iiliiiiiiiilii /x '^^ X >*■ J!' "o. i / V >w^ . z % / V \PP Ti . .# .# .^*^ erk^ SYMPTOMS AND THEIR INTERPRETATION. SYMPTOMS AND THEIR INTERPRETATIOK Sm JAMES MACKENZIE, M.D., F.R.C.R, LL.D., | Aber. & Edin., F.R.S., F.R.C.P.I. (Hon.). j Honorary Consulting Physician to His Majesty the King in Scotland. ^ Consulting Physician to the London Hospital and the Victoria Hospital, Burnley. ^ Director of the St. Andrews Institute for Clinical Research. 4TH Edition. Also translations into French, German, etc., etc. LONDON : SHAW & SONS, 7 & 8, FETTER LANE, E.C.4, printers anD publisbers. 1920. tliOLOGY LIBRARY PREFACE TO THE FOURTH EDITION. TT Then, a dozen years ago, I wrote the first edition of this book, I dimly perceived that there was a field of medical knowledge essential to pro- gress wliich was left almost unexplored. It might seem absurd to say that the field of symptomatology was in tliis state, for has not the study of symptoms been prosecuted since the dawn of medicine ? Nevertheless, the more I have gone into this subject the more evident has it become that symptoms have not been properly investigated, and even the methods by which the investigation should be pm'sued have not yet been understood. Though the value of symptoms has been recog- nised, yet the nature of their importance has not been clearly understood, and in consequence the vi. Preface. manner in which they should be studied was not guided by a definite object or on sound principles. Disease is only made manifest to us by the symptoms it produces, but when a man falls ill the function of every organ of the body may be disturbed, and did we possess the means an infinitude of symptoms would be discovered. As it is, new methods are continually being devised for the detection of new symptoms, and medicine is breaking up into an ever-increasing number of sections. Men are devoting much time to special subjects and using the resources of other sciences in developing their speciahty. In this way, an ever-increasing number of symptoms are being revealed. Tliis kind of research seems justified by the behef that because a new fact is discovered knowledge is progressing. Whereas, the reverse is the case, for this kind of research only tends to defeat its object. In place of advancing knowledge, it actually hampers it by Preface. vii. clouding over the methods and the objects of the science of medicine, by an ever-accumulating mass of details. The progress of true knowledge is ever accompanied by a simplifying of the subject. This is because the laws of Nature are few in number. Details, mth no understanding of the laws which govern their -production, only lead to confusion. The discovery of the laws, on the other hand, tends to bring order out of the chaos by classifying the details according to those laws which govern their production. This book is an attempt to find those laws, and by indicating principles to guide research on better defined Hnes. J. M. St. Andrews, Scotland. October, 1920. January, 1921. In the foregoing preface of this edition the need for understanding the laws which govern the pro- viii. Preface. cluction of symptoms was dwelt upon, and it was then stated that this book was an attempt to find those laws. While the book was in the Press the search for these laws was being conducted by the staff of the St. Andrews Institute for Clinical Research. They have been successful in dis- covering that the principle governing the produc- tion of manj^ symptoms is due to the disturbance of normal reflexes. This idea is foreshadowed in the book, and although it is impossible at this stage to tell the effect it may have on medicine in general, its application in practice and in research has already shown it to be of service. The description of this theory, which appears in an Appendix at the end of this volume, was published in the British Medical Journal of 29th January, 1921, by whose kind permission it is reproduced. ( ix. ) CONTENTS Chapter 1. Pages 1 — 10. The Importance of Symptoms in Medical Practice and Research. 1. The science of medicine. 2. The importance of symptoms in diagnosis. 3. The importance of symptoms in prognosis. 4. The importance of symptoms in treatment. 5. The importance of symptoms in research. Chapter II. Pages 11 — 18. The Mechanism of a Disease Process. 6. Definition of disease. 7. The detection of disease. 8. Definition of symptoms. 9. Methods for investigating symptoms. 10. Principles of research. Chapter III. Pages 19—29. The Training of the Observer. 11. Medicine a science. 12. Accuracy of observation. 13. Clinical and laboratory observation compared, li. Limitation of laboratory methods. 15. The importance of a clinical training. 16. The requirements of a clinical observer. X. Contents. Chapter IV. Pages 30— ;j9. Principles of Investigation. 17. The need for principles. 18. Differentiation. 19. Classi- fication. 20. The law of progression. 21. The law of associated phenomena. 22. The significance of symptoms (prognosis). 23. The use of laboratory methods. 24. The expectation of results. Chapter V. Pages 40 — 47. Classification of Symptotns. 25. The anatomical classification of disease. 26. The clinical classification of symptoms, 27. Structural symptoms. 28. Functional symptoms. Chapter VI. Pages 48 — 56. Reflex Symptoms. 29. The discovery of the seat of disturbance. 30. The discovery of the agent causing ill-health. 31. The discovery of the functions of the nervous system. 32. Methods of examination. 33. Mechanism of an ordinary stimulation. 34. Mechanism of the radiation of sensation. 35. Mechanism of a motor reflex. 36. Mechanism of radiation in disease. Chapter VII. Pages 57 — 60. Pain. 37. Definition of pain. 38. Constitution of the nervous system. 39. Difference in the functions of the two nervous svstems. Conteyits, xi. Chapter VIII. Pages 61—73. The Sensitiveness of the Tissues. 40. The tissues sensitive and not sensitive to mechanical stimulus. 41. Sensitive tissues of the external body wall. 42. Insensitiveness of the viscera to mechanical stimulation. 43. Methods for testing the sensibility of organs. 44. Testicular pain. 45. Artificial production of visceral pain. Chapter IX. Pages 74—83. Visceral Reflexes. 46. The mechanism of visceral pain. (The viscero-sensory reflex.) 47. The mechanism of the viscero-motor reflex. 48. Multiple reflexes from visceral stimulation. Chapter X. Pages 84 — 96. Diminished Resistance to Stimulation. 49. Focal. 50. General. 51. The soldier's heart. 52. War neurosis. 53. The mechanism of symptoms. 54. Practical importance of recognising the mechanism of symptoms. Chapter XI. Pages 97—103. Organic Reflexes. 55. Vomiting. 56. Dyspnoea. 57. Secretory reflexes. 58. Cardiac reflexes. 59. Vaso-motor and pilo-motor reflexes. Chapter XII. Pages 104—113. Preliminary Examination of the Patient. 60. The patient's appearance. 61, The patienlj^s sensations. 62. Facial aspect. 63. The general condition. 64. A revieW of all the organs. xii. Contents. Chapter XIII. Pages 114—129. Affections of the Digestive Organs. 65. The nerve supply of the digestive tract. 66. Distri- bution of sensory symptoms in affections of the digestive tract. 67. Appetite. 68. Hunger. 69. Nausea. 70. Mouth and fauces. 71. Tongue. 72. Swallowing. 73. (Esophagus. Chapter XIV. Pages 130—154. Affections of the Digestive Organs [continued). The Stomach. 74. The nature of the symptoms. 75. The nerve supply of the stomach. 76. The site of pain in affections of the stomach. 77. The character of the pain. 78. Hyperalgesia. 79. Superficial reflexes. 80. Visceromotor reflexes. 81. Vomiting. 82. Pyrosis and heart-burn. 83, Air suction. 84. Functional symptoms. 85. Structural symptoms. 86. The diagnosis of stomach affections. 87. Pain in gastric ulcer. Chapter XV. Pages 155 — 165. The Liver, Gall-bladder, and Ducts. 88. Nerve supply. 89. Keflex symptoms in gall-stone disease. 90. Gastric symptoms in gaU-stone disease. 91. The result of reflex symptoms. 92. Functional symptoms in gall- stone disease. 93. Structural symptoms in gall-stone disease. 94. Fever in gall-stone disease. 95. Nature of reflex symptoms in affections of the liver. 96. Functional symptoms in affections of the liver. 97. Structural symptoms in affections of the liver. Contents. xiii. Chapter XVI. Pages 166—179. The Great and Small Intestine. 98. Difficulties in diagnosis. 99. Pain. 100. Appendicitis. 101. Affections about the anus and perineum. 102. Perineal reflex. 103. Functional symptoms. 104. Structural symptoms. Chapter XVII. Pages 180—195. Ajjcctions of the Urinary System. 105. Symptoms of affections of the kidney. 106. Symptoms of affections of the pelvis of the kidney and ureter. 107. Symptoms of affections of the bladder. Chapter XVIII. Pages 196—200. Affections of Female Pelvic Organs. 108. The uterus. 109. The ovaries. 110. The vagina. Chapter XIX. Pages 201—206. Peritonitis and Peritoneal Adhesions. 111. Insensitiveness of the peritoneum. 112. Symptoms in peritonitis. 113. Symptoms in peritoneal adhesions (parietal). 114. Symptoms in peritoneal adhesions (visceral). Chapter XX. Pages 207—221. Affections of the Lungs and Pleura. 115. Nature of the subjective sensations. 116. The respira- tion. 117. Reflex symptoms. 118 Functional symptoms. 119. Structural symptoms. 120. Affections of the pleura. 121. Nature of the pain in pleurisy. xiv. Contents. Chapter XXI. Pages 222—255. Affections of the Circulatorij System. 122. Heart failure. 123. The nature of the symptoms in heart failure. 124. Consciousness of the heart's action. 125. Breathlessness. 126. Viscero-sensory and viscero-motor reflexes. 127. The viscero-sensory reflexes in dilatation of the heart and liver. 128. The pain of angina pectoris is a viscero-sensory reflex. 129. Evidences of the viscero-motor reflex. 130. Organic reflexes. 131. Summation of stimuli the cause of angina pectoris. Chapter XXII. Pages 256 — 274. Estimation of the Value of Symptoms. 132. The relation of the symptoms to the general state. 133. Kemote effects of the lesion. 134. Relation of symptoms arising from different causes. 135. The bearing of symptoms on prognosis. 136. The bearing of symptoms on treatment. Index. Pages 276—308. Appendix 309—332. The theory of disturbed reflexes in the production of symptoms of disease, page 309. ( XV. ) LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS. PAGE Fig. I. Diagram of a simple reflex - - . - - 52 Fig. 2. Diagram of the radiation of sensation - - - - 52 Fig. 3. Diagram of motor reflex - - - - - - 54 Fig. 4. Diagram of radiation of sensation in disease - - - 54 Fig. 5. Diagram of the origin and distribution of the efferent auto- nomic nerve fibres ------ 59 Fig. 6. The relation of cutaneous hyperalgesia to enlargement of the liver -----... 67 Fig. 7. The relation of cutaneous hyperalgesia to gastric ulcer - - 75 Fig. 8. Diagram of an organic reflex - - - - - 76 Fig. 9. Diagram of a viscero-sensory reflex - - - - 76 Fig. 10. Diagramof a viscero-motor reflex - - - - 78 Fig. 1L Diagram of a combined viscero-motor and viscero-sensory reflex .----.. 79 Fig. 12. Diagram of multiple reflexes - - - . - 81 Fig. 13. Shows the areas in which pain is felt in affections of the digestive tube - - - - - - 116 Fig. 14. Shows the seat of pain, and the position of the gastric ulcer causing the pain . . - . . 149 xvi. Ldst of Illustrations. PAGE Fig. 15. Shows the seat of pain, and the position of the gastric ulcer causing the pain - - - - - 151 Fig, 16. Shows the seat of pain, and the position of the gastric ulcer' causing the pain - - - - - 153 Fig. 17. Shows the region of cutaneous hyperalgesia after an attack of gall-stone colic - . - . . 157 Fig. 18. Shows the region of cutaneous h%-peralgesia after an attack of renal cohc - - - - - 185 Fig. 19. Shows the region of cutaneous hyperalgesia after an attack of renal colic - - - - - 191 Fig. 20. Shows areas of pain and hyperalgesia in diapliragm- atic pleurisy - - - - - - 219 Fig. 21. Shows areas of cutaneous hyperalgesia in acute dilata- tion of the heart and liver - - - - 238 Fig. 22. Area of pain and hyperalgesia in angina pectoris - - 240 Fig. 23. Area of eruption in a case of herpes zoster affecting the upper thoracic nerves - - - - - - 241 Fig. 24. Shows the area of cutaneous hyperalgesia after the first attack of angina pectoris . - . . 243 Fig. 25. Shows the areas of cutaneous hyperalgesia after a number of attacks of angina pectoris - - - 243 APPENDIX. Figs. 1 to 8 309-332 Chai>ter 1. the importance of symptoms in medical practice and researc^h. 1. The Scieiice of Medicine. 2. The l7nportance of Symptoms in Diagnosis. 3. The hnportance of Symptoms in Prognosis. 4. The Importance of Symptoms in Treatment. 5. The Importajice of Symptoms in Research. 1. The Science of Medicine. — There are many matters in medicine seemingly so simple that it is taken for granted that they are beyond further discussion or investigation. Of these matters, seemingly so simple, the symptoms common to ill-health are the most prominent. Ever since medicine was seriously studied symptoms have received attention, and the notion is firmly held by medical men, specialists, physicians and sur- geons, and experts of all Idnds, that the symptoms revealed by the doctor's unaided senses are so well understood that the information to be gathered from them has been exhausted, and that their further study is not necessary to the progress of medicine. Hence has arisen the belief that, for the progress of clinical medicine new methods are necessary for the further elucidation of symptoms. 2 Chaptei' I. This mistaken attitude towards symptoms is not only misleading, but hampers medicine in Practice and Research. Indeed, the importance of symptoms is so imperfectly realised that a description of the meaning, mechanism and significance of symptoms is nowhere to be found, and this con- stitutes a great defect in medical knowledge. Although this defect in the knowledge of symptoms may be recognised, it is not easy to under- stand how it is to be remedied. This is due to the fact that, notwithstanding the strenuous efforts that have been made to advance medical knowledge, the manner in which medical science should be prose- cuted has never been understood. It has been assumed that investigation in clinical medicine was a simple matter, and that anyone with the usual medical education was fitted to undertake research in medicine. It has never been recognised that for its resea.rch principles and methods are required which are different from those necessary to other sciences, and even from those used in other branches of medical science, while a long training of the investigator is necessary, of a kind rarely undertaken. 2. Diagnosis. — The urgent need for a better knowledge of symptoms can only be realised if the great defects in medical knowledge are recognised. The general practitioners are the people who are brought into contact with the iUnesses which impair the health of the communit3^ An analysis of the complaints which the general practitioner sees reveals the present state of medical knowledge. If we put aside the trivial ailments and consider the illnesses which lower the health of the great majority Symptoms in Medical Practice and Research 3 of people, it is found that only a small percentage (5-10) are capable of being diagnosed with any degree of accuracy. Most of this small percentage are cases of disease so advanced that the organs are damaged beyond repair, such as apoplexy, chronic Bright's disease, gangrene of the feet, advanced heart disease ; and these are the end results of a long period of ill-health, whilst the origin of the ill-health was not detectable. Even such diseases as consumption, and gastric ulcer, are in all probability secondary or superadded diseases — at all events, their diagnosis before gross changes take place cannot be made. The backward state of diagnosis is illustrated in these two latter conditions. They are such common complaints and have been the subject of long and careful investigation by innumerable doctors, yet to-day we cannot detect consumption until the lungs are damaged, usually beyond repair, while, as regards gastric ulcer, Berldey Moynihan, out of his great experience, states that it is disheartening and huniihating to have to confess that at this time we are still often unable to detect this complaint. To appreciate the significance of this lack of knowledge of diagnosis one has to see it in relation to the healtli of a community. In a town of 100,000 inhabitants 25,000 will Ukely consult doctors in one year. 5,000 may suffer from trivial complaints and accidents. Of the 20,000 only a small proportion will suffer from diseases that are diagnosable, on a safe assumption, 2,000. We have then, in a population of 100,000, 18,000 people ailing, of the nature of whose complaint we are still pro- foundly ignorant. 4 Chapter I. When the significance of these figures is grasped it will be seen that there is an urgent need for some method in the investigation of disease, different from that which has been pursued in the past. 3. Prognosis. — Prognosis is the judgment of the significance of symptoms as indicating the future course of the patient's complaint. A knowledge of tliis branch of medicine is absolutely essential to the intelligent practice of medicine, and it concerns every- one who has to deal with the sick. Before any course of treatment is taken it is necessary to under- stand whether the complaint is amenable to treat- ment. The fitness for the patient to do his work, or to live in a given place, all depends on a Iviiowledge of prognosis. It is scarcely necessary to refer to its importance in regard to fife insurance and the examination for military, naval, and government services. A knowledge of prognosis can only be acquired by the detection of symptoms, and the ability to recognise whether these symptoms are the expression of a diseased state or a variation of the normal, indicative neither of disease nor of impairment, is required. When it is recognised that the symptoms are an expression of disease it is necessary to tell whether they represent a damage to the body which impahs its functions, and whether the damage is stationary or progressive. The need for this kind of knowledge is readily visualised if some illustration be given by reference to well-estabhshed practice. ]\Iany eminent and experienced surgeons have seen disastrous results Symptoms in Medical Practice and Be search. 5 from a delay in operating on a}:)pendicitis, so that they strongly recommend that all suspect appendices should be removed. The result of such a procedure is that a great many people are subjected to the operation when there is no disease of the appendix. This is simply due to the fact that medical knowledge has not advanced so far as to interpret correctly the symptoms of appendicitis, nor to understand their significance, so that large numbers are operated wpon unnecessarily. Anti-toxin is believed to be such a potent remedy in diphtheria that, as a matter of routine, every one who contracts this disease is given it. We know that diphtheria in the great majority of cases is not a serious disease, yet because of an absence of knowledge of prognosis, the remedy is given indiscriminately to every one. It will be said, of course, that in the cases of appendicitis and diphtheria it is impossible to fortell which case mil become dangerous, but that is merely stating in another way the fact that medical knowledge has not yet advanced far enough to understand the prognostic significance of certain symptoms. Not only has medical knowledge not advanced so far as to permit a prognosis in such instances, but it has not gone so far as to recognise either the importance of the subject or the manner in which the knowledge can be acquired. A little consider- ation wdll reveal that this knowledge can only come from long experience, yet in matters dealing essentiall}^ with prognosis, such as in life insurance examinations and in recruiting, a medical quahfica- tion is deemed to carry mth it the ability to give 6 Chapter I. a prognosis, and young doctors with little experience are supposed to be capable of doing this kind of work. When the real significance of a prognosis comes to be understood, the attitude of tlie profession to-day will seem amazing. There is no branch of medicine which requires so profound a knowledge of disease and its manifestations, a knowledge that can only come through long experience and pains- taking observation of symptoms. But, so far, the profession have not yet awakened to the great defect in knowledge of this very important subject, and how httle progress is being made in its development. It is now more than 40 years since, as a young graduate I was permitted to examine a lady with a systolic murmur. It had been discovered acci- dentally by a distinguished Edinburgh physician, and he had ordered the patient to bed and prescribed digitaUs, which she was taking in large quantities. She and her husband were warned of the danger of the heart conditions, particularly in regard to pregnancy. For a time a careful hfe was led, but gradually she resumed her old life, and hved for a great many years an energetic hfe. Although the murmur persisted, she is now well over 70 years of age and shows no sign of heart failure. A short time ago I was asked to see a youth, who had been confined to bed for three months because a physician attached to a large teaching hospital had detected a systohc murmur, wliich I had no difficulty in recog- nising as being physiological. This inabihty to recognise the prognostic significance of a murmur Symptoms in Medical Practice and Research. 7 is not exceptional by any means, and I place these two instances in juxtaposition to show how little progress has been made in 40 years in even such a simple matter as this. But one has evidences of this lack of progress everjrwhere, and nowhere more strildngly than in the matter of life insurance. The medical forms in regard to the circulation have scarcely altered, if at all, for over 50 years, as if medical science had not advanced since then. Nor is there any prospect of advance in this important matter till a better knowledge of symptoms is acquired and it is recognised that the onlj^ person who can advance this kind of knowledge is one who has the opportunity of seeing the progress of disease in individual patients, watching them intelligently through complaints from start to finish. 4. Treatment. — Treatment which consists of the introduction into the body of an agent — drug, vaccine, serum, electricity. X-rays, radium emanation — produces reactions, often indistinguishable from the symptoms produced by certain diseases, as in patients with vomiting, diarrhoea, drowsiness, head- ache. Certain of these remedies act by removing a noxious agent, as by vomiting or purging, or by killing it, as mercury in syphilis. But the vast majority of remedies when they have any effect act by modifying the symptoms of disease. This is the justification for treatment — particularly in the case of suffering — to relieve the distressful symptoms. It will thus be seen how important is a Imowledge of symptoms for the inteUigent investigation of drugs or other remedy. The drugs which find a place in the pharmacopoeia have never been studied from 8 Chapter I. this point of view, with the result that a great number of utterly useless drugs are included, while those which are of use have never been studied with that care and accuracy necessary to recognise the real effect of the drug on the diseased human being. Experimental investigation shows how a drug may act on healthy tissues, but drugs are not given to the healthy but to the sick who show symptoms of disease, and it is for the removal of the causes of the symptoms or for their modification the remedy is given. This is Avell illustrated by the use of digitahs. For 150 years the drug was known to have a beneficent effect in heart disease, but no clear conception of the kind of case existed, so that it was given indiscriminately to all patients who had, or were supposed to have, a cardiac affection. Many attempts had been made to find out its effect in the human heart, including experiments on animals, but it was not until the symptoms, particularly the abnormal rhji^hms, w^ere recognised that its effect in the human heart was chscovered, and the kind of case in which it acted beneficially was recognised. The principle which guided to this discovery was the intelligent perception of the symptoms with a knowledge of their mechanism, and then the careful observation of the effect of the drug in producing or modifying these symptoms. Before an intelligent investigation into the action of remedies in the sick human body is undertaken a knowledge of symptoms is necessary. Sy)nptotns in Medical Practice and Research. 9 5. Research. — There is to-day a recognition that medical knowledge is greatly lacking in many essentials, and strenuous endeavours are made for the encouragement and prosecution of research. Where a clear conception of the problem is attainable, an orderly and well-planned investigation may result in a successful issue, as in the investigation of malaria and allied diseases and syphilis. An absence of a clear conception of how research should be pursued leads to a disorderly attack on disease, and a great waste of time and energy is bound to result. The vast majority of diseases which afflict a com- munity in this country have not been clearly recognised, so that any attempt to prevent or cure such diseases is sure to lead to failure. It is therefore manifest, before we can attempt to deal effectively with the more common diseases, a knowledge is first required of the manner in which these diseases affect the human body. While the study of how a noxious agent which produces disease, such as a microbe, may behave in culture media or in animals, may be necessary to the inquiry, yet a knowledge of how the noxious agent reacts upon the human bod}^ is also necessary. As this knowledge can only be acquired by the study of the reactions produced in the body, the importance of symptoms is apparent. A wider view of research must also be taken. As already indicated, diagnosis, prognosis and treatment are essential to the practice of medicine. Research in these subjects is urgently called for. Such research can only be undertaken by those who have the opportunity of seeing individuals in ill- health, so that an investigation of the symptoms of 10 Chapter L disease, carried out systematically and with a precision liitherto unattained, is urgently called for. Investigators are recognising that they have only an experience limited to certain aspects of disease, and they find it necessary to associate themselves in bands or teams for the prosecution of research. Many of these teams include men profoundly informed in their particular branch, but there is one member essential to this work who is invariably absent — one with a knowledge of the symptoms of disease. The need of this type of investigator is recognised, and one with clinical experience is sometimes included in such a team, but medical knowledge has not yet advanced so far as to enable such an investigator to recognise that his knowledge of symptoms is so imperfect that he is unfitted for such work. In the intelligent prosecution of medical re- search, therefore, a knowledge of symptoms is essential. ( 11 ) Chapter II. THE MECHANISM OF A DISEASE PROCESS. 6. Definition of Disease. 1. The Detection of Disease. 8. Definition of Symptoms. 9. Methods for Investigating Symptoms. 10. Principles of Research. 6. Definition of Disease. — Before an attempt is made to investigate any phase of ill-health it is necessary to hold clearly in mind what disease is. Attempts have been made to give a logical definition to the term, but it has resulted too often in so many refinements that in the end confusion exists where clarity is required. Apart from the original meaning of the word as signifying a lack of ease, the term disease, as commonly applied, refers to a distinct condition or entity. When, for instance, a patient suffers from pain in the eyeball and lachrymation, and the conjunctiva is seen to be red and injected, we recognise that he suffers from a disease, or a diseased state. An examination by one doctor may fail to reveal any further facts, and he would call the disease conjunctivitis. Another doctor may recog- nise that the symptoms of photophobia, lachrymation 12 Chapter 11. and injection of the capillaries are phenomena produced by a foreign body acting on certain tissues of the eye, and may detect a speck of coal implanted on the cornea. The removal of the foreign body is followed by the disappearance of the phenomena. Here we have a clear example of v^ hat constitutes disease, and from such an instance disease can be defined as " A state induced by an agent acting injuriously on the tissues." The speck of coal by itself is not a disease, nor is the lachrymation and other signs. When, how- ever, the speck of coal produces these signs the whole sjmdrome (agent and attendant phenomena) can be conveniently grouped under a definite term a disease. When the ailments that affect the human body are carefully analysed it will be found that the vast majority conform to this definition of disease. The noxious agent may be a foreign body, a microbic infection, or chemical in its nature, all of them innocuous structures outside the body, but on their entrance into the body, as soon as thej^ cause a re-action, a state of disease may be said to be l^roduced. In many people who suffer from ill-health the matter is much more complicated, but this is simply due to the fact that with the persistence and progress of the diseased state new reactions are set up until the number is so great that the original disturbance is lost sight of. Nevertheless, fundamentally, the onset of ill-health was provoked in the manner described in the definition. Mechanism of a Disease Process. 13 7. The Detection of Disease. — In the simple illustration of the foreign body in the eye the noxious agent and its effect upon the tissues are readily seen and recognised. Unfortunately the cause of ill-health in the vast majority of cases cannot be so clearly ehcited. The noxious agent is more subtle ; its mode of entrance into the body is undetected and the original seat of disturbance obscure, and as a rule there is no direct evidence of its nature, so that seldom can a diagnosis be made based on its detection. While the agent which provokes the ill-health is therefore not recognisable, the phenomena or the symptoms it produces afford the clue by which it may be detected. These phenomena vary widely, but they depend in the main upon two factors : — (1) The nature of the noxious agent ; (2) The tissue acted on. It is scarcely necessarj^ to elaborate this point. The agent may be, as already stated, a foreign body, a microbe, or chemical in its nature, and it can be under- stood that the action of these different agents on the tissues would vary ; while the tissues acted upon, as fibrous tissue, muscular tissue, nervous tissue, or secretory cells, would give reactions peculiar to their functions. Direct detection of the noxious agent being often impossible, we are driven to seek for it by following uj) the clues afforded by the reaction of the different tissues, which in the human body we recognise as the signs and symptoms of disease. 14 Chapter II. 8. Definition of Symptoms. — Much consideration has been given to the definition of the terms — signs and symptoms of disease. In this book no distinc- tion is made between them. The terms symptoms, signs, manifestations, phenom-ena, are used inter- changeably, and mean a reaction of the tissues of the hody to a noxious agent. 9. Methods for Investigating Symptoms. —Recog- nising that symptoms are the reaction of the tissues of the body to a stimulus by an agent, for a due appreciation of their meaning certain of their features have to be clearly understood. A person in ill-health may present some readily detected sign, as pallor, or suffer from some sensation, as pain; and the custom has been, when the doctor has failed to find the causes of these symptoms, to diagnose the cases as anaemia and nem-algia. So long as such diagnoses satisfy it is manifest no progress can be made. No doubt there is a difficulty in getting beyond this step, and medical science has not yet advanced so far as to recognise the method by which further Imowledge can be acquired. The first step to be taken is to find the mechanism by wliich symptoms are produced. No doubt, many observers have attempted this study, and a limited advance has been m.ade. The state of the blood has been the subject of much inquiry, and a great many facts have been accumulated. Likewise, pain has been the subject of much study, and again many facts have been accumulated, yet the results have led us on but a little way, and are scarcely commensurate A\4th the time and energy spent on them. The reason for this comparative Mechanism of a Disease Process. 