A SYSTEMATIC TREATISE ON THE OCULAR MUSCLES, BY G^c. SAVAGE, M.D., Professor of Ophthalmology (Defects of the Eye) in the Medical Department of Vanderbilt University, Author of " New Truths in Ophthalmology," and " Ophthalmic Neuro- Myology," Ex-President of the Nashville Academy of Medicine, Ex-Presider.t of the Tennessee State M.edical Association, Ex-President of the Southern Medical Association. EIGHTY-FOUR ILLUSTRATIVE CUTS AND SIX PLATES SECOND EDITION PUBLISHED BY THE AUTHOR, 137 Eighth Avenue, North, Nashville, Tenn. PRINTED BY McQuiDDY PRINTING COMPANY, Nashville, Tenn. 1911. (i) o Entered, according to Act of Congress, in the year 1902, BY THE AUTHOR, G. C. SAVAGE, In the Office of the Librarian of Congress, at Washington. Revised, reprinted, and re-copyrighted BY THE AUTHOR, G. C. SAVAGE, July, 1911. (ii) PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION. The bringing 1 out of this edition has been delayed two years longer than the author had intended, the first edition having been practically exhausted three years ago. He trusts that the delay has served to make this book both more interesting and more convincing. The first chapter has been enlarged by the addition of ninety-one pages; but one hundred and thirty of the one hundred and forty-five pages of this chapter have been newly written. This chapter has been enriched by the addition of twenty-three new illustrative cuts, every one of which helps to make the light of truth, as the author sees it, shine the more vividly. For twenty years the author has been at a disadvan- tage in that his teaching, concerning the fundamental principles of ocular rotations, has not been in accord with that of the immortal Helmholtz. The contrast is shown in the parallel columns on pages 32 to 34. The whole of the difference between the teaching of Helm- holtz and that of the author hinges on the correct answers to the following four questions: (1) Is the cen- ter of the cornea always the anterior pole? (2) Is the (iii) iv PREFACE. central point of the macula always the posterior pole of the eye? (3) Do all secondary visual lines cross the vis- ual axis at the nodal point? (4) Do all secondary visual lines cross the visual axis at the center of retinal curva- ture the center of rotation? To question (1) Helmholtz' answer was "yes," but the author's answer is "no;" to question (2) the author's answer is " yes," but Helm- holtz' answer was "no;" to question (3) Helmholtz' an- swer was "yes," but the author's answer is "no;" to question (4) the author's answer is "yes," but Helm- holtz' answer was "no." The unbiased reading- of the first chapter of this edition will convince the most skeptical that the central point of the macula is the posterior pole of every eye, whether it be the ideal or the nonideal eye; that the cen- ter of the cornea is the anterior pole of the ideal eye only. All related principles of ocular rotations stand out clearly in the light of the demonstrated fact that the cen- tral point of the macula is the posterior pole of the eye. The so-called "optic axis " of Helmholtz, when not the visual axis, as in the ideal eye, is nothing more nor less than one of the secondary lines of vision. The mastery of the fundamental principles, set forth in the first chapter, can be easily accomplished with the aid of the Muscle Indicator, five times depicted in this chapter. There is no condition of a single ocular mus- PREFACE. V cle, or any combination of the ocular muscles, normal, abnormal, or pathologic, which this device will not show; and its construction is based on the following 1 two facts: (1) The center of the macula is the posterior pole of the eye; (2) all indirect visual lines cross the visual axis at the center of retinal curvature, which is the center of rotation. No book on the ocular muscles, except this one and its companion volume. "Ophthalmic Neuro-Myology, " has been based on the fundamental principles set forth in the first chapter of this book. If these principles are not correct, this book and its companion volume are worthless. The author has no doubt but that the final verdict of the scientific world will be: " These princi- ples are veritable truths." The practical chapters, II. to XII., have had such cor- rections and additions made to them as appeared neces- sary in order to make them clearer and stronger. This is especially true of Chapter III., to which four pages have been added. Everything taught in these chapters is based on the fundamental principles set forth in Chap- ter I.; and the value of every procedure advocated has been established by abundant practical experience. PREFACE TO THE FIRST EDITION 1902 ART cannot succeed when principles are unknown or ignored. The mechanical or surgical art of readjusting the ocular muscles, when there is maladjustment or im- balance, should be based on the scientific principles un- derlying ocular rotations, else such art should not be practiced. These principles, as simple as they are en- during, are discussed in Chapter I. of this volume, and with sufficient clearness, the author hopes, to enable the reader to fully comprehend them. Without a clear un- derstanding of these principles, it will be impossible to grasp the teaching of subsequent chapters; but in the light of the teaching of Chapter I., the remainder of the book may be easily understood. Whenever it has been possible, in the interest of truth, the author has been glad to agree with writers who have preceded him; when forced to dissent from their teach- ings, he has done so cautiously and respectfully, invari- ably giving his reasons for so doing. If the aim of the critic and the author hopes that every reader will be a critical reader who may review this volume, shall be the establishment of truth and the (vi) PREFACE. Vll dethronement of error, he will not become an object of terror to the author; for he himself has been laboring 1 , through a period of fifteen years, for the accomplishment of the same end, as it relates to the ocular muscles. He believes that what he has taught in this book is true, and that practice based on what he has taught will be correct. He would not promulgate nor perpetuate error if he knew it; therefore the reviewer who, in his own way, points out what he may find to be erroneous is as much his friend as the one who may give emphasis to what he finds to be true. In the body of the book the author has given due credit to every writer and thinker on the subjects discussed, whom he had an opportunity to consult, and it only re- mains for him to acknowledge here the helpfulness he has derived from their labors. For the mechanical excellencies of the volume, the author, who is also the publisher, is indebted to the printing establishment whose inscription can be found on the title-page. ILLUSTRATIONS. FIG. i. Illustrating rotation of a sphere 3 FIG. 2. The plane and axis of a rotation 1 1 FIG. 3. The left monocular spacial pole and field of vision 18 FIG. 4. The right monocular spacial pole and field of vision 18 FIG. 5. Rotation of the eye in its orbit 23 FIGS. 6, 7. Erroneous teaching as to poles of the eye and lines of direc- tion 28 FIG. 8. True teaching as to poles of the eye and lines of direction ... 28 FIG. 9. The Le Conte 1882 horopter 36 FIG. 10. The Le Conte 1898 horopter _ 37 FlG. II. The equator and the two axes of rotation 44 FlG. 12. For calculating false torsion in oblique conjugate rotations ... 47 FlG. 13. Professor Daniel's figure for calculating false torsion 49 FIG. 14. The fixed vertical and horizontal planes of the head, the two eyes in ideal orbits and Listing's plane 72 FIG. 15. The conjugate and fusion brain centers 93 FlG. 16. Isogonal circle of the author 97 FIG. 17. A section of a wooden body representing the surface of binoc- ular single vision 100 FlG. 18. The primary and secondary isogonal circles 103 FlG. 19. The Muscle Indicator showing the eyes in their primary posi- tion 112 FlG. 20. The Muscle Indicator showing the right cardinal secondary position of the visual axes 1 16 FIG. 21. The Muscle Indicator showing the upward cardinal secondary position of the visual axes 1 18 FlG. 22. The Muscle Indicator showing an oblique secondary position of the visual axes without torsion (normal) 120 (viii) ILLUSTRATIONS. IX FIG. 23. The Muscle Indicator showing an oblique secondary position of the visual axes with torsion (unreal j 122 FIG. 24. The field of binocular single vision 124 FIG. 25. The field of binocular view 124 FIGS. 24, 25. Both show the binocular spacial pole, meridians and parallels 124 FIG. 26. Binocular spacial meridians and parallels, groups of, and the primary isogonal circle 131 FIG. 27. Shows that a binocular spacial pole, meridians and parallels, would be impossible if the maculas are not the posterior poles of the eyes 138 FIGS. 28, 29. The Stevens phorometer 149, 150 FIG. 30. The Wilson phorometer 151 FIG. 31. The monocular phorometer 155 FiG. 32. The cyclo-phorometer 162 FiG. 33. The Stevens clinoscope 164 FIG. 34. The Stevens tropometer 170 FiG. 35. Showing the retinal areas of binocular fusion 175 FiG. 36. The Stevens scissors 249 FIGS. 37, 38. The Stevens forceps 249, 250 FiG. 39. The Stevens hook 250 FiG. 40. The Stevens needle holder 250 FIG. 41. The Price tendonometer 254 FiG. 42. Retinal fusion areas 289 FIG. 43. Showing malformation of left Orbit 343 FiGS. 44, 45, 46. Showing malformation of both orbits 344, 345, 346 FiG. 47. Hyperphoric exercise set 373 FiGS. 48, 49, 50, 51. Illustrating cyclophoria 391 FiG. 52. Illustrating orthophoria of the obliques 391 FiG. 53. Perry's device for taking cyclo-duction 399 FiG. 54. Showing frames containing exercise cylinders 411 FiG. 55. A square, illustrating the changes by non-oblique astigmatism 421 X ILLUSTRATIONS. FIG. 56. A rectangular parallelogram, illustrating the changes by non- oblique astigmatism 425 FIG. 57. A square, showing changes by oblique astigmatism 426 FIG. 58. Showing the shape of a fused square by one who has oblique astigmatism, most curved meridians diverging 427 FIG. 59. Showing changes caused in lines not parallel with chief me- ridians of astigmatic corneas 429 FIG. 60. A photograph of a rectangle, made by a non-astigmatic lens 431 FIG. 61. A photograph of a rectangle, made by an oblique astigmatic lens 432 FlG. 62. A photograph of a rectangle, made by a vertical astigmatic lens 433 FIG. 63. A photograph of a rectangle, made by an oblique astigmatic lens 434 FlG. 64. A photograph of a rectangle, made by a horizontal astigmatic lens 435 FIG. 65. Same as Fig. 61, except that the sensitized plate was not at the focal interval 436 FIG. 66. Showing the retinal images of an arrow, when there is no as- tigmatism, or when the astigmatism is vertical or horizontal 437 FIG. 67. Showing one horizontal image and one oblique image of an arrow 441 FlG. 68. Showing how the arrow would be doubled, except for compen- sating cyclotropia 441 FIG. 69. Showing the minus cyclotropia that has occurred for fusing the two images shown in Fig. 68 443 FlG. 70. Showing one horizontal image and one oblique image of an arrow 444 FIG. 71. Showing the plus cyclotropia that has occurred for fusing the images shown in Fig. 70 445 FIG 72. Showing two oblique images of the arrow, the obliquity up in one eye and down in the other 445 ILLUSTRATIONS. Xl FIG. 73. Showing the minus cyclotropia that has fused the images shown in Fig. 72 446 PLATE I. Showing images and object in non-oblique astigmatism and in hyperopia and myopia 448 PLATE II. Showing object and images in diverging oblique astigmatism 453 PLATE III. Showing the minus cyclotropia that has fused the dissimilar images 457 PLATES IV., V., VI. Showing arcs of distortion by cylinders. 472, 477, 485 FlG. 74. Representing compensating vertical and lateral heterotropia 491 FIG. 75. The amblyoscope of Worth 546 FlG. 76. Illustrating the first step of the Fox operation for exotropia . 577 FlG. 77. Illustrating the second step of the Fox operation 578 FlG. 78. Illustrating the third step of the Fox operation 579 FlG. 79. Illustrating paralysis of the right-vertors 606 FlG. 80. Illustrating paralysis of the left-vertors 607 FiGS. 81, 82. Illustrating paralysis of the sub-vertors 608 FiGS. 83, 84. Illustrating paralysis of the supervertors 609 CONTENTS. CHAPTER I. PAGES. The Fundamental Principles of Ocular Rotations 1-145 CHAPTER II. Orthophoria 146-179 CHAPTER III. Heteropboria 180-272 CHAPTER IV. Esophoria 273-313 CHAPTER V. Exophoria 314-340 CHAPTER VI. Hyperphoria and Cataphoria 341-381 CHAPTER VII. Cyclophoria 382-416 CHAPTER VIII. Compensating Cyclotropia 417-488 CHAPTER IX. Compensating Heterotropia 489-496 CHAPTER X. Comitant Esotropia, Exotropia, Hypertropia, Catatropia, and Cyclo- tropia 497-594 CHAPTER XI. Paralysis and Paresis of the Ocular Muscles 595-622 CHAPTER XII. Muscles of the Iris and of the Ciliary Body 623-661 Index 662-684 (xii) OPHTHALMIC MYOLOGY. CHAPTER I. THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. A SPHERE, in rotation around its center as a fixed point, has on its surface two points, and only two points, that do not move. These two points must be 180 apart, and the straight line that connects them must be a diameter of the sphere. This diameter is the axis of a given rotation. Every point on the surface of the sphere, other than the two fixed points, moves in a plane at right-angles to the axis of rotation, describing the circumference of a circle larger or smaller, on the sur- face of the sphere. The size of any one of these circles is determined by the distance of the given rotating point from the nearer of the two fixed points up to 90. A point 90 from the two fixed points describes the circum- ference of a great circle, while all other rotating points describe circumferences of small circles. The planes of all the circles thus created are rotation planes, and each 2 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES one is parallel with all the others. The planes of all these circles, except the plane of the one great circle, may be ignored, for the plane of any given rotation is the plane of the one great circle. This plane alone cuts the axis of rotation at its center the center of the sphere while the planes of all the small circles cut the axis at varying 1 points from the center. Only one of the infinite number of points lying* on the great circle should be studied in any given rotation, but it should not be forgotten that all points on this great circle bear an unalterable relationship to the one chosen point. The line extending- from the chosen point back through the center of rotation the center of the sphere to an- other point on its surface just 180 distant, is the rota- ting line, and all other moving lines, whether diameters or not, may be ignored, for all these lines must bear a fixed relationship to this one chosen line. Both the ro- tating line and the axis of rotation are diameters of the revolving sphere. They lie in the plane of the same great circle, and each is at right-angles to the other. This is always true, regardless of the point of applica- tion and the direction of the force effecting the rotation. A change in the direction of the force only means a new axis of rotation, a new rotating point and rotating line, and a new rotation plane. A careful study of Fig. 1 will serve to fix in the OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. mind the truths that have been taught above. In this figure, A-B-D represents the cross-section of a concave hemisphere whose center is c\ in which is set the con- Fig. i. vex sphere whose cross-section is a-d-b-e. and whose center is also c. The lines a-b and d-e are diameters of this sphere, at right-angles to each other and in 4 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES a plane common to the two. Since this sphere is loose in the concave hemisphere, any point on its surface may be chosen as the point for rotation. If a is to be rotated into the position of I), it must move in the plane of the great circle of which a-b is the diameter. The two fixed points on the surface of the sphere in this rotation are d and e, and the line connecting- them is the diameter d-e, which is the axis of this rotation. It lies in the same plane with a-b and also in the plane of the great circle which is equally distant at all points from both a and b. The force so applied as to make a the rotating point and a-b the rotating 1 line, fixes the axis of rotation as d-e, for d and e are the only two fixed points on the surface in this rotation. Any point on the surface between a and // and between a and n will rotate in a plane parallel with a-b, each circle growing" smaller as the point re- cedes toward h or n. Likewise circles for revolving points between h and wphavp ot 2 !/ sinX -vm = sinoz'c-^- me). COS I vers R cos 2 R-|-c "XT* * vin /c\r\o T\ "vnt sin X. = sin ovc = sin (90 I) c cos I vers R cos I vers R ' cos R+ cot 2 I = sn m~V c s * v^ R \ \ i/ cos* R+ cot 2 1 / 50 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES " That is, starting from a primary position, when the eye is rotated R degrees on axis inclined I degrees to the vertical (or horizontal) the resulting torsion, X, is an angle whose sine is equal to cos I times the vers R, di- vided by the square root of the sum of the squares of cos R and cos I. The numerical value of the torsion, X, when the inclination of the axis is 45, is as follows for angles of rotation, R, as follows: Angle of rotation = R 5 10 16 20 25 30 35 40 45 Torsion fi'4' ?6' 1 147' 249' 46' 540' 733' 944' " This was worked out independently of the simpler formula given in Maddox, but the two are equivalent." The mistake made by Maddox was in supposing that no effort was made by the obliques to correct the tor- sioning that otherwise would occur. The work of mastering the study of ocular rotations is greatly simplified by a knowledge of the fact that a car- dinal rotation is around one of two axes, and that oblique rotations are around both these axes simultaneously, and that no other axes are possible. Rotation planes are in- numerable, but the axes of rotations are only two, the vertical and the transverse axes of the eyes. Although every vertical motion is effected by two muscles, and every oblique rotation by three muscles, nevertheless it will be profitable to study each muscle and its plane. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 51 THE INDIVIDUAL MUSCLE AND ITS PLANE OF ROTATION. If the nine conjugate innervations were never defective, and if there were no such thing as heterophoria, there would be but little need for studying the ocular muscles as to their separate action or in the light of synergism and antagonism. In paralysis and paresis of an ocular muscle, a diagnosis can be made easily and quickly only when one knows what would be the result of unopposed action of the affected muscle. The reader is supposed to be fully acquainted with the extrinsic ocular muscles, as to their origin, course, inser- tion, and nerve supply. With this knowledge already ac- quired it is easy to pass to the study of the result or re- sults of the contraction of any single muscle. The axis of rotation of the eye by any one muscle must be at right- angles to the plane of rotation for this muscle. This plane must bisect the muscle at its origin and at its attachment, and must also pass through the center of rotation of the eye. As to the internus or externus, the relationship that the muscle plane bears to the horizontal plane of the eye indicates the exact rotation that will result from its action. If the rotation plane of the internus coincides with the horizontal plane of the eye, then this muscle will have only one result from its contraction; that is, 52 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES the eye will be turned directly in (ad version), the rota- tion taking- place around the vertical axis of the eye. This action of the internus may be termed its principal action; and under such condition there can be no subor- dinate action of the muscle. Some may prefer the terms used by Dr. Maddox, viz., pre-eminent and subsidiary action. If the plane of rotation of the internus does not coin- cide with the horizontal plane of the eye, the simple ro- tation is impossible. Let this muscle plane be inclined down and out, as it must be when the internus is attached too high, then the axis of rotation cannot be the vertical axis of the eye, for the former must bear the same rela- tionship to the latter that the muscle plane bears to the horizontal plane of the eye. The unopposed action of this muscle cannot rotate the eye directly in, but associ- ated with the adversion there will be superversion and in- ward torsion or declination, both of which are subordi- nate actions. Let the internus be attached too low on the globe, then its plane of rotation will have an inclina- tion down and in, forming- a definite angle with the hor- izontal plane of the eye. The axis of rotation will form the same angle with the vertical axis of the eye. Unop- posed, the internus thus attached cannot rotate the eye directly inward, but, associated with the adversion (prin- cipal), there will be sub-version and an outward torsion OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 53 or declination. It is reasonable to suppose that the plane of rotation of the internus does not always coincide with the horizontal plane of the eye. Thus H is shown that, by error of attachment (too high or too low), an internus may be one factor in a hyperphoria or a cataphoria, and in a minus or a plus cyclophoria. In like manner rotation by the external rectus muscle may be studied. If the plane bisecting the origin and in- sertion of this muscle, and passing throug~h the center of rotation, coincides with the horizontal plane of the eye, its action will result in abversion (principal, without any kind of subordinate, action). If the muscle be attached too high, its plane of rotation must be inclined down and in, its axis of rotation making the same angle with the vertical axis that its plane makes with the horizontal plane. The action of the externus thus attached will have a triple result: (a) abversion (principal); (Z>) super- version (subordinate); and (c) an outward torsion or dec- lination (subordinate). If the externus be attached too low, its plane of rota- tion will be tilted down and out, forming a definite angle with the horizontal plane, and its axis of rotation will form an equal angle with the vertical axis. In contract- ing, the eye will be abverted (principal action); it will also be sub-verted, and there will be an inward torsion (subordinate actions). Thus it is shown that an externus 54 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES attached too high or too low will be one factor in the pro- duction of a hyperphoria or a cataphoria, and just as cer- tainly a factor in the production of a plus or a minus cyclophoria. It will be observed, from the foregoing, that either an internus or an externus attached in greater part above the horizontal plane, will have the superverting effect of a superior rectus; and that either muscle attached in greater part below the horizontal plane of the eye, will have the sub-verting effect of the inferior rectus. The torsioning effect of an internus attached too high will be in, while that of an externus with a too high attachment will be out. An internus attached too low will produce a plus cyclophoria, while an externus attached too low will cause a minus cyclophoria. Thus it will be seen that an internus will have the same kind of verting and tor- sioning effects as the superior or inferior rectus towards which its attachment is displaced. The external rectus will have the superverting or sub-verting effect of the su- perior or inferior rectus towards which its attachment is displaced, but the opposite torsioning effect. The prac- tical nature of these observations will be shown in the study of operations on the lateral recti muscles. The correctness of what has been said about the action of the internus or externus, when the plane of rotation does not coincide with the horizontal plane of the eye, can OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 55 be easily demonstrated by the use of the rubber ball and the knitting needles, and in the same way can be studied the individual action of the superior and inferior recti and that of the obliques. The rotation plane of an indi- vidual muscle bisects the muscle and cuts the center of rotation. Only in properly attached lateral recti mus- cles does this plane correspond with a meridional plane, hence the axes of rotations by the other four muscles can- not lie in the equatorial plane. The origin, course, and insertion of the superior rectus, also of the inferior rectus, make it impossible for the plane of rotation for either one of these muscles to coin- cide with the vertical antero-posterior plane of the eye when in the primary position. The plane of rotation for either one of these muscles is made to pass through the center of the origin of the muscle, the center of rotation of the eye, and the center of the insertion of the muscle. It is only when either muscle has a very definite attach- ment that its plane of rotation can be vertical. A dis- placement in or out of the attachment of either the superior or the inferior rectus will not change the kind of rotation to be effected, but it would modify the ex- tent of the three effects of its contraction. For sim- plicity of study, it may be considered, therefore, that there is a common plane of rotation for both the supe- rior and the inferior recti, and that the plane is ver- tical, forming an angle of 27 with the vertical antero- 56 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES posterior plane of the eye. The axis of rotation must be at 'right-angles to the muscle plane, and consequently must form an angle of 27 with the transverse axis, but in the horizontal plane with it. The superior rectus unopposed has for its principal effect superversion, and for its subordinate results, adversion and an inward torsion or declination. The inferior rectus will have for its principal action sub-version, and for its secondary ac- tions, adversion and outward torsion. Thus the superior rectus, while being the chief factor in a hyperphoria, may be also a secondary factor in the production of an esophoria, and of a minus cyclophoria. The inferior rectus, while the chief factor of a cataphoria, may also be a secondary factor both in esophoria and in a plus cyclophoria. An oblique muscle, when unopposed, is incapable of a simple rotation. Its plane of rotation must be constructed in the same way as have been constructed the planes for the recti. In the case of the superior oblique, the point of origin through which the plane must pass is the pulley at the upper-inner angle of the orbit, and not at its real origin at the apex of the orbit. Since the inferior oblique arises beneath this pulley, and since the superior oblique may be supposed to pass directly above, while the inferior passes directly beneath, the center of motion of the eye, to be inserted directly opposite each other in the outer-posterior OF OCUIvAR ROTATIONS. 57 quadrant, they may be said to have a common plane of rotation, which means, also, that they have a common axis, around which each must revolve the eye when un- opposed by any other muscle. This plane is at an angle of 39 with the vertical transverse plane of the eye. The axis must be, therefore, at an angle of 39 with the visual axis. When the superior oblique contracts, its principal action is to tort the eye in; but always accompanying- this are the subordinate actions, sub-version and abversion. When the inferior oblique is unopposed and unaided, its principal action is outward torsion, and its secondary ef- fects are superversion and abversion. Thus it may be seen that the obliques may be a factor in three forms of heterophoria: (a) cyclophoria, (Z>) hyperphoria and cata- phoria, (c) exophoria. LAW OF MONOCULAR MOTION. The law g'overning' monocular rotations may be formu- lated as follows: (1) The visual axis, which is the line of intersection of the planes of all meridians, must be rotated in the plane of that meridian on which lie the first and sec- ond points of view and their retinal images. (2) In the plane of the horizontal, or that of the vertical, meridian, the rotation must be effected around a sing-le fixed axis, at rig-ht-ang"les to the rotation plane and cutting- it at the center of rotation if in the horizontal plane, around the 58 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES vertical axis of the eye; if in the -vertical plane, around the transverse axis of the eye. (3) In the plane of an oblique rotation, 'whatever the degree of obliquity, the rotation must be accomplished around two moving- axes by two forces acting simultaneously, these axes being the trans- verse and vertical axes, both at right-angles to the visual axis, but neither one at rig"ht-angles to any oblique ro- tation plane; while a third force prevents any rotation around the vistial axis. The only part of the above law open to controversy pertains to oblique rotations. These rotations are ac- complished by three forces; and they are so applied as to make it impossible for such a rotation to be around a re- sultant fixed axis, as would be the case if only two forces were acting". If a resultant axis for the three forces be possible, the axis would be a moving" one, for, in any ob- lique rotation of the eye, there are no two fixed points, directly opposite or otherwise related, on its surface, hence there could be no fixed diameter. If there be a resultant moving axis for an oblique rotation, it would be hard to locate. It is so easy to grasp the thoug-ht that, in an oblique rotation, one oblique muscle prevents rotation around the visual axis, while one rectus muscle is moving" the eye around the vertical axis and one other rectus muscle is moving- the eye around the transverse axis, it seems to the author that the wording 1 of the third section of the law of monocular rotation must be correct. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 59 The purpose of the above law is that, while the visual axis is being- rotated in a fixed plane, the vertical axis of the eye shall never lose parallelism with the median plane of the head, for on this parallelism depends correct orientation. A single muscle can obey this law only when the rota- tion is in the horizontal plane, and then only when the externus or the internus is bisected by this plane. In vertical and oblique rotations, an oblique muscle pre- vents rotation around the visual axis, hence performs the task of keeping 1 the vertical axis of the eye parallel with the median plane of the head, while one rectus (as in vertical rotations), or at most two recti (as in oblique ro- tations), moves the visual axis from point to point in space. In the light of this law the six extrinsic ocular muscles should be studied as they are related to each other in performing their tasks. The helping of one muscle by other muscles is s} r ner- gism; the opposing of one muscle by other muscles is an- tagonism. The old method of studying these is set forth in the following TABLE. I Superior rectus } f Synergists . . . -I [ Tonicity. ( Inferior rectus ) Internus External rectus . I Antagonists < Superior oblique '- Tonicity. ( Inferior oblique ) 60 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES { Superior oblique 1 Synergists . . . 4 > Tonicity. ( Inferior oblique ) Externus . i Internus ', Antagonists . . -< Superior rectus [ Tonicity. ( Inferior rectus ) I Inferior oblique Contractility Synergists . . . ] A too high internus, or } Trmi _: tv e / A too high externus . f * Superior rectus in superversiou , , nterior rectl]s - Antagonists. | l^?^^,,,; or P"-^ [ A too low externus . . . . j i. Superior oblique Contractility f Synergists . . . 4 A too low internus, or j T>nni+ , , . ( A too low externus ....?.* Inferior rectus in sub-version ... f Supertor retus , ! An^gonists. | ngrnus, or [ A too high externus . ( Superior rectus } f Synergists . . . ] A too high internus, or > Tonicity. (( A too low externus . . . . ) [Inferior oblique... Antagonists ItoTfo^ernus, or [ A too high externus . {Inferior rectus ....... } A too low internus, or > Tonicity. A too high externus . . . ) Inferior oblique Superior oblique 1 Antagonists. ^n^'Sernus, or ["^- [ A too low externus . . . . j It will be noticed that in the above table each ocular muscle has one more antagonistic muscle than syner- gistic. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 61 Another point to be noted in the study of the table is that synergism more often comes from tonicity than from contractility, and that the antagonism shown comes entirely from tonicity. In this table the synergistic muscle, either by tonicity or contractility, aids a con- tracting" muscle in the performance of its principle func- tion only; and the antagonistic muscle, by its tonicity, hinders the contracting muscle in the performance of its principal function only. A single muscle may be both synergistic and antago- nistic. To illustrate: Rotation directly up is accom- plished by two muscles, the superior rectus and the in- ferior oblique. The principal action of the superior rec- tus would be to elevate the eye, but the secondary re- sults would be to tort the eye in and to turn it in. The inferior oblique, in its principal function, prevents the intorting, and the subsidiar} T result is to prevent the in- turning and to help in the upturning. The inferior ob- lique is, therefore, both synergistic and antagonistic to the superior rectus. In the truest sense, a synergistic muscle, by contrac- tility, should be one helping another muscle in the per- formance of its task of rotating the visual axis correctly; while an antagonistic muscle, by contractility, should be one to prevent an error in the relationship of the verti- cal axis of the eye with the median plane of the head. 62 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES It would seem, therefore, that a new table of syner- g-ism and antagonism should be constructed, in which no mention of muscles not contracting- should be made. Since this is a study of monocular rotation, the following- table may be considered a study of the rig-ht eye: TABLE. ( Synergist: None. (1) Rotation directly to the right: Externus -j ( Antagonist: None. ( Synergist: None. (2) Rotation directly to the left: Internus. . ] / Antagonist: None. ( Synergist: Inferior oblique. (3) Rotation directly up: Superior rectus . . -j ( Antagonist: Inferior oblique. ( Synergist: Superior oblique. (4) Rotation directly down: Inferior rectus . < ( Antagonist: Superior oblique. ( Synergist: Superior oblique. f Externus < (5) Rotation obliquely] ( Antagonist: Superior oblique, up-and-right - and ( Synergist: None. (_ Superior rectus . -J ( Antagonist: Superior oblique. i Synergist: None. ( Internus < (6) Rotation obliquely I ( Antagonist: Superior oblique, down-and-left -I and ( Synergist: Superior oblique. Inferior rectus. . -| ( Antagonist: Superior oblique. ( Synergist: None. Internus < (7) Rotation obliquely] ( Antagonist: Inferior oblique, up-and-left -j ( Synergist: Inferior oblique. ! Superior rectus . < ( Antagonist: Inferior oblique. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 63 i Synergist: Inferior oblique. f Externus - (8) Rotation obliquely j ( Antagonist: Inferior oblique. down-and-right -j ( Synergist: None, i Inferior rectus / Antagonist: Inferior oblique. BINOCULAR ROTATION. In the human being the two eyes are so placed and so adjusted as to make binocular single vision possible, in obedience to the supreme law of corresponding retinal points. To say that the macula of one eye must corre- spond, point for point, with the macula of the other e} T e; that the vertical meridian of one eye must correspond, point for point, with the vertical meridian of the other eye; that the horizontal meridian of the one eye, in like manner, must correspond with the horizontal meridian of the other eye, does not explain the fundamental fact which makes binocular single vision possible. There are eyes whose maculas and whose vertical and horizontal meridians do not correspond eyes that have never had binocular single vision and can never be made to have it. This abnormal condition was known to Von Graefe and was by him named "antipathy to binocular single vi- sion," but he made no attempt to explain it. For such eyes there is no circle (horopter or monoscopter) of single seeing with the two eyes. There is no such thing as a binocular spacial pole, and over such eyes the law of bin- 64 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES ocular rotation has no power. For these eyes, and they are fairly numerous, the fundamental fact underlying- binocular single vision does not exist. "What is the fundamental fact of corresponding- reti- nal points?" is a question worth the asking-. The au- thor believes he uttered the truth when he taug-ht, some years ag*o, that the secret of corresponding- retinal points is common brain-cell connection; that one macula corre- sponds, point for point, with the other macula only be- cause these corresponding- points have, g"oing- from them, two fibers which meet in the optic tract and go, side by side, back into the same cuneus to terminate in one com- mon cell in the visual center. Corresponding- points on the two vertical and the two horizontal meridians, like- wise corresponding points on any two oblique meridians similarly related to the vertical and horizontal meridians and to the maculas, must have common brain-cell con- nection. This explains double impressions, yet a single sensation two images, yet a single object. If the fibers from the right macula should go to the right side of the brain, and the fibers of the left macula should go to the left side of the brain, there would be two sensations excited, as certainly as that there have been two ret- inal impressions* Or, if the fibers going from retinal points that normally correspond, find their way back to the same cuneus, but terminate each in a separate brain- OF OCUIvAR ROTATIONS. 65 cell, there would be two sensations as certainly as that there have been the two retinal impressions. One or the other of these anatomic faults must exist in every person who has antipathy to binocular vision who has never seen singly with the two eyes and who can never be made to do so. A good illustration of common brain- cell connection may be had by any one riding- on a street car, when there are two individuals who wish to signal the conductor their desire to leave the car at the same crossing. The two press the button at the same moment of time with but one result, the ringing of one bell, just as if only one button had been pressed a double impres- sion with only one bell excited. If one button were con- nected with a bell at one end of the car, while the other button is connected with a bell at the other end of the car, or if the two bells be at the same end of the car and very near to each other, the pressing of the two buttons would make the two bells ring, but their sounds could not be blended into one. The law of visible direction does not explain corre- sponding retinal points, for this law is violated in the in- terest of binocular single vision whenever the prism is placed before either of the two eyes. Duction is possible only in violation of the law of direction. When there are no corresponding retinal points, nothing can interfere with the law of direction: Everything seen is on a line 66 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES connecting* the object and its image, which line passes through the center of rotation "every line of direction is a radius of retinal curvature prolonged." Nor is this law ever violated when there is only one eye. A person whose eyes have not corresponding retinal points is always strabismic, and while he may prefer to use almost constantly one eye, the other eye does not be- come blind from want of use, although mental suppres- sion alone prevents diplopia. This crossing exists from the day of birth to the day of death, yet the crossed eye continues to see well, whenever permitted to do so by the temporary obstruction of the other eye. In ordinary strabismus, as is well known, the deviating eye becomes mentally blind. Nature has two methods of preventing diplopia (1) fusion of the two images by bringing the two corre- sponding retinal points into conjunction with the two images; (2) mental suppression of one of the two images. Fusion is possible only when there are corresponding reti- nal points. Suppression is a necessity, (1) when there are no corresponding retinal points; (2) when there are corre- sponding retinal points, but mal-adjustment of the ocu- lar muscles makes it impossible to correctly relate these two points to the two images of the single object. In strabismic (heterotropic) eyes, when the seeing eye is being rotated in obedience to the law of monocular rota- OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 67 tion, the other eye moves, but not in the interest of either binocular single vision or correct orientation. The com- itant rotation of a squinting" eye differs in nothing- from the rotation of an eye that is stone-blind. Binocular rotation, as it will now be studied, is the rotation of the two eyes in the interest of binocular sin- gle vision and correct orientation. Binocular rotations are either cardinal or oblique. They are effected by the same muscles that are concerned in monocular rotations, but not wholly by the volitional brain-centers, which alone are concerned in monocular rotations. Besides the nine conjugate brain-centers, all under the control of the will, and each connected with two muscles, one belong- ing 1 to each eye, there are twelve centers controlled by the fusion faculty of the mind, each center being con- nected with only a single muscle. These fusion centers, as their name indicates, exist in the interest of binocular single vision; hence when there is a condition that would cause diplopia, whether the eyes be at rest or in rota- tion, one or more of these fusion centers must be in ac- tion. These fusion centers must be ready to act both independently of, and coordinately with, the conjugate centers. The conjugate centers furnish the neuricity that brings about all voluntary movements of the eyes, such as verting and converging. The single centers, in the sense of acting on only one muscle, are the centers 68 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES that effect duction, the power that overcomes double vi- sion, and planing-, the power that prevents diplopia. Fig. 15 shows, in a schematic way, the conjugate cen- ters and the single fusion centers. This figure also shows the two eyes with their twelve muscles; but, in its study, the imagination must make the connections. In "Ophthalmic Neuro-Myology " there is a separate il- lustration for each conjugate brain-center and the mus- cles controlled by it, in which the connecting nerve-fibers appear; and the connection of each fusion center and its muscle is also shown. As related to the volitional brain-centers which con- trol them, the twelve muscles belonging to the two eyes are arranged in pairs as follows: (1) The two superior recti, (2) the two inferior recti, (3) the two interni, (4) the right externus and the left internus, (5) the right in- ternus and the left externus, (6) the two superior ob- liques, (7) the two inferior obliques, (8) the right supe- rior oblique and the left inferior oblique, (9) the left su- perior oblique and the right inferior oblique. For each of the nine groups of two muscles there is a conjugate innervation center from which go fibers to be distributed equally to the two muscles to be controlled by it. The internal recti and the four obliques are each connected with two conjugate innervation centers, while the re- maining muscles are each under the control of only one conjugate innervation center. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 69 THE INNERVATIONS. To accomplish their work these muscles have nine conjugate innervations: (1) The one to elevate both eyes. (2) The one to depress both eyes. (3) The one to converge both eyes. (4) The one to move both eyes to the right. (5) The one to move both eyes to the left. (6) The one to keep the vertical axes from diverging above. (7) The one to prevent their converging above. These (6 and 7) are called into action by the guiding sensation, when the point of view is primary or in either one of the four cardinal directions. (8) The one to maintain the parallelism of the vertical axes and the median plane of the head when the point of view is obliquely up and to the right, or down and to the left; and (9) The one to maintain the parallelism of the vertical axes of the eyes and the median plane of the head, when the point of view is obliquely up and to the left, or down and to the right. The innervations one to five are for the recti, and the sixth, seventh, eighth, and ninth are for the ob- liques. Each of the conjugate innervation centers con- 70 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES trols one of the several pairs of muscles. The first con- trols the two superior recti; the second, the two inferior recti; the third, the two interni; the fourth, the right ex- ternus and the left internus; the fifth, the right internus and the left extern us; the sixth, the two superior ob- liques; the seventh, the two inferior obliques; the eighth, the right superior oblique and the left inferior oblique; the ninth, the left superior oblique and the right inferior oblique. The verting centers act singly only in the sweeping of the eyes in the horizontal plane; they act in pairs only when the eyes are moved directly up or down; three of these centers act together in oblique rotations of the eyes. It is only in oblique rotations that nor- mal ocular muscles need any other centers of nerve- power than the conjugate centers already studied. Eyes whose muscles are unequal in tonicity cannot have binocular single vision when controlled only by the con- jugate innervation centers, or even when at rest. Ob- lique rotations of eyes with normal muscles, and rota- tions of any character, or even rest, when muscles are unequal in tone, require that there shall be emergency or fusion brain-centers, one for each individual muscle. These centers, twelve in number, are certainly not under control of the will. They exist in the interest of binoc- ular single vision, and, therefore, may be said to be tinder the control of the fusion faculty of the mind. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 71 Their location is, probably, at the base of the brain; and certainly each of these centers has power over one ocu- lar muscle. Six of these fusion centers must be on either side of the brain. These should be numbered in harmony with the numbering- of the conjugate centers, and the same number should be given to two basal cen- ters, the words right and left to distinguish the one from the other. The right first center belongs to the right superior rectus, and the left first center belongs to the left superior rectus; the right second center belongs to the right inferior rectus, and the left second center be- longs to the left inferior rectus; the right third center belongs to the right internus, and the left third center belongs to the left internus; the right fourth center be- longs to the right externus, and the left fourth center belongs to the left externus; the right sixth center be- longs to the right superior oblique, and the left sixth center belongs to the left superior oblique; the right seventh center belongs to the right inferior oblique, and the left seventh center belongs to left inferior oblique. When one of these basal centers discharges neuricity, only a single muscle responds; when a conjugate center discharges neuricity, both muscles of a pair respond. The necessity for the existence of both the conjugate volitional centers and the basal fusion centers is shown in Fig. 14, which will now be studied. A careful study 72 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES ** H- bi) OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 73 of this figure will bring- many things into the light of the understanding. First of all, the figure shows three planes (1) the fixed horizontal plane of the head, a-b-c-d; (2) the fixed vertical plane of the head, g-h-i-j ; (3) Listing's plane, or the fixed transverse plane of the head, cutting the centers of the two eyes, this plane be- ing k-l-m-n. Each of these planes is at right-angles to the other two planes, and the three lines of intersec- tion are e-f, a-b, and h-g. In the horizontal plane are shown horizontal sections of the two eyes, r and s, and the two visual axes are ry and s-f. PLANES OF REFERENCE. The extended fixed vertical and horizontal planes of the head are planes of reference. With the head in the primary position, if a point is not in the horizontal plane, a-b-c-d, it is instantly known to be above or below it; if a point is not in the vertical plane, h-i-j-k, it is certainly known to be to the right or left of it; and an approxi- mately correct estimate of the degrees of removal of such point from these planes can be made in obedience to the law of visible direction. In the primary position of the head, the plane a-b-c-d is horizontal, and the eyes are in their primary positions when the two visual axes lie in this plane, and are converged on some point, at practical infinity, lying on the line of intersection, e-f, of 74 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES the horizontal and vertical planes. Converged on such a point, the visual axes are practically, though never really, parallel. To be perfectly parallel, the visual axis (r-f) of the right eye would have to be in the posi- tion r-f", and the visual axis (s-f) of the left eye would have to be s-f. In locating the position of any point in viewable space, the word right and left would be used in reference to the vertical plane of the head, h-i-j-k; the words above and below would be used in reference to the horizontal plane, a-b-c-d; and the word in should be used in reference to both the vertical and horizontal planes. The points/"' and/" are in the horizontal plane, the one to the right, the other to the left, of the vertical plane; the points z and z are in the vertical plane, respectively above and below the horizontal plane. All points lying on the line of intersection (e-f] of the two planes would be points directly ahead, and these would be primary points of view. All points off this line of intersection (e-f) would be secondary points of view. The four cardinal direc- tions a.rzf-i,f-j,f-c, zndf-d. All points not in the line c-d (or in the plane a-b-c-d) and not in the line i-j (or in the plane g-h-i-j) are obliquely related to these lines (or planes). The point of binocular view whether direct, cardinal, or oblique is the point of intersection of the OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. ?D visual axes, to be studied a little further on as the binoc- ular spacial pole. With the head fixed in the primary position, the point of binocular view may be changed from the primary point,/", to any secondary point, whether cardinal or ob- lique, in obedience to the law of binocular rotation. The absurdity of the claim that the axes of all binocular ro- tations, from the primary to a secondary point of view, or vice versa, must lie in Listing's plane, is made appar- ent by even a casual study of Fig. 14. In this figure, k-l-m-n represents Listing's plane, which is at right-an- gles to the planes a-b-c-d and h-i-j-k. Listing's plane is, therefore, a transverse vertical plane of the head, and it cuts the centers of rotation of the two eyes. It has been shown that the axes of all rotations lie in the plane of the equator of the eye, and that the equatorial plane is always at right-angles to the visual axis. Therefore, with every change in the position of the visual axis, there must be a change in the relationship of the plane of the equator with Listing's plane. There is only one point in the plane of the equator that never leaves the Listing plane, and that is the center of rotation. Since the transverse and vertical axes of the eyes, alone or to- gether, are the only axes of rotations, the relationship that these bear to Listing's plane is determined by the relationship that the visual axes bear to the fixed verti- 76 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES cal and horizontal planes of the head. When the visual axes, r-f and s-f, lie in the plane a-b-c-d, the vertical axes of the eyes must be in Listing's plane. Any move- ment of the eye that compels the visual axis to remain in the plane a-b-c-d, as from/" to/' or from fiof", must be around the vertical axis, which must lie in Listing's plane. Any rotation around the transverse axis will carry the point of fixation up to z or down to z', but in these rotations the transverse axis of each eye forms the same angle with a-b that r-f and s-f form with e-f, hence the rotation from f to z or z is about an axis that does not lie in Listing's plane, any more nearly than is there parallelism of r-/"and s-f with e-f. If the rota- tion upward were to start from/", the axis of rotation of the left eye would lie in Listing's plane, for the visual axis, in position of s-f , would be parallel with e-f; but the axis of the right eye would form the same angle with Listing's plane as its visual axis, in the position of r-f , forms with e-f. In a rotation starting upward from a point between /and/' or beyond/', neither of the two axes of rotation would lie in Listing's plane. The conclusion compelled by the above study of Fig. 14 is that Listing's plane cannot be a plane of reference, nor can it be a plane containing the axes of all rotations starting from, or returning to, the primary point of view. The equatorial plane of the eye contains both the verti- OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 77 cal and transverse axes of the eye, and it is around one or the other or both of these that all rotations, car- dinal and oblique, occur. Listing's plane, like List- ing's law, should be forgotten in the interest of truth. BRAIN-CENTERS FOR BINOCULAR ROTATIONS. These brain-centers, volitional and fusional, have al- ready been named. The study of Fig. 14 will convince even a skeptic that these centers are real and not imag- inary. The convergence of the visual axes, r-/"and s-f, to any point on e-f, is effected by the 3rd volitional center acting on both interni; turning the visual axes from f to/" is accomplished by the 4th volitional cen- ter acting on the right externus and the left internus; sweeping the visual axes from ftof is made possible by the action of the 5th volitional center on the right in- ternus and the left externus. The above centers have been pointed out first because they effect the simpler ro- tations. More complicated are the rotations directly up and down. Elevating the visual axes from^to z is accom- plished by the combined activity of two volitional cen- ters (a) the 1st conjugate acting on the two superior recti, and (b) the 7th conjugate acting on the two in- ferior obliques. Depressing the visual axes from f to z is effected by the harmonious activity of two volitional 78 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES centers (a) the 2nd conjugate acting on the inferior rectus of each eye, and (b) the 6th conjugate acting on the two superior obliques. More complicated still are oblique rotations, for each of these must be effected by the harmonious action of three volitional brain-centers; and, in addition to these, every oblique rotation requires activity on the part of two basal or fusion centers. If the visual axes are to be moved from fto any point above the horizontal plane and to the right of the vertical plane, this motion must re- sult from the harmonious action of the 1st conjugate center on the two superior recti, the 4th conjugate on the right externus and left internus, and the 8th con- jugate on the right superior oblique and the left inferior oblique. In the right-sweep part of this rotation, since the left inferior oblique would hinder the left internus, while the right superior oblique would help the right ex- ternus, the hindered left internus must receive a supple- mental impulse from the left 3rd basal or fusion center, whose sole power pertains to that muscle. Since, in the upward part of this oblique rotation, the right superior rectus would be hindered by the right superior oblique, while the left superior rectus would be helped by the left inferior oblique, the hindered right superior rectus must receive supplemental neuricity from the right 1st basal center, else the right visual axis would lag behind the OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 79 left visual axis. Whatever the degree of obliquity and in whatsoever direction, the rotation must be accom- plished by the harmonious action of three conjugate vo- litional centers and two basal or fusion centers. In order to bring" into view the only remaining 1 conju- gate brain-center connected with the extrinsic ocular muscles while studying- Fig-. 14, the rotation may be con- sidered as from/" to a point above the horizontal plane and to the left of the vertical plane. This point can be reached only as the result of the harmonious activity of the 1st, 5th, and 9th conjugate centers. The left superior rectus, hindered by the left superior oblique, must receive supplemental neuricity from the left 1st basal center; and the right internus, hindered by the right inferior oblique, must receive supplemental neuricit} r from the right 3rd basal center. In like manner might be studied rotations from/" to some point below the horizon- tal plane and to the left of the vertical plane, or to some point below the horizontal plane and to the right of the vertical plane, but in every such study the fact would be- come apparent that three volitional and two fusion cen- ters have been acting in the interest of binocular single vision and correct orientation. In every rotation above the horizontal plane, a-b-c-d, the 1st conjugate center participates; in every rotation below this plane the 2nd conjugate center has a part. 80 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES In every rotation to the right of the vertical plane, g-h-i-j, the 4th conjugate center is active, and in every rota- tion to the left of this plane the 5th conjugate center participates. The 3rd conjugate center is concerned in the fixing of any point in space. Thus the five con- jugate centers controlling the four recti muscles, become more real when studied in the light of Fig. 14. Rotation from/" to any point in space above or below the horizontal plane, a-b-c-d, must call into action some one of the four conjugate centers connected with the ob- lique muscles. In rotations from /"to z, the 7th conju- gate center keeps the vertical axes of the eyes parallel with the vertical plane, g-h-i-j', and in rotations from/" to z ', the 6th conjugate center maintains this parallelism. In rotations up-and-to-the-right and down-and-to-the- left, the 8th conjugate center prevents torsioning to- ward the right; and torsioning toward the left is pre- vented by the 9th conjugate center when the rotations are up-and-to-the-left and down-and-to-the-right. Ac- tivity of the 8th and 9th conjugate centers arouses into action six of the right and left fusion or basal centers belonging to the recti, but only two of these at any one time. The two basal centers never excited in oblique rotations of eyes whose muscles are normal, are the right 4th and the left 4th. Cardinal rotations by nor- mal muscles do not excite a single fusion center. Thus OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 81 far the study of Fig 1 . 14 has demonstrated the existence of all of the nine conjugate brain-centers belonging" to the recti and obliques, and six of the eight right and left basal or fusion centers connected with the recti. The demonstration of the fact of the existence of the right and left individual fusion centers connected with the recti, and the four like centers connected with the obliques, is made easy by a further study of Fig. 14. Nor- mal recti muscles, in a state of tonicity, would converge the visual axes at/", if that point were twenty feet distant, and would keep the two visual axes in the plane a-b-c-d\ and the obliques of equal tonicity would confine the two horizontal retinal meridians in this plane. In such a condition of equal tonicity, neither volitional nor fusion brain-centers are active, when both the head and the eyes are in their primary positions. Convergence of the visual axes and planing these axes and the horizontal meridians are conditions essential to binocular single vision; but the existence of single vision with the two eyes does not guarantee that all the muscles are normal in tone. If tonicity cannot effect binocular single vision, this must be accomplished by contractility of the muscles wanting in tone. The source of the required neuricity is not con- trolled by the will, but by the fusion faculty of the mind, itself dominated by the desire for single vision. Every ocular muscle has its individual fusion center, the neu- 82 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES ricity from which acts on only the one muscle. These centers have already been individualized by numbers, and the existence of six of the right and left fusion cen- ters for the recti has been proved in the study of oblique rotations, in the light of Fig. 14. By means of this fig- ure the existence of all the fusion centers, eight for the recti and four for the obliques, can be demonstrated from two different vantage grounds viz., the tests for heterophoria and the duction tests. The proof, from the standpoint of heterophoric tests, will presuppose the uncomplicated existence of each of the several errors to be studied in Chapter III. One eye must be taken off its guard in order that any one of these errors may become manifest. The placing of a 6 prism, base up, before the right eye in Fig. 14, would double/", the false f being thrown to the position of /. The right eye now off its guard will assume the position that muscle tonicity would give to it, and not a single muscle belonging to this eye will receive neuricity from any source, provided the mind uses the left eye for fixing the true/. If the false /"be directly under the true/", it thus appears because the externus and internus of the right eye have equal tonicity, and the visual axis of this eye, notwithstanding the prism diplopia, will be pointing to the true/. By removal of the prism the diplopia is made to disappear without the turning of either eye. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 83 If the false/", instead of being- at z\ stands directly under f", it is so because the right eye, in assuming' its position of rest, has its visual axis pointing to f (esophoria). On removing" the prism the false / would jump into the position of f" and would remain there so long 1 as the right visual axis points to/'. This diplopia would not be tolerated. To overcome it, the fusion faculty would unlock the right 4th basal center and cause neuricity to flow to the rig-ht externus, whose contraction would pull the eye so as to quickly bring- its visual axis from/' to/". The left eye, throughout the diplopia, and during the recovery of binocular single vision, has remained sta- tionary, for it has received no fusion impulse. The right fourth basal center alone has acted, and only the exter- nal rectus of the right eye has responded. Any impulse to any muscle of the left eye would have made the vis- ual axis leave the point/". In the same manner the left eye could be tested, and by the test the left 4th fu- sion center could be found. Placing the prism, base up, before the right eye, the false f would be thrown directly down to z'\ but instead of appearing directly below/", it would seem to be under f , and only because excessive tonicity of the externus of the right eye (exophoria) has turned it out so that its vis- ual axis points tof". On removing the prism the false/ would jump to the position off and would remain there 84 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES if no fusion effort were made; but instantly the right 3rd fusion center alone would be excited, and its dis- charged neuricity would compel the right internus to pull the right eye so that its visual axis shall move from f" to f. In this fusion effort not a muscle connected with the left eye has received any impulse, else its vis- ual axis would have been carried away from point f. By applying the test to the left eye, the existence of the left 3rd basal center could be demonstrated. By placing a 10 prism, base in, before the right eye, a false /"would be thrown to d. If the false/ appears to be at d, it is because of the fact that the superior and inferior recti of the right eye are equal in tonicity; and notwithstanding the diplopia, the right visual axis con- tinues to point to/. Removal of the prisrn makes the false/ jump at once into the true/, and that, too, with- out any movement of either eye. But if the false f appears under d, on a level with z , it is because the right superior rectus, endowed with an excess of tonicity (hyperphoria),has elevated the eye so that its visual axis points to z. On removal of the prism the false f would jump into z' and would remain there if no effort at fusion were put forth; but this effort at fusion will be made and the diplopia will disappear. The power this time comes from the right 2nd basal center, and it acts only OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 85 on the right inferior rectus, which at once turns the eye down so that its visual axis shall point to /. With the 10 prism, base in, before the right eye, the false f may be made to appear above d, on a level with z, because of excessive tonicity on the part of the right inferior rectus (cataphoria). Now the right visual axis will point to z . On removal of the prism the false / will jump into z and will remain there, should the right visual axis continue to point to z . To overcome this diplopia, the right 1st basal center calls into action the right superior rectus alone, and by this action the right eye is elevated so that its visual axis may point to/", the diplopia disappearing at that moment. The existence of the rig-Jit 1st and 2nd basal or fu- sion centers has thus been proved from a second stand- point. Placing the 10 prism before the left eye, the fact of the existence of the left 1st and 2nd fusion centers can be shown. The eight fusion centers, each controlling a single rectus muscle in the interest of binocular single vision, certainly exist. A Maddox triple rod placed vertically before each eye, with a 6 prism, base up, behind the right rod, will make two streaks of light when a candle or gas jet is the test object. The upper streak should be fixed. If the lower streak is parallel with the upper one, it is because the right superior and inferior obliques are equal in tonicity. 