, they certainly marry earlier as a rule than others, one cause of which lies in their frequent great attractiveness; and again, when they marry, they produce children more quickly than others. Consequently those who die even long before middle age, often contrive to leave large families. The greater rapidity with which the generations follow each other, is also a consideration of some importance. There is therefore a fair doubt whether a group of young persons destined to die of consumption, contribute considerably less to the future population than an equally large group who are destined to die of other diseases. I will at all events assume that consumptivity does not affect the numbers of the adult children, simply as a working hypothesis, and will afterwards compare its results with observed facts. I should add that the question whether the sexes transmit consumption equally, lies outside the present work, at least for practical purposes; for whether they transmit it equally or not would not affect the results materially. Our list of data is therefore limited to these:—that 16 per cent, of the population die of consumption, that consumptivity is normally distri-X.] DISEASE. 183 buted, and that the law of hereditary regression from a deviation of three units on the part of either parent to an average of one unit in the child, may be supposed to apply here, just as it did to Stature and to the other subjects of the preceding chapters. Let the scale by which consumptivity is measured be such that the Q of the general population = 1. Let its M = N, when measured on the same scale ; the value of N is and will remain unknown. Let N + C be the number of units of consumptivity that just amount to actual consumption. Our data tell us that 16 per cent, of the population have an amount of consumptivity that exceeds N + C. On referring to Table 8, we find the value of C that corresponds to the Grade of (100°—16°), or of 84°, to be T'47. Therefore whenever the consumptivity of a person exceeds N + 1'47, he has actual consumption. Adding together the tabular values in Table 8 at all the odd grades above 84°, we shall find their average value to be 2'23. "We may therefore assume (see p. 160) that a group of persons each of whom has a consumptivity of N + 2-23 will approximately represent all the grades above 84°. The Co-Fraternity descended from such a group will have an M whose value according to the law of Regression ought to be [N + ^ (2 *23)] or [N + O'74 units.] Those members of the Co-Fraternity are consumptive whose consumptivity exceeds N + 1*47; these are the same as those whose deviation from [N + 0'74] which is the M of the Co-Fraternity, exceeds + 0-73 unit.184 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. I jet the Q of the Co-Fraternity he called n. The Grade at which this amount of deviation occurs should be found in Table 8 opposite to the value of 0'73 divided by n. Next as regards the value to be assigned to n, we may be assured that the Q of a Co-Fraternity cannot exceed that of the general population. Therefore n cannot exceed 1. In the case of Stature the relation between the Q of the Co-Fraternity and that of the Population was found to be as 15 to 17. If the same proportion held good here, its value would be 0-9. This is I think too high an estimate for the following reasons. The variability of the Co-Fraternity depends on two groups of causes. First, on fraternal variability ; which itself is due in part to mixed ancestry, and in part to variety of nurture in the same Fraternity, both before as well as after birth. Secondly, it depends upon the variety of ancestry and nurture in different Fraternities. As to the first of the two groups of causes, they seem to affect consumptive fraternities in the same way as others, but not so with respect to the second group. The household arrangements of vigorous, of moderately vigorous, and of invalided parents are not alike. I have already spoken of infection. There is also a tradition in families that are not vigorous, of the necessity of avoiding risks and of never entering professions that involve physical hardship. There is no such tradition in families who are vigorous. Thus there must be much greater variability in the environments of a group of persons taken from the populationX.] DISEASE. 185 at large, than there is in a group of consumptive families. It would be quite fair to estimate the value of n at least as low as 0'8. We have thus three values of n to try; viz. 1, 0-9, and 0'8, of which the first is scarcely possible and the last is much the more suitable of the other two. The corresponding values of 0’73 divided by n, are + 0.73, H- 0-81, and + 0-91. Eeferring to Table 8 we find the Grades corresponding to those deviations to be 69, 71, and 73. We should therefore expect 69, 71, or 73 per cent, of the Co-Fraternity to be non-consumptive, according to the value of n we please to adopt, and the complement to those percentages, viz. 31, 29, or 27, to be consumptive. Observation (p. 173), gave the value of 26 by one method of calculation, and of 28 by another. Too much stress must not be laid on this coincidence, because many important points had to be slurred over, as already explained. Still, the prima facie result is successful, and enables us to say that so far as this evidence goes, the statistical method we have employed in treating consumptivity seems correct, and that the law of heredity found to govern all the different faculties as yet examined, appears to govern that of consumptivity also, although the constants of the formula differ slightly. Data for Hereditary Diseases.—The knowledge of the officers of Insurance Companies as to the average value of unsound lives is by the confession of many of186 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. X. them far from being as exact as is desirable. [See, for example, the discussion on a memoir by G. Humphreys, Actuary to the Eagle Insurance Company, read before the Institute of Actuaries.—Insur. Mag. xviii. p. 178.] Considering the enormous money value concerned, it would seem well worth the while of the higher class of those offices to combine in order to obtain a collection of completed cases for at least two generations, or better still, for three ; such as those in Examples A and B, Appendix G, but much fuller ¡in detail. Being completed and anonymous, there could be little objection on the score of invaded privacy. They would have no perceptible effect on the future insurances of descendants of the families, even if these were identified, and they would lay the basis of a very much better knowledge of hereditary disease than we now possess, serving as a step for fresh departures. A main point is that the cases should not be picked and chosen to support any theory, but taken as they come to hand. There must be a vast amount of good material in existence at the command of the medical officers of Insurance Companies. If it were combined and made freely accessible, it would give material for many years’ work to competent statisticians, and would be certain, judging from all experience of a like kind, to lead to unexpected results.CHAPTER XI. LATENT ELEMENTS. Latent Elements not very numerous.—Pure Breed.—Simplification of Hereditary Inquiry. Latent Elements not very numerous.—It is not possible that more than one half of the varieties and number of each of the parental elements, latent or personal, can on the average subsist in the offspring. For if every variety contributed its representative, each child would on the average contain actually or potentially twice the variety and twice the number of the elements (whatever they may be) that were possessed at the same stage of its life by either of its parents, four times that of any one of its grandparents, 1024 times as many as any one of its ancestors in the 10th degree, and so on, which is absurd. Therefore as regards any variety of the entire inheritance, whether it be dormant or personal, the chance of its dropping out must on the whole be equal to that of its being retained, and only one half of the varieties can on the average be passed on by inherit-188 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. ance. Now we have seen tliat the personal heritage from either Parent is one quarter, therefore as the total heritage is one half, it follows that the Latent Elements must follow the same law of inheritance as the Personal ones. In other words, either Parent must contribute on the average only one quarter of the Latent Elements, the remainder of them dropping out and their breed becoming absolutely extinguished. There seems to be much confusion in current ideas about the extent to which ancestral qualities are transmitted, supposing that what occurs occasionally must occur invariably. If a maternal grandparent be found to contribute some particular quality in one case, and a paternal grandparent in another, it seems to be argued that both contribute elements in every case. This is not a fair inference, as will be seen by the following illustration. A pack of playing cards consists, as we know, of 13 cards of each sort—hearts, diamonds, spades, and clubs. Let these be shuffled together and a batch of 13 cards dealt out from them, forming the deal, No. 1. There* is not a single card in the entire pack that may not appear in these 13, but assuredly they do not all appear. Again, let the 13 cards derived from the above pack, which we will suppose to have green backs, be shuffled with another 13 similarly obtained from a pack with blue backs, and that a deal, No. 2, of 13 cards be made from the combined batches. The result will be of the same kind as before. Any card of either of the two original packs may be found in the deal, No. 2, but certainlyXI.] LATENT ELEMENTS. 189 not all of them. So I conceive it to be with hereditary-transmission. No given pair can possibly transmit the whole of their ancestral qualities; on the other hand, there is probably no description of ancestor whose qualities have not been in some cases transmitted to a descendant. The fact that certain ancestral forms persist in breaking out, such as the zebra-looking stripes on the donkey, is no argument against this view. The reversion may fairly be ascribed to precisely the same cause that makes it almost impossible to wholly destroy the breed of certain weeds in a garden, inasmuch as they are prolific and very hardy, and wage successful battle with their vegetable competitors whenever they are not heavily outmatched in numbers. If the Personal and Latent Elements are transmitted on the average in equal numbers, it is difficult to suppose that there can be much difference in their variety. Pure Breed.—In a perfectly pure breed, maintained during an indefinitely long period by careful selection, the tendency to regress towards the M of the general population, would disappear, so far as that tendency may be due to the inheritance of mediocre ancestral qualities, and not to causes connected with the relative stability of different types. The Q of Fraternal Deviations from their respective true Mid-Fraternities which we called b, would also diminish, because it is partly dependent on the children in the same family taking190 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap variously after different and unlike progenitors. But the difference between 5 in a mixed breed such as we have been considering, and the value which we may call /3, which it would have in a pure breed, would be very small. Suppose the Prob: Error of the implied Stature of each separate Grand-Parent to be even as great as the Q of the general Population, which is 1 7 inch (it would be less, but we need not stop to discuss its precise value), then the Prob : Error of the implied Mid-Grand-Parental stature would be v/jx 17 inch, or say 0‘8 inch. The share of this, which would on the average be transmitted to the child, would be only ^ as much, or 0'2. From all the higher Ancestry, put together, the contribution would be much less even than this small value, and we may disregard it. It results that b2 is a trifle greater than /32 + 0'04. But 6 = 1-0 ; therefore /3 is only a trifle less than 0‘98. Simplification of Hereditary Inquiry. — These considerations make it probable that inquiries into human heredity may be much simplified. They assure us that the possibilities of inheritance are not likely to differ much more than the varieties actually observed among the members of a large Fraternity. If then we have full life-histories of the Parents and of numerous Uncles and Aunts on both sides, we ought to have a very fair basis for hereditary inquiry. Information of this limited kind is incomparably more easy to obtain than that which I have hitherto striven for, namely, family histories during four successive generations.XI.] LATENT ELEMENTS. 