15 failure is that we do not yet understand the principles which should guide research in medicine. If we take one of tlie commonest of symptoms, that of pain in disease of the viscera, and consider what information it is capable of yielding were it thoroughly investigated, we will understand some of the steps necessary to be taken for advancing our knowledge of disease. Investigations have shown that the pain in disease of the viscera is referred to some portion of the external body wall, frequently remote from the seat of disturbance. The mechanism by which this is brought about seems to be as follows. A stimulus of a particular Idnd arises in an organ produced by some noxious agent. This stimulus passes by a sympathetic nerve to its cell in the central nervous system. There the stimulus passes from this cell to other cells in its immediate neighbourhood, and these cells, when stimulated, re-act according to their function, a secretory cell modifying the secretion, a muscular cell giving rise to contraction in its muscle, a pain cell producing pain referred to the peripheral distribution of definite nerves in the external body wall. There is thus good reason for assuming that there is a relation, precise and definite, between the viscera and areas of the external body wall, through the nervous system. When this relation is better understood it wiU then be possible to say, when a patient complains of a pain in a definite region, in which organ the disturbance is which produces the pain. The next step Vvdll be to recognise what tissues 16 Chapter II, of the organ are capable of giving rise to pain when stimulated. We do know that certain tissues may be subjecte'd to much injury and destruction mthout pain, while other tissues readily cause pain when stimulated in a particular manner. The last and most important step is to recognise the nature of the stimulus — it may be the noxious agent which is the cause of ill-health. We know tliat not all stimuh apphed to an organ will give rise to pain. For instance, the cutting or tearing or burning of organs may occur and no sensation be elicited. Yet we do know that visceral disease is capable of giving rise to pain of all degrees of severit}^ Manifestly, then, it is only stimuli of a peculiar kind that are capable of producing pain. Inquiry so far has revealed that there are probably but a few kinds of stimuli capable of producing pain, and that these can be chfferentiated in several ways, as by the character and duration of the pain, by the conditions that tend to provoke it, and by the presence of other phenomena, which have been provoked at the same time and by the same stimulus. It will be seen that this line of investigation holds out the expectation that pain and its associated phenomena may not only indicate the (1) site of the organ, (2) the tissues disturbed, but also (3) the nature of the agent producing it. There is now sufficient evidence to show that specific agents on entrance into the body produce specific reactions. This is recognised in the case of the exanthemata, even though the agent has not been actually recognised in all cases. With a better understanding of the mechanism by which sjanptom.s Mechanism of a Disease Process. 17 are produced, and ])y the detection and correlation of associated symptoms, and the careful study of the conditions found post-mortem, or on the exposure of the viscera by operation, combined with bacterio- logical and other laboratory inquiry, the morbid state — provoking agent and reactions— will be eluci- dated. 10. Principles of Research. — The reasons for the foregoing definition of disease and of symptoms is that it helps to render clear the object to be aimed at in any investigation into symptoms. Hitherto much chnical research has been rendered ineffective because the different methods of inquiry have not been clearly visualised, and much time has been spent on the study of the nature of the symptoms and too little on their mechanism. The distinction seems, no doubt, subtle and slight, but it is funda- mental. We see, for instance, that the peculiar features of such symptoms as anaemia, Cheyne- Stokes breathing, anoxemia, hyperchlorhydria, and albuminuria have been studied with meticulous care, but httle has been done to understand the mechanism by which they are produced. In the study of i:)ain the effects of the various drugs that reheve the suffering have been the main object, while the mechanism by which pain is produced has been so imperfectly studied that little real knowledge of this clamant symptom has been found. Yet, as I have said, it is a symptom vv^hich not only calls attention to the presence of disease, but if intelli- gently interpreted would lead directly to the organ affected, and even reveal the nature of the injurious agent producing the ill-health. 18 Chapter II. I frankly confess that I have but a dim idea of the method to be pm^sued in discovering the mechanism of many symptoms, but success has attended the inquiry in regard to some, and the principles which have guided to these discoveries win serve to direct the inquiries in our investigation of the others. These principles are dealt with in Chapter IV. 19 ) CHAPTER Til. THE TRAINING OF THE OBSERVER. 11. Medicine a Science. 12. Accuracy of Observation. 13. Clinical and Laboratory Observation compared. 14. Limitation of Laboratory Methods. 15. The Importance of a Clinical Training. 16. The Requirements of a Clinical Observer. 11. Medicine a Science. — Medicine has not attained that position in science which ought of right to belong to her. Instead of leading in scientific derelopment, and giving guides and indications to allied branches, she is too often content languidly to foUow in their wake, or to pursue some erratic course of her o^vn. The observations made in her name are frequently made more to support some vague specu- lation or far-fetched theory than to reahse the actual condition of the observed phenomena. The sister sciences, in place of seeldng for assistance from medicine, look askance at the wdld speculations put forth in the name of medical science, and at the loose thinldng and play of the imagination which many mecUcal writers deem legitimate in dealing with the phenomena of disease. To emancipate medicine from 20 Chapter III. this position of inferiority, and to secure for it that status which it ought to possess, an effort must be made, as far as possible, to free it from the habih- ments that have hampered it in the past ; and if this appears an unattainable goal at present, its votaries may at least aim at greater precision in thought and in observation. Although this doctrine may seem the commonest of platitudes, and teachers and writers of text-books are unwearied in inculcating it, neverthe- less precision, in thinking and in observation, are among the rarest qualities. 12. Accuracy of Observation. — The power of accurate observation and precise thinking is seldom acquired, because methods have become so stereo- typed that many observers do not realise that they are fettered in the bonds of tradition. Even in the writings of those who claim to be exponents of exact observation and logical reasoning, loose methods of thinking and observing too often appear, even when the scientist imagines himself supreme ; for tradi- tional teaching influences their minds and gives a bias to their deductions. What are called obser- vations are often but a mixture of imperfect observation and unwarranted assumption. While a fact is supposed to be recorded, an opinion is at the same time expressed. As an illustration let us observe how the symptoms of a patient with an enlarged liver are often investigated. The position of the hver having been ascertained, pressure over it is found to elicit pain. The surgeon or physician proceeds to record the "fact" that the "liver is painful on pressure," and such a description is universally accepted as truthful. Yet, if the matter The Training of the Observer. 21 be carefully analysed, the statement will appear not to be a fact at all. Had the statement been that the patient felt pain when pressure was made over the liver, then a plain fact might have been recorded, if the patient's testimony was reliable. But to say that tlie " hver was painful " is to make an assertion that •may or may not be true, but which is not warranted by the evidence, seeing that pressure was being exerted on the sensitive structures of the external body w^all, and no attempt was made to eliminate the possibility of the painful sensation being produced by their stimulation. This tendency to embody assertions, warranted or unwarranted, in the record of a fact is a fault common among medical investi- gators, and impedes the progress of medicine. An inquirer should keep his mind free from bias and ready to review his most cherished beliefs. What is to-day accepted as axiomatic may be shown to- morrow to be but a part of the truth. The tendency to be led bj^ tradition is very powerful, and it is difficult to free the mind from beliefs that have been inculcated with the acquisition of knowledge. In consequence of this, many observations are funda- mentally untrue, and only covered by a veneer of science. There have been many stumbling-blocks in the path of medical progress. They are mainly due to the lack of understanding of the distinctive character of the science of medicine. The knowledge of disease is so incomplete that we do not yet even know what steps should be taken to advance our knowledge. It has been assumed that the methods which are suitable for various branches of science 22 Chapter III. ■ — as chemistry and phj^sics — should be apphed to medical investigation, but the methods of these sciences are not appHcable to the investigation of the most important phenomena of disease. The methods which have helped some sections of medicine — as physiology and bacteriology — are not applicable by themselves to the wider fields of clinical medicine. The present-day conception takes too narrow a, view of the field with which medical science has to deal. It assumes that instrumental methods are of necessity the only scientific methods. It has been assumed that because recording and measuring instruments and other methods have greatly ad- vanced such hmited fields of medicine as physiology, bacteriology, and chemistry, tliat therefore clinical medicine should adopt the same methods. In medicine there are phenomena which the scientific instruments of to-day, however dehcate, can neither register nor measure, and there are methods neces- sary for the investigation of disease which no laboratory experience can supply. As an outcome of this misunderstanding, large fields for investigation which are essential to the progress of medicine have been ignored, methods and principles have been unrecognised or imperfectly apphed, while the appropriate investigator has been neither trained nor encouraged to do the work which he alone has the opportunity to perform. 13. Clinical and Laboratory Observation Com- pared. — It is necessary to grasp fully what is required of a trained chnical observer, because to-day the essential qualities to a great extent have been lost sight of. This has arisen through an imperfect The Training of the Observer. 23 conception of the requirements for clinical observa- tion. Methods that have been found suitable and necessary for other sciences have been introduced, and have been substituted for those that are essential and pecuHar to clinical medicine. Men are now recommended to go into laboratories devoted to one or other of the branches into which medicine is split up, and are trained in methods that are supposed to be more exact and scientific than the clinical methods. This view is the one dominant to-day, so that we find preference given to men trained in tliis way in all matters concerned mth research, and even in the teaching of medicine. This practice is based upon a misunderstanding of what chnical observation requnes and what chnical research means, and it leads directly to incompetence of the laboratory- trained man as a clinical observer and as a chnical investigator. A very brief consideration tv411 render this apparent. The physiological laboratory is assumed to be the most scientific of all our branches, and a training there is supposed to qualify a man for research in medicine. An experiment is performed which produces a reaction which an instrument can record. This record is carefully studied, and certain conclusions — which may be of value — are drawn. From a great many experiments of this kind a large in- crease of valuable knowledge has been obtained. The success and usefulness of this method is undoubted, but its success has blinded people to its hmitations as a method to be apphed in clinical medicine. Each of these experiments produced a great many more reactions than the one recorded, but as these 24 Chajjter III. reactions were not capable of being recorded by an instrument they were ignored. The result is that a laboratory-trained observer cannot recognise any sign except those of a grosser kind, ^\hile the subtler and more elusive signs pass unrecognised. It there- fore happens that the result of an experiment is only partly recognised. The clinical observer is like the physiologist in this respect, that both are searcliing for a reaction to a stimulus. In physiology the stimulus is artificial ; in clinical medicine it is natural, or the result of disease. In both cases a variety of reactions follow the stimulus. The grosser reactions are only noted by the physiologist, and he has no means of detecting the subtler. In clinical medicine the grosser kinds have also to be detected, and the laboratory-trained observer can detect them, but his training has not enabled him to detect the subtler reactions, and hence he fails to attain that skill in observation which is essential to the clinician. 14. Limitation of Laboratory Methods.— The bulk of the most instructive j^henomena produced by disease are incapable of detection by mechanical aids. Many valuable signs are only perceptible to the trained eye or the trained ear or the trained finger. Still more valuable signs are only revealed by the sensations experienced by the patient. To interpret these requires a training that can only be acquired by many years of patient observation, during which the mind is stored by the experiences of the past, by methods which are peculiar to medicine. These methods can never be acquired by a laboratory-trained observer, and it is because of The Trainiyig of the Observer. 25 this that men trained in the laboratory fail as cUnical investigators, however distinguished they may be as physiologists, chemists, or bacteriologists. 15. The Importance of a Clinical Training. — Before we can make progress towards the solution of any problem there are certain preliminary steps to be taken. The first is the perfecting of the instruments to be used. Before a man shaves he first sharpens his razor. Before we undertake an investigation we must see we have the appropriate implements. One implement essential to the success of our enterprise is a trained observer. It is scarcely reahsed what a difference there is between a doctor Avho has S3^stematically trained himself to observe, and another who has perfunctorily examined his patients witliout attempting to improve his powers of observation. This can be shown in a simple matter like the feeling of the pulse. Many experi- enced doctors fail to detect irregularities except when very marked. Some physicians will recognise every beat in an irregular pulse ; others will fail to detect a large proportion of the beats. Indeed, so common is the inability to count the beats in certain cases that I view many observations with a good deal of suspicion. Certain steps are necessary to train an observer so that he can acquire the abihty to detect the peculiarities of the pulse, and to recognise their significance. He must, for instance, have seen a large number of cases, and studied them with great care, so as to correlate the sensations of his fingers with the result of observations made by his other senses. Thus he must correlate the pulse peculiarities with the sounds of the heart, with the movements 26 Chapter III. of the apex and of the jugular veins, and Avith the character of the sphygmogram. The significance of the pulse can only be recognised by watching the patient for long periods to ascertain what happens to him, and the variations that take place in the pulse and in the other symptoms as the case pro- gresses have to be observed. The same methods are necessary to train the other senses. A glance at the face will often reveal a great amount of information to the trained observer. Consider the years of study and observa- tion that have been necessary to acquire that know- ledge — a kind of knowledge essential to medical investigation, and impossible to acquire by the use of instruments however scientific. It is now well recognised that the symptoms provoked in the early stages of disease are mainly subjective. There are a number of these sensations, and it is evident that, if the early stages of disease are ever to be recognised, the nature and significance of these sensations w\\\ have to be understood. There is, unfortunately, a widespread belief that all the information that the patients can jield is easily acquired, and it is generally supjDOsed that the information is often so misleading as to be of little value. Moreover, the behef has obtained tha.t abnormal signs revealed by an examination of the patient, especially by the use of an instrument, are of much greater importance, so that practically all the instruction at the schools is devoted to the study of physical signs. This is a great mista.ke, and is the chief reason why the knowledge of the early stages of disease is so defective. The Training of the Observer. 27 This aspect of medicine is brought out if the sensations which indicate the onset of disease be considered. Most patients, when they fall into ill- health, become conscious of it by the fact that they are easily exhausted — exertion which they used to undertake with ease and comfort now renders them tired. The questions that arise are : " What is exhaustion ? " and " what is the mechanism of its production ? " Put in that way it will be recognised at once how little we know of this important symp- tom. An inquiry which I have been making for a number of years has led to a limited knowledge, and I can recognise that this sensation can arise from a number of causes, and where the condition of the patient is carefully investigated it will be found that this sensation of exhaustion can be divided into a number of different kinds. The same careful training is necessary for the investigation of the most clamant of all symptoms, that of pain. To understand the full significance of pain in any case we have to know a great many matters which are still hidden from us. The tissues capable of producing pain, the nerves in whose cUstribution the pain is felt, the manner in which the pain spreads, and the laws governing the spread of pain ; the character of the pain itself ; the manner of its onset and its variations, and the phenomena with which it is associated, are all matters which it is necessary to understand before we are qualified to undertake an investigation into disease. So with all other sensations. It is manifest that before a patient's sensations are understood the doctor must have a knowledge of 28 Chaj>ter III. the mechanism of their production and of their significance. As a rule, the patient is merely concerned witli detailing the sensation which troubles him most. It rests with the doctor to obtain hy means of judicious questions the particulars of the different sensations. But the doctor cannot ask the proper questions unless he has sufficient experi- ence and a knowledge of the nature of the sensations. It will be gathered from this that the phj'sician who would undertake the investigation of the early stages of disease must not only be a man of very wdde experience but must have trained himself to observe on lines that have hitherto received little attention. The training, amongst other things, must have included the watching of patients for long periods to see the outcome of the complaint. If this is grasped it will be understood how vain it is to expect the early stages to be revealed in hospitals, where the custom is to hand the out-patient depart- ment over to the junior physician, who lacks that experience which should make him a competent examiner. I have for many years been calling attention to this error in education and showing how it hampers practice and research. 16. The Requirements of a Clinical Observer. — To qualify a man to be a skilled investigator in bacteriology, in physiology, and in chemistry, many years of special training are necessary. If it be realised that before a man is qualified to undertake, on the lines laid down, an investigation for the prevention and cure of disease — ^the real object of medical research — he must have a knowledge of symptoms, it will be seen that a training is required The Trauiimj of the Observer. 29 which is bound to take a great many years. It is curious that men see tlie necessity for this in bacteriological, physiological, and chemical research, and will undergo tlie training, but so far the necessity has not been recognised for such a training before undertaking research in clinical medicine. It is necessar}^ to recognise that a competent observer must ever be learning. When face to face with patients and unable to detect the nature of their ill-heeJth, he must not say that tlie disease is not capable of recognition, but rather sa}^ that the signs of disease are there, but he is incapable of detecting or understanding them. This is a humilia- ting confession, but a salutary one, for the recog- nition of a lack of knowledge is the first step to making that defect good. ( 30 ) CHAPTER IV. PRINCIPLES OF INVESTIGATION. 17. The Need for Principles. 18. Differentiation. 19. Classification. 20. The Laiv of Progressio7i. 21. The Law of Associated Phenomejia. 22. The Significance of Symptoms {Prognosis). 23. The Use of Laboratory Methods. 24. The Expectation of Besvlts. 17. The Need for Principles. — An investigation to be sj^stematic and orderl}" naust be guided by principles clearly denned. There are certain simple principles wliicli are useful in making observations on disease. These principles are but provisional, but they may be a guide until the knowledge they reveal is exhausted, b}- which time an insight into research and what is wanted will be gained, and these principles can either be added to or supplanted. It must be recognised that in this work all are but learners, and while the steps taken to-day may seem very important and the discoveries may bulk largely in our visions, as time goes on their place in the perspective may be very small. First steps are alwaj^s feeble and uncertain, but they are a necessarj^ prelude to the vigour of full achievement. Principles of Investigation. 31 These principles have no doubt been in use during the whole history of medicine, only they have been so imperfectly appreciated and applied that their significance has been overlooked. These principles are : — First, the clear differentiation of symptoms ; Seco7id, the classification of symptoms ; Third, the employment of the recognition of a new factiis a foothold for further advance (the law of progression) ; and Fourth, the search for other symptoms (the law of associated phenomena). 18. — Differentiation. — Having detected a symp- tom, whether subjective or objective, it is necessar}' to separate it clearly from all others that it resembles. This proposition is so self-evident that it seems unnecessary to dwell upon it, yet its significance has not been appreciated. The importance of this step was forced upon me many years ago when I began an inquiry into the significance of irregular heart action. I had not gone far into the study of the subject before I recognised that there were different kinds of irregularity, and though others had also, no doubt, recognised this, no one had attempted to differentiate them with any degree of acciurac}^ on a rational basis. It is manifest that no progress in the knowledge of the meaning and significance of heart irregularities could be made until each form was clearly difterentiated. This inquiry I undertook, and was able to found a differentiation based upon the mechanism of their production. 32 Chapter I V. But in this inquiry I found that differentiation means more than the mere recognition of the mechanism of production. A differentiation based on the mechanism rarely leads to a recognition of the significance of a symptom. We know, for instance, that physicians have for long differentiated the murmurs of the heart on the basis of the mechanism of their production, but not knowing how to carry the inquiry further they left the matter there, and a misunderstanding of the significance of the mur- murs has resulted. We know to-day how widespread is the misinterpretation of the significance of mur- murs, and what injury has been and is being done to the individual patients and to the progress of medicine because of this limited differentiation. ^Medicine calls imperatively for a further differen- tiation — one based upon the significance of the sign in its relation to the progress of the disease that produces it. A murmur may be differentiated according to the valve orifice at which it arises, but it is necessary that it should also be differentiated by the effect its cause has upon the functional efficiency of the heart. This will at once be seen to be of the first importance, not only in the practice of medicine but also in the pursuit of all kinds of research in which the heart is concerned. Some y^ars after I had begun the investigation of irregular heart action, other investigators took the matter up and helped greatly to determine the mechanism by which they were produced ; but I was struck by the fact that they all stopped there, and practically no one but myself undertook the far m.ore difficult task of differentiating them so as to Principles of Investigation. 33 determine their significance. This could only be done by applying certain principles of investigation which are essential to medical research. These will be described later. It is necessary to insist that we should always keep in mind that not only have we to detect the symptoms of disease, but we must differentiate them clearly on two principles — one on the basis of the mechanism of their production, the other on the bearing they have on the patient's future. 19. Classification. — The accumulation of symp- toms is so bewildering in their numbers and complexity that it seems impossible to obtain a clear and simple comprehension of their significance. So long as what is called research is but the addition of new symptoms and of new methods for their detection, it can safely be said that little progress will be made in our knowledge of the fundamental principles of research. A classification based upon Nature's laws tends towards simplicity and a fuller understanding. Such a classification I have attempted, and although T am far from stating that it fulfils all the requirements, it is nevertheless of distinct practical use, and as it is based upon natural laws it leads us to a clearer understanding of symptoms. Later I give a classifica- tion of symptoms showing they can be divided into three groups, according to the mechanism of their production, namely : 1. A Structural group. 2. A Functional group. 3. A Reflex group. 34 Chapter I V. I pointed out, in dealing with differentiation, that, in addition to a differentiation based on the mechanism of a symptom, another sort of differentiation is required showing the significance of the symptom. For instance, when we recognise a sign due to a structural alteration, whether it be a change in the size or shape of the organ or a modified sound of the heart due to a deformed valve, it should be recognised that the knowledge obtained from the mere recognition is extremely hmited. What is required to know is — what effect has the cause of the sign upon the patient's future ? This question, which should be apphed to the con- sideration of every symptom, cannot be answered by the study of the symptom alone, we must look for other signs, being guided in our search by the natural question, whether the functional efficiency of the organ is affected. Thus we are guided to seek for evidence of the second group of symptoms. There are a great many diseases in which we fail to detect any structural sign or any functional impair- ment, yet we can recognise the disease with great accuracy. Most cases of gastric ulcer are recognised by such signs as pain, tenderness of the skin of the epigastrium, and hardness of the upper part of the recti muscles. There may be no structural sign nor sign of functional disturbance. Such symptoms are produced by an irritation of a hmited portion of the central nervous system in a reflex manner, the source of irritation being in the ulcer. While these groups form the basis of a classification, there are combinations of symptoms Principles of Investigatio7i. 35 which give occasion for further grouping and subdivision, which will be discussed in Chapter V. 20. The Law of Progression. — For many years no advance has been made in the use of many methods, such as the thermometer or the stetho- scope. These and manj^ other instruments are of the greatest use in clinical medicine, but it has been assumed that the limits of their usefulness have been reached. As a matter of fact, the chnical significance of the information which they yield has only been partially understood. This restricted use has arisen because the laws governing research have not been understood. There has been a desire to improve a method, but it has not been recognised how it could be done. This has usually taken the form of modif3dng the instrument, as in the different forms of stethoscope that have been evolved. No doubt much benefit has resulted by perfecting the X-ray methods and laborator}^ methods generally, but the progress has rarefy been commensurate with the time and trouble spent, because it has not been guided by an understanding of the principles of chnical investigation. The discovery of a new fact or a new method must not be the end of the inquiry which has revealed it. Rather must it be looked upon as a means to an end, a stepping-stone to help a further advance. Medicine has failed full}^ to appreciate this aspect, cliiefiy because it was not understood how progress should be made. If the path had been clearly indicated, a forward movement D 2 36 Chapter I V. would have been made in many instances where our knowledge has stood still, as in the signs discovered by the stethoscope. The chief causes that have hampered progress are the failure to recognise the necessity for under- standing the prognostic significance of symptoms, and the lack of understanding of how this knowledge can be acquired. 21. The Law of Associated Phenomena. — One principle of supreme importance for the advance of medicine is the law of associated phenomena. This law is based on the fact that ill-health is aiwa3's accompanied by a number of srmptoms, and in every case it is incumbent upon us to search for other symptoms besides those which are most prominent. When, for instance, we detect a structiural symptom, we must recognise the hmited knowledge it reveals, and we must extend our observation and seek for signs of functional derangement. If we detect a sign and recognise it as the product of reflex stimulation, we are at once given a suggestion for further inquiry, and we must search for other signs wliich will lead us to the area of stimulation. The discovery of this area will lead us to the organ at fault, and it ma}^ be to the nature of the condition that provokes the reflex, and thus we get nearer the actual disease. A ver}^ little study will soon bring conviction of the necessity for constantly keeping this law in mind. From the simplest complaint, a headache or a cough, to the most obvious phj^sical sign indicating gross changes, the due appreciation of the case will depend on the application of this law. Principles of Investigation. 37 22. The Significance of Symptoms (Prog- nosis).— The law of associated plienomena is necessary to the understanding of another main objective. It is necessary to find out the effect the cause of a s^^mptom has on the patient's future — that is, prognosis, a part of medical science which lies at the foundation of the in- telligent practice of medicine. To do this, contact must be kept with the patient during the remainder of his life or for the duration of his ill-health, and in doing this a look-out must be kept for the modification of the symptom and the development of new S3'mptoms — that is, the detection of all associated phenomena. It is only by this method that the early stages of disease can be detected. At first we may have no true conception of the cause of a symptom we may detect in an individual, but in course of time, as the disease develops, we may be able to recognise it. B}^ referring back to our notes we will see how the s\mptoms were developed, and thus acquire a knowledge of the early stages. ^y finding similar symptoms in other patients later we ma}" be able to detect the disease earlier, and so back to the beginning. Naturally in this way the prognostic significance will also be revealed. 23. The Use of Laboratory Methods.