86 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES If the lower streak diverges from the upper at their left ends, it is because the right inferior oblique, possessing an excess of tonicity, has tilted the horizontal retinal meridian down at the right (plus cyclophoria). Remov- ing the 6 prism, the two streaks can be made to blend throughout only because the right 6th fusional or bas- al center compels the right superior oblique to restore the horizontal retinal meridian to its correct position in the horizontal plane, a-b-c-d. No other center has acted on any other muscle connected with either eye. If, with the prism behind the right rod, the lower streak has lost parallelism by leaning down to the right, this would be due to the fact that the right superior oblique, with excessive tonicity, has torted the right eye in (minus cyclophoria). On removal of the prism the two streaks could blend only because of activity of the 7th basal center compelling the weak inferior oblique to replace the horizontal meridian in the horizontal plane, from which it had been thrown when the eye was off its guard. Thus has it been proved that there are right 6th and 7th basal or fusion centers. By placing the prism behind the left rod, the fact of the existence of the left 6th and 7th basal or fusion centers is also prov- able. No additional proof should be necessary in order to convince even the most skeptical concerning the twelve OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 87 fusion centers, and the tasks they are set to perform. The one remaining' method of proving- their existence is the duction test. This must be studied in connection with Fig. 14, in order that the whole truth may be told about them. The best means for taking- the duction power of the recti muscles is the rota.ry prism of the Monocular Pho- rometer, but this test may be made by means of the loose prism in the refraction case. With the rotary prism, the fusion centers involved will discharg-e neuricity in in- creasing 1 quantity, as the imag-e is being- gradually moved, up to the point when fusion is no longer possible. The placing- of a loose prism before the eye excites suddenly and to the full extent the fusion center and the muscle under its control. In the former method the eye glides g-ently; in the latter method the eye jumps violently. Each method is as capable as the other in proving- the existence of the individual fusion centers connected with the recti muscles. Since the author prefers the revolv- ing- prism, that method will be made to give evidence concerning the existence of these centers in the still fur- ther study of Pig. 14. The phorometer, without the supernumerary displacing prism, should be placed in front of the right 'eye in position for taking abduction and adduction. With the index at zero, the two or- thophoric eyes will fix the pointy as a result of muscle 88 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES tonicity only. By turning- the index in the temporal arc, the image of /on the right retina will be carried nasal- ward that is, toward e in that eye. The only way to prevent diplopia is for the macula to keep under the mov- ing image. Since the image is moving nasal-ward on the horizontal meridian, the macula must move directly nasal-ward and in no other direction. This motion of the eye is accomplished by a discharge of neuricity from the right 4th basal center to the right externus alone. During this revolution of the prism, no other brain-cen- ter has been excited, and no other ocular muscle has contracted. When the index has been returned to zero, there is easy binocular vision at the expense of muscle tonicity only. Revolving the index of the prism into the nasal arc will make the image of f move temple-ward that is, toward a. At the very beginning of the rotation of the prism the right 3rd fusion center becomes active, and the neuricity from it makes the right internus move the eye so that the macula may remain beneath the moving image. This action of the right 3rd fusion center on the right internus alone, has prevented diplopia. No im- pulse has gone from any other center to any other muscle belonging to either eye. Thus abduction and adduction of the right eye prove the existence of the rig-Jit 4th and 3rd fusion centers. In the same manner abduction OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 89 and adduction of the left eye may be made to prove the existence of the left 4th and 3rd fusion centers. The prism should now be placed before the right eye for the taking" of superduction and subduction. With the index at zero, the two orthophoric eyes will have easy binocular single vision at the expense of muscle tonicity only. When the index is being rotated into the upper arc, the image of f on the right retina is made to move downward on the vertical meridian. The macula must remain under the moving image, else the object/ will be doubled, the false /"appearing directly above the true f. This moving of the macula is made possible by the con- traction of the right superior rectus, in response to an impulse coming from the right 1st fusion center. No other muscle contracts, nor does any other center dis- charge neuricity for effecting this fusion. Returning the index to zero relieves both the right 1st fusion cen- ter and the right superior rectus from further activity. If the index is now made to move downward, the image of f on the right retina will be moved upward on the ver- tical meridian. The macula must be kept under the moving image or a false /"will appear directly below the true f. To prevent this diplopia, the right 2nd fusion center will be made to discharge neuricity to the right inferior rectus alone. No other center and no other mus- cle will be brought into action for maintaining this fu- 90 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES sion, and this center and its muscle cease activity the moment the index is returned to zero. Thus the taking- of right superduction and subduction proves the exist- ence of the right 1st and 2nd fusion centers. By plac- ing 1 the prism before the left eye in position for tak- ing- left superduction and subduction, the existence of the left 1st and 2nd fusion centers may be proved. In all these duction tests it will be observed that fusion has been accomplished in violation of the law of direction as it applies to the rig-lit eye. As the macula moves in, the visual axis moves from y 7 toward /"'; as the macula moves out, the visual axis moves f rom f toward/"'; as the macula moves downward, the visual axis moves from f toward z; and as the macula moves upward, the visual axis moves from/" toward z '. The fused point, /, is on the visual axis of the eye not under test, but is off the visual axis of the ducted eye. In the duction act of fusion, the line connecting- the fused point and its dis- placed imag-e is not a true line of direction, for it does not cut the center of the retinal curve. The existence of the individual fusion centers for the obliques cannot be proved by prisms, but the Maddox rods g-ive positive evidence. With a triple rod before each eye, set vertically, and with no prism behind either rod, the test candle will appear as a sing-le streak of lig-ht. Turning- the rig-lit rod toward the temple, with- in the arc of possible fusion for an oblique muscle, will OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 91 not result in doubling- the line, but this doubling- will be prevented by the rig-lit 7th fusion center acting- on the right inferior oblique. This activity of center and muscle places the horizontal retinal meridian under the inclined streak of lig-ht. If the rig-ht rod had been turned toward the nose, in the arc of possible fusion for an oblique mus- cle, the diplopia would have been prevented by the dis- charg-e of neuricity from the rig-ht 6th fusion center to the rig-ht superior oblique. In each of these efforts at fusion, only one center and one muscle have been active. By these tests the existence of the right 7th and 6th fu- sion centers has been proved. Leaving- the rig-lit rod vertical and turning- the left rod slig-htly out and in would prove, in like manner, the existence of the left 7th and 6th centers. A final source of proof concerning- the existence of the four fusion centers for the oblique muscles is oblique astig-matism of one eye, while the astigmatism of the other eye is either vertical or horizontal. In the latter eye a horizontal line will have a horizontal imagfe, but in the former eye the imag-e of the horizontal line will be displaced toward the meridian of greatest curva- ture, hence this imag-e will be oblique. To fuse the ob- lique imag-e with the one that is horizontal, an oblique muscle belong-ing- to the oblique-astig-matic e}*e, under the influence of the 6th or the 7th fusion center on the 92 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES corresponding 1 side, must force the horizontal retinal meridian under the displaced image. It would be the 7th center and the inferior oblique if the most curved meridian is in the upper nasal quadrant; it would be the 6th center and superior oblique if the meridian of great- est curvature is in the upper temporal arc, whether it be the one eye or the other. For further, and a more comprehensive, study of the ocular muscles, normal and abnormal, from the brain- side of all questions that can arise, the reader is referred to the author's other book, "Ophthalmic Neuro-Myol- ogy," a necessary companion volume to this book. In this companion volume the brain-centers, voluntary and fusion, and their muscle connections, are freely and fully illustrated. The origin, course, and distribution of all the motor nerves of the eye are also fully illustrated. In all, there are thirty-nine full-page plates, the reproduced one, (Fig 1 . 15) on next page, constituting 1 the foundation for all the others. Besides these full-pag'e cuts, there are twelve smaller illustrations. The book contains 210 pages of illustrations and reading matter, all pertaining to the nervous system as related to the ocular muscles, extrinsic and intrinsic. In Fig. 15 the upper part rep- resents, schematically, the volitional and fusional brain- centers the former at the margin, the latter grouped near the median line. All these centers exist in dupli- OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 93 3 Fig. 15- 94 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES cate, but the volitional centers are not all active. The smaller circles at the periphery represent the centers unused by a right-handed man, only one of which is in the left side of the brain. Conjugate and basal centers 10 and 11 belong to the ciliary body and iris. While only half of the conjugate centers are ever used, all of the basal centers stand ready for action at the command of the fusion faculty of the mind. THE HOROPTER MONOSCOPTER ISOGONAL ClRCLE. There is now neither mystery nor complicated mathe- matics connected with the circle bearing the three names: Horopter, Monoscopter, and Isogonal Circle. The sim- plicity of this comes as the result of the author's discov- ery that the macula is the posterior pole of the eye, and that the center of rotation is the point of crossing of all visual lines. Fig. 16 is a photographic reproduction of the first correct horopteric figure ever published, and was constructed by the author in 1892, immediately after his discovery of the true law of visible direction viz., All lines of direction are radii of retinal curvature pro- longed. Le Conte, in "Sight," first edition (1882), thought he had correctly constructed Mueller's horopter. This is shown in his reproduced figure, Fig. 9 (page 36), in which the circle is constructed through the two cen- ters of rotation and the point of fixation; but he has OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 95 ruined the figure by constructing 1 his indirect visual lines in such a way as to make them cross the visual axes at the nodal points. Noyes evidently had the same concep- tion of Mueller's horopter, for he said: "It is a circle which passes through the center of rotation of each eye and the point of fixation of the visual axes." If Noyes had drawn indirect visual lines, he would have made them cross the visual axes at the nodal points, as did Le Conte. At any rate, there was something" that confused him, for he said: " This statement is not strictly correct, but will suffice for our purposes." No horopteric circle is correct that does not make indirect visual lines cross the visual axes at the center of rotation. Le Conte, in his 1897 edition of " Sight," published a new figure which he claimed as a true presentation of the Mueller horopter. This 1897 figure is reproduced in Fig. 10 (page 37). This circle is constructed through the two nodal points and the point of fixation, and the direct and indirect visual lines are made to cross each other on the circle. Le Conte's two figures as re- produced in this chapter, on pages 36 and 37, should be studied in contrast with each other. In Fig. 9, the vis- ual axes, under the same angle of convergence, could move from point to point on the circle, each eye around the point common to the visual axis and the circle; but the visual axes A-c and A-c could never be made to take 96 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES the place of the indirect visual lines B-b and B-b' , for his indirect visual lines do not cut the visual axes at the cen- ters of rotation, but at n and ri , the so-called nodal points. It will be further observed that the two eyes in Fig. 9 are ideal eyes of Helmholtz, in that the visual axes pass through the centers of rotation. As erroneous as is Fig-. 9, Fig-. 10 is still worse. If the visual axes A-c and A-c should move from A to B, neither eye would rotate around a point common to its visual axis and the horopter, hence the points n and n would be made to leave the circle; or if the circle should move with the nodal points, it would be forced to leave the points A and B. It is equally clear that A-c and A-c could never be made to take the positions B-b and B-b'. The true horopter, in the sense that it is the circle of binocular single vision, both direct and indirect, as shown in Fig-. 16, is based on three great facts: (a) The macula of all eyes is the posterior pole, and the visual axis is the antero-posterior axis of the eye; (b) all indirect lines of vision cross the visual axis at the center of rotation; (c) corresponding retinal points have a common brain-cell connection, and these points bear identical relationship, in degrees, to their respective maculas. In this figure the circle is constructed through two fixed points and one changeable point, the former being the centers of ro- OP OCULAR ROTATIONS. 97 tation of the eyes, b and d, and the latter the point of direct fixation. This circle is b-d-e-c-a. Any point so situated on the circle as to throw light into the two eyes will be seen as a single point, for the images will be on corresponding retinal points. The direct point of view, Fig. i 6. c, and its images, h and gr, will be connected by lines that cut the centers of rotation, b and d. The secondary point of view a and its images, / and /, will be connected by lines passing through the centers of rotation, b and d; and the secondary point e will be connected with its images, / and k, by lines that cut the visual axes at the 98 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES centers of rotation. That all points on this circle, whether direct or indirect, will be seen under the same angle, is proved by the fact that each angle is measured by half the arc b-d, for each is an inscribed angle, with this com- mon arc. If the visual axes should be moved from c to , they will take the positions of the indirect visual lines, a-j and a-f; if the visual axes should be moved from c to e, they will take the positions of the indirect lines, e-l and e-k. The figure also shows that the direct and the indi- rect points of view are related in degrees as are their re- spective images. The angle c-b-a is an inscribed angle and is measured by half the arc a-c. The angle h-b-j is an angle at the center and is measured by the whole arc h-j. But these angles are opposite and are, therefore, equal. The angle a-d-c is equal to the angle a-b-c, for it, too, is measured by half the same arc, a-c; therefore the angle g'-d-f is equal to the angle h-b-j. Since/ and f are similarly related, in degrees, to the maculas, ^and^, they are corresponding retinal points, for such points have common brain-cell connection. The statements made above are strictly correct, and, therefore, Fig. 16 TV ill suffice for all purposes in the study of binocular sin- gle vision and binocular rotations. Points on the circle whose indirect visual lines cross m-a at the same point are equally far removed from the direct point of view. To show this, the line m-c was OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 99 drawn. For a different purpose entirely, and one more practical, the line b-d was placed in the figure. Having 1 demonstrated the curved line of binocular sin- gle vision, it was only one step to the demonstration of the curved surface of binocular single vision. The au- thor suggested to Dr. Manning- Brown, now of Hopkins- ville, Ky., then his private student, that this surface could be generated by revolving- the circular plane, b-d-e-c-a, on the cord, b-d. Dr. Brown at once volun- teered to have this peculiar surface of single seeing- made in wood, by means of the skillful use of the turning- lathe. This he succeeded in doing-. This oddly, but beautifully, shaped piece of wood was then cut along- a plane including- the line b-d, into two equal parts, with a very delicate saw. E}ach of the two cut surfaces pre- sented a plane the outlines of which were larg-e seg-ments of two circles, as shown in Fig-. 17, which represents a vertical section of this model. It is clear that the parts above and below b-d are pre- cisely alike. In this figure, b and d represent the cen- ters of the two eyes, and b-d is the line connecting- these centers. The circular planes shown on either side of b-d, each represents perfectly the plane of the horop- ter. A section made from any point on the surface of this model, along- a plane including- the line b-d, would have shown the two conjoined horopteric planes just as 100 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES Fig. 17. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 101 depicted in Fig. 17. As on the line, so on the surface generated, there is not a point so situated as to send light into the two eyes but that it would be seen as a single point with the two eyes. The concave area of binocular single seeing is clearly shown in Fig. 25. The section of this model should have led at cnce to the making of Fig. 18, but, in fact, it was fifteen years later (1907) before the author found the artist who could make this complicated figure, to be studied further on. Referring again to Fig. 16, it may be stated that, since b-a-c-e-d is the line of binocular single vision, all points within and beyond it should be seen double. This is lit- erally true as applied to small circles; and it is alwa} T s true as to near-by points, however large the circle may be. If the horopteric circle has a diameter of thirty feet, objects beyond will not appear as double, though points, if they could be seen so far, would be double. Even the double appearance of near-by objects, when the circle is small or large, is not confusing, nor is it hurtful, for no attempt is made by the mind or eyes to fuse such images. In 1898, Maddox published in his book, " The Ocular Muscles," a cut which he named the " Isogonal Circle." This circle he constructed through the centers of rotation and the point of fixation. In the cut he had no indirect visual lines, but only the two visual axes, converged first on the direct point on the circle, and then rotated to a sec- 102 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES ondary point. His purpose was to show that the angle of conversance was the same for the two points. If he had seen, he had not accepted, the teaching of the au- thor that all points on this circle, whether direct or indi- rect, were seen as single points and under a common an- gle. The author, some years before, had named Fig. 16 "the monoscopter, " the meaning of which is "line of binocular single vision." Either one of the names is bet- ter than the older name, " horopter," which means " the limit of seeing." Since points, to be seen as single, must be seen under the same angle, and for other reasons, the name " isogonal circle " is preferable to the name "mono- scopter." The name "isogonal circle " may be defined as follows: The circle on which all visual lines, lying in a common plane and coming from corresponding retinal points, converge, each two lines forming the same angle as do all other two lines. This has been shown to be true in the study of Pig. 16. The name " isogonal " may be made to apply to the surface of single seeing as well as to the circle. ' ' Isogonal surface ' ' may be defined as fol- lows: The two visual lines, whether direct or indirect, from corresponding retinal points, converging at any point on this surface, form the same angle as the two vis- ual lines converging at any other point on this surface. The definition of the isogonal circle as given by Mad- dox, in connection with his figure of the circle constructed OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 103 through the centers of rotation and the point of fixation, is as follows: " It is the curve of uniform convergences and of equal lateral ductions for the two eyes." c' M Fig. i 8. In the study of Fig 1 . 18, published for the first time in 1907, the author accepts the name " isogonal circle " in its fuller meaning, in preference to either of the two other names " horopter " and "monoscopter." Equal 104 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES angles of all two visual lines from corresponding- retinal points on a circle (isogonal) means binocular single vi- sion; binocular single vision of points on a circle (mono- scopteric) means equal angles. Since the two terms mean practically the same thing, "isogonal" has the preference over " monoscopteric " when joined with "circle," because it is more easily pronounced and is more pleasing to both the ear and the eye. The spherical concavity of the retinas, corresponding retinal points, and the law of visible direction, make pos- sible the mathematical circle and surface of binocular single vision. In the light of Fig. 18, the isogonal circles will be studied as belonging to one of two classes. To the first class belongs only one circle, and there is no bet- ter way to distinguish it from the many members of the other class, than by naming it the Primary Isogonal Cir- cle. The circles of the other class should be known as Secondary Isogonal Circles. THE PRIMARY ISOGONAL CIRCLE. In Fig. 18, M-C represents the extended median plane of the head, and C is a point on the line of intersection of this plane and the horizontal plane of the head. With C as the point of fixation, the primary isogonal circle must pass through it. The other two points through which this circle must pass are the centers of rotation of the two eyes, D and B. The primary isogonal circle, B-D-C, thus constructed, OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 105 presupposes that both the head and the eyes are in their primary positions. The distinguishing fact of this circle is that in its plane lie the two visual axes and the two horizontal retinal meridians, and that the visual axes are converged to a point on it. On either side of the point of fixation are many indirect points of view on this circle, and in its plane lie twice as many indirect lines of vision, for to each point belong two lines. THE SECONDARY ISOGONAL, CIRCLES are constructed through the two centers of rotation, B and D, and through indirect points of view lying in the extended vertical plane of the head, both above and below C, each and all of these points being the same distance from the point of intersection of M-C and B-D as is the point C. Fig. 18 shows only one of these secondary circles, B-D-C '. Lying on this circle to either side of C', itself a second- ary point are many other secondary points, as E. Each of these secondary circles has on it only secondary points of view, and in its plane lie only indirect visual lines, two for each point. The plane of no secondary isogonal cir- cle contains a retinal meridian, but it intersects the planes of all the retinal meridians. The number of sec- ondary isogonal circles can be computed if they should be considered as only 1" apart. By reference to Fig. 24, it will be seen that the upper part of the field of binocular single vision is 55 and the lower part of this field is 70. 106 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES This would give 198,000 secondary isogonal circles above the primary circle, and 252,000 below it, making- a total of 450,000 secondary isogonal circles to one primary isogo- nal circle. All isogonal circles, whether primary or secondary, are alike in the following respects: (a) They are all con- structed through two common points, the centers of ro- tation, B and Z?, of the two eyes; (b) they all have a common cord (J3-D], the lines connecting the centers of the two eyes; (c) they are all bisected b} r the extended median plane of the head; (d) all points on all of the cir- cles, belonging to one group, so located as to send light into the two eyes, will be seen as single points; (e) the two lines of vision connecting any secondary point, on any circle of a given group, with its two images, have the same angle as that formed by the convergence of the visual axes on the point of direct view the angles B-A-D, B-C'-D, and B-E-D are equal to the angle B-C-D. What has been said above in (a), (b), and (c) applies to all circles of all groups. What has been said in (d) and (e) applies to any single group of isogonal circles circles that have the same diameter. An infinite num- ber of points lie on the line of intersection of the extended vertical and horizontal planes of the head, and each of these points may become the primary point of view the OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 107 point of direct fixation. For each of these points there is a possible primary isogonal circle; hence the number of possible primary isogonal circles is infinite, but only one can exist at a time. Each new primary isogonal circle creates a new group of secondary circles, all of equal size. There is no point in the space devoted to binocular sin- gle vision (see Fig 1 . 24) that does not lie on some isogonal circle, or in the plane of one of these circles. The field of binocular rotations is a little smaller than the field of binocular single vision, but any secondary point within this smaller field may become the point of fixation, and that, too, regardless of the size of the circle on which the secondary point may be located. The degree of convergence of the visual axes at a sec- ondary point will be the same as if they were converged on the direct point of that primary circle which belongs to the same group. This is well shown in Fig. 18, in which the visual axes can be considered as moved, first from C to C', and again from C' to E. In either case the visual axes have been made to take the position of indirect visual lines, under the same angle of convergence. If the point for indirect fixation is on a smaller or lar- ger circle than is the primary point of view, the primary circle, which must move with the visual axes, must grow smaller or larger until it shall finally include the second- 108 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES ary point to be fixed. If, at the beginning- of a rotation, the second point of view lies on the circumference of the primary isogonal circle, that circle neither enlarges nor does its plane move, while the visual axes move from one point of view to the other. If the second point of view is at a greater distance than the first point, but in the plane with it, the circle enlarges, but the plane does not move, as the visual axes pass from the one point to the other. If the second point of view is in the plane of a secondary isogonal circle, the plane of the primary circle moves into the position of the secondary circle as the vis- ual axes converge on the second point. No point in viewable space can be fixed until the plane of the pri- mary isogonal circle is made to include it, as the visual axes converge upon it. From the foregoing it will be seen that the plane of all isogonal circles are movable planes, and that the common axis around which they rotate is the line connecting the centers of the two eyes the line B-D in Fig. 18. If the visual axes rise, the primary isogonal rises with them; if the visual axes must be depressed, the primary isogonal circle must go down with them. As the primar} T isogo- nal circle rotates, all secondary isogonal circles rotate with it, in the same direction and to the same extent. Thus within the limit of vertical rotations, the plane of the primary circle may be made to assume the former OF OCUL,AR ROTATIONS. 109 position of the plane of any secondary circle. As the plane of the primary circle rises or falls, the visual axes move in the moving- plane, if the second point is obliquely located, so as to converge on the second point of view at the moment that the plane reaches that point. In doing this the point of convergence moves along a binocular spacial meridian. (See Fig. 25.) THE LAW OF BINOCULAR REST AND MOTION. THE TWELVE EXTRINSIC MUSCLES OF NORMAL EYES, UNDER THE CONTROL OF THE NINE CONJUGATE, AND THE TWELVE FUSION, BRAIN-CENTERS, MUST SO RE- LATE THE TWO EYES THAT THEIR TWO VISUAL AXES AND THE TWO HORIZONTAL RETINAL MERIDIANS SHALL ALWAYS LIE IN THE PLANE OF THE PRIMARY ISOGONAL CIRCLE, WHETHER AT REST OR IN MOTION, AND THAT THE TWO VISUAL AXES SHALL CONVERGE AT SOME POINT ON THIS CIRCLE, IN THE INTEREST OF BOTH BIN- OCULAR SINGLE VISION AND CORRECT ORIENTATION. The two eyes, while obeying the law of binocular ro- tation, do not violate the law of monocular motion. Each visual axis moves in the plane of that individual retinal meridian projected into space, on which lie both the first and the second points of view, during which motion the vertical axis of the eye will be kept parallel with the median plane of the head. The fusion centers, not used 110 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES in monocular rotations, are essential in binocular rota- tions. Without the fusion centers, even when muscles are normal in tone, all oblique rotations would be at- tended by diplopia; and without them, muscles of unequal tonicity would cause diplopia, whether the e} r es be at rest or in motion, as shown in the study of Fig-. 14. The cord common to all isog-onal circles the line, JB-D, connecting 1 the centers of the two eyes should always lie in the horizontal plane of the head; but this cannot be if one eye is set lower in the orbit than is the other. Even in such faulty eyes the horizontal retinal meridians must lie in the plane of the laterally inclined primary isog-- onal circle, when both eyes are being 1 used. Should the plane be inclined 5, the vertical axes of the two eyes must be inclined throug-h the same arc, toward the side of the lower eye. This would be effected by activity of either the 8th or 9th conjugate center, on the superior oblique of one eye and the inferior oblique of the other. With one eye covered, as in the work of refraction, the con jug-ate cen- ter would cease its activity and the eye would then have its vertical axis normally related to the median plane of the head. On uncovering- the eye, the two vertical axes would be tilted ag-ain toward the side of the lower eye. The practical point growing- out of this observation is that such eyes would require the shifting- of the axes of correcting- cylinders toward the side of the lower eye, OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. Ill through arcs corresponding- to the lateral displacement of the horizontal retinal meridians. In the interest of binocular single vision, but against correct orientation, the horizontal retinal meridians of uncorrected oblique astigmatic eyes must be forced out of the plane of the primary isogonal circle. This sub- ject will be given proper emphasis in the chapter on "Compensating Cyclotropia." In that chapter the 6th and 7th conjugate centers are represented, respectively, as the source of power for converging and diverging the vertical axes of the eyes in the interest of fusion of dis- placed images. It probably would be more nearly cor- rect to say that this work is accomplished by the right and left 6th, and the right and left 7th, basal centers, as has been pointed out in the study of Fig. 14. The fusion function must be under the control of the fusion faculty of the mind, and this faculty presides over the single basal centers, right and left. Emmetropia and orthophoria make it easy for the ocu- lar muscles to obey the law of binocular rotation. Hy- peropia and myopia interfere with the act of convergence; oblique astigmatism, with meridians of greatest curva- ture diverging or converging, makes it impossible for the obliques to keep the horizontal retinal meridians in the plane of the primary isogonal circle; hyperphoria and cataphoria make it hard for the superior and inferior 112 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES recti to plane the visual axes; exophoria and esophoria make difficult the task of converging the visual axes to points on the circle, and the shifting of the visual axes in Fig. 19. the plane of the circle, by the lateral recti. In the light of these facts, it would appear that the e} T es, not nat- OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 113 urally so, should be made emmetropic and orthophoric by art. THE MUSCLE INDICATOR. This device is the primary isogonal circle put in material form, as shown in the half-tone picture, Fig-. 19. The circle o-M-N-o passes through the centers of rotation, o and o, and through the direct point of fixation, shown at the intersection of the visual axes. The horizontal retinal meridians are represented by the two circles, E and E, each passing- through two vertical slots, S and S. They both lie in the plane of the larg-er circle, and each is supported in that position by the two clips, C and C. Each visual axis, V-A, extends from the macula at E, throug-h the cen- ter of rotation, o, on through the cornea, either at or near its center, thence across the circle to meet its fel- low-axis at the point on the circle through which passes the extended median plane of the head. The two visual axes, as well as the two horizontal retinal meridians, are lying in the plane of the circle. The clips, C and C, across the front vertical slot, S, prevent the visual axes from rising above or falling below this plane. Each vis- ual axis is made of two pieces of copper wire and a tube, so as to make both lengthening and shortening possible, as the point of view may be changed. The horizontal slot, M-N, allows the moving of the point of fixation of the visual axes both to the left and to the right. The slot, M-N, does not quite reach the- limit of lateral rota- 114 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES tions, but it extends far enough to show how the visual axes can be made to move in the plane of the circle, around the centers of rotation, o and o. The circle is supported by the upright, U, attached to the base, B. At the upper end of the upright there is a screw attach- ment for either fixing the circle in the horizontal posi- tion, or allowing it to rotate up or down, on the cord, o-o, the line that connects the centers of the two eyes. The screw is worked by the sliding rod, R-R. Imagining that the real circle in Fig. 19 has a diameter of twenty feet, then it will not be hard to conceive that the tonicity of the superior and inferior recti has planed the visual axes, and that tonicity of the lateral recti has converged them; nor will it be a difficult matter to understand that the tonicity of the superior and inferior obliques has planed the horizontal retinal meridians. The clips, C and C, front slot, S, represent the power that keeps the visual axes planed, and that power resides in the superior and inferior recti. The four clips, C, across the vertical slots through which the horizontal meridians pass, represent the power that planes these meridians, and this power resides in the superior and inferior ob- liques. When the tonicity of either of these pairs of muscles is unequal, the planing of the visual axes or the horizontal retinal meridians, as the case may be, can be maintained only by contractility of the weaker muscle of OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 115 a pair. Should the lateral recti muscles be unequal in tone, the visual axes would tend to cross, either within or beyond the circle, and only contractility of the weaker muscle of each pair would compel them to converge on the direct point of view. If tonicity keeps the horizontal retinal meridians and visual axes in the plane of the primary isogonal circle, when lying in the horizontal plane of the head, and con- verges these axes at the direct point of view on this cir- cle, the rotations will all be effected by the normal ex- penditure of neuricity. That the visual axes, when ro- tated from any one point of view to any other point of view, assume the exact positions of the two indirect vis- ual lines which connected the second point of view and its two images, before the rotation began, can easily be shown by a further study of Fig. 19, in connection with a glance at Fig. 20. Before rotating the visual axes from the primary point, shown in Fig. 19, to the sec- ondary point, TV, directly to the right, two wires should be made to extend from TV, directly over the right end of the horizontal slot, J/-TV, the one wire back to the right retina and the other back to the left retina, each passing over the center of rotation, o, of its eye. These wires should be firmly held in their respective positions while the visual axes are rotated from the direct point of view to TV, as shown in Fig. 20. At the end of the rotation it 116 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES will be found that the two visual axes lie directly under the two wires which were made to represent the two indi- rect visual lines. Fig. 20. If each of the two wires representing- indirect visual lines, going from TV, were carried over the nodal point OF OCUIvAR ROTATIONS. 117 of its eye, back to a point directly above the supposed location of the image on its retina, and these wires should be firmly held while the visual axes are rotated from the direct point to JV, it would be seen that the vis- ual axes do not lie directly under these wires thus placed. This would show that, while the two visual axes have reached the second point of view, the maculas have fallen short of the two supposed images of that point. No man can make these two experiments with the Muscle Indica- tor and not be convinced that all lines of direction are radii of retinal curvature prolonged. Fig. 20 shows that when the second point of view al- ready lies on the primary isogonal circle, the visual axes move from the primary point, in the plane of that circle, but the circle itself remains stationary. Fig. 21 shows that when the second point of view is in the vertical plane above the primary point of view, the visual axes are carried upward in the rotating plane of the primary isogonal circle, without change of posi- tion in that plane, to the second point of view. The pri- mary circle has been made to take the position of that secondary circle on which rested the second point before the rotation began, and the visual axes have been made to assume the positions of the two indirect visual lines which connected the second point and its two images be- fore the rotation started. 118 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES In both Figs. 20 and 21, the point of convergence of the visual axes has moved along that binocular spacial meridian on which were lying both the first and second Fig. 21. points of view. In the two cardinal directions right and left, the visual axes must move in a motionless plane; in OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 119 the two cardinal directions up and down, the visual axes must remain motionless in the moving 1 plane throughout the rotation. The rotations shown in Fig's. 20 and 21 have been accomplished without either the hor- izontal retinal meridians or the two visual axes leaving the plane of the primary isogonal circle, and the visual axes are still converged at a point on the circle. The rotation shown in Fig 1 . 20 has been effected around the vertical axis of the eye, a fixed axis throughout the ro- tation. That shown in Fig. 21 has been accomplished around the transverse or horizontal axis of the eye, like- wise a fixed axis for that rotation. In both of these ro- tations each eye has obeyed the law of monocular rota- tion, and the two eyes together have obeyed the law of binocular rotation. Fig. 22 shows a rotation that has been effected from the primary point of view, as shown in Fig. 19, to a sec- ond point of view obliquely up-and-to-the-right. The primary isogonal circle has been so elevated as to take the place of that secondary circle on which rested the second point of view before the rotation began; and the visual axes have been made to assume the position of the two indirect visual lines which connected that second point with its two images while the eyes were yet fixed on the first point. To fix this obliquely placed second point of view, the visual axes must rise with the plane, 120 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES and must move in this plane to the right, as it ascends, so as to converge at the second point the moment the plane reaches it. In this motion with the plane and the Fig. 22. other movement in the plane, the point of convergence of the visual axes has moved along that binocular spacial OP OCULAR ROTATIONS. 121 meridian on which were lying- both the first and second points of view. Pig 1 . 22 shows that this oblique rotation has been ac- complished with the horizontal retinal meridians still tying- in the plane of the primar} T isog-onal circle; and it also shows that the visual axes have remained in that plane and are converged at the proper point on the circle. The plane of the primary isog-onal circle has been carried up- ward with the visual axes of the two eyes, which have been rotated, each around its transverse axis, and the visual axes have been moved in the slot toward JV by an accompanying- rotation of each eye around its ver- tical axis. Each of these axes, the vertical and the transverse, have been themselves in motion throughout the oblique rotation. The perfectness of this double ro- tation about the two moving- axes has been made possi- ble by the obliques preventing- any rotation of either e} T e around its visual axis. The clips supporting- the two horizontal retinal meridians show that these meridians have not been allowed to leave the moving- plane of the primary circle. Fig 1 . 22 is representative of all oblique rotations. In oblique, as in cardinal, rotations, as illustrated, each eye has obeyed the law of monocular rotation, and the two tog-ether have obeyed the law of binocular ro- tation. 122 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES If the oblique rotation, illustrated in Fig. 22, had been effected around fixed axes, at right-angles to their respective rotation planes, the horizontal retinal merid- Fig. 23. ians would have been made to leave the plane of the pri- mary isogonal circle, each tilting- down-and-to-the-right, OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 123 as shown in Fig. 23, introduced here to show ivhat docs not occur in any oblique rotation. Two clips are down. The Muscle Indicator proves the existence of corre- sponding- points, in that it would not be a possibility if there were no such points. It proves the correctness of the law of direction, as discovered by the author viz.: All lines of direction are radii of retinal curvature pro- longed; for if this were not the true law of direction, the Muscle Indicator could not have been made. It proves that the macula is the posterior pole of the eye; for if this were not true, the Muscle Indicator would be a mus- cle mystifier. Of a muscle mystifier this could not be said: " There is not a single phase of a single ocular muscle, or any combination of ocular muscles, normal, abnormal, or pathological, which the Muscle Indicator will not show." It is the embodiment of truth, con- densed and clarified, concerning all muscle problems. With its aid the mediocre mind may become master of muscle questions which were baffling to the brightest intellects of other days. It is the only device that teaches "the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth," concerning binocular rest and easy motions of orthophoric eyes, and concerning binocular unrest and uneasy rotations of heterophoric eyes. In an early part of this chapter, Figs. 3 and 4 were introduced to make easier the study of monocular motion. 124 THE; FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 125 These two figures are so familiar to all readers, having been seen in practically all books on the eye, it would seem superfluous to sa} T anything- more about them. In other books, however, Figs. 3 and 4 only represented the field of vision for the left and the right eyes respec- tively, as shown within the shadings above, below, and to the inner side. Heretofore the lines traversing the white space were not called spacial meridians, and the point of their crossing has not been known as the spacial pole. From these two figures the author has been able to construct Figs. 24 and 25. He did this by dividing both Figs. 3 and 4, along the vertical meridians, and then pasting the divided parts together as follows: He trans- posed the right half of the left and the left half of the right fields, and, bringing these together, he formed Fig. 24, for which there is no name better than binocular field of vision; then, without transposing, he brought to- gether the left half of the left field and the right half of the right field, thus creating Fig. 25, which he has named the binocular field of vieiv. Fig. 24, although standing separate from, is, in reality, a part of, Fig. 25. Fig. 24 represents the field of binoc- ular vision when both the head and the eyes are station- ary and in their primary positions. In this peculiarly shaped field every object is seen by the two eyes; and 126 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES every object within this field, further removed than the point of fixation, is seen singly by the two eyes, if that point is distant thirty or more feet. The measure- ments of this field in the four cardinal directions are: 55 up, 70 down, and 60 to the right and left. In the center of this field is the binocular spacial pole, and passing through this pole are the binocular spacial me- ridians. Encircling the pole are the parallels, located 10 apart. Since the field of binocular rotations is only a little smaller than the field of binocular single vision, as shown in Fig. 24, practically all the points in this field may become points of fixation, by rotating the eyes, while the head remains stationary. There cannot be a point in this space which will not lie on one of the spa- cial meridians, except the point of fixation the binocular spacial pole which lies on every meridian, at the point of their crossing. Two points in space are to be considered in every ro- tation the first point of view, which is the point of fixa- tion, and the second point of view, which is the point to be fixed. The changing from the one to the other is neither more nor less than the moving of the binocular spacial pole along that binocular spacial meridian on which lie both the first and second points. If the first point of view is the primary point, as in Fig. 24, the second point may be the point of crossing of parallel circle OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 127 40 and meridian 120-300, above (any other meridian and parallel might have been chosen). In this case the point of fixation the binocular spacial pole would move along" this meridian until it reaches the point which was on circle 40. In every rotation the parallel circles move with the pole, so that when the pole (point of fixation) reaches the second point of view, the point of crossing of parallel 40 and meridian 120-300, below, will have moved up to the point which was the primary point before the rotation began. All points that were on that meridian before the rotation began will be on it when the rotation has ended, but they will be differently related to both the binocular pole and the binocular parallels. In the binocular field of vision (Fig. 24), when the head and eyes are in their primary positions, every point will lie on three circles common, or belonging, to the two eyes (1) a spacial meridian which determines the re- lationship (cardinal or oblique) that the point bears to the vertical and horizontal planes of the head; (2) a spacial parallel which marks its distance in degrees from the line of intersection of the two planes of reference; (3) an isogonal circle, primary or secondary, of some group, which determines the angle under which it is seen by the two eyes. Any one spacial meridian and any one spacial parallel can cut each other at only two points, which 128 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES points are in opposite directions from the spacial pole and equally distant from it. The easy coexistence of the binocular spacial pole, meri- dians, and parallels depends on normal conditions of the ocular muscles. There may be a binocular spacial pole and parallels, but no binocular meridians; but such a state can be established only by unconnected oblique astigmatism, through abnormal work on the part of the obliques. Normal recti and oblique muscles easily create and maintain the binocular spacial pole, meridians and parallels, as shown in Fig-. 24. Abnormal recti and oblique muscles make it either difficult or impossible to have the binocular spacial pole, meridians, and parallels. The treatment, surgical and otherwise, of abnormal muscle conditions has for its aim the easy creation and maintenance of the binocular spacial pole, meridians, and parallels. Fig. 25 is a combination of both the field of binocular vision and the field of binocular view. Objects located anywhere within the white area of Fig. 25 will be seen with the two eyes together if located within the space corresponding in shape and size with Fig. 24, but with one or the other eye only if removed further from the spacial pole. Objects beyond parallel 60 to the left will be seen with the left eye only, and objects beyond OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 129 the same parallel to the right will be seen by the right eye only. The spacial pole of an eye must be on the same straight line with the anterior and posterior poles of the eye. The spacial pole of an eye can be the direct point of view, for that eye, only because the center of the macula is the posterior pole, for the center of the macula and the direct point of view must be connected by a straight line. If the spacial pole for each eye is the direct point of view for that eye, as in Figs. 3 and 4, the fixing of the two eyes on one point brings the two poles into one, as in Figs. 24 and 25. The perfect fusion of the two poles brings into practical fusion the spacial meridians and parallels of the two eyes, also well shown in Figs 24 and 25. The monocular spacial parallel is everywhere equally distant from the visual axis of the eye to which it be- longs; therefore, strictly speaking, no two circles equal- ly distant from the two poles could be perfectly fused, for their centers could not be made the same point. In the formation of the binocular pole, the two vertical spacial meridians would be perfectly fused, but the two horizontal spacial meridians would cross each other at the binocular pole, and they would then diverge, but so slightly that, at 90, they would be just that distance apart corresponding with the measurement between the 130 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES two eyes. At the extreme limit of lateral binocular ro- tation they would be much closer together, probably 1 inches or less. For practical infinity, so slight a sepa- ration would amount to nothing. When the binocular spacial pole is at practical infinity, a line going from this pole to a point halfway between the two eyes may be considered as the binocular vis- ual axis. This conception would give perfect binoc- ular spacial meridians and parallels. Fig. 26 has been constructed on this conception, and the mathematics of the figure proves the value of the conception. In the study of the Isogonal Circles in Fig. 18, it has been stated that, for every point on the line of intersec- tion of the vertical and horizontal fixed planes of the head, there is a new group of isogonal circles, all the members of any one of the infinite number of groups having the same diameter. The point of direct fixation not only creates an independent group of isogonal circles, but it also creates an independent group of binocular spacial meridians and an associated group of binocular parallels. In Fig. 26, the primary isogonal circle is constructed through B and D, the centers of the two eyes, and the point of fixation, C. The horizontal binocular spacial meridian, /-C-/, for that point of fixation, has been cre- ated by M-C, the binocular axis, as a radius. The ver- OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 131 132 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES tical and oblique meridians of this group would be con- structed with the same radius. Binocular spacial meridians, constituting- any one group, are meridians with the same radius. The binocular parallels created by the point of fixation, C, are A'-E' ', R"-S" , V"'- W" . If G had been chosen as the point of fixation, the pri- mary isogonal circle would have passed through it and the two centers of rotation, B and D. With C' as the point of fixation, the binocular axis, M-C' , as a radius, would generate the horizontal binocular spacial meridian, 2-C'-2, for that point, and all the other members of that group of spacial meridians would have the same radius. The parallels for this new group of meridians and par- allels would be A-E, R'-S', and V"-W". If C" had been chosen as the point of fixation, then the primary isogonal circle for that point would have passed through C" and the two centers of rotation, B and D. M-C" would have been the binocular axis and the radius of curvature for all the binocular spacial meridians for that point. The circle representing the horizontal binocular spacial meridian, with M-C" as the radius, would be j-C"-j. The associated parallels would be X-S and V- W. If C'" had been chosen as the point of fixation, the primary isogonal circle for that point would have passed through C"' and the two centers of rotation B and D, and OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 133 M-C'" would have become the binocular axis. With M- C'" as a radius, the horizontal binocular spacial meridian would be 4-C'"-4, and all the other meridians belonging to the group created by the point C'" would have M-C'" as the radius. The parallel for this group of meridians and parallels would be V- W. The equator of any one of these four groups of meridians and parallels would have the same radius of curvature as the meridians. The diameter of the equator of group C is X-Z, that of group C v&X'-Z' , that of group C" is X"- Z" , and that of group C" is X'"-Z'" . The diameter of each equator is the same as that of the meridians be- longing to the same group. The diameters of the par- allels belonging to group C are A-E ', R"-S" , and V'"- W"; the diameters of the parallels belonging to group C' a,reA-J, J?'-S', and V"-W"\ those for group C" are R- S and V'-W'\ and the one for group C" is V-W. An interesting feature of Fig. 26 is that the two in- direct visual lines drawn from A and E respectively, that from A through D, and that from^ 1 through B, intersect each other on the line M-C. Lines similarly drawn from R and ,5* and from Fand W, respectively, would inter- sect each other on the line M-C. Mathematically, A and E are equally distant from C ', R and S are equally dis- tant from C", and Fand PFare equally distant from C'". 134 THE FUNDAMENTAL, PRINCIPLES The very definition of a parallel makes apparent the correctness of the above statement. A binocular spacial parallel cuts the horizontal spacial meridian (and all other meridians) at only two points, and these points are equally distant from the binocular spacial pole. This is true of every parallel of every single group of parallels and meridians, as group C, C", C", and C'". It is through a succession of such two points on diminishing horizontal spacial meridians that the primary isogonal circle passes, as A and , R and S, and Fand W. Three other primary isogonal circles could be con- structed in Fig. 26, all of which must pass through the centers of rotation, B and D. One of these, in going from B, would pass through V , R ', A', thence through a new point of fixation on M-C extended, and thence on through ', 6", and W to D, thence to the beginning at B. Another isogonal circle may start from B and go through V" and R" on to a still more distant point of di- rect fixation on M-C extended, thence around to S", through W" and D, to the point of beginning at B. And again, a still larger isogonal circle may be started at B, carried thence through V"\ to a point of direct fixation, still further removed, on the line M-C extended, around to W" t thence through D to B, the starting point. OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 135 Thus it is shown by Fig 1 . 26 that any two points on any horizontal binocular spacial meridian cut by a parallel circle may become points of binocular single vision, for these two points either lie on a constructed pri- mary isogonal circle or in the line of a possible primary isogonal circle. There is no point in viewable space which is not at the crossing of a spacial meridian and a spacial parallel be- longing to some group; and such points as lie in the ro- tation field (only slightly smaller than that represented in Pig. 24) are also on either the primary or some secondary isogonal circle of some group the three groups of circles as related to any one point of direct view constitute one common triple group. When the binocular spacial pole moves from point to point on the primary isogonal circle the first point of view, whether direct or indirect, is always on the primary isogonal circle it moves along the intervening arc of that circle, as from C to A, without change of angle of convergence; or if it moves along the cord of that arc, the angle of convergence increases as it moves from C to the middle of the cord, and thence on decreases until the pole arrives at A . The angle at the end of the ro- tation is the same as at the beginning. In this rotation the pole has moved from a point on the horizontal spacial meridian, /-C-/, of one group, to a point on the horizon- 136 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES tal spacial meridian, 2-C-2 of another group, but the visual axes have not left the plane common to these two meridians and the retinal meridian on which were lying the two images; and the macula, in passing from one image to the other, has not deviated from that retinal meridian on which were lying the two images before the rotation began, nor does it ever deviate from such a meridian. If the second point of view is A', it will be seen that the two points, C and A\ lie on the same horizontal spacial meridian, i-C-i. This rotation cannot be effected without a change of angle of convergence which will grow smaller, for the second point is on a larger isogo- nal circle. The spacial pole, in moving from C to A', may go along the arc C-A' or along the cord of this arc, but in either case the angle of convergence has continually changed. The maculas have moved from the images of C to the images of A ', along the horizontal retinal meridian. Thus might be studied rotations from any one point of view to any other point of view, cardinal or oblique. Some one primary isogonal circle passes through the two points of intersection of all successively diminishing horizontal binocular spacial meridians and any one of the parallels belonging to the same group. Every secondary isogonal circle cuts every parallel at two points similarly related to the spacial pole and the vertical and horizontal meridians; but at these points it OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 137 cuts two different meridians which bear the same relation- ship to the vertical and horizontal meridians. This is made evident by the fact that the spacial part of every secondary isogonal circle is wholly either above or be- low the plane of the horizontal spacial meridian, while one-half of every oblique meridian, and the vertical meri- dian as well, is above, and the other half is below, the plane of the horizontal meridian. Since the two points of intersection of a parallel and any oblique, or the vertical, meridian are on opposite sides of the plane of the horizontal meridian, it would not be possible for any secondary isogonal circle to pass through both points. The primary isogonal circle passes through a point common to all meridians of any one group, which point is the binocular spacial pole, and intersects the horizontal meridian of all diminishing groups at two points. The spacial part of no secondary isogonal circle cuts any horizontal spacial meridian at any point, but every secondary circle cuts all other meridians, each at only a single point. If the maculas were not the posterior poles of the eyes; if the centers of the retinal concaves were not the centers of rotation; and if all lines of direction did not cross each other at the centers of rotation, then Fig. 26, with all of its mathematical beauties, could have no exist- ence; nor would Figs. 24 and 25 have any true foundation. 138 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES If the macula is not the posterior pole, then the mo- nocular spacial pole cannot be on the visual axis, except in ideal eyes, but would be to its inner side about 5. SO ffff' 210 Fig. 27. The convergence of the two visual axes would leave the two spacial poles 10 apart, as shown in Fig-. 27. A binocular spacial pole would be impossible, and without OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 139 a binocular pole there could be no binocular meridians and parallels. As shown in Fig-. 27, the two vertical meridians would be made parallel with each other, but 10 apart, while the two horizontal meridians seem to be fused into one. If such a condition as depicted in Fig-. 27 were true, the point of convergence of the visual axes, in rotating 1 , could not move along 1 a single meridian except the horizontal. The confusion in Fig. 27, when con- trasted with the clearness of Fig. 25, must conve} r a forceful lesson to the mind of the searcher after the truth. ANGLE OF CONVERGENCE. In all binocular rotations the binocular spacial pole is made to move along the plane of the binocular spacial meridian in which lie the first and second points of view. The spacial meridians all accompany the motion of the spacial pole; but that meridian whose plane is the rota- tion plane, and it alone, has a wheel-like motion. All parallels also move with the rotating pole, but they have no wheel-like motion. As the spacial pole rises or falls, the planes of all the parallels look up or down; as the pole moves to the right or left, or obliquely up or down, the planes of the parallels face in a corresponding di- rection, but in no case does a parallel move in its plane. Several interesting facts have already appeared in connection with the study of the isogonal circle. 140 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES Another important feature growing- out of the study of the isogonal circle is the easy determination of the angle of convergence, whatever may be the distance of the point of fixation. The mathematical formula for solving this problem is : As the circumference of the isogonal circle is to 360, so is the arc extending- from the center of one eye to the center of the other, divided by 2, to the angle of convergence. The first member of this proportion is found by multi- plying the length of the diameter of the circle, which is the distance of the point of fixation, by 3.1416. The third member of the proportion depends on the size of the circle and the distance between the centers of the two eyes. If the circle is large, the arc from the center of one eye to the center of the other is practically the same length as its chord, which is the straight line from the center of the one eye to the center of the other. If the diameter of the circle is in feet, then the arc must be expressed in a frac- tion of a foot, but if the one is expressed in inches, the other must be also. To illustrate : Let the diameter of the circle be 16 inches, and let the arc subtending the angle of the visual axes be 2\ inches, then the following is the formula expressed in figures : 50.26 :360::2i-2 : X. From this it will be found that X=8.9. If the first and third members of the proportion were feet and a fraction OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 141 of a foot, and if the arc subtending the angle formed by the visual axes was always 2^ inches, or i of a foot, the work of determining' the angle of convergence could be simplified as follows : Divide 36 (the result of multi- plying 360 by | -^- 2) by the product of the distance of the point of fixation (the diameter of the isogonal cir- cle) and 3.1416. To illustrate: Fixation is at 1 foot. Multiply 1 by 3. 1416 and the product is 3.1416; with this number divide 36 and the quotient will be 11.35. For any given length of the diameter of the isogonal circle, the visual axes will form a greater angle if the eyes are wide apart than if they are close together. For compar- ison, let the diameter be 16 inches and the arc 2\ inches; then the angle of convergence will be, as already shown, 8.9; but let the arc be 2 inches, then the angle of conver- gence will be 7.16, a difference of 1.74. The following table is interesting and helpful, and is approximately cor- rect. The point of fixation being 16 inches, the upper row of figures represents the pupillary distance and the lower row the angle of convergence of the visual axes for each: 2 2K 2% 2^ m 23/ 4 2% 3 7.16 8.06 8.5 8.95 9.4 9.85 10.3 10.74 As is well known, the metre-angle of Nagel is not formed by the intersection of the two visual axes, but it 142 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES is the angle formed by the intersection of one visual axis with the extended median plane of the head, the head in the primary position, the point of fixation being 1 at a dis- tance of one meter. Under these conditions, the angle formed by the visual axis of the other eye and the ex- tended median plane of the head, is also a metre-angle; the one exactly equal to the other. The sum of the two angles constitutes the angle of convergence, so that the angle of convergence is two metre-angles of Nagel. Both the metre-angle and the angle of convergence vary with variations of the distance between the centres of the two eyes. The angle of convergence is a thing to be meas- ured, but not more certainly than that the metre-angle is also a thing to be measured. The metre-angle, therefore, cannot be taken as a standard of measurement, because a standard must never vary. A yard must be 36 inches, whether one is buying or selling, and whether the thing bought or sold is cloth or tape. The very word standard means unvarying. The unvarying standard for measur- ing angles is the arc of a circle in degrees, minutes, and seconds. This standard, for reasons to be shown, should apply to the angle of convergence. If Nagel had taught that the metre-angle is the angle formed by the intersec- tion of the visual axes at a distance of one metre, and had given to it twice the value in degrees that he did give it, there would be less objection to it. Even then, the me- OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 143 tre-ang-le would mean 3 20' with the distance from center to center of the two eyes 58mm; while it would mean 3 40' with the distance from center to center of the two eyes 64mm. After having- determined the value of the metre-angle (the angle formed by the intersection of the visual axes of the eyes at the distance of one metre, the head invariably in the primary position) in any given case it would be very easy to translate any fraction of a metre-angle or any number of metre-angles into degrees. Distances less than a metre, therefore a fraction of a metre, would increase the metre-angle in inverse ratio; for distances greater than a metre, the metre-angle would decrease in inverse ratio. To illustrate: Fixation at i a metre would give convergence of two metre-angles; g metre would give a convergence of 8 metre-angles; but fixation at 2 metres would give convergence of \ metre- angle; fixation at 8 metres would give convergence of | metre -angle. Let the value of the metre -angle be 3 20', then the above would be translated: 2 ma = 6 40', 8 ma = 26 40', \ ma = 1 40', | ma == 25'. The value of the metre-angle (the angle of convergence) for various distances between the eyes is given in the accompanying table : Distance be- tween the eyes in inches. 2 2!/ 8 2^ 2% 2/2 2% 2* 2-j 3 Value of one metre-angle. 254'38" 35'33" 316"28" 327'23" 338'18" 349'13" 4(X8" 411'3" 421'58" 144 THE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES An interesting 1 fact developed in working- out the size of the metre-angle, is that for every g of an inch in- crease of the distance between the eyes there is an in- crease of the angle to the extent of 10' 55". Knowing* the size of the metre-angle when the base-line (distance be- tween the centers of the eyes) is 2 inches, the size of the angle with the base-line 2| inches is found by adding to the former 54' 35", which is 5 times 10' 55". This would give 2 54' 38" + 54' 35"= 3 49' 13", just the size of the angle shown in the table, when the base-line is 2f inches. To find the size of the angle of convergence in any given case, when the point of fixation is less than one metre dis- tant, divide the size of the metre-angle in degrees by that part of a metre that measures the distance of the point of fixation, which, of course, means that }~ou are to invert the terms of the divisor and multiply. To illustrate: Fixation at 16 inches is fixation at (1 4-, metre. Let the i.4b base-line be 2i inches and we have the following : 3 38' 18" -H tt --= 3 38' 18" X = 8 57' 1". Again, let the point of fixation be 3 m, and the base-line be 2J inches. We now have 3 38' 18" -^-3 = 3 38' 18'' X | = 1 12' 46", the size of the angle of convergence at 3 m. The base- line remaining the same, the angle of convergence at a distance less or greater than one metre, is to the angle of OF OCULAR ROTATIONS. 145 convergence at one metre (the metre-angle), inversely, as the distance of the point of fixation is to one metre. The mathematical formula is that the tangent of half the angle of convergence varies inversely as the dis- tance of the point of fixation from the middle of the line joining the centers of the eyes. But for small angles the above rule gives approximately the same results. The reason for suggesting that the metre-angle be the angle formed by the intersection of the visual axes at one metre, and not the angle formed by the visual axis and the extended median plane, and that it be given a value double that given it by Nagel, is that the angle of con- vergence, or rather the nervous impulse from the third conjugate center, necessary to make this angle, is the chief factor in the formation of judgment as to distance. In fixing points to the right and left on the isogonal curve the angle formed by the intersection of the visual axis and the median plane of the head is confined to one eye, and is constantly changing in value, whereas the metre- angle, which is synonymous with the angle of convergence of the visual axes at one metre, remains the same every- where when carried along the isogonal curve. CHAPTER II. ORTHOPHORIA. THE terminology introduced by Stevens for indicating the relationship, normal and abnormal, between the oc- ular muscles, being 1 of pure derivation, leaves no room for change and but little room for addition. These terms are so widely used and are now so well known, they need mentioning 1 only when used in connection with the condi- tions indicated by them. The Stevens nomenclature was adopted as both scientific and correct, by the section of Ophthalmology of the American Medical Association, at the Washington meeting in 1891. Orthophoria is the term applied to a perfect balance of the ocular muscles when the head is in the primary position and the eyes are looking straight forward. This condition, in the strictest sense, includes the idea that the twelve extrinsic muscles have all been perfectly devel- oped, that each has its correct origin, pursues its proper course through the orbit to the eye, and is rightly at- tached to the globe; and that the orbits themselves are perfectly formed. It also includes the idea that the nine conjugate innervations are wanting in nothing. When (146) ORTHOPHORIA. 