191 When the “ children ” in the pedigree are from 40 to 55 years of age, their own life-histories are sufficiently advanced to be useful, though they are incomplete, and it is still easy for them to compile good histories of their Parents, Uncles, and Aunts. Friends who knew them all would still be alive, and numerous documents such as near relations or personal friends preserve, but which are mostly destroyed at their decease, would still exist. If I were undertaking a fresh inquiry in order to verify and to extend my previous work, it would be on this basis. I should not care to deal with any family that did not number at least six adult children, and the same number of uncles and aunts on both the paternal and maternal sides. Whatever could be learnt about the grandparents and their brothers and sisters, would of course be acceptable, as throwing further light. I should however expect that the peculiarities distributed among any large Fraternity of Uncles and Aunts would fairly indicate the variety of the Latent Elements in the Parent. The complete heritage of the child, on the average of many cases, might then be assigned as follows: One quarter to the personal characteristics of the Father; one quarter to the average of the personal characteristics of the Fraternity taken as a whole, of whom the Father was one of the members; and similarly as regards the Mother’s side.CHAPTER XII. SUMMARY The investigation now concluded is based on the fact that the characteristics of any population that is in harmony with its environment, may remain statistically identical during successive generations. This is true for every characteristic whether it be affected to a great degree by a natural selection, or only so slightly as to be practically independent of it. It was easy to see in a vague way, that an equation admits of being based on this fact; that the equation might serve to suggest a theory of descent, and that no theory of descent that failed to satisfy it could possibly be true. A large part of the book is occupied with preparations for putting this equation into a working form. Obstacles in the way of doing so, which I need not recapitulate, appeared on every side ; they had to be confronted in turns, and then to be either evaded or overcome. The final result was that the higher methods of statistics, which consist in applications of the law of Frequency of Error, were found eminently suitable for expressingCHAP. XII.] SUMMARY. 193 the processes of heredity. By their aid, the desired equation was thrown into an exceedingly simple form of approximative accuracy, and it 'became easy to compare both it and its consequences with the varied results of observation, and thence to deduce numerical results. A brief account of the chief hereditary processes occupies the first part of the book. It was inserted principally in order to show that a reasonable a priori probability existed, of the law of Frequency of Error being found to apply to them. It was not necessary for that purpose to embarrass ourselves with any details of theories of heredity beyond the fact, that descent either was particulate or acted as if it were so. I need hardly say that the idea, though not the phrase of particulate inheritance, is borrowed from Darwin’s provisional theory of Pangenesis, but there is no need in the present inquiry to borrow more from it. Neither is it requisite to take Weissmann’s views into account, unless I am mistaken as to their scope. It is freely conceded that particulate ■ inheritance is not the only factor to be reckoned with in a complete theory of heredity, but that the stability of the organism has also to be regarded. This may perhaps become a factor of great importance in forecasting the issue of highly bred animals, but it was not found to exercise any sensible influence on those calculations with which this book is chiefly concerned. Its existence has therefore been only nbted, and not otherwise taken into account. The data on which the results mainly depend had to be o194 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. collected specially, as no suitable material for the purpose was, so far as I know, in existence. This was done by means of an offer of prizes some years since, that placed in my hands a collection of about 160 useful Family Records. These furnished an adequate though only just an adequate supply of the required data. In order to show the degree of dependence that might be placed on them they were subjected to various analyses, and the result proved to be even more satisfactory than might have been fairly hoped for. Moreover the errors in the Records probably affect different generations in the same way, and would thus be eliminated from the comparative results. As soon as the character of the problem of Filial descent had become well understood, it was seen that a general equation of the same form as that by which it was expressed, also expressed the connection between Kinsmen in every degree. The unexpected law of universal Regression became a theoretical necessity, and on appealing to fact its existence was found to be conspicuous. If the word “peculiarity” be used to signify the difference between the amount of any faculty possessed by a man, and the average of that possessed by the population at large, then the law of Regression may be described as follows. Each peculiarity in a man is shared by his kinsmen, but on the average in a less degree. It is reduced to a definite fraction of its amount, quite independently of what its amount might be. The fraction differs in different orders of kinship, becoming smaller as they are more remote. When theXII.] SUMMARY. 195 kinship is so distant that its effects are not worth taking into account, the peculiarity of the man, however remarkable it may have been, is reduced to zero in his kinsmen. This apparent paradox is fundamentally due to the greater frequency of mediocre deviations than of extreme ones, occurring between limits separated by equal widths. Two causes affect family resemblance; the one is Heredity, the other is Circumstance. That which is transmitted is only a sample taken partly through the operation of “accidents,” out of a store of otherwise unused material, and circumstance must always play a large part in the selection of the sample. Circumstance comprises all the additional accidents, and all the peculiarities of nurture both before and after birth, and every influence that may conduce to make the characteristics of one brother differ from those of another. The circumstances of nurture are more varied in Co-Fraternities than in Fraternities, and the Grandparents and previous ancestry of members of Co-Fraternities differ; consequently Co-Fraternals differ among themselves more widely than Fraternals. The average contributions of each separate ancestor to the heritage of the child were determined apparently within narrow limits, for a couple of generations at least. The results proved to be very simple; they assign an average of one quarter from each parent, and one sixteenth from each grandparent. According to this geometrical scale continued indefinitely backwards, the total heritage of the child would be .0 2196 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap accounted for, but the factor of stability of type has to be reckoned with, and this has not yet been adequately discussed. The ratio of filial Regression is found to be so bound up with co-fraternal variability, that when either is given the other can be calculated. There are no means of deducing the measure of fraternal variability solely from that of co-fraternal. They differ by an element of which the value is thus far unknown. Consequently the measure of fraternal variability has to be calculated separately, and this cannot be done directly, owing to the small size of human families. Four different and indirect methods of attacking the problem suggested themselves, but the calculations were of too delicate a kind to justify reliance on the R.F.F. data. Separate and more accurate measures, suitable for the purpose, had therefore to be collected. The four problems were then solved by their means, and although different groups of these measures had to be used with the different problems, the results were found to agree together. The problem of expressing the relative nearness of different degrees of kinship, down to the point where kinship is so distant as not to be worth taking into account, was easily solved. It is merely a question of the amount of the Regression that is appropriate to the different degrees of kinship. This admits of being directly observed when a sufficiency of data are accessible, or else of being calculated from the values found in this inquiry. A table of these Regressions was given.XII.] SUMMARY. 197 Finally, considerations were offered to show that latent elements probably follow the same law as personal ones, and that though a child may inherit qualities from any one of his ancestors (in one case from this one, and in another case from another), it does not follow that the store of hidden property so to speak, that exists in any parent, is made up of contributions from all or even very many of his ancestry. Two other topics may be mentioned. Reason was given in p. 16 why experimenters upon the transmission of Acquired Faculty should not be discouraged on meeting with no affirmative evidence of its existence in the first generation, because it is among the grandchildren rather than among the children that it should be looked for. Again, it is hardly to be expected that an acquired faculty, if transmissible at all, would be transmitted without dilution. It could at the best be no more than a variation liable to Regression, which would probably so much diminish its original amount on passing to the grandchildren as to render it barely recognizable. The difficulty of devising experiments on the transmission of acquired faculties is much increased by these considerations. The other subject to be alluded to is the fundamental distinction that may exist between two couples whose personal faculties are naturally alike. If one of the couples consist of two gifted members of a poor stock, and the other of two ordinary members of a gifted stock, the difference between198 NATURAL INHERITANCE. [chap. XII. them will betray itself in their offspringcv The children of the former will tend to regress; those of the latter will not. The value of a good stock to the wellbeing of future generations is therefore obvious, and it is well to recall attention to an early sign by which we may be assured that a new and gifted variety possesses the necessary stability to easily originate a new stock. It is its refusal to blend freely with other forms. Some among the members of the same fraternity might possess the characteristics in question with much completeness, and the remainder hardly or not at all. If this alternative tendency was also witnessed among cousins, there could be little doubt that the new variety was of a stable character, and therefore capable of being easily developed by interbreeding into a pure and durable race.TABLES. Table 1. Stkength of Pull. 519 Males aged 23 -26. From measures made at the International Health Exhibition in 1884. Percentages. Strength of Pull. No. of cases observed. No. of cases observed. Sums from beginning. Under 50 lbs. 10 2 2 „ 60 „ 42 8 10 „ 70 „ 140 27 37 „ 80 „ 168 33 70 ,, 90 ,, 113 21 91 „ 100 „ 22 4 95 Above 100 „ 24 5 100 Total ..... 519 100 Table 2. Data for Schemes of Distribution of various qualities and faculties among the persons measured at the Anthropometric ______________________•_____ Laboratory in the International Exhibition of 1884. Subject of measurement. Age. Unit of Sex. No. of Values at the undermentioned Grades, from 0° to 160°. measure- ment. in the group. 5° 10° 20° 30° 40° 50° 60° 70° 80° 90° 95° Height, standing, ) 23-51 Inches | M. 811 63*2 6.4*5 65*8 665 67*3 67*9 68*5 69*2 70*0 71*3 72*4 without shoes . j F. 770 58*9 59*9 61*3 62*1 62*7 63*3 63*9 64*6 65*3 66*4 67'3 Height, sitting, ) 23-51 Inches | M. 1013 33*6 34*2 34*9 35*3 35*4 36*0 36*3 36*7 37 1 37*7 38*2 from seat of chair \ F. 775 31-8 32*3 32*9 33 3 33*6 33*9 34*2 34*6 34*9 35*6 36*0 Span of arms . . . 23-51 Inches j M. F. 811 770 65-0 58-6 66*1 59*5 67*2 60*7 68*2 61*7 69*0 62*4 69*9 63*0 70*6 63*7 71*4 64*5 72*3 65*4 73*6 66*7 74*8 68*0 Weight in ordinary ) 23-26 Pounds{ M. 520 121 125 131 135 139 143 147 150 156 165 172 indoor clothes . j F. 276 102 105 110 114 118 122 129 132 136 142 149 Breathing capacity. 23-26 Cubic f M. 212 161 177 187 199 211 219 226 236 248 277 290 Inches \ F. 277 92 102 115 124 131 138 144 151 164 177 186 Strength of pull as \ 23-26 Pounds| M. 519 56 60 64 68 71 74 77 80 82 89 96 archer with bow. } F. 276 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 47 51 54 Strength of squeeze ) with strongest / hand ) 23-26 Pounds[ M. F. 519 276 67- 36 71 39 76 43 79 47 82 49 85 52 88 55 91 58 95 62 100 67 104 72 Swiftness of blow . 23-26 Ft. per J M. 516 132 14*1 15*2 16*2 17*3 18*1 19*1 20*0 20*9 22*3 23*6 second ( F. 271 9 2 10*1 11*3 12*1 12*8 13*4 14*0 14*5 15*1 16*3 16*9 Sight, keenness % of — by distance f 23-26 Inches j M. 398 13 17 20 22 23 25 26 28 30 32 34 of reading dia- l mond test-type . ^ F. 433 10 12 16 19 22 24 26 27 29 31 32 200 NATURAL INHERITANCE.Table 3. Deviations from M in each of the series in Table 2, after reduction to a Scale in which Q' = 1, where Q' is the Mean of the ______________________________observed Deviations at the Grades 20°, 30°, 70°, and 80°. __________________ Subject of measurement. Values of Q' Unit of measurement in Table 2. Sex. No. of persons Deviations reckoned in units of 5° 10° 20° 30° 40° 50° 60° 70° 80° 90° 95° Height, standing, \ without shoes . . j Height, sitting, \ from seat of chair j Span of arms . . . Weight in ordinary \ indoor clothes . . J Breathing capacity . Strength of pull as \ archer with bow . J Strength of squeeze ) with strongest > hand J Swiftness of blow - Sight, keenness >. of — by distance ) of reading dia- } mond test-type J 1*72 1-62 0*95- 0-82 2*07 1-87 10-00 11*00 24-50 19*00 7-50 5'22 7'75 7*50 2*37 1*55 4- 00 5- 22 Inches -j Inches Inches Pounds{ Cubic f Inches \ Pounds J Pounds| Ft. per f second \ Inches M. F. M. F. M. F. M. F. M. F. M. F. M. F. M. F. M. F. 811 770 1013 775 811 770 520 276 212 277 519 276 519 276 516 271 398 433 2*73 2*71 2-52 2*55 2*36 2*35 2*20 1-80 2*32 2-39 2-39 1- 92 2- 32 2 12 2-06 2 71 3 00 2-66 1'98 210 1*89 1*95 1*83 1*87 1-80 1-60 1*68 1-87 1-86 1-06 1-81 1*73 1*68 2*13 2*00 2*28 1-22 1-23 115 1*22 1-30 1-23 1*20 110 1-28 1-20 1-33 *80 116 1*20 1-22 1*35 1'25 1*52 0*81 *74 •73 •73 •82 *69 •80 '70 •80 '73 •80 '53 •77 •66 *80 •84 •75 '95 0*35 *37 *63 •36 .