mm'i>A' -^ UC-NRLF lllllllillililllllllli B M bDl D=1D !IBfVEHS?TY HF ^«t^r^KF: iii.f I'l ..■^ «»in.uj J i"!iijiia.. £y f Heterophoria le Memorial Lecture of the phthalmologlcal Congress 1921 By Ernest E. Maddox, M. D., F. R. C. S. Ed. Reprinted from Volume 3, No. 1 The American Journal of Physiological Optics Published by the Division of Ocular Interests American Optical Company ^l^.^ni£s J ^ a^ -■^r n t Tirtil^j Heterophoria Doyne Memorial Lecture of the Oxford Ophthalmological Congress 1921 By Ernest E. Maddox, M. D., F. R. C. S. Ed. Reprinted from Volume 3, No. 1 The American Journal of Physiological Optics Published by the Division of Ocular Interests American Optical Ckimpany ^^^/ C.A\ Ty^tn-^ — .— mj — OPTOMETRY LIBRARY Heterophoria being the ." • ' . •; • Doyne Memorial Lecture of the Oxford Ophthalmo- logical Congress 1921 Ernest E. Maddox, M. D., F. R. C. S. Ed. THE founder, and first "master" of this Congress, Mr. Robert W. Doyne, was an exceptionally ardent benefactor of ophthalmology. To him Oxford practically owed its Eye Infirmary, and the University its diploma in Ophthalmology. The early meetings of the Congress are alive with memories to many of us, of his genial presence, setting everyone at ease, even including, to our admiration, those unfortunate if honoured patients upon whom members of the Congress were allowed to operate! On later occasions, the scene changed, and 'pathos' entered. The well-knit frame, inured to manly sports, had been manifestly overworked (for he did the work of two men) and we saw the evening time of life gathering about him all too prematurely; illustrating how "Swift to its close ebbs out life's little day, Earth's joys grow dim, its glories pass away," Among many other mementoes, he has left this Congress behind him but not without first committing its welfare to the able hands in which it has continued to prosper. Introduction We owe the name, and most of the classification, of heterophoria (which means a 'tendency to differ'), to George Stevens, of New York, whose English ancestors, it is interesting to note, were ancient benefactors of Wadham College, Oxford. For twenty years before his dav, hetero- 25 GoGOai AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS j5horia Was^'ftidied under other names, such as 'insufficiency' ^nd'i&t^nt doyi^t'ons,' under the presiding genius of von Gtde'fe, ahd'I'nbtlce in the literature that even what we now call hyperphoria was corrected by Mr. W. A. Brailey, more than forty years ago. Heterophoria is a tendency to imperfection in the oculo- motor apparatus, so let us first glance at that apparatus itself. To begin with, there are several things it shares in common with the rest of nature, viz. ow the surface diversity and beauty, and under the surface, unitv, balance, habit and rhythm. The diversity of nature is very evident, but the under- lying unity is just as real. Gravitation, for instance, binds the whole universe in one, while radiant energy every- where fills it with light and warmth. The same laws of motion which guide a schoolboy's marble, are implicitly obeyed by the remotest planet. This principle of diversity in unity is found in every object of nature, for the small re/3ects the great. It is strikingly so in living organisms, ane is particularly well seen in the eyes. The cerebral hemispheres make the brain appear double, yet it is really one, and the eyes are windows (also two in one) through which that most deeply hidden of all the systems, the central nervous system, comes to the surface to look out upon the world. It is no wonder, therefore, that Hering long ago described the eyes as a single organ with two limbs. This single organ he called the 'Doppelauge' and located it virtually behind the root of the nose. I have ventured to name it the 'Binoculus' as more easily pronounced. Pictures on the maculae are normally referred to the line which connects the Binoculus with the point of intersection of the visual lines. You may ask; "If the eyes are virtually one, why are there two?" The duality of the eyes is necessary for stereoscopic vision, to give us two points of view meeting in one; for a wider field; for safety in the event of accident or foreign body; for beauty (for could you imagine yourselves proposing 26 HETEROPHORIA— MADDOX to a Cyclopean young lady? or a lady with three eyes?), and lastly for silent eloquence; for eyes can speak, and, as the poet expressed it, "Soft eyes spake love to eyes which spake again." Together with the lids and eyebrows, the eyes express the passing emotions, as a silent lake reflects the changing sky. Just as the mind is linked with the cerebral cortex, so, it would seem the emotions are linked with the vegetative nervous system, and the eyes are linked with both, for they express both thought and emotion. Their link with the vegetative system is well shewn by the gastric disturbance so invariably met with in acute glaucoma; and conversely, an ice-cream, as Lucien Howe mentions, will often cause a brow-ache. This "reverse action" has a great bearing on heterophoria, for we may sometimes consider it the cause of symptoms when itself is caused by visceral irritation. As regards the muscles, nerves and terminal motor nuclei the eyes are two, but with respect to all higher neurons they are one. It is a most important clinical distinction that all affec- tions of the final common paths are non-comitant, while all those of higher structures are comitant. Hence in the presence of comitancy we cannot use such expressions as 'weakness of the interni, of the superior recti, the obliques,' or the like. It is not that such conditions are impossible, but if they are present non-comitancy is present. If the internal recti were weak there would be increase of the defect on looking to either side; if the superior recti, there would be hyperphoria on looking to one side, and the opposite hyperphoria on looking to the other side, and similar 'alternating hyperphoria' as we might call it, would be in evidence if the superior or inferior obliques were weak. Every intraorbital explanation of comitant deviatio77S must be fallacious. Divisions of Heterophoria The first great division of heterophoria, therefore, is 27 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS into what I would call 'lower' and 'higher' heterophoria, according as it is non-comitant or comitant. It is clear that we need two different kinds of charts, one for the muscles (Chart I) and one for the reflexes (Chart II), and we need to be somewhat on the alert to know which chart is appropriate to a special case. I believe all reflexes act through the higher neurons. No single muscle is afl^ected by a reflex. CHART I muscue: chart ELEVATORS K SUP I^ecT ' i_- SUP RECT. l_.