— The principle which should guide clinical investigation is first to recognise and understand the symptoms of disease, then endeavour to find out the agents producing the disease. Many of the symptoms will not be recognisable by the unaided senses, so in applying the law of associated phenomena 38 Chapter I V. mechanical and other laboratory methods should be employed such as are found in a chemical laboratory and an X-ray department. So many diseases arise from bacterial invasion that no method of research into the cause of disease would be complete without a skilled bacteriologist. An endeavour should be made to find out the nature of the symptoms provoked by the different bacteria. We recognise by the clinical symptoms the occurrence of a great many infectious diseases, where the specific microbe has been discovered, as typhoid fever and pneu- monia. The recognition of other diseases is dependent entirely on the clinical symptoms, as measles and scarlet fever. We infer that the symptoms produced by other microbes may give rise to specific symptoms, and we can use this idea as a guide in one field of research. It is manifest that bacteriology must be greatly hampered until the symptoms of invasion are related to the infective agent causing the ill- health. 24. The Expectation of Results. — True clinical investigation holds out no expectation of results in the immediate future, so far as achieving the chief aim in medicine — the prevention and cure of disease. A long and weary road has to be travelled by those who would make clinical medicine an object for research. The training alone requires many years of patient toil, and the practice of setting young men fresh from the schools to undertake research in chnical medicine shows how little the subject is understood. Principles of Investigation, 39 Such a contemplation of clinical research seems discouraging, for we all like to see the fruits of our endeavours. But it must be borne in mind that all great enterprises are based on work that has been done by individuals whose part is lost in obUvion. Someone has to do the obscure but necessary work of digging a foundation, and whoever undertakes this kind of work must be content with the knowledge that he is playing a necessary part in a great enterprise. 40 Chapter V. CLASSIFICATION OF SYMPTOMS. 25. The Anatomical Classification of Disease. 26. The Clinical Classification of Symptoms. 27. Structural Symptoms. 28. Functional Symptoms. 25. The Anatomical Classification of Disease. It is a matter of knowledge common to every general practitioner that an explanation of the complaints of a large majority of his patients cannot be found by reference to any textbook. The cases he can recognise according to textbook descriptions are mainly those where the disease has reached an advanced stage, as in dropsy from heart disease, and consumption after the lungs break down, or when some terminal affection such as apoplexy occurs. In consequence the doctor is often at a loss what name to give the complaints from which his patients suffer, and he has to resort to the substitution of symptoms in place of the real cause, so that we get names such as neurasthenia, gastralgia, hyperchlorhj^dria, angina pectoris, tachycardia, albuminuria, which convey no definite information. The reason for this is that the classification of diseases is not based Classification of Symptoms. 41 upon a true knowledge of disease. It is tlie out- come of the time when pathology was dominant. With the introduction of accurate methods of observation, the nature of the diseases found after death was clearly demonstrated, and a classification of the different diseases that affected the different organs was made. This classification was so precise and definite, and seemingly so accurate and scientific, that it was adopted not only b}^ the pathologist but by the clinician. The latter used this classification as a guide to search for and explain the physical signs which he detected in the living patient, and thus this classification became the standard for clinical medicine. For a long time this seemed quite satisfactory, and was of value, but time has shown that for the practice of medicine it is not only faulty but misleading, in that it diverts attention from the real causes of disease and from the more important sjmiptoms — those that are not included in w^hat are called physical signs. The need for a presentation of disease wiiich describes the phenomena in the living human being is urgently called for, and an endeavour is made in the following pages to meet this need. I have already pointed out that there has been no orderly arrangement of the large mass of symptoms which have been recognised. So long as facts are accumulated with no orderly arrange- ment the progress of medicine will be hampered and matters essential to its progress will be 42 Chapter V. obscured. I have therefore attempted a classifica- tion Avhich is simple, yet helps greatly in under- standing the nature of symptoms. This classifica- tion of S3^mptoms is based upon the mechanism of their production. 26. The Clinical Classification. — In searching for a basis I have adopted one which in my present state of knowledge fulfils certain essential requirements, inasmuch as it is based upon natural laws, and gives at the same time information of a kind that is necessary in practical medicine, while its application guides one to an efficient examination of the patient. ,,To understand on what basis a classification of disease should be made we have to consider the manner in which symptoms are produced. Dis- ease, where it impairs the health of the bodj^, is due to a noxious agent producing a variety of pheno- mena or symptoms. Some of these may result from the agent, causing a structural change in the tissues, producing what is called a physical sign of disease. In every case of impaired health, the agent acts, directty or indirectly, upon the organs, causing a disturbance in their function. Such disturbances may be more evident than the structural signs or the agent of disease, and because of their prominence the}^ ma}^ be mistaken for the disease itself. Amongst these disturbances of organs there is one class so distinctive and peculiar in its mechanism that it can be separated into a group by itseK, particularly as its function is in many cases protective. The symptoms of this class arise from stimulation of the central nervous Classification of Symptoms. 43 system, and their appearance is often the first to direct attention to the fact that the individual is ill, and they also indicate the source of the trouble. Taking the mechanism of their production as a basis, symptoms can thus be classified in three groups : (1) Structural sj^mptoms. (2) Functional symptoms. (3) Reflex symptoms. 27. Structural Symptoms. — Before we can detect a change in an organ we must have a knowledge of its position, its shape, and size, and other characteristics as revealed in the healthy body. A deviation from what we recog- nise as its appearance in health is called a physical sign, and phj^sical diagnosis is the term applied to the detection of disease by the presence of these signs. It is necessary to hold clearly in mind what is revealed when we detect a physical sign, either by the unaided senses, or by means of the many mechanical means employed. In the main, a phj^sical sign is due to an alteration in the structure of the organs; though in this class is included also such signs as the modification in the sounds and movements of the heart, and altered sensations. The detection of a phj'sical sign gives httle information beyond the fact that a change has taken place in an organ. There may, from experience, arise a knowledge that certain signs are associated with conditions of a definite nature bearing upon the health of the patient, as an enlarged heart is often associated 44 Chapter V. with heart failure, or a mahgnant tumour is accompanied bj^ ill-health ; but, properly speaking, the signs of heart failure and impaired health are not revealed by the physical sign. It might seem to be a needless refinement to insist upon such a distinction, but for the purpose of keeping clearly in mind the limitations of knowledge revealed by a j)hysical sign, such a distinction is necessar}^, for we find that in practice the detection of a physical sign is often thought to convey information far beyond what it actually reveals. Thus for the last 100 3^ears the detection of a murmur in the examination of the heart has led to the assumption that the heart was seriously affected, and we see to-day how this view misleads the profession. This failure to appreciate the significance of a physical sign is found in connection with most diseases. The detection of an impaired percussion note, or of a shadow in the lungs revealed by the X-raj's, is judged to be sufficient evidence for a prolonged course of treatment. Totally unnecessary fears are frequently raised by the findings in an X-ray examination, or b}^ other instrumental methods. The limited knowledge and the peculiar kind of knowledge revealed by a physical sign must always be kept in view, as the neglect of this misleads, particularly in the use of mechanical devices in the detection of symptoms, as I shall point out later. Another matter to be kept in view is that a physical sign, due to some structural alteration in an organ, may be the result of a functional disturbance of another or2;an, as in Classification of Sym'ptoms. 45 the e3-e signs and circulatory disturbances in disease of the th^Toid gland. 28. Functional Symptoms. — The essential matter in the maintenance of health is the functional efficiency of the organs of the body. A structural modification may take place and leave the efficienc}^ unimpaired. Thus it arises that after the detection of any physical sign, a careful inquiry must be made into the efficiency of the affected organ. The evidences of function are not always easy to detect, but it may be taken for granted that the disease of any organ will never be properly recognised until the function of the organ, and the part it plays in maintaining the health of tlie body, is understood. While a certain amount of knowledge may be obtained by studj-ing the activity of the organ during health, a far better understanding will be obtained by studying the modifications of functions. These modifications can onty be recognised b}^ detecting the manifestations they produce on the body — i.e.y functional symptoms. Functional symptoms are rarely detected from direct evidence in the organ affected, but rather from the effects produced on other organs. This is seen in its simplest form in certain affections of the thj'roid gland. The structural signs give no indication as to the serious or simple nature of the affection ; this is only found out by the effect of the perverted activity of the gland upon the heart, eye, and nervous system, and in the increased oxidation of the tissues. Diminished functional 46 Chapter V. activity, as in myxoedema, is shown also only by its effects on remote organs and tissues. Even in an organ like the heart, whose con- chtion and activity can be so easily studied, the information essential to a knowledge of its con- dition can only be made out by the reaction on other organs. The study of its various mani- festations gives no idea of its functional efficiency, and this knowledge can only be acquired by observing how the circulation is maintained in other organs, as shown by dropsy or enlarged liver due to heart failure. Even in the early stages of heart weakness, the essential signs are brought about by a failure of the heart to supply sufficient blood to remote organs. In affections of tlie kidney, the symptoms essential to a knowledge of the state of the kidney are not elucidated by an examination of the kidney, and even the elaborate chemical and microscopic examination of the urine fails to vield information so important as the presence or absence of changes in the heart and blood vessels. It will be seen that not only do organs remote from the original disease exhibit the essential symptoms produced by depraved function, but organs thus affected in this secondary manner may show such signs from this cause that they form the principal feature in the picture of impaired health. Indeed, it will be found that a great many diseases, which are described as independent and distinct, are but the reaction to the depraved function of another organ. This is best seen in Classification of Stjmptoms. 47 the response of the heart. Probably the depraved function of every organ reacts upon the heart, either through the nervous mechanism of the heart, or from its nutrition being impaired, as in anaemia. The importance of recognising this class of symptoms as distinct is because it gives a line of in- vestigation in each case. Already we know of man}^ symptoms produced by different organs wliich are associated in their appearance, and we recognise certain groups of sj^mptoms as being due to the altered or impaired function of an organ, so that the appear- ance of one of a group leads to a search for others. Moreover, the increase in our knowledge of the mechanism of the individual symptom will inevitably lead to a better understanding of the organ whose depraved function is the cause of the disturbance, and thus contribute largely to that very necessary branch of knowledge — the pathology of function. It might seem umiecessary to point out how Important this view is from the standpoint of treatment. It is manifest that if such an organ as the heart is disturbed by the depraved functions of other organs, any treatment which is devoted to the heart wiU be useless. Yet this view needs emphasising, for to mj^ knowledge, large numbers of individuals are submitted to prolonged treatment for cardiac symptoms in which the organ is only secondarih^ affected. Indeed, so important is tliis aspect of the matter that the question should arise in connection with every disease, whether the symptoms are not manifestations of a disease provoked by some other organ. The importance of this point of view will be more fulh' considered later. ( 48 ) Chapter VI. REFLEX SYMPTOMS. 29. The Discovery of the Seat of Disturbance. 30. The Discovery of the Agent causing ill-heaWh 31. The Discovery of the Function of the Nervous System. 32. Methods of Examination. 33. Mechanism of an Ordinary Stimulation. 34. Mechanism of the Radiation of Sensation. 35. Mechanism of a Motor Reflex. 36. Mechanism of Radiation in Disease. 29. The Discovery of the Seat of Disturbance. — I have already remarked that much vahiable time is often spent in research work which fails to achieve results at all commensurate with the labour devoted to it. One half of the battle in all research work consists in having a definite object, with a clear conception of how the object is to be achieved. The study of reflex symptoms leads directh^ to the organ at fault, and may discover the agent causing the ill-health. The inquiry into the nature of reflex symptoms has not only helped in the discovery of facts of great value, but has revealed the methods by which other objects essential to the progress of medicine may be pursued. It has demonstrated, Reflex Symptoms. 49 for instance, that there is an intimate relation between the organs and definite areas of the skin, or rather between the organs and the distribution of the sensory nerves in those areas, and between the organs and parts of the skeletal muscular system. A knowledge of this relation at once reveals which organ is at fault. When we detect sensory dis- turbances — pain or hyperalgesia — in these parts, or when we find a persistent contraction of the muscles, we are able to relate these to a definite structure within the body. In this way we seek to link up the organ with the disturbed region in the external body wall. This can be accomplished by the careful examination of the disturbed regions and correlating them with the disease discovered on the operation or j)ost-morte7n tables. 30. The Discovery of the Agent causing Ill- health. — In addition to the discovery of the organ at fault, we have the opportunity of discovering another very important matter — namely, the nature of the stimulus which is capable of producing these disturbances in the external body wall. It requires a stimulus of a peculiar kind to produce these reactions. 8ome disease processes produce one form of reaction and others produce other reactions. The careful noting of the different reactions and their correlation with the disease process will reveal the nature of the stimulus which may be acting, and so we get, at a very early stage, from the symptoms produced in the external body wall, a sure indication not only of the organ at fault but of the kind of disease present in the organ. 50 Chapter VI. 31. The Discovery of the Functions of the Nervous System.— The recognition of the phenomena produced by reflex stimulation hj disease will be found to open new fields of investigation in the central nervous system. The radiation of stimula- tion, for instance, shows there are paths in the central nervous system hitherto unrecognised. The peculiar fields of cutaneous hyperalgesia and of the radiation of pain in diseases of such organs as the heart, gall bladder, ureter, and bowel, shows that the stimulus entering the central nervous system pursues a very definite course. This is also seen in the area of cutaneous hyperalgesia in renal colic, as represented in Fig. 18, page 185. In addition, it is only by the study of these symptoms that a full knowledge of the afferent system of the nerves belonging to the involuntary nervous system can be acquired. 32. Methods of Examination. — As we know, ill- health leads to the production of a great variety of symptoms, and we also know that some simple cause is capable of producing these symptoms. The trained investigator may detect the cause in a variety of ways^by the intelhgent questioning of the patients as to the onset of their ill-health and t.heir sensations, by the recognition of the peculiar character of the symptoms and in the way they are grouped. Certain toxins give rise to reactions pecuhar to them, and we may recognise the diseases they represent by the grouping of the symptoms. These reactions are shown not onJy by the pecuhar association of phenomena but also by specific effects upon individual organs. The recent observations Be flex Symptoms. 51 of Marris and R. M. Wilson, for instance, demonstrate that certain microbes produce toxins that have a peculiar effect upon the heart. By such means there is every reason to expect that we will recognise that groups of symptoms are indications of and peculiar to certain diseased states, although at present our outlook is cdn;fused by an inabihtj^ to distiaguish these groups. It will thus be seen how incumbent it is to note carefulh^ each individual symptom, to watch the development of symptoms by keeping in touch with the patients, and applying the prin- ciples which are the basis of any scheme for the investigation of disease. 33. Mechanism of an Ordinary Stimulation. — Before discussing the nature of these reflex symptoms it is necessary to appreciate the mechanism by which many symptoms are produced. When a common sensation arises in any part of the body, no matter how it is produced, it is always accompanied by a sense of locality. This sense of locality may be precise and definite, or it may be vague and diffuse, but it has a reference to some particular part of the body. Fig. 1 represents diagrammatic ally the simplest form of sensation with its attendant locaUzation. It represents a portion of skin (a) which is stimulated and from which a sensation passes into the central nervous system and reaches the sensorium (s), where the sensation is not only perceived, but the part of the body recognised to which the stimu- lated nerves are distributed. 34. Mechanism of the Radiation of Sensation.— The place in which the sensation is felt is not neces- sarily the place which received the stimulus. In most 52 Chapter VI. instances a local stimulus applied to the external body wall gives rise to a sensation wliose localization by the sensorium corresponds to the part stimulated. It often happens that the area in which the sensation is felt is greater than the part stimulated, and it is then difficult to explain the radiation. An experi- ence related by Professor Sherrington supplies a Fig. 2. Fig. 1. A stimulus applied to the skin, a, is not only recognised by the sensorium, S, but is referred to a definite area, a. Fig. 2. Illustrates tlie radiation of sensation. A stim\ilus applied at a is felt not only at a, but in a part of the skin,B, at some distance. This is repre- sented as taking place in some part of the central nervous system where the cells a' and b' lie in close proximity. fitting explanation. The application of a mustard plaster to his chest over the region of the upper part of the sternum gave rise to an unpleasant sensation on the inner side of each arm just above the internal cond3des. It is known that the nerves supplying the skin of the upper part of the chest and the skin on the Reflex Symptoms. 53 inner side of the elbow arise from cells situated close together in the spinal cord, and leave the cord by the second thoracic nerve — one branch of this nerve going to the chest and the other to the arm. The manner in which the radiation of the sensation takes place is shown diagrammatically in Fig. 2. The stimulus conveyed from a enters the cell a' in coimexion with the nerve fibre, and not onh' gives rise to a sensation referred to a, the part stimulated, but affects the cell b' in its immediate neighbourhood. The sensorium recognises the ex- tended stimulation, and refers the resulting sensation to that portion of the skin, b, suppUed by the nerve b'. There are many other instances of this kind of referred sensation — for example, the well known instance of the knee pain in hip-joint disease. From these facts another principle can be deduced — namety, that if any part of the nervous system which conveys the sensation of pain from the skin to the sensorium be stimulated, the resultant sensation is referred not to the part stimulated but to the distribution of the nerve at its periphery. Thus, when the cell b' was stimulated, the brain became conscious of the stimulation, but the sensa- tion was felt in the skin at b. 35. Mechanism of a Motor Reflex. — A stimulus applied to the sole of the foot ma}^ give rise to a sensation of touch, or tickling, or pain, and at the same time may produce a contraction of certain muscles. It is not necessary to infer that it requires a special peripheral end- organ and a separate nerve fibre to receive and convey the stimulus to the sensorium, and another to convey 54 Chapter VI. the stimulus to the motor centre in the spinal cord. A simple diagram (Fig. 3) shows how this comes about. The stimulus appHed to the skin, a, passes into the cell a', and from this cell one stimulus proceeds to the sensorium, and another to the motor cell c\ causing a contraction of the muscle, c. Ftg. 3. Fig. 4. Fig. 3. Illustrates the mechanism of a motor reflex. The stimulus to the skia A produces not only a sensation and its locaUzation, but the stimulus passing through a' affects the cell c' and produces a contraction of the muscle c. Fig. 4. Illustrates the radiation of pain set up by disease. The diseased tooth A causes pain not only in the neighbovu-ing tooth, b, but also in the skin of the cheek, c. This reflex stimulation may affect many other centres, some of them easily recognisable, others so elusive that their presence can only be inferred. Later on, when deahng with the reaction produced by visceral disease, it will be shown that a stimulus arising at the periphery may provoke a variety of demonstrable reactions differing widely in character. Reflex Symptoms. 55 36. — ^Mechanism of Radiation in Disease. — These simple illustrations give a clue to the manner in wliich symptoms are produced bj^ disease reacting on the central nervous system. This is shown in the simplest way when a demonstrable disease gives rise to symptoms that are easily recognised. Most of us know from personal experience what toothache is, and we have often the opportunity of stud^'ing its symptomatology on our patients, if not on ourselves. It frequently happens that the pain set up b}' a diseased tooth is not hmited to the offending tooth, but is felt along the jaw, in the other teeth, and sometimes in the cheek. Indeed, the sound teeth may be so painful, on pressure being applied to them, that the dentist may pull a sound tooth in place of the diseased one. Pain may not only be felt in the cheek, the skin may be so hypersensitive that on brushing the hair pain is produced in place of the normal sensation of touch. It is to be noted that the spreading of the pain and tenderness in such an instance is not due to the extension of any morbid condition at the peripherj^ — inflammation, for instance — to the neighbouring teeth, for there is no sign of an}i:hing the matter with those teeth, while the skin of the cheek is demonstrabl}^ free from any diseased condition. Moreover, the discovery and removal of the diseased tooth is followed b}^ the complete disappearance of all pain and tenderness from the other teeth and from the skin of the cheek. Seeing tliat these phenomena are produced by one diseased tooth, and seeing that the pain and tenderness of the sound teeth' and of the skin of the 56 CJmpter V I. cheek is not due to any connection with the offending tooth at the periphery, we are driven to look else- where for some connection between these widely separated parts. As there is no communication between nerves except at the cells in the central nervous system from which they arise, we seek for a relation between the nerves of these teeth an(^ the skin of the cheek in the central nervous system. In the case of the diseased tooth the pain was felt not only in the diseased tooth or its immediate neighbourhood, but also in other teeth and in the skin of the cheek, and tliis radiation is explained bj^ a reference to the diagram Fig. 4, where it is shown that a stimulus arising from the diseased tooth a sends a stimulus into the central nervous system affecting the cell a' belonging to the nerve of the diseased tooth, so that the sensorium refers the resultant pain to the diseased tooth ; but the stimulus also spreads from the cell a' to the cells b' and &, and the resultant pain is referred by the sensation to the healthy tooth b and the health}^ skin of the cheek c. CHAPTP]R VIL PAIN. 37. Definition of Pain. 38. Constitution of the Nervous Systeyn. 39. Difference in the Functions of the two Nervous Systems. 37. Definition of Pain. — The due recognition of the factors concerned in the production of pain is of the first importance in the stud}^ of disease. Not only is pain the most important of complaints, but it is the most instructive diagnostic sign, for the study of its mechanism gives often the key to the best means for attaining relief. The term " pain " used here is easy to understand though difficult to define. It is beside my purpose to enter into abstruse metaphysical considerations regarding the conscious- ness of pain and its mental affinities. Nor do I include other disagreeable sensations, which are sometimes spoken of as pain, as when a brilliant light or a piercing noise unpleasantly affects the sense of sight or hearing. The term is limited to that very definite form of disagreeable sensation which everyone has experienced, and we all recognise. 58 Chapter VII. The meaning attached to the term pain in this book may be summarised shortly as follows : — Pain is a disagreeable sensation due to stimula- tion of some portion of the cerebro- spinal nervous system and referred to the periplieral distribution of cerebro-spinal sensory nerves in the external body wall. 38. Constitution of the Nervous System.— The nervous system consists of two great divisions, which are distinctly separated in their functions, viz., the cerebro-spinal and the sympathetic or autonomic. The former of these divisions consists of the brain and spinal cord, and the peripheral nerves which are distributed to the external body wall and subserve the functions of sensation and muscular contraction. Incorporated within the cerebro-spinal system is the other division, the sympathetic or autonomic nervous system, which includes the origin of such nerves as the vagus and the sympathetic. The position and distribution of the efferent fibres of the autonomic S3^stem is shown in Langley's diagram (Fig. 5, page 59). It will be seen that this system presides over the functions peculiar to the different organs. While much experimental work has been done to establish the distribution and functions of the nerves that pass from the centres to the periphery (efferent nerves), little has been done to examine the nerves that pass from the viscera to the central nervous system (afferent nerves). The reason for this is that the nerves passing from the organs to the nerve centres afford little direct evidence of their function, and it has not yet been understood in w4iat way these afferent nerves react to stimulation. Paitu 59 Sphincter of iris\ Ciliary muscle j Dilator of iris. Orbital muscle Heart. Blood-vessels of mucous membrane of head. Walls of gut from mouth to de- scending colon. Outgrowths from this region of the gut (muscle of trachea and lungs ; gastric glands, liver, pancreas). The skin (arteries, muscles, glands). Blood-vessels of gut between mouth and rectimi, of lungs and of abdommal viscera. Arteries of skeletal muscle. Muscle of spleen, ureter, and of internal generative organs. Walls of stomach, intestine, gall bladder and ducts, urinary bladder. Arteries of rectum, anus and- external generative organs. Walls of descending colon to end of gut. Walls of bladder and urethra. Muscle of external generative organs. Mid-brain autonomic. Bulbar autonomic. Sympathetic. (T. Th. to 11. or III. L. in man.) Sacral autonomic. (II. to TV. Sacral in man.) Fig. 5. Diagram to show the general origin and distribution of efferent autonomic fibres. By "muscle" is, of course, meant unstriated muscle only. By the " walls " of a structure are meant all the unstriated muscle in it. The irmervation in some cases is still a matter of controversy (gastric glands, liver, and pancreas ; vessels of lungs ; small arteries of skeletal muscles, and arteries of the central nervous system). (Langley. ) 60 Chapter VII. 39. Difference in the Functions of the two Nervous Systems. — In the scheme put forth here, pain and other sensations are regarded as functions pecuhar to the cerebro-spinal system, and that for the production of visceral pain it is suggested that under certain circumstances these afferent nerves of the autonomic system convej^ a stimukis to the cerebro-spinal nerves, so that such phenomena as pain, hyperalgesia and muscular contraction in the external body wall are the evidences of stimulation of the cerebro-spinal nerve centres by the afferent autonomic nerves. ( 61 CHAPTER VIII. THE SENSITIVENESS OF THE TISSUES. 40. The Tissues Sensitive and Not Sensitive to Meclmnical Stimulus. 41. Sensitive Tissues of the External Body Wall. 42. Insensitiveness of the Viscera to Mechanical Stimulation. 43. Methods for Testing the Sensibility of Organs. 4:4:. Testicular Pain. 45. Artificial Production of Visceral Pain. 40. The Tissues Sensitive and Not Sensitive to Mechanical Stimulus.— A step preliminary and necessary to understanding the nature of the symptoms of disease is a knowledge of the sensibility of the different tissues of the body. A great field for investigation still lies unexplored, and so long as it is neglected the understanding of the symptoms of disease will be defective. In the attempt to investigate this field I have only been able to make a slight advance, but such as it is, it has thrown new and unexpected light on a great many problems connected with the symptomatology of disease. 62 Chapter VIII. If we inquire into the response of the different tissues to such mechanical stimuli as produce the common sensations of pain, touch, heat, pressure, etc., we discover that these sensations are limited to cer- tain portions and organs of the body, and that there are large portions of the body totally insensitive to all such stimuH. Looking at the matter broadlj^, we find that while all the structures which make up the external body wall are more or less sensitive to such stimuli, the viscera and the serous lining of cavities are, with one exception, irresponsive to this kind of stimulation. Thus, if we appty a mechanical stimulus to the skin we produce a sensation peculiar to the nature of the stimulus, as, for example, touch, pain, heat, or cold. If we apply the same stimuh to the viscera or to the serous surfaces or internal struct\u:es of organs we get no response, or rarely a response of a different nature. Thus, if we prick the skin near such an orifice of the body as the anus, we can produce pain, but as soon as the mucous membrane is reached the pricking no longer produces pain. In testing other orifices of the body — for example, the mouth — a modified sensation is felt, but a.t a certain depth of the gullet all sensation ceases. If we inquire into the reason for this difference in the response to stimulation we wiU find it in the nerve supply of these different tissues. Thus the tissues which give rise to sensation in response to mechanical stimuli are supphed by the cerebro-spinal nerves, wliile the tissues wliich do not respond receive no nerves from the cerebro-spinal nervous system, but are supplied only by the sympathetic nervous The Sensitiveness of the Tissues. 63 system, or what is sometimes spoken of as the in- vohmtarv' nervous system. 