*43 •32 •40 •40 •32 •36 •40 •27 *39 •40 •34 •38 •50 *38 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 35 '37 *31 •36 *33 •37 •40 •60 •28 •31 •40 '27 «•39 •40 •42 •38 *25 •38 \ *76 '73" •85 *72 •80 •70 •90 .68 '67 •80 53 *77 '80 •80 •71 •75 '57 1-22 1*23 1-15 1‘2> 116. 1-28 1-30 1-30 116 1-35 1-06 •93 1-29 1-33 118 110 1-25 '95 1*98 1- 91 1*79 2- 07 1- 79 1*98 2- 20 1-80 2'32 2*03 1*99 1-46 1-93 1-99 1-77 1-87 1-75 1*33 2-61 2*46 2-31 2-55 2 36 2-67 2-90 2*40 2'84 2-49 292 1*86 2*45 2'66 2*31 2'26 2*25 1-52 Sums. . 43-11 33*12 21-96 13-65 7-00 0 6-57 13*34 21*46 33*96 43*82 Means . . 2-40 1*84 1-22 076 0-39 0 0-37 074 119 1*89 2-43- Means multiplied by 1 '015, to change unit to Q = 1 2-44 1*87 1*24 0-77 0*40 0 0-38 075 1*21 1*92 2-47 Normal Values, when Q = 1 2-44 1-90 1*25 0-78 0'38 0 0*38 078 1*25 1*90 2'44 TABLES. 201202 NATURAL INHERITANCE. Tables 4 to 8 inclusive give data for drawing Normal Curves of Frequency and Distribution. They also show the way in which the latter is derived from the values of the Probability Integral. The equation for the Probablity Curve1 is y = Jc e in which /¿is “the Measure of Precision.” By taking 1c and h each as unity, the values in Table 4 are computed. Table 4. Data for a Normal Curve of Frequency. y=e X y X y X V X y 0 1-00 ± 1-0 0*37 ± 2-0 0-0183 ± 3-0 o-oooi ± 0-2 0-96 ± 1*2 0-23 ± 2-2 0-0079 ±04 0-85 ± 1-4 0T4 ± 2‘4 0-0032 ± infi- 0*0000 ± 0-6 0*70 ± 1-6 0*78 ± 26 0-0012 nity ± 0-8 0*53 ± 1*8 0-40 ± 2-8 0-0004 Table 5. Values of the 2 r Probability Integral,— -V e dt, for Argument t. t ( = hx) •o •1 •2 •3 •4 •5 •6 •7 •8 •9 0 0*00 0-11 0-22 0-33 0-43 0-52 0-60 0-68 0-74 0-80 1-0 0*843 0-880 0 910 0-934 0-952 0-966 0-976 0-984 0-989 0-923 2-0 •9953 •9970 •9981 •9989 •9993 •9996 •9998 9999 •9999 •9999 infinite 1-0000 When t = '4769 the corresponding tabular entry would be *50; therefore, *4769 is the value of the “Probable Error.” 1 See Merriman On the Method of Least Squares (Macmillan, 1885), pp. 26, 186, where fuller Tables than 4, 5, and 6 will be foundTABLES. 202 Table 6. Values of the Probability Integral for Argument ^.; that is, when the unit of measurement = the Probable error. Multiples of the Probable Error. *0 *1 *2 *3 •4 •5 •6 *7 *8 •9 0 0*00 0*65 0*11 0T6 0*21 026 0*31 0*36 0-41 0-46 1*0 •50 *54 *58 •62 *66 •69 •72 •75 •78 •80 2*0 •82 *84 •86 .88 •89 *91 •92 •93 •94 *95 3*0 •957 .964 •969 •974 •978 •982 *985 •987 •990 *992 4-0 •9930 .9943 ‘9954 •9963 *9970 •9976 •9981 •9985 •9988 •9990 5*0 *9993 •9994 *9996 •9997 *9997 •9998 •9998 •9999 *9999 *9999 infinite 1*000 Tables 5 and 6 show the proportion of cases in any Normal system, in which the amount of Error lies within various extreme values, the total number of cases being reckoned as 10. Here no regard is paid to the sign of the Error, whether it be plus or minus, but its amount is alone considered. The unit of the scale by which the Errors are measured, differs in the two Tables. In Table 5 it 13 the “ Modulus,” and the result is that the Errors in one half of the cases, that is in 0*50 of them lie within the extreme value (found by interpolation) of 0*4769, while the other half exceed that value. In Table 6 the unit of the scale is 0*4769. It is derived from Table 5 by dividing all the tabular entries by that amount. Consequently one half of the cases have Errors that do not exceed 1 *0 in terms of the new unit, and that unit is the Probable Error of the System. It will be seen in Table 6 that the entry of *50 stands opposite to the argument of 1*0. If it be desired to transform Tables 5 and 6 into others that shall show the proportion of cases in which the plus Errors and the minus Errors respectively lie within various extreme limits, their entries would have to be halved. Let us suppose this to have been done to Table 6, and that a new Table, which it is not necessary to print, has been thereby produced and which we will call 6a. Next multiply all the entries in the new Table by 100 in order to make them refer to a total number of 100 cases, and call this second Table 66. Lastly make a converse Table to 66; one in which the arguments of 66 become the entries, and the entries of 66 become the arguments. From this the Table 7NATURAL INHERITANCE. is made. For example, in Table 6, opposite to the argument 1*00, the entry of *50 is found; that entry becomes *25 in 6a, and 25 in 6b. In Table 7 the argument is 25, and the corresponding entry is 1*00. The meaning of this is, that in 25 per cent, of the cases the greatest of the Errors just attains to ± 1*0. Similarly Table 7 shows that the greatest of the Errors in 30 per cent, of the cases, just attains to ± 1 '25 ; in 40 per cent, to 1*90, and so on. These various percentages correspond to the centesimal Grades in a Curve of Distribution, when the Grade 0° is placed at the middle of the axis, which is the point where it is cut by the Curve, and where the other Grades are reckoned outwards on either hand, up to + 50° on the one side, and to — 50° on the other. To recapitulate:—In order to obtain Table 7 from the primary Table 5, we have to halve each of the entries in the body of Table 5, then to multiply each of the arguments by 100, and divide it by •4769. Then we expand the Table by interpolations, so as to include among its entries every whole number from 1 to 99 inclusive. Selecting these and disregarding the rest, we turn them into the arguments of Table 7, and we turn their corresponding arguments into the entries in Table 7. Table 7. ORDINATES TO NORMAL CURVE OF DISTRIBUTION on a scale whose unit = the Probable Error ; and in which the 100 Grades run from 0° to + 50° on the one side, and to - 50° on the other. Grades. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 0*00 0*04 0*07 0-11 015 0.19 0*22 0*26 0-30 0*34 10 0*38 0*41 0*45 0*49 0 53 0-57 0-61 0*65 0-69 074 20 078 0*82 0-86 0-97 0*95 1-00 1*05 110 1*15 1*20 30 3 *25 1*30 1*36 1*42 1*47 1-54 1*60 1*67 174 1-82 40 1-90 1*99 2*08 2-19 2*31 2*44 2-60 279 3-05 3-45 But in the Schemes, the 100 Grades do not run from—50° through 0° to + 50°, but from 0° to 100°. It is therefore convenient to modify Table 7 in a manner that will admit of its being used directly for drawing Schemes without troublesome additions or subtractions. This is done in Table 8, where the values from 50° onwards, and those from 50° backwards are identical with those in Table 7 from 0° to ± 50°, but the first half of those in Table 8 are positive and the latter half are negative.Table 8. Ordinates to Normal Curve of Distribution on a scale whose unit = the Probable Error, and in which the 100 Grades run from 0° to 100°. Grades 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 — 00 - 3*45 - 3*05 -2-79 — 2*60 -2*44 -2*31 -2*19 -2*08 -1*99 10 -1*90 -1*82 -1*74 -1*67 -1-60 -1*54 -1*47 -1*42 -1-36 -1*30 20 -1*25 -1*20 -1*15 -1*10 - 1*05 -1*00 -0*95 -0*91 -0*86 -0*82 30 -0*78 -0*74 -0.69 -0-65 -0*61 -0*57 -0*53 -0*49 -0*45 -0*41 40 -0*38 -0*34 -0*30 -0*26 -0*22 -0*19 -0*15 -0*11 -0*07 -0*04 50 0*00 + 0*04 + 0*07 + 0*11 + 0*15 + 0*19 + 0*22 + 0*26 -0*30 + 0*34 60 -f- 0*38 + 0*41 + 0*45 + 0*49 + 0*53 + 0*57 + 0*61 + 0*65 + 0*69 + 0*74 70 + 0*78 + 0*82 + 0*86 + 0*91 + 0*95 + 1*00 + 1*05 + 1*10 + 1*15 + 1*20 80 + 1-25 4-1*30 + 1*36 + 1*42 + 1*47 + 1*54 + 1*60 + 1*67 + 1*74 + 1*82 90 + 1*90 + 1*99 + 2*08 + 2*19 + 2-31 + 2*44 + 2*60 + 2*79 + 3*05 + 3*45 Examples of the way in which Table 8 is to be read :— The ordinate at 0° is -infinity; at 10° it is -1*90; at 11° it is -1*82; at 25° it is -1*00; at 75° it is +1*00. The Table does not go beyond Gr&de 99°, where the ordinate is + 3*45. At the Grade 100°, the ordinate would be + infinity. TABLES. 20520G NATURAL INHERITANCE, Table 9. Marriage Selection in restect to Stature. The 205 male parents and the 205 female parents are each divided into three groups—T, M, and S, and t, m, and s, respectively—that is, Tall, Medium, and Short (medium male measurements being taken as 67 inches, and upwards to 70 inches). The number of marriages in each possible combination between them were then counted, with the result that men and women of contrasted heights, Short and Tall, or Tall and Short, married about as frequently as men and women of similar heights, both Tall or both Short; there were 32 cases of the one to 27 of the other. S„ t. M., t. T., t. 12 cases. 20 cases. 18 cases. S., m. M., m. T., in. 25 cases. 51 cases. 28 cases. S., s. M., s. T., s. 9 cases. 28 cases. 14 cases. Short and tall, 12 + 14 = 32 cases. Short and short, 9 1 Tall and tall, 18 } = 27 eases‘ We may therefore regard the married folk as couples picked out of the general population at haphazard when applying the law of probabilities to heredity of stature. Table 9a. Marriage Selection in respect to Eye-Colour in 78 Parental Couples. Eye Colour of No. of Per Cents. Eye Colour cases observed. of Husband and Wife. Observed. Husband Wife. Obs. Chance. Chance. Light Light 29 37 37 Alike Hazel Hazel 2 3 2 y 48 46 Dark Dark 6 8 7 J Light Hazel Hazel Light } 18 23 15 [ 28 22 f Half-con- \ trasted Hazel Dark 5 7 \ Dark Hazel i 4 Light Dark Dark Light 1 19 24 32 24 32 Contrasted The chance combinations in pairs are calculated for a population containing 61*2 per cent, of light Eye-colour, 12'7 of Hazel, and 26T of Dark.TABLES. 207 Table 9 b. Marriages of the Artistic and the Not Artistic. Bank in Tedigrees. No. of persons. Percentages. Males. Females. Pairs of artistic and not artistic persons. Marriages observed. Chance combinations. art. not. art. not. both art. 1 art. 1 not. both not. both art. 1 art. lnot. both not. Parents Paternal grandparents.. Maternal grandparents.. 326 280 288 32 27 24 68 73 76 39 30 28 61 70 72 14 12 9 31 31 41 50 57 50 12 8 7 46 41 39 42 51 54 Totals and means... 894 28 72 33 67 12 36 52 9 42 49 Tastes of Husband and Wife—alike ,, ,, ,, contrasted 12 + 52 =64 36 9 + 49 = 58 42 Table 10. Effect upon Adult Children of Differences in Height of their Parents. Difference in inches between the Heights of the Parents. Proportion per 50 of cases in which the Heights1 of the Children deviated to various amounts from the Mid-filial Stature of their respective families. Number of Children whose Heights were observed. (Total 525.) Less than 1 inch. Less than 2 inches. Less than 3 inches. Less than 4 inches. Less than 5 inches. Under 1 inch 21 35 43 46 48 105 1 and under 2 23 37 46 49 50 122 2 „ 3 16 34 41 45 49 112 3 „ 5 24 35 41 47 49 108 j 5 and above 18 -30 40 47 49 78* 1 Every female height has been transmuted to its male equivalent by multiplying it by 1*08, and only those families have been included in which the number of adult children amounted to six, at least. Note.—When these figures are protracted into curves, it will be seen—(1) that they run much alike ; (2) that their peculiarities are not in sequence ; and (8) that the curve corresponding to the first line occupies a medium position. It is therefore certain that differences in the heights of the Parents have on the whole an inconsiderable effect on the heights of their Offspring.Table 11 (R.F.F. Data). Number of Adult Children of various Statures born of 205 Mid-Parents of various Statures. (All Female Heights have been multiplied by 1 #08.) Height of the midparents in inches. Heights of the adult children. Total number of Medians or Values of M. Below 62.2 63*2 64*2 65-2 66-2 67-2 68*2 69*2 70*2 71-2 72*2 73-2 Above. Adult children. Mid- parents. Above 72*5... 1 3 41 51 72*5... 1 2 i "2 7 2 *4 19 6 72-2 71*5... Ï ”3 4 3 5 10 4 9 2 2 43 11 69-9 70*5... Ï "i 1 1 3 12 18 14 7 4 3 3 68 22 69 5 69*5... 1 16 4 17 27 20 33 25 20 11 4 5 183 41 68*9 68*5... Ï 7 11 16 25 31 34 48 21 18 4 3 219 49 68-2 675... .... 3 5 14 15 36 38 28 38 19 11 4 211 33 67*6 66*5... 3 3 5 2 17 17 14 13 4 78 20 67*2 65*5... Ï 9 5 7 11 11 7 7 5 *2 1 66 12 66*7 64*5... 1 1 4 4 1 5 5 2 23 5 65*8 Below .. 1 2 4 1 2 2 Ï 1 14 1 Totals 5 7 32 59 48 117 138 120 167 99 64 41 17 14 928 205 Medians 66*3 67-8 67-9 67*7 67*9 68*3 68*5 69*0 69*0 70*0 Note.—In calculating the medians, the entries have been taken as referring to the middle of the squares in which they stand. The reason why the headings run 62'2, 63’2, &c., instead of 62’5, 63'5, &c., is that the observations are unequally distributed between 62 and 63, 63 and 64, &c., there being a strong bias in favour of integral inches. After careful consideration, I concluded that the headings, as adopted, best satisfied the conditions. This inequality was not apparent in the case of the mid-parents. 1 I have reprinted this Table without alteration from that published in the Proc. Boy. Soc., notwithstanding a small blunder since discovered in sorting the entries between the first and second lines. It is obvious that 4 children cannot have 5 Mid-Parents. The first line is not considered at all, on account of the paucity of the numbers it contains. The bottom line, which looks suspicious, is correct. NATURAL INHERITANCE.Table 12 (R.F.F. Data). Relative number of Brothers of various Heights to Men of various Heights, Families of Six Brothers and UPWARDS BEING EXCLUDED. Heights of the men in inches. Heights of their brothers in inches. Total Cases. Medians. Below 61-7 62*2 63*2 64-2 65*2 66-2 67-2 68*2 69*2 70-2 71'2 72-2 73-2 Above 73-2 Above 73*7... 1 1 1 4 3 3 3 2 18 73-2... 1 1 1 2 1 3 4 3 16 72-2... 1 1 2 1 1 8 6 8 11 5 4 3 51 70-3 71*2... 4 4 4 9 11 15 12 8 11 3 3 84 69-3 70*2... 