147 such a state of thing's exists, the visual axes are easily kept in the same plane through the first and second con- jugate innervations; are always perfectly converged through the third conjugate innervation; and by means of the first, second, fourth, and fifth conjugate innerva- tions, are made to sweep harmoniously along the hori- zontal plane, in the vertical plane, and in any oblique di- rection. Nor will the sixth, seventh, eighth, and ninth conjugate innervations fail to keep the vertical axes of the eyes parallel with each other and with the median plane of the head, while the transverse axes lie in the plane of the primary isogonal circle, regardless of the lo- cation of the point of fixation. Such a condition would also include the idea that the verting and ducting power of all of these muscles is up to the standard. But for these eyes, thus well balanced, to be perfect, there must be no error of refraction. There are such eyes, and the happy possessor of them knows of their existence only for the joy they give him. The workings of such eyes never add anything to the sum of human ills. There are accurate instruments for determining the ex- istence of orthophoria. The Stevens phorometer, the only one in use for a few years, is represented in Figs. 28 and 29. This instrument being incapable of making all the tests, even for orthophoria, it was natural that others should be invented, and that the evolution would go on to final 148 ORTHOPHORIA. perfection. The Stevens instrument is capable only of showing- the state of balance between the different pairs of muscles, and in that much can determine the existence of orthophoria but not the kind, for, after all that has been said, there are two kinds of orthophoria. It cannot acquaint the operator with the duction power of a sin- gle muscle. It cannot, nor can any other phorometer, already known or hereafter to be invented, give informa- tion about the verting power. One serious objection to the Stevens phorometer, which applies with equal force to the Wilson phorometer next to be considered, is that it is a binocular instrument, and to that extent must be faulty. In all phorometers, either diplopia must be pro- duced by prismatic action, the images in the two eyes being on non-corresponding points of the retinas, or the image in one eye must be made very different from the image in the other, as by the Maddox rod, so as to de- prive the guiding sensation of the reins of control. In the Stevens phorometer both images are displaced so that neither eye can be in the primary position while under test. Again, if the patient be not orthophoric, rotation of the instrument to properly relate the two images, moves the image in each eye, which must be a source of inaccuracy. The method of using the Stevens phorometer is simple. A candle or gas jet is placed twenty feet from the pa- ORTHOPHORIA. tient, who, in the sitting- posture, should hold his head erect. Placing- the instrument before him, he looks at the Jig-fat throug-h the prisms, when diplopia must become Fig. 28. manifest. In testing- for lateral orthophoria the lights are made to appear the one above the other, by rotating the instrument so that the base of one prism may be di- 150 ORTHOPHORIA. rectly up and the base of the other down. In orthopho- ria a vertical imaginary line will connect the two lights. If the one is not directly above the other, there is not lateral orthophoria. The slightest movement of these prisms will change the relationship of the images, and in this way whatever error may exist is measured. Fig. 29. In the test for vertical orthophoria, the instrument is so rotated as to place the prisms with bases directly in. The two lights should now be the same height, but if not, then there is not vertical orthophoria. The rota- tion necessary to make the lights level shows the kind and quantity of the error. This instrument can also determine the existence of oblique orthophoria. It must be rotated into the posi- ORTHOPHORIA. 151 tion for testing for lateral orthophoria; but, for the light, a horizontal line must be substituted. The line will be doubled by the prisms and they should be par- allel with each other. The test of the lateral muscles and of the obliques should be resorted to in the near also. For the former, Fig. 30- a card with a dot or cross in its center must be held at the reading 1 distance; for the latter, a card with a hori- zontal line on it should be held in the same manner. The dots and the lines should bear the same relationship as in the distant test. These tests made, the Stevens phorom- eter can do no more. 152 ORTHOPHORIA. The Wilson phorometer, Fig-. 30, can do all that the Stevens phorometer is capable of doing; and, besides, can determine the duction power of all the recti. The diplo- pia with this instrument is produced by a prism of 10 (found in one of the opening's in the revolving 1 disc) be- fore the right eye, whose base can be placed up for a test of the lateral muscles, or in, for a test of the vertically- acting- muscles. In lateral orthophoria the two images will be in a vertical line, when the slightest turning of the rotary prism constituting that part of the instrument which is always before the left eye, will displace the up- per (true) image either to the right or left. Should the lower image not be in a vertical line with the upper, mov- ing the rotary prism until the one is directly above the other will show both the kind and the quantity of the error. Changing the position of the disc, the 10 prism will present its base towards the nose. When the im- ages are found in the same horizontal plane there is ver- tical orthophoria, and the slightest movement of the ro- tary prism will elevate or depress the one seen by the left eye. Should the false image be higher or lower than the true, moving the rotary prism in the proper direction will bring the true image into the horizontal plane with the false, thus showing both the kind and quantity of the vertical error. Thus far the workings of these two in- struments practically correspond, the one being no bet- ORTHOPHORIA. 153 ter than the other. With the Stevens instrument both images are moved in every rotation; with the Wilson in- strument only the true image is made to change position. In connection with each of these phorometers there is the indispensable spirit level. With the open space of the disc before the right eye and the rotary prism in a neutral state before the left eye, the prism axes being horizontal, by moving the ro- tary prism, the duction power of the superior and infe- rior recti can be taken, which should be 3 certainly not less than 2 for each. Since this revolving prism can measure 10, it can always show, practically, the duction power of the vertically-acting muscles. Turning the ro- tary prism again into a neutral state, so the axis may be vertical, still keeping the open space before the other eye, abduction can be taken, which, in orthophoria, should be 8 certainly not less than 6. Rotating it in the other direction, adduction can be taken only up to 10, its max- imum power. To go higher than this, the disc before the right eye must be revolved until the 15 prism is brought into position, base out. Now, starting the ro- tary prism in the adduction arc, every movement adds to the 15 adduction caused by the prism before the right eye, until, when the end of the arc has been reached, 25 of adduction is shown. The doubling just now occur- ring, the conclusion is that adduction is normal; if soon- 154 ORTHOPHORIA. er, that it is below normal. It cannot be carried higher by this instrument. It will be noticed that the test has been applied not to one internus, but to both, and for this reason cannot be reliable. The Wilson phorometer is also capable of testing- the balance and imbalance of the oblique muscles. To do this, the open space in the disc is placed before the right eye, while the rotary prism is before the left in the neu- tral position, axis horizontal. By revolving- the prism as if testing for superduction, the point is finally reached when the horizontal line, which is now the test object, becomes double. If these two lines are parallel, then for that direction at least, there is orthophoria of the obliques. Returning to the neutral position, the prism should next be revolved in the direction of sub-duction. Presently the line is again doubled, the false line being below the true. If these lines are parallel, again there is evidence of orthophoria of the obliques. Not infre- quently the false line in the latter position will show an insufficiency of the superior obliques, while in the former position the lines may be parallel. By slowly revolving the prism back towards the neutral point, whether from the one direction or the other, the patient can observe whether or not the fusion of the two lines is simultane- ous throughout, or whether the fusion takes place at one end and then gradually throughout. When the lines are ORTHOPHORIA. not parallel, the kind of dipping- indicates the character of error. This instrument is wholly incapable of testing- the lifting- power of the obliques. Neither one of these instruments is constructed on the correct principle underlying the tests of the ocular mus- cles, even when orthophoria exists. The principle on which all the tests possible to a phorometer rest, is that the imag-e in one eye, throug-hout every test, shall be un- 156 ORTHOPHORIA. disturbed; that the head shall be erect; and that both eyes and the object better a white dot on a black back- ground shall be on the extended horizontal plane of the head. The false object must have its image thrown out- side the area of binocular fusion in the eye under test, while the true object will have its image on the macula of the eye not under test, thus making it not only possi- ble, but necessary, that this eye shall be in the primary position throughout the test, for it is not to have its im- age disturbed during any one of the several tests. An instrument based on the principle enunciated above is the monocular phorometer. It fulfills every essential condition, and is wholly reliable, and, except in rare cases, is invariable. The accompanying cut, Fig. 31, represents its appearance, but not its capabilities. The screw-and-spring arrangement, for regulating the spirit level, is as good as the best. In the base of the instru- ment there are slots on either side of the rotary prism, in one of which, towards the patient's face, is to be placed the displacing prism for causing the diplopia. If the instrument has been leveled, this prism, when placed in the slot, must have its axis either vertical or horizon- tal, and must produce a corresponding diplopia. The rotary prism differs from the one in the Wilson phorom- eter in that it has a face correctl}' lettered and marked in degrees, for each eye, and is easily reversible. ORTHOPHORIA. 157 With the instrument properly leveled before the right eye, the axis of the rotary prism vertical, and the 6 prism base up, in the slot toward the face, the false object is made to appear below the true, and if directly under it, there is lateral orthophoria. The rotary prism turned in either direction will make the false object go either to the right or to the left of the vertical line through the true object, which must be, at all times, the one looked at. Should the false object not be under the true, turn- ing the rotary prism in the proper direction will place it there. On the face of the instrument toward the op- erator, can be read the kind and quantity of the error. The test for lateral orthophoria, in the near, is made by holding a card with a dot or cross in its center, at the reading distance. To test the vertically-acting muscles, the rotary prism must be turned so as to have the revolv- ing screw vertical, and the axis horizontal. The 10 displacing prism, base in, must be placed in the slot to- wards the patient's face, so as to displace the image beyond the area of binocular fusion. The false object should be in the same horizontal plane with the true, if there is vertical orthophoria. Any movement of the rotary prism will displace the false object, either rais- ing or depressing it. When the false object is not found on a level with the true, there is a vertical hetero- phoria. Turning the rotary prism so as to bring the 158 ORTHOPHORIA. false object to a level with the true, shows, on the face towards the operator, the kind and quantity of the ver- tical error. With the instrument in the adjustment for detecting- vertical orthophoria, and without a displacing 1 prism, the balance of the obliques is found by moving- the rotary prism, first down, while the patient looks at a hori- zontal line until it doubles. The lines will be parallel if there is orthophoria of the obliques. Reversing- the movement of the rotary prism, the false line appears above the true, but should be parallel with it. If there is not a perfect balance between the obliques it will be shown by a want of parallelism between the false and true lines. The kind of error will be indicated by the direction in which the lines converge, but the quan- tity cannot be measured by this instrument. With the instrument still in the adjustment for the vertical muscles, sub-duction and superduction may be quickly determined, as by the Wilson phorometer. Ad- justing 1 it as for testing- the lateral muscles, abduction can be taken without the aid of a supernumerary prism, if the patient is orthophoric. To take the adduction, one or two supernumerary prisms will have to be used to aid the rotary prism. If adduction is not above 25, the 15 prism may be placed in the slot toward the face, with its base toward the temple. Turning- the ro- ORTHOPHORIA. 159 tary prism will add to the effect of the supernumerary prism up to 10. This added to 15 gives 25 of adduc- tion, provided the doubling- occurs only at the end of the rotation. If the adduction should be 30, or more, it can be shown by placing- the 5 or 10 prism, base out, in the slot in front, while the 15 prism remains behind, and again moving- the rotary prism throug-h the nasal quad- rant. This instrument can measure adduction only up to 35. In testing- for the adduction with the monocular phorometer, the only muscle to respond is the one inter- nal rectus. By reversing the instrument all of these tests can be repeated on the muscles of the left eye. In every one of these tests the imag-e in one eye remains undisturbed. The object seen by this eye must always be seen by di- rect vision, while the false object must be seen by indi- rect vision. The simpler methods of testing- for orthophoria of the recti, even including- the use of the Maddox rod, are "all faulty and should be discarded. The leveling- part of a phorometer is an absolute necessity, for without it there can be no exactness in the placing- of prisms be- fore an eye. In testing- for lateral orthophoria, slight errors, resulting from an improperly-placed prism, could be tolerated, but not so in testing for vertical orthopho- ria. The Maddox rod is objectionable in all tests of the 160 ORTHOPHORIA. recti for the reason that a part of the streak of light, whether it be vertical or horizontal, will fall on the field of binocular fusion, unless the error be great. The false image, whatever may be its character, should nev- er be on any part of this field; otherwise a greater or less effort at fusion will be made. There is a legitimate use for the Maddox rod. It is in testing the oblique muscles, both as to their ortho- phoria and their intrinsic strength. This instrument may be called the cyclo-phorometer, though Stevens has named the instrument he has invented for this pur- pose the clinoscope. The first of these instruments was invented by Price, in 1893, and was exhibited by him before the Section of Ophthalmology, at the meet- ing of the American Medical Association in San Fran- cisco, in 1894. It consisted of a double prism, line of bases horizontal and a rod at right-angles to this line of union, placed in a circular disc to fit the rim of a trial frame, and a Maddox rod only to be placed vertically in the other side of the frame. Looking at a candle, the patient would see two horizontal and necessarily parallel lines of light with the one eye, and a single horizontal line of light with the other, the latter appearing be- tween the other two, and parallel with them in ortho- phoria of the obliques. This was for testing the ob- liques when the visual axes were approximately paral- ORTHOPHORIA. 161 lei. It was faulty in that there was no adjustment by means of which the frames holding- the rods could be leveled. A little later, Baxter, of Boston, and Brewer, of Connecticut, each independently, invented a cyclo- phorometer, with the error in the Price instrument elim- inated. Brewer, not knowing of the Price invention when he made claim for himself, later wrote to the Ophthalmic Record as follows: "Dr. G. H. Price, of Nashville, Tenn., appears in your July [1898] issue as claimant to prior use of the Maddox rods in testing the position of the retinal meridians. Since he very clearly substantiates his claim, so far as I am concerned I tend him such laurels as I may have grasped, and trust he may wear them securely and gloriously." Dr. Brewer named his instrument the torsiometer. Later than this Stevens brought out his prism clinoscope, the construc- tion of which is not very different from the instruments of Baxter and Brewer. The cyclo-phorometer must, of necessity, be a binocu- lar instrument. The cyclo-phorometer, Fig. 32, made for use in connection with the monocular-phorometer stand, or the Wilson phorometer holder, consists of a base on which rest two graduated cells (E), in each of which is to be placed a triple Maddox rod (H) with the axis verti- cal. Behind each of these circular cells is a rectangular cell (F) for a displacing prism. There is an arrange- 162 ORTHOPHORIA. ment (D) by means of which the pupillary distance can be easily regulated so that the one streak of light may be brought directly under the other. There is beneath the base of the instrument, a spirit level (L) for regulating A - Fig. 32- the adjustment of the instrument. On each disc contain- ing the rods is marked below a line continuous with the axis of the central rod. The rods placed vertically, with a prism of 5 base up behind one of them, will show two ORTHOPHORIA. 163 horizontal lines of light, when a candle is looked at. The lower one will be seen by the eye before which is the combination rod-and-prism. The lines should be paral- lel, and their ends even. The latter can be regulated by turning- the screw (D) that controls the pupillary dis- tance. The slightest movement of either disc will cause a loss of parallelism of the streaks of light. If not par- allel, there is want of orthophoria of the obliques, the kind and quantity of the error being shown b} r the rota- tion of either disc. By removing the displacing prism, the intrinsic power the cyclo-duction of each oblique muscle can be taken alone, and then the combined cyclo-duction of either both superior or both inferior obliques. This is done, when only one muscle is being tested, by revolving the one rod in the temporal arc for a superior, and in the nasal arc for an inferior oblique. If both superior obliques are under the duction test, then both rods must be revolved in the temporal arc; if both inferior obliques, then both rods must be revolved in the nasal arc. The moment the two streaks separate, the rotations must stop. On the arc of the cell the extent of cyclo-duction can be read. The normal cyclo-duction for a single oblique muscle is somewhere between 7 and 14. The combined cyclo- duction of either pair of obliques is somewhere between 12 and 22. 164 ORTHOPHORIA. The method of determining the perfect balance of the oblique muscles, or the imbalance when it exists, by the Stevens clinoscope, will be better understood after a de- scription of the instrument itself. This description is given in the words of the inventor: Fig- 33- "The clinoscope [Fig. 33] is composed essentiall}" of two hollow tubes, each of which has at one end a minute pin-hole opening through which the eye can look, and at the other end a translucent disc on which is drawn a line, in the case of one tube from the centre straight up, and in that of the other tube straight down. " These tubes are so adjusted on a standard that they can be placed and maintained in the same horizontal plane, which is indicated by a spirit level, but from end to end the} 7 can be directed horizontally or up or down. ORTHOPHORIA. 165 They can, as above intimated, be made to converge or diverge to meet certain contingencies. "The tubes rotate on their long axes, and a pointer attached to each tube indicates on a scale the extent to which the tube is rotated. The small sight openings are so adjustable that the distance between them may be suited to the interpupillary distance of different persons. For the accommodation of those who, on account of pres- byopia, myopia, or any high degree of refractive error, cannot see at the distance of the test objects from the eyes, there are clips in which refracting glasses ma}" be placed. The sight openings being very small and ex- actly in the same horizontal plane, there can be no doubt as to the erect position of the median plane of the head when the two eyes are seeing, each through its appropri- ate sight opening, any existing hyperphoria being cor- rected. "The instrument is to be so adjusted in respect to height that the sight-holes will be on a level with the eyes of the examined person when sitting erect. This is best accomplished by the use of an adjustable table. The tubes may be exactly parallel or they may, in certain cases, be made to converge very slightly, thus making the distant point at 8 or 10 feet instead of infinite distance. Under other exceptional circum- stances they may be made to diverge. The tubes 166 ORTHOPHORIA. must be brought to an exact level with each other as shown by the spirit level. "Unless the subject of the examination is unable to see the test lines of the tubes, on account of presbyopia or high refractive error, no glasses should be used, and when glasses are necessary the weakest that will enable the person to see the lines clearly should be placed in the clips. A prism for the correction of hyperphoria may also be required. The glasses should not be worn, since, if a strong glass should not be held exactly at right- angles with the axis of the tube, the lens would itself induce a declination of the image. " The examiner must be sure that the examined person sees through both openings simultaneously, and that the view of both images is maintained throughout the exam- ination; otherwise there can be no certainty that the head is precisely erect. "When the examined person has secured a good view of both the test lines, he should endeavor, if they do not at once unite, to induce them to do so as in a stereoscope. Some people do not succeed in this, in which cases the examination may go on with the images separated, but it is less satisfactory. "When the apparent vertical position of the lines has been attained, the examiner should move them more or less backward and forward, in order that the true posi- ORTHOPHORIA. 167 tion may be more positively located. Few people can arrive at a satisfactory conclusion regarding- the position of the lines at the first trial, but after a da} 7 or two the tests become, for nearly all intelligent people, remark- ably uniform." With the clinoscope thus adjusted, the head of the pin with the point up should be fused with the head of the pin whose point is down, and both pins should be verti- cal if the oblique muscles are doing 1 their full duty if the vertical axes of the eyes are parallel with the median plane of the head. If the two pins are not now one ver- tical line there is a cyclophoria. Whether the C} T C!O- phoria is plus or minus is easily determined, and its quantity is measured by revolving the tubes till the two pins become one vertical line. In determining cyclo-duction by the clinoscope, the translucent discs with lines entirely across are to be used instead of those that have the lines only half way across. With the tubes properly adjusted the two lines would be seen as one. Revolving one tube would tend to displace the image of one line, which the eye would over- come by torsioning, as long as possible. The conclusion which Stevens has reached is that the image may be dis- placed as much as 14 in one eye before doubling occurs, and that the combined displacement, in opposite direc- tions, of the images in the two eyes, may be as much as 168 ORTHOPHORIA. 22. He also claims that a little greater displacement may be overcome by the inferior obliques than by the superior. No test for orthophoria is complete until the verting- power of the recti has been determined. In the study of the field of fixation, or, better, the field of rotations, it must not be confounded with the field of vision which, in healthy eyes, is much larg-er than the former. The rotations in the four cardinal directions are those to be studied; and the best means at our com- mand for this study is the tropometer, invented by Stev- ens. A fair degree of accuracy may be obtained by the use of the perimeter and a lighted candle, or a small electric lig"ht, in a dark room. This method, though not the better of the two, will be described first. The pa- tient should be placed in front of the perimeter as for the taking- of the field of vision. The eye to be tested must be in the center of the perimeter curve. The ex- tent of the outward rotation is determined by asking- the patient to fix the blaze of a small candle, or a small elec- tric light, as it is moved behind the arm of the perimeter, toward the temporal side of the eye under test. When the patient can turn the eye no further out, the operator putting- his open eye (one eye should be closed) in line with the candle and the center of the rotated cornea, ob- serves the imag-e of the candle reflected from the center ORTHOPHORIA. 169 of the cornea, and then reads the number of degrees marked at the point of location of the candle. In like manner the extent of rotation of the same eve in the op- posite direction is determined and noted. The arms of the perimeter are now to be placed in the vertical posi- tion, when the extent of the upward and downward ro- tations can be measured in the same way. There is no necessity for other than these measurements in the four cardinal directions. Muscles found capable of making these rotations reach the standard, will be fully capable of doing the work of effecting any other rotation, which, after all, must be a combination of the forces effecting the cardinal rotations. Both eyes should be thus tested. The Stevens tropometer, shown in the accompany- ing cut, Fig. 34, is an instrument of greater precision and is more convenient for use. The arrangement for fixing the head needs no description, since it is easil} 7 un- derstood. At the base of the instrument is a thumb- screw by means of which the tropometer proper can be placed at varying distances from the patient's eye. The object of this arrangement is to so adjust the instrument that the reflected image of the cornea will extend from one of the darker lines in the scale, to the other one, and this adjustment should be made at the beginning of every examination. Near the center of the upright piece there is a thumb-screw for elevating or depressing the 170 ORTHOPHORIA. Fig- 34- mirror so that its center may be on a level with the pa- tient's eye. At the top of this upright there is a flat base by means of which the mirror-box of the tropometer may be placed directly in front of the eye to be examined. This is effected by simply sliding the tropometer in either the one direction or the other. The horizontal part of the tropometer is a little more difficult to under- stand, and yet it is simplicity itself. It consists of a square box, closed completely by metal on all sides ex- cept the one facing the patient, and in the center of this ORTHOPHORIA. 17l side is an opening* which is filled with a disc of perfectly plane transparent glass, in the center of which is a white dot at which the patient is directed to look, at the beginning of the examination. Inside of this box is the mirror, placed at an angle of 45 on a vertical axis. From this mirror the patient's eye is reflected, an aerial image of which is formed on the graduated disc, so that the operator at the other end of the instrument may see it. The sharpness of the image is regulated by the thumb-screw in the center of the telescope part, by means of which the lenses contained in the tube are so adjusted as to enable the operator to get perfect sharp- ness of outline of the aerial image. The disc containing the graduated scale has been constructed with mathe- matical correctness. In the center of this disc there is a heavy line extending entirely across. At right-angles to this base-line there extends from each side a heavy line, the distance between the two being nearly 60. On either side of the base-line there are lighter lines placed at points 10 apart. When the handle of this disc is vertical, the position is for measuring superversion and sub-version. With this instrument adjusted so the pa- tient's cornea extends from one heavy line to the other, the base-line passing down through the center of the cornea, and the image itself being sharply focused, we take the superversion by asking the patient to look up as 172 ORTHOPHORIA. far as possible. In the reflected image the eye appears to move downward, for the image is irverted. The po- sition of the lower margin of the cornea (upper of image; is now noted and the extent of the rotation is read off on the scale. In a normal condition the superversion should be 33. This having been noted, the patient is asked to look straight forward again, when the image of the cor- nea will extend from one heavy line to the other as be- fore, while the base-line will pass directly down through the center of the pupil. Now the patient is asked to look down as far as possible. Unless the upper lid is held up by external force, it will so cover the cornea that the measurement cannot possibly be taken. An assistant is necessary then to elevate the upper lid in order that sub- version may be taken. While the patient is looking down as far as possible, the position of the upper margin of the cornea (lower as it appears in the image) is noted, and the extent of the rotation is read off on the scale. This should be about 50. The superversion and sub- version having been taken, the handle connected with the scale-disc is turned from the vertical to the horizontal. Now the instrument must be so adjusted that the base- line will coincide with the horizontal meridian of the cor- nea, while the cornea itself extends from one heavy line to the other. If the left eye be under test, abversion is taken by asking the patient to look as far towards the ORTHOPHORIA. 173 left as possible. The location of the nasal margin of the cornea, when the eye is in extreme abversion, is noted on the scale and the extent of the rotation is read off. This should be about 50. This done, the patient is asked to look straight forward, when the instrument is adjusted as before. Now he is asked to look as far towards the right as possible, when the extent of the adversion can easily be determined. This should be about 50. The power of rotation in the four cardinal directions having been found normal, it would be correct to conclude that rotation in any one of the oblique directions would also be normal. Any marked variations in the different ver- sions from the standard, as noted above, should be con- sidered a very important guide as to any surgical pro- cedure to be resorted to, but this will be more clearly set forth in the study of heterophoria. Both eyes should be thus tested. The candle method of simply watching the eye as it rotates in each of the four cardinal directions, does not commend itself as being at all accurate; and yet it is better than no examination to determine the extent of these rotations. Unless the temporal rotation carries the outer margin of the cornea to the external canthus, and the inner rotation carries the inner corneal margin to the internal canthus, it would appear that these rota- tions are too limited. In the upward and downward ro- 174 ORTHOPHORIA. tations there are only the lid margins, themselves mov- able, to give us an approximate judgment as to their extent. This method is of use in a case of paresis or paralysis, but it ought never to be relied on for other purposes. The extent of these rotations, as given by different authors, varies but little. L/andolt makes the standard of these rotations as follows: Out, 46; in, 44; down, 50; up, 33. Stevens places the standard as follows: Out, 48 to 53; in, 48 to 53; down, 50; up, 33. The standard set by Stevens is probably more nearly cor- rect. A knowledge of an excess of, or deficiency in, these measurements can but be helpful when the question of a muscle operation presents itself. The rotating power of a muscle should never be reduced by operation below the standard measurement for that muscle. The importance of the study of the field of rotation should not lead to a disregard of the field of binocular fusion. The latter can be determined only by the use of prisms. In this study again, it is not important to find the extent of the field except in the four cardinal direc- tions. Authors differ as to the extent of this, while none of them sufficiently emphasize its importance, in the study of heterophoria. The accompanying cut, Fig. 35, shows approximately the shape and size of this field of fusion. When an image is displaced by a prism to any ORTHOPHORIA. 175 point within the field, while the image in the other eye is on the macula, an effort at fusion will be made, and if the muscle that must respond is sufficiently strong", fusion will at once take place, caused by such a rotation as will bring- the macula under the displaced image. When the image is thrown, by a stronger prism, entirely outside of the field of fusion, the guiding sensation, RISHT UEFT Fig- 35- which seems to reside in this area only, will not call on any muscle to move the eye for the purpose of fusion. The nasal limit of this retinal area, as measured by a prism in front of the eye, is 8; the temporal limit, 25; the upper limit, 3; and the lower limit, 3. The line drawn through these four points marks the entire boundary of the field. ' This may be considered the nor- mal size of the fusion area. In some cases it mav 176 ORTHOPHORIA. appear to be smaller, while in still other cases it may be larger. It is by means of prisms which displace an image within this field that we can determine the fusion power of a muscle. This may be termed the intrinsic or lift- ing- power of the muscle. A determination of this power is important, even in the study of orthophoria, but of much more importance in the study of heterophoria. A knowledge of the fusion power, associated with a knowledge of the verting power of a muscle, is indis- pensable in the formation of a judgment as to what ought, or ought not, to be done in an operative way, in any given case of heterophoria. The power of the recti muscles for fusing images, ex- pressed in prism degrees, is for the internus (adduction), 25; for the externus (abduction), 8; for the superior rectus (superduction), 3, and for the inferior rectus (sub- duction), 3. A muscle that has the normal fusion power should also possess the normal verting power; and when the one is abnormal the other is likely to be abnormal also. The standard of the fusion power of the recti might safely be set a little lower than that given above, for all except that of the internus. A fusion abduction of 6 and a sub-duction and superduction of 2 or 2.3 may be considered as favorable. ' If all the muscles respond correctly to the diplopia ORTHOPHORIA. 177 test; if the duction power of the recti and the obliques reaches the standard; and if the verting- power of the recti does not fail short, then there is sthenic ortho- phoria. Such a patient needs no help for his ocular ad- justments. ASTHENIC ORTHOPHORIA. There is an orthophoria that is not in strength, but in weakness. The diplopia tests may elicit responses indi- cating orthophoria of all the pairs of muscles, but these muscles may show a want of duction power, also a want of verting power. Such eyes, though orthophoric, as judged by the diplopia tests, cannot be as strong as they would be if the muscles were possessed of full intrinsic power. If an externus perfectly balances its antagonis- tic internus and there is an abduction of only 4, there must be a correspondingly low adduction. If there is harmony between the superior and inferior recti, and they show a superduction and sub-duction of only 1, there is weakness that demands attention. Such cases are often met in actual practice. The treatment is ceil- ingf-to-floor and wall-to-wall exercise. The method of o carrying out this exercise is both simple and efficient. The patient is directed to stand against one wall of his room, midway between the walls to the right and left. Having previously fastened, by pin or tack, a piece of paper on each wall to right and left, at an angle of 35, 178 ORTHOPHORIA. approximately, and on a level with his eyes; and having placed some object on the floor immediately in front of him and at a distance equal to his height, he must stand with his head erect while he looks up at the ceiling where it joins the wall in front of him, then down at the object on the floor, and so on for six or eight movements in the vertical plane; then he must change his movements to the horizontal plane, looking first at the paper to the right, then at the paper to the left, and so on for six or eight movements in this plane. He then passes again to the vertical plane, changing the point of view rhythmic- ally every three seconds; and, at regular intervals, al- ternating the vertical and horizontal movements. He should stop the exercise always short of fatigue, and should not continue it longer than ten minutes at a time. Once a day is often enough to resort to the exercise. The time of day for the exercise may be suited to the convenience of the patient. The duction power should be taken at intervals of a few weeks, and the exercise should be continued until the recti show the normal lift- ing power. In this way an asthenic orthophoria may be converted into a sthenic orthophoria. The alternate contraction and relaxation of the recti, under the stimulus of the first, second, fourth, and fifth conjugate innervations, if not carried to excess, can re- sult only in the up-building of the muscles. Since every ORTHOPHORIA. 179 muscle is exercised in the same way and to the same ex- tent as its antagonist, there is no danger of interfering with the equal balance that existed between the muscles before the exercise was commenced. There are cases in which there is a sthenic lateral orthophoria and an asthenic vertical orthophoria. In such cases the ceiling-to-floor exercise alone should be advised. There are other cases in which there is a sthenic vertical orthophoria and an asthenic lateral orthophoria. In these cases only the wall-to-wall ex- ercise should be given. Since the strength of opposing muscles is correspond- ingly increased, there is never any danger of accomplish- ing too much. A lateral orthophoria with an abduction of 12 and an adduction of 36, is a better condition than when the abduction is 8 and the adduction is 25. A vertical orthophoria with sub-duction and superduction of 5 is better than if these ductions were only 3. The diagnosis between sthenic and asthenic ortho- phoria should always be made. CHAPTER III. HETEROPHORIA. Heterophoria is a generic term and includes all errors of tendency of all the extrinsic ocular muscles. It is the disposition on the part of a muscle, or muscles, to dis- obey the law governing it; that is, the supreme law of corresponding- retinal points. To obey this law, the recti, in the final result of their action, are concerned only with the visual axes; the superior and inferior recti of the two eyes keeping these axes in the same plane, the external and internal recti causing- them to intersect at the point of fixation. The obliques must keep the verti- cal axes of the eyes parallel with each other and with the median plane of the head. In orthophoria the de- mands of this law are easily met; in heterophoria the de- mands are met, but not with ease; there is strain or overwork. In heterophoria involving- the superior and inferior recti, there is a disposition on the part of these muscles not to keep the visual axes in the same plane, the visual axis of one eye tending- above the plane that ought to be common to the two axes, while the visual axis of the (180) HETEROPHORIA. 181 other eye has a corresponding- tendency downward. The direction of this tendency gives name to the error: up- ward tendency, hyperphoria; downward tendency, cata- phoria. In heterophoria involving the lateral recti, there is a tendency toward intersection of the visual axes be- tween the observer and the point observed, or they tend to intersect beyond the point of view, or even to become divergent. The tendency to cross too soon is esophoria; the tendency to cross too far away is exophoria. In heterophoria involving the obliques, there is either a tendency on the part of the inferior obliques to cause the vertical axes to diverge from each other above, or of the superior obliques to converge these above. The for- mer is properly termed -plus cyclop Jioria; the latter, minus cyclophoria. Heterophoria has its causes, and just as certainly has its consequences. To exist in any one of its several forms, one muscle must have an advantage over its an- tagonist; or, what is the same thing, in reverse order, one muscle must be at a disadvantage as compared with its antagonist. The difference may be in the compara- tive sizes of the two muscles, the one being larger, and, therefore, stronger than the other. There may be a difference in the insertions of the two muscles, the one being too near the corneo-scleral junction and the other too far back. These muscles may be of proper size, but 182 HETEROPHORIA. it is certain that the one with insertion too far forward will exert more power in rotating the globe than its an- tagonist not so favorably attached. It must also be conceded as possible that one muscle of proper size as compared with its antagonist, and with no more favor- able attachment, is more powerful than its antagonist because of an excess of nerve impulse sent to it. Wheth- er the one cause exists alone, or whether two or more of them combine in the production of heterophoria, the error is corrected by an extraordinary nerve impulse which is sent to the weaker muscle of a pair, and thus, with the undue expenditure of nerve force, binocular single vision is maintained. ORBITAL MALFORMATIONS. As has been claimed by Stevens, Risley, and others, malformation of the orbits may be a cause of heteropho- ria. Such malformation can be the direct cause of vertical and lateral errors, but not of errors of the obliques. As is shown in Chapter I., ideally-con- structed orbits are such that the eyes they contain, when in the primary positions, will have their horizon- tal planes lie in the fixed horizontal plane of the head. As already defined, this fixed horizontal plane must nec- essarily be at right-angles to the median plane of the head. It may be said always to pass through the optic HETEROPHORIA. 183 chiasm. Thence anteriorly it should pass through the centers of origin of the extern! and interni of both eyes; thence on through the centers of rotation of the two eyes. Posteriorly this plane passes, approximately, be- tween the cerebrum and cerebellum, just as the median plane of the head passes between the two halves of the cerebrum. In the chapter on hyperphoria and cataphoria will be found illustrations showing how an orbit that is too low will contain a cataphoric eye, while an orbit that is too high will contain a hyperphoric eye. If the two orbits are neither too high nor too low, but only too far apart or too close together, it is hard to see how any form of heterophoria could result; yet it is pos- sible that a lateral error might be thus caused: an ex- ophoria when the eyes are too far apart, an esophoria when they are too close together. However interesting may be the study of malforma- tion of the orbits as causative of heterophoria, the treat- ment, whether operative or otherwise, must be directed to the muscles; since it would be utterly impossible, by manipulation or operation, to convert a malformed orbit into an ideal one. It stands to reason that prisms in po- sitions of rest would be the ideal treatment of hetero- phorias dependent on orbital malformation, there being no muscle imbalance -per se. 184 HETEROPHORIA. To Stevens, of New York, is due much credit for his pioneer work in the study of the ocular muscles. Be- fore him, Graefe, in his study of insufficiency of the in- terni, gave us a glimpse of that light which Stevens af- terwards turned on more fully. By means of the pho- rometer which he invented, he found it much easier to investigate the recti muscles. He prosecuted this study for many years, almost alone, and under fire of most se- vere criticism. The information given us by Graefe about insufficiency of the recti muscles is so incomplete, as compared with the results of Stevens' labors, that the latter may well be looked upon as the discoverer of these conditions. The remarkable feature of Stevens' work is that he so long ignored any study of the oblique muscles, which have a duty to perform no less impor- tant and exacting than that required of the recti. It has already been shown that the obliques, under the in- fluence of four conjugate innervations, must keep the vertical axes of the eyes parallel with each other and with the median plane of the head. As far back as 1891,* it was shown that the obliques were not always capable of accomplishing their work with ease; and a little later the method of exercising these muscles so as to strengthen them, was introduced. t In 1893 it was * See Archives of Ophthalmology, Vol. XX., No. 1. t See Ophthalmic Record, Vol. II., No. 1. HETEROPHORIA. 185 shown that one danger attending an advancement of a rectus muscle was that its new attachment might be so displaced as to throw unbearable work on one or other of the obliques.* Again, in 1893, following- a suggestion by Swan M. Burnett that one or more of the recti might naturally be so attached that the obliques would be in- sufficient for the work demanded of them, an operation on a rectus for strengthening an oblique muscle was suggested. t It was not until 1895 or 1896 that Stevens commenced his study of the obliques. Soon thereafter he invented his clinoscope, the capabilities of which will be fully shown in the discussion of c} T clophoria. His work in this direction, as might have been expected, has been helpful; but in his paper published in the January (1899) number of the Archives of Ophthalmology, he claims entirely too much credit for himself, as expressed in these words: "Anomalous declinations not related to disabilities of the muscles had, previous to my own con- tributions, t obtained no recognition as a practical sub- ject, if, indeed, it had been recognized at all, although it is probably one of the most practical of the various im- portant phases of the adjustments of the eyes." In the matter of nomenclature Stevens gave us per- fect terms, but not a complete list. There should be a * See Ophthalmic Record, Vol. II., No. 9. f See New Truths in Ophthalmology, 1893, pp. 40. 41. t Not earlier than 1897. 186 HETEROPHORIA. name for every deviating tendency, but he gave none for the downward tendency. In conformity with his nomen- clature, the downward tendency of an eye has been named cataphoria.* Before Stevens began the study of the obliques, the name cyclophoria was given to insufficiency of the ob- liques, in conformity with the nomenclature applied to the recti. To distinguish insufficiency of the superior from insufficiency of the inferior obliques, the term plus cyclophoria has been applied to the former and minus cyclophoria to the latter. Maddox, for some reason, preferred the terms plus torsion and minus torsion. Still later Stevens gives to these conditions the names plus declination and minus declination. Unless there is a very special reason for doing otherwise, there should be uniformity in the nomenclature applied to the ocular muscles. As there appears no valid reason against this uniformity, the term cyclophoria alone will be used in the chapter on errors of the obliques. Terms should be multiplied only when there is abso- lute need for them. The terms anaphoria and katapho- ria added to nomenclature by Stevens a while ago, and applied, respectively, to an upward tendency of both eyes and the reverse, a downward tendency of both eyes, would tend only to confusion. These conditions exist, * See New Truths in Ophthalmology, 1893, page 68. HETEROPHORIA. 187 but it is far better to say double kyperphoria and double cataphoria. Maddox did good work when he offered as substitutes for sursum - duction and deorsum - duction the simpler and easier terms super-duction and sub-duction. But even these terms should be given only a single meaning. They should be made to apply only to the power the superior and inferior recti have for overcoming prisms in the interest of binocular single vision. Likewise ad- duction and abduction should be restricted in meaning so as to apply only to the interni and externi in their ef- forts to overcome the lateral displacing of images by prisms. The fact that it is better to have two terms, each with a single meaning, than to have one term with two very different applications, must be the author's apol- ogy for adopting the following nomenclature for the turning of the eyes in the four cardinal directions: Ab- version, turning the eye out; adversion, turning the eye in; superversion, turning the eye up; and sub-version, turning the eye down. These terms are shorter and bet- ter than outward rotation, inward rotation, upward rota- tion, and downward rotation. Duane deserves credit for his very careful study of the ocular muscles in his little brochure, ' ' Motor Anom- alies of the Eye; " but the terms hyperkinesis and hypo- 188 HETEROPHORIA kinesis, introduced by him, are not nearly so simple or easy as the terms sthcnic and asthenia esophoria, sthenic and asthenic exophoria, and sthenic and asthenic hyper- phoria, and so on for all the phorias. The meaning", however, is precisely the same. The heterophoria that is purely innervational should be designated by the prefix pseudo as pseudo-esophoria, a condition depending* on the relationship existing- be- tween accommodation and convergence, and not depend- ent on any error inherent in the interni. The following- is a list of the heterophorias: (1) Esophoria, of which there are two varieties, viz.: pseudo-esophoria and intrinsic esophoria. Of the intrin- sic variety there are two kinds, sthenic and asthenic. (2) Exophoria, pseudo and intrinsic. Of the intrinsic variety there are two kinds, sthenic and asthenic. (3) Hyperphoria and cataphoria, which are always in- trinsic when the superior and inferior recti are the only factors. These errors are either sthenic or asthenic. There is now and then to be found a double hyperphoria or a double cataphoria. (4) Cyclophoria, plus and minus, both pseudo and in- trinsic. The intrinsic variety may be either sthenic or asthenic. Two or three of these errors may be found combined in many cases; but it is probably better not to have HETEROPHORIA. 189 compound names for such combinations of errors, as it is important that the quantity of each error should be noted. It would be difficult to indicate the quantity of the two errors if the note was made hyper-esopohria or hyper-cyclophoria. TESTS. There are some interesting tests for heterophoria that can be made, in a rough way, independent of the pho- rometer. One is the cover test. If there is an error of any magnitude, it will manifest itself on covering one eye while the patient is looking with both eyes at a test object twenty feet distant. The covered eye will imme- diately place itself in a state of equilibrium. When the cover is removed, it will return to the position of har- mony with the fellow eye. This readjustment can be easily seen, in many cases, by the observer. The direc- tion in which the eye moves when uncovered indicates the kind, but not the quantity, of the heterophoria. A patient of keen observation will be made conscious of the disturbance. This is better done by covering and uncovering the eyes alternately. In high degrees of heterophoria a plane red glass placed before one eye will suspend, to some extent, the effort at fusion, and diplopia will result. A candle should now be the test object. The position of the red blaze, the patient the 190 HETEROPHORIA. while looking- at the real candle, will indicate the kind of error. Only the higher errors respond to the red- glass test. The test by means of the phorometer, preferably the monocular instrument, for the reasons given in the chapter on orthophoria, is the only one to be relied upon. By it the kind of error is quickly determined and the quantity is easily measured. In testing the interni and externi, the 6 prism is placed, base up, in the cell next to the eye. This will throw the false image above on the retina and entirely outside the field of binocular fusion, so that no attempt can be made at fusion. The false object will be below the true, and will bear that relationship to it determined by the exist- ing imbalance. The handle of the rotary prisms must be horizontal, the index at zero, and the instrument per- fectly level. The true object must be the one looked at. If the false object is toward the opposite side, there is exophoria. Turning the controlling screw, the index is moved toward the corresponding side until the patient says the false object is directly under the true. The number at which the index stands marks the quantity of exophoria for that eye. If the false object, instead of being on the opposite side, shows itself on the corresponding side, the existing error is esophoria. The controlling screw is now turned HETEROPHORIA. 191 so that the index moves toward the opposite side. The revolution is stopped when the patient says the false ob- ject is in a vertical line with the true one. The number at which the index stands shows the quantity of esopho- ria for that eye. The test having been made for the distance, it should now be repeated at the reading- point. The test object now should be a white card in the center of which is a black dot, and the card should be held directly in front of the eyes. There should be no line drawn vertically throug-h the dot, as advised by Graefe, for the reason that this line would cross the area of binocular fusion, and would thus lead to an attempt, on the part of the eye, to correct its error. The kind of error is deter- mined and its quantity is measured, as in the distant test. The result, not always the same as found in the distant test, should be noted. The lateral error having- been thus found and meas- ured, the 6 prism should be removed and the 10 prism, base toward the nose, should be placed in the cell; the handle of the rotary prism should be placed vertically, and the index must be made to stand at zero. It is now of vast importance that the instrument shall be perfect- ly level; otherwise, a vertical error may be shown when none exists. The instrument thus adjusted, any verti- cal imbalance may be detected and measured. As in the 192 HETEROPHORIA. test for lateral errors, the true object must be the one fixed. The false image will be thrown outside the area of binocular fusion, toward the nose, while the false ob- ject will appear on that side of the true, corresponding to the eye under test. If the false object is below the horizontal line passing" through the true, there is hyper- phoria of that eye, the quantity of which is determined by revolving the controlling- screw so that the index shall move upward. The number at which the index stands when the patient says the two objects are level shows the quantity of the error. If in the test the false object is above the horizontal line passing through the true, there is cataphoria of that eye. The screw is now turned so that the index shall move downward. The number at which the index stands, when the patient says the objects are level, shows the quantity of the cataphoria. The test for ver- tical imbalance need not be repeated at the near point. The one eye having been tested thus for imbalance of all the recti, the instrument should now be turned into position before the fellow eye, that the lateral and ver- tical imbalances it may have may be found and measured. Usually, if there is esophoria of one eye, the other will also show esophoria; the amount in the one is about equal to that in the other. Occasionally, however, there will be a difference of 1 or even more. The same may HETEROPHORIA. 193 be said of exophoria. When there is hyperphoria of one eye, the other is usually cataphoric, and the one error is about equal to the other. A double hyperphoria and a double cataphoria cannot so easily be shown, in the imbalance test, by the phorom- eter. The proof test, which is by means of the Mad- dox double prism, quickly shows either of these errors when they exist. The base-line of these prisms (4) should be held horizontally before first one eye and then the other, having- each eye look at the distant test ob- ject, first through the upper prism and then through the lower, observing- that position which throws the double objects closer tog-ether. If they are closer for both eyes when the false object is seen through the upper prism, there is double hyperphoria; but if the}" are closer for both eyes when the false object is seen through the lower prism, there is double cataphoria. It can be readily understood that, if there is hyper- phoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, the two objects will be closer together when the false one is seen, by the hyperphoric eye, through the upper prism, and by the cataphoric eye when seen through the lower prism. Hence the reason for calling this use of the double prism the proof test of hyperphoria and cata- phoria. The next step in the testing is to determine the ability 194 HETEROPHORIA. of the obliques to parallel the vertical axes of the eyes with the median plane of the head. This can be done rudely in any one of several ways : First, by means of the Maddox double prism, the object looked at being- a horizontal line on a blackboard, twenty feet distant, or a horizontal line on a card held at the reading- distance; preferably, both. The base-line of the double prism (4) should be horizontal and so held before one eye as to double the test-line. The two lines seen by this eye must be parallel. The third line, seen by the other eye, should be between the other two and parallel with them. A dipping- of the true line toward the opposite side would show a plus cyclophoria, while a dipping- toward the corresponding side would show a minus cyclophoria. The quantity of the error thus shown, however, cannot be measured. The same test may be made by means of a sing-le prism of 4, base up or down, before the eye; but even still more easily it may be made by the revolving- prism in position for taking- the sub-ducting and superducting power. The rotation, of course, must be carried beyond the possibility of fusion. The false line is seen by the eye before which the single prism is held or the rotary prism has been placed, while the true line is seen by the other eye. The refracting angle of the prism points in the direction of the false line. When this line is seen HETEROPHORIA. 195 below, by the right eye, and the ends of the two toward the right converge, there is plus cyclophoria; if they diverge toward the right, then there is minus cyclopho- ria. There is no method of measuring the error thus found. In revolving the rotary prism or in turning the single prism so as to make it possible for fusion to take place, it is interesting for the patient to watch the man- ner of fusing; if there is cyclophoria, the two lines will come together at one end first and then quickly fuse throughout. The Stevens clinoscope will detect and measure any existing cyclophoria; but the best instrument for detect- ing cyclophoria and measuring the amount of the error is the cyclo-phorometer. It is much cheaper than the clinoscope, and, better still, it is simpler in construction and much more easily manipulated. The 5 prism, base up, behind one rod gives the second streak of light; the thumbscrew makes it easy to place the one streak di- rectly under the other ends even. If the axes of the rods are at zero and the two streaks are not parallel, cyclophoria is positively shown; if the lower streak is seen by the left eye and the two streaks converge at the left, there is plus cyclophoria; but if they diverge at the left, there is minus cyclophoria. The extent of the turning of the rod on the one side or the other, neces- sary for paralleling the streaks, measures the quantity 196 HETEROPHORIA. of the cyclophoria. It is not necessary, with the cyclo- phorometer, to keep in mind the fact that the lower streak is seen by the eye that has the displacing- prism before it, for the position of the axis of the rod at the time the streaks are made parallel names the error as well as measures its quantity: when the axes must be moved into the nasal arc, there is plus cyclophoria; into the temporal arc, there is minus cyclophoria. The spirit level of the cyclo-phorometer enables one to determine if the cyclophoria is monocular or binocular. When the two streaks of light converge at one end or the other, if the error is binocular, neither of the lines will be horizontal. If one is horizontal while the other is oblique, the error is monocular; if both lines are in- clined in the same direction, it shows plus cyclophoria in one eye and minus cyclophoria in the other. Having- followed out the tests already described the tests for imbalance one knows the kind of error or er- rors in the individual case, but remains ignorant of the character of these errors. The duction and version tests alone can reveal the fact that an error is sthenic or as- thenic, just as the study of the refractive errors alone re- veals whether or not a given heterophoria is pseudo or intrinsic. The duction test is to determine the power the mus- cles have for overcoming the displacing of images by HETEROPHORIA. 197 means of prisms. To this meaning- the word ditction, with its several prefixes, should be restricted. The method of determining duction power by means of the monocular phorometer has already been set forth in Chapter II. It can be accomplished by means of the prisms in the refraction case, but not so quickly nor so accurately. Duction is wholly an involuntary act, and it is accomplished through the guiding sensation, in obedience to the law of corresponding- retinal points; but it has its limitations. Abduction is the power of an externus to fuse with the image on the macula of the fellow eye, an image that has been displaced to the nasal side of the macula of its own eye. Less than 6 of such power is subnormal; more than 8 is supernor- mal. Adduction is the power of an internus to move the macula outward until it shall stand under the image that has been displaced temporally, so that it may be fused with the image on the macula in the fellow eye. It is certainly subnormal if less than 18 to 25. The adduction stimulus is much greater than any other, and is much more variable. Its variableness makes it less reliable than any other duction; but, neverthe- less, it must be known in dealing intelligently with esophoria. Superduction is the power the superior rectus has for fusing an image displaced below the macula, with the 198 HETEROPHORIA. image on the macula in the fellow eye. Less than 2 is subnormal; more than 3 is supernormal. Sub-duction is the power the inferior rectus has for fusing- an image displaced above its macula, with the image on the macula in the fellow eye. Less than 2 is subnormal; more than 3 is supernormal. In all duction tests it is better that the image should be slowly moved away from the point occupied by the macula, when the eye is in the primary position, toward the boundary line of the field of binocular fusion, which should be considered as immovably fixed. So long as this image is within this field there is binocular single vision, for the macula moves with the moving image as far as possible; but the moment it passes the border line there results diplopia. The index of the rotary prism marks the duction power of the muscle concerned. It should be noted. The field of binocular fusion is larger if the muscles are stronger, smaller if the muscles are weaker. Its size can be changed both by exer- cise and by operations. If it is too small, it may be in- creased by exercise and by shortening and advancement operations; if too large, it can be reduced only by tenoto- mies, which should always be partial. Cycloduction, which is involuntary, can be taken only by the clinoscope or the cyclo-phorometer. With the for- mer instrument the line as seen by one eye is turned by HETEROPHORIA. 199 means of the proper screw up to the point of doubling-. The index marks the torsioning power of the oblique involved. With the cyclo-phorometer the axis of one rod is moved from zero toward the nose to test the torsioning power of the inferior oblique, and toward the temple for determining the power of the superior oblique. An oblique should have a fusing power of from 7 to 10. The inferior oblique has a little greater fusing power than the superior oblique. In determining the several ductions the tests should be monocular, except in cycloduction. In any given heterophoric condition the duction test, aided by the version test, determines whether the error is sthenic or asthenic, and, therefore, the kind of opera- tion, if any, that should be performed. No muscle whose duction power is normal or subnormal should be weak- ened by a partial tenotomy; but the imbalance should be cured either by a shortening or an advancement of its still weaker antagonist. No examination of the recti muscles is complete with- out the taking of the verting power of every one. As shown in Chapter II., this can be rudely done by simply watching the eyes while the patient looks as far as pos- sible in the four cardinal directions. The objection to this test is that there is no accuracy in it, and yet it is better than no test. The reason for introducing the 200 HETEROPHORIA. terms adversion, abversion, superversion, and sub-ver~ sion has been given already, and the extent of each, considered as normal, has been shown in the same con- nection. Of all instruments for making- the version tests, the Stevens tropometer stands first, because of simplicity, accuracy, and speed. The only part of the instrument that should be dispensed with is the head rest, and this for two reasons: First, it interferes with the manipulation of the upper lid, when the sub-version is being 1 taken, for now the lid must be held up or it will entirely obscure the cornea; second, it obscures too soon the small electric light or other test object which the patient should fix as the superversion is being- taken. The mouthpiece is necessary in order to insure that the patient's head shall not turn while the verting power of the eye is being taken. A fairly good substitute for the tropometer is the per- imeter, if properly used. There should be some means for preventing the movement of the head, and nothing could do this better than a mouthpiece, such as consti- tutes a part of the tropometer. The eye under test should be in the center of the perimeter curve. As with the tropometer, the rotations should be taken in the four cardinal directions only. With the arms of the perimeter in the horizontal plane, abversion and adver- sion can be easily taken; and if proper care is observed, HETEROPHORIA. 201 the result will be practically accurate. The small elec- tric light, shaded toward the observer, should be placed first directly in front of the eye while in the primary position. From this position, the light, while being kept in contact with the perimeter arm, should be moved in the temporal arc the arc for abversion slowly, the patient fixing the moving light, while the operator fixes its image reflected from the center of the cornea, moving his own head harmoniously with the moving light. The patient should speak when he finds himself no longer able to fix the light. At the same moment the observer can see that the image is no longer reflected from the center of the cornea. Thus the patient serves as a check to the operator, while the operator also serves as a check to the patient. When 5 beyond the point of fixation, the small light becomes so blurred that the patient can easily detect the change; hence, if the patient is closely observant, there is small room for error. The operator cannot so easily detect an error of 5, as shown by the reflected image. For these reasons the subjective part of the test is more reliable than the objective. The po- sition of the light on the arc, when the patient can no longer fix it, and when the reflected image begins to leave the center of the cornea, indicates the degree of abversion. With the light moving along the nasal arc, adversion is taken in like manner, and its extent should be noted. 202 HETEROPHORIA. With the arms of the perimeter rotated into the ver- tical plane, superversion and sub-version are taken by moving* the light along the upper and lower arcs, respectively, and their extent is noted. In taking- sub-version, the reflected image cannot be so easily watched, even when the upper lid is held out of the way. Here the subjective part of the test must be re- lied upon. One eye having been thus tested, in the four cardinal directions, the other eye should be properly placed and the various duction powers should be determined and noted. Unlike the duction power, which is involuntary, ver- sion power is a thing of volition. Neither one should be depended on to the exclusion of the other. The result of thess two tests (duction and version) should be com- pounded, ir the surgeon would be safely guided in his operative work, or even in the non-operative treatment of heterophoria. Cycloversion has no existence, since voluntary rota- tion around the visual axis is impossible. No muscle whose ducting or verting power is nor- mal or subnormal should be weakened by a partial tenotomy. No muscle should be increased in strength by an advancement or by a shortening when the duction and version are not subnormal. HETEROPHORIA. 203 SYMPTOMS OP HETEROPHORIA. That there are cases of heterophoria without symp- toms must be conceded, but such cases are not often seen by the ophthalmic surgeon. It is a symptom, or symptoms, of eye-strain that drives the patient to the doctor. It may be that the symptoms, in a given case, are dependent in part, if not wholly, on errors of refrac- tion; but it is a serious mistake to suppose, as some do, that eye-strain is always and only associated with the ciliary muscle. The ciliary muscle is only one of eight muscles connected with each eye; and each of the seven other muscles, when called on to do abnormal work, is just as capable of developing S3 r mptoms. People who have no symptoms, and yet have heterophoria, are pos- sessed of a stable nervous system and are physically strong. The physically weak and the nervously unstable must be sufferers from eye-strain, of whatever character. The nervous centers of the one may be compared with the steady leaves of the oak, which are shaken only by a wind; while the nervous centers of the other may be compared to the leaves of the aspen tree, which quiver in the slightest zephyr. Or, again, the easily-disturbed nerve centers may be compared to the leaves of the trail- ing little vine seen in the old turned-out field, all the leaves of which fold themselves up, if but one leaf be touched by a human finger. 204 HETEROPHORIA. The strong, healthy person, with a stable nervous system, may never have had a symptom resulting- from muscle or focal errors that have always existed. Let this individual have an attack of typhoid fever, measles, or other depressing disease, or let her pass through a pregnancy and confinement; now, on attempting too soon the use of her eyes in near work, she begins to be a suf- ferer. The suffering becomes a habit, and she gets no permanent relief until the focal or muscle error has been corrected. A sudden shock to a nervous system that has been strong, brings about a change that ever after makes the patient feel the effects of errors whose existence before made no impression. Growing children, especially those that are delicate, when too hard pressed in their school work, almost in- variably present some one of the many symptoms of strain. More women than men feel the effects of muscle and refractive errors, mainly because of the fact that the former are forced to spend a greater number of hours every day in near work, than the latter. Book- keepers, or men who are engaged in other continuous near work, are often forced to seek aids to vision. HEADACHE. The most common of all the symptoms caused by heterophoria is headache. The aching may be in the temple, brow, at the top of the head, over the HETEROPHORIA. 