41. Sensitive Tissues of the External Body Wall. — If we take the abdominal wall we find three great layers endowed with exquisite sensibilit}' to pain. The first of these, the skin, I need not dwell upon, save to point out how its sensibility frequently becomes increased in visceral disease, and how this increased sensitiveness is united to an exalted muscular reflex. The second of these sensitive layers is the voluntar}' muscular system, best observed in the flat muscles of the abdomen. It is the sensitiveness of this muscular layer which is most commonly exalted in visceral disease, its sensibilit}^ being very readily increased. Muscular hyperalgesia is such a striking phenomenon, is so frequently present and pla.ys such an important part in the protective mechanism, that it is astonisMng to find it almost universally overlooked. One can read elaborate treatises devoted to special organs, in which this symptom is the most striking and the most instruc- tive feature, but its presence is nevertheless over- looked or misinterpreted. In an ordinary case of stomach ulcer, appendicitis, gaU-stone, renal colic, or enlarged liver, if one notes the tenderness of the abdominal wall, and observes how this deep tender- ness extends far beyond the site of the organ affected, one can appreciate the nature and significance of this sensitive lajfer. Vv^ith a little care one will be able to distinguish tliis muscular hyperalgesia from cutane- ous hyperalgesia and from hyperalgesia of the deeper tissues. The third sensitive layer is one of which anatomists and physiologists Avere quite ignorant till 64 Chapter VIII. recently, though it has long been suspected from clinical observations. It is the layer of loose connective tissue lying immediately outside the peritoneum. I suspected its presence for a long time as I could frequently get exquisite tenderness in pushing my fingers between the recti muscles, for instance in cases of gastric ulcer, the stomach not being affected by the pressure. Its existence can be shovxii in an operation for the radical cure of hernia under cocaine anaesthesia. The skin and muscles can be cut tlirough, and the patient experiences no pain. When the loose connective tissue outside the peritoneum is gently torn tlurough the patient may experience most exquisite pain. After the peritoneum is exposed it can be incised, its visceral layer scratched and after- wards stitched, and the patient feels no pain. I have verified this observation on several occasions. Ramstrom has made a careful histological exam- ination of the abdominal wall of man and other mammals, and showed that this region is riehh^ endowed with nerves and nerve endings, the nerves being derived from those which supply the muscles of the abdominal wall. This observation may probably afford a clue to the confused statements that exist in regard to the sensitiveness of the peritoneum. I can only say this, that I have on numerous occasions in the course of operations scratched and cut the serous surface of the peritoneum on conscious subjects Avith- out anv analgesic, local or general, and have never known the slightest sensation ehcited. One can understand, however, that the inflamed peritoneum and adhesions might readily affect this remarkable nervous laver. Peritonitis, however, so readity The Sensitiveness of the Tissues. 65 produces muscular lipj^eralgesia and tonic muscular contractions (viscero- motor reflex), that the pain and tenderness are demonstrably, in the majority of cases, of spinal origin. 42. Insensitiveness of the Viscera to Mechanical Stimulation. — The insensitiveness of the viscera to mechanical stimulation has been repeatedly demon- strated. Haller described a series of experiments where he exposed in animals certain viscera by operation, leaving an opening by which they could be reached. Afterwards, while the animal was feeding, he introduced through the opening instru- ments that cut and burnt the organ, and the animal paid no attention to what he was doing. I have myself repeatedly tested in the conscious human subject the various organs by cutting, stitching, and tearing, and no sensation was elicited. The insensitiveness of the viscera and its full significance has not been grasped. Indeed, the belief is common that the viscera are endowed with common sensations, and w^iat is supposed to be evidence is easily obtained. 43. Methods for Testing the Sensibility of Organs. The sensitiveness of tissues to stimulation is one of those apparently simple problems which it is assumed anyone can solve. As a matter of fact, no one has been sufficiently trained to undertake such an investigation. A large amount of work has been done on the subject by physiologists, neurologists, psychologists, phj^sicians, and surgeons, yet, notwith- standing their opportunities, they have failed to inves- tigate the matter in a manner calculated to reveal the true facts, and so the conclusions Avhich are 66 Chdjyter VIII. current to-day are based on imperfect observations, and in consequence are misleading. I comment on this fact as one of many instances where the progress of medicine is hampered by a lack of understanding of the manner in which investigation should be conducted. I do not at present wish to enter fully into the matter, but mention a few points to show the imperfect methods which have been used. The first point which arises is, that no conclusion should be drawn as to the sensitiveness of an organ which has been stimulated through a structure itself sensitive. Nearly all conclusions have been drawn from observations made by pressing on the organs through the sensitive abdominal wall. Many ob- servers state that they have demonstrated the sensitiveness of an organ by first observing its position by various methods, such as the x rays, and later by pressing over it and ehciting pain. The error here lies in the failure of the observer to take the precaution of excluding the sensitive external bodj^ wall. If, for instance, when such sensitiveness has been detected, an attempt is made to delimit the area that is sensitive, it will be found that the size and shape of the area painful on pressm^e bears no relation to the size and shape of the organ supposed to be tender. I discussed this matter with a very skilled physician. He demurred to my statement that there was no evidence of the sensitiveness of organs. He declared that he had a patient with a large liver which he could demonstrate to me was extremely tender. I asked to see this patient and was shown him. The enlarged liver was easily palpated, the lower margin being sharp and well The Sensitiveness of the Tissues. 67 Fig. 6. The shaded area represents the liver, the dotted area the extension of hyperalgesia beyond the liver margin, showing the pain elicited by pressure over the liver was due to an increased sensitiveness of the tissues of the external body wall. defined. On gently pressing over the liver the patient winced from the pain. I asked the phy- sician to map out the region that was so manifestly tender, and to his surprise he found the tenderness extended 3 in. lower than the margin of the liver (Fig. 6). 68 Chapter VIII. It is well known that pain does occui' in disease of the viscera, and in animals distress has been provoked by certain experiments. But here the experimenter never realised that the evidences of suffering, even if these were due to pain, gave no information as to the locality of the pain — a fact absolutely essential to the understanding of the mechanism hy which visceral pain is produced. The same neglect of this essential matter is found in the observations of surgeons and others who have studied pain in the exposed organs of the human subject. With the recognition of these sensitive struc- tures — frequently rendered exquisitely sensitive to painful stimuli in visceral disease — it ^^ ill be under- stood how impossible it is to judge of the sensitiveness of the viscera from external exploration. When, therefore, we find the surgeon or phj^sician demon- strating the sensitiveness of any viscus, it will be realised that he is in reality stimulating, in his examination, those extremely sensitive structures of the external abdominal wall, and referring the pain he elicits to an organ that is totally insensitive to any such stimulation. 44. Testicular Pain. — On the other hand, one cannot always be sure of the soiu'ce of pain, as when pressure is applied over a movable kidney or readily palpated abdominal tumoiu*. That pain arises on pressure on a viscus is undoubted, but the pain does not seem to arise from the direct stimula- tion but by reflex stimulation of a sensory cerebro- spinal nerve. This can be demonstrated in the case of the testicle. In ordinarv cases when the cord is The Sensitiveness of the Tissues. 69 short the pains felt on applying pressure to the testicle are not readily differentiated. If, however, an individual with a long cord, where the testicle hangs down a long way from the groin, be examined, the pains resulting from pressure on the testicle can be separately recognised. In such an instance a pain is felt at once readily localised over the point of pressm-e. A few seconds later another pain is felt gradually increasing in intensity, and gradually passing away and referred to the groin. Accom- panying the pain there is sometimes a sensation of faintness, very slight with light pressure, but evidently of the same nature as the intense depression following on a blow on the testicle. This dej)ression and pain are similar to those which are evoked by pressure on the kidney and ovary. Concerning the first of these pains when it is felt at once and referred to the place of stimulation, a curious question arises bearing on the sensibility of serous membranes. As I have already pointed out, I have scratched the serous surfaces of both visceral and parietal peritoneum and pleura, and elicited no sensation of pain, but exquisite pain may be elicited by scratching the tunica vaginalis. In certain cases in tapping a hydrocele, if the testicle be gently held with one hand and the visceral laj^er of the tunica vaginalis lightly scratched with the canula, the patient at once experiences pain and refers the pain imerringty to the region scratched. However lightly the stimulus is made no sensation is experienced beyond that of pain, resembling in this respect the sensibility of the cornea. As demonstrating the difference between the sensibility of the tunica 70 G}iaj>ter VIII. vaginalis and the peritoneum, I cite the following experience. A patient consulted me with his scrotum greatly enlarged and full of fluid, which I took to be a hydrocele. I tapped him and tested the sensibility of the testicle. I found the patient did not feel pain when I scratched what I took to be his tmiica vaginalis. I scratched rather roughl}^ jet no painful sensation was experienced. Finding I could not reduce the whole of the swelling I concluded that the case was not one of hydrocele, and on operating I found the case was one of omental hernia with the sac distended by peritoneal fluid. What I had been scratching was the peritoneum. As the tunica vaginalis and the peritoneum have the same origin it appeared strange that there should be this difference in sensation, until on inquiry I found that a cerebro- spinal nerve is distributed to the tunica vaginalis, viz., a twig of the genital branch of the genitp- crural nerve. The tunica vaginalis is the only sensitive serous membrane covering an organ that I have detected, and it is the only one to which a branch of a cerebro- spinal nerve has been traced. In certain cases the tunica vaginalis becomes hj^peralgesic {see page 183). Professor Waterston tells me that the visceral layer of the tunica vaginalis is not of the same origin as the parietal, but is looked on as a persistence of the germinal epithelium. This may have some bearing on the sensibility of the testicle, and my suggestion as to the nature of the testicular sensitiveness is therefore provisional. The Sensifive7ies.9 of the Tissues. 71 45. Artificial Production of Visceral Pain. — It is ta cimoiis fact tliat altliough the belief is so universally held that the viscera are endowed with " sensory " nerves, and that physiologists refer to afferent s^'mpathetic nerves as " sensory " in func- tion, not a single authentic observation has been rendered to sho^^' that the viscera have a direct sensibility of their own, i.e., a sensibility derived from the possession of nerves which when stimulated produce a sensation. Of course, a great deal depends on what is considered evidence, many people being perfectly satisfied if they elicit pain by pressing over i\n organ. Pli3^siologists have interpreted certain movements as an expression of pain after stimulating afferent S3^mpathetic nerves. But this does not prove that pain was evoked nor does it prove that the pain was a direct pain, nor does it show in what situation the pain was felt, for the location of the pain is the key to the problem. It is therefore necessary in investigating this matter to be sure of the tissue stinaulated, and the region in which the resultant pain is felt. It is because of the absence of the specification of the locus of the pain that many otherwise important observations are rendered of little value in respect to this investigation. That pain can be produced by visceral stimula- tion is easily demonstrated if one emplo^^s an adequate stimulus. It is now many 3'ears since I pointed out that the most \iolent pains of which we are conscious are associated with hollow muscular organs, and that by producing violent contraction of a hollow viscus pain can be elicited. The easiest way to do this is to give a distending enema of warm 72 Chapter VIII. water, and to retain the enema until painful peri- stalsis results. That the pain is really due to the contraction of the muscle wall of the bowel is evident from the fact that with the relaxation of the sphincter during the pain the contents of the bowel are expelled with considerable force, and the pain at the same time subsides. Here it is evident that a considerable portion of the descending colon and rectum must have contracted, but the pain is not felt along the position occupied by these structures, but, in the majority of people, it is referred across the middle line immediately above the pubis. The following observation demonstrates an exactly similar series of facts : — I had occasion to resect a small portion of the small intestine in a conscious subject, for umbilical fistula, w^hose abdominal cavity I laid open. He refused to take an anaesthetic, and no analgesic, local or general, was administered. There were numerous peritoneal adhesions, and while I cut and tore these the patient was unconscious of any sensation. I cut and stitched the serous surfaces of parietal and vis- ceral peritoneum, I tore adhesions from the liver, I cut and sutured the bowel and mesenterj^, and no sensation was felt. After preparing the upper part of the bowel it was wrapped in a warm cloth and laid on one side. Dmring the subsequent steps the patient frequently" moaned. I asked him if he felt pain, and he replied that he did. I asked him where he felt the pain, and he indicated with his hand that it was across the middle line at tlie level of the umbilicus. I at first felt that it might be due to the part that I was manipulating, but the pain was The Sensitiveness of the Tissues. 73 intermittent. Chancing to look at the prepared upper part of the bowel that lay on the left side of the abdomen, I observed that every few minutes a peristaltic wave passed over the lower portion of it, and when this occurred the patient moaned in pain. I made certain that the pain was connected with the peristaltic wave, and I produced the peristalsis several times by lightly j)inching the bowel. I also made sure the patient had no doubt as to the place in which it was felt, with the result that here before my eyes was the cause of the pain wiiich the patient felt, and yet the patient referred the site of the pain with precision to an area ten inches or twelve inches away from the contracting bowel. ( 74 ) CHAPTEK IX. VISCERAL REFLEXES. 46. The Mechanism, of Visceral Pain {the Viscero- sensory Reflex). 47. The Mechanism of the Viscero-motor Reflex, 48. Multiple Reflexes from. Visceral Stimnlation. 46. The Mechanism of Visceral Pain (the Viscero-sensory Reflex). — The remarkable difference between the sensitiveness of the external body wall and the viscera in response to mechanical stimulation brings out clearly the difference in function of the two divisions of the nervous S3^stem — a difference which it is necessary clearly to understand if the symptoms of disease are to be fully comi)rehended. It shows that when pain is evoked from the organs or tissues which are not supplied by the sensory nerves of the cerebro- spinal system some other mechanism must take part in its production. The nature of this mechanism is revealed by tlie study of the symptoms produced in certain forms of visceral disease. If we take the symptoms that are present in certain simple diseases — for example, a gastric ulcer, appendicitis, or renal colic — the mechanism by which a number of symptoms are produced will become clear. In many cases of gastric ulcer there is pain, limited to a small area. Visceral Be flexes. 75 Fig. 7. The shaded area in the centre shows the position of pain and hyper- algesia in a case of ulcer of the pjlorus situated at x. in the epigastric region. This pain has a different location from the nicer. Thus the pain may be situated in the middle line, as in Fig. 7, where the shaded area indicates the site of pain, while the ulcer revealed at the post-mortem examination is situated at the p^dorus, which was found at x in Fig. 7. Moreover, if a patient with this pain breathes deeph% the stomach, with the ulcer, shifts its position, but the site of pain remains fixed. With these facts before us, the mechanism of pain in visceral disease becomes clear. When the disordered sensations of toothache were considered 76 Chapter IX. it was seen that the only reasonable explanation Avas that from the offending tooth a stimulus had been sent into the central nervous system (page 54). The effects of this stimulus were not limited to the cells of the nerve supplying the tooth, but spread to the ceUs of sensory nerves in the immediate neigh- bourhood, w-ith the result that there was pain and fA -ver in bed may exhaust the heart's strength, or the patient may be unable to walk across the room, or up a flight of stairs, without some form of discomfort checking him. The exhaustion of the patient's strength in such circumstances is seen simply as a limita^tion of the heart's power to respond to effort. Each individual has become accustomed to what he can do with comfort, and he recognises his heart failure by not being able to exert himself with com- fort to the extent he had formerly done. The symptoms produced in health by over-exertion are frequently identical with those produced by slight exertion when the heart has become weakened and where this slight exertion is more than can be done with comfort. It will thus be seen that the symptoms of heart failure are really recognised by a limitation 224 Chapter XXI. of the field of cardiac response, that is, the patient finds he is stopped, on a sHght exertion, by the symptoms which, when in health, only stopped him after a prolonged, exhausting exertion. This weakness of the heart can be expressed in another way, namely, the premature exhaustion of reserve force. It is because the heart possesses the power to lay up a reserve of force that it is able to respond to calls on its energy, so that it can accom- modate itself to the varying activities of the body. When the body is at rest the heart not only over- comes the resistances opposed to its work with ease, employing only a portion of its power, but it is at the same time building up a store of energy ready to be liberated when the next call is made by the body for more work. After severe bodily labour the store of reserve forces become exhausted, and if the labom- be persisted in, there arises a feeling of dis- tress, which expresses the exhaustion of the heart's reserve force. It is this faculty of building up a sufficient reserve store that distinguishes a healthy heart from a weakened heart, and the first evidence of weakness is shown by a too speedy exhaustion of the reserve. This is made evident by distress being aroused when the individual undertakes some form of exertion he had been wont to do with ease — that is, by a limitation of the field of cardiac response. It will thus be seen that the symptoms of heart failure in the first instance are personal, due to the patient' s recognition of his limitations. The estimate is therefore a very variable one, and depends on each individual recognising his own limitations, and detecting Avhen these limitations become narrowed. Affections of the Circulatory System. 225 Each individual obtains a fair estimate of his power of exertion, and this is his measure of health. A limitation of these powers in an intelligent patient calls attention to his condition. It will further be observed that the main s^'mptoms of exhaustion of reserve force are the same when a healthy heart exhausts its store after a prolonged effort as when an enfeebled or diseased heart exhausts its limited store by a slight effort. These s^^mptoms are in the main subjective and reflex, though certain changes may also be made out in the size of the heart and its rate and rhythm. It is, however, the sub- jective and reflex symptoms that are of the greatest importance, whatever may be the nature of the functional disorder or structural lesion. It wiU frequently be found that patients whose hearts show many forms of functional and structural abnormalities (valvular murmurs, cardiac enlarge- ment, irregular action) have such a store of reserve force that they can pursue laborious occupation Avith ease and comfort, and live to a good age. On the other hand, patients may show no physical sign of abnormality ; the heart may be normal in size and in rate, and regular in rhythm, and the sounds be clear and free from murmur ; but the reserve force be so small that the slightest exertion entails at once distressful sj^mptoms of heart exhaustion, and the lives of these persons may be Yery precarious. 123. The Nature of the Symptoms in Heart Failure. — The value of s^maptoms thus depends upon the estimation of the amount of reserve force stored up in the heart muscle. An imperfect valvular appar- atus is but an embarrassment to the heart muscle in 226 Chapter XXI. its work. We can detect that valvular imperfection by the presence of a murmur, but we can only draw a very limited conclusion as to its bearing on the heart's work. This is really obtained by considering the amount of reserve force, and the estimation of the reserve is made by observing how the patient's heart responds to effort. The same rule applies to other forms of circulatory changes, as affections of the myocardium and of the arterial system. When, therefore, we detect what we consider to be an abnormal sign, we must not draw our conclusions from that sign alone, but must consider how far its cause has proved an impediment to the heart's action, and this is done by estimating the amount of reserve force. In doing this a wise discretion must be exercised, for other factors may have precipitated the exhaustion. An individual may have an organic lesion, as a valvular defect, but in addition may have been subjected to a life that predisposes to exhaustion, as excessive labour, improper or insuffi- cient food, mental anxiety, sleeplessness, infections, diseases of other organs ; and such factors, rather than the mere valvular flaw, may be the actual cause of the exhaustion of the reserve force. Or the individual may have a sedentary occupation, wherein, owing to lack of judicious exercise, his reserve force has gradually diminished, until some unaccustomed but not excessive effort calls attention to the limited field of cardiac response. Not only must a careful calculation be made of these accessory, and it may be all- important, factors, but there must be a recognition of the significance of the cardiac abnormalities or supposed abnor- Ajjections of the Circulatory System. 227 malities. I do not enter here on the points which indicate the nature and seriousness of such abnor- mahties, as, for instance, murmurs and heart irregularities, but I wish to insist upon the fact that the chnician should familiarise himself with such points. For instance, a young person may have a fainting attack, and when lying quietly in bed the pulse is found very irregular. This sign, which is a perfectly normal one, though rarely recognised as such, is not infrequently linked up with the fainting attack, with which, as a matter of fact, it has no connection, and the patient is subjected to unnecessary treatment and restriction, and he becomes alarmed by the idea that he has a weak or diseased heart. In advanced life another form of irregularity is of extreme frequency (the extra-systole), and the recogni- tion of its presence, with some limitation of the field of cardiac response, often leads to the conclusion that the irregularit}^ is in some way responsible for the other cardiac signs, and energetic but futile means are taken in the attempt to cure the irregularity. In the same way this morbid dread of the unknown leads to the idea that certain cardiac symptoms as pain, especially when dignified by the term of angina pectoris, is of very grave significance. If it be once realised that pain is as constant a symptom in affections of the heart as in other hollow muscular organs, and that the pain is as readil}" induced by adequate causes in the heart as in the stomach, a truer perception will be obtained of the symptoms of many obscure heart affections. I shall show later that pain is one of the expressions Q 2 228 Chapter XXI. of an exhausted heart muscle, and in order to appreciate its significance the conditions that have led up to the exhaustion of the heart muscle should be ascertained — a matter usually of no great difficulty — and these conditions will guide us to a safe conclusion. If the idea be followed up that the earliest of heart S3'mptoms are simply due to an exhaustion of the store of reserve force, it will be found that there is a great resemblance in the reflex symptoms among all forms of heart affections — functional and structmal. The most important symptoms are confined mainly to the patient's sensations, and demand careful consideration. The mechanism by which they are produced is not at all times clear, and some of them are undoubtedly reflex ; although I cannot give a full explanation of their production,, I attempt here a brief description of the most im- portant of the subjective symptoms and some of the phenomena associated with them. 124. Consciousness of the Heart's Action. — Under normal circumstances the movements of the heart and circulation are carried on without the individual being conscious of their action unless he voluntarily directs his attention to the subject. When the heart is over-stimulated it may contract with such force that the individual becomes conscious of its action. The stimula- tion may arise from a great many circumstances, as, for instance, from heat, either by a rise in the patient's temperature or from a hot bath. It may arise from nerve stimulation, as from mental excitement, or from some peripheral irritation in Affections of the Circulatory System. 229 the viscera. Its significance is greatest when it arises in consequence of exhaustion of the heart ; coming on in consequence of bodil}^ exertion, it is often the first sign of exhaustion of the reserve force. When it is thus produced, it forms a valuable sign for estimating the amount of reserve force present in the heart-muscle. It gives no indication of the conditions that have led to this exhaustion, for it may occur in a healthy heart after prolonged exertion, as well as in a diseased heart after a very slight exertion. Under these circumstances the heart's action is usually rapid as well as forcible, but in some cases there is not much increase in rate. The consciousness of the heart's action is often spoken of as palpitation. While it may not occasion much suffering, there are individuals of a neurotic type in whom this consciousness of the heart's action causes much distress. In these patients the heart may be quite healthy, and the palpitation is then the action of an undue excita- bility of the sympathetic nerve supply of the heart. As other portions of the nervous system are abnor- mally excitable the heart's action may cause con- siderable distress bodily and mentally. In patients with this neurotic temperament, where there is real heart trouble, as in mitral stenosis, palpitation on exertion may be so readily induced that it acts as a protection from over- exertion, because, in order to avoid its occurrence, the patient is perforce kept quiet. The occmrence of palpitation should always lead to an examinaton of the nervous system as well as of the heart's condition. 230 Chapter XXI. Other sensations of the heart's abnormal action may be felt by the patient. A gentle fluttering maj^ be felt within the chest during a period of irregular action. This may be brief and transient, or, as in certain cases of paroxysmal tachycardia, it may continue during an attack of many hours. Accompanying the latter condition there is often a feeling of exhaustion which leads the patient to rest, or to go about carefully and quietly. When the heart resumes its normal action the patient is at once conscious of a change and of a sense of relief. A more common sensation is that when the heart is felt to stand still, in what is called intermittent action of the heart. This sensation is usuall}^ felt in cases of extra-systole, which is so frequent in advanced life though occasionally present in the young. Here the ventricle con- tracts prematureh", and often before the auricle ; the individual is not conscious of this prematiure beat or extra-systole which is followed by a long pause, and it is this pause which often alarms the patient. After the long pause the next beat is frequently big and powerful, and the patient may be conscious of the shock due to this. In nervous people this big beat often causes much mental anxietj^ Another curious reflex is sometimes met with in these extra- systoles, the patient giving a little gasp or cough when the extra-systole occurs. 125. Breathlessness. — Breathlessness is so fre- quently associated with affections of the heart that its occurrence under any circumstances necessitates a consideration of the heart's condition. It is so Affections of the Circulator y System. 231 common a sign of exhaustion of the reserve force that in an inquiry into a patient's cardiac symptoms the amount of exertion that can be undertaken before breathlessness occurs should be carefully inquired into. In many cases the breathlessness may occur independently of any bodily exertion, coming on when the patient is in bed, and persisting till the patient has to sit up and breathe in a laboured fashion (orthopiioea). The mechanism by which breathlessness is brought about in heart affections is far from clear, and so many factors are concerned in respira- tion that 2i\\Y endeavour to explain its cause would lead to such vague and indeterminate speculation that, in the present state of our knowledge, little good would result. The great point is to observe the fact, to exclude other possible causes of breathlessness (as affections of the lungs, mechanical obstructions to the breathing, anaemia, and other blood affections), and then from a study of the cardiac conditions to determine the nature of the exhaustion that has been produced. This is seldom a matter of much difficulty when a sufficient experi- ence of the various forms of heart disease has been acquired. The presence of a demonstrable lesion in the heart may be a guide, as shown by modification of the heart sounds, or changes in the size, rate and rhythm of the heart. In the absence of these the age of the patient may be suggestive — if old, degenerative changes in the myocardium ; if young, the probabilit}^ of some general infection, as tuber- culosis ; if middle-aged, the possibility of exhaus- tion of the reserve force from over-work, worry, bad 232 Chapter X X I. nourishment — in fact, the consideration of the factors that tend to exhaust the reserve force of the heart, breathlessness being often the first sign of such exhaustion. Apart from breathlessness brought on by exertion there are some definite forms of breathless- ness whijch are recognised as especial^ associated with heart affection. The best known of these is the Cheyne-Stokes respiration, where periods of apnoea alternate with periods of laboured breathing. Another characteristic form is that in which patients are seized in the night with attacks of breathlessness, and have to sit up and breathe in laboured fashion for periods of varying, duration, from half-an-hour to several hours. This form of laboured breathing is sometimes spoken of as cardiac asthma, and its onset is often mistaken for the more common form of asthma. When asthma is found to occur in people of middle or advanced age for the first time, the possibilit}^ of its cardiac origin should always be borne in mind. In many cases the patient breathes quietly though hurriedly, with no distress, and the respira- tory condition may, in consequence, be overlooked, though the rate of respiration may be from twenty to thirty times per minute. With failing hearts (as in typhoid fever, in conditions in which elderly people are forced to lie in bed, or in valvular disease) this rate tends to increase, and on examination respiration is found to be shallow and limited to the upper portion of the chest. In such cases there will almost always be found evidence of stasis or oedema at the bases of the lunss. The first si^n of this Affections of the Circulatory System. 