1 2 4 3 7 6 12 25 18 11 8 1 3 101 69-3 69*2... 4 6 13 12 18 29 29 24 15 6 2 1 159 68-6 68*2... 1 3 6 7 15 16 29 12 11 8 1 109 68-9 67-2... 1 4 3 8 14 21 15 19 6 9 1 1 102 67'7 66*2... 1 7 10 12 14 7 12 7 4 1 75 67-2 65*2... 1 1 4 13 9 8 6 13 3 4 1 1 64 67'2 64-2... 1 6 4 7 3 3 6 4 4 2 40 67’3 68*2... 1 1 4 4 2 ... 1 13 62*2... ... 1 ... 1 Below 61*7... ... 1 1 1 ... 1 1 5 5 2 18 39 65 74 101 109 161 102 83 51 16 17 838 TABLES. 209Table 13 (Special Data), Relative number of Brothers of various Heights to Men of various Heights, Families of Five Brothers and UPWARDS BEING EXCLUDED. Heights of the men in inches. Heights of their brothers in inches. Total cases. Medians. Below 63 63*5 64*5 65-5 66-5 67-5 68*5 69*5 70-5 71*5 72-5 73 5 Above 74 74 and above 1 1 1 1 5 3 12 24 73-5 1 3 4 8 3 3 2 3 27 72-5 1 1 6 5 9 9 8 3 5 47 71 1 71*5 I 1 2 8 11 18 14 20 9 4 88 70*2 70-5 1 1 7 19 30 45 36 14 9 8 1 171 69*6 69*5 1 2 1 11 20 36 55 44 17 5 4 2 198 69-5 68-5 1 & 9 18 38 46 36 30 11 6 3 203 687 67*5 2 4 8 26 35 38 38 20 18 8 1 1 199 67 7 66-5 4 3 10 33 28 35 20 12 7 2 1 155 67'0 65*5 3 3 15 18 33 36 8 2 1 1 110 66-5 64*5 3 8 12 15 10 8 5 2 1 64 65-6 63-5 5 2 8 3 3 4 1 1 1 1 20 Below 63.. 5 5 3 3 4 2 1 23 Totals^ 23 29 64 110 152 200 204 201 169 86 47 28 25 1329 210 NATURAL INHERITANCE.TABLES. 211 Table 14 (Special Data). Deviations of individual Brothers from their Mid-Fraternal Statures. Number of brothers in each family...... 4 5 6 7 Number of Families 39 23 8 6 Amount of Deviation. Number of cases. Number of cases. Number of cases. Number of cases. Under 1 inch 88 62 20 21 1 and under 2 49 30 18 14 2 and under 3 15 17 5 6 3 and under 4 4 3 3 1 4 and above 3 2 Table 15. Frequency of Different Eye-colours in Four Successive Generations. V Sex and the No. of the (ascending) generation. No of cases of eye-colour observed. Percentages. 1. Light blue. 2. Blue. Dark blue. ¡3 o o> & Í' 8. £ viz., on OP. 3. Sections of the “ surface of frequency ” parallel to the plane of xz, are, from the nature of the question, evidently curves of frequency with a probable error 1*50, and the locus of their vertices lies in the plane £ OP. Sections of the same surface parallel to the plane of yz are got from the exponential factor (1) by making x constant. The result is simplified by taking the origin on the line OM. Thus putting x =: xx and y = yx + y, where by (3) V\ _ ~ _ o (1-22)2 9(1-50)2 the exponential takes the form f 1 + 4 \ „ f y* +(3x1-2^ \ (1-22)2 9(1 -50)2 ) J \ (1-22)2 9(l-50;2 whence, if e be the probable error of this section, 1 _ 1 , 4 e2 (1-22)2 9(1-50) or [on referring to (3)] e = 1*50 j 9 17*6 } (4) (5) that is, the probable error of sections parallel to the plane of yz is nearly ^7=- times that of those parallel to the plane of xz, and the locus of their vertices lies in the plane «OM.APPENDIX B. 223 It is important to notice that all sections parallel to the same co-ordinate plane have the same probable error. 4. The ellipses (2) when referred to their principal axes become, after some arithmetical simplification, y '2 20*68 5-92 = constant, • (6) the major axis being inclined to the axis of x at an angle whose tangent is 0#5014. [In the approximate case the ellipses are y2 + -^— = const., and the major axis is inclined to the axis of x at an angle tan 1i.] 5. The question may be solved in general terms by putting YON = 0, XOM = , and replacing the probable errors 1*22 and 1*50 by a and b respectively; then the ellipses (2) are, y1 j. (* - ytan ¿O2 _ n a2 62 equation (3) becomes y2 + tan Qx y tan 6 0 a2 b2 ) or y : X , , a2 tan 6 C b2 + a2 tan20 / ... (8) and (5) becomes 1 _ 1 e2 a2 tan 20 ¥ ■ ■ ■ ■ ... (9) whence tan (f> tan 0 % |& II . . .(10) If c be the probable error of the projection of p's whole motion on the plane of xz, then c2 = a2 tan2 0 + b2, which is independent of the distance of p's line of motion from the axis of x. Hence also tan <£ _ a2 /1 n x teiT “~P.......................{ } Problem 2.—An index q moves under some restraint up and down a bar AQB, its mean position for any given position of the bar224 NATURAL INHERITANCE. being Q ; tbe bar, always carrying tbe index with it, moves under some restraint up and down a fixed frame YMY', the mean position of Q being M : the movements of the index relatively to the bar and of the bar relatively to the frame being quite independent. For any given observed position of q, required the most probable position of Q (which cannot be observed); it being known that the probable error of q relatively to Q in all positions is 6, and that of Q relatively to M is c. The ordinary law of error is to be assumed. If in any one observation, MQ = x, Qq = y, then the law of error requires x2 y1 c2 + b2 to be a minimum, subject to the condition x + y■ = a, a constant. Hence we have at once, to determine the most probable values of xf _y_ _ a> V2 b2 + cA • (13) and the most probable position of Q, measured from M, when «^s observed distance from M is a, is b2 + c2 a. It also follows at once that the probable error v of Q (which may be obtained by substituting a— x for y in (12)) is given by ?+i?0IV= be vP+1 (14) which it is important to notice, is the same for all values of a.APPENDIX C. 225 C. EXPERIMENTS ON SWEET PEAS BEARING ON THE LAW OF REGRESSION. The reason why Sweet Peas were chosen, and the methods of selecting and planting them are described in Chapter VI., p. 79. The following Table justifies their selection by the convenient and accurate method of weighing, as equivalent to that of measuring them. It will be seen that within the limits of observed variation a difference of 0T72 grain in weight corresponds closely to an average difference of 0*01 inch in diameter. Table 1. -Comparison of Weights of Sweet Peas with their Diameters. Distinguishing letter of seed. Weight of one seed in grains. Common difference = 0’172 grain. Length of row of 100 seeds in inches. Diameter of one seed in hundredths of inch. Common difference = 0 01 inch. K 1-750 21-0 21 L 1-578 20-2 20 M 1-406 19-2 19 3sr 1-234 17-9 18 0 1-062 17-0 17 p •890 16T 16 Q *718 15-2 15 The results of the experiment are given in Table 2; its first and last columns are those that especially interest us; the remaining columns showing how these two were obtained. It will be seen that for each increase of one unit on the part of the parent seed, there is a mean increase of only one-third of a unit in the filial seed ; and again that the mean filial seed resembles the parental when the latter is about 15*5 hundredths of an inch in diameter. Taking 15*5 as the point towards which Filial Regression points, whatever may be the parental deviation from that point, the mean Filial Deviation will be in the same direction, but only one-third as much. Q226 NATURAL INHERITANCE. Table 2. Parent Seeds and their Produce. The proportionate number of sweet peas of different sizes, produced by parent seeds also of different sizes, are given below. The measurements are those of their mean diameters, in hundredths of an inch. Diameter i Diameters of Filial Seeds. Total. Mean Diameter of Filial Seeds. of Parent Seed. Under 15. 15- 16- 17- 18- 19- 20- Above 21- Observed Smoothed 21 22 8 10 18 21 13 6 2 100 17*5 17'3 20 23 10 12 17 20 13 3 2 100 17-3 17*0 19 35 16 12 13 11 10 2 1 100 16*0 166 18 34 12 13 17 16 6 2 0 100 16-3 163 17 37 16 13 16 13 4 1 0 100 15-6 16-0 16 34 15 18 16 13 3 1 0 100 16*0 157 15 46 14 9 11 14 4 2 0 100 15*3 15-4 This point is so low in the scale, that I possess less evidence than I desired to prove the bettering of the produce of very small seeds. The seeds smaller than Q were such a miserable set that I could hardly deal with them. Moreover, they were very infertile. It did, however, happen that in a few of the sets some of the Q seeds turned out very well. If I desired to lay much stress on these experiments, I could make my case considerably stronger by going minutely into other details, including confirmatory measurements of the foliage and length of pod, but I do not care to do so. D. GOOD AND BAD TEMPER IN ENGLISH FAMILIES.1 One of the questions put to the compilers of the Family Records spoken of in page 72, referred to the “ Character and Temperament ” of the persons described. These were distributed through 1 Reprinted after slight revision from Fortnightly Review, July, 1887.APPENDIX D. 22 7 three and sometimes four generations, and consisted of those who lay in the main line of descent, together with their brothers and sisters. Among the replies, I find that much information has been incidentally included concerning what is familiarly called the “ temper’' of no less than 1,981 persons. As this is an adequate number to allow for many inductions, and as temper is a strongly marked characteristic in all animals; and again, as it is of social interest from the large part it plays in influencing domestic happiness for good or ill, it seemed a proper subject for investigation. The best explanation of what I myself mean by the word “ temper ” will be inferred from a list of the various epithets used by the compilers of the Records, which I have interpreted as expressing one or other of its qualities or degrees. The epithets are as follows, arranged alphabetically in the two main divisions of good and bad temper :— Good temper.—Amiable, buoyant, calm, cool, equable, forbearing, gentle, good, mild, placid, self-controlled, submissive, sunny, timid, yielding. (15 epithets in all.) Bad temper.—Acrimonious, aggressive, arbitrary,- bickering, capricious, captious, choleric, contentious, crotchety, decisive, despotic, domineering, easily offended, fiery, fits of anger, gloomy, grumpy, harsh, hasty, headstrong, huffy, impatient, imperative, impetuous, insane temper, irritable, morose, nagging, obstinate, odd-tempered, passionate, peevish, peppery, proud, pugnacious, quarrelsome, quick-tempered, scolding, short, sharp, sulky, sullen, surly, uncertain, vicious, vindictive. (46 epithets in all.) I also grouped the epithets as well as I could, into the following five classes : 1, mild ; 2, docile; 3, fretful; 4, violent; 5, masterful. Though the number of epithets denoting the various kinds of bad temper is three times as large as that used for the good, yet the number of persons described under the one general head is about the same as that described under the other. The first set of data that I tried, gave the proportion of the good to the bad-tempered as 48 to 52; the second set as 47 to 53. There is little difference between the two sexes in the frequency of good and bad temper, but that little is in favour of the women, since about 45 men are re- 2 Q228 NATURAI. INHERITANCE. corded as good-tempered for every 55 who are bad, and conversely 55 women as good-tempered for 45 who are bad. I will not dwell on the immense amount of unhappiness, ranging from family discomfort down to absolute misery, or on the breaches of friendship that must have been occasioned by the cross-grained, sour, and savage dispositions of those who are justly labelled by some of the severer epithets ; or on the comfort, peace, and goodwill diffused through domestic circles by those who are rightly described by many of the epithets in the first group. We can hardly, too, help speculating uneasily upon the terms that our own relatives would select as most appropriate to our particular selves. But these considerations, interesting as they are in themselves, lie altogether outside the special purpose of this inquiry. In order to ascertain the facts of which the above statistics are a brief summary, I began by selecting the larger families out of my lists, namely, those that consisted of not less than four brothers or sisters, and by noting the persons they included who were described as good or bad-tempered; also the remainder about whose temper nothing was said either one way or the other, and whom perforce I must call neutral. I am at the same time well aware that, in some few cases a tacit refusal to describe the temper should be interpreted as reticence in respect to what it was thought undesirable even to touch upon. I found that out of a total of 1,361 children, 321 were described as good-tempered, 705 were not described at all, and 342 were described as bad-tempered. These numbers are nearly in the proportion of 1, 2, and 1, that is to say, the good are equal in number to the bad-tempered, and the neutral are just as numerous as the good and bad-tempered combined. The equality in the total records of good and bad tempers is an emphatic testimony to the correct judgments of the compilers in the choice of their epithets, for whenever a group has to be divided into three classes, of which the second is called neutral, or medium, or any other equivalent term, its nomenclature demands that it should occupy a strictly middlemost position, an equal number of contrasted cases flanking it on either hand. If more cases were recorded of good temper than of bad, the compilers would have laid down the boundaries of the neutral zone unsymmetrically, too farAPPENDIX D. 229 from the good end of the scale of temper, and too near the bad end. If the number of cases of bad temper exceeded that of the good, the error would have been in the opposite direction. But it appears, on the whole, that the compilers of the records have erred neither to the right hand nor to the left. So far, therefore, their judgments are shown to be correct. Next as regards the proportion between the number of those who rank as neutrals to that of the good or of the bad. It was recorded as 2 to 1; is that the proper poportion ? Whenever the nomenclature is obliged to be somewhat arbitrary, a doubtful term should be interpreted in the sense that may have the