205 parietal region, or in the back of the head. In some cases the suffering is in the back of the neck. The pain may be on both sides of the head, but often it is unilat- eral. It is periodic in character, and usually comes on as the result of prolonged, hard near work. The headache which one has on awaking in the morning or, more prop- erly speaking, the headache which awakens the patient- is usually due to disturbances in the sinuses, or cells, that open into the nasal passages, brought about by mouth- breathing, the mouth-breathing depending, of course, on nasal stenosis. Rest in sleep usually relieves the head- ache of heterophoria and of refractive errors. Head- aches due to eye-strain, that come on unassociated with near work, are usually heterophoric. and not refractive. The headache that one has on bright days and when amid bright surroundings, as the white buildings and white walks of an exposition, is often due to overwork of a weak sphincter of the iris, which is compelled to keep the pupil small that the retina may be protected from the glare. The headache of eye-strain is usually of the nervous variety that is, unassociated with nausea and vomiting. However, genuine sick headache pure migraine is sometimes caused by both refractive and muscle errors. The migraine which disappears as pres- byopia comes on, proves itself clearly dependent on an error of refraction; and the same may be said of other 206 HETEROPHORIA. headaches that disappear as one grows old. Indeed, this coincidence should have attracted attention to focal errors as causative of headache long- before any thing- was known on this subject. Not so with headaches that are dependent on hetero- phoria, for strain of heterophoria once means strain of heterophoria throughout life, unless relieved by treat- ment, surg-ical or otherwise. VERTIGO AND NAUSEA. The kind of muscle error that is the most common cause of vertigo and nausea is insufficiency of the obliques to prevent cyclophoria. The correctness of this teaching is emphasized in cases of paresis of an oblique or of a superior or an inferior rectus, either one of which would be attended by a tor- sioning of the eyes. The earliest and most marked symptoms presented by these cases are vertigo and nau- sea, which continue so long as the patient tries to use both eyes. Excluding the vision of the affected eye, the symptoms vanish. When cyclophoria is the cause, these symptoms will be periodic, and will present them- selves only when the weak obliques are no longer able to maintain perfectly the parallelism between the verti- cal axes of the eyes and the median plane of the head. Overwork, worry, shock, ill health, sleeplessness all tend to make these symptoms worse. CONFUSION OF THOUGHT. Any one of the hetero- HETEROPHORIA. 207 phorias, whether associated with errors of refraction or not, necessarily interferes with that clearness of com- prehension one would have if his eyes were free from all errors. Reading becomes a burden to heterophorics for the reason that their thought centers work confusedly, through sympathy with the motor centers that are over- taxed in efforts at harmonizing the ocular muscles. How far confusion of thought may be carried toward insan- ity, because of continued existence of a muscle error, re- mains to be shown. Cases of undoubted insanity have been cured by operations on the ocular muscles. It has been a matter of common observation that school chil- dren who were counted as dull and incapable, not able to comprehend clearly either books or teachers, have been transformed into apt scholars by ocular treatment. In the race for an education, a child who has any form of heterophoria, or an error of refraction, is consider- ably handicapped. CHOREA. A spasmodic condition of the muscles of the face a local chorea in children with unstable nerve centers, is often caused by eye-strain, as is shown by the quick relief that follows a correction of the condi- tion causing the strain. The cause continuing to act, the transformation of a local into a more general chorea is often effected. It cannot be denied that chorea, some- times in a very aggravated form, is caused by visual 208 HETEROPHORIA. errors; but it cannot be asserted that all choreas are caused by ocular defects. It is safe and proper to say that every child suffering- with chorea should be exam- ined by an ophthalmic surgeon with the view of having any existing- ocular error adjusted. It is generally con- sidered that, whatever may be the cause, chorea is a re- flex neurosis, and that finding- and removing the cause brings a speedy cure. Medical treatment, without ref- erence to cause, is at best slow. EPILEPSY. There can be no longer any room for doubt that, in many cases, epilepsy, whether in the se- vere or in the light form, is often reflex in origin. If wax impacted in the ear can be the cause of epilepsy, is it unreasonable to suppose that hyperopia may cause this motor-psychic disturbance? But it is no longer a matter of supposition; for, beyond all question, man} 7 cases of epilepsy have been cured by the convex lenses that corrected the focal error. If a phimosis can excite epileptic seizures, is it any wonder that the excessive tension of muscles, in cases of heterophoria, may now and then be the cause of these attacks? In fact, scores of epileptics have been cured by operations for the estab- lishment of normal equilibrium between the ocular mus- cles. Hundreds of other cases would have been cured, before now, by partial tenotomies and advancements, if the principles underlying heterophoria had been properly HETEROPHORIA. 209 understood. The proportion of cases of epilepsy caused by heterophoria may not be large no one knows but every case of epilepsy should be subjected to a most careful examination of the visual apparatus, by a compe- tent investigator; and all focal errors found should be corrected and muscle imbalance should not be ig-nored. o That Stevens, Ranney, and others have spoken their convictions, based on observation and practical experi- ence, on this subject, the author believes. He himself has had and has cured some cases, while failing on oth- ers. In the light of no distant future, the statements that have been made by Stevens and Ranney will not appear to be so extravagant as some now judge them. Of all the apparently extravagant statements, this one is taken from "Ranney on Nervous Diseases," page 481: "One of the most remarkable cases that ever came un- der my observation was that of a combination of chorea, epilepsy, and idiocy, in a girl about eleven years of age, who completely recovered her health, strength, and men- tal faculties, when a refractive error in her eyes was corrected by glasses and a serious combination of muscu- lar defects in the orbit was adjusted by tenotomy. This case was one that I saw some three years ago, in con- nection with the practice of Dr. Stevens. At the first examinition, the child could not walk without being sup- ported on both sides, drooled constantly, talked unintel- 210 HETEROPHORIA. ligibly, answered questions with apparently little con- ception of their import, could hardly sit unsupported in a chair on account of chorea, had epileptic seizures re- peatedly during the day and night, and presented a piti- able and apparently hopeless aspect. I saw her about a year after the operations were performed, at the request of Dr. Stevens. I found her free from chorea and epi- lepsy, able to run and skip a rope unaided, rosy cheeked, and in full possession of her mental faculties." Who can wonder that Ranney, having observed this remarka- ble case, has become a firm believer in the reflex charac- ter of functional neurotic troubles? It is not remarkable that the refractive error was de- tected and measured in this case, but it must have been exceedingly difficult to arrive at a correct understanding of the complicating heterophoria. The phenomenal re- sults that followed but emphasize the importance of the early removal of the cause, before molecular changes in both the motor and psychic areas of the brain shall have become unalterably fixed. That epilepsy should be caused by abnormal tension of the delicate ocular muscles is certainly no more won- derful than were the results of some experiments made by Drs. Dercum and Parker, of the University of Penn- sylvania, as published in the Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, in 1884. Some of the subjects of ex- HETEROPHORIA. 21 1 perimentation were placed by a table, which they bare- ly touched with the tips of the fingers of one or both hands. "The fingers were not allowed to rest on the table, but were maintained, by constant muscular effort, barely in contact with it." Some of these subjects, in from a few minutes to an hour, first became tremulous and then violently convulsed, falling to the ground. The oftener the experiments were repeated on the sus- ceptible subjects, the more easily were the convulsions induced. Abnormal muscular tension, in persons with unstable nerve-centers, caused these convulsions, which must have been very much like epileptic attacks. Those experimented on who had stable nerve-centers did not show violent symptoms, but, judging from that part of the report published by Ranney, page 463, lighter symp- toms must have shown themselves, even in these cases. Other experiments showed that severe, long-continued thought, fixed on one thing, excited direct trouble in the psychic centers, and, sympathetically, disturbed the mo- tor centers, resulting in convulsions. If the result of the experiments made by Dercum and Parker had been more generally known, there would be fewer to doubt that severe functional neuroses may be caused by abnormal muscle tension in cases of hetero- phoria and errors of refraction. No other muscles of the body have such a wonderful nervous endowment as 212 HETEROPHORIA. the muscles (extrinsic and intrinsic) of the eye. Each of the twelve external muscles and each of the four mus- cles inside the two eyes has its individual nerve-center, and, besides these, there must be at least nine or ten con- jugate nerve-centers. Only the eye muscles must work with mathematical precision so as to have sharp images and binocular single vision. Any necessity for overaction abnormal tension on the part of any ocular muscle should be counteracted, if the delicate nervous system is to be freed from a prolific source of disturbance that may be either psychic, motor, sensory, or visceral. The emphasis given above is not intended to impress the reader with the idea that the orbit is Pandora's box, out of which come all functional ailments. Undue exci- tation of the brain-center controlling any organ of the body can reflexively disturb other centers near by and remote centers psychic, centers motor, centers sensory, centers controlling the viscera. It would be well for humanity, if all other organs and parts of the body could be so thoroughly and scientifically investigated as can the eyes. CATALEPSY. If clonic muscular contractions, asso- ciated with unconsciousness, can be caused by errors of refraction and heterophoria, the same errors may cause tonic muscular contractions rigidity with mental ob- livion. The cause should be sought and, when found, HETEROPHORIA. 213 removed. Occasionally excessive tension of the ocular muscles is causative of catalepsy. HYSTERIA. This disease is one of the terrible func- tional neuroses, and, like the others already considered, may depend on focal errors and imbalance of the ocular muscles. The character of the hysteria in a given case does not point to any definite cause. Whether the man- ifestations are sensory, motor, psychic, visceral, or vaso- motor, a cure can be effected, if the cause can be found; hence the importance of a most thorough investigation of these unfortunates. In these cases no investigation is complete that leaves out the visual apparatus. Er- rors of focus should be corrected, imbalanced muscles sh'ould be adjusted, with the fair prospect that at least some will be cured. NEURASTHENIA. This condition of weakness of the neuron elements is the very opposite of those just con- sidered, and yet it may have the same cause. If errors of refraction and heterophoric conditions do not cause neurasthenia in certain cases, it must be conceded that they can perpetuate it in all cases. The results of the correction of errors of refraction, and the regulation of the tension of the recti muscles by tenotomies and short- enings or advancements, have been so marvelous as to justify the declaration that every subject of neuras- thenia should have the visual apparatus investigated. 214 HETEROPHORIA. The correction of any existing- errors, to say the least, would tend to hasten a cure, whatever may have been the chief cause. Many cases have been speedily cured by these means alone, in which internal medication, electric- ity, and rest had been tried in vain. In any case in which but little nerve force is generated, the undue expendi- ture of that little should be prevented, thus giving med- icine, food, and rest a better chance to have full regen- erating power restored to the weak brain cells. In no case should the correction of visual errors be wholly re- lied on, but other organs and parts of the body should be investigated, and all local diseases found should be treated such as those of the stomach, the rectum, the bladder, the ovaries, and the womb; for these may have been the chief cause of the prostration, the visual errors having served only to aggravate and perpetuate the neu- rasthenia. Through the nervous system, errors of refraction and heterophoria may cause functional derangement of the thoracic, abdominal, and pelvic viscera. Indiges- tion, torpidity of the liver, and constipation have dis- appeared, in some cases, as a result of the correction of visual errors. A case of stammering was unexpect- edly cured by the wearing of prisms prescribed, by a Denver oculist, for an exophoria. The Doctor was so astonished at the result that he decided to remove the KETEROPHORIA. 21 5 prisms, so that he migLL determine whether the cure was a coincidence or a consequence, impost hoc or a prop- ter hoc. The stammering- retu. - A, and was again re- lieved by the wearing- of the prisms. Disturbed respira- tion and an irritable heart have been quieted by lenses and by operations on the eye muscles. Dr. Hale, of Nash- ville, once had a little patient suffering from a refractive error, whose bladder was so irritable that micturition in sleep was almost a nightly occurrence; but at the time of the examination of the eyes nothing was told the Doc- tor about the irritable bladder. Later the parents re- ported that the glasses had done more than was contem- plated, in that the irritability of the bladder had van- ished. The Doctor's astonishment was great. He de- cided to settle the question of relationship between the wearing of the glasses and the disappearance of the irritability of the bladder, by withholding the glasses. Almost immediately the bladder trouble returned, to dis- appear again, and permanently, when the spectacles were restored to the child. It has been a matter of common observation that dis- menorrhoea in girls and young women has been wholly or in part relieved by a correction of errors of the visual apparatus. These things are marvelous and can be ex- plained only by assuming that these remote organs have sympathized with the eyes in their efforts to correct 216 HETEROPHORIA. errors of adjustment and errors of focus. To claim that all cases like those referred to above have the exciting- cause in the eyes would be absurd; but when the cause is so simple, how easy and rapid the cure! So far the symptoms considered have been in parts more or less re- mote from the eyes. In many cases the only symptoms of eye-strain are in the eyes themselves or in their ap- pendages. ASTHENOPIA. This is a weakness of the eyes that may be shown in a sense of fatigue associated with more or less pain in the eyes, tog-ether with an excessive secre- tion of tears, whenever an attempt is made to do near work. Letters, while being- looked at, may fade away for a moment, because of relaxation of weak ciliary mus- cles; the pag-e may become blurred or mixed from side to side, because the imbalance of the lateral recti muscles momentarily increases the angle of convergence, as in esophoria, or by the temporary lessening- of this ang-le, as in exophoria; or the blurring- may be from top to bottom, caused by the sudden elevation of one visual axis above the other, as in hyperphoria. The conjunctival vessels often become cong-ested be- cause of visual errors. Likewise the lid marg-ins become eng-org-ed with blood, scales forming among the roots of the lashes, and the nutrition of the lashes themselves suffering. In high degrees of muscle imbalance, objects HETEROPHORIA. 217 in the distance sometimes become double momentarily. Not only may the external structures of the eyes be- come congested because of strain to overcome errors, but the structures within the eyes also may become congested. Functional disturbances without and with- in the eye, if long continued and much aggravated, may lead to organic changes and even result in the development of some of those diseases that bring blind- ness. One of the most troublesome asthenopias presents itself when a patient is exposed to bright light, either natural or artificial, and is caused by a weak sphincter of the iris, which must keep the pupil small so as to protect the delicate retina. TREATMENT OF HETEROPHORIA. The treatment of heterophoria must be determined by the kind and quantity of the error. Small 'errors of the recti may be treated by prisms in positions of rest for the too weak muscles. The base of the prism must always point toward the muscle to be favored. In esophoria the base would be out, and prisms of equal strength should be placed before the two eyes.* If the error is small and the interni are properly attached, or even if they are attached in greater part above the horizontal plane, in * Exceptions will be shown in the chapter on Esophoria 218 HETEROPHORIA. most cases they can be comfortably worn. If they do not give comfort, it becomes evident that the interni are attached too low, and that their forced action develops a plus cyclophoria, which becomes a source of discomfort. In exophoria the prisms should be of equal strength be- fore the two eyes,* and their bases should be in. If the error is small and the externi are correct!}" attached, the rest prisms would certainly bring comfort, at least for a time. If the externi are attached too low, the prisms, as a rule, can be comfortably worn; but if they are at- tached too high, the use of the prisms cannot bring com- fort because of the plus cyclophoria developed. In hyperphoria the correcting prism, in nearly all cases, should be worn only in front of the hyperphoric eye, the base being placed down, for the reason that the action of the superior rectus for overcoming the prism develops a minus cyclophoria, which, to a certain extent, neutralizes the plus cyclophoria which nearly always ex- ists. In the rare cases in which there is minus cyclopho- ria, the rest prism should be placed, base up, before the cataphoric eye, that a neutralizing plus cyclophoria may be caused. Only when there is perfect balance of the obliques would it be correct practice to divide the pris- matic effect between the two eyes, base down before the hyperphoric eye, base up before the cataphoric eye. The * Exceptions will be shown in the chapter on Esophoria. HETEROPHORIA. 219 per cent of cases accepting- kindly the rest prism, base down before the hyperphoric eye, is very large ; while only a very small per cent of such cases would be im- proved by placing- the rest prism, base up, before the cataphoric eye. A full prismatic correction of a hyper- phoria should be given only when there is a marked com- plicating- cyclophoria. In uncomplicated cyclophoria, the patient may be ben- efited by wearing a weak pair of cylinders, axes in the arcs of distortion for the stronger muscles, even when there is no astigmatism ; or, if there is astigmatism, by displacing the axes of the correcting cylinders in the arcs of distortion for the stronger pair of obliques. As will be shown in the chapter on cyclophoria, the arc of distortion by plus cylinders for the superior oblique of the right eye has its center always at 45, and for the left eye it has it at 135; while the center of the arc of distortion by plus cylinders for the inferior oblique of the right eye is always at 135, and for the left eye it is at 45. The reverse is true of minus cylinders. These arcs are equal in extent only when the astigmatism is vertical or hori- zontal, but their sum is always 180. The extent of the displacement of the axes of the cylinders, requisite for the relief sought, depends on the quantity of cyclo- phoria and the strength of the astigmatic correction a weak cylinder, more displacement; a strong cylinder, 220 HETEROPHORIA. less displacement. If there is no complicating hyper- phoria, the displacing" effect of the cylinders should be divided equally before the two eyes; but if there is a hyperphoria complicating a plus cyclophoria, only the cylinder before the cataphoric eye should be displaced; while the reverse would be true when a hyperphoria complicates a minus cyclophoria. It is clear that the enforced action of an inferior oblique would elevate the corresponding eye, to that extent neutralizing or correct- ing the cataphoria; while the enforced action of a supe- rior oblique would correspondingly depress the eye to which it belongs, thus diminishing, if not correcting, the hyperphoria. The displaced cylinders do for the weak obliques what rest prisms do for the weak recti. In both instances the law of direction is infringed, which, in itself, is not good. For this reason it is better prac- tice to relieve all forms of intrinsic heterophoria either by exercise or by operations. An objection applies to displaced cylinders that does not apply to prisms: by the former vision is rendered less acute, while by the latter there is no such interference. GYMNASTIC EXERCISE. In low degrees of heterophoria, of whatever kind, de- velopment of the weaker muscles by exercise is the best practice. The time necessary for curing these cases by HETEROPHORIA. 221 exercise and the trouble involved in carrying it out reg- ularly and systematically, constitute the chief objections to this method of treatment. What the author wrote in 1893 so perfectly harmo- nizes with his present views on the exercise of the ocu- lar muscles that it is reproduced in the few following pages: The development of the ocular muscles, by means of gymnastic exercise, has received but little attention from modern authors. Noyes devotes about one page to the subject; DeSchweinitz, less than one page; Schmidt- Rimpler, three lines, as follows, "No improvement is to be expected, as a general thing, from exercise of the in- terni; overexertion, that is apt to occur, may result, on the contrary, in a serious impairment of their power; " Fuchs, not a line; Berry, not a line; Meyer, ten lines; Landolt, not a line; Wells, one paragraph of ten lines; Schweigger, not a line; Nettleship, not a line; Juler, not one word; Carter, not a line. Those of the twelve authors above named who teach anything on the subject teach the same thing. This teaching is illustrated by the following quotation from DeSchweinitz: "Thus, to exercise the interni [in exo- phoria] a prism of 10 is placed, base out, before one eye, and as soon as the diplopia produced is overcome, 5 more are added, and so on until the limit of adductive 222 HETEROPHORIA. power is reached. . . . These exercises should be repeated every day for ten or fifteen minutes at a time, until the patient has acquired the power to overcome readily a prism of 50." He recommends the same char- acter of exercise for developing 1 the externi, beginning with a 3 prism and increasing" to 8. Noyes, following- Dyer, who wrote on this subject in 1865, says: "He [the patient] takes a candle flame or door knob at twenty feet for his object, and performs the efforts of adduction and abduction by means of these prisms. He begins, say, with adduction, and at first holds the prism of 5, with base out, before one eye; then substitutes the 10; then before the other eye places 5, making- a total of 15; then, if practicable, substitutes the other prism of 10 for the 5; and so climbs up the ladder of adduction prisms by such steps as he can make. If the interval of 5 becomes too great, he may take that of 23." He speaks of the exercise of the externi after the same plan, using weaker prisms with their bases in. He directs that the exercise be continued ten minutes at each sit- ting, and that it be repeated not oftener than twice a day, until, in case of the interni, a prism of 42^ can be readily overcome, and, in the case of the externi, a prism of 10. The prism of maximum strength having been reached, its use should be continued, says this author, once daily for a time. Noyes closes by saying: "A de- HETEROPHORIA. 223 cided gain in comfort and use of the eyes may be ob- tained by this proceeding; and if this result is not ade- quate, the true state of the muscular relations is brought to view." It is not necessary to make further quotations in order to bring 1 clearly into view the character of the exercise. It is the object of this paper to show that the plan is unsound in principle, and must necessarily be unsuccess- ful in practice. Continuous muscular contraction, aug- mented at short intervals, for ten minutes, or for even five minutes, may show what a muscle is capable of doing in an emergency; but it is not calculated to build up or de- velop the inherent power of the muscle. In a modified form Dr. Charles E}. Michel, of St. Louis, has persistently practiced the development of weak in- ternal recti muscles by means of prisms, since 1877. The prisms used by him have not been stronger than 4 nor weaker than 1. Beginning- with the weaker prism, he directs the patient to exercise frequently (ten to fif- teen times) during the day, each period of exercise to last only four or five minutes for the first few days; later they are to be worn only four or five times daily, increasing the time of exercise by two to five minutes daily, until they can be worn comfortably one hour. When the patient, looking in the distance, becomes able to wear the 4 prism one hour without discomfiture, he 224 HETEROPHORIA. is directed to commence reading. At first he must read only from three to five minutes at a time; but later he increases this time by two to five minutes daily, until he can read comfortably one hour, four times a day. When- ever this can be done, the patient is directed to continue for several months the reading-exercise practice for from a half to one hour, two or three times a day. To suit individual cases, modifications as to strength of prism and length of time and frequency of exercise must be made. Under Dr. Michel's treatment, fjilly 60 per cent of his patients have full muscular power developed, and in this way are enabled to use their eyes with comfort; 25 per cent have greater or less gain in comfort; while 15 per cent derive no benefit from the treatment. As a prelim- inary step to the muscle treatment, the Doctor always corrects any existing refractive errors, and has the pa- tient wear these lenses behind the exercise prisms. Dr. Michel's method of developing ocular muscles is given for the reason that it differs essentially from that set forth in the books, and for the additional reason that a high percentage of cures results from his method. The Doctor's success has been due to the fact that his \veak prisms used made but little demand on the weak muscles, thus making it possible for the continuous con- traction to be borne and the muscle strengthened. HETEROPHORIA. 225 In contrast with Dr. Michel's practice, the method of Dr. George T. Stevens, of New York, is here given in his own words: "Adduction may be greatly improved by gymnastic exercises of the interni, by means of prisms. In these exercises the eyes are required to unite images in overcoming gradually-increasing obstacles. A prism of a few degrees, perhaps 10, is placed, base out, before one of the eyes, \vhile gazing at a lighted candle at twenty feet distance, when an effort is at once made to prevent diplopia. As soon as the images are blended, another prism, of perhaps less degree, is placed in the same man- ner. The images being united, a stronger prism takes the place of one of those already in place, or one is add- ed to those already in position. Thus, little b} r little, the eyes are required to overcome prisms until the im- ages can no longer be united. Then all the glasses are removed and the process is repeated; with each repeti- tion something may be gained. The exercise should not be continued, at a single sitting, more than five or six minutes; and only a single sitting daily is desirable. By this means the adducting power can, in most cases, be raised, after a few exercises, to the desired point. "The effect of such exercise upon the eyes is very of- ten extremely salutary. With greater freedom of mus- cular action comes a sense of relief from nervous strain, which is often of a most gratifying character. Such an 226 HETEROPHORIA. exercise is in no way related to the practice sometimes adopted, and which should be condemned, of requiring the patient to gaze for a long time at a near object." The virtue of Dr. Michel's method lies in the fact that, though he taxes the muscles for a long while, gradually reaching the maximum of time, he taxes them but slightly, using only weak prisms; while the virtue of Dr. Stevens' method lies in the fact that, though he taxes the muscles severely, using the strongest prisms possible, reaching the maximum strength by degrees, he does not continue the exercise very long and does not re- peat the sitting again the same day. And, too, he al- most strikes the right principle in his method of inter- mitting- the exercise. In contrast with both of these methods, and with the methods laid down in the books, the method of RHYTHMIC EXERCISE will now be given, the author feeling confident that it is founded on sound principles and that it, therefore, can be carried out successfully in practice. Contraction and relaxation, alternating in short and rhythmic order, and continued short of fatigue, is the kind of exercise that develops a muscle in any part of the body. It is the alternate contraction and relaxation that develops the muscles of the arm of the blacksmith. If HETEROPHORIA. 227 the forearm should be flexed on the arm and held in that position ten minutes, no one would suppose that the muscles concerned could be developed thereby. There would be greater reason for believing- that such action would enfeeble the muscles. This is precisely the kind of contraction effected by prisms in the old method of exercising the recti muscles. There can be no wonder that better results have not followed, and that the prac- tice has been abandoned by almost all oculists. It would be of little worth to condemn the old practice as bad without setting forth a new line of practice, based on sound principles, and one that must be success- ful, in suitable cases. While rhythmic contraction and relaxation, regulated as to intensity and time, will develop any one of the recti muscles, as is developed the biceps of the blacksmith's arm, the writer would not be understood as believing that one of these muscles can be developed out of a loiv state of weakness into a high state of strength. There are cases of exophoria that will remain exophoric still, in spite of long-continued rhythmic and graduated exercise; and these cases, to be cured at all, must be cured either by partial tenotomies alone or by these supplemented with rhythmic exercise. The same may be said of eso- phoria and of hyperphoria. Only low degrees (not more than 6) of lateral heterophoria can be converted, by 228 HETEROPHORIA. rhythmic exercise alone, into orthophoria; the higher degrees can be corrected by partial tenotomies, shorten- ing's, and exercise combined. While, in suitable cases, the aim of partial tenotomies and shortenings should be to approach orthophoria, yet the greatest care should be exercised not to go beyond the "balance" line. The safest thing is to leave, for correction by exercise, some of the original condition. Any one of the recti and either of the obliques weaker than its opposing muscle, the difference in correspond- ing strength not being too great, may be developed by rhythmic exercise into a state that will enable it to work harmoniously with its fellow. EXERCISE FOR EXOPHORIA. Exophoria may be taken first for study. The quanti- ty should not be more than 6. The internal recti are the muscles wanting in strength. There are two plans of exercise, rhythmic in their nature, by either one of which, or by both combined, these muscles can be per- ceptibly strengthened: (1) The wax taper method; (2) The method by prisms, bases out. The exercise with the taper (small wax candle) must be conducted as follows: The patient is directed to light the taper and hold it at arm's length from, and on a HETEROPHORIA. 229 plane with, the eyes, immediately in front of the face. Fixing- his vision on the flame, he continues to look at it while he brings it slowly to within seven inches of his eyes, holding- it there about two seconds. He then closes his eyes for a moment (at the same time moving- the candle to one side) and, on opening them, fixes his vision on some distant object. The same procedure is gone through with a second time, and so on for five to fifteen times at one sitting. The sittings may be repeated one or more times daily for weeks or months. The best time for this exercise is early in the morning, while the mus- cles are fresh from sleep. In many cases the morning sitting will be sufficient for the day. This is especially so if the exophoria is low in degree. Reading or other near work should not be done within the hour after the exercise is taken. In this taper exercise no one can doubt that the guid- ing- sensation compels the internal recti to contract, in obedience to the law of corresponding retinal points, as the light advances, the maximum of contraction being reached when the taper is seven inches from the eyes. On closing the eyes partial relaxation of the interni oc- curs (keeping the eyes closed long enough, the relaxa- tion would become complete). The moment the eyes are opened and the vision is fixed on a distant object, in quick response to the guiding sensation, the relaxation 230 HETEROPHORIA. becomes complete. Thus is brought about contraction and relaxation, which should be discontinued short of fatigue. That this rhythmic exercise, properly regulat- ed as to frequency and force, will develop the internal recti, is susceptible of demonstration on the part of any one who wishes to know the truth. The second method for developing the interni is by means of prisms, bases out. The prisms to be used may be from 1 to 8, and one should be placed before each eye. The treatment should be commenced with the weaker prisms, and as development of the muscles advances, the stronger should be brought into use. The object looked at should be a candle, lamp, or gas jet, fifteen to twenty feet distant. With the prisms before the eyes, the image in each eye is displaced out, when the guiding- sensation calls quickly into action the interni for fusing them. After three seconds the interni must be allowed to relax for the same length of time (three seconds), which is readily effected by lifting the prisms up and al- lowing the light to enter the eyes uninfluenced. The guiding sensation at once causes the relaxation to take place, so that the yellow spots may receive the images. At the end of three seconds the prisms are again dropped before the eyes, when the interni again contract. Then a second time the relaxation is effected by lifting the prisms; and so on throughout every sitting, which should HETEROPHORIA. 231 last from two to ten minutes, but should always be dis- continued short of fatigue. The sittings should be re- peated two or more times a day. While it will take weeks, if not months, to establish orthophoria, neverthe- less this end can be attained, in suitable cases, by this method. It may be better in most cases to resort to the two methods of development, the taper and the prisms, each day, but not at the same sitting. In resorting to the prism exercise, it would be more con- venient to close the eyes, for the purpose of getting re- laxation, than to lift the prisms; but when the eyes are closed the relaxation is slow to take place, and is rarely complete at the end of sixty seconds; whereas, when the prisms are raised, the guiding sensation effects at once' complete relaxation, which continues till the prisms are again placed before the eyes. The rhythmic nature of the exercise is more perfect in the latter than in the for- mer, and results are better necessarily. The method of exercise of the interni by means of strong prisms, introduced by Dr. Deady, of New York, and later reintroduced and earnestly advocated by Gould, may have its merits, but certainly not in the line of mus- cle building. The good resulting from this method must come through excitation of the converging center, that of the third conjugate innervation. An overdraft on a nerve-center may be endured for a time, but should be 232 HETEROPHORIA. avoided, if possible. Ultimately exhaustion would be expected to follow. At any rate, it would seem to be far better to change the condition of the muscles so that the normal nerve impulse would make them do their work properly. That a muscle can be made stronger by light rhythmic exercise, never carried to the point of fatigue, does not admit of a doubt. The muscular Sandow, capa- ble of lifting many hundred pounds, developed his mus- cles by rhythmic exercise with three-pound dumb- bells. Without endorsing the use of strong prisms, in exo- phoria, the method must here be given. The exer- cise begins at a point twenty inches distant from a lighted candle or gas jet, by placing before the eyes the strongest prisms, bases out, that can possibly be over- come. At once the light is carried from the patient, or the patient recedes from the light, until a distance of twenty feet intervenes. The prisms are then raised, when, of course, relaxation occurs. When again within twenty inches of the light, the prisms are lowered, and recession follows as before. Thus the exercise is con- tinued from three to five minutes, the powerful contrac- ' tions and full relaxations following each other every seven seconds. The periods of exercise are to be repeat- ed several times a day. Very strong claims have been made for this method, and there may be more in it than HETEROPHORIA. 233 would appear from reasoning- about it; but its most ar- dent advocates confine its use to the treatment of ex- ophoria. ESOPHORIA. In this condition the muscles to be built up are the ex- ternal recti. There is but one method for doing this, and that is by means of prisms. These should be from 2 to 3, certainly not more than 4, and their bases must be placed in. Beginning with the weaker prisms, the patient should look at the candle twenty feet distant for three seconds, during which time the guiding sensation has caused the externi to undergo contraction; and then the prisms should be held up for three seconds to allow relaxation to take place. These steps should be thus regularly repeated throughout each sitting of two to ten minutes, the sittings themselves being repeated two or more times daily, as in the treatment of exophoria. In suitable cases orthophoria can be brought about. HYPERPHORIA. Hyperphoria and cataphoria, like esophoria, are sus- ceptible to exercise only by means of prisms. Given a case of left hyperphoria (right cataphoria) of not more than 1^, there is a possibility of developing vertical or- thophoria by means of rhythmic exercise. The muscle on the left side to be developed is the inferior rectus, 234 HETEROPHORIA. and that on the right side is the superior rectus. The prisms used should vary from ^ to 2; most cases will not require a stronger than a 1 prism. The base of the left prism must be up; that of the right prism, down. As in exophoria and esophoria, the patient should exer- cise from two to ten minutes at a time, and two or more times a day. The object looked at should be twenty feet distant, and it should be seen through the prisms three seconds, then without the prisms three seconds, and so on throughout each sitting. Thus contraction and relaxation of the weak left inferior rectus and weak 'right superior rectus are effected in rh} r thmic order. If . the hyperphoria is on the right side (left cataphoria), the base of the right prism must be up and that of the left prism must be down, when exercising. In every form of heterophoria the apex of the prism must point in the direction of the muscle to be developed by it. Occasionally cases present themselves in which there is a general weakness of the recti muscles, and especial- ly of the external and internal recti, unaccompanied by general physical weakness. Such cases generally mani- fest esophoria for distance and exophoria in the near, neither muscle being able to overcome a prism of any- thing like the usual strength. To operate on such a case would be improper, since relief of the exophoria in the near would be attended by a corresponding increase HETEROPHORIA. 235 of the esophoria for distance, and rice rcrsa. In such cases the intern! should be brought under the influence of the rhythmic exercise, as already set forth in the study of exophoria, at one time of the day; and, at some other time of the day, like attention should be paid to the external recti, as in simple esophoria. In these cases strychnia and electricity could be used with some promise of aiding the exercise treatment. Such patients should be allowed to undertake but little near work, un- til the exercise treatment by means of prisms has be- come well advanced. In these cases the wax-taper treat- ment of the internal recti is not applicable until late in the course. These cases are far more stubborn than cases of simple exophoria or simple esophoria; and yet great advantage can be derived from the rhythmic exer- cise by means of weak prisms, aided by strychnia and electricity. For cases showing esophoria in the distant test and | exophoria in the near, adduction and abduction both be- ing low, wall-to-wall exercise, probably, can accomplish more, in a shorter time, than the prism exercise referred to above. To perform this task the patient must stand against one wall of his room, equally distant from the walls on the right and left; previously there must have been pinned to the right and left walls two pieces of white paper, each at an angle of 35 from the patient 236 HETEROPHORIA. when in position for exercising, and as high from the floor as are his eyes. With his head in the primary po- sition and his face directed toward the middle line of the opposite wall, he must stiffen his neck while looking first at one piece of paper and then at the other, chang- ing from the one to the other with the regularity and in- terval of the tick of an old-time clock. This should be discontinued short of fatigue, and need not be prolonged over five minutes. It may be done once or twice a day. If exophoria in the near is 5 or more, the candle exercise may be resorted to once a day and the wall-to-wall ex- ercise once a day. Since this method of exercise costs nothing, and the patient is more impressed with the fact that he is doing something, in many cases it is better to prescribe it than the prism method. As in lateral, so in vertical, heterophoria, the imbal- ance may be associated with subnormal superduction and sub-duction. In such a case the prism exercise should be resorted to once a day for curing the imbal- ance, and the ceiling-to-floor exercise once a day, always short of fatigue, but never longer than five minutes. To do the ceiling-to-floor exercise, the patient must place himself as for the wall-to-wall exercise. A piece of paper, a spool of thread, or a pocketknife should be placed on the floor as far in front of the patient as he is tall. Fixing his head in the primary position, he is di- HETEROPHORIA. 237 rected to look first at the object on the floor, and then up at the junction of ceiling and wall, changing- from the one point of view to the other, as in the wall-to-wall exercise. Occasionally there will be both a lateral and a vertical imbalance with the duction power of every rectus below normal. In such a case the candle exercise for exopho- ria in the near, the prism exercise for the vertical im- balance, and the conjoined wall-to-wall and ceiling-to- floor exercise should be resorted to. In combining the wall-to-wall and ceiling-to-floor exercise, it is best done by looking, from four to six times, from wall to wall, and then, from four to six times, from ceiling to floor, con- tinuing thus to alternate for not longer than ten min- utes, but always short of fatigue. In cases in which the recti muscles are weak, because of a low state of general health, no treatment should be thought of except that intended for the well-being of the whole system. Use of the eyes in near work should be prohibited until recovery of the general health has occurred. CYCLOPHORIA. The treatment of insufficiency of the oblique muscles* is by means of cylindrical lenses (preferably convex), so placed as to lead the guiding sensation to demand con- See Ophthalmic Record, Vol. II., No. I. 238 HETEROPHORIA. traction on the part of the weak muscles. The +1.50 D. cylinder is the most useful, but a weaker one may be used at the beginning. One should be placed before each eye; and if the weak muscles are the superior ob- liques, their axes must be placed in the lower temporal quadrant, at first 15 from the vertical, when, because of slight retinal displacement of the object looked at, only a slight demand is made on the muscles, which should be kept up, in an intermitting way, for five minutes; then the axes should be revolved to 30 from the verti- cal, when, because of a greater displacement of the im- ages, a greater demand for contraction is made on the part of the weak obliques, which should be kept up intermittingly for three minutes; now, lasth', the axes of the cylinders are revolved to 45 from the vertical, when the maximum displacement of the images occurs, and hence the maximum demand is made on the muscles, which should be continued intermittingly for two minutes only. As in the exercise of the recti by means of prisms, the best way to get contraction and relaxation alter- nately and in rhythmic order, is to lower and raise the frames containing the prisms every three seconds, so, to get rhythmic contraction and relaxation of the ob- liques under exercise, it is best to raise and lower the frames containing the cylinders every three seconds throughout the sitting. It is unfortunate for the con- HETEROPHORIA. 239 venience of the patient that the relaxation of ocular muscles does not quickly follow the closing* of the eyes, since it would be much easier to open and close the eyes every three seconds than to lower and raise the frames at the same interval. The oblique muscles re- lax much more readily than the recti, on closing 1 the eyes, but even these do not completely relax in the short time of five seconds. In most cases of insufficiency of the obliques, exercis- ing" by means of cylinders once a day is sufficient; the best time for this exercise is before breakfast. The ob- ject looked at should be a horizontal black line on a white background or a white line on a black background, at a distance of ten feet. The cylinders should be properly centered. What prisms are to the recti, cylinders are to the ob- liques. In either case the lenses correcting- refractive errors should be worn during- the exercise, in order that the best results may follow. The only exception to this rule is the wax-taper exercise of the internal recti, when no lenses should be worn. OPERATIVE TREATMENT. The heterophorias not curable by correction of errors of refraction, by prisms in position of rest, or by rhyth- mic exercise, should be subjected to operative procedure. 240 HETEROPHORIA. Such cases are not infrequent, and the relief from opera- tions skillfully done is by no means uncertain. There is no department of surgery that requires more care in the making- of the diagnosis. The condition of every extrin- sic ocular muscle must be determined before any one muscle is to be operated upon. There are but two ob- jects in view in muscle operations: the one is altering the tension of a muscle, the other is chang-ing- its plane of action. The tension of a muscle is to be altered either by a central partial tenotomy, as when operating on the too strong muscle; by shortening the muscle in the line of its original plane, or by advancing it straight for- ward, as when operating on the too weak muscle. In making either one of these operations, the existence of a cyclophoria must be first excluded. When there is a cy- clophoria complicating any one of the other heteropho- rias, the operation on a rectus muscle should alter the tension of the muscle and, at the same time, change the plane of its action. In such a case a partial tenotomy should not be central only, but should include those pe- ripheral fibers, a division of which would be corrective of the cyclophoria. A shortening should be done in such a way as either to raise or depress the plane of action of the muscle as might be indicated by the complicating cy- clophoria. In making- advancements, the new attach- ment should be carried either higher or lower than the HETEROPHORIA. 241 original attachment, as the character of the cyclophoria might determine. The operation to simply alter the tension of a rectus muscle with the view of lessening its power is a central partial tenotomy. The operator should always be care- ful to leave a sufficient number of peripheral fibers to act as stay cords to prevent the cut muscle from re- tracting- too much. The strength of the uncut fibers in both directions should be equal, so that the plane of the muscle may not be changed. Both judgment and skill must be exercised, else too much or too little of the tendon may be cut. It is better to aim at leaving some of the old error uncorrected than to transform it into the opposite condition. The conjunctiva may, but the capsule of Tenon must, be divided coextensively with the division of the tendon, to obtain the effect desired. In no kind of heterophoria should a complete tenotomy ever be done; but if by accident it should happen, the tendon should be stitched to the sclera directly behind the original insertion, and at that distance behind de- termined by a correct understanding of the exact char- acter of the error for which the operation has been un- dertaken. If a complicating cyclophoria is to be corrected by a partial tenotomy of a rectus, not only must the tension of the muscle be altered for the correction of the main 242 HETEROPHORIA. error, but its plane must be changed so as to correct the complicating- cyclophoria. The kind of cyclophoria hav- ing- been determined and it is nearly always a plus cy- clophoria one is not left in doubt as to how the opera- tion should be done. To cure a sthenic hyperphoria and a plus cyclophoria, the nasal and central fibers of the su- perior rectus of the hyperphoric eye should be divided, while the temporal fibers should be left uncut and suffi- ciently strong to prevent any over-correction of the hy- perphoria; or the temporal and central fibers of the inferior rectus of the cataphoric eye should be divided, leaving the nasal fibers uncut and of sufficient strength to prevent an over-correction of the error. In a partial tenotomy for a sthenic esophoria compli- cated with a plus cyclophoria, there being no hyperpho- ria, the lower and some of the central fibers of both interni should be divided, leaving the upper fibers uncut. In this way the tension of the muscle is altered, curing, wholly or in part, the esophoria, and the plane of each muscle is elevated so as to correct the plus cyclophoria. The plane of both interni having been equally elevated, there is of necessity developed a slight double hyperpho- ria which, however, will give no trouble, being easily overcome by a pose of the head. In operating on a case of sthenic esophoria compli- cated by a right hyperphoria and a plus cyclophoria, the HETEROPHORIA. 243 first operation must be done on the internus of the cata- phoric eye, and should consist of a complete division of the lower and central fibers, leaving uncut the upper fibers of the tendon. The threefold effect of this pro- cedure is a correction, in part or wholly, of the esophoria; a correction of thecataphoria; and a cure of the plus cy- clophoria. Whatever part of the esophoria may remain after this operation should be corrected by a partial cen- tral tenotomy of the right internus. Should some of the right hyperphoria remain, but no cyclophoria, a par- tial central tenotomy of the right superior rectus should be done; but if there should remain some uncorrected plus cyclophoria as well as right hyperphoria, the inner and enough of the central fibers of the right superior rectus should be cut to cure these conditions. These three operations sometimes even one or two of them will cure a complicated case of this character. If the esophoria is asthenic and uncomplicated, the operation of shortening should be done on one or both externi, and the plane of these muscles should be the same after as before the operation, otherwise a hyper- cyclophoria would be created. If the asthenic esophoria is complicated with a right hyperphoria only, the externi should be shortened as though no complication existed, and later the hyperpho- ria could be treated by exercise, by a prism in position 244 HETEROPHORIA. of rest for the hyperphoric eye, or by a central tenotomy of the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye. In such a condition no muscle plane should ever be changed. The plane of a muscle should never be changed unless there is a cyclophoria to be corrected. If the asthenic esophoria should be complicated with a plus cyclophoria alone, not only should both externi be shortened, but the plane of each should be depressed so as to cure both the esophoria and plus cyclophoria. If the asthenic esophoria is complicated by a right hy- perphoria and a plus cyclophoria, the first operation should be on the externus of the hyperphoric eye, and should be a shortening so done as, at the same time, to depress the plane of its action. The triple effect will be to cure, more or less completely, the esophoria, the right hyperphoria, and the plus cyclophoria. If the left externus must be shortened for a remaining esophoria, whether complicated or not by a cataphoria and plus cy- clophoria, one or both, for obvious reasons the plane of this muscle should not be altered. If the plane were elevated, it would lessen the cataphoria, but would in- crease the plus cyclophoria; if the plane were lowered, it would decrease the plus cyclophoria, but would in- crease the cataphoria. After the straight-forward short- ening of the externus of the cataphoric eye, whatever hyperphoria alone may exist should be treated, if suffi- HETEROPHORIA. 245 ciently great in quantity, by a central partial tenotomy of the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye; but if the remaining- hyperphoria should be complicated with a re- maining- plus cyclophoria, the inner, and as much as necessary of the central, fibers of the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye should be cut. The operation for sthenic exophoria, uncomplicated, is, a central partial tenotomy of one or both externi, prefer- ably both. The object in view being- only the alteration of tension, care must be exercised that the plane of rota- tion shall not be chang-ed. When sthenic exophoria is complicated by a hyper- phoria, the operation on the externi must not be done with a view of affecting- the hyperphoria, hence central partial tenotomies are indicated. Later the hyperpho- ria must be relieved by a central partial tenotomy of the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye and, if necessary, a central partial tenotomy of the inferior rectus of the cataphoric eye. The tension of these muscles should be altered without a chang-e of plane. In sthenic exophoria, complicated by a right hyper- phoria and a plus cyclophoria, the first operation should be done on the externus of the hyperphoric eye, and it should consist of a division of the upper and central fibers, leaving- uncut the lower fibers. The threefold re- sult of this operation will be: (1) relaxing- the tension of 246 HETEROPHORIA. the externus, lessening, if not curing, the exophoria; (2) a turning- of the eye down, thus counteracting- the hy- perphoria; (3) torting the eye in, curing the cyclophoria. If some of the exophoria remain, whether still compli- cated or not by a right hyperphoria and a plus cyclopho- ria, the operation on the left externus must be a central partial tenotomy. The reason is clear: a division of the upper and central fibers would so change the muscle plane as to increase the cataphoria, although diminish- ing the cyclophoria; while a cutting of the lower and central fibers would so change the plane as to lessen the cataphoria, but increase the plus cyclophoria. The only safe course between this Scylla and Charybdis is a cen- tral partial tenotomy of the left externus. These two operations having been done, any remaining right hyper- phoria, without a plus cyclophoria, should be relieved by a central partial tenotomy of the right superior rectus; but if the remaining hyperphoria should be complicated with plus cyclophoria, the nasal and central fibers should be cut, with the double purpose of altering the tension for the hyperphoria and changing the plane for the cyclophoria. When sthenic exophoria is complicated by a plus cyclo- phoria only, the upper and central fibers of both externi (not one alone) should be cut. The triple effect of these operations is: (1) alteration of tension for the exo- HETEROPHORIA. 247 phoria; (2) lowering plane of both extern! for the plus cyclophoria; (3) the development of a double cataphoria, which, in itself, is not bad. In asthenic exophoria, uncomplicated, the tension of the interni must be increased by shortenings or advance- ments so done as not to change the plane of rotation. The same is true of asthenic exophoria complicated by a hyperphoria alone. Later the hyperphoria may be re- lieved by a central partial tenotomy of the superior rec- tus of the hyperphoric eye. When an asthenic exophoria is complicated with a right hyperphoria and a plus cyclophoria, the internus of the cataphoric eye should be so shortened or advanced as to alter its tension, for the exophoria, and elevate its plane, for counteracting the cataphoria and curing the plus cyclophoria. If the internus of the once hyper- phoric eye must be shortened or advanced to still fur- ther correct the exophoria, not any longer complicated, it must be so done as not to change its plane; and the same is true if the only remaining complication is a hy- perphoria, for in counteracting the hyperphoria, a plus cyclophoria would be developed. It is also true that only the tension of the internus should be altered by a shortening or advancement when the remaining exopho- ria is complicated by hyperphoria and plus cyclophoria, for the reason that lowering the plane would increase 248 HETEROPHORIA. the cyclophoria, although lessening" the hyperphoria; while elevating 1 the plane would increase the hyperpho- ria, although diminishing the cyclophoria. Later the hyperphoria or hyper-cyclophoria should be remedied by the correct operation on the superior rectus. An asthenic exophoria complicated by a plus cyclopho- ria must be treated by such shortening or advancement of both interni as to alter the tension for the exophoria and elevate both planes for the plus cyclophoria. The double hyperphoria resulting would be counteracted by a pose of the head. Cyclophoria may exist alone and may be so high in de- gree as to demand relief by operation. This can be ac- complished by operating on both superior or both infe- rior recti. A plus cyclophoria, uncomplicated, can be relieved by dividing a few of the nasal fibers or advan- cing a few of the temporal fibers of both superior recti. In doing the former a double cataphoria is developed, while the cyclophoria is cured; in doing the latter the cyclophoria is cured, but a double hyperphoria results. Since a double cataphoria is preferable to a double hyper- phoria, a division of the nasal fibers of the superior recti should always be chosen. The plus cyclophoria can be cured by a division of the temporal fibers or an advancement of the nasal fibers of both inferior recti. The former would give a double HETEROPHORIA. 249 hyperphoria, while the latter would give a double cata- phoria; hence, of the two the latter should be chosen. As to final results, a division of the nasal fibers of the superior recti and an advancement of the nasal fibers of the inferior recti are precisely alike; but the former should be preferred, for it is more easily done and gives the patient much less inconvenience. OPERATIONS ON THE RECTI. The strictest antiseptic precautions must be observed in the preparation of the patient and the instruments; and the operator and assistant must have clean, asep- Fig. 36. THE STEVENS SCISSORS. tic hands. Unless the patient is a child who cannot be controlled, or a very nervous adult, general anesthesia should not be produced. Under cocaine anesthesia, the Fig- 37- THE STEVENS FORCEPS. solution always being made fresh and sterile for each case, muscle operations are practically painless. These 250 HETEROPHORIA. operations may be made almost bloodless by the use of two or three drops of adrenaline chloride solution (1-1000) dropped into the eye while cocainization is being- effected in the usual way one drop of a 10 per cent solution at Fig. 38. THE STEVENS FORCEPS. intervals of two minutes, until three or four drops have been instilled. In five minutes after the instillation of the last drop of cocaine solution the operation should be commenced. The stop-speculum, fixation forceps, Ste- r iii ^~ Fig- 39- THE STEVENS HOOK. vens scissors, and Stevens hook constitute the array of instruments necessary for doing partial tenotomies. If a muscle is to be advanced, a needle holder, two small nee- dles curved at the point, number five silk (either white Fig. 40. THE STEVENS NEEDLE HOLDER. or iron dyed), a large strabismus hook, and a silver su- ture plate must be added to the instruments named; and if a shortening operation is to be done, an additional large strabismus hook, or, probably what would be bet- HETEROPHORIA. 25l ter, the muscle forceps devised by Clark, of Columbus, O. Most of these instruments are shown in accom- panying 1 cuts. The time occupied in doing any of these operations is short. The after treatment consists in using- freely, several times a day, while the redness lasts, an anodyne-antiseptic wash (Tinct. Opii, gtt. xxx,Acid Boracic, gr. xx, Dist. water, oz. i). Both eyes should be kept open, to favor easier binocular adjustment. A muscle suture should be removed on the seventh day, unless severe reaction should indicate an earlier removal. The Price silver suture plate does its best work in facilitat- ing- the removal of the suture, making- this step painless as well as easy. It prevents the swollen tissue from concealing- the knot. PARTIAL TENOTOMY. There are two kinds of partial tenotomy, central and marginal. The indications for the one or the other may always be well understood in any given case, as has been set forth in another part of this chapter. The object of the central tenotomy is only to lessen the tension of the muscle; the object of the marginal tenotomy is both to lessen the tension of the muscle and to change its plane of action. To do a central partial tenotomy, the lids must be well separated by the speculum. The patient must look 252 HETEROPHORIA. as far as possible in the direction opposite the muscle to be operated upon. The conjunctiva over the insertion of the tendon must be lifted in a meridional fold with the forceps, and this must be snipped with the scissors. Through the cut in the conjunctiva the forceps should be made to grasp the capsule of Tenon, which in turn should be snipped. Through the openings in conjunctiva and capsule, the central fibers of the tendon should be grasped with the forceps and slightly raised from the sclera, so that they may be cut with the scissors be- tween the forceps and the attachment, as close to the latter as possible. Thus the tendon is buttonholed. If the operator is certain, from the resistance he feels with the forceps, that he is not too near either margin of the tendon, he may divide a few more fibers, in both directions, while still holding the tendon with the forceps; but in doing so he takes some risk of doing too much. Now the forceps should be laid down for the small hook, vhich should be passed through the buttonhole in the tendon, first in one direction, then in the other, beneath the uncut fibers, so as to determine the resistance. Guided by the hook, the operator now divides fiber after fiber with the scissors, until the lessened resistance warns him that he has gone far enough in that direction. He then repeats this step toward the other margin in the same careful way. Some of the fibers in both directions HETEROPHORIA. 253 must be left uncut, so as to act as stay cords to prevent a too far recession of the cut part of the tendon. The strength of the uncut fibers at the margin should be left as nearly equal as possible, so that the muscle plane may be the same after as before the operation. Should' the fibers be left stronger at the one margin than at the other, the plane will be certainly shifted toward the stronger fibers, and an undesired torsioning effect will accompany the lessening of the tension. To get the full effect of a partial tenotomy, the capsule of Tenon must be cut coextensively with the division of the tendon. The cut in the conjunctiva may or may not be of the same ex- tent. There is no necessity for making either a very small or a very large conjunctival incision; but for those just beginning to operate, a large conjunctival incision would make the tenotomy both easier and safer. Dr. George H. Price, of Nashville, has just invented an instrument that may prove to be most useful, in that it can measure the amount of resistance of uncut fibers when a partial tenotomy is being done. Up to the pres- ent, operators have been guided only by an indefinable sense of resistance when drawing on the hook. If that resistance can be measured and the tendonometer gives promise in that direction eventually a rule of practice may be established which will be valuable to any opera- tor, whether experienced or inexperienced. Fig. 41 will 254 HETEROPHORIA. give the reader a fair understanding" as to the construc- tion of the instrument; however, a description by the in- ventor follows : THE TENDONOMETER. " This instrument, as its name implies, is designed for measuring- the resistance of the ocular muscles, either in part or as a whole. It consists of a muscle hook (a), with graduated shaft (s), which is suspended by a coil spring (c) in a hollow metal handle (h), the construction being upon the same general principle as an ordinary spring balance. The hook proper is 5-16 of an inch long and nearly straight, so that it can be passed under the 1 I I I I I I .' I II I I I II I ; I I j 3 * j I 7 j t> 11 * H TEROPHORIA. done. If there is no complicating- cyclophoria, the ad- vancement must be made directly forward, so that after the operation the plane of rotation shall be the same as before. The same careful preparation of patient, instruments, and hands must be made as for shortening-; and the same instruments are needed, except that only one hook will be required. This operation, done under cocaine, is less painful than a shortening 1 , and is about as quickly done. The lids should be separated widely with a speculum. The conjunctiva should be grasped in line with the center of attachment of the tendon, half- way between the attachment and the margin of the cor- nea, and in such a way as to lift a meridional fold, which should be cut with the scissors. This cut, after gaping, should be about as large as the tendon insertion is wide. The posterior flap should be drawn backward to a point just behind the insertion. Now, by means of forceps and scissors, a snip should be made through the capsule at one border of the tendon, and through the opening thus made a large hook should be passed beneath the tendon. While the assistant holds the conjunctival flap back, the operator raises the tendon with his hook, and then passes one of the two needles with which the suture is armed, twice through the capsule and tendon, so as to include its center, and a little way behind its insertion. This is done in the manner of taking a stitch in cloth. HETEROPHORIA. 265 The assistant now takes hold of the two ends of the suture and draws the loop well up against the under surface of the tendon, at the same time lifting- the ten- don slightly away from the sclera. Aided by the hook, the operator now completely divides the tendon, at its in- sertion, with one or two snips of the scissors. The next step is to make a pouch beneath the anterior conjunctival flap, which is easily done with forceps to hold and scis- sors to cut. This pouch should be made directly in front of the old attachment, and neither higher nor lower, if the muscle is an externus or an internus, nor farther out or in, if the muscle is a superior or inferior rectus. In either case, the pouch should extend up to the corneal margin. The next step is to pass the two needles through the posterior conjunctival flap a little way be- hind its edge. Now the operator lifts the anterior con- junctival flap with the forceps and carefully passes one needle, held by the needle holder, into the pouch already prepared, and makes it dip well into the sclera, but not through it, a little above (if it be the upper needle) an imaginary line bisecting the original attachment, and only the slightest distance away from the cornea. In passing out of the sclera the needle penetrates the con- junctiva. This needle is now held out of the way by the assistant, while the operator passes the second needle in the same way, but a little below the line bisecting the 266 HETEROPHORIA. original insertion. The needles are now passed through the holes in the silver plate, after which they are re- moved. The assistant now grasps the conjunctiva, cap- sule, and muscle just behind the loop and forcibly draws these structures well forward, while the operator ties the knot. The farther the loop of the suture is passed through the tendon behind the insertion, the greater will be the effect of the operation. The suture passed, as described above, the tension of the muscle will be in- creased, but its plane of rotation will not be changed. Since a suture thus taken is not likely to cut its way out, even to a small extent, too much over-effect should not be attempted. If the needles were not made to dip into the sclera, but simply passed through the conjunc- tiva near the corneal margin, an over-effect would be necessary, for the reason that the thread will partly cut its way out before adhesion has formed, allowing some re- cession of the advanced tendon. There is even danger that the thread will cut its way entirely out if passed only through the conjunctiva, when, of course, the re- cession would be excessive. No advancement operation is safe unless the suture is well anchored in the sclera. An advancement intended not only to alter the tension, but also to change its plane of rotation, differs from the operation thus described only in selecting the place for the sclero-conjunctival stitches. If the muscle to be HETEROPHORIA. 