233 condition, apart from the increased rapidity of the respiratory movement, is the detection of fine crepi- tations on deep inspiration heard over the base of the lung, on that side towards which the patient habitu- ally lies. My usual method of detecting this is to ask the patient on which side he has lain, and then to ask him to sit up and to auscultate the base of the lung on the side on which he has lain. This is the first step in the physical examination of the patient. If done at a later stage the movements may have deepened the respiration, so that the crepitations have disappeared. One usually detects the fine crepitations on the first full and deep nspiration. In the early stages of pulmonary stasis, after this thorough ventilation of the bases of the lungs, the crepitations disappear. If measures are not taken to stop this tendency to oedema, or if in spite of all endeavours the oedema increases, the crex)itations become more numerous and persistent, and the lung resonance may become impaired. In fatal cases the bases of the lungs become dull, there is an absence of the respiratory sounds, and post- mortem the lungs are found sodden and airless. In suitable cases there can be detected coincident with these lung s^anptoms evidences of the dilatation of the right heart, as epigastric pulsation due to the filling and emptying of the dilated right ventricle. A sense of suffocation is an occasional symptom in heart affections. It may be the first sign of exhaustion of the reserve force, coming on when the patient exerts himself. It may suddenly seize a patient when he is lying in bed. Its mechanism is obscure. The sensation is usually referred to the upper part of the chest and throat. 234 Chapter XXI. 126. Viscero-sensory and Viscero - motor Reflexes. — Under these terms I include such sen- sory phenomena as pain and hyperalgesia of the skin, muscles, mammary glands, and the contraction of the muscles, which gives rise to a sensation described " as if the breast - bone were breaking." After obtaining a full account of the patient's sensations and experiences, if careful inquiry be made in regard to a number of sjmiptoms, as pain, constriction of the chest, soreness of the chest or arms, frequent micturition, increased flow of saliva, bad dreams, there will be found a number of phenomena which the patient does not include in his description, because his mind is occupied with the sensations which cause him suffering and discomfort. The accurate noting of these less obtrusive phenomena will often tlirow^ a flood of light on many an obscure process and' reveal the mechanism by which the more obtrusive phenomena are produced. In the physical examination careful testing for hyx3eralgesia should fu-st be made. The skin of the left chest should be lightly pinched and compared with that of the right. The breasts should be lightly compressed and the resultant sensations compared. The tenderness to pressure of the pectoralis major where it forms the anterior wall of the left axilla should be tested. In the same way the skin of the neck and the sterno-mastoid should be tested. The upper edge of the trapezius muscle where it passes from the scapula to the neck should be lightty compressed along its whole border, and it will sometimes happen that certain areas will be found very tender. These are places where a small Affections of the Circulatory System. 235 nerve trunk is pressed upon. Special areas of tenderness may sometimes be found, as under the left breast, and over the second and third ribs in the nipple line. In one patient, where this latter ten- derness was very marked, I found, at the post-mortem examination, on dissection, a small nerve trunk (the internal anterior thoracic nerve). Angina pectoris affords an excellent illustration of the application of the principles I have en- deavoured to enunciate, as the symptoms can be with certainty referred to the organ at fault, and because the complex of symptoms that are included in an attack of angina pectoris are capable of being analysed with great precision. This is, in a great measure, due to the peculiar distribution of the sensory nerves in whose peripheral distribution the characteristic phenomena are shown, and whose centres in the spinal cord and medulla are in close relationship to the centres of the autonomic nerves of the heart (sympathetic and vagus). The cases I cite demonstrate that the term " angina pectoris " includes a number of reflexes, and I give in detail the more conspicuous of these, namely, sensory reflexes, where the pain and hyperalgesia affect the chest, arm, head, and neck ; motor reflexes, resulting in spasm of the intercostal muscles ; secretory reflexes, shown by profuse secretion of saliva and urine. Some cases show other reflexes, as the respiratory, but the discussion of these would lead me beyond the object I had in writing this book. Notwithstanding the numerous papers devoted to the consideration of angina pectoris, practically none have dealt adequately with the analysis of the 236 Chapter XXI. symptoms present dm^ing an attack. Angina pectoris is often surrounded by such tragic circum- stances that it forms a suitable theme for disquisition, and on that account we too often get the lurid description of an impressionist artist instead of the plain, matter-of-fact description of an accurate observer. A careful sifting of all the details brings out the fact that the essential principles underlying the pains associated with affections of the heart differ in no way from those of any other hollow muscular organ. So terrifying is the attack to the patient that his perceptions of the details of his suffering are generalh^ confused, so that often no clear account can be obtained from his description ; but if he is intelligent and is asked to note particu- lars in subsequent attacks, he may be able to throw a very valuable light on the onset and character of the sensations he experiences. The observations made by the physician of patients during an attack also afford great help in this respect. 127. The Viscero-sensory Reflexes in Dilata- tion of the Heart and Liver.— Before dealing with the more characteristic attacks of heart pain which go by the name of angina pectoris, I wish first to draw attention to the very distinct sensory evidences that arise from -the dilatation of the heart. These sensory symptoms are practically identical with those that arise in cases of distension of any other viscus, as the stomach, bladder or liver. To illustrate this I select cases where the dilatation of the heart occurs rapidly, and is followed by a rapid distension of the liver, such as we find in certain forms of paroxysmal tachycardia. In Affections of the Cirmdatory System. 237 certain of these cases the rhythm of the heart starts suddenly at some abnormal place in the auricle or ventricle, or at the fibres joining auricle and ven- tricle. The heart at once beats with great rapidity, but fails to maintain the circulation, and in conse- quence we have very rapidly developed great dila- tation of the heart, fullness of the veins, enlargement of the liver, and dropsy. I have seen a number of these cases, and in two particularly I have observed some fifteen to twenty attacks, of which the following is a typical description. The patient may be in good health and no abnormality be detected save, in some cases, the occurrence of an extra- systole. Suddenly the heart's rate becomes greatly accelerated, to 150 beats and more per minute. In a few hours the patient's face becomes dusky, the lips swollen and livid, and there appears great shortness of breath on exertion, marked increase in the size of the heart, distension and pulsation of the veins of the neck, and enlarge ment and pulsation of the liver. The jugular and liver pulsation are of the ventricular type. Pain and oppression may be felt over the chest. The skin and deeper tissues of the left chest become extremely tender on pressure in the area shaded in Fig. 21. If the left pectoralis major muscle be grasped where it forms the axillary fold it wiU be found extremely tender. The skin of the left side of the neck may also be tender, and if the left sterno-mastoid muscle and the left trapezius muscle, above the middle of the scapula, are lightly grasped they may be found exquisitely sensitive. The skin and muscles over the liver will also be found extremely sensitive to 238 Chapter XXI. The areas shaded in the neck, chest and upper part of the abdomen represent the distribution of cutaneous hyperalgesia in a patient suffering from acute dilatation of the heart and liver. pressure, and the parts hyperalgesic extend over a much larger area than the enlarged liver. If the heart's rate revert to the normal the patient at once experiences great relief, and in a few hours all signs of the circulatory disturbances disappear. The hyper- algesia may last with diminishing severity for a few days. The tenderness of the skin and muscles in the regions described above may be found in patients during the early stages of dilatation of the heart from any cause, ctnd is very common in heart failure Affections of the Circulatory System. 239 following the onset of auricular fibrillation. In some cases one can tell when improvement is taking place by noting the diminution of this tenderness. There can be little doubt as to the mechanism by which the hyperalgesia of the tissues in the three regions is brought about. The relation of the dilatation of the heart and liver with these sensorj^ phenomena is undoubtedly that of cause and effect. The tenderness to pressure of the tissues in the left chest is due to stimulation of the afferent sympathetic nerves by the dilated heart. These nerves stimulate the sensor}^ centres of the third and fourth thoracic nerves in the spinal cord, so that a stimulus reaching them from their peripheral dis- tribution gives rise to a painful impression. The tenderness of the left sterno-mastoid and trapezius muscle, and of the skin of the neck, is due to the afferent fibres of the " bulbar autonomic " system — that is the vagus, conveying a stimulus to the sensor}' roots of the second and third cervical nerves. The hyperalgesia of the tissues covering the liver is due to stimulation of the sensory centres in the spinal cord by the afferent sympathetic fibres from the engorged liver. 128. The Pain of Angina Pectoris is a Viscero-sensory Reflex. — The usual description given of the pain in angina pectoris is that it is felt in the heart and shoots into the arm, or that there are two pains, a local pain in the heart and a referred pain in the arm. If, however, a careful analysis be made of all the symptoms present, facts will be found that practically demonstrate that in angina pectoris there is but one knid of pain, and 240 Chapter XXI. Fig. 22. The shaded area shows the distribution of the pain and cutaneous hyperalgesia in a typical case of angina pectoris. The roman numbers refer to the nerves implicated, viz., I, II, III and IV, thoracic ner^^es and VIII cervical nerve. that its production is in accordance with the law I have attempted to estabhsh, namely, that it is a viscero-sensory reflex. One is not able in every case to demonstrate the proofs of this hypothesis, but facts derived from suitable cases afford legitimate conclusions applicable to all cases. Shortty, these facts are, that the pain in the very gravest cases may be felt in regions distant from the heart ; that Affections of the Circulatory System. 241 Fig. 23. The shaded areas show the distribution of the eruption in a case of herpes zoster affecting the upper thoracic nerves. this pain is identical in character with that felt over the heart ; that the pain may originally start in parts distant from the heart, and gradually approach and settle over the heart ; and, lastly, that the tissues of the external body wall, in the exact region in which the pain was felt, may be found extremely hyperalgesic after the pain has passed away. From this last fact it is inferred that, inasmuch as the seat 242 Chapter X X I. of pain corresponds to the region of hyperalgesia, therefore the pain was due to stimulation of the hyperalgesia nerves. To assume otherwise would be to ignore a principle that explains satisfactorily the sensation of pain wherever arising. Another fact tends to support this view, namely that the eruption of herpes zoster affecting the arms may occupy a situation similar to that in which pain is felt in angina pectoris. Herpes zoster is due to an inflammation of the ganglia on the posterior roots of spinal nerves. Compare Figs. 22 and 23. The following observations illustrate this point. They are examples chosen from a large number of cases that demonstrate the same feature. For the sake of brevity only those points bearing upon this argument are referred to. Female, aged 30, suffering from stenosis of the aortic, mitral and tricuspid valves, was seized with a violent pain, referred to the outer part of the left side of the chest wall. The pain passed off, but a sense of soreness and smarting remained over the part in which the pain v/as felt. On examining her I foimd a portion of the sldn of the chest extremely tender to touch, corresponding to the area shaded in Fig. 24. A few days later she began to suffer from attacks of pain in the left breast and down the inside of the left arm, and on examination I found that the hyperalgesia had extended and occupied an area similar to that shaded in Fig. 25. These attacks of pain became so severe on the shghtest exertion that she was obUged to keep to her bed. She partially recovered from these attacks. Affections of the Circulatory System. 243 Fig. 24. The shaded area shows the distribu- tion of the cutaneous hyperalgesia after the first attack of angina pectoris (com- pare with Fig. 25). FiCx. 25. After repeated attacks of angina pectoris the pain and hyperalgesia ex- tended to the regions shaded here. Note the areas in the neck and inner side of right elbow (compare with Figs. 22 and 23). 244 Chapter X X I. but they recurred with increased severity. When suffering the most severe attacks the hyperalgesia embraced nearly the whole of the left chest and inside of the left arm, and also a portion of the right chest. The left sterno-mastoid muscle and trapezius also became very tender, and the patient would sometimes complain of pain on the inner surface of the right arm, at the elbow, where also I found a patch of cutaneous hyperalgesia. Two years and a half after the first attack of pain she was recovering from a series of violent attacks, when, on getting out of bed, she fell forward and died immediately. The fact that in this case the attacks of pain were followed by a hyperalgesia of the sldn in the region where the pain was felt, and the further fact that the pain started at a distance from the cardiac region, and was often felt most severely at a distance from the heart, proves that the cardiac pain in this case was a viscero-sensory reflex, and that the pain felt over the precordia was presumably of the same nature. Female, aged 56, with high blood pressure,, suffers from pain in the foot (gout), and has had several severe attacks of true angina pectoris, in which the pain is referred over the left chest and through to the shoulder. After the attack she passes a large quantity of clear urine, and the skin and deeper tissues of the left breast and the left sterno- mastoid and trapezius muscles become very tender on pressure. In one attack the pain was felt in a limited area over the second left interspace, and next morning I found the skin and deeper tissues at this place extremely tender to the slightest pressure. Affections of the Circulatory System. 245 In tills instance, also, the hyperalgesia of the skin corresponded to the region where pain was felt, and is presumptive evidence in favour of the view that the pain as well as the hyperalgesia is the result of the viscero-sensory reflex. Male, aged 42, consulted me on October 18th, 1905, complaining of pain in the left little finger and ulnar border of the left arm and forearm, coming on when he exerted himself at his work and on going up hill. For some months he suffered from a dull aching pain at the back over his shoulder blades. I found dilatation of the aorta, slight incompetence of the aortic valves, and sHght enlargement of the heart (verified at the subsequent post-mortem examination). The blood pressure was 150 mm. Hg. During the following week§ the pain in the left arm increased in severity, gradually extended up the arm till it was felt in the axilla, and finally invaded the left chest. At first the pain was wont to start at the little finger and to pass rapidly up the arm, but latterly it seized him with such severity and suddenness that he could not tell where it began. It usually held him with the greatest severity either over the heart or in the inner surface of the left arm immediately above the internal condyle. I par- ticularly asked him to note in his frequent attacks if there was a difference between the arm pain and the chest pain, and his reply was that there was no difference in the character of the pain, but, if any- thing, the arm pain was the worse. Sometimes the pain was very severe up the left side of the neck and behind the left ear. Under treatment he seemed to make considerable improvement. During January, 246 Chapter XXI. 1906, these attacks recurred. During some of them the pain was so agonising that he felt he was dying, and wished to die. The pain was equally severe in chest and arm, and saUva sometimes dribbled from his mouth. On February 12th the least exertion was sufficient to induce a severe attack of pain ; from three p.m. to seven p.m. he was scarcely free from pain, and ultimately became unconscious. When I saw him next day he was having another series of attacks. He was keeping his left arm very still, and helping himself to food entirely with his right hand. He told me he dare not move his left arm, as even the act of lifting the left hand to his mouth was sufficient to induce an attack. His pulse was extremely soft and weak. His blood pressure had fallen to 95 mm. Hg. During the following night he had another series of attacks, became unconscious and expired. 1 omit many details in this case on purpose to emphasise the regions in which the pain was felt in a case of the most severe form of angina pectoris. It seems to me that no other explanation save that of the viscero-sensory reflex can satisfactorily account for the pains in this case, and to attempt to distinguish the chest pain as a heart pain and the pain in the arm as a referred pain would be arbitrary, illogical, and opposed to the e\ddence. The radiation of the pain from the hand to the chest was practically of the same nature as the more common radiation of the pain from the chest to the hand. The pain in the neck and behind the ear, on the same hypothesis, would be induced by the stimulus passing from the heart by the vagus. Affections of the Circulatory System. 247 a view that would also explain the increased flow of saliva during some of the attacks, points to whicli I shall afterwards revert. It is interesting to note here that the movement of the left arm would induce an attack of angina pectoris. I have already pointed out that a stimulus from any source reaching an irritable focus in the cord will cause the characteristic pain to arise. In the next observation the stimulus reached the cord from the skin of the chest. The two following observations of attacks of angina pectoris which I witnessed illustrate true heart pain of the most severe type at places remote from the heart. Male, aged 14, suffering from adhesive medias- tinitis, with enormous enlargement of the heart. The patient lay propped up in bed. As I was gently testing the sensibility of the skin outside and under the left nipple the patient was suddenly attacked with severe pain. He gave a great sob, and leant forward with his left upper arm across his chest, and his right hand pressing it gently. He rocked backwards and forwards with deep sobs, while tears streamed down his cheeks. His pulse became very soft and his face pale, with beads of perspiration on his forehead. In a few minutes the pain sub- sided and he lay back exhausted. Afterwards he said that, on my touching a certain spot, a pain shot from his chest to his arm, and during the whole time the awful pain remained in his arm, and he put his hand over the fleshy part of the upper arm. His doctor told me he had given up examining him by auscultation, because on a few occasions, on applying 248 Chapter X X I. the stethoscope, attacks similar to those I had witnessed were induced. Female, aged 60, complained of pain of agon- ising severity limited to the ulnar border of the left forearm. Coming to see me one day she hiurried to catch the train, and when she reached my con- sulting room she sat down. In a few minutes the pain seized her, and she took up her left arm and nursed it across her breast with evidences of great suffering. The pain subsided in a few moments. and she said she felt as if she would have died. The pain was felt nowhere but in the left forearm. Three months afterwards the patient died from heart failure. At the post-mortem examination there was found marked atheroma, calcification of the coronary arteries and extensive chronic fibrous myocarditis. In this last case the pain was doubtless limited to the highest sensory nerve centre (first thoracic), whose fibres are associated with the sympathetic supply of the heart. 129. Evidences of the Viscero-motor Reflex. — So far I have dealt with the viscero-sensory reflex, and e\ddence no less striking can be found of the viscero-motor reflex among the group of symp- toms included in the term " angina pectoris." Some would limit the term " angina pectoris " to that class of cases where, in addition to the pain, there is a sense of constriction in the chest, amounting at times to a sensation as if the chest were gripped in a \ace, or as if the breast-bone would break. I am convinced that these sensations arise from spasm of the intercostal muscles, and correspond to tlie Affections of the Circulatory System. 249 hard contraction of the flat abdominal muscles in affections of the abdominal viscera. If one watches a case of what is called " muscular rheumatism " where the intercostal muscles are affected, and where these muscles are stimulated by the shghtest movement to violent cramp-like contractions, one cannot but be struck by the resemblance to the description given of the " gripping " sensation experienced by patients suffering from certain affections of the heart. I have watched the attacks in such cases and could find no difference between them and those where the sense of constriction was the chief symptom in heart disease. The viscero-motor reflex may be present alone, or, as is more commonly the case, it may be associated with pain. The purely viscero-motor reflex is seen best in the elderly, where it may be considered as a symptom of one form of the terminal affections of the heart due to arterio-sclerosis or old age. I have found it a precursor of steadily advancing cardiac weakness, and although for a time considerable relief may be afforded, the changes in the heart are so advanced that, in the nature of things, only one end' can be looked for. The three foUowing observations illustrate these views. Male, aged 82, with large tortuous arteries, was seized while Avalldng with a sense of constriction across the chest that compelled him to stand still. These attacks became so frequent and so severe that he could scarcely walk fifty yards before he had to stop and lean against the wall. He described the sensation as one not of pain, but as if somebody gripped the upper part of the chest with a strong 250 Chapter XXI. hand. With rest and suitable treatment these attacks gradually disappeared. Three months later the heart suddenly became irregular (auricular fibrillation), dropsy set in, and he died seven weeks later from heart failure. Male, aged 56, was seized with a spasm which held his chest as in a vice when he walked up a hill. There was no pain but the sense of constriction and a sense of suffocation produced such discomfort that he was forced to stand still. Within a few minutes tlie chest would feel free, but the sensation would at once recur if he attempted further effort of the same kind. Thus, in going to business, he had to go up a steep hill, but frequently found it impossible to do so, and then had to go downhill and reach his destination by another and less steep road. Female, aged 78. Two years before her death she experienced attacks of breathlessness with a sensation of constriction across the chest. This feeling of tightness was so readily set up that she was obUged to stay in bed. The attacks dis- appeared, but recurred again shortly before her death. These latter attacks were accompanied by slight precordial pain. She became gradually weaker and died. At the post-mortem examination the coronary arteries were found markedly thickened, with calcareous patches in their walls. The following experience illustrates the fact that the viscero-motor reflex is a symptom distinct from the pain. Male, aged 48, consulted me on November 25th, 1905, for a pain he felt across the middle of his chest. He had felt a slight pain here for some Affections of the Circulatory System. 251 months on walldng up a hill. He was a master-buMer, and on this day, while watching his men at work, feeUng cold, he began to help them to dig up some earth, to warm himself. He did tliis for a quarter of an hour with a good deal of energy. He then examined a few partially built houses, running up and down a great many steps. On his way home he became conscious of pain in his chest, and as it con- tinued to increase in severity he called on me. I examined him carefully, and found a slight dilata- tion of the heart with an impure first sound. The blood pressure was 130 mm. Hg. On his way home the pain increased in severity, and after he reached home it became very violent. A colleague saw him and prescribed opium, which relieved him. When I saw him next morning he gave a graphic account of his sufferings. He said : " In the tram coming home the pain got worse, and after getting home it became so severe that I felt I was going to die. The pain spread from my chest down my left arm to my little finger. You asked me, when I saw you yesterday, if I felt any gripping sensation, and I did not know what you meant ; but, by George ! I Imow now. When the pain was at its worst, I felt my chest suddenly seized as in a vice, and I rolled on the floor in agony. The pain and the gripping eased off for a time and then came on again. This continued tiU I got the opium. This morning I awoke all right, but at 10.30 that gripping sensation came on and held me tight for ten minutes. I dare not move for fear the awful pain should come on, and I felt every moment it was about to come, and I was in such terror of it that the sweat poured off me." 252 Chapter X X I. With rest and treatment these attacks grew less,tiU he only felt a sUght pain when he over-exerted himself. So far the symptoms I have dealt mth have been mainly concerned with the reflexes connected with the sympathetic nerve supply. Equalh^ instructive symptoms, though less frequent, can be shown to arise from stimulation of the vagus. At its centre in the medulla this nerve is in near rela- tionship to the upper cervical nerves, and, it would seem, more particularly the sensory nerves supplying the sterno-mastoid and trapezius muscles. Not only may these muscles become extremely tender in various heart affections, but the pain from heart affections may be felt in the region of distribution of the cervical nerves as already noted. The following observations also show the same thing. Male, aged 62, complained of great pain striking into his chest and behind Ms ears when walking. Thus, in going to his work, he allowed seven or eight minutes to walk to the station, but nov,^ it took liim over half an hour, as he had to stop on account of the pain every fifty yards. After accurately noting the pain he described it as arising in the left breast, extending across to the right breast, seizing him in the neck, and extending up beliind the ears, where it held him. \^'ith great severity. In showing me the situation, he laid the fingers of both hands over the insertion of the sterno-mastoid muscle into the mastoid process. On one occasion the pain extended from the breasts to the armpits, and down the side of each arm to the elbow. This patient dropped dead while at his work, and on post-mortem, examination T found extreme calcareous degeneration of the coronary arteries. uiffedions of the Circulatory System. 253 130. Organic Reflexes. — Other very striking phenomena are sometimes met with during an attack of angina pectoris. During or after an attack an abundant flow of sahva and the secretion of large quantities of pale urine may occur ; both symptoms I suggest are due to reflex stimulation of nuclei in the floor of the fourth ventricle. Possibly the polyuria may be of the same character as that in diabetes insipidus, which, as is well known, may be induced in animals by puncture of the floor of the fourth ventricle. Male, aged 58, of gouty diathesis, complained of pain, induced by the slightest exertion, which arose in the left breast, passed up the armpit, and extended down the inner surface of the left arm to the Uttle finger. During an attack an abundant flow of sahva took place into the left side of the mouth. These attacks became so frequent that he could only walk a very short distance without inducing an attack. He died while sitting at his desk, and at the post-mortem examination I found that the heart had ruptured, and the coronary artery was very atheromatous. Where the rupture had taken place the m.yocardivmi had nearly disappeared. Male, aged 46, whose work entailed periods of great muscular exertion, complained of pain striking into the chest when walking up a hill. If he stopped as soon as he felt the pain coming on, it passed off, but if he persisted the pain increased to an agonising severity and radiated into both arms, but worse into the left, as far down as the httle finger. At the same time the chest was gripped, 254 Chapter XXI. so that he was forced to straighten himself and to breathe deeply, and at the same time his mouth filled with saliva, and an aching pain was felt in the throat. A few minutes after each attack he had to pass urine, which was always abundant and very clear. These details I had from him after he had carefully noted a number of attacks. 131. Summation of Stimuli the Cause of Angina Pectoris. — The fundamental functions of the heart muscle correspond to those of other in- voluntary muscles that form the walls of holloAv organs ; these functions being modified to suit its special work. Like the other viscera the heart is insensitive when stimulated in a manner that pro- vokes pain when apphed to the tissues of the exter- nal body wall. I have pointed out that a long strong contraction of a hollow organ can produce pain, and that this is undoubtedly the cause of the severe pain associated with renal calculus, gaU-stones, spasm of the bowel, and uterine contractions. Can the heart give rise to pain in a similar manner ? On account of the modification of its functions, the heart cannot pass into a prolonged state of contraction. Imme- diately it contracts, the function of contractility is abolished and the muscle passes at once into a state of relaxation, and for this reason the pain cannot be produced by a " spasm of the heart." But I suggest that the heart muscle may produce pain when it is confronted with work greater than it can readily overcome, a condition which produces strong peri- stalsis and pain in other hollow viscera. But the pain in the heart arises by a slightly different mechanism. A skeletal muscle wiVL contract in obedience to Affections of the Circulatory System. 255 stimulation of a sensory nerve going to the spinal centre of its nerve, if a stimulus of sufficient strength be appUed. If the stimulus be too weak, no contrac- tion follows, but if this weak stimulus be frequently and rapidly repeated, then the muscle contracts in accordance with the law of the summation of stimuli. I suggest that the heart muscle induces pain on the principle of summation of stimuH. If we minutely study our cases we shall find that the pain rarely arises at the fii'st exposure of the heart to the effort that induces the pain. Sometimes effort has been undertaken a few minutes before the pain comes on, and in certain cases it may not come on for hours after the casual exertion has ceased. From such observations we can infer that the heart muscle was exhausted by the exertion, and so ofreat was the exhaustion of the reserve force that it was unable to regain its reserve on cessation of effort ; thus the exhaustion persisted till it culminated in an attack of angina pectoris. ( 256 ) CHAPTER XXIT. ESTIMATION OF THE VALUE OF SYMPTOMS. 132. The Relation of the Symptoms to the General State. 133. Remote Effects of the Lesion. 134. Relation of Symptoms arising from different Causes. 135. The Bearing of Symptoms on Progiiosis. 