widest suitability. Now a large class of cases exist in which the interpretation of the word neutral is fixed. It is that in which the three grades of magnitude are conceived to result from the various possible combinations of two elements, one of which is positive and the other negative, such as good and bad, and which are supposed to occur on each occasion at haphazard, but in the long run with equal frequency. The number of possible combinations of the two elements is only four, and each of these must also in the long run occur with equal frequency. They are: 1, both positive; 2, the first positive, the second negative ; 3, the first negative, the second positive; 4, both negative. In the second and third of these combinations the negative counterbalances the positive, and the result is neutral. Therefore the proportions in which the several events of good, neutral, and bad would occur is as 1, 2, and 1. These proportions further commend themselves on the ground that the whole body of cases is thereby divided into two main groups, equal in number, one of which includes all neutral or medium cases, and the other all that are exceptional. Probably it was this latter view that was taken, it may be half unconsciously, by the compilers of the Becords. Anyhow, their entries conform excellently to the proportions specified, and 1 give them credit for their practical appreciation of what seems theoretically to be the fittest standard. I speak, of course, of the Becords taken as a whole ; in small groups of cases the proportion of the neutral to the rest is not so regular. The results shown in Table I. are obtained from all my returns. It is instructive in many ways, and not least in showing to a statistical eye how much and how little value may reasonably be230 NATURAL INHERITANCE. attached to my materials. It was primarily intended to discover whether any strong bias existed among the compilers to spare the characters of their nearest relatives. In not a few cases they have written to me, saying that their records had been drawn up with perfect frankness, and earnestly reminding me of the importance of not allowing their remarks to come to the knowledge of the persons described. It is almost needless to repeat what I have published more than once already, that I treat the Records quite confidentially. I have left written instructions that in case of my death they should all be destroyed unread, except where I have left a note to say that the compiler wished them returned. In some instances I know that the Records were compiled by a sort of family council, one of its members acting as secretary ; but I doubt much whether it often happened that the Records were known to many of the members of the family in their complete form. Bearing these and other considerations in mind, I thought the best test for bias would be to divide the entries into two contrasted groups, one including those who figured in the pedigrees as either father, mother, son, or daughter—that is to say, the compiler and those who were very nearly related to him—and the other including the uncles and aunts on both sides. Table 1. Distribution of Temper in Families (per cents.) Relationships. 2 rH 2. Docile. 3. Fretful. 4. Violent. 1 5. Masterful. | Total. No. of cases observed. ' a. Fathers and Sons 35 12 32 12 9 100 188 b. Mothers and Daughters 39 18 31 8 4 100 179 c. Uncles 32 13 25 18 12 100 272 d. Aunts 39 14 29 9 9 100 238 a + b. Direct line 74 30 63 20 13 200 367 c + d. Collaterals 71 27 54 27 21 200 510 Good. Bad Temper. a + b. Direct line 104 96 200 367 c + d. Collaterals 98 102 200 510APPENDIX D. 231 On comparing the entries, especially the summaries in the lower lines of the Tabler it does not seem that the characters of near relatives are treated much more tenderly than those of the more remote. There is little indication of the compilers having been biased by affection, respect, or fear. More cases of a record being left blank when a bad temper ought to have been recorded, would probably occur in the direct line, but I do not see how this could be tested. An omission may be due to pure ignorance ; indeed I find it not uncommon for compilers to know very little of some of their uncles or aunts. The Records seem to be serious and careful compositions, hardly ever used as vehicles for personal animosity, but written in much the same fair frame of mind that most people force themselves into when they write their wills. Table 2. Combinations of Temper in Marriage (per cents.). A.—Observed Pairs. B.—Haphazard Pairs. Tempers Tempers of Wives. Tempers of Wives. of Husbands. Good. Bad Tempers. Good. Bad Tempers. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Good 1 6 10 9 6 2 13 5 10 3 2 „ 2 4 2 5 2 — 5 2 4 1 1 Bad 3 14 4 9 3 2 11 5 8 2 2 „ 4 7 3 3 2 1 6 2 5 1 1 „ 5 3 — 2 — 1 4 2 3 1 1 Good 22 24 25 21 Bad 31 23 30 24 The sexes are separated in the Table, to show the distribution of the five classes of temper among them severally. There is a large proportion of the violent and masterful among the men, of the fretful, the mild, and the docile among the women. On adding the entries it will be found that the proportion of those who fall232 NATURAL INHERITANCE. within the several classes are 36 per cent, of mild-tempered, 15 per cent, of docile, 29 per cent, of fretful, 12 per cent of violent, 8 per cent, of masterful. The importance assigned in marriage-selection to good and bad temper is an interesting question, not only from its bearing on domestic happiness, but also from the influence it may have in promoting or retarding the natural good temper of our race, assuming, as we may do for the moment, that temper is hereditary. I cannot deal with the question directly, but will give some curious facts in Table II. that throw indirect light upon it. There a comparison is made of (A) the actual frequency of marriage between persons, each of the various classes of temper, with (B) the calculated frequency according to the' laws of chance, on the supposition that there had been no marriage-selection at all, but that the pairings, so far as temper is concerned, had been purely at haphazard. There are only 111 marriages in my lists in which the tempers of both parents are recorded. On the other hand, the number of possible combinations in couples of persons who belong to the five classes of temper is very large, so I make the two groups comparable by reducing both to percentages. It will be seen that with two apparent exceptions in the upper left-hand corners of either Table (of 6 against 13, and of 10 against 5), there are no indications of predilection for, or avoidance of marriage between persons of any of the five classes, but that the figures taken from observation run as closely with those derived through calculation, as could be expected from the small number of observations. The apparent exceptions are that the percentage of mild-tempered men who marry mild-tempered women is only 6, as against 13 calculated by the laws of chance, and that those who marry docile wives are 10, as against a calculated 5. There is little difference between mildness and docility, so w’e may throw these entries together without much error, and then we have 6 and 10, or 16, as against 13 and 5, or 18, which is a close approximation. We may compare the frequency of marriages between persons of like temper in each of the five classes by reading the Table diagonally. They are as (6), 2, 9, 2, 1, in the observed cases, against (13), 2, 8, 1, 1, in the calculated ones; here the irregularity of the 6 and 13, which are put in brackets for distinction sake, isAPPENDIX D. 233 conspicuous. Elsewhere there is not the slightest indication of a dislike in persons of similar tempers, whether mild, docile, fretful, violent, or masterful, to marry one another. The large initial figures 6 and 13 catch the eye, and at a first glance impress themselves unduly on the imagination, and might lead to erroneous speculations about mild-tempered person's, perhaps that they find one another rather insipid ; but the reasons I have given, show conclusively that the recorded rarity of the marriages between mild-tempered persons is only apparent. Lastly, if we disregard the five smaller classes and attend only to the main divisions of good and bad temper, there does not appear to be much bias for, or against, the marriage of good or bad-tempered persons in their own or into the opposite division. The admixture of different tempers among the brothers and sisters of the same family is a notable fact, due to various causes which act in different directions. It is best to consider them before we proceed to collect evidence and attempt its interpretation. It becomes clear enough, and may be now taken for granted, that the tempers of progenitors do not readily blend in the offspring, but that some of the children take mainly after one of them, some after another, but with a few threads, as it were, of various ancestral tempers woven in, which occasionally manifest themselves. If no other influences intervened, the tempers of the children in the same family would on this account be almost as varied as those of their ancestors; and these, as we have just seen, married at haphazard, so far as their tempers were concerned; therefore the numbers of good and bad children in families would be regulated by the same laws of chance that apply to a gambling table. But there are other influences to be considered. There is a well-known tendency to family likeness among brothers and sisters, which is due, not to the blending of ancestral peculiarities, but to the prepotence of one of the progenitors, who stamped more than his or her fair share of qualities upon the descendants. It may be due also to a familiar occurrence that deserves but has not yet received a distinctive name, namely, where all the children are alike and yet their common likeness cannot be traced to their progenitors. A new variety has come into existence through a process that affects the whole Fraternity and may result in an unusually stable variety (see Chapter III.). The most strongly marked family type that I have personally met234 NATURAL INHERITANCE. with, first arose simultaneously in the three brothers of a family who transmitted their peculiarities with unusual tenacity to numerous descendants through at least four generations. Other influences act in antagonism to the foregoing; they are the events of domestic life, which instead of assimilating tempers tend to accentuate slight differences in them. Thus if some members of a family are a little submissive by nature, others who are naturally domineering are tempted to become more so. Then the acquired habit of dictation in these reacts upon the others and makes them still more submissive. In the collection I made of the histories of twins who were closely alike, it was most commonly said that one of the twins was guided by the other. I suppose that after their many childish struggles for supremacy, each finally discovered his own relative strength of character, and thenceforth the stronger developed into the leader, while the weaker contentedly subsided into the position of being led. Again, it is sometimes observed that one member of an otherwise easygoing family, discovers that he or she may exercise considerable power by adopting the habit of being persistently disagreeable whenever he or she does not get the first and best of everything. Some wives contrive to tyrannise over husbands who are mild and sensitive, who hate family scenes and dread the disgrace attending them, by holding themselves in readiness to fly into a passion whenever their wishes are withstood. They thus acquire a habit of “ breaking out,” to use a term familiar to the warders of female prisons and lunatic asylums; and though their relatives and connections would describe their tempers by severe epithets, yet if they had married masterful husbands their characters might have developed more favourably. To recapitulate briefly, one set of influences tends to mix good and bad tempers in a family at haphazard; another set tends to assimilate them, so that they shall all be good or all be bad; a third set tends to divide each family into contrasted portions. We have now to ascertain the facts and learn the results of these opposing influences. In dealing with the distribution of temper in Fraternities,1 * 3 we 1 A Fraternity consists of the brothers of a family, and of the sisters after the qualities of the latter have been transmuted to their Male Equivalents ; hut as no change in the Female values seems really needed, so none has been made in respect to Temper.APPENDIX D. 235 can only make use of those in which at least two cases of temper are recorded ; they are 146 in number. I have removed all the cases of neutral temper, treating them as if they were non-existent, and dealing only with the remainder that are good or bad. We have next to eliminate the haphazard element. Beginning with Fraternities of two persons only, either of whom is just as likely to be good as bad tempered, there are, as we have already seen, four possible combinations, resulting in the proportions of 1 case of both good, 2 cases one good and one bad, and one case of both bad. I have 42 such Fraternities, and the observed facts are that in 10 of them both are good tempered, in 20 one is good and one bad, and in 12 both are bad tempered. Here only a trifling and untrustworthy 'difference is found between the observed and the haphazard distribution, the other conditions having neutralised each other. But when we proceed to larger Fraternities the test becomes shrewder, and the trifling difference already observed becomes more marked, and is at length unmistakable. Thus the successive lines of Table III. show a continually increasing divergence between the observed and the haphazard distribution of temper, as the Fraternities increase in size. A compendious com- TABLE 3. Distribution of Temper in Fraternities. A.