267 thus operated upon is an interims, and the exophoria is complicated by a left hyper-cyclophoria, the one to be advanced is the right internus, when one of the objects in view is the elevation of its plane of rotation. The tendon must be found, and a loop of the suture must be passed through it, as for a straight-forward advancement. After completely severing the tendon from the attach- ment, the conjunctival pouch must be made higher than the original attachment. The two needles must now be passed into the pouch, and made to dip into the sclera, then out through the conjunctiva, at chosen points, higher up than the old attachment, and close to the cor- neal margin. On tying the suture thus passed, the muscle is carried higher up on the globe, as well as farther forward. Thus its tension is altered so as to cure the exophoria, and its plane is changed so as to counteract the cataphoria and the plus cyclophoria. In like manner the plane of either the externus or the superior or inferior recti may be changed by an advance- ment, the only difference being, in cases of plus cyclo- phoria complicating other phorias, the direction in which the plane is to be shifted. That of the externus must be carried lower; that of the superior rectus, farther out; that of the inferior rectus, farther in. Marginal advancements may be done on both intern! when there is only a slight asthenic exophoria complicated 268 HETEROPHORIA. by a plus cyclophoria. In doing- this, the stitch in the tendon should be in its upper border; only the upper fibers should be cut at the insertion, after which, by means of scleral stitches, this part alone should be brought straight forward, or only slightly higher, and not very far in advance of the old insertion. Similar operations on the externi, or on the superior and inferior recti, may be done under proper indications, the operator being certain that he has selected the proper part of the tendon for the partial advancement. In all advancement operations the suture should be tied over the silver plate, and should be allowed to re- main in place seven days, so as to give enough time for the formation of adhesions. In removing the suture, care should be exercised that the adhesions may not be torn loose. The after-treatment should be the same as for partial tenotomies; that is, the free and frequent use of the antiseptic-anodyne solution. Other methods of making the advancement operation might be given. The main objection to the high-and-low stitch operations, devised and advocated by Beard, of Chicago, and Black, of Denver, is that the advanced tendon cannot be accurately placed, so that in one case the plane of rotation may be changed, when it should have remained as before; while in another case it might HETEROPHORIA. 269 remain as before, when it should have been changed. Nor are these operations so simple or so free from trau- matism as the very easy and safe method indorsed and advised in this chapter. Again, let it be said, advancements are rarely indicated in the treatment of heterophorias, and, whenever pos- sible, should be substituted by the operation of shorten- ing", which is easier, safer, and better. But in Hetero- tropias, as will be shown in Chapter IX., advancements are indicated in a large proportion of cases. The best of all advancement operations, therefore the only one that should be done, is the " flat advancement " without severing the tendon, devised by Lagleize. A horseshoe incision, with the convexity toward the cor- nea, should be made through the conjunctiva and capsule of Tenon, from a point well in advance of the tendon at- tachment, backward, over the muscle to be advanced, sufficiently far to expose that part of the muscle through which the loop of the suture must be passed. The as- sistant must hold this flap out of the way by means of a forceps. The operator then passes a large strabismus hook beneath the tendon, and draws it well up against the insertion of the tendon. Next he passes a second large hook beneath the tendon and carries it backward beneath the belly of the muscle to a point beyond where he expects to pass the loop of suture. He now places 270 HETEROPHORIA. the second hook in the hand of the assistant, who gently lifts the muscle away from the globe. With the first hook the operator steadies the eye while passing" the suture through the muscle, which he may do in one of two ways: (1) He may take the muscle part of the stitch with only one of the two needles with which the suture is armed, by passing it through the muscle near one bor- der and bringing it out at a point directly opposite, near the other border. Drawing the suture after the needle thus passed places the loop beneath the belly of the muscle. Or (2) he may pass one needle beneath the mus- cle and force it through near the far border, and then pass the second needle through the near border, from beneath, at a point directly opposite the puncture of the other needle. Drawing the suture after the two needles places the loop beneath the belly of the muscle, as if it had been taken with one needle, as described in (1). This loop must always be just as far behind the inser- tion of the tendon as the later scleral stitches shall be in front. The muscle part of the suture having been passed, the two hooks should be removed. The next step of the operation is the passing of both needles through the anterior margin of the capsulo-conjunctival flap. The operation must be completed without cutting- the tendon. To enable him to pass easily the scleral stitches, the operator fixes the eyeball by firmly grasp- HETEROPHORIA. 271 ing, with fixation forceps, the exposed tendon at its in- sertion, and then passes first one needle and then the other deep into, but not through, the sclera, at points just as far in advance of the tendon insertion as the loop is be- hind it. The scleral stitches are to be neither hig-her nor lower than the two points of passing- of the muscle part of the suture, if the rotation plane of the muscle is not to be chang-ed. The needles should now be passed throug-h the two holes of the Price suture plate, after which they should be removed. In tying- the surg-eon's knot on the silver plate, the muscle is carried over its attachment until that part throug-h which the loop was passed is at the line of the two scleral punctures, and it is kept there by the completion of the knot- The cap- sulo-conjunctival flap completel} T covers the operative field by having- been included in the suturing-. The stitches must be allowed to remain seven days. If the rotation plane of the muscle must be chang-ed, the scleral punctures must be made in the direction in- dicated in any g-iven case. There is no advancement operation that will compare with the "flat advancement" operation of Lag-leize, in the ease with which it is done, in simplicit}', and in safe- ty; but even this operation should be done only when more effect is needed than can be g-otten from the sim- pler and safer operation of tucking-, or folding-, or short- 272 HETEROPHORIA. ening, devised by the author, but erroneously claimed by sundry others. The shortening 1 operation is fully de- scribed on pages 257 to 263. In the treatment of heterophoric conditions by any method outlined in this chapter, the aim is to relieve the basal or fusion brain-centers from abnormal work, by giving" equal tonicity to the two muscles of any pair the establishment of orthophoria. To withhold the treatment indicated in any given case of heterophoria is unjust to the patient and will be hurt- ful to the practitioner. The day has forever passed when an oculist can securely boast of ignorance of heter- ophorias, on his own part, and, in turn, speak dispara- gingly of others who claim to know. Confession of ig- norance on his own part disqualifies one for passing un- favorable judgment on others who, by means of hard study and close observation, have a right to claim that they know. CHAPTER IV. ESOPHORIA. THERE is an esophoria which, because of its nature, may be called "true," or "intrinsic;" while there is another form that should be termed " pseudo." The one kind is entirely distinct from the other, and yet the two often co- exist, the one being- grafted on to the other. Whatever may be the kind of esophoria, there is a tendency on the part of the interni alone, or with the aid of their syner- gists, to converge the visual axes at a point between the observer and the object fixed; but this too near intersec- tion of the visual axes is prevented by excessive nerve impulses sent to the antagonizing muscles, increasing their tension abnormally. In the interest of binocular single vision the too great inherent tension of the interni is counteracted by a corresponding nervous tension of the externi. INTRINSIC ESOPHORIA. In this condition the interni have an advantage over the externi, which may be due to any one of several conditions. It may be that the in- terni are over-developed, or, what would result in the same thing, the externi may be under-developed. In (278) 274 ESOPHORIA. either case there would be an imbalance in favor of the interni. That this is often true hardly admits of a doubt; for every surgeon who has operated often will testify that, in operating on the internus to lessen its tension, he has frequently found it very large and strong, and that, in operating on the externus to increase its tension, he has found it small and weak. The interni may not be over-developed, but abnor- mally short; or the externi may be abnormally long, thus giving an imbalance in favor of the interni. In oper- ating on an internus to lessen its tension, it is sometimes found to be tense, as if stretched; while in operating on an externus to increase its tension, this muscle is found loose and flabby. The interni may not be over-developed nor the ex- terni under-developed, nor may the interni be too tense while the externi are too lax. The esophoria found in such a case would be due to the interni having their at- tachments too far forward, while the externi have their attachments too far removed from the corneal margin. If either of the conditions mentioned above should be the cause of an esophoria, it can be understood readily how the esophoria may be greater in the one eye than in the other, and that it may exist only in one eye, the lat- eral muscles of the other eye being perfectly balanced. This can be determined quickly and accurately by means ESOPHORIA. 275 of the monocular phorometer, the binocular phorometer being wholly unreliable for this purpose. Whatever may be the cause, esophoria is usually about equal in the two eyes. Regardless of the existence of the one or the other of the four causes discussed, or that two or more of them may coexist, the treatment, as will be shown, must be directed either toward the interni with the view of lessening their tension by partial tenoto- mies, or toward the externi with the view of increasing their power by means of exercise or by shortening or ad- vancing one or both of them. In low degrees of the er- ror, prisms in positions of rest for the weak externi may be tried. True, or intrinsic, esophoria may not depend on either one of the four causes mentioned, but may be caused by a naturally over-developed third conjugate innervation center which, without being over-stimulated, continu- ally, during waking hours, is sending excessive nerve impulses to the otherwise normal interni, to counteract which, over-stimulation of the centers controlling the ex- terni is demanded; or there may be an under-develop- ment of the centers controlling the externi, requiring that these shall be over-stimulated in order that a nerve impulse sufficiently strong shall be sent to the externi to counteract the normal impulse sent to the interni. The third conjugate innervation center is wholly undeveloped 276 ESOPHORIA. in some cases, there being- entire absence of power to converge; in other cases it appears that this center is not fully developed; hence it is reasonable to conclude that now and then an over-development of this center exists. If so, the esophoria is as true, or intrinsic, as if the muscles themselves were at fault. There may be, or there may not be, a conjugate innervation center for the externi; but, if so, it is only intended that it shall force the externi to counteract- the tendency on the part of the interni to make the visual axes cross too soon. It would not be in the interest of binocular single vision for a conjugate divergence brain center to exist, hence the supposition that there is no such center. Unless one or more of the above-mentioned causes ex- ists, there can be no such thing as an intrinsic esophoria. The superior and inferior recti, when attached, in great- er part, on the nasal side of the vertical meridian of the eye, act as a secondary cause of true esophoria. Malformation of the orbits cannot have much to do, directly, with the causation of intrinsic or even pseudo- esophoria. As has been shown in Chapter I., the an- gle of convergence for eyes that are wide apart is but little greater than the angle of convergence when the eyes are close together, and yet that little may consti- tute one of the factors in the production of an esophoria, or, vice versa, of an exophoria. When the eyes are 3 ESOPHORIA. 277 inches apart and the point of fixation is 16 inches, the angle of convergence is 10.7, while the angle of con- vergence for the same point, the eyes being 2 inches apart, would be 7.16, a difference of 3.54. It is, there- fore, reasonable to conclude that a muscle adjustment that would give orthophoria when the eyes are 3 inches apart would give esophoria if they were only 2 inches apart. It is also reasonable to conclude that a muscle adjustment that would give orthophoria, the base-line being 2 inches, would give exophoria if the base-line were 3 inches. Malformation of the orbit must play only a very small part in the production of an esophoria or an exophoria. STHENIC ESOPHORIA. The quantity of the esophoria does not determine its character with any certainty. If the error is sthenic in character, it can be told only by resorting to the duction and version tests. How to make these tests has been fully set forth in Chapter II. and Chapter III.; and so important are these tests, from a therapeutic standpoint, they can never be safely neglected. In sthenic esophoria adduction should be more than 25, and adversion should be more than 50. Abduction and abversion may be only little less than normal, and rarely would these exceed the normal. More dependence must be placed on abduction and ab- version than on adduction and adversion in determining 278 ESOPHORIA. if an esophoria is sthenic. If, in a case of esophoria, ab- duction and abversion are nearly normal, it is of the sthenic type. ASTHENIC ESOPHORIA. Here, again, it is not the quantity of the error that determines its character. It is only by the duction and version tests that the asthenic may be distinguished from the sthenic. In such a case the adduction power is less than 25, and the ad ver- sion is less than 50. Abduction and abversion will be correspondingly low. If in responding to these tests a patient should show less duction and version power than he really has, the error will be on the safe side. In as- thenic esophoria the intern! should never be operated upon. What to do for such cases will be fully set forth under the head "Treatment." PSEUDO-ESOPHORIA. As its name implies, it is an esophoria that has neither of the above-mentioned causes. It is wholly dependent on the relationship that exists between the third conjugate innervation center and the center controlling the ciliary muscles. It is never found in a myope; it is never shown in the distant test when the patient is an emmetrope. Even an emme- trope may show a pseudo-esophoria in the near, but only when the ciliary muscles are weak and must re- ceive an abnormal impulse in order to focus near objects. Pseudo-esophoria always exists in cases of hyperopia, ESOPHORIA. 279 manifesting- itself in one of three ways: first, lessening- the quantity of an intrinsic exophoria; second, showing- an esophoria when, in reality, there is no imbalance be- tween the lateral recti; third, showing- a greater quan- tity of esophoria than really exists. Thoug-h false in character, it is nevertheless harmful, unless it compli- cates an exophoria. Then, as will be shown in the chapter on exophoria, it is helpful in the absence of any treatment of the exophoria. Bonders was rig-ht when he emphasized the influence that accommodation has over converg-ence, thoug-h some have doubted that there is any truth in his teaching- on this subject. He may have attached too much impor- tance to it; doubtless he did g-o be} T ond bounds when he taug-ht that hyperopia was the chief cause of internal strabismus. Some idea of the probable effect that ac- commodation has over convergence may be obtained by the study of eyes that are emmetropic and orthophoric. Such eyes accommodate 3 D for a distance of 13 inches. The distance between the centers of the eyes being- 2-1 inches, the angle of converg-ence for 13 inches will be 11. Each eye accommodates 3 D, and its visual axis is turned toward the median plane of the head throug-h an arc of 5.5, showing- nearly 2 of converg-ence for 1 D of accommodation as the normal. This holds almost true when the accommodation is 1 D, the point of fixation be- 280 ESOPHORIA. ing at a distance of 1 M. The base-line being 2i inches, the angle of convergence will be 3:6, half of which (1.8) will show the converging of each axis toward the extended median plane of the head. To show how nearly the proportion holds good, the following is given: 3 D: 5.5 : : 1 D: 1.8. Thus it would appear thai 1 D of hyperopia would give 1.8 of pseudo-esophoria, the proportion practically holding good up to 6 D, there being a variation of only .1. In 6 D of hyperopia the pseudo-esophoria for each dioptre would be 1.9. A pseudo-esophoria may be chargeable, in some unac- countable way, to a complicating plus cyclophoria. This surmise becomes stronger since it is well known that an esotropia occasionally has for one of its causative factors a plus cyclophoria, which must be corrected to make it possible for the esotropia to be cured. There must be, however, an intrinsic esophoria, which is only aggravated by the cyclophoria. In many cases of esophoria the distant test shows a greater error than the test in the near, and in some cases an esophoria is shown in the distant test and an exo- phoria in the near. Occasionally the esophoria shown in the near test is from 1 to 10 greater than that shown in the far test. This variation will not show itself when the esophoria is wholly intrinsic. If there is emmetropia, the esophoria in the distant test is intrinsic. ESOPHORIA. 281 and the same quantity of the error will be shown in the near test, if the ciliary muscles are ideal in structure and size and ready to g-ive full response to the normal stimulus sent to them from the brain-center that controls their action, when accommodating- for the near point. If the point to be fixed is 16 inches from the eyes, the impulse sent from the brain to the ciliary muscles will be a 2-50 D impulse and the muscles will respond so as to increase the refraction of the lenses 2.50 D. The associated impulse sent to the interni, the distance be- tween the eyes being- 2 2 inches, would be enough to make the two visual axes swing- toward each other 4.5, so that the ang-le of convergence would be 9 (a small fraction less). If the diplopia test in the distance shows 6 of esophoria, the near test will show the same, for nothing exists to break the evenness of the error. In another case of emmetropia, suppose the ciliary muscles to be subnormal in their development, so that more than a 2.50 D impulse must be sent from the brain-center in order to make them respond sufficiently to increase the refraction of the lenses 2.50 D. For the sake of arg-ument, let a nerve impulse of 5 D be neces- sary to elicit a 2.50 D response on the part of the ciliary muscles; the associated impulse sent to each internus would be double that of the normal (4.5 X 2 = 9) in the effort to accommodate at 16 inches. The distant 282 ESOPHORIA. test showing- 6 of esophoria, the near test would show 10.5 of esophoria (6 intrinsic esophoria + 4.5 pseudo- esophoria = 10.5). In another case of emmetropia, suppose the ciliary muscles to be hyper-developed, so that only a 1.25 D nerve impulse is necessary to elicit a 2.50 D response by the ciliary muscles; the associated impulse sent to the interni would be just half the normal (4.50 -4- 2 = 2.25) when accommodating for a point at 16 inches. The deficiency of associated impulse must be supplied by a corresponding 1 amount of the inherent tension of the interni, diminishing 1 the esophoria in the near test just to that extent. To the convergence impulse (2.25) must be added 2.25 of the esophoria in order to have the visual axes form an angle of convergence of 9. Then, the distance test showing 6 of esophoria, the near test would show 3.75 of esophoria (6 -2. 25 = 3.75). In still another case of emmetropia, there may be an intrinsic esophoria of 2 in the distance. The ciliary muscles may be such as to require only a 1 D impulse to call forth 2.50 D of activity on the part of hyper-devel- oped ciliary muscles, in accommodating for 16 inches. The associated impulse sent to the interni would be only 1.8, when an impulse of 4.5 is required for convergence at 16 inches. When the whole of the 2 of intrinsic ten- sion of the interni is used in aiding convergence, the ESOPHORIA. 233 guiding- sensation must make a special call on the third innervation center for .7 more of convergence on the part of each internus in order that the proper angle of convergence may be formed (associated impulse 1.8 -f 2 intrinsic esophoria -f .7 extra impulse from the third conjugate center = 4.5). The diplopia test in the near withdraws the extra impulse from the third conjugate center and exophoria of .7 is shown. It is not the response of the ciliary muscles in diop- tre changes of the lenses that causes a definite asso- ciated contraction of the interni, but it is the greater or smaller impulse, measured in dioptres, generated by the brain - center controlling the ciliary muscles, that develops the associated action of the interni; for every 1 D of ciliary impulse there is 1.8 of convergence impulse. Experiments and observation do not show that an associated nerve impulse is sent to the ciliary muscles from the center controlling them because of an over- stimulation of the third conjugate innervation center. This would be shown in spasm of accommodation pseudo-myopia a condition never seen in cases of eso- phoria, and rarely seen at all, even in a case of exo- phoria. The condition so often spoken of as spasm of accommodation is not such, but is the temporary con- tinuance of the manifestation of an acquired tonicity of 284 ESOPHORIA. the ciliary muscles, when one begins the wearing 1 of con- vex lenses for the correction of hyperopia. Whatever may be true of other associated brain-cen- ters, it appears that the center of the ciliary muscles and the third conjugate innervation center can have the associated impulse run in only one direction; that is, from the former to the latter. TESTS FOR ESOPHORIA. No test for esophoria can be relied on when the eyes are under the influence of a mydriatic. Within an hour, and it may be for a much longer time, after the instilla- tion of a mydriatic, the third conjugate innervation cen- ter is excessively stimulated, either directly or indirectly, more likely the latter, so that an esophoria will be shown when there is none; existing esophoria will be increased more or less; and an exophoria will be made to appear less than it really is. All of these statements are certainly true, in both the far and the near tests, if the patient is a hyperope; they are true in the near, if not in the far, if the patient is an emmetrope; they are also true in the near, if not in the far, if the patient is a myope of less than 2.50 or 3 D. The explanation for this phenomenon, given by the author in 1892, he still believes to be true. This ex- planation is as follows: ESOPHORIA. 285 A very peculiar feature of the use of a mydriatic is that at first probably from one to several, hours a mydriatic, in hypermetropic eyes, will increase the eso- phoria, will lessen an exophoria or convert it into ortho- phoria, or even into an esophoria. Until now this has been unexplained. The following- explanation must be true: The mydriatic acts on either the ending's of the accommodative nerve fibers or on the fibers of the muscle of accommodation, certainly not on the accommo- dative center, which, therefore, must remain in a state susceptible of excitation by the demands from the guid- ing- sensation. As the muscles of accommodation pass into their forced rest, the retinal images become less sharp in outline, the blurring increasing up to the point of full suspension of accommodation. The guiding sen- sation calls on the accommodative center for sharper images, and the impulse is sent out, but finds the mus- cles unresponsive; the call is repeated more eagerly, and a stronger impulse is sent to the sleeping muscles, and still no change is effected in the images; and thus the calls and the responses are kept up for a longer or a shorter time. For every degree of activity thus excited in the accommodative center, there is a corresponding tendency to activity generated in the converging center. So long as the calls are made on, and responses are made by, the accommodative center, the center of convergence 286 ESOPHORIA. stands ready to call into unusual action the interni, which they do the moment the guiding- sensation is robbed of its restraining 1 power by the test for hetero- phoria, when an increased pseudo-esophoria is shown. But finally the guiding sensation ceases its calls, or, from exhaustion, the accommodative center ceases to re- spond, and now the normal muscular condition is again shown, and will remain manifest, although the mydriatic may be continued. From this observation on the myd- riatic as a disturber of the salutary relationship of the centers of accommodation and convergence, we deduce the following- conclusion: All tests for lateral hetero- phoria are wholly unreliable within the first few hours after eyes have been brought under the influence of a mydriatic. TESTS. The exclusion test will always show an outward re- setting- whenever the card is removed. The resetting may be so slig-ht as not to be detected objectively; but the patient will always be conscious of the apparent movement of the test object toward the opposite side, however slight may be the esophoria. The red-glass test, by taking the eye slightly off its guard because of the change in the color of the image, will result in diplopia in many cases, the red light ap- ESOPHORIA. 287 pearing- on the side corresponding- to the eye before which the red glass is held homonymous diplopia. It will be distant from the true candle, more or less, depending on the quantity of the error. The diplopia developed by the red glass practically always indicates that an operation should be done, but it does not determine the muscle to be operated upon nor the kind of operation to be done. This can be shown only by the duction and version tests of both the interni and the externi. The double prism, held with the 'line of union of the bases horizontal, shows the middle, or true, object dis- placed toward the corresponding side; or, if the true object be fixed, the upper and lower false object will ap- pear on the side corresponding to the position of the dou- ble prism. A prism from the refraction case that places the false and true objects in a vertical line, measures the error, but does not indicate the method of procedure to be adopted for effecting a cure. The duction and ver- sion powers must be taken. If the single prism be correctly held before the eye- that is, the axis vertical and the base up the resulting diplopia will be homonymous; and the extent of the devi- ation can be measured by prisms, as when the Maddox double prism is made the means of producing the diplo- pia. The double prism has the advantage over the sin- gle prism, whether they are placed in a trial frame or held 288 ESOPHORIA. in the hand, in that the examiner can always be certain, with the double prism, that its axis is vertical, when the two resulting- images are made to appear the one di- rectly over the other. With the single prism there is no such means of knowing the exact position of the axis, hence the chance for the creeping in of an error that, on the one hand, may show more esophoria, while, on the other hand, it may show less, than really exists. The ease with which this source of error might be eliminated by the double prism was really the thought that led Maddox to invent it. Of the two means for developing diplopia by prisms, independent of the phorometer armed with the spirit level, the double prism is by far the better and more reliable. The Maddox rod was invented because of a defect in the double prism as first made, and as made even now by some manufacturers. The grinding of both prisms on one piece of glass left a somewhat rounded line of union of the bases, so that not only would the candle blaze be doubled when the base-line passed across the pupil, but a streak of light, formed by the refraction of the rays passing through the rounded line of union, ex- tends more or less completely from the one false light to the other. The streak served a good purpose, in that it led to the invention of the indispensable rod; but since it can no longer serve a good purpose, it should ESOPHORIA. 239 be eliminated by uniting- two separate prisms, base to base. While there is but one certain, therefore legitimate, use for the Maddox rod viz., in testing 1 the oblique muscles it is, nevertheless, frequently used in testing the recti. When the rod is horizontal before the rig-ht eye, the vertical streak of light is seen to the rig-ht of the candle UEFT RIGHT Fig. 42. RETINAL FUSION AREAS. in every case of esophoria. The prism, base out, that causes the streak to pass down through the light meas- ures, but not accurately, the quantity of the esophoria. The want of accuracy is due to the fact that the vertical retinal meridian is not turned so far out, by contraction of the internus, as to throw the streak entirely outside 290 ESOPHORIA. the retinal area in which resides the guiding- sensation; therefore there would be some effort made at fusing the part of the much-changed image with the true and unchanged image in the other eye. Fig. 42 shows this better than words can possibly portray. The figure also shows how a displacing prism, with base up, would throw the unchanged image of the candle blaze above and entirely beyond the fusion area, so that no effort would be made by the eye to disturb the position of equilibrium into which it has turned. It will be seen also that the rotary prism would carry the displaced image to the vertical meridian without having it infringe anywhere on the fusion area. The dotted line from e represents the line of travel of the image while the measurement of the error is being taken with the rotary prism. At no time would a fusion impulse be excited. Not so with the streak of light which crosses the fusion area. The nearer this is carried by a rotary prism, or a simple prism, base out, to the vertical retinal meridian, the greater would be the demand made by the guiding sensation on the center controlling the external rectus. For this reason the rod test will give variable results from time to time, and will always show less esophoria than the patient really has. The same is true even to a greater extent when the rod is used for testing for exo- phoria and for hyperphoria and cataphoria. There is ESOPHORIA. 291 only one means for testing- for esophoria that is less reli- able than the rod, and that is the strong- plus lens sug- gested by Stevens. From every standpoint the phorometer constitutes the most desirable means for detecting- esophoria and meas- uring- it. For reasons g-iven in Chapter II., the mo- nocular phorometer is the best. The displacing- prism should always be placed, base up, in the cell toward the patient's eye; the thumbscrew for the rotary prism should be in the horizontal; the index of the rotary prism should stand at zero; and the spirit level should be exactly reg-ulated. The upper, or true, object should be fixed. The six-degree displacing- prism will throw the false image above and entirely beyond the retinal area of binocular fusion, taking the guiding sensation of that eye entirely off its guard, so that the eye will at once be turned in. The lower, or false, object will be proportionately displaced from the vertical toward the corresponding side- homonymous diplopia. Revolving the rotary prism so that its index moves in the nasal arc, the false object is brought farther and farther toward the vertical line passing down from the true object, un- til at last the patient observes that the false object is di- rectly under the true object. The point at which the index stops tells the degree of the error. The same re- sult, practically, will be shown by any number of tests 292 ESOPHORIA. on the same day or on consecutive days, if the patient is always careful to "fix" the true object. This point is absolutely essential to the greatest accuracy; and if strictly observed, there is no other factor to bring 1 in an error. Certainly at no time will the false imag'e be within the retinal area of binocular fusion. The one eye tested, the instrument should be reversed so as to test the other eye, for in this way only can it be determined if there is more esophoria in the one eye than in the other. To know how to proceed in the treatment of any grven case, this question must be answered: Is it pseudo-esophoria? This is answered by a study of the refraction under a mydriatic a little later. If there is no hyperopia or hyperopic astigrnatism, the answer is: "No." If there is hyperopia or hyperopic astig-matism, the answer is, "Yes, at least in part," which part can be easily calculated, for it would be 1.8, or nearly 2, for every dioptre of the hyperopia and as much for every 2 D of hyperopic astigfmatism. The quantity of the pseudo-esophoria thus determined, subtracted from the full error, as shown by the phorometer, gfives the amount of the true, or intrinsic, esophoria. But before the mydriatic is used the duction and ver- sion power of both interni and both externi should be taken in order that the following* two questions may ESOPHORIA. 203 be answered: Is the intrinsic esophoria sthenic? Is it asthenic ? These three questions answered, the method of pro- cedure becomes plain, as soon as complications have been found or eliminated. COMPLICATIONS OF ESOPHORIA. These are hypero- pia and hyperopic astigmatism, hyperphoria and cata- phoria, and plus and minus cyclophoria. The existence or non-existence of one or more of these complications must be known before it becomes possible to resort to the correct treatment of esophoria. It has already been shown how hyperopia and hyperopic astigmatism develop a pseudo-esophoria, which can be cured by proper lenses. A myopia and myopic astigmatism sometimes compli- cate an esophoria, and when they do, their correction in- creases the esophoria in the near, but not in the far. A hyperphoria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other will increase an esophoria, as will also a double hyperphoria and a double cataphoria. How to deal with these complications in the treatment of esophoria will be shown under the head " Treatment." As already stated, it is difficult to see how a plus or a minus cyclophoria can add to an esophoria, since the oblique muscles are abductors; but it can be seen readily how an esophoria might be lessened by a cyclophoria. It is possible, however, that a cyclophoria may excite 294 ESOPHORIA. the third conjugate innervation center in some way, so as to develop a spasm of the interni, such as is excited in cases of hyperopia; but there is not that definite rela- tionship between the obliques and the interni that there is between the ciliary muscles and the interni. Never- theless, when cyclophoria complicates an esophoria, the treatment of the esophoria must include the treatment of the cyclophoria if the best and quickest results are to follow; in fact, it is practically impossible to cure an esophoria while the cyclophoria remains uncorrected. For the methods of detecting- and measuring- hyper- phoria and cataphoria, and plus and minus cyclophoria, as complications of esophoria, the reader is referred to Chapter VI. and Chapter VII. SYMPTOMS OF ESOPHORIA. These are any one or several of those mentioned as resulting- from heterophoria, and the reader is referred to that part of Chapter III. treating- of symptoms; but not all the symptoms resulting- from abnormal nervous tension of the ocular muscles are mentioned in that chapter. It would be impossible to make a com- plete list. There is not a single brain-center controll- ing- any one organ or part of the body that may not be dis- turbed, in sympathy with the centers that control the ocular muscles; and,.wc ^> in which C is the centimeters of decentering; P, the desired prismatic effect; and D, the number of dioptres in the lens. Let P = lij and D = 3, then the formula w^ould be: C = 1H ^ 13 ^ = .875 cm., or 8.75 mm., which is about ^ of an inch of decentering. Jackson teaches that the following formula is practi- cally correct : C = ^, in which C is the mm. of decen- tering required; P, the prismatic effect desired; D, the number of dioptres in the lens to be decentered; while 10 is the mm. of decentering of a 1 D lens for 1 (1 centrad) of effect. Substituting figures for letters, as in the SSOPHORIA. 303 Maddox formula, we have : C = ULf == 5 mm>> or a b out B of an inch of decentering, as compared with Maddox's J- of an inch of decentering. Thorington and May agree in taking 8.7 mm. for the extent of decentering a 1 D lens in order to procure 1 of prismatic effect. For obtaining the amount of de- centration of any lens, this would be their formula: C = P$r?. Substituting figures for letters, as in the other formulas, we have: C == l%->pl == 4.35 mm . ? or about i of an inch of decentration, as compared with Maddox's | of an inch and Jackson's of an inch ; or, comparing in mm.: Maddox, 8.7; Jackson, 5; Thoring- ton, 4.35; May, 4.35. By a little experimentation the reader may satisfy himself that Jackson is practically correct. The extent of decentration advised by Maddox is entirely too much nearly double what it ought to be. It may be that Maddox's estimate was for 1 of arc, and not for 1 of prism, or 1 centrad. One advantage that decentering a lens has over the grinding of a lens on a prism is that the former is cheaper ; another advantage is that the wearing of a decentered lens is not attended by the reflected image of any bright object, which is always toward the refract- ing angle and in the line of the axis of the prism, unless the prism is ground on both surfaces. Chromatic aber- ration is no more, nor is it anv less, with a decentered 304 ESOPHORIA. lens than it is with a lens ground on a prism. The great objection to decentered lenses is that the spherical aberration must interfere to some extent \vith the sharp- ness of retinal images; this, however, is but little when the decentering is 6 mm. or less, and rarely is it more. In very strong lenses, say 6 D, the decentering would be only 5 mm. for 3 of effect. The greatest objection to "relieving prisms" and to decentered lenses is that they favor muscles in their weakness. They lessen nervous tension, but not by increasing the inherent power of weak muscles. It is far better practice to increase the inherent power of weak muscles, either by exercising them or by shorten- ing or advancing them, or to increase the relative power of the weaker muscles by lessening the tension of their stronger antagonists by partial tenotomies. If patients will not resort to exercise and decline to submit to operations, they should be given the benefit which is to be derived from prisms in positions of rest or from de- centered lenses. EXERCISE TREATMENT FOR ESOPHORIA. Uncomplicated intrinsic esophoria of low degree may be cured by proper exercise of the externi. The only means applicable are prisms. The same gymnastic principles apply to the externi as to the other muscles of the body. ESOPHORIA. 305 Light work, not continued too long, and rhythmic in character, is exercise that increases muscular power. Prisms of from 1 to 4, bases in, can be used for devel- oping- the externi. Beginning- with the weaker prisms and advancing to the next stronger at intervals of a week or ten days, the strongest to be used are soon reached. The exercise should be resorted to at the same time every day, so as to easily form the habit of exer- cising, and should be continued not longer than ten min- utes at a time. One exercise daily or, at most, two ex- ercises daily will be sufficient. I/enses correcting focal errors should be worn at the time of exercising. The exercise prisms in spectacle frames should be lowered and raised alternately every three seconds throughout the sitting. The object looked at may be anything that can be seen distinctly, and it should be distant from the observer not more than twenty feet nor less than ten feet. Looking at the object through the prisms, the externi are made to contract; raising the prisms, these muscles at once relax. Thus contraction and relaxation, rhythmic in character, are continued throughout the sit- ting. Exercise that fatigues does not build, hence the necessity of always stopping short of fatigue. A muscle develops as the result of exercise in proportion to the abundance of blood that can be brought into it. Abund- ance of blood supply means quick results; scanty blood 306 ESOPHORIA. supply, because of smallness of the vessels supplying the muscle, means slow results. These conditions can- not be known beforehand; therefore it is better to tell the patient that the treatment will have to be continued for months. Exercise of the externi that are attached, in greater part, below the horizontal plane, will exercise, at the same time, the inferior obliques, which would be good in a case of minus cyclophoria, but bad in a case of plus cyclophoria. If the externi have the ideal attachment that is, half above and half below the transverse plane of the eye exercise will always be well borne and will do good. If attached too high, the exercise of the externi will be associated with exercise of the superior obliques and will usually be well borne and ought to do good. These things can be known only as a result of exercise. The object in view in exercising the externi is to so increase their inherent power that, under a normal im- pulse, they will be able to do a normal amount of work; that their tension may be inherent, not nervous, and sufficient to balance the inherent tension of the interni. OPERATIONS. No operation for pseudo-esophoria should ever be done. The kind depending on hyperopia should be treated by a full correction of the focal error; the kind due to subnor ESOPHORIA. 307 mal development of the ciliary muscles, making it neces- sary that the center controlling them shall generate an extraordinary nerve impulse, should be treated by gym- nastic exercise of these muscles. There can be drawn no unvarying line between opera- tive and non-operative cases of intrinsic esophoria. If, under the red-glass test, there is homonymous diplopia, the case is almost certainly one to be operated upon; but the kind of operation is not shown by this test. If the esophoria at 16 inches is more than half the angle of con- vergence (nearly 9) for that distance, the question of operation should be considered; and if it is much in excess of 5 in the near, there is no reason why an operation should be delayed. Whatever the quantity of esophoria in the distance, which is usually a little greater than that in the near, it should not be relied on exclusively when de- termining on an operation. Both the near and the far tests should always be noted. The quantity of error be- ing sufficiently great to indicate an operation, the next thing to consider is the question as to whether the eso- phoria is sthenic or asthenic. More than 25 of adduc- tion and more than 50 of adversion would indicate a par- tial tenotomy of one or both interni; there is nothing in esophoria that can justify a complete tenotomy. If the adduction is 25 or less and the adversion is 50 or less, no operation should be done on the interni, but one or 308 ESOPHORIA. both extern! should be shortened. If in doubt as to which of the two operations should be done, it would be safer to choose the shortening- of an externus. Either operation would cure wholly or in part the imbalance the partial tenotomy, by lessening 1 the tension of the in- ternus; the shortening-, by increasing the tension of the externus. In properly selected cases either operation would establish a sthenic orthophoria; in unfortunately selected cases (those with normal or subnormal adduc- tion) the tenotomy would establish an asthenic orthopho- ria, a danger never attending the operation of shortening. Abduction in an operable case of esophoria is a safe guide in determining whether the operation should be done to lessen the tension of an internus or to increase the tension of an externus. If the abduction is above 5 or 6 and the abversion is but little below 50, the inter- nus should be cut; if abduction is below 5 and abver- sion is correspondingly low, the externus should be shortened. In uncomplicated cases of esophoria, the error about equal in the two eyes, the operative effect should be di- vided between them. In such cases the plane of rotation must not be altered, hence the partial tenotomy must be central and the shortening must be straight forward. When esophoria is complicated with only hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, the operations on ESOPHORIA. 309 the lateral muscles should be done as if there were no complications that is, the tension of the muscles should be altered, but their planes should not be changed. * o Later the hyperphoria should be treated either by a per- manent prism for the hyperphoric eye, by prismatic exer- cise of the weaker muscle of each eye, or by a central partial tenotomy of the superior rectus of the hyper- phoric eye. When esophoria is complicated by cyclophoria alone or by cyclophoria and hyperphoria, the operation done must not only alter the tension of the muscle, but must also change its plane of rotation. If the complication is plus cyclophoria alone and the operation is to lessen the tension of the internus, it should be so done as to ele- vate its plane of rotation. Th : s is accomplished by a lower marginal tenotomy, leaving uncut the fibers at the upper margin. A threefold effect attends this oper- ation: (a) the tension is lessened, (b) the cyclophoria is counteracted, (c) a hyperphoria is created. For the rea- son that an operation on only one internus would give a hyperphoria to the corresponding eye, the operative ef- fect should be divided between the two eyes, the opera- tion on the one internus being as nearly as possible like the operation on the other. The two operations should cure the esophoria and the plus cyclophoria; but at the same time a double hyperphoria would be created, a con- 310 ESOPHORIA. dition far less objectionable than the two errors for the cure of which the operations were done. If the sthenic esophoria is complicated by plus cyclo- phoria, with hyperphoria and cataphoria, the operative effect should be confined, if possible, to the internus of the cataphoric eye and should consist of a division of the lower and central fibers, leaving 1 uncut enough of the upper fibers to prevent an over-effect. The result of this operation would be threefold: (a) lessening- or cur- ing- the esophoria; (b) counteracting- the plus cyclophoria; (c) converting- the cataphoria into a hyperphoria, thus giving- the patient a double hyperphoria. If some of the esophoria should remain, it should be relieved by a cen- tral partial tenotomy of the internus of the other eye. A marginal tenotomy of this muscle should not be done, even if some of the plus cyclophoria and hyperphoria remained, for the reason that dividing- its lower fibers would in- crease the hyperphoria while curing the cyclophoria. How to deal with any remaining- cyclophoria and hyper- phoria will be shown in the discussion of those conditions. If the esophoria is asthenic and uncomplicated, one of the externi, if not both, should be shortened so as to in- crease its tension without changing its plane of action. If complicated by a hyperphoria of one eye and a cat- aphoria of the other, there being- no cyclophoria, the shortening of the externi should be done as if no compli- ESOPHORIA. 311 cation existed; that is, the tension should be increased, but the plane should not be changed. If complicated by a plus cyclophoria only, both externi should be shortened to the same extent and the plane of each should be low- ered sufficiently to effect a correction of the cyclophoria. The alteration of the tension would cure the esophoria. If the complications are plus cyclophoria and a hyper- phoria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other, the oper- ative effect, if possible, should be limited to the hyper- phoric eye, and should be accomplished by a shortening of the externus so as both to alter its tension and to de- press its plane of action. The triple effect would be: (a) increased tension for the esophoria; (b) lowered plane for the plus cyclophoria; (c) lowered plane for the hyper- phoria, converting it into a cataphoria, so that there would be a double cataphoria. If any remaining esopho- ria should require a shortening of the externus of the other eye, the operation should be done so as to increase its tension without changing its plane, even if there should also remain, from the first operation, some of the plus cyclophoria and some of the cataphoria; for a change of the plane of action that would lessen one of these complications would increase the other. The complication of minus cyclophoria has only been mentioned, for the reason that it is so rare. When it does exist in connection with esophoria, every step for 312 ESOPHORIA. changing- the muscle plane, as set forth in the treatment of a plus cyclophoria, must be reversed. The operation of shortening- an externus, when enough increase of tension can be had, should always be pre- ferred to an advancement. While this can be done in nearly all cases of asthenic esophoria, in which an in* crease of tension of the externi is always indicated, nevertheless, in some cases these muscles have their at- tachment so far removed from the corneo-scleral junc- tion that the advancement operation becomes clearly in- dicated. The same rules as to alteration of tension and chang-e of plane apply to advancements as have been set forth in connection with shortening's. For the technic of these operations, and the after-treatment, the reader is referred to that part of Chapter III. in which these operations are described. While doing 1 these operations the judg-ment of the operator must decide as to their extent. His judgment cannot be g^ood unless he keeps in mind the exact nature of the conditions for the relief of which he is operating". The true essence of these conditions cannot be known except as a result of the most skillful and careful use of the phorometer, cyclo-phorometer, and tropometer or perimeter. Before any operation is done, the refraction of the eyes should be determined, under a mydriatic, by means of the standard objective and subjective tests. ESOPHORIA. 313 The operator should always be careful not to do too much; for it is far better to leave the patient with some of his esophoria than it is to give him the smallest quan- tity of the opposite error exophoria. The danger of re- sorting to tests while operating is that it may lead to the doing of too much. Tests while operating cannot be reliable, and should, therefore, be avoided. When more than one operation is needed, it is better to allow from two to four weeks to intervene; but in errors of high degree, two operations, a partial tenotomy of an in- ternus and a shortening of the opposing externus, or a partial tenotomy of both interni, or a shortening of both externi, may be done at the same time. The object of partial tenotomies of the interni, in the treatment of esophoria, is to so reduce their tension that the externi, under a normal impulse, may perfectly balance them in action. The object of shortening the externi is to so increase their inherent tension that, under a normal impulse, they may perfectly balance the interni. In either case the nervous tension of the ex- terni would be relieved. Whenever an intrinsic esophoria has been reduced by operations so that the remainder can be cured by non- operative means, these should be resorted to, but not until the muscles operated upon have had time to com- pletely recover from the operations. CHAPTER V. EXOPHORIA. As the word indicates, there is, in exophoria, a ten- dency on the part of the external recti muscles and their synergists to make the visual axes deviate from the point of fixation. If this tendency were not counteracted by antagonists of the externi, the visual axes would either intersect beyond the point of fixation, or they would become parallel or even divergent. Abnormal nervous tension of the interni and their synergists counteracts the inherent tension of the externi and their synergists, so that the tendency is not allowed to become a turning. The visual axes are thus forced to intersect at the point of fixation; and binocular single vision is maintained, but at the expenditure of an undue amount of nerve force. Exophoria, like esophoria, is of two kinds, intrinsic and pseudo. As to causation, the one kind is wholly different from the other, but the two often coexist. As to the results, the one is the same in kind as the other, but the treatment of the one is not at all similar to the treat- ment of the other. (314) EXOPHORIA. 315 INTRINSIC EXOPHORIA. In this the extern! have the advantage over the interni. This imbalance may be due to the fact that the externi are hyper-developed or that the interni are of subnormal development. It may be that the error is not in the size of the muscles, but in the nature of their attachment to the globe, the externi hav- ing their attachment nearer the corneo-scleral junction than normal, or that the interni are attached too far back; it may be that the externi are short and tense or that the interni are long and somewhat lax. When either of these conditions causes exophoria, the error may be greater in the one eye than in the other, though, as a rule, the exophoria is about equal in the two eyes. When there is a difference, the monocular phorometer quickly shows it. It must be conceded that the cause of intrinsic exopho- ria may not reside in the muscles themselves, but may be found in an unequal supply of nerve force, the centers for the externi generating a quantity of nerve impulse greater than normal, or the third conjugate brain-center, of subnormal development, sending a weaker current to the interni. An intrinsic exophoria can exist without there being a state of imbalance between the externi and the interni. The oblique muscles are always more or less powerful as abvertors. There is but little room for doubt that, in 316 EXOPHORIA. some cases, they may be too short and tense, or they may be too large and powerful, or their attachments may be nearer the posterior pole than normal, so that, in either case, their abverting power would be increased. This increase may be sufficiently great to cause an exophoria, even when there is no cyclophoria. Malformation of the orbits, only in the sense of their being too far apart, can cause an exophoria; but when this cause exists alone, the muscle imbalance cannot be great. The superior and inferior recti may have their attach- ments so far toward the temples as to greatly lessen their power to help the interni, and thus become a factor in the production of exophoria. Whether the one or the other of the several conditions named is the cause of exophoria, or whether two or more of them become factors in the production of this error, the treatment, whether surgical or non-surgical, must be directed toward the lateral muscles. As to surgical means, either the tension of the externi must be lessened or the tension of the interni must be increased. If brain- centers are structurally over-developed or under-devel- oped, they must remain so always; if the obliques, be- cause of structure, attachment, or innervation, abvert too powerfully, they cannot be changed; if the orbits are too wide apart, surgery cannot bring them closer to- EXOPHORIA. 317 gether. If the superior and inferior recti, because of faulty attachments, are feeble advertors, they must not be subjected to operations on this account. For these reasons it becomes apparent that any and every treat- ment of intrinsic exophoria, whatever may be the cause, must be directed toward the externi or toward the in- terni. An exophoria that is wholly muscular, all inner- vation centers being normal, will show the same num- ber of degrees in the near as in the far. Intrinsic exophoria is of two kinds, sthenic and asthen- ic. The quantity of the error does not determine its char- acter. Only the abduction and the abversion tests, in any given case, can tell the operator that the error is sthenic or that it is asthenic. Exophoria with abduction of less than 8 and abversion of less than 50 is asthenic, and clearly indicates that the externi should not have their tension lessened, and just as clearly indicates that the interni must have their tension increased. Exophoria with abduction of more than 8 and abversion of more than 50 is sthenic, and the case should be treated with the view of lessening the tension of the externi. Tests in myopic and emmetropic cases will always show the full amount of intrinsic exophoria in the far. In the near test of a myope, the intrinsic exophoria will have the associated pseudo-exophoria added to it. If the emmetrope does not show the same exophoria in the near 318 EXOPHORIA. as in the far, it is increased or diminished because of an abnormal development of the ciliary muscles. If the ciliary muscles are hyper-developed, there will be more exophoria in the near than in the far, for the reason that an impulse less powerful than normal is required of the brain-center controlling 1 them, so that a correspondingly slight associated impulse is sent to the interni. 'If the ciliary muscles are subnormally developed, they will re- quire an impulse more powerful than the normal, and a correspondingly strong associated impulse must be sent to the interni, causing a pseudo-esophoria, which, to a certain extent, would neutralize the intrinsic exophoria. The hyperope will always show less than the full amount of intrinsic exophoria in both the far and the near tests, for the reason that hyperopia always has a pseudo- esophoria associated with it, which neutralizes in part an existing intrinsic exophoria. PSEUDO-EXOPHORIA. There can be but two causes for this condition. One is myopia, or myopic astigma- tism; the other is hyper-development of the ciliary mus- cles, making it necessary for the centers controlling them to generate a less powerful nerve current than would be required by these muscles if normally developed. When the cause is myopia, or myopic astigmatism, the pseudo-exophoria shows itself only in the near, and is due to the fact that the guiding sensation calls either for EXOPHORIA. 319 no nerve force to excite ciliary action or for a quantity less than is required by an emmetrope, depending- on the amount of the focal error; and a correspondingly slight associated impulse is sent to the interni. If the point of view is 16 inches distant, there should be 1.8 of pseudo-exophoria for each dioptre of myopia up to 2.50 D, and .9 for each dioptre of myopic astigmatism up to 5 D. This kind of pseudo-exophoria does one of three things: (a) it increases an intrinsic exophoria in the near, (b) it shows an exophoria in the near when there is real orthophoria, or (c) it lessens an intrinsic esophoria in the near. When the pseudo-exophoria is due to a hyper-develop- ment of the ciliary muscles and the patient is an emme- trope, the error will show itself only in the near test. If a 1.50 D impulse is all that is necessary to effect a 3 D contraction of the ciliary muscles, the pseudo-exopho- ria, with the test object at 13 inches, should be 2.7. This may manifest itself in the same three ways as that caused by myopia. Strictly speaking, a pseudo-exopho- ria cannot exist in a hyperope; although, as shown in the chapter on esophoria, the hyperope who has hyper-devel- oped ciliary muscles will show a less amount of pseudo- esophoria than would be shown if these muscles were of normal development. The difference in amount is equiv- alent to pseudo-exophoria. 320 EXOPHORIA. When the far test shows orthophoria and the near test shows exophoria, the error is pseudo in character, and is dependent on one or other of the two causes above men- tioned; and the same is true when an exophoria is less in the far than it is in the near. The same explanation applies when there is esophoria in the far and exophoria in the near. If in an emmetrope there is more exophoria in the far than there is in the near, the ciliary muscles are subnormally developed, and require an excessive im- pulse to make them perform their work. The associated impulse to the interni is correspondingly great. There is a form of exophoria not yet referred to in this chapter, and probably not fully set forth in any book. The cause unquestionably resides in the third conju- gate innervation center, and is structural in character; in other words, the third conjugate innervation center is subnormally developed, and, for this reason, sends a feeble impulse to the interni. The most exaggerated manifes- tation of this condition would lead one to judge that this brain-center is entirely absent, for occasionally a case is seen who has no power of convergence. Such a person enjoys binocular vision in the distance, but has only mo- nocular vision in the near. In such a case there is no ad- duction power, for the one visual axis cannot be made to approach the other. Abduction will be normal or even above the normal. Adversion is unimpaired, showing EXOPHORIA. 321 that the fourth and fifth conjugate innervations have full sway. The abversion of the right eye equals the adver- sion of the left eye, and, vice versa, the abversion of the left eye equals the adversion of the right eye. In these movements the visual axes are kept parallel, as when the eyes are looking straight ahead. That the condition is congenital in most cases is shown by the fact that there is no diplopia in the near. This must be due to an acquired mental suppression of images that fall on the temporal half of the retina, or, at least, a portion of it. The power of mental suppression can be acquired only in the earliest years of life. Hansell & Reber speak of a patient in whom this loss of convergence power was acquired, the result of some disease process in the part of the brain in which is located the con- vergence center. It is reasonable to suppose that this center might be destroyed by disease. In such a case, however, diplopia would exist everywhere except in the distance. If the third innervation center can be entirely absent in one person and be present and fully developed in another, it is reasonable to conclude that in still another it may be present, but in a state of subnormal development. There may be as many different grades of development of this as there are individuals; but in the majority of persons this center is able, doubtless, 322 EXOPHORIA. to generate 1 of impulse for every 1 of convergence, in association with the center of the ciliary muscles. Subnormal development of this center must manifest itself in an exophoria in the near many degrees in excess of the exophoria in the far (of itself it can never cause exophoria in the far, but it may be associated with some of those conditions that cause intrinsic exophoria); or, if there is orthophoria or even slight esophoria in the far, there will be considerable exophoria in the near. The two ordinary causes of pseudo-exophoria that is, myo- pia and hyper-developed ciliary muscles will not cause more than 5 of the error. A greater degree of varia- tion between the far and the near tests than this, in the exophoric direction, must be attributed to a subnormally developed third conjugate brain-center. A diagnostic feature of this condition is the manifestation of very low abduction power much lower than is found in intrinsic exophoria of the same degree. It is barely possible that a failure of connection be- tween the ciliary center and the convergence center accounts for the absence of convergence power in some cases; and a slight connection may account for a feeble convergence. TESTS. The cover test, allowing the eye to turn toward the temple, will be attended by a resetting of the eye to- EXOPHORIA. 323 ward the nose when the cover is removed, and the false object will move rapidly toward the corresponding- side until fused with the true object. The examiner can often see the resetting of the eye, but not so readily as an intelligent patient can detect the apparent movement of the test object. The red glass, in the higher grades of exophoria, will develop crossed diplopia. The distance between the red light and the true light will give a fair idea of the quan- tity of the error. This test, resulting in crossed diplo- pia, practically always indicates operative treatment; but since it does not show whether the case is sthenic or asthenic, it cannot indicate the character of the opera- tion to be done. The double prism held before the right eye so that the two lights seen through it shall be in the same vertical line, the light seen by the left eye will be to the right, if there is exophoria. The extent of the error is shown by that prism, base toward the nose, that will place the middle light in line with the other two. This test, so far as it goes, is safe and accurate; but it cannot show whether the exophoria is sthenic or asthenic, and cannot, therefore, be relied upon in answering the question: "What operation, if any, shall be done?" The single six-degree prism, held base up before the right eye, with the axis perfectly vertical, is as reliable 324 EXOPHORIA. as the double prism, though one can never be so certain that the axis is vertical as he can be when using- the dou- ble prism. The lower, or false, light will be on the op- posite side crossed diplopia. The prism, base in, that brings it directly under the true candle, measures the amount of the exophoria. Like the double-prism test, this one does not show whether the exophoria is sthenic or asthenic. The rod test is less reliable in exophoria than in eso- phoria, for the reason that images displaced in the tem- poral part of the retinal fusion area seem to excite a greater demand for fusion than when displaced in the nasal part. Nevertheless, if the exophoria is sufficiently great, the rod held with its axis horizontal before the right eye will cause the streak of light to appear to the left of the candle. The prism, base in, that brings this vertical streak into the candle, measures, but not with accuracy, the exophoria. It always shows less exopho- ria than really exists. Maddox thinks that a red rod makes this test practically perfect, if, at the same time, a plain green or blue glass be held before the other eye. The safe, sure, speedy, and easy test for exophoria is by means of the phorometer, and of all the phorometers the monocular is the most reliable in its results. The method of testing for exophoria is the same as that for esophoria, the position of the false object always deter- EXOPHORIA. 325 mining' whether it is the one or the other error. It is always on the opposite side in exophoria. The error is measured by revolving- the rotary prism until the false object is brought under the true object, when the index will mark the quantity of the error. In the same way the other eye should be tested. In the phorometer test, as in all others, the exophoria at 16 inches should also be determined. The next step is the taking- of the abduction. This is the chief means for determining whether the exophoria is sthenic or asthenic. Unless the character of the error is known, it is not possible to resort to rational treat- ment. Whatever means may have been used in detect- ing- the imbalance, the lifting- power of the externi ab- duction must be taken. This can be done, but not quickly nor accurately, by holding- prism after prism, base in, before one eye, until the patient can no longer fuse the imag-es. The chief objection to this method is the uncertainty about the axis of this prism being- perfectly horizontal. The rotary prism of the phorometer, the instrument being- perfectly leveled, is the quickest and best means for de- termining- abduction or any other kind of duction. To test abduction with the rotary prism, the handle must be horizontal and the index must start from zero. Mov- ing the index toward the temple it must be stopped the 326 EXOPHORIA. moment the patient says the test object becomes double. The index stands opposite the number indicating the de- gree of abduction. If this is less than 8, the exophoria is asthenic; if more than 8, it is sthenic. If abduction is just 8, since it would indicate that the tension of the externus should not be lessened, the exophoria should be classed as asthenic, from an operative standpoint. Lastly, abversion should be taken either with the perimeter or the tropometer. This will usually be found less than 50 if abduction is low, and more than 50 if abduction is high. While abduction and abversion are to be relied on most, adduction and adversion should always be taken. In fact, the study of no one muscle error is complete until all other errors have either been found or elimi- nated; and the individual strength of every muscle must be known. It is only in this way that the real nature of an exophoria can be known, and without this knowl- edge, rational treatment is impossible. COMPLICATIONS OF EXOPHORIA. These are the same as found in connection with the study of esophoria. They need only be mentioned here, as, under the head "Treatment,'-' it w r ill be shown how they modify the management of the exophoria. They are: myopia and myopic astigmatism, hyperopia and hyperopic astigma- tism, and plus and minus cyclophoria. Thus it appears EXOPHORIA. 