136. The Bearing of Symptoms on Treatment. In this chapter I endeavour to sum up the general principles which have been applied in more detail in the earlier chapters. That the interpre- tation of symptoms should be imperfect can readily be recognised, for not only is the subject too vast to be dealt with in a few pages, but the knowledge to deal with it efficiently is lacking. I confine myself, therefore, to a few general principles that have been of service to me in my endeavours to estimate the value of symptoms in individual cases. 132. The Relation of the Symptoms to the General State.— Before a final opinion is formed of the value of any symptom, the physician must consider the patient as a whole, and the relation Estimation of the Value of Sipnptoms. 257 of aiw abnormal sign to the general health. The patient's complaint may be of a trivial nature, and the examination of the different organs may reveal no abnormality^ ; nevertheless the contemplation of the patient's whole economy may help one not to dismiss the symptoms too hastily because of their seeming triviality. Malignant disease of the stomach may cause the symptoms of a simple indigestion, but a slight loss in weight and a slight change in the patient's colour ma^^ ultimately be found to indicate the grave nature of the illness. The description of the patient's complaint maj^ be given in such terms that the physician may fanc}^ it is exaggerated and due to the patient being of a neurotic habit. This may be true, yet the complaint from which the patient suffers may have so undermined his strength that the neurotic habit has been induced by his prolonged suffering. It is necessary to state this because the presence of many reflex phenomena is apt to be pooh-poohed, because of the manifest hypersensitiveness of the patient's nervous system. There is no doubt that in people of a neurotic habit there is a greater tendency for reflex phenomena to be readily produced, yet the phenomena should not on that account be ignored, but should be utilised for the purpose of discovering the lesion if possible, and the neurotic tendency being duly discounted, their true value should be estimated. However widespread such symptoms as pain and hyperalgesia may be, there is always some irritation in the cord, induced, as a rule, bj^ some trouble in the viscera or external bod}^ wall. This was well illustrated in the following case, which was under nw observation for many 258 Chapter XXII. years. I attended the patient during several attacks of rheumatic fever from 1880 to 1884. She developed well-marked symptoms of aortic, mitral and tricuspid valvular disease, and was for years very short of breath occasionally, but had no pain or hyperalgesia. In 1895 she began to complain of pain, particular^ after meals, referred to the lower part of the epigastric region, with a limited area of hyperalgesia. The pain became very severe, so that I came to the opinion that she had a gastric ulcer near the pyloric orifice. The hyperalgesia spread widely round to the left chest. She kept in bed for a few weeks, but began to go about her household duties before the pain and hyperalgesia had dis- appeared. She then began to have attacks of pain in the chest on exertion, slight at first, but gradually becoming more severe, until they resembled in every respect attacks of angina pectoris. Coincident with the pain, hyperalgesia appeared in the chest and arm. The distribution of this hyperalgesia, due manifestly to the heart lesion, coalesced with that due to the gastric ulcer, so that there was an exten- sive field of hyperalgesia embracing the left chest and abdomen, from the level of the second rib to below the umbilicus. At the patient's death there were found the lesions of the three valves mentioned, and an ulcer at the pyloric orifice. Anyone seeing the patient after the development of tlie extreme field of hyperalgesia might have attributed the whole condition to some such vague complaint as " neuras- thenia," particularly if the nature of the cardiac lesions were not detected. I have seen another case with very severe attacks of angina pectoris and wide- Estimation of the Value of Symjitoms. 259 spread liyi^eralgesia where no cardiac abnormality could be detected. The extraordinary acuteness and extent of the sensory phenomena might have led to the surmise that there was only a neurasthenic con- dition, yet at the post-mortem examination the coronary artery was found almost impermeable, and the muscle of the heart greatly degenerated. The rule I make in these cases is to recognise the fact that, however exaggerated the reflex symptoms, and however neurotic the patient may be, the symptoms are nevertheless due to actual affection of some viscus, and a careful consideration of all the other features of the case will lead one to an approximately correct estimation of the value of the symptoms. 133. Remote Effects of the Lesion.— Not only may the continuance of a visceral lesion and prolongation of suffering lead to the exhaustion of the patient's nervous system (which is probably the reason for the ease with which the reflex phenomena are produced in many people), but the original ailment, in consequence of persistent suffering, may alter the vvhole mental balance of the patient. Previous to an illness he may be sensible, unselfish, and considerate of his relatives and dependents, but during illness he may become utterly selfish, wear out the patience of his children and dependents with his peevishness and want of consideration, in order that his own requirements and comforts may be satisfied. Patients in this condition are extreme^ difficult subjects for diagnosis, for it is to their interest to magnify their complaints, and it is difficult to estimate the value of their symptoms. This is particularly the case if one searches for 260 Chapter XXII. hyperalgesia of skin or muscle, for they readily complain of pain and tenderness. To discriminate the symptoms in such people the distribution of the sensory phenomena (pain and hyperalgesia) affords a .very good guide. Thus if one tests for hyperalgesia of a heart affection, and the symptoms are found to extend up the chest and over the clavicle, one might reject the symptoms, for the skin over the clavicle as low as the second rib is supplied by the fourth cervical nerve, while below it is supplied by the second thoracic, so that the extension of the hyperalgesia from the second thoracic to the fourth cervical is not conceivable. So it is in other complaints ; the peculiar distribution of the sensory phenomena in affections of any given organ being unknown to the patient, one can test his reliability by noting the distribution of his pain and hyperalgesia. This method of discrimination is also to be used in distinguishing true visceral sensory phenomena in cases of suspected hj^steria and malingering. If the symptoms are found to follow the distribution peculiar to one organ, even if the patient be evidently hysterical, then it may be concluded that there is an affection of the viscus, and it remains to make care- ful consideration of the other factors in the diagnosis in order to estimate what value the symptoms possess. The intensification of reflex phenomena is par- ticularly noticeable where the conditions of living have reduced the bodily strength, through worry, sleeplessness, or improper nourishment. I have been particular^ struck, for instance, with the s^^mptoms Estimation of the Value of Symptoms. 261 of angina pectoris that ma}^ be induced in young people who have had a long and trying period of strain. Women who work hard for their living, or who look after household duties during the day, and have to attend an invalid parent or ailing child during the night, who are frequently disturbed in sleep, or who pass the greater part of the night in constant attendance, become gradually exhausted, and the struggle may go on until an attack of pain in the chest imperatively calls attention to the exhausted heart. In such people the attacks of angina pectoris may be extremeh^ severe, and the hyperalgesia may be widespread, affecting both sides of the chest (the left breast particularly often becoming extremely tender), and, it may be, the neck, particularly the left sterno-mastoid and trapezius muscles. In estimating the value of the symptoms in such cases the history of the patient's life gives an indication, and one can then recognise, with assur- ance, the condition of the heart that has provoked these manifestations. Similar exaggerated sjanptoms, having a peculiar distribution, may arise should any other organ be affected ; as in stomach affections, a slight indigestion may give rise to such s^'mptoms as to make it difficult to tell whether som.e more serious condition, as gastric ulcer, may not be present. In doubtful cases one feature when present ma}^ be taken as a rule to distinguish an affection of an organ from some general nervous manifestation, that is, the viscero-motor reflex. Un- fortunately, this symptom is practically limited to 262 Chapter XXII. affections of the abdominal organs, and needs to be sought for with care, lest a too susceptible super- ficial reflex be started. But when detected it ma}^ be looked upon as demonstrating the presence of some visceral trouble. 134. Relation of Symptoms arising from different Causes. — When a patient presents him- self one may be able, on physical examination, to detect some abnormal condition to which one can refer with certainty the cause of the patient's suffering. It may happen, however, that we detect an abnormality having no direct bearing upon the complaint of the pa,tient, and in the absence of one having such a direct bearing, there is too often a tendency to refer the patient's complaint to the pres- ence of the recognisable abnormality. There is no doubt that symptoms may be provoked by lesions remote from the place where they are ex- perienced, as in referred pain, and there is no doubt that many other symptoms may be produced by lesions of remote organs, as in the widespread effects of kidne}^ disease. But keeping all this in view, there is still need of a wise discretion in estimating the influence of lesions in which there is no recognisable connection with the symptoms. Thus epileptic attacks may be brought on b}^ affections of the heart as in heart-block (Adams-Stokes S3aidrome), where the left ventricle becomes so slow in its action that the brain does not receive a sufficient supply of blood. In consequence of this anaemia the patient may faint or have an epileptic seizure. The weU-recognised connection between a heart abnormality and an epileptic attack in this particular instance has led Estimation of the Value of Symptoms. 263 to the assumption that, if a patient has epilepsy and at the same time has some affection of the heart such as irregular action, there is a connection between the two. Under such circumstances it is necessar}^ to recognise the nature of the irregularity, and as it is now possible to demonstrate with pre- cision the different forms of heart irregularity, the recognition of the particular form of irregular heart action at once permits of determining the probabilities of the heart being the cause of the epileptic attack in any given condition. Irregular action of the heart being so common, the occiurence of epilepsy is in the majorit}^ of cases due to an independent affection, and there is no casual relation necessarily present between the two conditions. I have already referred to errors arising from attributing the cause of symptoms to some demon- strable structural affection which may be merely coincident or independent. It is impossible to lay down rules applicable to all cases, and I write this in order that the subject should in every case receive consideration, since the recognition of the possibility of error may prevent the error being made. 135. The Bearing of Symptoms on Prognosis. — Of all branches of medicine there is none which has received so little real consideration as the matter of prognosis. The subject itself is one which has to be considered in nearly every case that comes under the notice of a medical man. Its im.portance is appreciated in all stages of life, and the just con- sideration of the meaning of symptoms is of cardinal importance in regard to the patient's future. 264 Chapter XX 1 1. In addition to recognising the meaning of any abnormal sign or sj^mptom, we should endeavour to acquire a knowledge of what bearing it has upon the future history of the patient. This knowledge can only be obtained by watching how patients exhibit- ing the abnormality withstand the storm and stress of life. This should be a special object of every general practitioner, for it is he who has the oppor- tunity of watching individual cases over a long period of years, and of estimating the bearing of any abnormality on the patient's future life. I am afraid that our profession as a body does not sufficiently recognise its responsibility in regard to prognosis. When an individual submits himself for an opinion, he does so with such implicit confidence that the verdict given may alter the whole tenor of his life. He may, for instance, be seeking to enter some profession, when a preliminary medical exam- ination reveals what the medical man takes to be an abnormality. An imperfect knowledge of its nature may, and unfortunately often does, lead to its being regarded as presaging possibly grave consequences, and the candidate is rejected. He is thus shut off from the prospect of his chosen calling, and, know- ing the reason of his rejection, passes through life uneasily apprehensive of some impending disaster, whilst all the time the supposed abnormality may be a sign of little or no consequence. If we look at an insurance form we realise the hardships to which applicants are exposed. " Is the pulse regular ? " " Are the sounds of the heart pure ? " "Is the urine free from albumen ? " When such questions are answered in the negative the Estimation of the Valve of Symptoms. 265 applicant may nevertheless be perfectly health}', yet is either rejected or is penalised for life by having to pay a higher preniium, and, in addition, he is burdened with the consciousness of infirmity. I dwell on this matter with some insistence, because I have known of so many instances in which gross injustice has been done to individuals not onh' from a pecuniary aspect, but in having imposed upon them great expense, unnecessary treatment, and mental disquiet, because the meaning and prognostic significance of some simple symptom had not been recognised. A serious responsibility is thrown upon every practitioner at times in advising upon other ques- tions. Should a man give up his business ? is a question upon which advice is constantly sought ; and whether the individual be a statesman or a labourer, the greatest care is necessary in formulating the answer. " Should a woman with some heart affection marr}' ? " or, " If she be pregnant, should the preg- nancy be allowed to proceed ? " are problems that every general practitioner at one time or another will have to meet ; and if he seeks for guidance in the text-books he finds merely vague views which he cannot apph^ to the individual case. This fact alone should arrest the attention of the profession, and make it conscious how insufficient are the indications for an intelligent prognosis. In estimating the value of an}^ abnormal sign, or in determining the condition of the patient, a clear idea must be obtained of the mechanism by which any given symptom is produced, and of the effects that the underlying lesion has upon the 266 Chapter XXII. economy. It is impossible to give here indications that would be of value, partty because the subject traverses the whole field of clinical medicine and partly because I am not competent to deal with the matter, being only impressed with the importance of the subject and the necessit}^ for its further con- sideration. So far as my experience goes, I can only say that one should never base a prognosis upon the presence of a single symptom, but should carefully investigate the effect of any abnormal sign on the functional efficiency of the organ and upon the economy as a whole. Tlie presence of albumen in the urine is often a sign of variable import. In many cases it is a sign of great gravity, and this being recognised, it is too often regarded as being invariably a serious matter. It is now recognised that its significance depends on the conditions induc- ing it, and it may appear when there is no serious affection of the kidney, or when the kidney affection is of such a nature that it may have little effect upon the system. Cases of albuminuria should not therefore be hastily condemned until a complete review of the whole circumstances of the case, such as the history of the iUness, the condition of the other constituents in the urine and their effect upon the cir- culatory system, has been made. In the same way the presence of a cardiac murmur or irregularity, or even an attack of angina pectoris, should never be con- sidered as aif ording grounds for a grave prognosis until the whole circumstances of the case are taken into consideration. I mention these instances merely as indications as to what comse to pursue when doubts arise as to the significance of any abnormal sign. Estimation of the Value of Symj)to7ns. 267 The syinptonis which arise reflexly in like manner liave to be carefully weighed. Intensity of suffering may have no relation to the gravity of the complaint. Toothache causes no anxiety as to the prognosis, though the immediate suffering is severe. Were the cause of the pain not so easily recognised the agonising distress would at times be viewed with the gravest anxiety. Extreme suffering from some trivial disease may be found among the symptoms of many organs. When it is recognised that the most agonising pains are associated with the contraction of non-striped muscular fibres, it will be realised that the cause inducing a contraction capable of calling forth violent pain may be of the most varied kinds, trivial as well as important. Even in the matter of angina pectoris the violence of the pain bears no necessary relation to the gravitj' of the heart complaint. In many cases the onlj^ sensation brought on by exhaustion of the heart muscle may be limited to a mere sense of constriction, and this, if properly appreciated, may indeed be the one sign which calls attention to the serious condition of the heart. I have repeatedly been con- sulted by elderly people for this sense of constric- tion across the chest when they exerted themselves, and in many cases it was the earliest symptom that heralded the termination of the patient's life. On the other hand, some of the most violent attacks of angina pectoris have occurred in people in whom the exhaustion of the heart was but temporary, and the restoration of reserve force resulted in a complete cessation of pain and in permanent recovery. Between these extremes there are many intermediate 268 Chapter XXI I. forms, and it needs a careful inquiry into all the circumstances before a definite prognosis can be given. In regard to the reflex phenomena the tendency to the exaggeration of s3'mptoms by people with a hypersensitive nervous system must always be borne in mind. 136. The Bearing of Symptoms on Treatment. — The due appreciation of the mechanism by which symptoms are produced has a profound influence on treatment. It is often stated that treatment has to be symptomatic, that is, the symptoms are to be treated because the nature of the affection inducing those symptoms cannot be detected. Whole systems of treatment are based upon this idea, and though at times we may be forced to accept this line, it should always be done with regret at our incapacity to recognise the underlying cause of the suffering. A constant endeavour to make out the meaning of these symptoms, which have perforce to be treated without knoAvledge of their cause, will gradually diminish the number of patients who have to be treated symptomatically. As pain is the most common complaint from which so many suffer, its relief is an aim of our treatment. But it should never be supposed that the assuagement of pain is the only object. It has become stereotyped to say " remove the cause," and if this advice had not become so much of a platitude more attention might be given to the " search for the cause." The recognition of the cause can only be attained in the majority of sufferers by a recognition of the mechanism bv which the suffering is produced. Estimation of the Value of Symptoms. 269 Recognising, for instance, tliat contraction of non- striped muscle produces a referred pain, the know- ledge of liow the pain is produced leads to the recog- nition of the hollow viscus producing it. There must be an abnormal stimulus exciting the muscle to con- traction, and experience leads us to conclude what is the most common cause likely to produce the stimu- lation in a particular viscus. A pain with accom- pamdng phenomena located in a certain region in- forms us that the stimulus arises from the gall- duct or the ureter. Experience tells us that a gall-stone or renal calculus is the most frequent caiuse. The absence of s^aiiptoms indicative of smy other lesion confirms this view. On recognising that this is the cause, the question arises : Can we remove it ? In the vast majority of cases this is only possible by surgical operation, and the question of the propriety of this procedure has to be considered. If, as is often the case, this proves inadvisable, then the treatment must proceed on other lines, and the recognition of the mechanism by which the pain arises again gives indications. As it is manifestly due to the strong contraction of non- striped muscle, measures that will relax the contraction of the muscle will naturally be the remedy in this particular case for the time being. On the other hand, if the pain be aroused by some hollow muscular organ, where it is possible by simple means to remove the cause that stimulates the contraction, to this end treatment should be primarily directed. If, for instance, the indications point to the pain arising from painful peristalsis of the boAvel, the inquiry wall proceed to find out the probable nature of the stimulus. If 270 CJiapter XXI I. there has been a histor}- of constipation, or incom- plete evacuation of the bowel, the retention of scybalous masses is suggested as the cause of the peristalsis, and the treatment will be guided to measures that will lead to evacuation. So also with regard to any other viscus that can be emptied, the recognition of the nature of the symptoms affording the best guide for a rational and effective treatment. In an organ, such as the heart, that cannot have the cause of the suffering removed by some mechani- cal process, the consideration of the conditions that induce the symptoms leads to a rational guide in treatment. As I have pointed out, any symptom of suffering points to an exhaustion of the reserve force, whatever be the nature of the functional exhaustion or structural lesion. The recognition of this indicates that treatment in the first instance must be directed to the restoration of this reserve force, and this can be done when consideration of the factors inducing the exhaustion are appreciated, such as over- work, worry, sleeplessness, or the ham- pering effect of some organic lesion. This demands careful investigation into the special features of each individual case. These remarks may seem so evident as to be altogether unnecessarj^, as everyone recognises them. But though as a matter of theory they are the commonest of platitudes, as a matter of practice they are often neglected. If we consider the matter in relation to the treatment of heart affections, for instance, it would be found that, w^hat- ever the nature of the heart failure, a routine method of treatment is, with few exceptions, invariably Estimation of the Value of Symptoms. 271 adopted. Thus lieart failure is supposed to demand ^\llat are called " heart tonics," and the usual treatment is to prescribe the tonic which is dictated by the fancy or the physician or by the fashion of the day. In our inquiries into S3'stems of treatment, sucli as are elaborated at places like Nauheim, it will be found that routine methods are employed, after few or no discriminating inquiries into the peculiar features of each case. One could indefinitely extend illustrations, drawn from other systems, where rule of thumb treatment is followed, to the neglect of the simple and obvious methods suggested by the careful appreciation of the meaning of symptoms. I have already pointed out that treatment may be a factor in diagnosis — so-called diagnosis "ex juvantibus " — the manner in which symptoms react to treatment being often a useful help, as, for in- stance, the use of mercury or iodide of potassium in suspected cases of syphilis. But it is necessary also to appreciate when possible the manner in which the treatment acts. It must be the experience of every- one who has seen a large number of cases of stomach affections, that many cases of great suffering, where there is manifest structural lesion, obtain relief by some simple remedy or change in diet. Thus in pyloric stenosis Avith dilatation of the stomach the patient may have suffered for a long period, and some simple remedy, as bicarbonate of soda, may give instant relief and freedom from suffering for a long time. So also a change in the diet may have the same result. Too often such remarkable experi- ences are mistaken by physician and patient as evidences of the curative value and potency of the 272 Chapter XXI I. drug or system of diet, and so we get the exaggerated praises of different drugs and systems so common nowadays. If it be recognised that some constituent was present in the stomach which occasioned the sufferings of the patient, and that the drug had neutraHsed its effects, or the change of diet had pre- vented its formation, it would have led to a truer appreciation of the benefits obtained by the treatment. It cannot be too strongly insisted upon that the reflex sj^mptoms, which are those that are thus " cured," may arise, not from the actual lesion, but from a susceptibility to stimulation, or from some agent capable of inducing an adequate stimulation, and that the symptoms give no clue to the nature of the stimulation or to the agent causing it. The recognition of the meaning of the reflex phenomena is of much use in so many ways that it is scarcely possible to do more than indicate certain phases of their value. The muscular contraction and hyperalgesia are always indications that some active process is going on. In cases of gastric ulcer, for instance, the treatment may have been so success- ful that the patient's sufferings are relieved and a " cure " is said to have resulted. But a careful examination of the left rectus muscle may reveal tenderness of its upper division with increased tone ; and these symptoms indicate that the stomach lesion is still so active that it keeps up an irritable focus in the cord, and give indication that the treatment should be continued if a permanent recovery is to be attained. Occasionally one meets with cases where this viscero-motor reflex has been recognised and Estimation of the Value of Symptoms. 273 taken as the factor needing treatment. In gall-stone disease, when there is present the tell-tale sign of contracted muscles in the epigastrium, energetic means, as baths, electricit}^ massage, are frequently employed to reduce the " hardness " of the belly wall ! The conception of the nature of the reflex phenomena may give a more direct aim to our therapeutic endeavours. I have already pointed out how the suffering in stomach affections may be due to the accidental presence of an agent that is capable of producing pain, and that remedies may neutralise the effect of the agent without modifying the disease process. It is conceivable that an effect ma}' be produced in other ways, whereby treatment may influence the reflex symptoms at some portion of the chain between the lesion and the mental conception of the suffering. Thus a drug may act upon the muscular spasm when it causes pain, or it may act upon the spinal cord at the level where the visceral nerve stimulates the sensory nerve. It is just possible that it is here the relief is obtained in certain forms of counter-irritation. Thus I have seen a patient with a pyloric ulcer of the stomach obtain relief b}' a blister on the epigastrium over the limited area in which the pain was felt. This blister did not directlv affect the ulcer, because the ulcer did not lie at the same level [see Fig. 16). Nor could one imagine that any reflex effect was produced in the ulcer itself. It seems more probable that the stimulus from the skin affecting directly the peri- pheral distribution of the sensory nerve prevented by some inhibiting process the stimulus from the visGus passing to the sensory cells in the spinal cord. 274 Cha^Jter XXII. This seems the more reasonable, because it was found that on the heahng of the bhster the old pain from the ulcer returned, but if the blistered skin were kept raw by the application of some ointment the pain from the ulcer was subdued. This view receives support from the result of the researches of Sherrington, b}^ which it has been shown that nerve paths may be stimulated from a variety of sources, but that of several contemporaneous stimuli one stimulus may be received and the others inhibited. INDEX. INDEX ABDOMINAL tumour, 178, 205. wall, 63, 203, 205, 273. ABSCESS in lung, 213. subphrenic, 216. ADAMS-STOKES syndrome, 262. ADHESIONS after operation, 204. peritoneal, 175, 203. parietal, 203. Adsceral, 205. AERATION of blood, 214 tissues in heart disease, 211. Index. 277 AFFECTIONS of anus, 175. bladder, 192. circulatory systeiU; 222. digestive organs, 114. female pehac organs, 196. intestines, 166. kidney, 180, 266. liver, etc., 155. lungs, 207. mouth and fauces, 122. ovaries, 198. pelvis of kidney, 182. perineum, 175. pleura, 207^ 217. stomach, 130. ureter, 182. urinary system, 180. uterus, 197. vagina, 199. AIR-HUNGER, 208. AIR-SUCTION in stomach, 1-11. ALBUMINURIA, 180, 181, 266. ALCOHOLIC gastric catarrh, 122. morning sickness, 140. ANGINA PECTORIS, 88, 100, 124, 235, 239, 240 (fig), 243 (fig), 254, 261. cause, 254. ANOREXIA, 119. ANUS affections of, 175. nerve-supply, 115. 278 Index. AORTIC dilatation, case, 245. disease, facial aspect, 107. APPEARANCE OF PATIENTS, 104. APPENDICITIS, .5, 100, 171, 204. APPETITE, 118. perverted, 120. AREAS of cutaneous hyperalgesia in angina pectoris, 240, 243. dilatation of heart and liver, 67, 238. diaphragmatic pleurisy, 219. gall-stone colic, 157. gastric ulcer, 75. peritonitis, 202. pneumonia, 220. renal colic, 185, 191. of eruption in herpes zoster, 241. pain. See " Paix." ARTERIO-SCLEROSIS, 249. ASTHENIA, neuro muscular, 90, ASTHMA, 99, 210. cardiac, 99, 232. ATONIC DYSPEPSIA, 144. AURICULAR FIBRILLATION, 162, 250 AUTONOMIC nerves, diagram, 59. nervous system. 5S, 114. Index. 279 B BACKACHE, 180. BISMUTH in intestines, 167. BLADDER afiections of, 192. calculus in, 186, 193. development, 192. functional symptoms, 195. irritation of, 193. nerve-supply, 192. over-distension, 194. pain in, 118. secretion, 195. structural symptoms, 196. BLOOD aeration of, 208, 214. in f seces, 177. heart disease, 211. BOULIMIA, 120. BOWELS. See " Intestines." BREATHERS mouth-, 126. BREATHLESSNESS, 230. BRONCHITIS, expectoration in, 216 280 Index. CALCULUS in bladder, 186, 193. renal. See " Colic, Eexal." ureteral, site of, 184, 186. CAMPBELL on herpes zoster, 127. CANCER of liver, 165. stomach, 140, 142, 147, 257. CARDIAC asthma, 99, 232. condition, facial aspect, 107. irregularity, 263, 266 murmur, 6, 94, 113, 226, 266. pain, 88. reflexes, 100. valvular imperfection, 94, 225, 242, 258. CASE REPORTED of adhesive mediastinitis, 247. angina pectoris, 242, 244, 245, 247, 248, 250, 253. aortic dilatation, 245. appendicitis, 171. atheroma, 248, 250, 252, 253. auricular fibrillation, 250. calculus, renal, 187, 188, 190. cardiac rupture, 253. cardiac valvular disease, 242, 258. enlargement of heart, 247. fibrous myocarditis, 248. gastric ulcer, 148, 149, 150, 152, 154, 258. mediastinitis. adhesive, 247. Index. 281 CASE REPORTED— co^^iimef/. of perforation of gastric ulcer, 148, 149, 150, 152, 154, 202. peritonitis, 202. pyloric stenosis, 205. renal calculus. colic. See " Colic." rupture of heart, 253. stomach affections, series, 135. ulcer, gastric. See " Gastric." CEREBRO-SPINAL nervous system, 58, 115. CHEYNE-STOKES respiration, 232. CHLOROFORM parturition, 177. CHLOROSIS facial aspect, 108. CHOLANGITIS, 161. CHOLERA stools, 178. CIRCULATION afiections of, 222. enquiry concerning, 112. CIRRHOSIS of liver, 165. CLINICAL OBSERVATION, 22. CLINICAL TRAINING, 25. 282 Index. CLASSIFICATION OF DISEASE, anatomical, 40. clinical, 42. CLASSIFICATION OF SYMPTOMS, 40. COLIC appendicular, 173. gall-stone, 133, 136, 156, 203. intestinal, 117. -like pains, 117. renal, 82, 106, 118, 183, 185, 187, 188, 190. COMPLEXION, 107. 163, 215. CONDITION of organs, 112. patient, general, 109. CONJUNCTIVITIS, 11. CONSCIOUSNESS of heart's action. 228. CONTRACTION of heart-muscle, 256. muscles, 78. non-striped muscle, 183, 267, 269. stomach, hourglass, 145. uterus, 197. COOK on swallowing fluids, 128. COUGH, 212. Index. 283 CRAMP of stoinach, 136, 159. CREMASTER muscle, 183. CURES value of, 272. CUTANEOUS. ^ee"SKTN. DEVELOPMENT of appendix, 175. bladder, 192. liver, etc., 155. tunica vaginalis, 70. DIAGNOSIS, 2. of intestinal affections, 166. stomach afiections, 145. treatment as a factor in, 271. DIAPHRAGMATIC PLEURISY, 219 (fig.), 220. DIARRH(EA facial aspect, 108. DIARRH(JiIC stools, 178. DIETS. 