—Observed. B.—Haphazard. Number in each Fraternity. Number of Fraternities. All good- tem- pers. Intermediate cases. All bad- tem- pers. All good- tem- pers. Intermediate cases. All bad- tem- pers. 2 42 10 20 12 10 21 11 3 55 11 15 21 8 7 20 21 7 4 29 5 6 9 8 1 2 8 12 8 2 5 6 1 0 2 10 2 0 12 2 1 0 6 14 1 0 13 3 2 4 0 2 4 5 4 2 0 4 to 6 49 7 7 2 2236 NATURAL INHERITANCE. parison is made in the bottom line of the Table by adding together the instances in which the Fraternities are from 4 to 6 in number, and in taking only those in which all the members of the Fraternity were alike in temper, whether good or bad. There are 7 + 7, or 14, observed cases of this against 2 + 2, or 4, haphazard cases, found in a total of 49 Fraternities. Hence it follows that the domestic influences that tend to differentiate temper wholly fail to overcome the influences, hereditary and other, that tend to make it uniform in the same Fraternity. As regards direct evidences of heredity of temper, we must frame our inquiries under a just sense of the sort of materials we have to depend upon. They are but coarse portraits scored with white or black, and sorted into two heaps, irrespective of the gradations of tint in the originals. The processes I have used in discussing the heredity of stature, eye-colour, and artistic faculty, cannot be employed in dealing wTith the heredity of temper. I must now renounce those refined operations and set to work with ruder tools on my rough material. The first inquiry will be, Do good-tempered parents have, on the whole, good-tempered children, and do bad-tempered parents have bad-tempered ones ? I have 43 cases where both parents are recorded as good-tempered, and 25 where they were both bad-tempered. Out of the children of the former, 30 per cent, were good-tempered and 10 per cent, bad; out of the latter, 4 per cent, were good and 52 per cent, bad-tempered. This is emphatic testimony to the heredity of temper. I have worked out the other less contrasted combinations of parental temper, but the results are hardly worth giving. There is also much variability in the proportions of the neutral cases. I then attempted, with still more success, to answer the converse question, Do good-tempered Fraternities have, on the whole, good-tempered ancestors, and bad-tempered Fraternities bad-tempered ones ? After some consideration of the materials, I defined—rightly or wrongly—a good-tempered Fraternity as one in which at least two members were good-tempered and none were bad, and a bad-tempered Fraternity as one in which at least two members were bad-tempered, whether or no any cases of good temper were said to be associated with them. Then, as regards the ancestors, I thoughtAPPENDIX D. 237 by far the most trustworthy group was that which consisted of the two parents and of the uncles and aunts on both sides. I have thus 46 good-tempered Fraternities with an aggregate of 333 parents, uncles, and aunts ; and 71 bad-tempered, with 633 parents, uncles, and aunts. In the former group, 26 per cent, were good tempered and 18 bad; in the latter group, 18 were good-tempered and 29 were bad, the remainder being neutral. These results are almost the exact counterparts of one another, so I seem to have made good hits in framing the definitions. More briefly, we mav say that when the Fraternity is good-tempered as above defined, the number of good-tempered parents, uncles, and aunts, exceeds that of the bad-tempered in the proportion of 3 to 2; and that when the Fraternity is bad-tempered, the proportions are exactly reversed. I have attempted in other ways to work out the statistics of hereditary tempers, but none proved to be of sufficient value for publication. I can trace no prepotency of one sex over the other in transmitting their tempers to their children. I find clear indications of strains of bad temper clinging to families for three generations, but I cannot succeed in putting them into a numerical form. It must not be thought that I have wished to deal with temper as if it were an unchangeable characteristic, or to assign more trustworthiness to my material than it deserves. Both these views have been discussed; they are again alluded to to show that they are not dismissed from my mind, and partly to give the opportunity of adding a very few further remarks. Persons highly respected for social and public qualities may be well-known to their relatives as having sharp tempers under strong but insecure control, so that they “flare up” now and then. I have heard the remark that those who are over-suave in ordinary demeanour have often vile tempers. If this be the case—and I have some evidence of its truth—I suppose they are painfully conscious of their infirmity, and through habitual endeavours to subdue it, have insensibly acquired an exaggerated suavity at the times when their temper is unprovoked. Illness, too, has much influence in affecting the temper. Thus I sometimes come across entries to the effect of, “ not naturally ill-tempered, but peevish238 NATURAL INHERITANCE. through illness.” Overwork and worry will make even mild-tempered men exceedingly touchy and cross. The accurate discernment and designation of character is almost beyond the reach of any one, but, on the other hand, a rough estimate and a fair description of its prominent features is easily obtainable; and it seems to me that the testimony of a member of a family who has seen and observed a person in his unguarded moments and under very varied circumstances for many years, is a verdict deserving of much confidence. I shall have fulfilled my object in writing this paper if it leaves a clear impression of the great range and variety of temper among persons of both sexes in the upper and middle classes of English society; of its disregard in Marriage Selection; of the great admixture of its good and bad varieties in the same family; and of its being, nevertheless, as hereditary as any other quality. Also, that although it exerts an immense influence for good or ill on domestic happiness, it seems that good temper has not been especially looked for, nor ill temper especially shunned, as it ought to be in marriage-selection. E. THE GEOMETRIC MEAN, IN VITAL AND SOCIAL STATISTICS.1 My purpose is to show that an assumption which lies at the basis of the well-known law of “ Frequency of Error” is incorrect when applied to many groups of vital and social phenomena, although that law has been applied to them by statisticians with partial success. Next, I will point out the correct hypothesis upon which a Law of Error suitable to these cases ought to be calculated; and subsequently I will communicate a memoir by Mr. (now Dr.) Donald Macalister, who, at my suggestion, has mathematically investigated the subject. The assumption to which I refer is, that errors in excess or in deficiency of the truth are equally probable ; or conversely, that if two fallible measurements have been made of the same object, their 1 Reprinted, with slight revision, from the Proceedings of the Royal Society, No. 198, 1879.APPENDIX E. 239 arithmetical mean is more likely to be the true measurement than any other quantity that can be named. This assumption cannot be justified in vital phenomena. For example, suppose we endeavour to match a tint; Weber’s law, in its approximative and simplest form, of Sensation varying as the logarithm of the Stimulus, tells us that a series of tints, in which the quantities of white scattered on a black ground are as 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, 32, &c., will appear to the eye to be separated by equal intervals of tint. Therefore, in matching a grey that contains 8 portions of white, we are just as likely to err by selecting one that has 16 portions as one that has 4 portions. In the first case there would be an error in excess, of 8 units of absolute tint; in the second there would be an error in deficiency, of 4. Therefore, an error of the same magnitude in excess or in deficiency is not equally probable in the judgment of tints by the eye. Conversely, if two persons, who are equally good judges, describe their impressions of a certain tint, and one says that it contains 4 portions of white and the other that it contains 16 portions, the most reasonable conclusion is that it really contains 8 portions. The arithmetic mean of the two estimates is 10, which is not the most probable value; it is the geometric mean 8, (4 : 8 : : 8 :16), which is the most probable. Precisely the same condition characterises every determination by each of the senses; for example, in judging of the weight of bodies or of their temperatures, of the loudness and of the pitches of tones, and of estimates of lengths and distances as wholes. Thus, three rods of the lengths a, b, c, when taken successively in the hand, appear to differ by equal intervals when a :b : : b : c, and not when a — b~b - c. In all physiological phenomena, where there is on the one hand a stimulus and on the other a response to that stimulus Weber’s or some other geometric law may be assumed to prevail in other words, the true mean is geometric rather than arithmetic. The geometric mean appears to be equally applicable to the majority of the influences, which, combined with those of purely vital phenomena, give rise to the events with which sociology deals. It is difficult to find terms sufficiently general to apply to the varied topics of sociology, but there are two categories which are of common occurrence in which the geometric mean is certainly appropriate. The one is increase, as exemplified by the growth of population, where an240 NATURAL INHERITANCE. already large nation tends to receive larger accessions than a small oce under similar circumstances, or when a capital employed in a business increases in proportion to its size. The other category is the influences of circumstances or of “ milieux ” as they are often called, such as a period of plenty in which a larger field or a larger business yields a greater excess over its mean yield than a smaller one. Most of the causes of those differences with which sociology are concerned, and which are not purely vital phenomena, such as those previously discussed, may be classified under one or other of these two categories, or under such as are in principle almost the same. In short, sociological phenomena, like vital phenomena are, as a general rule, subject to the condition of the geometric mean. The ordinary law of Frequency of Error, based on the arithmetic mean, corresponds, no doubt, sufficiently well with the observed facts of vital and social phenomena, to be very serviceable to statisticians, but it is far from satisfying their wants, and it may lead to absurdity when applied to wide deviations. It asserts that deviations in excess must be balanced by deviations of equal magnitude in deficiency; therefore, if the former be greater than the mean itself, the latter must be less than zero, that is, must be negative. This is an impossibility in many cases, to which the law is nevertheless applied by statisticians with no small success, so long as they are content to confine its application within a narrow range of deviation. Thus, in respect of Stature, the law is very correct in respect to ordinary measurements, although it asserts that the existence of giants, whose height is more than double the mean height of their race, implies the possibility of the existence of dwarfs, whose stature is less than nothing at all. It is therefore an object not only of theoretical interest but of practical use, to thoroughly investigate a Law of Error, based on the geometric mean, even though some of the expected results may perhaps be apparent at first sight. With this view I placed the foregoing remarks in Mr. Donald Macalister’s hands, who contributed a memoir that will be found in the Proc. Royal JSoc., No. 198, 1879, following my own. It should be referred to by such mathematicians as may read this book.APPENDIX F. 241 F. PROBABLE EXTINCTION OF FAMILIES.1 The decay of the families of men who occupied conspicuous positions in past times has been a subject of frequent remark, and has given rise to various conjectures. It is not only the families of men of geuius or those of the aristocracy who tend to perish, but it is those of all with whom history deals, in any way, even such men as the burgesses of towns, concerning whom Mr. Doubleday has inquired and written. The instances are very numerous in which surnames that were once common have since become scarce or have wholly disappeared. The tendency is universal, and, in explanation of it, the conclusion has been hastily drawn that a rise in physical comfort and intellectual capacity is necessarily accompanied by diminution in “fertility”—using that phrase in its widest sense and reckoning abstinence from marriage as one cause of sterility. If that conclusion be true, our population is chiefly maintained through the “ proletariat,” and thus a large element of degradation is inseparably connected with those other elements which tend to ameliorate the race. On the other hand, M. Alphonse de Candolle has directed attention to the fact that, by the ordinary law of chances, a large proportion of families are continually dying out, and it evidently follows that, until we know what that proportion is, we cannot estimate whether any observed diminution of surnames among the families whose history we can trace, is or is not a sign of their diminished “fertility.” I give extracts from M. De Candolle’s work in a foot-note,2 and may add that, although I have not hitherto published anything on the matter, I took considerable pains some years ago to obtain numerical results in respect to this 1 Reprinted, with slight revision, from the Journ. Anthropol. List. 1888. 