327 that not only the relationship of every pair of muscles, and the condition of every individual muscle, must be known, but the refraction must also be understood, if one would deal successfully with exophoria. SYMPTOMS. The subjective symptoms or, more correctly speak- ing-, the reflex nervous symptoms caused by exophoria are those outlined in Chapter III. of this book, under the head "Symptoms of Heterophoria. " The symp- toms, whatever they may be, are not due to the inher- ent tension of the externi and their synergists, but to the nervous tension of the interni and their synergists, necessary for maintaining binocular singular vision. A symptom of which exophorics very commonly com- plain is a blurring or running together of the letters of the printed page, after more or less prolonged reading. At such times the reader feels compelled to close the eyes tightly before resuming his reading. Another symptom, often present when near work is being done, is a heavy, sleepy feeling of the upper lids, also a stiff feeling of the upper lids, as if they were adherent to the globes. Prolonged near work congests the margins of the lids, even developing a marginal blepharitis, more commonly in exophoria than in any other form of hetero- phoria. A drawing sensation on the nasal side of the 328 EXOPHORIA. eyes is often complained of. There is no facial expres- sion or pose of the head that is peculiar either to exopho- ria or to esophoria. TREATMENT. NON- OPERATIVE. In pseudo-exophoria the cause should always be removed, if practicable, by non-opera- tive means. The pseudo-exophoria caused by myopia and found only in the near, when it serves to neutralize a part of an inherent esophoria, should be allowed to continue until the esophoria has been cured by prisms in positions of rest, by exercise of the externi, or by op- erations. By the non-treatment of a pseudo-exophoria of this character is meant that the myopic correction should not be worn in near work. For distant seeing the myopic correction should always be worn, for it neither adds to nor diminishes any form of heterophoria. If a myope is orthophoric for distance, the concave lenses should be worn for all purposes. With the lenses on for distant seeing" there will still be orthophoria; with them on in near work the pseudo-exophoria is relieved and the patient becomes orthophoric in the near as well. If the myope is exophoric in the distance, the concave lenses should be worn for all purposes. The distant test will show the same exophoria with and without the lenses. In the near test without the lenses, the exophoria shown will be the intrinsic plus the pseudo; and with the EXOPHORIA. 329 lenses will be only the intrinsic, the pseudo-exophoria having been cured by the establishment of the normal relationship between the center of convergence and the center of ciliary action. The pseudo-exophoria caused by over-development of the ciliary muscles, requiring less than a 1 D impulse to effect a 1 D contraction of these muscles, is best treated by the wearing of concave lenses, only in the near if the patient is an emmetrope, but both in the far and in the near if the patient is slightly hyperopic. By so doing a pseudo-esophoria is developed which lessens the exopho- ria. If the diagnosis is correct that is, if the exopho- ria is wholly or in part pseudo the wearing of concave lenses will be attended by a source of relief. "When they cause discomfort, they should be discarded; for the exophoria is due to some other cause than hyper-devel- opment of the ciliary muscles. It will be remembered by many that J. J. Chisolm, of Baltimore, was in the habit, for many years, of pre- scribing concave cylinders when his patients had hy- peropic astigmatism. Although he did not so teach, nevertheless his patients that were benefited had exo- phoria. An esophoric patient would not have tolerated such lenses. Patients who are hyperopic and have either pseudo or inherent exophoria should never be given the full 330 EXOPHORIA. correction of the hyperopia, for the imbalance would be made worse. If the hyperopia is less than 2 diop- tres and the exophoria in the near is more than 4, no correction should be given; if more than 2 dioptres, only the excess should be corrected. After an exophoria has been cured by exercise or by operation, a full correction of the hyperopia may be given, but in most cases 0.50 D should go uncorrected. What has been said of myopic and hyperopic correc- tions, in cases of exophoria, applies proportionately to astigmatic (myopic or hyperopic) corrections. How to deal with these errors when there is a complicating eso- phoria has been emphasized in Chapter IV. Those unfortunate subjects who have no converging power, probably because of absence of the third con- jugate innervation center, cannot be relieved by either lenses, prisms, exercise, or operations. INHERENT EXOPHORIA. The treatment of the two forms of inherent exophoria is the same, so far as con- cerns non-operative means. The first of these is prisms in positions of rest (bases in) for the weak interni. The full correction of exophoria by prisms should not be at- tempted; probably only a half correction of the error should be given. Maddox suggests a correction of half or a third of the distant and a quarter of the near exo- phoria. When there is no complicating cyclophoria, the EXOPHORIA. 331 prismatic effect should be equally divided between the two eyes, and the axes of the prisms should be perfectly horizontal. The same rule holds good when there is a hyperphoria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other. If there is a complicating- plus cyclophoria without any hyperphoria, the prismatic effect should be equally di- vided between the two eyes; but the axis of each should be tilted down at the temporal end, so as to make the extern! tort the eyes in while turning- them out to fuse the displaced images. The axes should be tilted in the opposite direction if the complication is a minus cyclo- phoria. When the complication is a plus cyclophoria with a right hyperphoria and a left cataphoria, the exo- phoric prism should be placed only before the hyper- phoric eye and its axis should be tilted down at the temporal end. The muscular action necessary for over- coming the prism will turn the eye out and down and tort it in. If discomfort results, it will be due to the work that the inferior rectus has had to do to overcome the prismatic displacement. If any prism is placed before the left (cataphoric) eye, its axis should be perfectly hori- zontal, for, if tilted down at the temporal end, it would favor the cyclophoria, but increase the cataphoria; while, if tilted up, it would force a correction of the cataphoria, but would increase the plus cyclophoria. If there is doubt as to whether the axes of the exophoric prisms should 332 EXOPHORIA. be tilted, it is better to place them exactly horizontal. Weak exophoric prisms, with their axes perfectly hori- zontal, should bring some relief to most patients. When they do not relieve, it thus becomes evident that one externus, if not both, is attached too high, and there is developed a plus cyclophoria. The objection raised against esophoric rest prisms, that they interfere with the law of direction, applies with equal force to prisms in positions of rest for exo- phoria. Decentration of lenses, in for convex and out for con- cave, will accomplish the same results for exophoria as will prisms with bases in. The rules for decentration are given in Chapter IV. EXERCISE TREATMENT. There are two useful methods of exercising the weak interni in cases of exophoria. The simplest, if not the best, and certainly the cheapest, is the candle exercise. The candle is mentioned for the reason that the images of its blaze stimulate the two retinas so as to make it more certain that the center of convergence will be ex- cited sufficiently to converge the visual axes, as the candle is brought from arm's length to a point six or seven inches from the eyes. Images less bright, such as those of a pen- cil, in some cases would not sufficiently stimulate. The EXOPHORIA. 333 method of conducting- the candle exercise is sufficiently set forth in Chapter III., to which the reader is referred. If properly conducted and continued sufficiently long, it will do good in all cases except those in whom the interni have attachments too high on the globes. Interni thus attached, when exercised either with the candle or by means of prisms, will call into simultaneous action the inferior obliques, that they may prevent the convergence of the vertical axes of the eyes. In the greater number of cases this would either add to or develop a plus cyclo- phoria while curing the exophoria. The patient would not be benefited. But when the interni have the ideal attachments, or even when they are attached too low, the candle exercise, as well as prism exercise, will do good. In cases of ideal attachment only the interni are exercised; in cases with attachment too low every con- traction of the interni is attended by a contraction of the superior obliques, so that development of the interni is attended by a corresponding development of the superior obliques a thing to be desired in many cases. The rapidity of a cure of exophoria by the candle ex- ercise depends in part on the quantity of the exophoria and in part on the character of the blood supply of the interni; abundant blood supply means quicker results. Gentle rhythmic exercise will increase the size and power of a muscle, whether voluntary or involuntary. 334 EXOPHORIA. Permanent results follow such a method. No one can doubt that a muscle can be developed; there is reasonable doubt if a nerve center can be developed as a result of either mild or severe stimulation. A nerve cell is very different from a muscle fiber. PRISM EXERCISE. There are two methods of exercising- the interni by means of prisms. The one is gentle rhythmic ex- ercise by means of weak prisms witk their bases out; the other method is that first suggested by Deady, and later advocated by Gould loading the converg- ence by means of the strongest prisms possible. The former is intended for the strengthening of the mus- cles themselves, while the latter is designed to stim- ulate the convergence center to greater activity. The advocates of the latter method claim that exophoria, in most cases, is purely innervational and should be cured by forced stimulation of the convergence brain-center, the third conjugate innervation center. That this cen- ter is susceptible to excessive stimulation cannot be de- nied, but it is doubtful if this should be done. It is cer- tainly more rational to develop the interni so as to make them respond normally to the impulse that the brain - center can easily generate in its real, though it may be subnormal, state of development. If it were possible to EXOPHORIA. 335 enlarge the capacity of a brain-center, as it is possible to increase the size and power of a muscle, the Deady method would not be objectionable. The reader is ag-ain referred to Chapter III., where the method is described. In the rhythmic exercise of the interni by prisms, the design is to produce slight contractions by means of weak prisms (from 1 to 8) with their bases out, to be followed by complete relaxation, each contraction and relaxation to last about three seconds, throughout a sitting of not more than ten minutes. The exercise should al- ways stop short of fatigue, for exercise that tires does not build. To get practically complete relaxation, the object of fixation should be twenty feet distant. Per- sistent exercise, after this method, in low degrees of in- herent exophoria, will produce permanent results. This method is fully set forth in Chapter III. In high degrees of intrinsic exophoria, non-operative measures will be productive of but little, and that little will be slow of accomplishment. Exophoria in the distance of 4 or more, and an exophoria in the near equal to the angle of convergence at that point, give little promise of yielding to non-operative means. An exophoria that gives diplopia in the distance under the red glass test, is practically always a case for surgical treatment. All cases not showing good re- sults, in a reasonable length of time, under non-oper- 336 EXOPHORIA. ative measures, should be given the advantage offered by skilled surgery. The object in view when exercising the interni in ex- ophoria is to so develop them that they may respond normally to a normal nerve impulse 1 of contraction for every degree of impulse. OPERATIVE TREATMENT. Before any operation for exophoria is done, the pos- sibility of a cure by non-operative means should be elim- inated, and the condition of every extrinsic ocular muscle should be known. Complicating muscle imbalances must be taken into account, and, if possible, should be cor- rected by the operations for the exophoria. In uncom- plicated cases of exophoria, and in cases complicated only by hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, the operations must either diminish the tension of the extern! or increase the tension of the interni. When the exophoria is complicated by a cyclophoria, not only must the muscle tension be altered, but the muscle plane must also be changed. In sthenic exophoria the externi should be first sub- jected to the operation of partial tenotomy, with the view of reducing their tension. The case being uncom- plicated, the tenotomy should be central. The operative effect should be equally divided between the two externi, EXOPHORIA. 337 and should not be so extensive as to reduce abduction be- low 8 or abversion below 50. In no case of exophoria should a complete tenotomy of an externus ever be done, for the reason that the risk of reducing both the duction and version power below the normal would be too great. After the two partial tenotomies, any remaining exo- phoria that cannot be cured by non- operative meas- ures should be still further relieved by a straight- forward shortening of one or both interni, with the view of increasing tension without changing the plane of rotation. When there is a complication of hyperphoria and cata- phoria only, the operations, whether partial tenotomies or shortenings, should be done as if no complication existed. At some other time the vertical error must be given the proper treatment. A sthenic exophoria that is complicated by a plus cy- clophoria only should be treated with the view of lessen- ing the tension of both externi and lowering their planes of action. This would be accomplished by cutting the upper and central fibers of each externus as nearly alike as possible, leaving the lower fibers intact. The three- fold effect of these two operations would be: (a) lessen- ing or curing the exophoria; (b) correction, wholly or in part, of the plus cyclophoria; (c) the production of a dou- ble cataphoria. 338 EXOPHORIA. A sthenic exophoria complicated by a plus cyclopho- ria and a right hyperphoria and left cataphoria should be subjected first to a partial marginal tenotomy of the externus of the hyperphoric eye. The operation of cut- ting the upper and central fibers of this externus would be attended by these three results: (a) lessening of the ex- ophoria; (b) a partial or complete correction of the plus cyclophoria; (c) the production of a cataphoria equal to, or a little less than, the cataphoria in the other eye. If any remaining 1 exophoria should still be complicated with plus cyclophoria and left cataphoria, the second opera- tion should be a shortening of the left internus in such a way as to both increase its tension and elevate its plane of action. This would have three results: (a) still further diminishing, if not curing, the exophoria; (b) a further correction of the plus cyclophoria; (c) an elevation of the cataphoric eye so as to bring it as nearly as possible in the same horizontal plane with the eye that was pri- marily hyperphoric. Should the first operation cure the complicating plus cyclophoria, even if the hyperphoria were not cured, the remaining exophoria should be re- lieved by a central partial tenotomy of the externus of the left eye, which would alter its tension without chang- ing its plane of action. Asthenic exophoria uncomplicated should be treated by straight-forward shortening of both interni, the op- EXOPHORIA. 339 erative effect being as equally divided between them as possible. In this way their tension would be increased, but their planes of rotation would not be changed. The same operations should be done when the exophoria is complicated by hyperphoria and cataphoria. Operations for a lateral error should attempt the simultaneous cor- rection of a vertical error only when there is a complicat- ing cyclophoria. Asthenic exophoria, complicated by a plus cyclophoria only, should have both conditions relieved by shortenings of both interni in such a way as to increase their tension and elevate their planes. The triple effect would be: (a) correction of the exophoria; (b) cure of the plus cyclophoria; (c) the production of a double hyperphoria. When the complication is not only a plus cyclophoria, but a right hyperphoria and left cataphoria as well, the first operation should be a shortening of the left internus in such a way as to both increase its tension and elevate its plane. These would be the effects of this operation: (a) correction, wholly or in part, of the exophoria; (b) a par- tial or complete cure of the cyclophoria; (c) the produc- tion of a double hyperphoria. If the internus of the right eye must be operated upon, the shortening must be straight-forward, even if the two complications still existed; for an elevation of its plane would increase the hyperphoria while lessening the plus cyclophoria, and 340 EXOPHORIA. lowering 1 its plane would increase the cyclophoria while diminishing 1 the hyperphoria. If a minus cyclophoria, which is rare, should alone complicate an exophoria, the marginal tenotomies of the externi would be below, and the shortenings of the in- terni would have to be done so as to depress their plane of rotation. If the minus cyclophoria \vith a hyperpho- ria and cataphoria should complicate an exophoria, a lower marginal tenotomy of an externus should be per- formed only on the externus of the cataphoric eye; while a shortening of an internus with depression of its plane should be done only on the internus of the hyperphoric eye, for reasons that are apparent. The chief object in operating for exophoria, whether the operation be partial tenotomies of the externi for sthenic exophoria or shortenings of the interni for as- thenic exophoria, is to so change the relative tension of the interni as to enable them to respond normally to a normal impulse from the third conjugate innervation center 1 degree of convergence for every degree of im- pulse and thus establish harmony between the externi and the interni. The change of the plane of action, though of vast im- portance, depends solely on the existence of a compli- cating cyclophoria. CHAPTER VI. HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. THESE conditions can be studied intelligently only when the head is in the primary position, with the test object on the line of intersection of the extended median and horizontal fixed planes of the head. The object should be twenty feet distant from the eyes. If there is no imbal- ance of the vertically-acting" muscles and the lateral recti are properly attached and the eyes are contained in orbits that have been normally developed, when the test object is fixed, the two visual axes will lie in the ex- tended horizontal plane, with no tendency for either axis to rise above or dip below this plane. This will be shown under any one or all of the tests for determining the balance of the ocular muscles. Such a condition, as already noted in Chapter II., is vertical orthophoria. Hyperphoria is a tendency of one visual axis to rise above this plane, the actual turning- easily occurring- as soon as the eye is freed from the control of the guiding- sen- sation, by any one of the tests to be given farther on. Cataphoria is a tendency on the part of one visual axis to fall below this plane, the tendency becoming a turning (311) 342 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. so soon as the image has been changed in character or position so that no effort at fusion will be made. Usu- ally a hyperphoria of one eye is associated with a cata- phoria of the other, and the two errors are practically equal. Occasionally there will be found a case in which there is a vertical orthophoria of one eye and a hyperpho- ria or a cataphoria of the other. Less frequently there will be double hyperphoria or double cataphoria. Any one of these errors makes it a difficult matter for the superior and inferior recti to obey the law governing them to wit, they must keep the visual axes in the same plane, in order to help relate, properly, corresponding retinal points. CAUSES. There are several conditions that may cause a vertical imbalance. Since malformation of the orbits has been emphasized, in recent years, as a cause of hy- perphoria, this will be studied first. Only in the sense of one orbit's being higher or lower than the other, can a malformed orbit be the only cause either of a hyperpho- ria or a cataphoria. Fig. 43 represents the median plane of the head, A B; the horizontal plane of the head, C D; and the two eyes. The right one is in a normal orbit, so that its vertical axis g-h is parallel with the median plane of the head and its transverse axis efis contained in the horizontal plane of the head. The left eye is represented as contained in a malformed orbit, in the sense that it is HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 343 lower than the fellow orbit; therefore the contained eye is lower than its fellow, as is shown by its transverse axis flying below the fixed horizontal plane of the head, C D. It will be seen that the vertical axis g-h is parallel with the median plane A B. The muscles of these two eyes may be supposed to be perfectly balanced. Under the phorometer test of the vertically-acting- muscles, the B Fig- 43- rig-ht eye would show orthophoria, but the left eye would show cataphoria. In binocular fixation of a point lying- in the extended horizontal plane of the head, the visual axis of the rig-lit eye, the muscles being- in a state of equilibrium, would point to the object; while the visual axis of the left eye would have to be raised by the supe- rior rectus and inferior oblique, so as to intersect its fel- low at the point of view. Thus elevated, its vertically- 344 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. acting muscles cannot be in a state of equilibrium. Un- der test this eye would drop into a state of equilibrium for all its muscles and would thus show cataphoria. No posing- of the head would change the condition or lessen the error. The right eye under test would continue in its state of equilibrium, and would, therefore, show verti- cal orthophoria. B Fig. A figure could have been constructed showing the right eye in a normal orbit, with its axes properly re- lated to the median and horizontal planes of the head; and the left eye in a malformed orbit, in the sense of its being higher than the fellow orbit, with its transverse axis ef above the fixed horizontal plane of the head, al- though its vertical axis gh would be parallel with the median plane. The right eye would show vertical ortho- HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 345 phoria, but the left eye would show hyperphoria. No posing- of the head can change the relationship that these two eyes bear to the two fixed planes of the head. Fig-. 44 represents malformation of both orbits in the sense that the right one is too high and the left one is too low. The vertical axis of each eye is parallel with the median plane of the head, but the transverse axis of -J) f 3 Fig- 45- neither eye lies in the horizontal plane of the head; that of the rig-lit eye is above, while that of the left eye is below it, but both are necessarily parallel with it. The mal- position of the rig-ht eye would g-ive to it hyperphoria, while the malposition of the left eye would g-ive to it cataphoria. A state of equilibrium of the vertically-act- ing- muscles (granted to be normal) of the right eye would place its axis above, but parallel with, the extended hor- izontal plane of the head; while the same muscular state 346 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. of the left eye would place its visual axis below, but parallel with this plane. No pose of the head can help these eyes in the effort at binocular fixation. Fig. 45 represents a pair of eyes that are set in mal- formed orbits, in the sense that both are too low; hence both of these eyes have their transverse axes below the horizontal plane of the head, but parallel with it. This B Fig. 46. kind of malformation gives a double cataphoria, which can be detected with a fair degree of readiness by means of the monocular phorometer, but is more certainly and more easily shown by the proof test of hyperphoria a double prism, the use of which, for this purpose, will be described under the head "Tests." A pose of the head cannot bring 1 the transverse axes of the eyes into the hor- izontal plane of the head, but it can make vision easier. HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 347 The characteristic pose, in such cases, is an elevation of the chin. Such people are high-headed. Fig. 46 represents a pair of eyes set in malformed or- bits, in the sense that they are both too hig-h. The ver- tical axes are parallel with the median plane of the head, but the transverse axes lie above the horizontal plane, though parallel with it. With the head in the primary position, a point in the extended horizontal plane and in the line of its intersection by the extended median plane cannot be fixed by these eyes without depression of the visual axes by contraction of the inferior recti, aided by the superior obliques. Under- test, either one of these eyes not under control of the guiding sensation will turn up into the position of muscle equilibrium, showing- a double hyperphoria. In the study of all these figures, all of the muscles are supposed to be normal in development, correct in attach- ment, and perfectly innervated. A hyperphoria, single or double; a cataphoria, single or double; a h} r perphoria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other, having mal- formations of the orbits as the sole causative agent, will not have a complicating cyclophoria; nor can any kind of malformation of the orbits ever cause c\ r clo- phoria. It has already been shown that orbits that are too wide apart cause exophoria, while orbits that are too close to each other will cause esophoria. 348 HYPERPHORIA AXD CATAPHORIA. It should be remembered that other causes of hyper- phoria and cataphoria may exist when e} T es are set in malformed orbits, and that the other causes may show themselves in an increase of the error caused by the mal- formation of the orbit, or ma} 7 neutralize it, or may even reverse it. To illustrate: the right orbit may be nor- mal, the contained eye having- its vertical and transverse axes properly related to the median and horizontal fixed planes of the head; while the left orbit may be too low, so that the contained eye has its transverse axis below the horizontal plane of the head (see Fig. 43). As a con- sequence, the muscles being well balanced, there will be a left cataphoria; but if the left superior rectus is too strong for its opposing inferior rectus, the cataphoria of orbital causation either will be neutralized or there will be a left hyperphoria. When malformation of the orbits is the only cause for vertical imbalances, the resulting errors may be said to be pseudo-h}-perphoria and pseudo-cataphoria. The treatment of such errors should be by means of prisms in positions of rest, of such strength as to fully correct the errors. There is no direct connection between the brain-center for the ciliary muscles and those centers controlling the muscles that elevate and depress the eyes; so that, through these muscles, a focal error cannot cause a HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 349 hyperphoria or a cataphoria. It cannot be denied, how- ever, that convex lenses given to correct hyperopia, sometimes cure a hyperphoria or a cataphoria. Such cases have always had a pseudo-esophoria which, like- wise, was cured by the convex lenses. It is clear that in such cases one internus is attached too high or the other internus is attached too low, so that the one eye, on being 1 turned in, is also turned up; while the other eye, on being turned in, is also turned down. The same agent (convex lenses) that relieved the in-turning (pseudo- esophoria) relieved also the up-turning (pseudo-hyper- phoria) and the down-turning (pseudo-cataphoria). Both intern! attached too high would give, under the same conditions, a double pseudo-hyperphoria; while both interni attached too low would give a double pseudo- cataphoria. There would also be a complicating cyclo- phoria, which will be studied more fully in Chapter VII. In the same way it could be shown how a pseudo-exo- phoria, because of too high or too low attachment of one or both externi, might cause a pseudo-hyperphoria or cataphoria, one or both, or either in the double form, all of which would be relieved by concave lenses. Hyperphoria and cataphoria, in the great majority of cases, are intrinsic, or inherent, in character. They are never pseudo except when caused by pseudo-esophoria and pseudo-exophoria, or malformed orbits. 350 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. The cause of an intrinsic vertical error may reside wholly in the interni, but only when there is an intrin- sic esophoria or an intrinsic exophoria. In such cases of esophoria one or both interni are attached too high, or one or both are attached too low, or one is attached too high and the other is attached too low. In the one, the esophoria would cause a double hyperphoria and a minus cyclophoria; in the second case the esophoria would cause double cataphoria and plus cyclophoria; and in the last case the esophoria in one eye would cause a hyperphoria and a minus cyclophoria, and in the other a cataphoria and a plus cyclophoria. How to deal with such interni has been pointed out in the chapter on esophoria. Intrinsic exophoria, in which the externi are attached too high, will cause double hyperphoria and plus cyclo- phoria; if both interni are attached too low, the exophoria will cause double cataphoria and minus cyclophoria. If one internus is too high and the other is too low, the exo- phoria of the former would cause hyperphoria and plus cyclophoria; the exophoria of the latter would cause cat- aphoria and minus cyclophoria. How to deal with the externi in such cases has been set forth in the chapter on exophoria. Hyperphoria and cataphoria, in the greater number of cases, are caused by imbalance of the vertically-acting muscles the supervertors and subvertors, which are the HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORlA. 351 superior recti and inferior obliques and the inferior recti and the superior obliques. When the cause is either in the straight or oblique supervertors or in the straight or oblique subvertors, the error is always inherent, and never pseudo. DOUBLE HYPERPHORIA. This condition may be caused by the two superior recti being hyper-developed, or by a subnormal development of the inferior recti; or it may be caused by the superior recti having their at- tachments too near the corneo-scleral junction, or by the inferior recti having their attachments too far removed from the corneo-scleral junction; or it may be that the first conjugate innervation center is normally so endowed as to send a more powerful impulse to the superior recti than goes to the inferior recti from the second con- jugate innervation center. If either of these conditions is the cause of a double hyperphoria, there will also be a minus cyclophoria, independent of any imbalance of the obliques. The superior and inferior recti may be well balanced, and the externi and interni may have ideal attachments, and yet there may be a double hyperphoria. The cause would be found in imbalance of the obliques, the inferior having the. advantage over the superior, either because the former are more highly developed or because they are attached nearer the posterior pole or because the 352 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. seventh conjugate innervation impulse is more intense than that from the sixth conjugate innervation center. In either case the double hyperphoria would be asso- ciated with plus cyclophoria. In double hyperphoria caused by the superior recti being too strong, the nervous tension of the inferior recti would counteract both the hyperphoria and the minus cyclophoria, while nervous tension of the superior obliques would help to counteract the hyperphoria, but would augment the minus cyclophoria. It is reasonable, therefore, to conclude that the counteracting nerve im- pulse in such a case is sent only to the inferior recti. Depressing the chin a downcast face would help re- lieve the inferior recti of nervous tension. In double hyperphoria caused by the inferior obliques being too strong, nervous tension of the superior ob- liques would counteract both the hyperphoria and the plus cyclophoria, while nervous tension of the inferior recti would help to counteract the hyperphoria, but would increase the plus cyclophoria; hence the conclu- sion that the counteracting impulse, in such a case, is sent only to the superior obliques. Such a patient would instinctively elevate the chin carry a high head to relieve the nervous. tension of the superior obliques. The most advantageous posi- tion of the eyes for the in-torting action of the superior HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 353 obliques is a depression of the visual axes below the fixed horizontal plane of the head; the greater this depression of the visual axes (elevation of the head), the more powerful the in-torting- action of the superior obliques. DOUBLE CATAPHORIA. This condition may be the result of hyper-development of the inferior recti or sub- normal development of the superior recti, or it may result from the inferior recti having- their attachment too far forward or from the superior recti being" at- tached too far back; or it may be caused by a more pow- ful impulse sent from the second con jug-ate innervation center to the inferior recti than is generated by the first conjug-ate center for the superior recti. In either case the double cataphoria will be associated with plus cyclo- phoria. Nervous tension of the superior recti will coun- teract both the cataphoria and the plus cyclophoria; while nervous tension of the inferior obliques would counteract the cataphoria, but would increase the plus cyclophoria. Hence, in cases like the above, the correct- ive nerve impulse must be sent only to the superior recti. The position of the eyes most favorable for ef- fective action of the superior recti is a depression of the visual axes below the horizontal plane of the head; hence such patients will carry their heads hig"h, so as to lessen the nervous tension of the superior recti. 354 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. Double cataphoria may be caused by imbalance of the obliques, the superior being stronger than the inferior, either because the former are hyper-developed or the lat- ter are subnormally developed, or because the former are attached nearer the posterior pole, or because the sixth conjugate innervation is more powerful than the seventh. In either case the double cataphoria would be associated with minus cyclophoria. The corrective nerve impulse would be sent to the inferior obliques, which would coun- teract both the double cataphoria and the minus cyclo- phoria. The position of the eyes most favorable for cor- rective action of the inferior obliques is an elevation of the visual axes above the extended horizontal plane of the head; hence such patients would have their faces downcast, for this pose of the head would help to relieve the nervous tension of the inferior obliques. Hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, independent of malformation of the orbits and faulty attachments of the lateral recti muscles, are always in- herent in the vertically-acting muscles, and never inner- vational. For convenience of study the right eye will be considered as hyperphoric and the left eye as cataphoric, although the reverse is often found. The right hyper- phoria is due to the fact that the superior rectus is too strong for its direct antagonist, the inferior rectus, or that the inferior oblique is too strong for the superior HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 355 oblique; or both of these conditions may unite in the de- velopment of the hyperphoria. If the superior rectus alone is the cause of the hyperphoria, it is because this muscle is hyper -developed or that the inferior rectus is of subnormal development, or that the supe- rior rectus is attached too near the cornea or that the attachment of the inferior rectus is too far removed from the cornea. The hyperphoria would be sthenic if the superior rectus is hyper-developed or is attached too near the cornea; it would be asthenic if the inferior rectus is subnormally developed or is attached too far away from the cornea. In either case the hyperphoria would manifest itself in association with minus cyclophoria. If the inferior oblique alone is the cause of the hyper- phoria, it is because of hyper-development of this muscle or a subnormal development of the superior oblique; or because the inferior oblique is attached too far behind the equator or the superior oblique is attached too near the equator. The hyperphoria thus caused is sthenic in cases in which the inferior oblique is hyper-developed or is attached too far behind the equator; it is asthenic in those cases in which the superior oblique is subnormally developed or is attached too close to the equator. In either condition the hyperphoria would show itself in association with plus cyclophoria. 356 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. When the hyperphoria manifests itself unassociated with either minus or plus cyclophoria, it becomes evident that both the superior rectus and inferior oblique enter into the causation. The left eye under test will show cataphoria, usually the same in quantity as the hyperphoria of the right eye. The cataphoria is caused by either a too powerful inferior rectus or a too powerful superior oblique; or both of these muscles ma} T unite in the production of the cataphoria. In a case in which the inferior rectus alone is the causative agent, it is either hyper-developed or has its at- tachment too close to the cornea; or its direct antagonist, the superior rectus, is subnormally developed or is at- tached too far away from the cornea. The cataphoria would be sthenic if the inferior rectus is either hyper- developed or is attached too near the cornea; it would be asthenic if the superior rectus is under-developed or has its attachment too far removed from the cornea. In either case the cataphoria would be associated with a plus cyclophoria. In a case in which the superior oblique is the sole cause of the cataphoria, it is either because it is hyper- developed or because it has its attachment too near the posterior pole; or because its direct antagonist, the in- ferior oblique, is subnormally developed or is attached HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 357 too near the equator. The resulting- cataphoria is sthenic in those cases in which the superior oblique is either hyper-developed or is attached too near the posterior pole; it is asthenic when the inferior oblique is under- developed or is attached too near the equator. In either case the cataphoria would be associated with a minus cyclophoria. Cataphoria will be unassociated with either plus or minus cyclophoria only when both the inferior rectus and superior oblique are united in the causation. Nervous tension of the inferior rectus counteracts the rig-ht hyperphoria, if caused by the superior rectus; while nervous tension of the superior rectus will coun- teract the left cataphoria, if caused by the inferior rectus. Not only will the right hyperphoria and left cataphoria be thus neutralized, but the minus cyclopho- ria of the rig-ht and plus c} 7 clophoria of the left would be suppressed by the nervous tension of the same muscles. The corrective impulse would come not from one conjugate center, as in double hyperphoria and double cataphoria, but from two separate centers. Nervous tension of the superior oblique counteracts the rig-ht hyperphoria which is caused by the inferior oblique, while nervous tension of the inferior oblique will counteract the left cataphoria that is caused by the superior oblique. The plus cyclophoria of the right eye and the minus cyclophoria of the left eye will be sup- 358 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. pressed by the nervous tension of the same muscles that counteract the hyperphoria and cataphoria. TESTS. The phorometer, with perfectly ad-justed prisms and spirit level, alone can be depended on in testing for hyperphoria and cataphoria. The slightest error in testing will be followed by bad results in practice. In attempting to test for these errors by means of a dis- placing prism with its axis horizontal, held in the hand, one would have to be very careful or the axis would be slightly inclined in one direction or the other; so that, if the eye under test is hyperphoric and the temporal end of the prism inclines down, the error will be exagger- ated, and if it inclines upward, the hyperphoria would be neutralized more or less completely, or even a cata- phoria might be shown. For reasons already given the rod test is not to be trusted implicitly, but it is much to be preferred over the hand-prism test, or even the prism when set in the trial frame of the refraction case. The test object being a candle, a rod held with its axis vertical before one eye will show the streak of light which, in orthophoria, should pass directly through the blaze seen by the other eye; in hyperphoria, the streak would pass below the blaze, while in cataphoria it would pass above the blaze. HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 359 In the low degrees of vertical heterophoria, the streak falling- on the area of binocular fusion will excite some effort, however small, at fusion. In the higher errors, the prism used for measuring the amount of the error, by throwing the streak on the fusion area, would excite some effort at fusion. When the rod is the means of testing, the error is never shown in excess, and for this reason is more safe than accurate. The use of the plus 13 D lens before one eye, if not worthy of trust in the examinations for esophoria and exophoria, would certainly be less trustworthy in exami- nations for hyperphoria and cataphoria. In high degrees of a vertical error the plane red glass held before one eye will cause diplopia, the red light below in hyperphoria and above in cataphoria. When the red image is entirely outside the area of binocular fusion, the full error will be shown, but cannot be accu- rately measured, for the reason that the rotary or other prism that carries the red image into the fusion area, at once excites some effort at fusion. Like the rod test, the red-glass test will mislead only in that it will not show, even on careful measurement, the full error. The cover test will show the vertical errors, but no one would think of basing the treatment of a case on this test. Any of the standard phorometers may be used in test- ing for vertical heterophoria, but in this the monocular 360 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. instrument is most useful and trustworthy. The 10 prism, base in, should be placed in the cell behind the rotary prism; the controlling- screw should be vertical, and the index should stand at zero. The instrument should be perfectly level. The patient's head should be in the primary position. The test object should be,a white spot on a black background, and should be distant twenty feet. With the instrument before the right eye there will appear to be two spots, the true one to the left and the false one to the right. If they are too widely separated, as in cases that are esophoric, the 6 prism should be substituted for the 10 prism. The patient should constantly fix the true spot, and by indirect vision alone should locate and relate the false spot. If the eye is hyperphoric, the false spot will be lower than the true; and since its image is not on -the area of binocular fusion, the full error will be shown. The index of the rotary prism moved upward \vill accurately measure the error; for in carrying the false object up to the level of the true, the image of the former is not made to invade the fusion area, provided the true object alone has been fixed throughout the test. The vertical imbalance of one eye having been taken, the phorometer should be turned in front of the other. Precisely the same steps should be taken in determining the condition of its vertically-acting muscles. Hyper- HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 361 phoria having been found in the eye first tested, the fel- low eye, as a rule, will be found cataphoric. The false object will appear higher than the true. The index of the rotary prism should be carried downward until the false object reaches a level with the true object. The quantity of the error, as indicated on the scale, should be noted. In the greater number of cases the degree of cataphoria will be the same as the hyperphoria of the other eye. One eye having shown hyperphoria, the other may show vertical orthophoria or even hyperphoria. The eye under test, seeing the false object by indirect vision, does not receive any fusion stimulus; hence it always turns into the position of equilibrium of all its muscles. For this reason it is just as easy to determine the exist- ence of a double hyperphoria, with the monocular pho- rometer, as it is to ascertain the existence of any other form of heterophoria. The patient must be impressed with the absolute importance of always fixing- the true object that is, must see it by direct vision. Whether one or the other of the tests referred to above should be adopted, the proof test for vertical im- balance should not be neglected. Errors that may have crept in because of carelessness of the operator or indif- ference of the patient can be eliminated easily by the proof test. The means of -proving- is the Maddox double 362 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. prism (4 to 6 each). This should be held in the hand first before one eye and then the other, so that the line of union of the prism bases shall be horizontal. The fixing 1 eye should be the one not under test. The prism should be moved up and down before the eye under test, so that one moment the false object would be seen below the true and the next moment above it, but always by indirect vision. If the distance from the true to the false object is the same when below as it is when above, there is vertical orthophoria. If there is hyperphoria, the false object, when seen through the upper prism, will be closer to the true object, by twice the amount of the error, than when seen through the lower; while the reverse will be true if there is cataphoria. In double hyperphoria the objects will be closer together for each eye when the false object is seen through the upper prism; while in double cataphoria the false object, when seen through the lower prism by each eye, will be nearer the true object than when seen through the upper prism. If there is hyperphoria of one eye and catapho- ria of the other, when the double-prism proof test is applied to the hyperphoric eye, the false object seen through the upper prism will be close to the true, and will be farther removed from it when seen through the lower prism. On shifting the test to the other eye, the false object seen through the lower prism will be nearer HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 363 the true than when it is seen through the upper prism. The double prisms should be of equal strength, and each should be strong- enough to throw the image of the test object entirely above or below the limits of the field of fusion. Dr. Doak, Assistant in Ophthalmology, Vanderbilt University, has devised a test for vertical imbalances that not only detects and measures the error, but is also in itself a proof test. By this test the kind of error is at once detected, but its quantity is not known until the proof feature has been applied. The delicacy of the test is shown by an apparent doubling of the quantity of the error. This delicacy makes it dangerous only when the operator forgets that the apparent error is twice that of the real error. For making this test the monocular phorometer must be placed before one eye, and in the cell next to the eye must be placed either the 10 or 6 prism, base toward the nose. The controlling screw must be vertical, and the index, at the beginning, should stand at zero. The instrument must be level. The patient must hold before the other eye a double prism in such position as to make the test object (white spot on a black background) double for that eye, the one directly above the other, and the two should be 12 apart that is, each of the double prisms should be 6. With the double prism thus adjusted, these two spots 364 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. must be seen by indirect vision, while the single spot seen by the other eye should be observed by direct vision. Because of the displacing prism behind the phorometer, the single object will not be in line with the other two; and when it is seen by direct vision, the other eye will be so turned that the vertical imaginary line connecting the two false objects will fall to the nasal side of the fusion area, so that, as the test proceeds, there will be made no effort at fusion. The eye behind the double prism will be wholly off its guard. The moment the single object is fixed, the patient can usually say whether or not it would be halfway between the other two ob- jects if it were in line with them. If the middle object is seen nearer the lower, that eye is hyperphoric; if nearer the upper, it is cataphoric. The proof feature of the test results from the use of the rotary prism. When the screw is turned so as to carry the index upward, the patient is asked to speak the moment the single object is in a horizontal line with the upper of the two false ob- jects. The extent of the rotation is noted, after which the rotary prism is again made neutral. The next step is to carry the index of the rotary prism downward until the patient says that the single object is in a horizontal line with the lower of the two false objects. The extent of downward rotation is now noted. If the two arcs traversed by the index are equal, there is undoubtedly HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 365 vertical orthophoria of this eye. If the lower arc is 5 and the upper arc is 7, this eye is certainly hyperphoric not to the extent of the difference between these two arcs, which would be 2, but only half this amount viz., 1. If the upper arc is 5 and the lower arc is 7, there is cataphoria not of 2, but of 1. The reason for saying that the real vertical error is only one-half of the appar- ent error is clear. Since each of the double prisms is 6, the double objects seen through them, \vhen the base- line is in the extended horizontal plane of the head, are 12 apart. The extended horizontal plane of the head cuts the imaginary line connecting the two false objects at the midway point 6 from each. If the displaced ob- ject seen by direct vision with the other eye is in this plane, it would have to be carried up or down by the rotary prism just 6 to be placed in a horizontal line with the one or the other of the false objects, hence the eye would be orthophoric vertically. If the true object is 1 below the extended horizontal plane of the head, it will have to be carried downward by the rotary prism only 5 to be placed in a horizontal line with the lower object, while it would have to be carried upward 7 to stand in a horizontal line with the upper object. Thus it is clear that the hyperphoria shown by this test is one-half the difference between the arcs traversed by the index of the phorometer in placing the true object in a 366 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. horizontal line first with the one false object and then with the other, the index each time starting from zero. Throughout the entire test the single object must be fixed. Although this test is in a sense binocular, it is probably better than any other test for two reasons: (a) It doubles the real error, so that a small error will be less likely to be overlooked; (b) this test proves itself. The eye under the Doak test is the one behind the rotary prism. Both eyes" should be subjected to the same test. The duction and version power of the superior and in- ferior recti of both eyes must be taken in order to deter- mine whether the hyperphoria and cataphoria are sthenic or asthenic, for on this knowlege must depend the treat- ment of the case. If the superduction is less than 3 and the superver- sion is 33 or less, the hyperphoria is asthenic; if sub- duction is less than 3 and sub-version is below 50, the cataphoria is asthenic. If superduction is more than 3 and superversion is greater than 33, the hyperphoria is sthenic; if sub-duction is more than 3 and sub-version is greater than 50, the cataphoria is sthenic. COMPLICATIONS. Focal errors do not complicate ver- tical heterophorias, except in cases in which there is pseudo-esophoria or pseudo-exophoria with too high or too low attachments of the lateral recti muscles. A HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 367 vertical error thus caused is pseudo in character and is cured, as is also the lateral pseudo-error, by correction of the focal errors. The only complication that must always be thought of in the study of hyperphoria and cataphoria is cyclopho- ria; for by this complication is determined the treatment, surgical or otherwise, of these errors. How to find and measure this all-important complication will be set forth ; .n the next chapter. If a hyperphoria is only complicated by esophoria or exophoria or by any focal error, all these troubles must be treated as if they existed alone. SYMPTOMS. Any and all of the symptoms mentioned in the chapter on heterophoria may be caused by vertical imbalance. There is no facial expression that can be dignified as diagnostic of hyperphoria and cataphoria. There are poses of the head peculiar to both double hyperphoria and double cataphoria. High-headedness is a symptom of double hyperphoria when the inferior obliques cause the error, and is just as certainly a sign of double cata- phoria when the inferior recti are the cause of the error. The most favorable position of the eyes for effective ac- tion of weak superior obliques and weak superior recti under a corrective nerve impulse is a depression of the visual axes below the extended horizontal plane of the 368 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. head, or (what is the same thing") elevation of the ex- tended horizontal plane of the head. The downcast look is a sign of double hyperphoria when the error is caused by the superior recti, and of double cataphoria when the error is caused by the supe- rior obliques. The most favorable position of the eyes for effective action of weak inferior obliques and weak inferior recti, under a corrective nervous impulse, is an elevation of the visual axes above the extended horizontal plane of the head, or (what is equal to it) a depression of the hori- zontal plane of the head. A tilting- of the head toward one shoulder or the other occurs only in cases in which there is a hyperphoria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other, complicated by either plus or minus cyclophoria of both eyes, and never in simple cases of hyperphoria. The hyperphoria and cataphoria are not the cause of the tilting- of the head; the cause is the complicating- cyclophoria. If the com- plication is plus cyclophoria, the head will be tilted toward the cataphoric side. In a case of this kind, tilt- ing the head elevates the hyperphoric eye and depresses the cataphoric eye; so that to fix an object that would be in the extended horizontal plane of the head, if the head were erect, makes it necessary that the visual axis of the eye that is higher shall be depressed and that the HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 369 visual axis of the eye that is lower shall be elevated, so as to make them intersect at the object. Depression of the visual axis of the hyperphoric eye places the eye in such a position (elevated posterior pole) as to g-ive to the superior oblique, under the whole of the stimulus of the sixth fusional innervation, its greatest torsioning- power, which would enable it easily to parallel the vertical axis of the eye with the now-inclined median plane of the head. The hyperphoria in such a case is due largely to the inferior oblique, and the plus cyclophoria is wholly caused by it. The elevated posterior pole of the eye, made necessary by the tilting 1 of the head, places the inferior oblique at a disadvantage to the weak superior oblique; hence, the greater ease with which both the hyperphoria and the plus cyclophoria are counteracted. The cataphoria of the other eye must be caused by the inferior rectus, and the same muscle most probably causes the whole of the plus cyclophoria of this eve. The cor- rective stimulus most likely conies from the first fu- sional innervation center and is wholl} r expended on the superior rectus of the cataphoric eye (none of it is needed for the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye), enabling- it to counteract both the cataphoria and the plus cyclophoria. Its action is not favored by posi- tion. A corrective stimulus sent to the superior oblique 370 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. would lessen the cyclophoria, but increase the cata- phoria; therefore it is reasonable to conclude that none is sent to it. In a case of hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, complicated by a minus cyclophoria, the head is tilted toward the hyperphoric side. The cataphoric eye is elevated, the hyperphoric eye is depressed, by this position of the head; so that the visual axis of the higher eye must be depressed and that of the lower eye must be elevated, so as to intersect at a point that would be in the extended horizontal plane of the head, if it were erect. In this case the hyperphoria and minus cyclopho- ria must be due to the superior rectus of that eye, while the cataphoria and the minus cyclophoria of the other eye must be due to its superior oblique. The corrective impulse of the hyperphoria must be sent to the inferior rectus which is favored in its action by the necessary elevated position of its visual axis. It must also receive nearly the whole of the impulse that comes from the second fusional innervation center; for the inferior rectus of the other eye needs but little, if any, of it. Thus are counteracted both the hyperphoria and the minus cyclophoria. The cataphoria and the minus cyclophoria of the other eye are counteracted by a corrective nervous impulse that is sent to its inferior oblique whose action is not favored HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 371 by the position that this eye must assume an elevated posterior pole. From what has been said above it will be observed that the tilting- of the head toward the cataphoric side when there is plus cyclophoria, and toward the hyper- phoric side when there is minus cyclophoria, is favorable only to the muscle that must correct the double error of the hyperphoric eye, thus showing- that the depressor muscles are in greater need of the help that comes from posing. The tilting- is really unfavorable to the muscle that must correct the double error of the cataphoric eye. It may be that a nervous impulse gets a readier response, in unfavorable positions of the head, from the muscles that elevate the eyes than from those that depress them. It is well known that when the eyes are closed in sleep, or even in meditation, they turn slightly up; and the same is true under anesthesia that is not profound. This peculiar endowment of the supervertors seems to be necessary in order that the cornea may be carried instantly, for protection, into the position of greatest security. It is doubtful if there is a symptom or sign, other than the posing of the head, that is peculiar to vertical im- balance. Excessive secretion of tears may be associated, in some unaccountable way, with hyperphoria and cata- phoria. 372 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. TREATMENT. Vertical imbalance associated with pseudo-esophoria or pseudo-exophoria may be dependent on it; and if so, it should be relieved by the same lenses that cure the lateral pseudo error. Since a double hyperphoria may be caused by abnormal action of the inferior obliques, excited by oblique astig- matism with the meridians of greatest curvature con- verging above, and since double cataphoria may be caused by abnormal action of the superior obliques, excited by oblique astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curv- ature diverging above, these errors may be relieved, in such cases, by the correcting plus or minus cylinders. Inherent vertical imbalance is made neither better nor worse by the lenses that correct focal errors. These must be treated by exercise, by prisms in positions of rest, or by operations. EXERCISE. Double hyperphoria may be treated by exercise of the inferior recti, which is best done by looking straight ahead and then down to an object on the floor five or six feet distant, then again straight ahead and then down again, repeating this at regular intervals of three, seconds. This straight-forward-to-floor exer- cise should be stopped short of fatigue, and should not be HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 373 continued longer than ten minutes. One exercise a day is sufficient. The exercise for double cataphoria is straight-forward- to-ceiling exercise, and should be carried out in the same manner as the straight-forward-to-floor exercise. Prism exercise alone is applicable when there is hyper- phoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other. The Fig- 47. prisms with which to begin the exercise should be weak, and gradually stronger ones should be used, but they should never be stronger than 2. Except for the cost, the prisms should be in pairs of equal strength. The hyperphoric set shown in the accompanying cut (Fig. 47) consists of five prisms, as follows: ?, 5, 1, l^ * and 2. 374 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. The apex of the prism must point toward the muscle to be exercised that is, it must be down for the hyper- phoric eye and up for the cataphoric eye. When the prisms are of unequal strength, after three or four days' use they should be transferred and the exercise continued for three or four days longer. Now the weaker prism (j) may be removed and the 1 prism put in its place. At the proper time the prism and the 1 prism should be transferred, and the exercise should be continued with them for three days, when the 2 prism should be re- moved and the l^ prism put in its place. The substitu- tions should thus continue until the 1^ and 2 prisms are in the frames. The exercise should be continued indefinitely with these prisms, transferring- them from side to side at regular intervals, always placing apex down for the hyperphoric eye and apex up for the cata- phoric eye. The muscles should be exercised rhythmic- ally by lowering and raising the frames containing the prisms, at intervals of three seconds. The exercise should be stopped short of fatigue and need not be continued longer than ten minutes. Once a day is sufficiently often to exercise. The object of fixation while exercising should be twenty feet distant, and should be sufficiently sharp in outline and bright to excite ready response of the muscles for the purpose of fusing the two displaced images. HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 375 Relieving- prisms for vertical imbalance are often of great use. Hyperphoria and cataphoria, uncompli- cated, of less than 1|, should be relieved, if possible, either by exercise or by rest prisms. Cases in which these errors are as great as 2 or greater, whether complicated or not, may obtain some relief from non- surg-ical means, but cannot be cured short of operations. If the vertical errors are as low as 1 and there is a complicating" cyclophoria, surgery is the thing 1 indi- cated. The sub-ducting- muscles stand in greater need of rest prisms than do the superducting- muscles, for the reason g-iven in the study of the posing 1 of the head, in that part of this chapter devoted to symptoms. If there were no other reason, it would be g-ood practice in many cases, not complicated with minus cyclophoria, to apply the whole prismatic correction to the hyperphoric eye; and certainly this should be the practice if there is a com- plicating- plus cyclophoria, however small in quantity. The superior rectus raising- the eye to overcome the effect of the prism, in the interest of binocular single vision, torts it in, thus aiding- the superior oblique to parallel the vertical axis of the eye with the median plane of the head. In the few cases of hyperphoria and cataphoria that are complicated by minus cyclophoria, the whole of the 376 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. prismatic effect should be applied to the cataphoric eye. The prism, with its base up before this eye, calls into action the inferior rectus in the interest of fusion; and thus aid is given the inferior oblique in its efforts to parallel the vertical axis of the eye with the median plane of the head. Those cases of hyperphoria and cataphoria that are not complicated by cyclophoria will be given more com- fort if the greater part (two-thirds) of prismatic effect is applied to the hyperphoric eye, than would be given if this rule were reversed, or even if the effect were equally divided between the two eyes. If the rules given above are observed, many cases will take even a full correction of the hyperphoria, and not less than half the full correction should ever be given. The purpose may be attained either by prisms or lenses rendered prismatic by decentration. Double cataphoria that causes the patient to hold his head too high should be given prisms or prismatic lenses, bases up, so as to let them assume a more nearly normal position of the head, provided there is no complicating plus cyclophoria. The effect should be equally divided between the two eyes. Patients suffering from double hyperphoria should be given prisms or prismatic lenses, bases down, except when there is a complicating minus cyclophoria. These HYPERPHORIA AXD CATAPHoRIA. 377 would enable the patient to carry his head comfortably in a more nearly erect position. Hyperphoria, single or double; cataphoria, single or double; and hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, caused bv malformation of the orbits, should always be given full prismatic correction of the error found in each eye. OPERATIVE TREATMENT. A double hyperphoria that cannot be relieved by prisms or by straight-forward-to-floor exercise, to the extent of enabling the patient to carry his head erect, instead of downcast, should be subjected to a partial tenotomy of both superior recti. If there is no complicating minus cyclophoria, the tenotomies should be central; but should this complication exist as it would if the superior recti are wholly at fault the tenotomies should be peripheral, including only the temporal fibers. Should there be a complicating plus cyclophoria as there would be if the inferior obliques were wholly to blame for the double hyperphoria peripheral tenotomies of the superior recti should be done, including only the nasal fibers. In either case, the operative effect should be equally divided between the two muscles. A double cataphoria does not so urgently demand operations, for high-headedness is not so objectionable as 378 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. the downcast look. The position of the head in double cataphoria is more favorable to the respiratory act than is the position caused by double hyperphoria another reason why operative interference is less urgent in double cataphoria. The lid pressure pressure of the upper lids against the globe is much less in double cataphoria than in double hyperphoria. Since great lid pressure is probably more favorable to the retention and develop- ment of germs, especially the trachoma germ, as sug- gested by Stevens, there is an additional reason for operating more frequently for double hyperphoria than for double cataphoria. Straight-forward-to-ceiling exercise, or prisms with their bases up, should always be tried in cases of double cataphoria; but should these fail to enable the patient to carry his head in the natural, erect position, a partial tenotomy of both inferior recti should be done, and these operations should be central, unless there is a complicat- ing cyclophoria. In cases in which there is a complicat- ing plus cyclophoria as there would be if the double cataphoria is caused by the inferior recti peripheral tenotomies should be done, including only the temporal fibers. In those cases complicated by a minus cyclopho- ria, the superior obliques are the cause; but since these muscles cannot be, or ought not to be, operated upon, peripheral tenotomies of the inferior recti, including only HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 379 the nasal fibers, should be done. The operative effect should be equally divided between the two inferior recti. Great care should be exercised, in operating- for double cataphoria, not to convert it into a double hyperphoria. Operations on the inferior recti are as easily done as on the superior recti. Since double cataphoria is preferable to double hyper- phoria, there is g-ood reason for always operating- first on the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye in cases in which there is hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other. While the tenotomy should never be com- plete, it should be more extensive when done with the view of lowering- the hyperphoric e} r e than when done for elevating the cataphoric eye; hence, th<, reason for doing- the first operation as set forth in the beg-inning- of this paragraph. After the first operation, the remaining- imbalance must be corrected by a partial tenotomy of the inferior rectus of the cataphoric eye. As in the lateral heterophorias, so in the vertical errors, tenotomies are indicated only in the sthenic forms. A superduction of 3 or less should never be diminished by lessening- the tension of a superior rectus; and a sub-duction of 3 or less likewise contraindicates a tenotomy. An asthenic hyperphoria and cataphoria demands, first of all, a short- ening- of the inferior rectus of the hyperphoric eye, by means of which the greater part of the effect should be 380 HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. accomplished, the remaining 1 part of the imbalance to be corrected later by a shortening 1 of the superior rectus of the cataphoric eye. In all cases of hyperphoria and cataphoria, uncompli- cated by cyclophoria, the operations, if partial tenoto- mies, should be central; and if shortenings, should be straight-forward, so as not to change the plane of rota- tion. The complication of plus cyclophoria calls for a peripheral tenotomy of the superior rectus of the hyper- phoric eye, including* only the nasal fibers, and a periph- eral tenotomy of the inferior rectus of the cataphoric eye, including only the temporal fibers. These two operations should be as nearly coextensive as possible, because of the desire to correct the cyclophoria. Should a case of this character be asthenic, the inferior rectus of the hyperphoric eye should be shortened in such a way as to carry its plane of rotation farther in, and the supe- rior rectus of the cataphoric eye should be so shortened as to carry its plane of rotation farther toward the tem- ple. The operative effect should be equally divided be- tween the two muscles. The complication of minus cyclophoria, which is rare, indicates a peripheral tenotomy of the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye, including only its temporal fibers, so as to carry its plane of rotation farther toward the nose, and a like operation on the nasal fibers of the in- HYPERPHORIA AND CATAPHORIA. 