145. 284 Index. DIGESTIVE organs, afiections of, 114. system, inquiry into, 112. tract, areas of pain, 116. functional symptoms, 177. nerve-supply, 114. sensory symptoms, 115. DIGITALIS, 8 DILATATION of aorta, case, 245. heart and liver, 236, 238 (fig.), stomach, 113, 140, 143, 147. DIPHTHERIA, 5. DISEASE classification of, anatomical, 40. clinical; 42. definition of, 11. detection of, 13. DEUGS, 7. DUSKINESS of skin, 107, 215. DYSPEPSIA, 158. atonic, 144. DYSPNCEA, 98, 211. Index. 285 E EAR herpes zoster on, 127. EFFORT SYNDROME, 90. EMPHYSEMA liver in, 165. ENEMA pain of, 71. EPIGASTRIC blister, 152, 273. nerve supply, 134. pain, 116, 135. reflex, 138. EPILEPSY, 262. EUSTACHIAN TUBE sensation in, 125. EXAMINATION of patient, 50, 101. EXHAUSTION, 27, 82. eSect on symptoms, 260. of heart, 87, 90, 224, 229, 232, 267. EXPECTORATION, 215. EXTRA-SYSTOLE of heart, 135, 227, 2.30, 237. cough in, 213, 230. 286 Index. F FACIAL ASPECT in liver affections, 107, 163. respiratory affections, 215. of patients, 107, 215. F.^CES examination of, 177. impacted, 170. FAUCES affections of, 122. in swallowing, 127. nerve supply, 115. FEMALE pelvic organs, 196. FIBRILLATION, AURICULAR, 162, 250. FEVER in gall-stone disease, 161. FISSURE of anus, 176. FLATULENCE, 141. FOCI in spinal cord, irritable, 132, 25' FOOD pain after, 133, 137. retention in stomach. 144. Index. 287 FUNCTIONAL SYMPTOMS, 45. of bladder, 195. digestive tube, 177. gall-stone disease, 160. kidney, 181. liver, 163. respiratory affections, 214. stomach, 142. FUNCTIONS of stomach, 131. heart muscle, 254. FURRED TONGUE, 126. GALL-BLADDER, 155. GALL-STONE colic, 133, 136, 156. pain, 106, 118, 156. in shoulder, 106, 158. GALL-STONE DISEASE, 147. confused with appendicitis, 173. facial aspect, 107. fever in, 161. functional symptoms, 160. gastric symptoms, 136, 158. reflex symptoms, 156, 273. structural symptoms, 160. taken for pleurisy, 159. treatment, 273. GASTRIC catarrh of alcohoHcs, 122. symptoms in gall-stone disease, 136, 158. 288 Index. GASTRIC— CO Jiiinue^. ulcer, 132, 137, 145, 203, 258, 272. perforation, US, 149, 202. pain in, 74, 137, 147, et seq. site of pain, 147, 149 (fig.), 151 (fig.). 153 (^%.). 202. treatment, 273. vomiting in, 139, 140. GASTRITIS, 132. 140, 159. GENERAL CONDITION of patient, 109. GEOPftAGY, 120. GOOSE-SKIN, 102. GRAVES' DISEASE facial aspect, 108. H H./EMORRHOIDS, 175. HALDANE on air-hunger, 208. respiratory trouble, 211. HALLER on insensitiveness of viscera, 65. HEAD on herpes zoster, 127. Index. 289 HEART action, consciousness of, 228. affections. 222. auricular fibrillation. 150, 162. -block, 135, 262. dilatation, 236, 238 (fig.). enlargement, 247. exhaustion, 87, 90, 224, 229, 232, 267. extra-systole, 135, 217, 230, 237. failure, 223, 271. facial aspect, 107. enlarged liver, 162, 164. inquiry into condition, 112. irregularities, 113, 263. -muscle, 254. pain in, 254. response to stimuli, 85, 87. rupture, 253. soldier's, 89. spasm, 254. treatment, 271. valvular imperfection, 94, 225, 242, 258. HEART-BURN, 128, 134, 140, 158. HEPATOPTOSIS, 165. HERNIA mistaken tot hydrocele, 70. strangulated, 169. HERPES ZOSTER, 241, 242. in tonsilitis, 127. HIRST on swallowing fluids, 128. HUNGER, 121. Air-, 208. 290 Index. HYDATIDS of liver, 165, 216. HYPERALGESIA, 257, 260. cutaneous, 80. areas of, 84. (figs.) 67, 75, 157, 185, 191, 219, 238, 240, 243. in affections of respiration, 218. circulatory system, 234, 243, 258. gall-bladder and duct, 157, 203. intestines, 169. liver, 67. ovary, 199. stomach, 75, 138, 202, 258. angina pectoris, 239, 240, 243. dilatation of heart, 238, 239. obstruction of bowel, 170. peritonitis, 202. pleurisy, 218. pneumonia, 220. muscular, 63, 67 (fig.), 80, 160, 203, 218. of testicle, 70. HYPERCHLORHYDRIA, 137, 140. HYSTERIA, 260. ICE pain of swallowing, 135. INDIGESTION, 132, 257. INFANTILE DIARRHOEA stools, 178. Index. 291 INSENSITIVENESS of viscera. See '" Sensibility or Sensitiveness." INSURANCE examination for, 264. INTESTINES affections of, 166. obstruction, 140, 168, 180. pain in, 117 (fig.), 168. peristalsis of, 72, 117, 167, 168, 173. IRREGULARITIES of heart, 113, 263. IRRITABLE FOCI in spinal cord, 132, 257. IRRITABLE HEART OF SOLDIERS, 89, 94. JAUNDICE, 107, 160, 163. K KIDNEY affections of, 180, 266. calculus. See " Colic, Renal. movable, 182. nerve supply, 182, 186, 189. -pelvis and ureter, 182. 292 Index. L LABORATORY OBSERVATION, 22, 37. LABOUR uterus during, 197. LANGLEY on dilatation of pupil, 103. 's diagram of autonomic nerves, 59, 114. LARYNX, 212. LAW OF ASSOCIATED PHENOMENA, 36, 92. LAW OF PROGRESSION, 35. LIVER, 155. area of pain in afiections of, 67 (fig.), enlarged, 20, 161, 164. in heart failure, 162, 164, 236, 238 (fig.). facial aspect in afiections of, 107, 163. nerve supply, 155. painful, 20, 161. symptoms, functional, 163. reflex, 162. structural, 164. LOCALISATION of pain, 75, 106, 135, 147, 167, 169, 184. renal calculus, 184. sensation, 51. LUNG affection of, 207. condition, inquiry into, 112. crepitations at base,, 232. stasis or oedema, 232. Index. 293 M MALARIAL CACHEXIA facial aspect, 107. MALIGNANT DISEASE facial aspect, 107. of stomach, 140, 142, 147, 257. simulated by liver affections, 164. MALINGERING, 260. MARRIS on toxins, 51. MAYLARD on peritoneal adhesion, 204. MEDIASTINITIS adhesive, 247. MERYCISM, 140. MICTURITION, 112, 171, 176, 180, 193. MISCARRIAGE, 198. MORNING SICKNESS, 139. MOTOR REFLEX, 53. MOUTH affections of, 122. -breathers, 126. nerve-supply, 115. 294 Index. MURMUR, CARDIAC, 6, 94, 113. MUSCLES abdominal, hard contracted, 78, 203, 273. simulating tumour, 205. contraction of, 78. creniaster, 183. heart-, 254. intercostal, 218, 248. levator palati, 127. non-striped, 183, 267, 269. of respiration, 209. rectus, 133, 139. MUSCULAR hyperalgesia, 63, 67 (fig.), 80, 110, 203, 218. layer of body wall, sensitiveness of, 63, 203. rheumatism, 220, 249. N NAUHEIM TREATMENT, 271. NAUSEA, 121. NERVES cerebro-spinal, to digestive tract, 115. chorda tympani, 115. cranial, fifth, 115, 123. genito-crural, 186. glosso-pharyngeal, 115. laryngeal, 212. of taste, 115. olfactory, 115. phrenic, 156, 219 (fig.), 221. stimulation of, 103, 274. sympathetic to stomach, 133. thoracic, 53, 134, 241 (fig.), vagus, 133, 134, 156, 252. Index. NERVE-SUPPLY in appendicitis, 172. of anus, 175. bladder, 192. digestive tract, 114. epigastrium, 134. gall-bladder, 155. kidney, 182, 186, 189. larynx, 212. liver, etc., 155. oral orifice, 115. ovaries, 198. perineum, 176. pleura, 219. respiratory muscles, 209. stomach, 133. ureter, 182, 186, 189, vagina, 196. NERVOUS SYSTEM, autonomic, 58, 59 (fig.), 114. cerebro-spinal, 58. constitution, 58. NEURALGIA, 12-: NEURASTHENLA.. See " Neurotic." NEURITIS, 158. NEUROSIS, WAR, 91. NEUROTIC temperament, etc., 109, 141, 182, 229, 257, 258. x2 296 Index. OBSERVATION accuracy of, 20. clinical and laboratory, 22. OBSTRUCTION of intestines, 140, 168. pyloric, 140, 144, 205. OEDEMA pulmonary, 232. (ESOPHAGEAL PAIN, 117. area of, 116. (ESOPHAGUS, 128. nerve-supply, 115. stricture of, 129. ORAL ORIFICE nerve-supply, 115. ORGANIC REFLEXES, 97, 253. ORGANS digestive, aSections of, 114. female pelvic, affections of, 196. review of, 110. sensibility of, 65. ORTHOPNCEA, 231. OVARIES, 198. OVARIAN TUMOURS, 206. OVARY operation on, 113. Index. 291 PAIN, 27. after food, 133, 137. colic-like, 117. definition of, 57 in angina pectoris, 239, 240, 243. appendicitis, 171. bladder, 118. diaphragmatic pleurisv, 219. digestive tract, 116 (fig.). gall-stone disease, 118, 156. gastric ulcer. See " GtASTric Ulcer." heart affections, 254. intestine, 117, 168. liver, 66. movable kidney, 182. o?sophagus, 117, 128. ovarian trouble, 198. peristalsis of intestine, 72, 117, 168. pleurisy, 208, 218, 219 (fig.). pneumonia, 220. renal colic. See " Colic, Renal." shoulder, 106, 158, 219, 221. stomach, 117, 134, 135, 137, 147. testicle, 68, 82, 106, 183, 186. tunica vaginalis, 69. ureter, 184, 186. uterine labour, 197. uterus, 118. vagina, 199. localisation of, 75, 106, 135, 147, 167, 169, 184. radiation of, 52, 55. referred, 176, 182, 192, 197. relief of, 268. sensitiveness of tissues to, 61. visceral, 15, 68, 118. artificial production, 71. mechanism, 74. 298 Index. PALATAL MUSCLES, 127. PALLOR of face, 108. PALPITATION, 229. PARTURITION perineal reflex, 176. PATIENT appearance, 104. examination, 104. facial aspect, 107. general condition, 109. sensations, 105, 207. PELVIC ORGANS female, 196, PELVIS OF KIDNEY affections of, 182. PERFORATION of stonmch, 148, 149, 150, 152, 154, 202. PERINEAL REFLEX, 176. PERINEUM, 175. torn, 176. PERISTALSIS of intestine, 72, 117, 167, 168, 173. oesopliagus, 129. stomach, 134, 137, 140. ureter, 82, 184, 186. PERITONEAL ADHESIONS, 175, 201. parietal, 203. visceral, 205. PERITONEUM sensitiveness of, 64, 201. Index. 299 PERITONITIS, 64,171,201. PERNICIOUS AN.EMIA facia] aspect, 107. PHRENIC NERVE, 156,219,221. PHTHISIS cough, 213. expectoration, 216. PILES, 175. PILOMOTOR REFLEX, 102. PLEURA affectionsof, 207, 217. PLEURISY, 217. and gall-stone disease, 159. cough in, 213. diaphragmatic, area of pain, 219 (fig.), 220. effect on liver, 165. PNEUMONIA cough, 213. expectoration, 216. facial aspect, 108. pain, 220. stools, 178. POLYURIA, 253. PREGNANCY vomitingin, 139, 199. PREGNANT UTERUS, 197. PROGNOSIS, 4, 37, 263. 300 Index. PULMONARY apoplexy, expectoration, 216. condition, facial aspect, 107, 215. cEdema, 232. stasis, 232. PULSE irregular, 227. observation of, 25. PYLORIC STENOSIS, 140, 144, 205. PYROSIS, 140. R RADIATION IN DISEASE, 55. RAMSTROM on histology of abdominal wall, 64, 203. RAYNAUD'S DISEASE, 102. RECTUM ulceration of, 176. RECTUS MUSCLE, 133, 139. REFLEX cardiac, 100. epigastric, 138. motor, 53. organic, 97, 253. perineal, 176. pilo-motor, 102. secretory, 99. skin or superficial, 138. symptoms, 48, 132, 156, 159, 162, 172, 176, 19-3, 199, 212, 234, 257. vaso-motor, 102. viscero-motor, 77, 139, 234, 248. viscero-sensory, 74, 234, 236, 239. Index. 301 EEFLEXES in angina pectoris, 235. appendicitis, 172. gall-stone disease, 156. kidney affections, 180. liver affections, 162. parturition, 176. respiratory affections, 212. stomach affections, 138. visceral disease, 74, 81 (fig.)- KEGURGITATION from stomach, 128, 134, 140. REMOTE EFFECTS of lesion, 259. RENAL calculus colic. See " Colic, Renal." nerve-supply. See '" Nerve-Supply. RESEARCH, 9, 17. RESISTANCE TO STIMULATION, 84. RESPIRATION, 209. Cheyne-Stokes, 232. with failing heart, 201, 232. RETENTION of food in stomach, 144. urine, 176, 193. RHEUMATISM, 158, 220, 249. RIGIDITY protective, 78. 302 Index. S SALIVA flow in angina pectoris, 253. SCHLESINGER on swallowing fluids, 128. SECRETIONS of bladder, 196. respiratory tract, 213, 215. stomach, 142. SECRETORY REFLEXES, 99. SENSATION radiation of, 52, 54. SENSATIONS in afiections of lungs and pleura, 207. of patient, 105. SENSIBILITY OR SENSITIVENESS of female pelvic organs, 196. muscular layer of body-wall, 63. organs, 65. peritoneum, 64, 201. pleura, 217. tissues, 61. viscera, 62. SHERRINGTON, PROFESSOR on radiation of pain, 52. stimulation of nerves, 103, 274. SHOULDER-PAIN, 106, 158, 210, 219 (fig.), 221, SICKNESS morning, alcobolic, 140. Index. 303- SITE OF PAIN. See " Area," and " Localisation." SKIN duskiness, 107, 215. goose-, 102. hyperalgesia. See " Hyperalgesia." -reflex, 138. sensibility, in, 62. SNEEZING, 2U. SOLDIERS HEART, 89, 94. SPASM in swallowing, 129. of tlie heart, 254. intercostal nucleus, 248. SPINAL cord, irritable foci, 132, 257. STASIS pulmonary, 232, STENOSIS of cardiac valves, 242. pyloric, 140, 144, 205. STIMULATION mechanism of, 51. resistance to, 84. sensitiveness to, 61. threshold of, 85. STIMULI summation of, as cause of angina pectoris, 254. 304 Index. STOMACH affections of, 130. irritable focus in, 132. reflexes in, 138. cancer of, 140, 142, 147, 257. constriction of, 145. cramp, 136, 159. diagnosis, 145. dilatation of, 113, 140, 143, 147. functions, 131. functional symptoms, 142. hyperalgesia from, 138. malignant disease, 140, 142, 147, 257, nerve-supply, 133. pain, areas, 117, 134, 135, 137, 147. pain with gall-stone colic, 106. perforation, 148, 149, 150, 152, 154. peristalsis, 134, 137, 140. pyloric stenosis, 140, 144, 205. regurgitation from, 128, 134, 137, 140. retention of food, 144. secretion, 142. structural symptoms, 144. treatment, 145, 271. tumour, 144. ulcer. See " Gastric Ulcer." STONE in bladder kidney See " Calculus. STOOLS character of, 178. STEICTURE of oesophagus, 129. lyidex 305 STRUCTURAL SYMPTOMS, 43. of bladder, 196. digestive tube, 178. gall-stone disease, 160, kidneys, 181. liver affections, 164. respiratory affections, 217. stomach affections, 144. SUFFOCATION sense in heart affections, 233 SWALLOWING, 127. hot and cold fluids, 128. ice, pain in, 135. in angina pectoris, 124. painful, 125. spasm in, 129. SYMPATHETIC NERVES. See " Autonomic. to stomach, 133. SYMPTOMS bearing on prognosis, 263. treatment, 268. classification of, 33. definition of, 14. differentiation of, 31. functional, 20. See " Functional." mechanism of, 92. methods of investigation of, 14. of affections of organs. See " Affections." reflex. See " Reflex." relation of, 256, 262. sensory, in digestive tract, 115. structural. See " Structural." value of, 256. SYSTEM, NERVOUS. See " Nervous." ,306 Inder. T TACHYCARDIA, 162, 230, 236. TEETH false, inflammation from, 125. TEMPERAMENTS, 109. neurotic, 109, 141, 229, 257, 258. TESTICULAR PAIN, 68. in renal colic, 82, 106, 183, 186. TIC DOULOUREUX, 123. TONGUE, 126. TONSILITIS, 127. TONSILS inflamed, 124. in swallowing, 127. TOOTHACHE, 55, 122, 267. TREATMENT, 7. as factor in diagnosis, 271. bearing on symptoms on, 268. of affections of heart, 271. stomach, 145, 271. TRIGEMINAL NEURALGIA, 123. TUMOUR abdominal, 178, 205. in stomach, 144. ovarian, 206. TUNICA VAGINALIS, 69. TYPHOID FEVER facial aspect, 108. stools, 178. Index. 307 U ULCER gastric. See '" Gastric." ULCERATION of pelvis of kidney, 184. rectum, 176. ureter, 184. UMBILICAL FISTULA pain in operation for. 72. URETHRAL CARUNCLE, 200. URETER afEections of, 182. nerve-supply, 182, 186, 189. peristalsis, 184, 186. site of stone in, 184, 186. URINARY SYSTEM affections of, 180. inquiry into, 112. URINE albumen in, 180, 181, 266. retention of, 193. secretion, in angina pectoris, 253. UTERUS, 197. pain in, 118, 196, 197. VAGINA, 199. nerve-supply, 196. VAGUS NERVE, 133, 134, 156, 252. VALUE of symptoms, 256. 308 Index. VALVULAE IMPEEFECTION of heart, 94, 225, 242, 258. VASO-MOTOR REFLEXES, 102. VISCERA hollow, pain in, 118. * insensitiveness of, 65. VISCERAL disease, cutaneous hyperalgesia, 84. pain in, 74. pain, 15, 68, 118. artificial production, 71. mechanism of, 74. reflexes, 74, 81 (fig.). VISCERO-MOTOR REFLEX, 77, 139, 234, 248. VISCERO-SENSORY REFLEX, 74, 234, 236, 239. VOMITING, 82, 98, 139, 163, 199. W WAR NEUROSIS, 91. WATER-BRASH, 140. WILSON, R.M., 89 on toxins, 51. X-RAYS in examination of digestive tract, 143, 167. London: Printed by Shaw and Sons, Fettek Lane, E.C.4. APPENDIX. THE THEORY OF DISTURBED REFLEXES IN THE PRODUCTION OF SYMPTOMS OF DISEASE. Sir JAMES MACKENZIE, M.D., F.R.S., etc., DIEECTOR OF THE ST. ANDREWS CLINICAL INSTITUTE. [Reprinted by land 'permission of the "British Medical Journal."] Whex we started the Institute for Clinical Research in St. Andrews we recognised that if success were to attend our efforts we must have a clear comprehension of our object, and a definite idea how that object should be attained. We define our object as the Prevention of the Diseases that are Common amongst the People. To understand how disease should be prevented it was necessary to know what are the diseases which are common among the people. On inquiry it was found that, except in a small percentage of cases, this knowledge was nowhere to be obtained. Here, then, the first step to be taken was to know what the diseases are which we wished to prevent. The next question was how to proceed to get this knowledge. The great majority of the sick suffered from complaints, and showed signs of ill health, but the disease was unknown. In a few cases the disease could be recognised, because medical know- ledge had advanced so far as to detect certain signs and symptoms which experience had shown to be due to definite causes. Seeing that the diseases we wished to prevent were for the most part unrecognisable, it would be futile to attempt to prevent them, so that it was manifest an undertaking must start with preliminary inquiry into the nature of the diseases. , 310 Appendix. Definition of Disease. In order that our inquiry should be pursued in a logical and systematic manner, we sought to get a clear grasjj of what we meant by disease. The definition of an abstract term such as disease would probably be given in different terms, according to the standpoint of the definer. After much consideration and discussion we analysed the phenomena in over 1,000 cases and worked out the following scheme, which revealed the present state of knowledge on the subject, and at the same time afforded a guide for our inquiry. If we take a simple disease like conjunctivitis, where the cause can be ascertained, we find that the diseased state consists of a foreign body which injuriously affects the tissues, causing certain reactions or symptoms, as pain, lacrymation, redness, blepharospasm. Such a condition may be considered a complete diagnosis and can be represented by the diagram (Fig. 1), where a is the injurious agent and the surrounding circles are the reactions or symptoms. Fig. 2. Fig. 1 represents a disease completely diagnosed, where A is the agent acting injuriously on the tissues and producing reactions or symptoms, which are represented by the circles at the periphery— as tj-phoid fever, or conjunctivitis due to a foreign body in the eye. Fig. 2 represents a disease not fully diagnosed, where, while the symptoms are recognised, the injurious agent a has not been reeogmsed, though its nature can be inferred, as in measles. We found in our inquiry that the complete diagnosis is possible only in a relatively few cases— where the injurious agent has been recognised as due to a foreign body or to a microbe, as m typhoid fever, pneumonia, diphtheria. In some cases the reactions occur in such definite groups that they can be differentiated, and it can be assumed with reasonable Appendix. 311 certainty that the disease is caused also by a microbe which has not yet been identified, as nieasles, small-pox, rabies, etc. These are represented in Fig. 2, in which the injurious agent is represented by a dotted ring. There were other cases in which the group of symptoms were also capable of differentiation, but in which the nature of the injurious agent was unknown, or a matter of speculation, as in migraine, epilepsy, diabetes, etc. This is represented in Fig. 3 where a point of interrogation represents the injurious agent. The bulk of patients suffer from diseases in which, so far, it has been found impossible either to recognise the agent or to arrange the symptoms into groups that permit of their clear differentiation from others that they resemble. The nomenclature of such conditions is usually based on the presence of a dominant symptom or of a number of symptoms associated with some organ, auch as anaemia, neuralgia, neurasthenia, debility, disordered action of the heart, indigestion. The knowledge of the disease in those cases may be represented diagrammatically (Fig. 4) as a confused heap of symptoms in which it is not possible to obtain a suggestion as to their relation to a common cause. ? o O o O o Fig. 4. Fig. 3. Fig. 3 represents a disease with a well-defined group of symptoms, Ijut where tlie nature of the agent is not recognised — as migraine, con- vulsions. Fig. 4 represents the vast majority of diseases, where a number of unco-ordinated symptoms are jiresent with no recognisable cause. To the latter group belong probably all chronic diseases, even though the ill health is definitely due to damaged organs which can be recognised by physical signs — as arterio-sclerosis, chronic heart disease, chronic kidney disease, and even consumption and cancer. The reason for including these is that they are prob- ably secondary diseases, the original cause not being capable of recognition. 312 Appendix. The Relation of Symptoms to Disease. This method of looking upon disease reveals not only the state of our knowledge of diagnosis, but gives a guide in research. It shows that disease is only revealed by the symptoms it produces, and that even in the few diseases where the nature of the agent is understood it can only be i'dentified in the human body by its symptoms. A consideration of the history of the recognisable diseases shows that the first step in research is to recognise the symptoms. The next step is to group them together, so that one group can be clearly differentiated from those that resemble it. By this method the clinical observer differentiates the disease, it may be, without recognising the cause. Having reached this stage, he can present a clear-cut problem to, for example, the bacteriologist, who may complete the diagnosis by discovering the injurious agent. The symptoms of the majority of diseases have not yet been clearly difierentiated, and there is little prospect of progress being made until this has been done. This brief review of our present knowledge of disease shows that the chief aim of medical science — the prevention of disease — can never be achieved till we are able to recognise the diseases. "Where success has been achieved and disease prevented, it was due in the first instance to the recognition of the symptoms. The clinical observer who obtained this knowledge succeeded because the symptoms were striking in character and easily recognised — as by the presence of a rash or eruption or other sign. It is reasonable to conclude that if we had a better understanding of the mechanism by which less obtrusive symptoms are produced we would get nearer to the detection of the agent producing them. When a person falls ill, the fact is made evident only by the symptoms, so that the only way in which the disease can be detected is by the recognition of symptoms. The study of symptoms has been carried on since the dawn of medicine, and from the early days additions have continually been made to their numbers, until to-day the number is so great that no one individual can recognise more than a fraction of them, and methods are ever being invented for the discovery of new symptoms. If our enterprise were but to seek for new symptoms, it would be in vain, for the addition of new symptoms would but add to the confusion of a subject already far too chaotic. Appendix. 313 The Evolution of a Science, All subjects that have attained the position of a science have passed through stages of which the collection of facts was the first. So long as these facts were unco-ordinated their accumulation tended to confusion, and there came to be recognised the need for an ordered arrangement based upon a law of nature. The history of chemistry is an illustration. The appreciation of Dalton's atomic theory gradually enabled the mass of detail to be classified according to a law of nature. Not only this, but it opened up possibilities and gave guidance to further re- search, and so enabled the science of chemistry to make remarkable progress. Symptomatology is in the same confused state as chemistry was before the recognition of the atomic theory ; and it calls as urgently for a classification based upon some law of nature that reveals the mechanism of the production of symptoms. The Classification of Symptoms. Hitherto there has been no clear conception of the nature of symptoms, and the classifications that have been made have hampered progress and misdirected the course of research. Thus symptoms are usually described from the standpoint of the organ giving rise to them, and specialists devote themselves to the study of the symptoms of individual qrgans or systems. When a person falls ill nearly every organ of the body may be disturbed, and each specialist has no difficulty in detecting symptoms belonging to his particular branch. In consequence of this we find that confusion of diagnosis M^hich results when a patient consults a number of specialists. Another source of error arises from the fact that when one organ is diseased its impaired function is revealed not by the organ at fault but by its effects upon the other organs of the body. It is now many years since I saw the need for a knowledge of the mechanism by which symptoms were produced. I have steadily pursued the subject, and several years ago I dimly recognised that there was some definite law governing this mechanism. I attempted a classification and I was able to group a number of symptoms according to their mechanism, such as a structural group, recognised by a physical sign, a functional group due to interference with the functions of an organ, and a reflex group, due to the peculiar stimulus setting up definite reactions. But there remained a large group, which I recognised somehow or other belonged to the last group, but their mechanism was not clear. 314 Appendix. This was the state of knowledge when we began our inquiry. We recognised that it was useless to attempt a research into any particular disease until we had acquired a knowledge of the laws that underlay the production of symptoms, and to that end our chief endeavour was given. We met twice a week ; at one meeting we discussed the nature of one particular symptom, and sought for an explanation in the daily routine of examining patients. At the other weekly meeting we discussed in detail the symptoms of individual patients. The Law Governing the Prodaction of Symptoms. As time went on we got a better insight into one»symptom after another, and we felt we were getting nearer the law of which we were in search, and gradually its recognition dawned upon us. At first we scarcely recognised it when it was put before us because of its extreme simplicity, for we found the law to be that the vast majority of the symptoms of disease are distdirhances of normal reflexes. I know quite well that no one who reads this will accept at present the view that this theory will do for clinical medicine what the atomic theory did for chemistry, but I know equally well that when the method of applying it is understood its significance will be appreciated, and it will have a very far-reaching influence on the progress of medicine. How the Theory was Discovered. The discovery of this law is the outcome of a long and pains- taking search wdth a definite object in view. Shortly after entering general practice, over forty years ago, I was impressed with my ignorance of the diseases from which my patients suffered. In the majority the evidence of ill health was confined to the patient's sensations, which I could not interpret. Eealising this, I resolved to see if I could not improve my know- ledge, and began to pay more attention to such signs as I could detect, and the principal sensations of w^hich the patient complained. I had not gone far when I realised that these signs and symptoms were so numerous that I could only hope to investigate a limited number. But even when I had selected a few for particular observation I did not know what method to pursue. On reflection I decided upon concentrating my attention on two aspects : (1) The mechanism by which a symptom ivas produced ; and (2) the bearing the cause of the symptom had on the patient's future. Of these aspects the latter is, of course, of the utmost practical value to a doctor, and although under the term " prognosis " it Appendix. 315 has received perfunctory attention, I found that neither the manner in which the subject should be studied nor the individual whose opportunities fitted him for the task had been realised ; but with that I do not deal here. The study of the mechanism by which symptoms were produced led me into several different fields. There was, for instance, the pulsatile movements in the jugular veins and the liver, the movements of the heart in health and disease, and the mechanism of irregular heart action to be considered. I devoted several years to finding out the mechanism of these signs, and the results were an ample reward for the labour and time spent, and helped to make that revolution which has made of human cardio- logy a totally different subject from what it was when these researches were begun. The study of the sensations of the patient was much more difficult. I began with the most clamant of all — pain. Hilton's book on Rest and Pain was the furthest step that had been taken, but valuable as it was it did not go to the root of the matter. For some ypars I made little progress — collecting isolated facts but unable to see any reasonable explanation for their causation. In 1888 Dr. James Ross published his article on referred pain, and I at once recognised that he had provided a hypothesis which not only helped to explain much that was obscure but gave a guide for carrying the subject further. In this paper he stated that pain, when arising in an organ, was felt in two ways — first by a pain in the organ itself, and second by the pain felt in the external body wall at a place remote from the organ. The first of these he called splanchnic and the second he called somatic. I had already gone so far in my inquiry that I recognised the significance of his somatic or referred pain, but I was doubtful of his splanchnic, considering that it also was really referred. He used as an illustration the pain of gastric ulcer when limited to the epigastrium as being a splanchnic pain, but when I examined a patient with a gastric ulcer I found that on a deep inspiration, while the stomach descended, the pain remained fixed. Though suggestive this was far from convincing, and I sought diligently for other signs, and in 1891 I found a sign which threw much light upon the matter. This was finding a large area of hyperalgesia of the skin over the liver in a patient after an attack of gall-stone colic. This led me to seek for cutaneous hyperalgesia in other cases, and I soon found that it was of frequent occurrence, particularly in affections of abdominal organs. It required, however, a long training to acquire the skill to detect it in many cases. 316 Appendix. Shortly after this another sign was recognised — namely, the contraction of the muscles of the abdominal wall, due to disease of an abdominal organ. It had been recognised before, and spoken of as " protective rigidity," but its real nature and significance had never been understood. I watched how portions of the muscle wall of the abdomen would becone hard and resistant during the course of a disease, such as gastric ulcer, apj)endicitis, gall-stone disease, cholecystitis. I observed it disappear with imj^rovement, until it was not evident, and I could recall it by gently rubbing over it. I noted in patients the sudden contraction of muscle that ensued under certain conditions. Thus, some of my patients described that on the onset of an attack of renal colic the testicle would be dragged up (by contraction of the cremaster muscle). I described these * symptoms of pain and hyperalgesia in consequence of disease of the viscera as a viscerosensory reflex, and the contraction of the muscle as a viscero-motor reflex. There was a large group of symptoms which occurred when l^atients fell ill from some obscure cause (as infection), such as the feeling of exhaustion, loss of appetite, breathlessness on exertion, palpitation, vomiting, mental depression. These had been vaguely described as toxic symptoms, and I had indeed described them as due to a hypersensitiveness of certain parts of the central nervous system, but I did not clearly realise the mechanism which produced them ; this was the stage reached when we started the Clinical Institute, and the matter was summarised in an address I gave on the " Soldier's heart and war neurosis : a study in symptomatology. ' " ' The Application of the Theory. In the description of the theory and its application which follows, some simple observations are given to show the manner in which certain reflexes are disturbed ; while we have found it already of value in the interpretation of our patients' symptoms, we recognise that there is required a much better knowledge of reflexes before the theory can be used in a systematic manner. The illustrations therefore are merely to indicate how it may become of use. While the stimulus and response parts of a reflex may often be recognised, the mechanism of that part of the reflex in the central nervous system is not yet quite clear. Consequently the diagrams of this j)art of the reflex do not pretend to represent the actual mechanism. In attempting so to explain the theory that symptoms arise from disturbed reflexes as to carry conviction we recognise that we are handicapped in that what we take to be facts may not be Appendix. 