2 “ Au milieu des renseignements précis et des opinions très-sensées de MM. Benoiston de Châteauneuf, Galton, et autres statisticiens, je n’ai pas rencontré la réflexion bien importante qu’ils auraient dû faire de l’extinction inévitable des noms de famille. Évidemment tous les noms doivent s’éteindre . . . . Un mathématicien pourrait calculer comment la réduction des noms ou titres aurait lieu, d’après la probabilité des naissances toutes féminines ou toutes masculines ou mélangées et la probabilité d’absence de naissances dans un couple quelconque,” &c.—Alphonse de Candolle, Histoire des Sciences et des Savants, 1873. R242 NATURAL INHERITANCE. very problem. I made certain very simple and not very inaccurate suppositions concerning average fertility, and I worked to the nearest integer, starting with 10,000 persons, but the computation became intolerably tedious after a few steps, and I had to abandon it. The Rev. H. W. Watson kindly, at my request, took the problem in hand, and his results form the subject of the following paper. They do not give what can properly be called a general solution, but they do give certain general results. They show (1) how to compute, though with great labour, any special case; (2) a remarkably easy way of computing those special cases in which the law of fertility approximates to a certain specified form; and (3) how all surnames tend to disappear. The form in which I originally stated the problem is as follows. I purposely limited it in the hope that its solution might be more practicable if unnecessary generalities were excluded :— A large nation, of whom we will only concern ourselves with the adult males, N in number, and who each bear separate surnames, colonise a district. Their law of population is such that, in each generation, a0 per cent, of the adult males have no male children who reach adult life; ai have one such male child; a2 have two; and so on up to who have five. Find (1) what proportion of the surnames will have become extinct after r generations; and (2) how many instances there will be of the same surname being held by m persons. Discussion of the problem by the Rev. H. W. Watson, D.Sc., F.R.S., formerly Fellow of Trinity College, Cambridge. Suppose that at any instant all the adult males of a large nation have different surnames, it is required to find how many of these surnames will have disappeared in a given number of generations upon any hypothesis, to be determined by statistical investigations, of the law of male population. Let, therefore, a0 be the percentage of males in any generation who have no sons reaching adult life, let al be the percentage that have one such son, a2 the percentage that have two, and so on up to aq, the percentage that have q such sons, q being so large that it is not worth while to consider the chance of any man having more than q adult sons—our first hypothesis will be that the numbersAPPENDIX F. 243 a0, av a2, etc.,. remain the same in each succeeding generation. We shall also, in what follows, neglect the overlapping of generations—that is to say, we shall treat the problem as if all the sons born to any man in any generation came into being at one birth, and as if every man’s sons were born and died at the same time. Of course it cannot be asserted that these assumptions are correct. Yery probably accurate statistics would discover variations in the values of a0, av etc., as the nation progressed or retrograded; but it is not at all likely that this variation is so rapid as seriously to vitiate any general conclusions arrived at on the assumption of the values remaining the same through many successive generations. It is obvious also that the generations must overlap, and the neglect to take account of this fact is equivalent to saying, that at any given time we leave out of consideration those male descendants, of any original ancestor who are more than a certain average number of generations removed from him, and compensate for this by giving credit for such male descendants, not yet come into being, as are not more than that same average number of generations removed from the original ancestors. Let then i, -5l, etc., up to be denoted by the sym- bols i0, tv t2, etc., up to tqi in other words, let £0, tv etc., be the chances in the first and each succeeding generation of any individual man, in any generation, having no son, one son, two sons, and so on, who reach adult life. Let N be the original number of distinct surnames, and let rms be the fraction of N which indicates the number of such surnames with s representatives in the rth generation. Now, if any surname have p representatives in any generation, it follows from the ordinary theory of chances that the chance of that same surname having s representatives in the next succeeding generation- is the coefficient of xs in the expansion of the multinomial (t0 + txx + t2x2 +, etc. + tqxq)p Let then the expression t0 + txx -f t2x2 -f- etc. + tqxq be represented by the symbol T. Then since, by the assumption already made, the number of surnames with no representative in the r-lth generation is N, the E 2244 NATURAL INHERITANCE. number with one representative y_1m1.N, the number with two r_pm2. N and so on, it follows, from what we last stated, that the number of surnames with s representatives in the rth generation must be the coefficient of xs in the expression | r-i«i0 + r_1M1T + )..1«î2T2+ etc. + r.1mqr_1T,r-1 }N If, therefore, the coefficient of N in this expression be denoted by /, (x) it follows that r.1mv and so on, are the coefficients of x, x1 and so on, in the expression fr_Y (x). If, therefore, a series of functions be found such that f1(x)=:t0 + txx + etc. + tqxq and fr (x) =fr_Y (t^ + t-^ etc. + tqxq) then the proportional number of groups of surnames with s representatives in the rth generation will be the coefficient of x* in fr (x) and the actual number of such surnames will be found by multiplying this coefficient by N. The number of surnames unrepresented or become extinct in the rth generation will be found by multiplying the term independent of x in fr (x) by the number N. The determination, therefore, of the rapidity of extinction of surnames, when the statistical data, t0, tv etc., are given, is reduced to the mechanical, but generally laborious process of successive substitution of t0 + tpx + t2x2 + etc., for x in successively determined values of /,( x), and no further progress can be made with the problem until these statistical data are fixed ; the following illustrations of the application of our formula are, however, not without interest. (1) The very simplest case by which the formula can be illustrated is when ç = 2 and t0, tv t2 are each equal to Here /x (x) = ~^+X/2(x) = I j 1 + -1 (l+a; + a:2)+ * 1+x + x2)^ and so on. Making the successive substitutions, we obtain 13 5x 6x2 2x x \ 9+ü + 9 +9+9i 1249 + 265a; 343a;2 166x3 109^ 34a:5 16a;6 43/ æ8 2187 2187 2187 2187 +2187 2187 + 2187 + 2187 + 2187 f4(x)= -63183 + -08306a; + -10635a:2 + -07804a;3 + ’06489a;4 + -05443a;5 + -01437a;6 + -01692a:7 + -01144a;8 + '00367a:9 + -00104a;1» + -00015a;11 + -00005a;12 + -00001a;13 + -00000a;14 + -00000a;15 + -00000a;'6APPENDIX F. 245 and the constant term in fb (x) or 5m0 is therefore «.01QQ -08306 , *10635 , *07804 , *06489 bolotj +-----— + —-— +---------— + - 27 81 *05443 *01437 *01692 *01144 + 243 + 729 + 2187 + 6561 *00367 *00104 *00015 19683 + 59049+177147 The value of which to five places of decimals is *67528. The constant terms, therefore, in fv f2 up to f5 when reduced to decimals, are in this case *33333, *48148, *57110, *64113, and *65628 respectively. That is to say, out of a million surnames at starting, there have disappeared in the course of one, two, etc., up to five generations, 333333, 481480, 571100, 641130, and 675280 respectively. The disappearances are much more rapid in the earlier than in the later generations. Three hundred thousand disappear in the first generation, one hundred and fifty thousand more in the second, and so on, while in passing from the fourth to the fifth, not more than thirty thousand surnames disappear. All this time the male population remains constant. For it is evident that the male population of any generation is to be found by multiplying that of the preceding generation, by tx + 2t2, and this quantity is in the present case equal to one. If axes Ox and Oy be drawn, and equal distances along Ox represent generations from starting, while two distances are marked along every ordinate, the one representing the total male population in any generation, and the other the number of remaining surnames in that generation, of the two curves passing through the extremities of these ordinates, the population curve wfill, in this case, be a straight line parallel to Ox, while the surname curve will intersect the population curve on the axis of y, will proceed always convex to the axis of x, and will have the positive part of that axis for an asymptote. The case just discussed illustrates the use to be made of the general formula, as well as the labour of successive substitutions, when the expressionsfx (x) does not follow some assigned law. The calculation may be infinitely simplified when such a law can be found; especially if that law be the expansion of a binomial, and only the extinctions are required. For example, suppose that the terms of the expression i0+ £]&■+• etc. + tqxq are proportional to the terms of the expanded binomial246 NATURAL INHERITANCE. (a-\-bx)q i.e. suppose that i0 = (a + b)' tL = q-V a -T b)q aq and so on. Here/‘ “d >”• - 1, and, therefore an indefinite increase of male population. The true interpretation is that each of the quantities, rmv rm2, etc., tends to become zero, as r is indefinitely increased, but that it does not follow that the product of each by the infinitely large number N is also zero. As, therefore, time proceeds indefinitely, the number of surnames extinguished becomes a number of the same order of magnitude as the total number at first starting in N, while the number of surnames represented by one, two, three, etc., lepresentatives is some infinitely smaller but finite number. When the finite numbers are multiplied by the corresponding number of representatives, sometimes infinite in number, and the products added together, the sum will generally exceed the original number N. In point of fact, just as in the cases calculated above to five generations, we had a continual, and, indeed248 NATURAL INHERITANCE. at first, a rapid extinction of surnames, combined in the one case with a stationary, and in the other case an increasing population, so is it when the number of generations is increased indefinitely. We have a continual extinction of surnames going on, combined with constancy, or increase of population, as the case may be, until at length the number of surnames remaining is absolutely insensible, as compared with the number at starting; but the total number of representatives of those remaining surnames is infinitely greater than the original number. We are not in a position to assert from actual calculation that a corresponding result is true for every form of fx (x), but the reasonable inference is that such is the case, seeing that it holds whenever fx (x) may be compared with whatever 0r q may be. {a + bf G. ORDERLY ARRANGEMENT OF HEREDITARY DATA. Tiiere are many methods both of drawing pedigrees and of describing kinship, but for my own purposes I still prefer those that I designed myself. The chief requirements that have to be fulfilled are compactness, an orderly and natural arrangement, and clearly intelligible symbols. Nomenclature.—A symbol ought to be suggestive, consequently the initial letter of a word is commonly used for the purpose. But this practice would lead to singular complications in symbolizing the various ranks of kinship, and it must be applied sparingly. The letter F is equally likely to suggest any one of the three very different words of Father, Female, and Fraternal. The letter M suggests both Mother and Male ; S would do equally for Son and for Sister. Whether they are English, French, or German words, much the same complexity prevails. The system employed in Hereditary Genius had the merit of brevity, but was apt to cause mistake ; it was awkward in manuscript and difficult to the printer, and I have now abandoned it in favour of the method employed in the RecordsAPPENDIX G. 249 of Family Faculties. This will now be briefly described again. Each kinsman can be described in two ways, either by letters or by a number. In ordinary cases both the letter and number are intended to be used simultaneously, thus FF. 8. means the Father’s Father of the person described, though either FF or 8, standing by themselves, would have the same meaning. The double nomenclature has great practical advantages. It is a check against mistake and makes reference and orderly arrangement easy. As regards the letters, F stands for Father and M for Mother, whenever no letter succeeds them ; otherwise they stand for Father’s and for Mother’s respectively. Thus F is Father; FM is Father’s Mother; FMF is Father’s Mother’s Father. As regards the principle upon which the numbers are assigned, arithmeticians will understand me when I say that it is in accordance with the binary system of notation, which runs parallel to the binary distribution of the successive ranks of ancestry, as two parents, four grandparents, eight great-grandparents, and so on. The “ subject ” of the pedigree is of generation O ; that of his parents, of generation 1 ; that of his grandparents, of generation 2, &c. This is clearly shown in the following table :— Kinship. Order of Genera- tion. Numerical Values in Binary Notation. in Decimal Notation. Child 0 1 1 Parents 1 10 11 2 3 Gr. Par 2 100 101 110 111 4 5 6 7 1 1000 1010 1100 1110 8 10 12 14 G. Gr. Par 3 * 1 1 ! 