381 ferior rectus of the cataphoric eye, so as to carry its plane of rotation farther toward the temple. An equal effect should be attained by these two operations. If a case of this character should be asthenic, the inferior rectus of the hyperphoric eye shonld be so shortened as to carry its plane of rotation farther toward the temple, while the superior rectus of the cataphoric eye should be so shortened as to carry its plane of rotation farther toward the nose. The methods of doing- these operations are set forth in the chapter on heterophoria, and the after-treatment is also described in that chapter. CHAPTER VII. CYCLOPHORIA. CYCLOPHORIA is the tendency of the vertical axes of the eyes to lose parallelism with the median plane of the head. In the interest of binocular single vision this parallelism must be maintained by the oblique muscles, except in cases of oblique astigmatism. For this pur- pose there are four conjugate innervation brain-centers: (1) The sixth conjugate center sends an impulse to the two superior obliques to prevent divergence of the verti- cal axes when the point of view is in the extended median plane of the head, but below the extended horizontal plane; (2) the seventh conjugate center sends an impulse to the two inferior obliques to prevent convergence of the vertical axes when there is to be cardinal fixation above the horizontal plane; (3) the eighth conjugate cen- ter sends an impulse to the superior oblique of the right e} T e and inferior oblique of the left eye to prevent torsion when the point of fixation is obliquely up and to the right, or down and to the left; and (4) the ninth conju- gate center sends an impulse to the superior oblique of the left eye and inferior oblique of the right eye to pre- (382) CYCLOPHORIA. 383 vent torsion when the point of fixation is up and to the left, or down and to the right. Under the influence of one or another of these conjugate centers, parallelism of the vertical axes of the eyes with the median plane of the head is maintained, regardless of the direction of the point of fixation. In a normal condition of all the extrinsic ocu- lar muscles, the obliques accomplish this purpose with ease. Whenever conditions are such as to make it dif- ficult for the obliques to maintain this parallelism, ex- cept when under an excessive nervous tension, there is cyclophoria. When this condition was first described in the Archives of Ophthalmology, in its issue of January, 1891, the name given it was "insufficiency of the obliques," which was not inapt; for whatever may be the chief cause or causes of this error, the obliques are insufficient for the work of easily keeping the vertical axes of the eyes parallel with the median plane of the head. In 1893, in conformity with the terminology introduced by Stevens, the term "cyclophoria" was coined by Price. Plus cyclophoria means that the vertical axes of the eyes have a tendency from the median plane of the head; minus cyclophoria is "a tendency of these axes toward the median plane. For the same conditions Maddox uses the terms "plus torsion " and "minus torsion; " and Stevens, " plus decli- nation " and " minus declination." Either of these term? 384 CYCLOPHORIA. would be as good as those coined by Price, except for the desirableness of uniformity in terminology. While cyclophoria is most important as it pertains to the two eyes in their efforts to maintain binocular single vision, it is, nevertheless, a factor for disturbance in monocular vision. In order that the law of direction may not be interfered with, in vision with one eye, its vertical axis must always be parallel with the median plane of the head; and, necessarily, its transverse axis must always lie in the plane of the primary isogonal circle. There are two kinds of cyclophoria viz., symmetrical and non-symmetrical. Symmetrical cyclophoria is either plus or minus for both eyes; non-symmetrical cyclophoria is plus for one eye and minus for the other. Rarely there may be a plus or minus cyclophoria for one eye, while the obliques of the other perform their work easily. Plus cyclophoria is by far more common than minus cyclophoria. CAUSES OF CYCLOPHORIA. The cause may be wholly in the obliques. The nearer the attachment of an oblique muscle is to the equator, the greater is its torsioning power; while attachment of an oblique nearer the poste- rior pole of the eye gives it less torsioning power. At- tachment of both inferior obliques nearer the equator than that of the superior obliques, would give a plus CYCLOPHORIA. 385 cyclophoria, the muscles themselves being normal in de- velopment. When the superior obliques are attached nearer the equator than are the inferior obliques, a minus cyclophoria would result. Granting- that the attach- ments are correct, hyper-development of the inferior ob- liques or subnormal development of the superior obliques would give a plus cyclophoria; while hyper-development of the superior obliques or subnormal development of the inferior obliques would cause a minus cyclophoria. This presupposes that the innervations are normal. Hyper-development of the seventh conjugate innerva- tion center, or subnormal development of the sixth, would cause a plus cyclophoria; while hyper-development of the sixth or subnormal development of the seventh conjugate center, would cause a minus cyclophoria. This presup- poses that the muscles themselves are normal in both structure and attachment. In either case the plus cy- clophoria would be complicated by a double hyperphoria, and the minus cyclophoria would be complicated by a double cataphoria. A too high attachment of the interni or a too low at- tachment of the extern! would cause a minus cyclopho- ria, while a too low attachment of the interni or a too high attachment of the extern! would cause a plus cyclo- phoria. When there is a normal attachment of the in- terni, there can result from their action no cyclophoria. 386 CYCLOPHORIA. The superior and inferior recti constitute the only remaining* source of symmetrical cyclophoria. When these muscles are normal in structure and attachment, there can be no cyclophoria resulting from their ac- tion. A double hyperphoria due to hyper-development of the superior recti or subnormal development of the inferior recti, gives a minus cyclophoria; while hyper- developed inferior recti or subnormally developed supe- rior recti, in causing double cataphoria, also cause plus cyclophoria. Non -symmetrical cyclophoria is a tendency to parallel deviation of the vertical axes of the eyes, being plus for one eye and minus for the other. This tendency may be to the right or to the left. If this kind of tendency should become a turning, diplopia would not result, but there would be interference with the law of direction. Because of this interference the weaker obliques (supe- rior of one eye and inferior of the other) are kept in a state of nervous tension, that they may keep the vertical axes of the eyes parallel with the median plane of the head. The corrective impulse comes from the eighth con- jugate center when the tendency of the vertical axes is toward the right, and from the ninth center when the tendency is toward the left. When the tendency is toward the right, rotation of the eyes obliquely up and to the right, or down and to the left, is more dif- CYCLOPHORIA. 387 ficult than rotation up and to the left, or down and to the right; and vice versa, when the tendency is toward the left. The obliques may cause this condition. To do so the superior oblique of one eye must be too strong for its in- ferior oblique, or the former must be attached nearer the equator than the latter; while the inferior oblique of the other eye is either stronger than its superior oblique or is attached nearer the equator. In such a case there would be not only parallel cyclophoria, but there would be also a cataphoria of the one eye and a hyperphoria of the other. Parallel cyclophoria can be caused by the interni when one is attached in greater part above the transverse plane of its eye, while the other is attached in greater part below this plane. There would also result a hyperphoria of the one eye and a cataphoria of the other. Faulty attachment of the externi, the one too high and the other too low, would cause parallel cyclo- phoria. In such a case there would also be a hyperpho- ria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other. When hy- perphoria of one eye is caused by a too strong superior rectus and cataphoria of the other is caused by a too powerful inferior rectus, parallel cyclophoria will also result, the tendency being- to the right when there is left hyperphoria and to the left when there is right hyper- phoria. 388 CYCLOPHORIA. TESTS. Cyclophoria was first discovered in 1890 by means of a Maddox double prism which was being used for deter- mining- an imbalance of the lateral recti. The patient was asked if the middle candle was in a vertical line with the upper and lower candles. She stated that the lower candle was not directly under the upper one, although the axis of the double prism was vertical, or so judged by the eye of the operator. The axis of the prism had to be tilted 5 or more toward the temple before the patient claimed that the upper and lower candles were in a vertical line. This showed clearly that the vertical retinal meridian was inclined toward the temple and to a greater extent than Helmholtz had taught as normal. At once a line was drawn across a card and held before the patient. She saw two lines with the eye before which the double prism was held, and these lines were parallel. The other eye was then uncovered, when she saw a third line between the other two lines, but not parallel with them. The middle line was seen by the left eye, and it was seen inclined down to the right. Other cases were investigated, about twenty-five per cent of them showing the same error found in the first patient, which was plus cyclophoria. The first publication was made in the Archives of Ophthalmology, Vol. XX., No. 1, page 105. This paper was gloomy in that it presented no CYCLOPHORIA. 389 prospect of either prevention or cure. From the time of the discovery of cyclophoria, in 1890, up to May, 1892, many cases had been found, but none of them had been treated; nor was it thought possible, up to this time, that any curative measure would ever be devised. On May 17, 1892, the thought of developing weak oblique muscles by exercising them with cylinders first presented itself. This thought was put into practice at once, and the re- sults were gratifying. This led to the presentation of a second paper which was read before the Section of Ophthalmology of the American Medical Association at the Detroit meeting, in June, 1892. The double prism will always show cyclophoria when the test- object is a horizontal line. The kind of cyclo- phoria whether plus or minus is easily ascertained in this way, but its quantity cannot be measured. Substi- tuting a dot for the line, the error may be measured by revolving the double prism until the two dots seen through the prism are in a vertical line. The extent of inclination of the axis of the prism would be equal to the amount of the cyclophoria. In using the double prism for this measurement test of cyclophoria, the operator must be careful to have the axis of the prism vertical in the beginning and note its inclination at the end of the test. He need not be so careful to have the patient's head erect, for the result will be the same whether the 390 CYCLOPHORIA. head is erect or inclined. However, since tests of some of the eye muscles require that the head shall be erect, it is better to have it thus in all tests. The cut used to illustrate the first paper is reproduced here, together with the descriptive text: " Place a double prism, axis vertical, before one eye, the other for the moment being- covered, and ask the patient to look at a horizontal line on a card held sixteen inches away. The effect of the double prism (each 6) is to make the line appear to be two, each parallel with the other. The other eye is now uncovered, and a third line is seen between the other two, with which it should be perfectly parallel. "While a change of the position of the axis, of the double prism, from the vertical toward the horizontal, will alter the distance between the lines, their direction will be unchanged hence, no loss of parallelism. This fact admits of a little carelessness in the placing of the prism in the trial frames, though the axis should be vertical, so as to give the maximum distance between the two extreme lines. "If there is a want of harmony on the part of the oblique muscles, this test will show it at once in a want of parallelism of the middle line with the two other lines, the right end of the middle line pointing toward the bottom line and the left end toward the top line, or CYCLOPHORIA. 391 vice versa, depending- on the nature of the individual case. " Consider the eye before which no prism is held as the one under test. With the double prism before the right eye, the patient is asked about the position and Fig. 48. Fig- 49- Fig. 50. Fig- Fig- 52. the direction of the middle line. It may be nearer the bottom line, thus showing left hyperphoria; or, again, it may extend farther to the right than the other two, and not so far to the left, thus showing exophoria; or vice versa, showing esophoria. 392 CYCLOPHORIA. "If the right ends of the middle and bottom lines con- verge while the left ends diverge, the superior oblique of the left eye is at once shown to be in a state of under- action. Fig. 48 represents such a test of the left eye; Fig. 49 shows a test of the left eye when the inferior oblique is the too weak muscle; Fig. 50 represents a test of the right eye, the loss of the parallelism between the lines being due to under-action of its superior oblique; Fig. 51, the same condition of the inferior oblique of the right eye; Fig. 52 represents a test of both eyes when there is perfect equilibrium of the oblique muscles." The single prism of 6, with its base up or down be- fore one eye, the test object being a horizontal line on a blackboard at twenty feet distance, or on a card at the reading distance, w r ill double the line. If the two are not parallel, there is cyclophoria. The false line inclined toward the opposite side shows plus cyclophoria; the same line inclined toward the corresponding side shows minus cyclophoria. The quantity of the cyclophoria cannot be measured with the single prism by substituting a dot for the line, as in the use of the double prism. The rotary prism of either the Wilson or the monocu- lar phorometer can easily show the existence of cyclo- phoria, or its absence. As with other prisms, the test object is a line. The rotary prism is adjusted for taking CYCLOPHORIA. 393 sub-duction and superduction. When it is rotated up or down beyond the point of possible fusion, the line be- comes double. If they converge at one end or the other, there is cyclophoria plus if the false line is inclined to- ward the opposite side, minus if it is inclined toward the same side. Rotating the prism slowly so as to carry the index toward zero, the two lines fuse, first at one end and then quickly fuse throughout. The rotary prism cannot possibly measure the amount of cyclophoria. The Stevens clinoscope both detects and measures any form of cyclophoria. The opaque discs with a single pin, with the head in the center, drawn on each, should be placed so that the point may be up for one eye and down for the other. E}ach pin should be vertical, and the in- strument should be so adjusted as to allow easy fusion of the heads of the pins. When the two discs appear as one, the two pins should be the vertical diameter of the fused disc. If the one pin is a radius pointing ob- liquely in one direction and the other is a radius pointing obliquely in the other direction (one toward the right and the other toward the left, making an oblique diam- eter), there is plus cyclophoria of one eye and minus cy- clophoria of the other. If the upper pin is seen by the left eye and the lower pin is seen by the right eye, the two pins pointing to the right would show plus cyclo- phoria; while minus cyclophoria would be shown by the 394 CYCLOPHORIA. two pins pointing 1 to the left. If the top pin is vertical and the bottom one points to the right, there is plus cy- clophoria of the right eye alone; or, if the bottom pin points to the left, while the top one is vertical, there is minus cyclophoria of the right eye alone. When both pins are oblique, the tubes to which the discs are fastened should be revolved until the two pins are vertical, forming apparently the vertical diameter of the fused disc. The index connected with each tube will point to the mark on the scale indicating the quantity of the error for each eye. The errors of the obliques could be detected with equal ease and measured with as much exactness, if the discs were placed so that the pins would be horizontal, the one seen by the left eye pointing to the left and the one seen by the right eye pointing to the right, their heads being fused. To eyes whose oblique muscles are perfectly balanced the pins would appear as the horizontal diame- ter of the fused disc. If the two pins appear to point downward, there is minus cyclophoria; if they appear to point upward, there is plus cyclophoria. If the left pin points downward, while the right pin points upward, there is minus cyclophoria of the left eye and plus cyclo- phoria of the right eye (parallel cyclophoria). In either case, rotation of the two tubes until the pins appear to be horizontal, constituting the apparent horizontal diam- eter of the fused disc, measures the error. CYCLOPHORIA. 395 The cyclo-phorometer will also detect and measure both symmetrical and non-symmetrical cyclophoria. The in- strument must be perfectly level, and the index of each triple rod must stand at zero. The 5 prism, base up, must be placed in the slot behind one rod, while a red glass may be put in the slot behind the other rod. The test object must be a candle, gas jet, or small electric light. The red streak above and the yellow streak be- low must be made even by the regulating screw. To perfectly balanced eyes these streaks would appear par- allel. If the red streak is seen by the right e} T e and the two converge at the left, there is plus cyclophoria; if they converge at the right, there is minus cyclophoria. If they appear parallel, but inclined, there is plus cyclo- phoria of one eye and minus cyclophoria of the other. If one is horizontal and the other is inclined, there is cy- clophoria of one eye alone. The disc containing the rods should be turned until both streaks are perfectly hori- zontal and, therefore, parallel. If the index of each stands in the nasal arc when the streaks are made to appear horizontal, there is plus cyclophoria; and if they stand in the temporal arcs, there is minus cyclophoria. The quantity of the error in each eye is measured by the arc traversed by the index and is shown on the scale. If only one rod is turned until the two streaks become par- allel, but not horizontal, the quantity marked by the in- 396 CYCLOPHORIA. dex is the sum of the cyclophoria of the two eyes. If there is parallel cyclophoria, the quantity is shown by revolving- the two rods until the two streaks are hori- zontal. Cyclo-duction can be taken either with the clinoscope or the cyclo-phorometer. If the clinoscope is used, the discs, with a diameter drawn across each, should be at- tached. They can be set with the diameters either ver- tical or horizontal. When vertical, revolving the tubes so that the lines shall deviate from each other above will put into action the inferior obliques. If only one tube is revolved, only one inferior oblique is called into action, while revolving both tubes will call into action both in- ferior obliques. The normal duction power of the ob- liques is not so well known as that of the recti. It is somewhere between 7 and 14 for one, and 22 or less, for both inferior obliques. Revolving the tubes so that the lines converge above puts the superior obliques to the strength test. If only one tube is revolved, only one superior oblique is called on for a fusion effort; but if both are revolved, both eyes must be cyclo-ducted by the two superior obliques. The fusion power of the superior obliques is less than that of the inferior obliques, but how much less normally is not known. In plus cyclophoria, minus cyclo-duction is di- minished and plus cyclo-duction is increased; while in CYCLOPHORIA. 397 minus cyclophoria, plus cyclo-duction is less and minus cyclo-duction is greater. In taking- either minus or plus cyclo-duction the revolution of the cylinders must be stopped the moment the lines begin to be seen separate- ly. The index will point to the number indicating the extent of the fusion rotation. Placing the discs so that the two diameters shall be horizontal, both plus and minus cyclo-duction can be taken as easily and accurately as, if not more accurate- ly than, when these lines are vertical. Rotating the tubes so that these lines are made to point downward at their outer ends will call into fusion activity the two inferior obliques, while rotating them so that they shall point upward will excite into fusion activity the superior ob- liques. When the cyclo-phorometer is used for measuring the fusion power of the obliques, the instrument should be adjusted as for testing for cyclophoria. As soon as the rods are so rotated that the one streak of light is di- rectly under the other, both the displacing prism and the red glass should be removed. At once the two lines would be fused into one horizontal line. Revolving one rod so that the index shall move in the nasal arc puts to the test the inferior oblique of that eye; revolving both rods so that each index shall be in the nasal arc puts both inferior obliques to the test. When the two streaks 398 CYCLOPHORIA. of light are well defined and exactly alike as to width and color, the plus cyclo-duction should be as great with this instrument as with the clinoscope. Revolving- the rods so that the index of each shall move in the temporal arc, excites into fusion activit}- both superior obliques. If one alone is to be tested, only the one rod should be revolved, the other being- allowed to stand at zero. Thus plus and minus cyclo-duction should be taken in all cases of cyclophoria; and even when there is no cyclophoria, these should be taken and noted, so that a standard may be attained. The first instrument for taking- cyclo-duction was in- vented by Dr. C. H. Perry, of Oneida, N. Y., and for this purpose is but little, if at all, inferior to the clino- scope or the cyclo-phorometer. A description of this in- strument and its use was published by the Ophthalmic Record, in its issue of November, 1895, in the inventor's own lang-uag-e. It reads as follows: "Fig 1 . 53 represents a stereoscopic card, to which are centrally pivoted two thin discs, each three inches in diameter, their centers being- three inches apart in a hor- izontal line. "These discs are mashed tog-ether at their point of contact by a single interlocking slot in each. " The left disc is moved by a lever passing under the right, and is provided with an index, which shows, on a CYCLOPHORIA. 399 graduated scale at the right-hand of the card, the num- ber of degrees that each disc is rotated. Having equal diameters, and being geared together, the}- move syn- chronously equal distances, but in opposite directions. . 53- Horizontally over the center of each disc is printed a word, and vertically through each of said centers is drawn a line. "When this apparatus is seen in a stereoscope and the discs are rotated by moving the lever, the two words will remain blended while each disc moves through an arc of about 5 (and much more in some subjects); and if attention is given to the perpendicular lines, they will appear as a single line during the rotation of about the same arc. If used with due care, this instrument gives a practically accurate measurement not only of the rela- 400 CYCLOPHORIA. tive, but of the absolute, power of rotation in each direc- tion." Cyclo-version is impossible, since voluntary rotation of the eyes around the visual axes cannot be accomplished. In the study of the causes of cyclophoria, all of the complications have been considered. Under the head "Treatment" they will be referred to ag-ain, since the treatment, whether surg-ical or otherwise, is largely de- termined by these complications. SYMPTOMS. Any one or several of the symptoms mentioned in the chapter on heterophoria may result from cyclophoria. Vertigo and nausea are more commonly found associated with cyclophoria than with any other form of heteropho- ria. As already shown in the study of hyperphoria and cataphoria, it is plus cyclophoria that causes the tilting- of the head toward the cataphoric side, and minus cyclo- phoria that causes the tilting" of the head toward the hyperphoric side. Symmetrical plus cyclophoria associated with double hyperphoria causes hig'h-headedness, for the reason that the superior obliques can more easily counteract a plus cyclophoria when the visual axes are depressed below the extended horizontal plane of the head. Symmetrical minus cyclophoria with double cataphoria causes the pa- CYCLOPHORIA. 401 tient to carry his head thrown forward, giving- a down- cast appearance, because, in this position, the inferior oblique can counteract more easily the minus cyclophoria. TREATMENT. Rest cylinders, exercise cylinders, and operations on the recti constitute the means for curing- cyclophoria. When cyclophoria was discovered, in 1890, a cure was thought to be impossible. In 1892 the first case was treated with exercise cylinders. In 1888 the late Dr. H. Culbertson, in a paper read by him before the Section of Ophthalmology of the American Medical Association, embodied the idea of the usefulness of cylinders not only for the correction of astigmatism, but also for giving rest to weak oblique muscles. The title of that paper was "Binocular Astigmatism," and in it he advocated rotat- ing cylinders out of the positions indicated in the monoc- ular test, so that distortions of objects might be over- come. He claimed, and correctly so, that many of his patients obtained comfort from this procedure. He claimed no basis for this practice other than empiri- cism, whereas there is a scientific basis for it, which will be given farther on. The germ of the idea of operating on the recti so as to help weak obliques is embodied in language found in the Ophthalmic Record, in its issue of March, 1893. These 402 CYCLOPHORIA. are the words: "/ doing- advancement operations on the recti muscles, one of the chief dangers is turning' the eye- ball on its antero-posterior diameter so as to throw un- bearable strain on either the superior or inferior oblique muscles." At once it should have occurred to the author of the quotation just made that, naturally, the recti might be so attached as to do the very same thing 1 that is, de- velop a cyclophoria. While in attendance on the first Pan-American Medi- cal Congress, Dr. Swan M. Burnett suggested to the author that the superior and inferior recti might have such a scleral attachment as to throw an undue amount of strain on the obliques. On hearing- this statement the author made this suggestion: " If you are correct, some cases of insufficiency of the obliques can be cured by dividing the offending- fibers of the inferior (or supe- rior} recti." This was published in the first edition of "New Truths in Ophthalmology" (1893), page 41. REST CYLINDERS. These can be given for either plus or minus cyclophoria even when there is no astigmatism to be corrected. The objection to this practice is that, while they give rest to the weak obliques, they, at the same time, make images on the retinas less distinct. It is the distorting, or it would probably be better to say " the displacing, " of images that gives rest to the weaker obliques. It is no longer denied that, in astigmatism, CYCLOPHORIA. 403 all lines not parallel with one or the other of the two principal meridians have their images displaced toward the meridian of greatest curvature. To fuse such dis- placed images (displaced in opposite directions) there must be cyclotropia, just as there must be esotropia in order to fuse images that are thrown to the temporal side of the maculas by prisms with their bases out. The law of corresponding retinal points, which is su- preme, compels the cyclotropia of oblique astigmatism, notwithstanding it must interfere with the law of direc- tion, in that the vertical axes of the eyes are made either to converge or diverge above. Artificial oblique astig- matism produces the same changes in images of vertical and horizontal lines as does natural oblique astigmatism. When there is plus cyclophoria, natural oblique astig- matism of 1 or 2 D, with the meridians of greatest curvature converging above, is often attended by more comfort, because of the rest it gives to the superior ob- liques, than the correcting cylinders would give; for when the correction is given, the image of every line in space is in a plane with the line itself, and to fuse these images the weak superior obliques must parallel the vertical axes of the eyes with the median plane of the head. To do this their normal tension must be sup- plemented by a nervous tension which they poorly bear. What natural astigmatism will do, artificial astigmatism 404 CYCLOPHORIA. will accomplish. Only weak cylinders should be used for non-astigmatics, with the view of resting- weak oblique muscles. More than 1 D, whether the axis be made to incline little or much, would blur objects un- necessarily. Cylinders of .50 D are usually strong- enough, especially when their axes are made to incline far toward maximum points, in the proper arcs. For plus cyclophoria the arc of distortion for plus cylinders is the lower nasal arc; for minus cylinders, the lower temporal arc. The maximum distortion by the cylin- der is accomplished when its axis stands at 45 from the vertical. For minus cyclophoria the arc of distor- tion for plus cylinders is the lower temporal arc; for minus cylinders, the lower nasal arc. For non-astig- matics each arc of distortion is 90, the distortion or dis- placement gradually increasing as the axis of the cylin- der is carried up to the midway point (45) of the arc, and then gradually grows less as the axis is carried on toward the horizontal, at which point there can be no distortion. The most useful cylinder is a plus .50 D when there is esophoria as well as cyclophoria, or a minus .50 D when there are exophoria and cyclophoria. The quantity of the cyclophoria determines the location of the axes of the cylin- ders, but in no case need they be placed farther from the vertical than 45, for this is the point where they accom- plish the maximum distortion, thereby securing, for the CYCLOPHORIA. 405 weak obliques, the greatest amount of rest. Given a case of plus cyclophoria that is also esophoric, but non- astigmatic, to obtain rest for the weak superior obliques a plus .50 D cylinder should be given for each eye, placing the axis of the right cylinder at some point between 90 and 135 or at the latter, while placing the axis of the left cylinder at some point between 90 and 45 or at the latter. These cylinders will give additional comfort to nearly all cases of this kind. It is better, however, to cure these cases either by ex- ercising the weak superior obliques or by operating on the interni. If there is a hyperphoria of one eye and a cataphoria of the other, as well as plus cyclophoria, the rest cylinder should be applied only to the cataphoric eye. The infe- rior oblique, in torting this eye out for fusing images with the fellow eye, would also elevate the eye, counteracting the cataphoria. For a like reason, the rest cylinder should be applied only to the hyperphoric eye for the relief of minus cyclophoria. But it would be better practice to cure the plus cyclophoria by a marginal tenotomy (on nasal side) of the superior rectus of the hyperphoric eye, or by exercising both superior obliques. Cylinders given for the correction of astigmatism, ob- lique or non-oblique, may be so placed as to give rest to weak superior obliques when there is plus cyclophoria, 406 CYCLOPHORIA. or to weak inferior obliques when there is minus cyclo- phoria. If the astigmatism is vertical and hyperopic, the arc of distortion by the plus cylinder for the superior oblique is 90, and that for the inferior oblique is also 90, their sum being 180; if the astigmatism is oblique, the meridian of greatest curvature of the right eye being at 60 and that of the left eye at 120, the arc of distor- tion by the correcting cylinders for the superior obliques will be 30, and that of the inferior obliques will be 150, their sum being 180. If the meridian of greatest curva- ture of the right eye is at 45, and of the left eye at 135, there is no arc of distortion for the superior obliques, but the arc of distortion for the inferior obliques is 180. The center of the arc of distortion by plus cylinders for the superior obliques is at 45 in the lower temporal arcs, and for the inferior obliques is at 45 in the lower nasal arcs. This is reversed in the use of minus cylinders. This will be elucidated further in the chapter on cy- clotropia. Prom what has just been said, it will be understood that, when plus cylinders are used for correcting astig- matic errors,, their axes must be turned from the point indicated by the astigmatism toward the center of the lower nasal arc for the relief of weak superior obliques in plus cyclophoria, and toward the center of the lower temporal arc for the relief of weak inferior obliques in CYCTvOPHOKIA. 407 minus cyclophoria. The extent of the displacement of the axes depends both on the strength of the cylinders and the quantity of the cyclophoria. Rarely should this displacement be more than 5 if the cylinders are plus 1 D, or stronger; but weaker cylinders may be revolved much farther. Culbertson displaced the axes of his cylinders with the view of correcting- the appearance of slanting floors, leaning walls, and distorted figures, without any refer- ence at all to the oblique muscles; but, after all, the benefit derived was from giving rest to the weak ob- liques. In vertical astigmatism, the cylinders do not cause the floor to slant or the wall to lean; and yet it is just as essential to displace the axes of the correcting cylinders in the proper arcs, when there is cyclophoria, as it is to displace the axes of cylinders correcting oblique astigmatism. In 1894, Nettleship, and probably others on the staff of the Royal Ophthalmic Hospital, London, was in the habit of directing that the axes of cylinders should be placed at 90 or 180, when accurate measurements showed that the two principal meridians were removed only a few degrees from these points. When asked why he did this, he said that some patients derived more com- fort from the displaced cylinders. There was then pres- ent a female patient who could not wear her cylinders 408 CYCLOPHORIA. thus displaced. Inquiry elicited the fact that she had been given plus cylinders axes, 90 for each eye, when the record showed that the meridian of greatest curvature of the right eye was at 100, and of the left eye at 80. These cylinders were displaced in the arcs of distortion for the superior obliques, which were evidently too weak to bear the consequent over-action. In that case, it would have been better to have displaced these axes farther from the vertical instead of to it. Better than displacing the axes of correcting cylinders, so that they may give rest to the weak oblique muscles, is to make these muscles strong by exercise, or else cor- rect the cyclophoria by operating on one or more of the recti. EXERCISE OF THE OBLIQUES. This can be accomplished by the Perry cards (see Fig. 53) used in the stereoscope. This would require the time of the surgeon or his assistant, throughout each exercise, for revolving the cards. Depressing the handle would make the printed word incline toward the opposite side; and to keep the two words fused, the superior obliques would be called into action. Again, raising the handle, so that the pointer shall stand at zero, will bring the two words into a horizontal line, and thus cause the superior obliques to relax. Repeating these steps, alternate con- traction and relaxation of the superior obliques can be CYCLOPHORIA. 409 accomplished. This exercise stopped short of fatigue would tend to strengthen these muscles, and thus cure the plus cyclophoria. In minus cyclophoria the discs would have to be so ro- tated as to make the words incline toward the corre- sponding- side, in order to call into action the inferior ob- liques. The Stevens clinoscope also can be used for exercising* the obliques; but this, too, would require the time of the surgeon or his assistant. The discs used should be those marked with the diameter, and not with the radius, un- less the two radii should be made both to point in the same direction. The former would be better. The discs could be placed with the diameters either vertical or horizontal. Revolving 1 the two tubes, so that the indicators would point toward each other, would call into action the superior oblique muscles; but when made to diverge from each other, the inferior obliques would be called into action. Reversing- the revolution, in either case, so that each indicator would stand at zero, would bring- the obliques into the state of rest. In this way rhythmic exercise of the obliques may be accomplished, and a plus or minus cyclophoria cured. The cyclo-phorometer likewise can be used for exer- cising- the obliques; but this, too, would require the time of the surgeon or his assistant. It should be adjusted 410 CYCLOPHORIA. for easy fusion of the two streaks of light, as if for the purpose of taking- the cyclo-duction. Revolving 1 the two rods, so that the indicators would move in the temporal arcs, would call into action the superior obliques; and revolving- them so that the indicators would move in the nasal arcs would call into action the inferior obliques. In either case, reversing- the motion, so that the indicators may be made to stand at zero, would cause relaxation of these muscles. The revolutions could be repeated so as to cause rhythmic exercise of the obliques, and thus cure cyclophoria. The displacement should not be more than half that for cyclo-duction in either of these methods. Whether the one or the other of these means should be used, the exercise should not cause fatigue, and in no case should it be continued longer than ten minutes. It need not be repeated oftener than once a day. The only means for exercising the oblique muscles, which a patient can use without assistance, consists of a pair of cylinders set in circular rims, so that they may be turned in the proper directions for displacing a hori- zontal line so as to call into action the proper muscles in the treatment of cyclophoria. The frames for this pur- pose are made of German silver, with circular rims. These rims are deeply grooved to allow a free rotation of the lenses. The rims are marked at points fifteen degrees apart, from 90 to 45, in either the lower tern- CYCLOPHORIA. 411 poral or lower nasal quadrant, depending- on the pair of muscles affected. The cylinders used are usually plus 1.50 D., and the axis of each is plainly marked, as shown in the cuts. The frames are not marked, nor are the cylinders cut, except by the oculist's order. Fig-. 54 represents a pair of exercise cylinders ordered for a patient's own use, whose superior obliques are in- sufficient. The rims, as shown, are marked in the lower Fig- 54- temporal quadrant, at four points fifteen degrees apart- three of which are numbered 1, 2, and 3. The cylinders, whose axes are distinctly marked, can be readily revolved- The patient is directed to place the mark on each lens at the notch marked No. 1. Placing- them now before her eyes, she is instructed to look at a horizontal line eigfht or ten feet distant for five seconds, then without them for five seconds, then ag-ain with them for five seconds, and so on 412 CYCLOPHORIA. for five minutes. Now the two lenses are to be revolved so that their marks point to the No. 2 notch on the rim. The line is now looked at, as above, for three minutes. Now the last change in position of the lenses is made by revolving- their marks to notch No. 3, the point of maximum action. The patient again looks at the line, as above, for two minutes, which ends the exercise for that day ten minutes in all. The marking of the rims must be in the lower nasal quadrant when the patient has insufficiency of the inferior obliques, the exercise lenses to be plus cylinders. The revolution is made in the direction of the notching- in both classes of cases. The points at which to stop and the time to look at the line are the same for both plus and minus cyclophoria. The exercise is accomplished by rais- ing- and lowering- the frames at intervals of three seconds. This exercise should not be carried to the point of fa- tig-ue, nor should it be continued long-er than ten minutes. Once a day is sufficiently often to resort to the exer- cise. The lig-htest work is done when the axes of the cylinders stand at the first notch, and it is continued long-est; while the heaviest work is demanded when the axes stand at the hig-hest notch, and for this reason it is continued the shortest time. It is clear that these cylinders are intended for the production of artificial oblique astig-matism, which must CYCLOPHORIA. 413 effect the same changes in images as are found in natural oblique astigmatism. Plus cylinders revolved as shown in Fig. 54 make the meridians of greatest refraction diverge from each other above; for the axes of the cylin- ders, representing the meridians of least curvature, con- verge above. These cylinders make horizontal lines dip toward the opposite side. To keep the images fused, the superior obliques must tort the eyes in. Raising the frames, the images of the horizontal line are no longer oblique, hence the muscles that torted the eyes in must now relax. If minus cylinders are used, their axes should be placed in the nasal arcs of the frames for exercising the superior obliques, and in the temporal arcs for exercising the in- ferior obliques. The axis of the minus cylinder repre- sents the meridian of most refraction, hence the need for rotating it in a direction different from that for the plus cylinder. The cylinders, whether plus or minus, may be of any strength from .50 D to 1.50 D. Rarely should the cylinder be stronger than 1.50 D. These cylinders were first used for exercising the ob- liques on May 17, 1892. The first case was cured of a plus cyclophoria after a reasonably short time of faith- ful exercise, and remains cured to this day September 26, 1901. 414 CYCLOPHORIA. OPERATIVE TREATMENT. A plus cyclophoria uncomplicated by any other form of imbalance, if high in degree and unrelievable by non- surgical means, should be treated by operating on both superior recti, dividing only their nasal fibers; or by operating on both inferior recti, shortening or advancing their nasal fibers. In either case, the cyclophoria would be cured and double cataphoria would be developed. A plus cyclophoria complicated by a double hyperpho- ria should be relieved by cutting the nasal fibers of both superior recti, which should result in a cure of both con- ditions. A plus cyclophoria complicated by double cataphoria should be treated by dividing the temporal fibers of both inferior recti. A plus cyclophoria complicated by right hyperphoria and left cataphoria calls for a division of the nasal fibers of the right superior rectus and the temporal fibers of the left inferior rectus. These operations should cure both conditions. A plus cyclophoria complicated by sthenic esophoria should be treated by dividing the lower fibers of both in- terni; but when the esophoria is asthenic, the operation should be a shortening or advancement of the lower fibers of both externi. A plus cyclophoria complicated by sthenic exophoria CYCLOPHORiA. 415 calls for a division of the upper fibers of both externi; but when the exophoria is asthenic, the upper fibers of both interni either should be shortened or advanced. A plus cyclophoria complicated by sthenic esophoria, right hyperphoria, and left cataphoria, demands that the lower fiber, of the left internus should be divided, so as both to elevate the cataphoric eye and tort it in. If there remains some of both the plus cyclophoria and right hy- perphoria, the nasal fibers of the right superior rectus should be cut. A plus cyclophoria complicated by sthenic exophoria, right hyperphoria, and left cataphoria, calls for a division of the upper fibers of the right externus, so as both to depress the hyperphoric eye and tort it in. Should there remain some of the plus cyclophoria and left cataphoria, the temporal fibers of the left inferior rectus should be severed. Very rarely there is minus cyclophoria, either compli- cated or uncomplicated, that calls for surgical treatment. It only must be remembered that the part of a muscle cut, shortened, or advanced for plus cyclophoria must remain intact when the condition is minus cyclopho- ria; and that the margins left intact in the treatment of plus cyclophoria must be either divided, shortened, or advanced for minus cyclophoria. Plus cyclophoria ol the right eye and minus cyclopho- 21. 416 CYCLOPHORIA. riaof the left eye (parallel cyclophoria), if uncomplicated, calls for a division of the nasal fibers of the right superior rectus and the temporal fibers of the left superior rectus. While these operations would parallel the vertical axes of the eyes with the median plane of the head, they would develop a double cataphoria. A case of this kind, complicated by right hyperphoria and left cataphoria, calls for a division of the nasal fibers of the right su- perior rectus and the nasal fibers of the left inferior rectus. The obliques themselves should never be subjected to an operation for cyclophoria; but, as will be shown in the next chapter, the inferior oblique may be divided when there is plus cyclotropia. CHAPTER VIII. COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. THE law governing- the oblique muscles is that they must keep the vertical axes of the two eyes parallel with the median plane of the head. They do this in obedience to the unalterable law of corresponding- retinal points, and must do it in all states of refraction not causing- a displacement, in opposite directions, of the two retinal imag-es as related to the one object. They must also maintain this parallelism of the vertical axes in those eyes whose refractive condition displaces both im- ag-es, but in the same direction and to the same extent. In emmetropia, hyperopia, and myopia there is no dis- placement of imag-es; hence the law of corresponding- retinal points is satisfied when the obliques obey the subordinate law governing- them that is, when they parallel the vertical axes with the median plane of the head. In vertical and horizontal astig-matism there is no displacement of imag-es of vertical and horizontal lines lines that are parallel with the two principal me- ridians while imag-es of oblique lines are displaced, but (417) 418 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. in the same direction and to the same extent in the two eyes; hence the law of corresponding- retinal points can be satisfied only when the subordinate law governing 1 the obliques is obeyed. In oblique astigmatism, with the meridians of great- est curvature either diverging 1 or converging- above, the imag-es of vertical and horizontal lines are displaced so that they no long-er bear a proper relationship to the lines themselves; hence the imag-es must fall on non- corresponding- retinal points more properly, lines. In such eyes, no line in space can have both imag-es prop- erly related to it, for a line that would be parallel with the meridian of greatest curvature of one eye would not be parallel with the meridian of greatest curvature of the other; therefore, while in the former the line and its image would be properly related, in the latter this could not be, for the imag-e would be displaced. The two imag-es of no line can fall on corresponding- retinal parts when, in oblique astig-matism, the meridians of greatest curvature are not parallel. To harmonize these images, and satisfy as best possible, but never perfectly, the law of corresponding retinal points, the individual law governing the obliques must be suspended, and the ver- tical axes of the eyes must be made to either converge or diverge above the former if the meridians of greatest curvature diverge above, the latter if these meridians COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 419 converg-e above. The same is true when the principal meridians of one eye are vertical and horizontal, while those of the other are oblique. While there seems to be no one who now doubts that, in astigmatism, there is displacement of the images of all lines not parallel with the one or the other of the two principal meridians, it will not be out of place here to present some incontrovertible demonstrations, show- ing- that these displacements are always toward the me- ridian of greatest curvature. The accompanying illustrations are simple, are easily understood, and are at the same time correct. These, at a glance, make clear what has been taught since 1891 con- cerning the obliquity of retinal images in oblique astig- matism. Criticism of this teaching would not have been made if the author had thought to use these illustrations in his first publication.* In justice to Dr. P. C. Hotz, of Chicago, it must be stated here that he gracefully retracted his criticism, which was most severe, and showed before the Chicago Ophthalmological Society how it was that he was led into the error that formed the basis of his criticism. It was before this society that he thought t he had demon- strated that oblique astigmatism did not cause any dis- placement of the retinal image of a vertical or horizontal *See Ophthalmic Record, Vol. I., No. 1, 1891. 420 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. line. He used for this purpose a camera, the lens of which he had rendered astigmatic by adding a cylinder with its axis at 45. On the ground glass of his camera he focused the image of a horizontal line. This image he believed to be horizontal, as did all who saw his dem- onstration, but it was not. With the same camera, he later focused the images of two lines crossing each other at right angles the one line, vertical; the other, horizon- tal. These images were readily seen displaced, in oppo- site directions, so that the angles formed at their point of crossing were not right angles. These two lines enabled the camera to magnify the truth so as to enable Hotz and his fellow-members of the Ophthalmological Society to see the error into which he had led them at a pre- vious meeting. Hotz was kind enough to invite the author, whom he had formerly criticised, to be present at his last demonstration and speak on oblique astig- matism. Dr. Harold Wilson (of Detroit), another critic, was later forced to yield to the argument of his camera ren- dered astigmatic, which he had focused on a church spire. He thought that the spire in the photograph was vertical, and so published. Later he studied both the axis of the spire and the base-line, and found that the photograph did not show them to be at right angles; that these were both inclined in opposite directions. His error in COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 421 observing only one line was very similar to the one into which Hotz had fallen. But to a study of the illustrations: Fig. 55 is complex, showing a square as seen by a non- astigmatic eye, as seen by an eye astigmatic according a" O f d' Fig- 55- to the rule, and as seen by the latter after the astigma- tism has been corrected by a plus cylinder. The rec- tangie a-b-c-d is the square seen by the non-astigmatic eye, and a-c and d-b show the diagonals of this square. The rectangle a'-b'-c'-d' is the figure seen by the astig- matic eye with the meridian of greatest curvature verti- 422 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. cal. The axial rays from the ends of the lines a-b and d-c enter the eye through parts of the cornea parallel with the meridian of greatest curvature and so near to it that their refractive power is practically the same. The re- fraction of these axial rays from a and b, by the cornea, is such as to make them cross each other, on their way back to the retina, sooner than they would have done if there had been no astigmatism; hence their points of impingement on the retina are more widely separated, and the line itself must be proportionately increased. The same is true of the axial rays from the ends of the line d-c. Hence it is clear that the line a-b must become the line a'-b' and the line d-c must become the line d'-c 1 . Because of the increase of the length of the line a-b and d-c the lines a-d and b-c are more widely separated, be- coming lines a'-d' and b'-c', and we have not a square, but the rectangular parallelogram a'-b'-c'-d'. The di- agonal a-c has been rotated toward the vertical and be- comes a'-c' ; and the diagonal d-b has been rotated in the opposite direction, but also toward the vertical, and be- comes d'-b' . They have both been rotated, by the refrac- tion of the astigmatic cornea, toward the meridian of greatest curvature. The image changes effected by the astigmatic cornea are, as shown in the figure: an in- crease in the length of the lines parallel with the merid- ian of greatest curvature, an increase in the distance COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 423 between the lines parallel with the meridian of least curvature, and a corresponding rotation of the diago- nals toward the meridian of greatest curvature. The proper plus cylinder placed before this eye gives such aid to the least curved meridian of the cornea as to make its refractive power exactly equal to the unaided refractive power of the meridian of greatest curvature. The result will be a lengthening of the horizontal lines a'-d' and d'-c' into the lines a"-d" and b"-c" and a dis- placement of the lines a'-b' and d'-c' until they become a"-b" and d"-c". Since two of the sides (a-b and d-c} of the square have been lengthened by the astigmatism and the remaining two sides (a-d and b-c) have been lengthened to exactly the same extent by the correct plus cylinder, the figure a"-b"-c"-d" , seen by the cor- rected astigmatic eye, is a square. The cylinder, in changing the rectangular parallelogram a'-b' -d-d' to the square a"-b"-c"-d", has also rotated the diagonals a'-c' and d'-b' back to their original positions; for the diagonal a' '-c' ' coincides with the diagonal a-c, and the diagonal d"-b" coincides with d-b. If the astigmatism had been corrected by a minus cylinder, the lines a'-b' and d'-c' would have been short- ened into the lines a-b and d-c; the lines a'-d' and b'-c 1 would have been brought closer together, a'-d' becoming a-d and b'-c! becoming b-c; and the diagonals a'-c? and 424 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. d'-b' would have been rotated back into the diagonals a-c and d-b, respectively, so that the figure thus seen would be the square a-b-c-d. Thus it is shown that an astig- matic eye, corrected with a minus cylinder, sees the square with the same measurements as that seen by the non-astigmatic eye; while the square seen by the astig- matic eye, corrected with a plus cylinder, is magnified. Turning the right side of Fig. 55 up, it shows the im- age changes when the meridian of greatest curvature is horizontal. In either case the lines parallel with the meridian of greatest curvature are made longer by the astigmatism, with a corresponding increase of distance between the lines parallel with the meridian of least curvature, and the diagonals are rotated toward the meridian of greatest curvature. If there is astigmatism of one eye with the meridian of greatest curvature vertical and astigmatism of the same kind in the other with the meridian of greatest curvature horizontal, the former would see a square changed into a rectangular parallelogram, with the longer sides vertical; while the latter would see the square similarly changed, but with the longer sides horizontal. The images in such eyes would be dissimilar and could not be perfectly fused; correcting cylinders would make the images alike and thus make complete fusion possible. What is true of squares is true of rectangular paral- COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 425 lelograms, as shown by Fig. 56 in .which there is the same proportionate lengthening- of two of the sides by the astigmatism, and of the other two sides by the astig- matic correction with plus cylinders, also the same char- Fig. 56. acter of rotations of the diagonals, the principal merid- ians being" vertical and horizontal. Fig. 57 shows the image changes when the astigma- tism is oblique, the meridian of greatest curvature be- ing at 135. That part of the complex figure shown by a-b-c-d is a square as seen by a non-astigmatic eye. L/ooked at by the oblique astigmatic eye already men- tioned, the diagonal a-c, being at an angle of 135, is in a plane with the meridian of greatest curvature, while the diagonal d-b is in a plane with the meridian of least curvature. For reasons already given in discussing 426 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. Fig. 55, the diagonal a-c is increased in length by the astigmatism into a'-c' , while the diagonal d-b is neither altered in length nor in direction. The sides of the square, not being parallel with the principal meridians, must be rotated toward the meridian of greatest curvature, a-b Fig- 57- becoming a'-b, a-d becoming a'-d^ b-c becoming b-c', and d-c becoming d-d '. The figure a'-b-d-d is a non-rectan- gular parallelogram leaning down and to the right. A plus cylinder correcting - the astigmatism will increase the length of diagonal d-b into d'-b' to exactly the length of the diagonal a'-J and at the same time will rotate the COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 427 lines a'-b to a'-b', a'-d to a'-d', c'-b to c'-b', and c'-d to c'-d ', thus converting the non-rectangular parallelogram a'-b-d-d into the magnified square a'-b' -c'-d'. Turning the right side of Fig. 57 up, the image changes are shown when the meridian of greatest curvature is at 45. It is clear that, if the astigmatism is equal and of Fig. 58- the same kind in the two eyes, the meridians of greatest curvature being parallel, though oblique, the two images of a square held vertically will be distorted alike, and hence will fuse readily and completely. If the meridian of greatest curvature in the right eye is at 135, and in the left eye at 45, the image in each eye will be a non- 428 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. rectangular parallelogram leaning in the opposite direc- tion from the image in the other eye; and the two cannot be perfectly fused, though an attempt at fusion will be made, in an effort on the part of the eyes to obey the supreme law of binocular single vision, the law of cor- responding retinal points. Fig. 58 shows how the fusion of the images would make the square appear. This fusion is effected by the superior obliques converging the vertical axes of the eyes. Soon after Hotz and Wilson had tried the camera and had published their conclusions unfavorable to the dis- tortion of retinal images by oblique astigmatism, Dr. Perry, of Oneida, N. Y., betook himself to the camera, with the result shown in the accompanying half-tone cut, Fig. 59: Dr. Perry's own words, descriptive of this cut, are as follows : "Fig. 59 was produced by taking a photograph of the graduated circle with printed words as shown; and then, without moving camera or object, placing a .50 D cylin- drical lens in front of the objective and exposing a sec- ond negative, and, when the photographs were finished, cutting away the outer circle from the astigmatic print and pasting it over the other in such a way as to make the horizontal and vertical lines, respectively, coincide on the two prints. If the cut is held so that the line of the Fig. 59- 430 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. print reading- 'Astigmatism Oblique, 135,' is horizontal, it will be observed that the distortion of the field is such that this particular line is moved along the scale nearly two degrees, while the line which is perpendicular to it is moved an equal distance, but in a contrary direction. This shows what must happen to a retinal image in ob- lique astigmatism." Dr. Lowry, at the time a private student of the au- thor, was incited by the published criticisms of Hotz and Wilson to use his camera. Half-tone cuts were made from his photographs, and, notwithstanding these speak for themselves, his descriptive text is here reproduced: "It has often been noted that the camera obscura is very strikingly similar, in its mechanism, to the human eye. In this simple optical instrument we have a me- chanical eye, so far as refraction is concerned. If we compare to the eye the component parts of the photo- graphic camera, which is merely a camera obscura with a device for receiving the image on a sensitized plate, we find the refractive media of the former correspond to the photographic lens; the iris, to the stop; the accommo- dation, to the focusing apparatus; and the retina, to the ground glass. Focus the camera properly, and we have the emmetropic eye. By placing a concave cylindrical lens, axis at 90, in apposition to the photographic lens, we have simple hypermetropic astigmatism according to COMPENSATING CYCJUOTKOPIA. 431 the rule; if we place the axis at 180, we have simple hy- permetropic astigmatism against the rule; if we place the axis anywhere between the \ertical and horizontal, Fig. 60. we get oblique astigmatism. Whether or not the image will be oblique on the ground glass will be seen later. "To illustrate these points, I have made the accompa- nying- photographs with a rapid rectilinear lens, used in the Rochester Optical Company's 5x7 midget camera. The camera was not moved or changed in any way foi 432 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. the first five photographs. Fig 1 . 65 was made at another time. The rectangle was made mathematically accurate on a piece of cardboard 24 x 30. The lines, one inch wide, Fig. 61. are prolonged beyond the rectangle to show more clearly the obliquity of imagfes that may be produced by the cylinders obliquely placed. The watch is used as a plumb, and is seen in the same position in all. The photographs are not inverted as the images \vould be on the ground glass or the retina. COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 433 "In Fig. 60, no cylindrical lens is used, and we get a perfect rectangle, sharp and distinct in its outline, as would be seen by an emmetropic eye. Fig. 62. "In making Fig. 61 a minus 3 D. cylindrical lens is placed just in front of, and in apposition to, the photo- graphic lens, with its axis at 45. * A plus 1.50 D spher- ical lens is used with the cylinder in order to give the * These prints are all the reverse o it would be seen. AUTHOR. f the images, therefore the reverse of the object as 434 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. middle of the focal interval without changing- the camera. In this the vertical and horizontal lines are equally indis- tinct. The vertical lines deviate to the left at the top, ""/ Fig. 63. and to the right at the bottom, while the horizontal lines are depressed at the right and elevated at the left. T^ne plumb shows that the card is in just the same position as in Fig. 60, and the camera has not been moved from its original position. This picture is clearly a non-rec- tangular parallelogram. COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 435 " If the axis of the cylinder be changed to 90, we get Fig. 62, which represents simple vertical hypermetropic astigmatism. This is made without the plus 1.50 D sphere and without the camera's being changed in the least from Fig. 64. its position in Fig. 60 and Fig. 61. The meridian of great- est curvature here is at 90, with the least at 180. It is a perfect rectangle, with its horizontal lines sharply cut and the vertical very indistinct. "Now if we place the axis of the cylinder at 135, again adding the plus 1.50 D sphere, a non-rectangular 436 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. parallelogram is formed with its sides deviating- in the opposite direction to those in Fig-. 61. This is shown in Fig-. 63. Every part is equally indistinct, and nowhere are the lines at right angles as in the original. Fig. 65. " By placing the axis of the cylinder at 180, without the plus 1.50 D sphere, we produce simple hypermetropic horizontal astigmatism, the effect of which is illustrated in Fig. 64. Here we have the meridian of greatest curva- ture at 180, and the least at 90. We obtain a perfect COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 437 rectangle, with its vertical lines clear and its horizontal very indistinct, in contradistinction to Fig. 62. "Fig 1 . 65 is the same as Fig 1 . 61 without the plus 1.50 D sphere to give the focal interval, nor is the camera re- focused to give it. This photograph was made at a dif- ferent time, and the camera was not in exactly the same position as for the other five. An eye with the meridian of greatest curvature at 45 and 3 D of simple hyper- metropic astigmatism would see the object as shown in this figure, if, under the influence of a mydriatic or in old age, it were relieved of all ciliary action. The rhomboidal figures are seen very clearly here at the angles and on the watch. " Suppose one of the meridians of greatest curvature to be at 45, and the other at 135, one image would be seen as in Fig 1 . 61, and the other as in Fig-. 63; in obedi- ence to the law of corresponding- retinal points, we would have these two fig-ures superimposed, forming- a trape- zoid. If the meridians diverged above, we would have the long- side above, and the short side below. In this form of astigmatism we would not only have a ciliary strain, but the superior obliques would make an attempt to bring the harmonizing parts of the two retinas under the dissimilar -images in order to have a single object. If the meridians converged above, the short side of the trape- Zoid would be above, and the long side below. This fu- 438 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. sion of dissimilar parallelograms into a trapezoid, long- side below, would be effected by the inferior obliques. "But the bone of contention has been principally the question of the deviation or the non-deviation of the image on the retina in oblique astigmatism. Others have proved it by the laws of optics, by clinical experience, and by logical reasoning; and it seems to me that my pho- tographic demonstrations have added very conclusive evidence to the theory that, in oblique astigmatism, the retinal images of vertical and horizontal objects deviate from their normal direction." Since 1887, it has been taught that, in oblique as- tigmatism, abnormal work is required of the obliques. However, it was not until 1891 that the cause of this ab- normal action on the part of the obliques was discovered to be a want of parallelism of the meridians of greatest curvature of the corneas and a consequent dissimilar dis- tortion of retinal images. It was then announced that, in oblique astigmatism, be the obliquity much or little, it is a physical impossibility for a horizontal line and its retinal image to lie in the same plane.* The same is true of all lines not parallel with one or the other of the two principal meridians. The obliquity of retinal images was first demonstrated, in the latter part of 1890, by the production of artificial *See Ophthalmic Record, Vol. I., No. 1. COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 439 oblique astigmatism, at which time the following law was formulated: " The retinal image is displaced toward the meridian of greatest curvature" This being true and there is no exception to this rule the image of a ver- tical or horizontal line is displaced toward the meridian of best curvature in oblique hyperopic astigmatism, from the best meridian in oblique myopic astigmatism, and to- ward the myopic meridian in oblique mixed astigmatism. RIGHT LEFT Fig. 66. As a result of the experiments with artificially pro- duced astigmatism, the next eight figures were con- structed, some of them showing the character of the images of a horizontal arrow, formed on the two retinas; while other of these figures show clearly the compensat- ing cyclotropia made necessary that the images might be fused. This cyclotropia corresponds with the compen- sating esotropia which occurs when both images are dis- 440 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. placed by prisms with their bases out; with the compen- sating' exotropia, when images are displaced by prisms with their bases in; with the compensating- hypertropia of one eye and catatropia of the other, when a prism is base down before one eye and base up before the other. In either case, the turning 1 must occur in obedience to the supreme law of binocular single vision, the law of cor- responding- retinal points. Fig-. 