317 accepted as facts by the reader, and it would take up too much space and time to give our reasons. When, for instance, I state that exhaustion of heart muscle produces pain and proceed to develo]) the argument in which that statement is accepted as a fact, the reader" may not accept the statement. The observations on which that statement is based have been carried on for many years, during which patients have been watched and the circum- stances provoking the pain have been considered, the gradual progress of the case observed, and the conditions at the posf- mortem examination carefully noted. Another long series of observations have been made to discover the laws that govern the production of pain in all muscular organs — a research, indeed, in which we are still engaged, and which every day tends to confirm the view advanced here. In like manner, when I take it as a fact that exhaustion is due to a disturbance of a vasomotor reflex, it is not possible to give the long series of observations on which the statement is based. Until we are able to publish the researches into these and other matters involved in this description, I would ask the reader to assume that our facts are proven, so that the explanation of this theorv mav be understood. Fig. 5. — Diagram of a simple reflex arc. The stimulus affects the receptor a, from which a nerve passes to a nerve cell at b, where it joins another cell which sends an efferent fibre to c — the effector or structure ivhich gives the response. Description of a Reflex Arc. In the reflex arc we recognise the following parts (Fig. 5). j^. The receptor or place where the reflex is initiated. An afferent nerve fibre conducts the stimulus to b, the nerve centre. Here the -afferent nerve ends in a synapse with one or more nerve cells — the actual number and relationship varying in complexity. The nerve centre in turn sends an efferent fibre to c — the efiector or structure, be it muscle or gland or centre of consciousness, which .gives the ultimate response characteristic of the reflex. The whole of this arc is necessary for the production of a simple reflex. Symptoms, or, in other words, alterations in the 318 Appendix. reflex, may be referable to causes acting upon any part of the arc-, but in practice they are found to be mainly confined to alteration; in A; the receptor ; B.. the nerve centre ; or c, the effector. The Part of the Reflex in Life. The whole economy of the body is built up of a system of organs whose activities dej^end on reflexes. A flood of stimuli is- continually being poured into the system through the skin and special senses, and each stimulus produces a definite reflex on some part of the body. In the deeper tissues of the body stimuli are continually arising and being sent to difierent- organs, which respond by some modification of their activity. The sources of stimulation, therefore, arise in every part of the body. Organs that at one time are the source of stimulation become at other times the effectors. Thus, when pain is felt, the- source (a part of the reflex) may be the peripheral distribution of any sensory nerve, while the brain is the organ which responds (the c part of the reflex). The difierent organs of the body during periods of activity and quiescence send out stimuli-producing reflexes which modify the activities of other organs — as the heart when it increases its activity in responding to a call for more blood, or decreases its activity in a cessation of effort. The interplay of these reflexes results in the harmonious action^ of the organs of the body which we recognise as " health."' The disturbance of one or more of these reflexes results in a disharmony which we recognise as symptoms of disease. Methods by which Disturbed Reflexes are Produced. There are two ways in which reflexes may be disturbed : 1. By the nature of the stimulus acting on the a part of the reflex arc, the impulse entering through the nervous system. 2. By altering in a positive or negative sense the receptivity of the B and c parts of the reflex, the agent, (chemical or thermic) entering through the blood stream. Disturbance of Reflexes due to the Stimulus Entering through the Nervous System. The following observations sufficiently illustrate the- mechanism by which this kind of disturbed reflex is produced. Appendix. 319 Many years ago I had shown that the only serous membrane in which a sensation of pain could be produced was the visceral layer of the tunica vaginalis. Observation 1. Dr. Orr, in tapping a hydi-ocele, tested this statement and noted the sensitiveness of the parietal and visceral layers of the tunica vaginalis. He found he could scratch the parietal layer and the patient feel nothing, but when he touched the visceral layer with the trocar the patient (1) drew back, (2) felt severe pain, (3) became pale, (4) nauseated, and (5) collapsed. Observation 2. A man was seized with an attack of renal colic and suffered violent pain in the left side of the abdomen, with contraction of the muscles. He became pale, and sweat poured out ; he vomited, and the pulse became soft, and he felt faint. In Observation 1 the touching of the visceral layer of the tunica vaginalis was the stimulus at the receptive or a part of the reflex. Then followed a variety of responses or effects on the c part of the reflex : (1) A muscular response in the sudden drawing back of the body ; (2) the sensation of pain — the centre of con- sciousness being the response part of the reflex ; (3) pallor — a cardiac reflex— due to a stimulation of the vagus, which also probably caused (4) the nausea, and (5) collapse, due to a vaso- motor reaction. In Observation 2 we have a similar series of reflexes, these being in addition, the vasomotor reflex causing the sweating, and the vomiting reflex. Alteration of the Receptivity of the B and C Parts of the Reflex Arc. The other method, by altering the b and c portions of the reflex arc, comes about through the circulation in the blood of certain agents which affect parts of the arc in a negative or positive way — that is, either increasing or diminishing the intensity of the impulses during their passage from A to c. The demonstration of the disturbance of the reflexes through the nervous system is readily understood from such observations as 1 and 2. The demonstration of the circulatory — chemical or toxic — influences is not so easy. Perhaps the best way is to consider the effects of certain drugs whose effects have been sufficiently observed. Observation 3. — The Effect of Atropine. An individual who receives a certain quantity of atropine may complain of impaired vision and palpitation. When examined he is found to have a dilated pupil and an increased heart rate, which is abnormally increased on effort. 320 Appendix. Each of these effects is due to the drug blocking the passage of normal impulses at c. The pupil of the eye is maintained in an ever- varying balance between dilatation and contraction, respond- ing with great sensitiveness to the stimulus of light. This is because of two reflexes, one presiding over the dilator muscles of the pupil and the other over the sphincter muscles. The atropine blocks the passage of the normal impulses to the sphincter at c and the balance is upset. The heart's activity is regulated in the same way by two sets of nerves, acceleration of rate by the sympathetic, and slowing of rate by the vagus. The heart is like the pupil, ever ready to respond to a demand for more or less blood from the tissues, according to their ever-varying activity. The atropine acts by paralysing the peripheral end of the vagus, or by depressing the c end of the reflex arc, and the uncontrolled sympathetic fibres cause the increased rate. Fig. 0. — Diagram of a mviscular reflex. The stimulus from to B, where it affects the nerve cells supj^lying the muscles c and c'. In response to the stimulus the muscle at c contracts, while the muscle at c' relaxes. The signs + and — indicate the different actions. Observation 4. — The Action of Strychnine. Tn the reflex action of muscles in causing the movement of a joint there occvirs normally at the same time as the contraction of the muscles a relaxation of the opposing muscles. Thus in Fig. 6 the stimulus arising at A, is diverted at b to c, which causes a contraction of the muscle, and to c', causing a relaxation of the muscle. These reactions are shown by -f and — . The effect of strychnine in producing convulsions is by converting the inhibition of the muscles which takes place in a reflex movement of a joint into a contraction of these miiscles, as in Fig. 7, where both reactions are seen, represented by + sign. This observation shows the influence of an agent modifying the reflex at the B portion of the arc, and explains the mechanism of the convulsions in rabies as well as in strychnine poisoning. It is to be noted that neither the strychnine nor the toxin in rabies can by themselves initiate a stimulus for the production of a reflex— this arises always from some part of the body through the A portion of the reflex arc, as at the skin. This fact shows that Appendix. 321 the drug does not cause convulsions, but only modifies the reflex in such a way that the stimulus from A produces the convulsions. These methods of action probably explain the action of all drugs or toxins which are supposed to act on centres. A great number of drugs are credited with causing vomiting for instance, but the manner in which they act is to render the B or c portion of the arc more susceptible to a stimulus so that a normal stimulus passing into the nervous system affects certain reflexes which have been rendered unduly sensitive. Fig. 7. — Diagram illustrating the effects of strychnine. The response from the stimvilus arising at a affects the cells at b differently, so that while the muscle at c contracts the muscle at c' does not relax, but also contracts. This is shown by the — sign in Fig. 6 being now a + sign. The following observation shows how a normal act can affect the reflex at b when it is rendered hypersensitive by an agent such as a drug. Observation 5. A man with a regular heart, after I had given digitalis, showed irregularities which a graphic record demonstrated to be due to the dropping out of ventricular systoles, the auricular rhythm being unaffected. The digitalis was stopped, and in a few days the heart's rhythm became quite regular, the ventricular systoles having ceased to drop out. Com- menting on this fact to the patient, he said he could bring the irregularity^ back, and I asked him how, and he replied, " By swallowing." I requested him to swallow, which he did, and a ventricular systole dropped out. I took a long tracing, and when he did not swallow the rhythm was regular, but as soon as he swallowed a beat was missed. This was repeated a great number of times. Four days later the heart was quite regular, and swallowing had no effect ujjon the heart's rate. Observation 6. A woman, aged 52, experiences attacks in which she becomes prostrated, suffers severe pain in the head, and vomits. If she lies quite still she does not vomit, but if she is startled by a noise — such as the banging of a door or ringing of a bell — or if she smells tobacco or cooking, vomiting occurs. In this condition there is manifestly a hypersensitive condition of the B part of the reflex arc, but it is to be observed that vomiting occurs from a stimulus from the periphery, just as the convulsion in rabies and strychnine poisoning. 322 Appendix. The Added or Reinforced Stimulation. In the production of reflexes in disease the reffex often takes place with unusual ease or facility, a slight stimulus producing an exaggerated reflex or calling into play an unusual reflex, as in Observation 5. The reason for this is that the disease disturbs some parts of the reflex arc rendering them more or less susceptible to stimulation. This may happen in several ways. In certain forms of visceral disease certain areas of the skin or other tissues of the external body wall show an increased susceptibility to stimulation. Thus, when pressure is applied to the skin of such a strength that in the unaffected part it jiroduces only the sensation of touch or pressure, it produces in these hyperalgesic areas the sensation of pain. The explanation of this disturbance of the normal reflex is that already a stimulus is affecting the arc from the diseased organ a, and the added stimulus of pressure a' is sufficient to produce pain (Fig. 8). Fig. 8. — Diagram to illustrate an added or reinforced stimulus. The presence of disease at a (say gastric ulcer, a diseased tooth) keeps up a persistent irritation at B, so that a stimulus from a' (the skin or other organ) not sufficient to cavise pain by itself, affects the irritated cells at B and produces pain (see also Observations 5, 6, 7). The contraction of the muscles of the abdominal wall in disease of abdominal organs (the " viscero-motor reflex ") is but an exaggeration of the normal reflex which maintains the tone of these muscles. If the gradual disappearance of these contractions be observed it will be found to merge into that state of resistance which we recognise as tone in these muscles, so that the contraction in visceral diseases is but an exaggeration of the normal tone. The increased sensitiveness which is shown by hyperalgesia of the tissues of the external body wall is capable of giving rise to pain from a stimulus reaching the central parts of the arc. Appendix. 323 Observation 7. A man with advanced aortic disease suffered from attacks of angina pectoris. He liad a diseased tooth, which caused a slight degree of jiain. During the attacl^s of angina pectoris, in addition to the pain in the chest and left arm he had pain along the jaw, but the pain was always most ■severe around the diseased tooth. In this instance the diseased tooth kept up a continual irritation of the A part of the sensory reflex, and the pain would become more severe when pressure was made on it in chewing, the reflex arc being disturbed by an increase of the stimuli at a. During the attacks of angina pectoris pain was felt along the jaw — where there was already a disturbed reflex — this additional ■stimtilus reaching the arc at b produced an increase of the pain, or in other words an increased response, at c. Observation 8. A man suffered from jaundice with severe attacks of gall-stone colic ior several days. The skin of the upper part of the abdomen on the right side was very tender on pressure (hyperalgesia). The pains suddenly oeased and a gall stone the size of a bean was passed by the bowel. The hyperalgesia of the skin persisted, but he sxif?ered pain over the hyperalgesia region on taking food. ^Vllen the hyperalgesia disappeared he suffered no pain on taking food. Here, as in Observation 5, a normal stimulus (resulting from the taking of food) on entrance into the central nervous system afiected a hypersensitive part of a reflex. Balanced Reflexes. There are a great many different kinds of reflexes, some so obscure that their mechanism is not clear. There is one form which from its symplicity illustrates the theory of disturbed reflexes as the basis of symptoms. This is the kind which might be called balanced reflexes, where two systems of reflexes mutually react. This was seen in the description of the reflex contraction of muscles, when at the same time one set of muscles contracts another set relaxes, where strychnine and the toxins of rabies and of tetanus disturb this balance (Figs. 6 and 7). It is also shown in the pupil, where normally the movements of the pupil are exquisitely balanced by the nerves supplying the dilator and sphincter fibres, and where atropine upsets this balance by jiaralysing the sphincter fibres. It also occurs in the heart, where the play between the accelerator and depressor nerves of the heart results in the delicate regulation of the heart's activity in response to stimuli from the other organs of the body. 324 Appendix. In many illnesses the balanced reflex is upset, so that we find the pulse rate a valuable indication of the patient's state. In most toxic cases the rate is increased, possibly by the excitation of the accelerator (sympathetic) part of the reflex. In some cases the toxin stimulates the depressor part, as in typhoid fever, and the efiect upon this part of the balanced reflex is seen in certain diseases where the heart's rate becomes very slow, as in the following observation. Observation 9. A man 50 years of age began to suffer from sudden attacks of loss of consciousness of brief duration. His pulse was found to be slow, and records — graphic and electrographic — showed that his ventricle was responding only to every second auricular beat. As this is typical of one kind of an over-action of the vagus, an attempt was made to remove this by atropine, which paralyses the peripheral end of the vagus. A hypo- dermic injection of one-fiftieth of a grain of atropine was follo^yed in ten mimites by the appearance of more beats, and in thirty-five minutes the ventricle responded to every auricular systole. In this instance the disturbed reflex was due to an over-action of one part of the balanced reflex (the vagus) and the removal of this over-activity at the c portion of the arc by the atropine restored the heart to its normal rhythm. An example of the disturbance of the balanced reflex, which not only illustrates the mechanism but throws light on a matter of real importance in practice, is afforded by the variation in the heart rate occasioned by the act of respiration. In differentiating the different forms of heart irregularity I recognised one form common in the young, which I called the youthful type of irregu- larity, and demonstrated that it was produced by the breathing ; the heart increased in rate during inspiration and decreased during: expiration. This variation was more marked if the individual breathed slowly and deeply. On inquiring into the significance of this form of irregularity I found it present in young people in perfect health. I found it disappeared when any febrile illness caused an increase in the heart's rate, and it returned when the fever fell and the rate returned to normal. I have used this irregularity for over twenty years as a guide in doubtful cases after recovery from illness, looking upon its presence as a proof that the disease had departed, and that the heart was free from any active process. This view is now justified. Under ordinary circumstances the balance between the accelerator and depressor nerves is so even that a Appendix. 325 stimulus such as breathing disturbs the balance. The presence of a toxic agent aSects the reflex, causing an increase in the rate, so that the slight stimulation from the act of breathing no longer has any effect, and hence the reappearance of the irregularity at the termination of a febrile illness. The Disturbed Fail of the Response. The tendency to heart-block by stimulating the vagus (as by the use of digitalis) is more apt to occur when the bundle connecting the auricle and ventricle is damaged, and the question arises, Is the effect at c which produces heart-block due to an increase of the vagal activity or an increased susceptibility of the responding organ — that is, the auriculo-ventricular bundle ? That the reflex may be impaired by the diminished susceptibility of the recipient organ at c can be inferred from the following observation : Observation 10. A man 68 years of age consulted me because he had found that on making a considerable effort, as running upstairs, his pulse, in place of increasing in rate, fell to nearly one-half its rate. I tested him, and found that this was so ; I considered that the bundle which conveys the stimulus from auricle to ventricle was damaged, so that while the damaged bimdle could convey a stimulus at the rate of 60 or 70 times a minute, it failed to do so when the rate became liigher. Subsequent records, graphic and electrographic, confirmed this view. In this instance the normal balance of the reflex between the augmenting and inhibiting nerves to the heart is disturbed by the damage to the recipient organ. Multiple Stimuli and Multiple Responses. The response at c may be due to stimuli originating at different places and in different ways. Thus an abdominal muscle may contract in response to a stimulus from the brain, or from the skin, or from the movements of respiration, or from the movements of the body, or from a visceral stimulus, as in Observation 2. A single stimulus can give rise to a number of responses — indeed, most stimuli do ; we have seen that a cutaneous reflex giving rise to a movement of a joint does so by the contraction of one group of muscles and the inhibition of another group, while Observations 1 and 2 showed that in response to a stimulus a number of reflexes followed. 326 Appendix. Multiple Reflexes. During ill health the patient may complain of a number of symptoms, and the doctor may detect a number of signs. This is due to the fact that the agent which causes ill health disturbs a great many reflexes. To look at these signs and symptoms, each one recorded as an isolated fact, presents such a confused picture that a coherent description of the patient's condition cannot be given. When the individual reflexes are recognised, then the symptoms can be grouped upon a rational principle. This plan of analysis can be employed in studying the reflexes produced by the impaired functions of organs, or by the specific reaction to the toxins of disease, as well as to drugs, in the manner already described. The Disturbed Reflexes due to Organic Disease. One essential question in cases of diseased organs is their functional efficiency. When all the organs are functioning in a normal manner the reflexes pass unnoticed by the individual. When a failure of function takes place the reflexes are disturbed and symptoms appear. It is difficult to recognise the real source of a reflex, for many organs when they fail to function in a normal fashion do not themselves show the signs of failure, but the signs are shown by other organs that are deprived of the material the erring organ contributes to the economy. While the failure of each organ will give rise to different reflexes the fundamental principles governing the production of the symptom will be the same. The Disturbed Reflexes due to Heart Failure. The symptoms produced by heart failure offer a good illustra- tion, as they have been worked out in some detail. When a healthy individual engages in violent physical effort he will in time produce exhaustion of the heart, so that it no longer supplies sufficient blood to the organs and tissues, and the individual suffers distress. The same thing happens when an individual with a diseased heart undergoes an effort which leads to exhaustion, only in the second instance the amount of effort is less. The signs of heart failure are those of distress and are not to be found by the examination of the heart but by effects of an insufficient supply of blood to other organs. This is well recognised in advanced heart failure when there are in addition to distress on effort, the physical signs of dropsy and an enlarged liver. Ajypendix. 327 In the great majority of people who sufEer from heart failure these physical signs never appear. In them the heart failure is made manifest by a limitation in their response to efiort ; they are pulled up by distress in some effort they were wont to perform in comfort. This is because of certain reflexes that are set up by organs which suffer from an insufficient supply of blood. The reflexes are of a nature that produce distress of such a clamant kind that cessation of effort is called for. They are mainly of two kinds — the distress associated with (1) breathlessness and (2) pain in a distinctive region. In healthy people the most frequent evidence of heart exhaustion (as in running) is breathlessness and the accompanying sensation of tightness at the throat and a sense of suffocation. Occasionally there is also a sensation of constriction of the chest with pain across the chest. What happens in a healthy person is also what happens in one with a weak and diseased heart, one diSerence being that less efiort produces the distress in the latter case and another that pain is a fairly frequent cause of distress. The mechanism of these two reflexes is quite distinct. Respiratory distress is the outcome of a disturbance of the respira- tory reflex on account of the heart being unable to supply purified blood to some part of the respiratory system. I do not discuss further this mechanism here, as it would lead us too far, but everyone recognises that breathlessness is a sign of heart exhaustion and that it is due to a disturbed reflex. The other reflex, of which pain is the dominant feature, is a reflex arising from exhaustion of the heart muscle itself. The pain is often accompanied by other reflex signs, as contraction of the intercostal muscles producing the constriction of the chest — sometimes of such extreme violence as to be more dreaded than the pain. Sometimes the mouth fills with saliva, and the pain may be felt not only in the chest but in the arms and along the jaw (Observation 7). These are multiple reflexes comparable with that described in Observation 2. Observation 11. A man 68 years of age complains of breathlessness on going up a hill or stairs. Also'^of pain across the chest, on effort vmder certain conditions, such as walking out on a cold day or after a full meal. Tlie pain across the chest is preceded by a sense of tightness, or constriction, such as used to pull him vip when rvmning a race in boyhood. These signs of limitation have come on gradually, and he noticed his limitation fifteen years ago. There are no physical signs of disease, except some arterial degeneration 3^8 Appendix. This account is one common to a great many people, in whom the coronary arteries become diseased so that the heart muscle does not receive an adequate supply of blood. In this case the heart in supplying the active muscles of the legs fails to supply other parts with enough blood, and amongst others the respiratory system is affected, and gives rise to the complex reflex which is called breathlessness. At other times the heart muscle does not receive a sufficient supply of blood, and it gives rise to its peculiar reflex — pain and contraction of the muscles of the chest wall. In heart exhaustion, then, we have two prominent reflexes — the one arising from a deficient supply of blood to the respiratory organs, and the other arising from exhaustion of the heart muscle. Which of these reflexes first appears on heart exhaustion depends on whether the respiratory or cardiac reflex is first called into play. In the case quoted, under certain circumstances it was the respira- tory, and under others it was pain. Occasionally they may both appear. In certain cases the respiratory reflex is readily produced, as in auricular fibrillation — a condition in which pain is infrequent and rarely severe, while in diseases which limit the supply of blood to the heart muscles pain is readily induced. In the following instance the reaction of the reflex to the different heart states is brought out. Observation 12. A man 66 years of age con-siilted me for pain on walking. The pain at first was only produced by walking, and only on certain occasions — mostly on walking after a meal. The pain was situated across the chest, and was accompanied by a gripping sensation. As years passed the pain became so easily provoked that walking under any circumstances would cause it. Tlie heart was normal in size and the rhythm of the heart normal. One day, when 72 years of age, he called to see me, and said that mitil recently he could walk 200 yards before the pain compelled him to stop. Now he can only walk 100 yards when he is pulled up, not by pain but by breathlessness. Indeed, since this breathlessness occ\u-red he had not suffered from pain. On examination I found the heart was irregular in its action, the irregularity being characteristic of that due to auricular fibrillation. This is not an isolated instance, as I have had several cases with a somewhat similar history, but explains an observation I made many years ago, that people with auricular fibrillation do not suffer from severe attacks of angina pectoris. Appendix. 329 The Effects of Drugs. It has already been shown that drugs act by causing a disturbance of reflexes. This way of looking at the action of drugs explains much that is obscure in pharmacology, as the following observation shows. For a long time it was recognised that digitalis acted in a remarkable manner in some cases of rapid pulse in reducing the rate, while it had no effect in other cases. It was never understood why there should be this difference. I found out about fifteen years ago that in the cases in which it had this slowing effect the heart was regulated by abnormal rhythms — mainly that abnormal rhythm due to the condition now recognised as auricular fibrillation. I have attempted many times to reduce the increased rate of the heart when the rhythm was normal, and invariably failed. I speculated for a long time as to the cause of this difference, but never understood it till the theory of disturbed reflexes made the matter plain. In infectious diseases — as pneumonia, measles, etc. — the balanced reflex that moderates the heart's action is disturbed by the toxins of the casual agent of disease, so that the rate of the whole heart is increased. In auricular fibrillation this reflex is not disturbed. When the rhythm is normal the ventricle contracts only to the stimulus that arises from an auricular systole. In auricular fibrillation there is no rhythmic contraction of the auricle, but a continuous fibrillary twitching of the muscle, so that in place of the regular stimulus from the contracting auricle there is a shower of weak stimuli which assail the conducting system between auricle and ventricle, and cause the rapid ventricular rate. Not only is there this difference in the cause of the increased rate, but there is a difference in the condition of the cardiac reflex. In rapid pulse with the normal rhythm the reflex is disturbed by the toxins of the diseased state, so that the digitalis can produce no effect. In auricular fibrillation the reflex is unaffected, so that the digitalis can act upon the vagus portion, and in doing so depresses the conducting mechanism to the ventricle and renders it not so susceptible to the numerous stimuli from the auricle. The Analysis of Symptoms. In many diseases all the symptoms on which a diagnosis is based are reflex in origin — in some the reflexes are disturbed by the entrance of the stimulus through the nervous system, and in others the disturbance is through the circulation. To the former 330 Appendix, belong the symptoms of such diseases as gastric ulcer, renal calculus, gall-stone disease, where the symptoms are of the type described in Observation 2. The symptoms in infections are due to the disturbance of the reflexes through the circulation, as influenza, malaria, typhoid and typhus fevers, measles, and abscess formation, apart from the swelling. In some diseases we get a mixture of both kinds of reflexes, as in appendicitis, where there is not only the local pain and tenderness of the tissues of the external body wall, with contraction of the muscles of the abdomen, but the feeling of exhaustion, rapid pulse, tendency to vomit. In cholecystitis we get a similar complex. The need for the more accurate recognition of symptoms is seen when it is considered how difficult it is to diagnose even such seemingly simple affections as gastric ulcer and appendicitis. Though surgeons have been operating for these complaints for many years, the most experienced recognise that in many cases they find they have been mistaken in their diagnosis. This is due in a great measure to the fact that the nature and mechanism of the symptoms of these diseases have never been understood, and .the symptoms were never clearly differentiated from those of other diseases which they resemble. It will thus be seen that symptomatology is like chemistry, where the combination of elements results in the production of a great number of compounds bewildering in their variety. Never- theless, as in chemistry, when they are subjected to strict analysis they can be resolved into their component elements. When the analysis of symptoms is studied as fully as the analysis of chemical compounds have been studied, then it will be possible to group the disturbed reflexes in an orderly manner. The next step then will be to find out the agents capable of provoking the different reflexes, so that we get nearer to the immediate cause of disease. The employment of this method of investigation is but a return to those methods of clinical research which were so fruitful in their results in the past, especially during the early half of the nineteenth century. To realise how great the progress was during that period we have to consider the discoveries associated with the names of Addison, Bright, Graves, Adams, Stokes, Cheyne, Paget, Hodgkin, and Jenner. These observers employed the most useful of all weapons in research — the trained senses. What we aim at is to recognise the methods which these great observers employed, and by improving and refining their methods restore clinical medicine to the van of research. Appetviix. 331 Structural and Functional Symptoms. A certain numbor of symptoms are due to structural changes and functional derangements. These are generally shown by physical signs, and are due to departures from the normal in various ways, as in alteration in the size and shape and consistence of organs, changes in the colour, as pallor, modification of the sounds of the heart and lungs. These are not dealt with here, but will have to be reconsidered in view of this theory of disturbed reflexes, because many apparently structural and functional signs are really disturbed reflexes, or are produced by disturbed reflexes. # # ' U.C. BERKELEY LIBRARIES X / .^'^ \ ■1^ ^' "^ .# . .^r^^A