1001 1011 1100 1111 9 11 13 15 All the male ancestry are thus described by even numbers and the female ancestry by odd ones. They run as follows :—250 NATURAL INHERITANCE. r, 2.] M, 3. IT, 4. FM, 5. MF, 6. MM, 7. FFF, 8. FMF, 10. MFF, 12. MMF, 14. FFM, 9. FMM, 11. MFM, 13. MMM, 15. It will be observed that the double of the number of any ancestor is that of his or her Father; and that the double of the number plus 1 is that of his or her Mother ; thus FM 5 has for her father FMF 10, and for her mother FMM 11. When the word Brother or Sister has to be abbreviated it is safer not to be too stingy in assigning letters, but to write hr, sr, and in the plural brs, srs ; also for the long phrase of “ brothers and sisters,” to write brss. All these symbols are brief enough to save a great deal of space, and they are perfectly explicit. When such a phrase has to be expressed as “ the Fraternity of whom FF is one ” I write in my own notes simply FF', but there has been no occasion to adopt this symbol in the present book. I have not satisfied myself as to any system for expressing descendants. Theoretically, the above binary system admits of extension by the use of negative indices, but the practical application of the idea seems cumbrous. We and the French sadly want a word that the Germans possess to stand for Brothers and Sisters. Fraternity refers properly to the brothers only, but its use has been legitimately extended here to mean the brothers and the sisters, after the qualities of the latter have been reduced to their male equivalents. The Greek word adelphic would dp for an adjective. Pedigrees.—The method employed in the Record of Family Faculties for entering all the facts concerning each kinsman in a methodical manner was fully described in that book, and could not easily be epitomised here; but a description of the method in which the manuscript extracts from the records have been made for my own use will be of service to others when epitomising their own family characteristics. It will be sufficient to describe the quarto books that contain the medical extracts. Each page is ten and a half inches high and eight and a half wide, and the two pages, 252, 253, that areSchedule. James Liphook. James Liphook. Father’s Father’s Father Father’s Mother’s Father Mother’s Father’s Father Mother’s Mother’s Father and his fraternity. and his fraternity. and his fraternity. and his fraternity. Father’s Father’s Mother Father’s Mother’s Mother Mother’s Father’s Mother Mother’s Mother’s Mother and her fraternity. and her fraternity. and her fraternity. and her fraternity. Father’s Father Father’s Mother Mother’s Father Mother’s Mother and his fraternity. and her fraternity. and his fraternity. and her fraternity. Spare Father Spare Spare Mother Spare space. and his fraternity. space. space. and her fraternity. space. Spare space. Children. APPENDIX G. 251252 NATURAL INHERITANCE. Example A. Father’s name . James Gladding. Mother’s maiden name .... Mary Claremont. Initials. Kin. Principal illnesses and cause of death. Age at death. J. G. Father Bad rheum, fever; agonising headaches; diar- rhoea; bronchitis ; pleurisy. . Heart disease 54 R. G. bro. Rheum, gout Apoplexy 56 W. G. bro. Good health except gout; paralysis later Apoplexy 88 F. L. sis. Rheum, fever and rheum, gout . . . Apoplexy 73 C. G. sis. Delicate (inoculated) Small pox M. G. Mother Tendency to lung disease ; biliousness ; fre- quent heart attacks . Heart disease and dropsy 63 A. C. bro. Good health Accident 46 W. C. bro. Led a wild life Premature old age 62 E. C. bro. Always delicate Consumption 19 F. R. sis. Small-pox three times .... General failure 85 R. N. sis. Bilious ; weak health Cancer 50 L. C. sis. 21 M. G. bro. Inflam, lungs; rheum, fever . . Heart disease 17 K. G. bro. Debility ; heart disease ; colds . Consumption 40 G. L. sis. Bad headaches ; coughs ; weak spine ; hysteria ; apoplexy Paralysis .5.° F. S. sis. Bad colds ; inflam, lungs ; hysteria living R. F. sis. Infantile paralysis ; colds ; nervous depression . living L. G. sis. Inflam, brain, also lungs ; neuralgia ; nervous fever , . . living (Space left for remarks.) APPENDIX G. 253 Example B. Father’s name Julius Fitzroy. Mother’s maiden name . . . Amelia Merryweather. Initials. Kin. Principal illnesses and cause of death. Age at death. R. F. Father Gouty Habit . . Weak Heart and Congest. Liver 73 L. F.i bro. 88 A.G. F. bro. 48 W. F. bro. 16 Mother Gallstones. . . . Internal Malady (?) Cancer 55 P. M. bro. Paralysis 86 A. M. bro. 85 N. M. bro. Still living. R. B. sis. 33 C. M. sis. . . Rheum. in Head 88 F. L. sis. Softening of Brain 76 1 died an infant. G. F. bro. Gout : tendency to mesenteric disease ; eruptive disorders . . . Blood poisoning after a cut 46 H. F. bro. Liver deranged ; bad headaches ; once supposed consumptive Gradual Paralysis 45 S. T. F. bro. Eruptive disorder ; mesentery disease ; inflam- mation of liver . . Inflammation of Lungs 42 H. G. sis. Eruptive disorder ; liver . . . Inflam, of Lungs 47 H. B.R. sis. Delicate ; tend, to mesent. disease . Consumption 29 N. F. sis. Colds ; liver disorder Consumption 30 E. L. F. sis. Mesenteric disease ; grandular swellings . Atrophy 16 2 died infants. (Space left for remarks.)254 NATURAL INHERITANCE. found wherever the book is opened, relate to the same family. The open book is ruled so as to resemble the accompanying schedule, which is drawn on a reduced scale on page 251. The printing within the compartments of the schedule does not appear in the MS. books, it is inserted here merely to show to whom each compartment refers. It will be seen that the paternal ancestry are described in the left page, the maternal in the right. The method of arrangement is quite orderly, but not altogether uniform. To avoid an unpleasing arrangement like a tree with branches, and which is very wasteful of space, each grandparent and his own two parents are arranged in a set of three compartments one above the other. There are, of course, four grandparents and therefore four such sets in the schedule. Reference to the examples A and B pages 252 and 253 will show how these compartments are filled up. The rest of the Schedule explains itself. The children of the pedigree are written below the compartment assigned to the mother and her brothers and sisters; the spare spaces are of much occasional service, to receive the overflow from some of the already filled compartments as well as for notes. It is astonishing how much can be got into such a schedule by writing on ruled paper with the lines one-sixth of an inch apart, which is not too close for use. Of course the writing must be small, but it may be bold, and the figures should be written very distinctly. For a less ambitious attempt, including the grandparents and their fraternity, but not going further back, the left-hand page would suffice, placing “ Children ” where “ Father ” now stands, “ Father’s Father ” for “ Father,” and so on throughout.INDEX.INDEX. Accidents, 19, 55, 65 Acquired peculiarities, inheritance of, 14 Album for Life History, 220 Ancestral contributions, 134 Adelpliic, 250 Anthropometric Committee of the British Assoc., 95 ; anthrop. laboratory data, 43, 46, 79 Apparatus, see Models Acquired faculties, transmission ofj 16, 197 Artistic Faculty, 154 Averages tell little, 36 ; comparison of with medians, 41 Awards for R.F.F., 75 Bartholomew’s Hospital, 47 Bias, Statistical effect of, in marriage selection, 162 ; in suppressing cause of death, 168 Blends, or refusals to blend, 12 ; in issue of unlike parents, 89 ; in hazel eyes, 145 ; in diseases, 169 ; in new varieties, 198 ; in temper, 233 ; have no effect on statistical results, 17 Cabs, 26, 30, 31 Candolle, A de, 142, 145, 241 n Cards, illustration by, 188 Chance, 19 Chaos, order in, 66 Child, its relation to mother, 15 ; to either parent, 19; of drunken mother, 15 ; of consumptive mother, 177 Cloud compared to a population, 164 Co-Fraternities, 94 Consumption, 171 ; consumptivity, 181 Contributions from separate ancestors, 134 Cookery, typical dishes, 24 Cousins, their nearness in kinship, 133 ; marriages between, 175 Crelle’s tables, 7 Crowds, characteristic forms, 23 Curve of Frequency, 40, 49 ; of Distribution, 40, 54 Darwin, 4, 19 Data, 71 De Candolle, 142, 145, 241 n Deviations, schemes of, 51, 60 ; cause of, 55. See Error. Dickson, J. D. H., 69, 102, 115, Appendix B., 221 Disease, 164; skipping a generation, 12 Distribution, schemes of, 37 ; normal curve of, 54 Error, curve of Frequency, 49 ; law of, 55 ; probable, 57 Evolution not by minute steps only, 32 Eye colour, 138 Families, extinction of, Appendix F., 241 Family Records, see Records Father, see Parent Files in a squadron, 111 Filial relation, 19 ; regression, 95 Fraternities, meaning of word, 94, 234 ; to be treated as units, 35 ; issued from unlike parents, 90 ; variability in, 124, 129 ; regression in, 108 S258 INDEX. Frequency, Scheme of, 49 ; model, 63 ; surface of, 102 Geometric Mean, 118, Appendix E, 238 Gouffd, cookery book, 24 Governments, 22 Grades, 37, 40 ; of modulus, mean error, &c., 57 Grove battery, 31 Hazel eyes, 144 Hospital, St. Bartholomew’s, 47 Humphreys, G., 186 Incomes of the English, 35 Infertility of mixed types, 31 Insurance companies, 185 Inventions, 25 Island and islets, 10 Kenilworth Castle, 21 Kinship, formula of, 114 ; table of nearness in different degrees, 132 Laboratory, anthropometric, 43, 46, 79 Landscape, characteristic features, 23 Latent Elements, 187 ; characters, 11 M, its signification, 41 Macalister, D., 238 McKendrick, Professor, 20 Malformation, 176 Marriage selection, in temper, 85, 232 ; in eye-colour, 86, 147 ; in stature, 87 ; in artistic faculty, 157 Means, 41 ; mean error, 57 Mechanical inventions, 25 Median, 41 Medical students, 47 Merrifield, F., 136 Mid, 41 ; Mid-parent, 87 ; mid-population, 92 ; mid-fraternity, 124 ; miderror, 58 Models, to illustrate Stability, 27 ; Curve of Frequency, 63 ; Forecast of stature of children, 107 ; Surface of Frequency, 115 Modulus, its grade, 57 Moths, pedigree, 136 Mother’s relation to her child, 15 ; in consumption, 177. See Parent Mulatto blends, 13 Museums to illustrate evolution, 33 Music, 155, 158 Natural, its meaning, 4 ; natural selection, 32, 119 Nephews, 133 Nile expedition, 23 Nomenclature of kinship, 248 Normal Variability, 51 O’Brien, 84 Omnibuses, 26, 32 Order in apparent chaos, 66 Organic Stability, 4 Paget, Sir J., 47 Palmer, 48 Pangenesis, 19, 193 Parents, unlike in stature, issue of, 88 ; indirect relation to children, 19 ; parental relation, 110, 132 Particulate inheritance, 7 Peas, experiments with, 79, 225 Peculiarities, natural and acquired, 4 ; inheritance of, 138 ; definition, 138, 194 Pedigree moths, 136 Pedigrees, arrangement of, see Appendix G., 248, 250 Percentiles, 46 Personal elements, 187; in incipient structure, 9 Petty influences, 16 Population to be treated as units, 35 ; their mid-stature, 92 ; their Q, 93 : successive generations of, 115 ; compared to a cloud, 164 Priestley, Dr. W. O., 15 • Probability integral, 54 ; tables, 202 to 205 Probable error, 53, 57 Problems in the law of error, 66 Processes in Heredity, 4 Pure Breed, 189 Q, its meaning, 53, 59 R. F.F., see Records of Family Faculties Rank of Faculty, 36, 46 ; in a squadron, 110 Records of Family Faculties, data, 72 ; arrangement of, 250 ; prefaced remarks, 168 ; object of book, 220 ; trustworthiness of, 130, 167, 231INDEX. 259 Regression, filial, 95, 98, 131 ; mid-parental, 99, 101 ; parental, 100; fraternal, 109 ; generally, 103, 110, 114 ; is a measure of nearness of kinship, 132 ; in artistic faculty, 158; in consumptivity, .181 ; an element of stability in characteristics of a people, 163 Schemes of Distbibution and Fee-quency, 35 Scientific societies, their government, 22 Selection in marriage, 85, 157, 147 ; effect of bias in, 162 ; Natural Selection, 32, 119 Ships of war, 123 Simplification of inquiries into heredity, 191 Solomon, 24 Special data, 78 Sports, stability of, 30, 198 Squadron, 110 Stable forms, 20, 123, 198 ; subordinate, 25 Stability of sports, 30, 234 ; of characteristics in a people, 163 Statistics, are processes of blending, 17 ; charms of, 62 Statuee, discussion of Data, 83 Strength, Scheme of, 37 Structure, incipient, 18 Sweet peas, 79, 225 Swiss guides, 23 Tempee, Appendix D., 236, 85 Traits, 9 Transmutation of female measures, 5, 42, 78 Trustworthiness of the data, 130, 167, 231 Types, 24 ; a marked family type, 234 Uncles, 133, 191 Variability of Stature, in population, 93 ; in Mid-Parents, 93 ; in Co-Fraternities, 94 ; in Fraternities, 124 ; in a pure breed, 189 Variation, individual, 9 Watson, Rev. H. W., 242 Weissman, 193 THE END. RICHARD CLAY AND SOls§/ LIMITED, LONDON AND BUNGAY,This book is a preservation facsimile. It is made in compliance with copyright law and produced on acid-free archival 60# book weight paper which meets the requirements of ANSI/NISO Z39.48-1992 (permanence of paper) Preservation facsimile printing and binding by Acme Bookbinding Charlestown, Massachusetts 2007