66 represents a pair of eyes in which the two principal meridians are vertical and horizontal (they can also represent eyes that are non-astigrnatic). If an arrow, or the picture of an arrow, be held horizontally before these eyes, the arrow-head toward the patient's left eye, it will throw a reversed imag'e on each retina, and the two images will be in the same plane with the object. These two imag-es fall on parts of the two retinas that act tog-ether; hence, but one object is seen. Fig". 67 represents a pair of eyes in which there is hy- peropic astig-matism, either simple or compound. The left eye has its best meridian vertical. In this eye the arrow, held as before, throws its image on the horizontal meridian of the retina, hence in the same plane with it. In the rig-ht eye the best meridian is at 135, as shown by the dotted line. In obedience to the well-known law of refraction by curved surfaces, the imag-e of the same arrow must be oblique in this eye, and, hence, not in the COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 441 same plane with the object. The obliquity of the image will be greater or less, depending- on the quantity of the astigmatism. It is represented as falling 1 on meridian 170 of the retina. The horizontal image in the left eye RIGHT Fig. 67. LEFT and the oblique image in the right eye do not fall on parts of the two retinas that harmonize. The direction of either image in relation to the other cannot be changed except by artificial means a proper cylindrical lens. Fig. 68. This being- true, the pair of unaided astigmatic eyes, represented by Fig. 67, must see the arrow double, as shown in Fig. 68, unless something is done by the eyes themselves for the purpose of harmonizing the images. 442 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. In all cases of oblique astigmatism, unless the obliquity is in the same direction in the two eyes, and the astig- matism the same in kind and quantity, something 1 must be done in order to prevent double vision, as represented in Fig 1 . 68. There are but two ways of accounting- for the absence of this peculiar kind of double vision in such forms of astigmatism as that represented in Fig". 67. Sectional ciliary contraction would account for it. If it were possible for the ciliary muscle thus to act, one can readily understand how the curvature of the lens could be so changed as to result in lenticular astigmatism equal, but at right angles, to the corneal astigmatism. If such ciliary action were to take place in the right eye of Fig. 67, the retinal image would not only be made as sharp as if in an emmetropic eye, but it would also be made to lose its obliquity, and thus double vision would be prevented. As beautifully as this sectional ciliary action would account for the absence of double vision in cases of oblique astigmatism, it is certainly a false theory, since, when all ciliary power has been suspended by atropine or age, the eyes are still able to do something by means of which the double vision represented by Fig. 68 is prevented. There must be double vision, unless the oblique image in the right eye and the horizontal image in the left eye can be made to occupy corresponding parts of the two COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 443 retinas. This could be effected easily by the harmonious symmetrical action of the superior oblique muscles. Fig. 69 shows how the eyes represented by Fig. 67 act in order to have the images fall on corresponding parts of the retinas. The superior oblique muscle of the right eye has so revolved it as to bring meridian 175 of the retina in position to receive the impress of the oblique image; while, at the same moment, the superior oblique RIGHT Fig. 69. LEFT muscle of the left eye has so revolved it as to bring me- ridian 175 to the horizontal, hence in position to receive the horizontal image. The oblique and horizontal im- ages being now on harmonizing portions of the retinas, there is no double vision. Fig. 70 represents a pair of hyperopic astigmatic eyes, the left one having its best meridian vertical and the right one having its best meridian at 45. In these eyes 444 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. there are a left horizontal image (image and arrow in same plane) and a right oblique image, this time on retinal meridian 10. Nothing but artificial means will change the relative direction of these images; and there must be double vision, unless the oblique image can be made to fall on a portion of the retina that will harmonize with that portion of the other retina on which the horizontal image may fall. RIGHT LEFT Fig. 70. The double vision that would exist in astigmatic eyes represented in Fig. 70 is prevented by the harmonious action of the inferior oblique muscles, as shown by Fig. 71, the inferior oblique of the right eye bringing meridian 5 under the oblique image, while the inferior oblique of the left eye causes meridian 5 to come under the hori- zontal image. Thus the two images are made to fall on corresponding parts of the two retinas. COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 445 RlSHT Fig. 71. LETT Fig. 72 represents a pair of hypermetropic astigmatic eyes, with half the quantity of astigmatism found in the eyes represented by Fig. 67 and Fig. 70; but in both eyes the best meridian is oblique in the left eye at 45, and in the right eye at 135. An arrow held in the horizontal position before these eyes will throw an oblique image on each retina, the one in the left eye on meridian 5, and the RI6H Fig. 72. LEFT 446 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROP1A. one in the right eye on meridian 175. Without some change double vision, as shown in Fig. 68, will be inevit- able. In the oblique astigmatism of the two eyes represented by Fig. 72, the two oblique images are made to fall on corresponding parts of the two retinas by the harmonious action of the two superior oblique muscles, as shown in Fig. 73. RIGHT . 73- The obliquity of the image and the consequent strain on the oblique muscles fully account for the greater trouble attending oblique astigmatism than is found connected with astigmatism in the vertical or horizontal. As is well known, non-oblique myopic astigmatism is un- attended by any sort of ciliary strain in distant vision. In oblique myopic astigmatism, there is strain on either the two superior or the two inferior oblique muscles in COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 447 both distant and near seeing". In all other forms of non- parallel oblique astigmatism, there is likewise strain on the oblique muscles. In all kinds of non-oblique astigmatism, also in simple hyperopia, the time comes when all nervous phenomena caused by their existence pass away. Their disappear- ance, being gradual, but finally complete, coincides with the failure and final loss of ciliary power brought about by advancing age. The symptoms caused by oblique astigmatism may be modified by old age putting at rest the ciliary muscles; but they cannot be made to vanish, for the oblique muscles are forced to continue to act in age as in youth, so as to harmonize the images on the two retinas. The plates made to illustrate the paper read by the author at the meeting of the Eighth International Con- gress of Ophthalmology (Edinburgh, 1894), are both in- teresting and instructive. They are reproduced here, together with the descriptive text. Plate I. represents a pair of eyes that are non-astig- matic; or, if astigmatism exists, the principal meridians are vertical and horizontal. These eyes are represented as looking at a rectangle. The line ep across the right eye is the horizontal meridian, and the line g~h is the vertical meridian, while their point of intersection (5) is the macula. Similarly the line ep in the left eye rep- COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 449 resents the horizontal meridian, and gh represents the vertical meridian, their point of intersection (5) being- the macula. The vertical meridian of the right eye and that of the left eye are parallel. Point 5 in the rectangle is the point of fixation. The line 5-5 from the macula of the right e} r e is the visual axis of that eye, and likewise the line 5-5 is the visual axis of the left eye. These intersect at point 5 of the rectangle. According to the well-known law of refraction by curved surfaces, such as are now under consideration, the rectangular object will throw a rectangular image on each retina, the size of which will bear a definite pro- portion to the size of the object. The center of retinal curvature of the right eye is x, through which all lines of direction from this eye must pass. The lower inner corner of this image is thus connected with the upper right-hand corner of the object by the line 1-1; in the same way the upper inner corner of the image is con- nected with the lower right-hand corner of the object by the visual line 2-2; and so on for the other corners of image and object. In like manner the corners of the rectangular image in the left eye may be connected with corresponding corners of the object by lines passing through the center of retinal curvature (x) of that eye. If the left eye should be excluded, the right eye would see the rectangle 1-2-3-4; if the right eye should be 450 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPlA. screened, the left eye would see the same rectangular fig- ure. Both eyes together, in obedience to both the law of corresponding retinal points and the law of projection, would see the one common rectangle 1-2-3-4. The supe- rior and inferior recti in these eyes have kept the visual axes in the same plane, the external and internal recti have regulated their tension so that they have converged these axes to the point 5, and the superior and inferior obliques have kept the naturally vertical axes parallel with the median plane of the head. The obliques have to perform only the simple function in oblique astigmatism, the meridians of greatest curva- ture being parallel, and the degree of astigmatism the same in the two eyes; but it would not be possible for such eyes to see the rectangle held in the position shown in Plate I. as a rectangle. L/et the meridians of greatest curvature be at 45 in the right eye and also at 45 in the left eye. As a result of the refraction of the astigmatic cornea of the right eye, the rectangular figure would throw a parallelogram image on the retina, the image inclining down and out. A parallelogram image would be thrown on the left retina also, and it would incline down and in. Looked at with either eye alone, the rec- tangle would be seen as a parallelogram inclined down and to the right; looked at with both eyes, it would be a parallelogram of the same shape and inclination as COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 451 seen by each eye separately. The extrinsic muscles of these eyes have performed the same function as the mus- cles of the eyes shown in Plate I. and with the same re- sult viz., binocular single vision. The law of corre- sponding- retinal points and the law of projection having full sway in both pairs of eyes, the one pair sees the figure as it is a rectangle while the other pair sees the same figure, when held in the same position, as a parallelogram leaning down and to the right. With the visual axes properly directed by the recti and the verti- cal meridians kept parallel by the obliques, the two eyes are kept so related that the two images of the object looked at fall on harmonizing parts of the two retinas, and the object is necessarily seen as one, and of the same shape as when seen with each eye sep- arately. In any state of refraction the relationship between corresponding points of the two retinas is unalterable. It is well known that, taken as a whole, the nasal half of one retina harmonizes with the temporal half of the other, and that all points of either retina bear a fixed and unalterable relationship to the macula and to the vertical and horizontal meridians. A retinal point in the nasal half of the right retina, bearing a definite re- lationship to the macula and the vertical and horizontal meridians, must harmonize with a point in the temporal 452 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. half of the left retina similarly located; and it can har- monize with no other retinal point under any conditions. The complicated function of the obliques is necessary in oblique astigmatism when the meridians of greatest curvature diverge or converge above. This is necessary that they may bring harmonizing parts of the two retinas under dissimilar images, and thus insure binocular single vision; but, as will be shown, the object, though seen as one, will be distorted. Plate II. may be taken for stud} T . Both eyes have oblique astigmatism of the same kind and quantity. In the right eye the meridian of greatest curvature is at 135 and in the left eye at 45. If the rectangular figure represented in Plate I. be held in the same position be- fore the eyes represented in Plate II., it would not be seen with one eye alone or with both together as a rectan- gle. The rectangle shown in Plate I., when held before the right eye in Plate II., instead of throwing a rec- tangular, would throw a non-rectangular, parallelogram image on the right retina; the same rectangle would also throw a non-rectangular parallelogram image on the left retina. The state of refraction of the right eye would make the distorted image lean down and toward the left side, while the distorted image in the left eye would lean down and toward the right side. Cutting off the view of the left eye, the law of direction would (453) 454 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. have full sway, while the law of corresponding- points would be suspended. Since in one eye alone the law of direction is unalterable, all lines of direction must cross in the center of retinal curvature; and the right eye, with the parallelogram image leaning down and to the left, must see the figure casting the image, not as a rectangle, but as a parallelogram leaning down and to the left. Screening the right eye while the left eye looks on the rectangle, it is seen, not as a rectangle, but as a parallelogram leaning down and to the right, the law of direction determining the shape of the figure seen by the left eye, just as it fixed the shape of the figure seen by the right eye. Fig. 1-2-3-4 is what is seen with the right eye alone; Fig. l'-2'-3'-4' is what is seen by the left eye alone. The moment these two eyes are allowed to look at the rectangular figure, the law of corresponding retinal points is brought into conflict with the law of direction, and the latter is modified by the former. There is no necessity for changing the visual axes when looking at the rectangle with these two eyes; but, unless some change is effected in some way, each eye would see its own parallelogram leaning down and toward the opposite side. Instantly a change does take place in both eyes, so that the two together see, not a rectangle nor a parallelogram, but a trapezoid, with the longer side above. A clear understanding of what this COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 455 change is and how it is effected may be had by a further study of Plate II. In the right eye is shown a dotted parallelogram a-b-c-d, of precisely the same form as the parallelogram image 1-2-3-4; but in the former the upper and lower lines are parallel with the horizontal meridian. In the left eye also is shown a dotted parallelogram a'-b'-c'-d', of the same form as the parallelogram l'-2'- 3'-4', with its upper and lower lines parallel with the horizontal meridian of this eye. The line c-b in the right eye bears throughout the same relation to the macula, the horizontal and vertical meridians of this eye, that the line c'-b' does to the same parts of the left eye, and they, therefore, correspond. The greater part of the line d-a in the right eye also corresponds with the greater part of the line d'-a' in the left eye, the parts of these lines not corresponding being their extremities. But the line c-d in the right eye nowhere corresponds with the line c'-d f in the left eye, except at the points of beginning above; and the same is true of lines b-a and d'-a', in their respective eyes. If the dotted parallelograms could be made to coincide with the parallelogram images, the re- sult would be that the two eyes together would see the figure a-b-c-d', a trapezoid, with the longer side above. How this is effected is shown in Plate III., where each eye has been revolved on its visual axis by its superior oblique muscle, so that the horizontal meridian is made 456 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. parallel with the upper and lower borders of the parallel- ogram image; and thus, as far as possible, correspond- ing 1 parts of the two retinas are brought under the two dissimilar images, and the figure seen binocularl} 7 " is a-b-c-d'. The part of this trapezoid seen in common by the two eyes is a'-b-c-d, the part seen by the right eye alone is a-b-a', and that seen by the left eye alone is d-c-d' '. As will be seen, the law of corresponding- points has so modified the law of projection that the visual lines no longer have a common crossing" point. This is anarchy, so far as projection is concerned, in these eyes. When the law of direction is interfered with, as a re- sult of the conflict between it and the more imperious law of corresponding- retinal points, the object seen is always in the position that it would have been in, had the images primarily fallen on the parts of the two ret- inas that have been rotated under them, in obedience to the supreme law of binocular single vision the law of corresponding retinal points. The displaced images, as a result of either natural or artificial means, cover areas of the two retinas that do not correspond. In order to have binocular single vision, retinal areas that more nearly correspond, and are of the same shape and size as the im- ages, must be brought under them. The object will be seen as though no rotation had taken place, as if the im- ages had primarily fallen on these parts, in perfect obe- (457) 458 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. dience to the law of projection, although the lines of di- rection drawn from the images to the single object will not cross at the center of retinal curvature. In cases of decentration of the maculas, and in displaced images by means of prisms, all lines of direction will cross at one point, but that point will be above, below, to the outer or inner side of the true point; while in oblique astigma- tism, and when the axes of correcting cylinders are dis- placed, no three lines of direction cross at the same point. In like manner a plate could be made showing how astigmatic eyes, with meridians of greatest curvature converging above, would see a rectangle distorted into a trapezoid, the longer side below. In each eye there would be a parallelogram image inclining down and out. To fuse these into a trapezoid, the inferior oblique mus- cles would be brought into action, in order, as far as possible, to bring corresponding retinal parts under dis- similar images, which is done the moment the obliques displace the horizontal meridians so that they become parallel with the upper and lower borders of the dis- torted images. Imperfect as is binocular single vision in uncorrected oblique astigmatism, the meridians of greatest curvature either diverging or converging above, it could be effected in no other way than by a revolution of the eyes by the symmetric harmonious action of the oblique muscles. It COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 459 is true that Nature has one other method of preventing diplopia namely, mental suppression of one of the dis- placed images. It may be that amblyopia resulting- from oblique astigmatism high in degree, and from insufficiency of the obliques, is more common than one would at first think. Certainly, if the obliques cannot do their proper work in effecting binocular single vision, in the first years of life, nothing is more reasonable than to suppose that amblyopia ex anopsia would develop. Who has not seen cases of amblyopia without being able to ac- count for it ? The phenomena outlined can be demonstrated experi- mentally by any one who desires to prove all things; for he can produce in his own case, at pleasure, any form of as- tigmatism. But some may be ready to say that artificial astigmatism is one thing and natural astigmatism is an- other thing. This is true, but only in name. That 3 D of artificial hyperopic astigmatism is the same error of refraction as 3 D of natural hyperopic astigmatism is abundantly proved by the fact that each is thoroughly corrected by a plus 3 cylinder, axis properly placed. Either plus or minus cylinders may be used in the ex- periments, for the one is as capable of producing arti- ficial astigmatism as the other. If the plus cylinders (3 D) be used, the astigmatism produced has its meridian of greatest curvature at right angles to the axis of the 460 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. cylinder, while the meridian of greatest curvature would correspond with the axis of the minus cylinder (3 D) if it were used. By either means it can be easily proved that in astig- matism of any kind (myopic, hyperopic, or mixed), whose meridians of greatest curvature diverge above, there is a necessity for action on the part of the superior oblique muscles in order to prevent diplopia. This action, hav- ing its beginning in the earliest days of infancy and con- tinuing during waking hours until the cause is corrected or one eye is lost, converges the naturally vertical axes above. If the meridians of greatest curvature converge above, the images of all objects are so displaced in the two eyes as to throw into activity the inferior obliques, so that diplopia may be prevented. In astigmatism with the principal meridians vertical and horizontal, the only eye muscles brought into ac- tion to remedy, in any way, the condition, are the ciliary muscles. In oblique astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curvature diverging above, there is the same state of ciliary strain to sharpen as much as possible the images, and there is also a necessary activity of the su- perior obliques so as to bring corresponding parts of the two retinas under the oblique images, that there may be binocular single vision. Again, in oblique astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curvature converging COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA 461 above, there is the ciliary strain for sharpening the images, and there is also a consequent activity of the inferior obliques so as to bring similar parts of the retinas under the dissimilar images, resulting in binoc- ular single vision. When there is equality of strength of the obliques of the two eyes, vertical and horizontal astigmatism will give less trouble than when the astigmatism is oblique in either direction, and astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curvature diverging above need give no more annoyance to the patient than if these meridians con- verged above; for, in the former case, the superior ob- liques would be as able to bear the strain as would the inferior obliques in the latter condition. But the obliques are not always harmonious : the superior obliques are insufficient in at least twenty-five per cent of all cases, while the inferior obliques are in- sufficient in less than one per cent of all cases. In cases of insufficiency of the superior obliques, the ver- tical form of astigmatism would be worse on the patient than if he had oblique astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curvature converging above; and the worst form of astigmatism would be that in which the merid- ians of greatest curvature diverge above. The reverse would be true if the inferior, obliques were insufficient a rare condition. 462 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. The complicated function of the oblique muscles exists only in cases of oblique astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curvature converging or diverging- above, and in unequal degrees of oblique astigmatism when the meridians of greatest curvature are parallel. The ne- cessity for this function is entirely destroyed when the astigmatism is properly corrected; but the action of the obliques does not always cease at once in binocular single vision through the correcting cylinders. The old habit of rotation often continues for hours, and sometimes for days (although there is no longer a need for it), and the result is metamorphopsia. Inherent weakness of the superior oblique muscles, in a large per cent of the cases, leads to a more speedy disappearance of the met- amorphopsia when the meridians of greatest curvature diverge above than when they converge. The reverse would be true in a case of insufficiency of the inferior obliques. The habit of action is more quickly suspended in a weak muscle than in a strong one. In all cases, however, it ceases, and the metamorphopsia vanishes, under the continuous wearing of the correcting cylinders. A careful study of what precedes in this chapter will show that, while the superior obliques must fuse the displaced images of a horizontal line, in cases of astig- matism with the meridians of greatest curvature diverg- ing above, the inferior obliques must fuse the displaced COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 463 images of a vertical line, in the same kind of cases. When the meridians of greatest curvature converge above, the displaced images of a horizontal line must be fused by the inferior obliques, while the displaced images of a vertical line would be fused by action of the superior obliques. In most cases of oblique astigmatism the inferior obliques can fuse images more easily than the superior obliques, for the reason that the former are usually stronger than the latter; but when a figure con- sists of both vertical and horizontal lines, such as a rectangle, the upper and lower borders, respectively, of the images must be fused, whether by the inferior obliques or the superior obliques. The lateral borders the verti- cal ends of the rectangle become divergent above if the fusion has been effected by the superior obliques, while they become divergent below if the fusion has been ef- fected by the inferior obliques. There is no known rea- son for the fusion of the upper and lower borders of images to the detriment of the lateral borders, and it may remain always an unexplainable fact. The extent of displacement of images of horizontal lines by oblique astigmatism can be measured only b} the cyclo-phorometer, but this measurement cannot be accurately made if there is a complicating cyclophoria. If the meridians of greatest curvature diverge above and there is a complicating plus cyclophoria, the measure- 464 COMPENSATING CYCIXXTROPIA. ment will show greater displacement than really exists; while the measurement will show less than the real dis- placement when the meridians of greatest curvature con- verge above and there is a complicating plus cyclophoria. In measuring the displacement of the image by oblique astigmatism, the single Maddox rod must be used, in the cyclophrometer, and the vertical displacing prism must be behind one rod so as to make one streak of lisrht be- O low the other. The axis of each rod should be vertical. Both eyes having oblique astigmatism, the meridians of greatest curvature diverging above, each streak of light will dip toward the opposite side. Revolving one rod until the two streaks are parallel, though not horizontal, shows the sum of the displacement of the two images; while revolving each rod until the two streaks are hori- zontal, therefore parallel, the extent of displacement of the image in each eye is easily read on the scale. This shows, also, the amount of minus cyclotropia necessary for the fusion of the two images of a horizontal line. The degree of displacement of the image of a horizontal line depends on both the quantity of the astigmatism and the extent of the obliquity of the meridian of great- est curvature up to 45 from the vertical, at which point there is the maximum of displacement. The compensat- ing cyclotropia of the two eyes always equals the sum of the displacements of the two images. COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 465 The apparent dipping of the streak of light due to cyclophoria may be differentiated from that caused by oblique astigmatism by substituting the triple rod for the simple rod. With the triple rod the dipping line of cyclophoria will be unbroken, while the dipping line of oblique astigmatism will be broken into as many parts as there are rods, the one part slightly over-riding the other in the direction of the obliquity of the most- curved meridian. This obliquity of the image varies from a few minutes to a few degrees, but is always enough, even in slight cases of oblique astigmatism, to force the obliques to disturb the parallelism of the vertical axes with the median plane of the head. Persons with oblique astig- matism, with the meridians of greatest curvature con- verging or diverging above, cannot have correct ideas of direction, except in line of the visual axes, nor can they judge correctly of verticality, horizontality, and goni- ometry. The abnormal work required of the obliques because of oblique astigmatism (non-parallel) develops symptoms not unlike those caused by cyclophoria. TREATMENT. The treatment of compensating cyclotropia is always non-surgical. The careful correction of the astigmatism 466 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. will counteract the distortion of the retinal images and relieve the cyclotropia, but not at once; the lifetime habit of the obliques cannot be broken at once; but sooner or later these muscles will learn that, under the new condition (the wearing of the correcting cylinders), they must parallel the vertical axes of the eyes with the median plane of the head, in order to satisfy the law of corresponding retinal points. In all cases of astigmatism, one eye looking through its correcting cylinder will see at once a rectangle as a rectangle; and if the meridians of greatest curvature are parallel whether vertical, horizontal, or oblique' the two eyes looking through the cylinders will show no dis- tortion of a rectangular figure. The reason for this is that, in such cases, the obliques have never done other work than the keeping of the vertical axes of the eyes parallel with the median plane of the head; therefore they have no habit to break when cylinders are given. In such cases the glasses are worn with gladness from the beginning. There is always metamorphopsia to annoy a patient whose astigmatism was such that the meridians of greatest curvature diverged above, when she begins the wearing of correcting cylinders, whether they be plus or minus. This distortion is easily noticed, for it is the opposite of that to which she has always been accustomed COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 467 and which she may never have noticed. Seen through the correcting 1 cylinders, a rectangle will appear as a a trapezoid, with the longer side below; a level surface will slant toward her; and a vertical object will lean toward her. The metamorphopsia is due to the fact that the superior obliques, always in the habit of con- verging the vertical axes of the eyes in binocular vision, continue to thus converge them for a time, so that the axis of the plus cylinder and the meridian of greatest curvature do not remain in the same plane. The supe- rior obliques, possibly because they are usually weaker than the inferior obliques, readily break from tljeir old habit, and the metamorphopsia vanishes. In such cases it is a question of only a few hours or, at most, a few days until the correcting cylinders can be worn without annoyance of any kind. The old habit broken, the me- ridian of greatest curvature (least if a minus cylinder is used) and the axis of the cylinder lie in the same plane in binocular as well as in monocular vision. When the meridians of greatest curvature converge above, the wearing of the cylinders will be attended by metamorphopsia for a much longer time, possibly be- cause the inferior obliques, being stronger than the su- perior obliques, are less inclined to give up the old habit of diverging the vertical axes of the eyes, in the act of binocular vision. With either eye alone, a rectangle, 468 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. seen through the correcting- cylinder, will be a rectangle; the floor will appear level, and a vertical object will not be inclined. In binocular vision througfh the cylinders a rectangle will appear as a trapezoid, with its longer side above; a level surface will slant from the patient; and a vertical object will lean from her. These appearances, being" new, will be easily noticed, and will often prove very annoying* to the patient, unless previously told about them. Finally, the old habit of rotation will cease, and the metamorphopsia will disappear, but only after days or weeks of constant wearing of the cylinders. After the disappearance of the metamorphopsia, caused by plus cylinders, whose axes diverge above, on raising the lenses a rectangle will appear as a trapezoid, with the longer side above; but if the axes of the cylinders converge above, on raising the lenses a rectangle will appear as a trapezoid, with the longer side below. The same changes in the rectangle existed before the cyl- inders were ever prescribed, but they were unnoticed. Now that the cylinders have corrected the misshaped images, giving perfect vision, on raising the lenses the misshaped images at once make the patient conscious of the distortion of the object. There are two methods of dealing with cases of non- parallel oblique astigmatism so as to shorten the annoy- COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 469 ing- period of habit-breaking on the part of the oblique muscles. One method was suggested by Lippincott, of Pittsburg, Pa. He advises that the full error be determined under a mydriatic, and that the exact lo- cation of the principal meridians be found, which can be easily done, if the compensating cyclotropia is not complicated by a cyclophoria, by excluding one eye whil testing the other; for then the one eye assumes that po- sition which makes its vertical axis parallel with the median plane of the head. The findings, both as to strength of cylinders and positions of axes, are to be recorded. At first the cylinders given should be one- third the full strength required, but their axes must be placed according to the record. When the little meta- morphopsia caused by the partial correction has disap- peared, new cylinders of two-thirds the required strength are given. The little metamorphopsia caused by these, having vanished, the full correction is given. These cause but slight metamorphopsia, and that for only a short while. This method is more necessary and more helpful when the meridians of greatest curvature converge above, and consequently when the inferior obliques are the muscles involved. A full correction of the astigmatism at once corrects the shape of the images, so as to make them correspond with the object. These images would now 470 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. fall on corresponding 1 retinal points if the vertical axes of the eyes were made parallel with the median plane of the head. To thus relate the vertical axes, the inferior obliques must cease their efforts to diverge them, and the superior obliques must assume the labor of paralleling them. Work must be transferred from the inferior ob- liques (usually stronger) to the superior obliques (usu- ally weaker). The whole load cannot be shifted at once; and as long as the inferior obliques continue to diverge the vertical axes, so long will the metamorphopsia re- main. Righting the images one-third transfers one-third of the work from the inferior obliques to the superior ob- liques. This small load is kindly and quickly accepted by the superior obliques. The next step rights the images two-thirds, and transfers another one-third of the work from the inferior obliques to the superior ob- liques. Having become accustomed to the first trans- ference, the superior obliques kindly take on the newly added load. The next step fully corrects the misshaped images, and transfers the balance of the abnormal work from the inferior obliques, in the shape of normal work to the superior obliques. Having already become accus- tomed to doing two-thirds of the work necessary for paralleling the vertical axes of the eyes, the superior ob- liques readily assume the remaining one-third of the load that they must now carry. For this class of astigmatics COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 471 the Lippincott plan is a good one. The only objection to the method is the cost of changing" the lenses. There is nothing 1 to contraindicate the giving of the full correction, at once, of astigmatism in which the meridians of greatest curvature are parallel, whether vertical, horizontal, or oblique; for, in these cases, the oblique muscles have done the same work without the correcting cylinders that they must do when these are given. In only high degrees of oblique astigmatism, with the meridians of greatest curvature diverging above, will it be necessary to adopt the Lippincott plan; for, as a rule, the weak superior obliques are ready enough to cease doing the work of converging the vertical axes of the eyes, \vhile the inferior obliques just as readily assume the new duty of paralleling these axes. In all cases, as soon as the obliques learn to parallel the verti- cal axes of the eyes with the median plane of the head, just that quickly does metamorphopsia vanish. The other method of correcting oblique astigmatism so that there shall be but little annoyance from metamor- phopsia is to give at once the cylinders that fully correct the errors, and to have each lens cut so that, placed straight in the frame, its axis shall be in a plane with the meridian of greatest curvature (least curvature if the lens is a minus cylinder) when the vertical axis of 472 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. the eye is parallel with the median plane of the head. However, these lenses must not be placed in their final positions in the rims at first, but each must have its axis rotated into the arc of distortion for the obliques that have been accustomed to doing abnormal work. The rule formulated by Dr. N. C. Steele, of Chattanooga, Tenn., for the placing of the axes of cylinders in oblique astigmatism, is a good one to follow temporarily under certain conditions. His rule, as applied to plus cylinders, is as follows: " In those cases in which the axes of the proper con- vex cylinders for the two eyes diverge, place the cylin- ders in those positions which will give the axes the great- est divergence permitted by the tests; and in those cases in which the axes converge, place them at the points which will give them the greatest convergence permitted by the tests." The Steele rule for placing the axes of plus cylinders is applicable only when these axes are \vithin 45 of the vertical. Above the 45 point the shifting of these axes should be from the vertical; below the 45 point the shifting should be from the horizontal. In every case of oblique astigmatism with the meridians of greatest curvature diverging or converging above, the axes of plus cylinders should be displaced toward the center of the quadrants in which they are found, and the axes of COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 473 minus cylinders should be shifted from the center of the quadrants in which they are found. The shifting should be only enough to counteract the metamorphopsia, and should be the same for the two cylinders. Every two or three days these axes should be turned a degree or two toward the location determined in the monocular test. With each turning the meta- morphopsia will be so little as hardly to be noticed; and finally, when the cylinders are properly located, there is no metamorphopsia. As the result of each backward turn- ing of the axes of the cylinders, the obliques more nearly parallel the vertical axes of the eyes; and when the last turn has been made, the vertical axes of the eyes stand parallel with the median plane of the head. As the Lippincott method is specially applicable to those cases of astigmatism in which the meridians of greatest curvature converge above, so is it with the ro- tation method. When the meridians of greatest curva- ture diverge above whether the astigmatism be hyper- opic, myopic, or mixed, unless high in degree the full correction should be given at once, and the axes should be placed in the positions determined by the monocular tests. If the Lippincott method be resorted to, full acuity of vision is not obtained until the full correction of the astigmatism has been given; in the method of rotating 474 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. the full-strength cylinders, vision is more or less blurred until the axes are placed at last in their final positions. As to acuteness of vision, the one method has no advan- tage over the other, and there is just as little annoying metamorphopsia in the one method as in the other. It is almost universally true that astigmatics whose meridians of greatest curvature diverge above, become speedily accustomed to the correcting cylinders, for the reason that insufficiency of the inferior obliques is so rare probably not found in more than one case in two hundred. There are some astigmatics whose meridians of greatest curvature converge above, who can never wear comfortably the correcting cylinders because of insufficiency of the superior obliques, which exists in about twenty-five per cent of all cases. There are three reasons why the arcs of distortion, by cylinders, for the oblique muscles, should be studied: (1) That the operator may know how to place cylinders that they may give rest to the weaker obliques in cyclopho- ria; (2) that he may know how to shift the cylinders for oblique astigmatics so as to lessen the annoyance from metamorphopsia; (3) that he may appreciate the impor- tance of having the frames, containing the cylinders that correct any kind of astigmatism, so shaped as to set perfectly straight before the eyes. Several curious facts may be brought forward here, RIGHT UFT V Fij. * PLATE IV (475) 476 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. and reasons have been given why advantage should be temporarily taken of these facts in certain cases. Fig 1 . 1, in Plate IV., represents a pair of hyperopic astig- matic eyes, the meridians of greatest curvature being vertical in each eye. The plus cylinders axes, 90 (a) insure against strain of either the superior obliques or the inferior obliques; but let the glasses be turned in their rims so that the axis of the right shall stand at 80 () and the axis of the left at 100 (&), images will be dis- torted, as shown in Plate II., which would necessitate strain on the part of the superior oblique muscles. The distortion of the images would increase, and the strain on the superior obliques would be greater, as the axes are revolved farther away from the vertical, the maxi- mum being reached at 45 (c) for the right eye and 135 (c) for the left eye. Passing these points, the distor- tion grows less, until at 180 (d~) for each eye. it disap- pears. Fig. 2 represents the same pair of eyes. If now the axis of the right cylinder should be revolved from 90 (a) to 100 () and that of the left cylinder from 90 () to 80 (), the distortion of images \vould be such as to call into activity the inferior obliques, so that there might be binocular single vision. This distortion would reach its maximum when the axis of the right cylinder stands at 135 (c) and that of the left cylinder at 45 (c), again les- COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. 477 sening as the axes are made to approach the horizontal, where the distortion ceases. Fig 1 . 3, Plate IV., represents a pair of hypermetropic astigmatic eyes with the meridian of greatest curvature of the right at 70 (a) and that of the left at 110 (a). (In all the figures of Plate IV., Plate V., and Plate VI., the mark within the circle shows the location of the meridian of greatest curvature.) These meridians, con- verging above, would cause strain of the inferior ob- liques, which -would be relieved by the correcting cylin- ders, axis of the right at 70 (a) and of the left at 110 (). A revolution of the axis of the right cylinder to 45 (&) and that of the left cylinder to 135 (&) would so dis- place the images as to call into action the superior ob- liques, the displacement increasing as the axes are moved until these points (b for each eye) are reached. Continu- ing the revolution of the cylinders in the same directions, the displacement lessens, and disappears entirely when the axis of the right reaches 20 (c) and that of the left reaches 160 (c), when the necessity for over-action of the obliques no longer exists. If the axes of the cylinders are moved from their correct positions (70 for the right eye and 110 for the left eye) tp 90 (/) for each eye, images will be so displaced as to call into compensating activity the inferior obliques. The maximum of displacement would be effected when the axis reaches 135 (e) in the right 478 COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPIA. eye and 45 (e] in the left eye. Continuing' the revolution in the same directions, the displacement would grow less, and finally disappear when the axis of the right eye stands at d, and that of the left eye at d, each 20 above the hori- zontal. As will be seen, the arc of distortion, so as to throw strain on the superior obliques, is 50 (from 70 to 20 in the right eye and from 110 to 160 in the left eye), while the arc of distortion that would throw strain on the inferior obliques is 130 (from a to d}. Fig 1 . 4, Plate IV., shows the meridians of greatest curvature of these hypermetropic astigmatic eyes at 110 () for the right and 70 () for the left. These me- ridians, diverging above, would call into compensating activity the superior oblique muscles. Correctly chosen and properly placed cylinders, by correcting the distor- tion of the images, would remove the necessity for the performance of the complicated function of the superior obliques. Displacing the axes of these cylinders, the right to 135 (&) and the left to 45 (&), would cause a maximum of distortion of the images, of the kind to call into action the inferior obliques. Continuing the revo- lution of the cylinders, the distortion would disappear when the axis of the right reaches 160 (c) and that of the left reaches 20 (c). Should the axes of the cylinders be revolved from their proper places at 110 (a) in the right and 70 (a) in the left to 90 (/) for each eye, the COMPENSATING CYCLOTROPlA. 479 images would be so changed as to call into harmonious activity the superior obliques. The maximum distortion would occur when the axis of the right is at 45 ( \ or the other. Below, in parallel columns, / \J will be shown the significance of each cut. ;[J & Inclination toward the opposite side points to the superior rectus, while inclination to- ward the same side points to the inferior oblique: Fig. 83 is illus- trative of paralysis of either the supe- rior rectus or the inferior oblique of the eye that sees candle b. If the right eye sees it, the affected mus- cle is the superior rectus; but if the Fig - 83 ' left eye sees it, the affected muscle is the in- ferior oblique. Fig. 84 is illus- trative of paralysis of either the supe- rior rectus or the inferior oblique of the eye that sees candle b. If the right eye sees it, the affected mus- cle is the inferior oblique; but if the left eye sees it, the affected muscle is the su- perior rectus. Fig. 84. As in paresis or paralysis of the sub-vertors, the false candle may be to the right or left of the true, depending on the relative strength of the laterally acting muscles. 610 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS In making a diagnosis of paralysis or paresis of the ocular muscles, by means of the diplopia test, the candle need be carried only in the four cardinal directions that is, the head should be erect; and in testing- for paresis of the right and left vertors, the candle should be carried only along the extended horizontal plane of the head di- rectly to the right and left of the vertical plane; and in testing for paresis of the sub-vertors and supervertors, the candle should be carried only in the extended median plane of the head, above and below the horizontal plane. For detecting paralysis or paresis of the sub-vertors and supervertors, nothing serves better than a horizontal line at a distance of twenty feet. If the sub-vertors are at fault, elevating the head, while still looking at the line, will cause it to double, the false line appearing be- low the true. If the false line leans toward the corre- sponding side, the affected muscle is the inferior rectus; but if it leans toward the opposite side, the affected mus- cle is the superior oblique. When a right vertor or a left vertor is paralyzed, the resulting deviation might be mistaken for comitant lat- eral heterotropia. This may be avoided in two ways first, by a test of the verting power, when the affected eye will always lag behind its fellow if the two eyes are turned in the direction of action of the paretic muscle, whereas, in comitant squint, the deviating eye moves OF THE OCULAR MUSCLES. 611 always through as great an arc as the fixing- eye; sec- ondly, by covering 1 the eyes alternately, the secondary deviation will always be greater than the primary, when there is paralysis. But in comitant squint the secondary and the primary deviations are always the same. In paralytic squint there is always diplopia in one part of the field, with binocular sing-le vision in the op- posite part; while in comitant squint there is no diplo- pia in any part of the field. A very good diagnostic feature is the pose of the head in cases of paralysis of an orbital muscle. In paralysis of a lateral rectus muscle, the face is always turned in the direction of action of the affected muscle that is, if a left vertor is paralyzed, the face will be turned to the left in the interest of binocular single vision, and vice versa if a right vertor is paralyzed; if a sub-vertor is paralyzed, the face will be depressed; and if a super- vertor is paralyzed, the face will be elevated. In "paralysis of motion, rather than of muscle," duc- tion power, which is reflex in the sense that it is not vo- litional, is not involved. This statement covers all the conjugate brain-centers from the first to the fifth, inclu- sive those centers that are concerned with the recti muscles. Since there is no voluntary action of the ob- liques, the cortical centers (if there be such) governing them must act independently of the will. These centers 612 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS are the sixth, seventh, eighth, and ninth. That these conjugate centers for the obliques may be involved in pathologic changes must be conceded Since the object of the sixth and seventh centers is to prevent diplopia, on looking down and up, respectively, these correspond per- fectly in action with the reflex centers of the recti that are also concerned with the prevention of diplopia when images are displaced by prisms; therefore they ought not to be affected in disease of the cortex. The eighth and ninth centers are not concerned with the prevention of diplopia; but, what is probably of as much importance, they are concerned with the steadying of all objects in the field of vision whenever the eyes are voluntarily moved in an oblique direction. For instance, when the gaze is directed up and to the right, or down and to the left, the eyes would be torted to the right, were it not for the eighth conjugate center, when all objects would be made to appear to incline to the left from their real position, their inclination corresponding precisely with the degree of torsioning. This is prevented by the eighth conjugate center, which maintains the paral- lelism between the vertical axes of the eyes and the me- dian plane of the head in such a voluntary rotation. It appears, therefore, that disease of this center would be attended by a wheel-like movement of objects whenever the visual axes are made to move up and to the right, or OF THE OCULAR MUSCLES. 613 down and to the left, which appearance would not be if the gaze were directed up and to the left, or down and to the right. But if the ninth conjugate center were in- volved in pathologic change, the wheel-like movement of objects would be observed only when the gaze is up and to the left, or down and to the right. In neither case would there be diplopia. Should the sixth conjugate center be involved, on look- ing down at a candle it would appear double, the one seen by the right eye leaning to the left and the one seen by the left eye leaning to the right; the diplopia would be attended by dizziness and nausea. In the upper field there would be no diplopia. Should the seventh conjugate center become diseased, the diplopia would be in the upper field, and the candle seen by the right eye would lean to the right, and the one seen by the left eye would incline to the left. It ap- pears that each oblique muscle is connected by individual nerve fibers with three centers one center, basal; the two others, probably cortical. The former center has connected with it fibers from only one muscle, but each of the latter has connected with it fibers from two ob- lique muscles, one of these belonging to one eye and the other to the other eye; and, therefore, they are conjugate centers. All the fibers from these three centers come together and form the trunk of the nerve, a disease of 614 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS which suspends the independent and conjugate action of the muscle supplied by it; and the muscles of the fellow eye are not involved. The right superior oblique is con- nected with the sixth conjugate center, as is also the left superior oblique; the right superior oblique is also con- nected with the eighth conjugate center, as is also the left inferior oblique. Disease of the sixth center, as al- ready shown, gives trouble only when looking directly down; disease of the eighth center causes trouble, as shown above, only when looking up and to the right, or down and to the left. Disease of these two conjugate centers would have no influence over the basal center that gives duction or fusion power to either of the two muscles mentioned that power that is exercised when images are displaced by oblique astigmatism, natural or artificial. Each internus muscle is also connected with three cen- ters one center, basal; the two others, cortical. The for- mer is reflex; the latter, volitional. To illustrate: The right internus has its reflex center the center giving it duction or fusion power in the nucleus of the mortor oculi; it is also connected with the third conjugate brain- center, as is also the left internus; it is also connected with the fifth conjugate center, as is also the left exter- nus. All the fibers from these three centers for the right internus form the bundle that constitutes the branch of OF THE OCULAR MUSCLES. 615 the third nerve, supplying it with its threefold power. Disease of this branch suspends both the reflex (fusion) and voluntary power of this muscle; it can neither ad- duct, converge, nor advert the eye to which it belongs. Disease of the third conjugate center involves only those fibers that convey to the muscle the convergence impulse; disease of the fifth conjugate center involves only those fibers that convey the adversion impulse; likewise dis- ease of the reflex nucleolus involves only those fibers that convey the fusion impulse. Disease of the third conjugate center would suspend, of course, the conver- ging power of the left internus also; while disease of the fifth conjugate center would affect the abverting power of the left externus as well as the adverting power of the right internus. Thus each muscle, with its several centers, might be studied. It only remains to speak of the symptoms that would present themselves, should any one of the five conjugate centers, controlling the recti, become diseased: (1) Disease of the first conjugate center: inability to supervert the eyes, but no diplopia. (2) Disease of the second conjugate center: inability to sub-vert the eyes, but no diplopia. (3) Disease of the third conjugate center: inability to converge the eyes, with diplopia in the near. 616 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS (4) Disease of the fourth conjugate center: inability to rotate the eyes to the right, either cardinally or obliquely, but no diplopia. (5) Disease of the fifth conjugate center: inability to rotate the eyes to the left, either cardinally or obliquely, but no diplopia. TREATMENT. In any form of paralysis of muscle that is, when the disease causing it is below the internal capsule the diplopia should be prevented by covering the affected eye, which will relieve all nervous symptoms, such as headache, dizziness, and nausea. The affected eye should be kept under cover until the disease has been cured. In paralysis of the third nerve, nature supplies the cover in the production of ptosis, and usually the last muscle, supplied by the third nerve, to regain its power is the elevator of the upper lid. If any case is clearly rheumatic, it should be treated with large doses of the salicylate of sodium or other anti-rheumatic rem- edy; if the cause is syphilis, iodide of potassium in in- creasingly large doses should be given after meals; if the cause is not known, the case should be treated with the iodide of potassium. Early in any case the admin- istration of the fluid extract of jaborandi, in doses of twenty drops at 9 A.M., 3 P.M., and 9 P.M., by pro- OF THE OCULAR MUSCLES. 617 moting absorption of effused serum, will greatly aid the iodides in the work of hastening- the absorption of plas- tic effusion. Bichloride of mercury in small doses may also be given. The above remedies should be continued until the diplopia has entirely disappeared. This much having been accomplished, the sulphate of strychnia in doses of iro to 53 of a grain should be g-iven before each meal. At this stag-e the interrupted current of electricity, used once daily for ten minutes, will do g-ood. While there is still diplopia, the strychnia and electricity would do harm, rather than g-ood. In old cases of ocular paralysis, when there can be no long-er any hope of restoration of power to the paralyzed muscle, surgery will do good, in that it will lessen the field of diplopia and give to the patient a more natural pose of the head. The operation should be either an extensive shortening or advancement of the paralyzed muscle, and never even a partial tenotomy of the antag- onist. The muscle plane should be changed or not, when making the shortening or advancement, as may be indicated by the existence or non-existence of torsion. 618 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS LAGOPHTHALMOS. The condition termed " lagophthalmos " was so named because it gave to the human eye the appearance of the eye of the hare always open, asleep or awake. The cause of the condition is disease of the seventh nerve in its course; or at its basal or cortical centers, or between these two in the internal capsule or in the corona striata. In either case there is a greater or less loss of power on the part of the corresponding- orbicularis, and usually the muscles of the face are also involved. The location of the disease or injury determines the array of symptoms to be presented in any case. When the part involved is the basal center of the nerve, or the body of the nerve as it finds its way out to be finally distributed to all the muscles of the face, includ- ing 1 the orbicularis of the corresponding" side, all these muscles will be paralyzed, and the log^aphthalmos will be only one of the symptoms. The skin of the forehead on the affected side is smooth, as if "ironed out," and the patient is wholly unable to throw it into wrinkles for the reason that this half of the anterior part of the occipito-frontalis is supplied by the diseased nerve; the corrug^ator supercilii must also be inactive, therefore the brow on that side cannot be drawn down; the vari- ous muscles connected with the nose and mouth, action of which gx>es so far to give agreeableness of expression to OF THE OCULAR MUSCLES. 619 the face, cannot receive a nerve impulse, therefore they must be inactive, and the face, on that side, becomes expressionless; the buccinator, which is also supplied by the seventh nerve, loses its power and thus the mastica- tion of food on that side becomes inconvenient almost impossible for the reason that, when the tongue presses the food between the teeth, the buccinator being unable to make counter pressure, the food cannot be kept between the grinders. Soon the patient learns to do all his chewing on the unaffected side. The mouth is always drawn toward the unaffected side, and a laugh will be confined wholly to this side. The patient drinks with difficulty and cannot whistle. All of these symptoms will be associated with the inability to close the eye. Both parts the voluntary and the involuntary of the orbicularis being involved, the eye will be wide open, and there will be no power to close it. Any great effort to close it will only cause the eye to move upward, as if to hide behind the upraised lid. The lower lid will not be held in contact with the globe, hence the punctum will be displaced. The absence of the batting power makes it impossible for the punctum of the upper lid to carry off the tears; hence there must be an excess of tears in the conjunctival sac. The unprotected eye becomes irritable, as shown by conjunctival redness, ex- 620 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS cessive secretion of tears, and some dread of light. If the eye is exposed too long and too severely, the cornea may ulcerate, when the eye, of course, becomes painful. By far the most common cause of lagophthalmos, as- sociated with the other symptoms already described, is inflammation of the middle ear, and the reason is not hard to find. The seventh nerve, as it passes through the aqueduct of Fallopius, in the inner bony wall of the drum cavity, should be completely covered in by bone structure. That this bone-covering is complete, in many cases, is made evident by the fact that otitis media is not attended by involvement of the seventh nerve ; that it is incomplete, in some cases, is made equally evi- dent because of the fact that in some cases even a very slight otitis media is attended by paralysis of all the muscles of the face. Whenever there is a break in the bony covering, the mucous membrane lining the inner wall of the drum cavity must lie directly in contact with the sheath of the seventh nerve, hence the readiness with which an inflammation of this membrane may in- volve the -nerve. Catarrhal, as well as suppurative, inflammation of the middle ear, in such cases, almost always causes facial paralysis. When such is the cause of facial paralysis, the usual symptoms of inflammation of the ear are present namely: pain, fullness, and deaf- ness, with some fever. The objective symptoms are OF THE OCULAR MUSCLES. 621 also present. If the inflammation causing- the paralysis is between the basal center of the seventh nerve and the point where the eighth nerve parts company with the facial nerve, to find its terminals in the internal ear, the resulting pressure will cause deafness, as well as pa- ralysis of the facial muscles; but other symptoms and signs of otitis media will be absent. In the latter case the paralysis of the orbicularis will be as complete as in the former. If the inflammation or injury of the facial nerve is at some point between where it leaves the aque- duct of Pallopius and the point where it divides into its several branches, the facial paralysis will be complete; but the hearing will not be involved, nor will there be any other symptoms referable to the ear. In all of these conditions the batting- power is lost. When. the cause of the lag-ophthalmos is in the cortex, the lids are not so widely separated, and their batting- power, thoug-h modified, is not lost. The other muscles of the face may not be involved. If the disease in the cortex, in the internal capsule, or in the corona radiata is extensive, other symptoms will be found in association with the lag-ophthalmos. Such cases are not so likely to recover full voluntary power over the orbicularis. TREATMENT. Whatever may be the cause, the eye should be pro- tected by a flap until such time as the lids may have 622 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS. recovered their power; otherwise, the cornea may ulcer- ate. If otitis media is the cause, this condition should be so treated as to prevent suppuration, if possible ; but if suppuration occurs, the disease should be so treated as to bring it, as speedily as possible, under control. In all cases the iodide of potassium and the bichloride of mercury should be administered for promoting- absorp- tion of inflammatory deposits within the sheath of the nerve. In this work these drug's can be greatly aided by the fluid extract of jaborandi, in twenty-drop doses, three times a day for the first two weeks. Strychnia should be given only when a return of voluntary power to the facial muscles shows that the pressure of inflam- matory deposits has been relieved more or less com- pletely. When the cause of lagophthalmos is above the basal, or reflex, center, sorbefacient treatment is indi- cated; but, as already stated, recovery is both slow and doubtful o CHAPTER XII. MUSCLES OF THE IRIS AND OF THE CILIARY BODY. THE heading- of this chapter is intended to convey the idea that the iris has more than one muscle-, and that in the ciliary body there is more than one muscle. Both anatomy and physiology g"ive evidence of the existence of two muscles in the iris; anatomy shows two sets of muscular fibers in the ciliary body, and there is also physiologic evidence of the existence of two independent muscles, each under a separate innervation. MUSCLES OF THE IRIS. At one time in the history of medicine it was taug-ht that there was erectile tissue in the iris which, by be- coming- filled with blood, would make the pupil small, and, by ag-ain becoming- empty, would effect the dilatation of the pupil. Other theories were offered from time to time, accounting- for the chang-eableness of the pupil- lary opening- in the iris, without taking- into account the necessity for muscular contraction to effect these (623) 624 MUSCLES OF THE IRIS. changes. As early as the tenth century an Arabian physician announced his belief in the existence of mus- cles in the iris, contractions of which varied the size of the pupil, his only evidence being physiologic. In his day theie were no means of investigation to determine anatomically the existence of muscle structure so small. Descartes, though neither an anatomist nor a physiolo- gist, announced in one of his publications, in the seven- teenth century, that muscles in the iris regulated the pupillary opening. The existence of radiating fibers was generally con- ceded a long time before Berger announced, in 1701, that orbicular fibers also existed in the iris. It was not until 1812 that the microscope, under the eye of Mau- noir, revealed the existence of circular fibers in the iris of the bird. Prom that time to the present there has been no one ready to deny the existence of the circular muscle of the iris. The microscope has since demon- strated the existence of the sphincter muscle of the iris in man. It is known to be a circular muscle-band, about one millimeter wide, at the pupillary margin of the iris and nearer its posterior surface. Radiating muscular fibers in the iris has been a mat- ter of much discussion, but now the preponderance of evidence is in favor of the existence of such fibers. Grunhagen, Collins, and others have taught that there is MUSCLES OF THE IRIS. 625 elastic tissue in the iris that effects the dilatation of the pupil whenever action of the sphincter is inhibited that the dilatation is effected passively, and not actively. Juler, at the eighth International Congress of Ophthal- mology, in 1894, announced that, by a new method of investigation, he had been able to show, under the microscope, the radiating muscular structure of the iris. This anatomic evidence, associated with the evidence given by physiology, removes the question of the exist- ence of a pupil-dilator muscle entirely from the domain of doubt. Juler has demonstrated that the radiating muscular fibers have their origin at the attached margin of the iris, and pass to the pupillary margin, where they become blended with the sphincter muscle. Petit observed, in 1727, that a division of the cervical sympathetic caused contraction of the pupil; and Biffi, in 1846, showed that stimulation of the cervical sympa- thetic effected dilatation of the pupil. By a similar method of investigation, Mayo, in 1823, proved that the sphincter muscle of the iris is supplied by the third cranial nerve, a division of which dilates the pupil, while stimulation of this nerve makes the pupil contract. There is no room for doubting the existence of these two muscles in the iris ; and that they are antagonistic the one contracting the pupil, the other effecting its dila- tation is as little open to doubt. 626 MUSCLES OF THE IRIS. Those fibers of the third nerve and of the cervical sympathetic, that give power, respectively, to the pupil- contractor muscle and the pupil-dilator muscle, pass first to the ciliary ganglion, in which they are joined by fibers from the fifth nerve. From this g-anglion the several short ciliary nerves, each containing fibers from the three different sources, make their way to the pos- terior part of the eye, which they enter. They are finally distributed to the ciliary body and the iris, giv- ing sensation to these structures and power to their mus- cular parts. The function of the iris is to regulate the quantity of light that enters the eye, and this it does reflexly. A too-bright light makes it necessary for the pupil to be made smaller, which is effected by an impulse sent from the nucleolus of the third nerve-center that controls the sphincter of the iris; a dim light takes the nucleolus of the sphincter off its guard and allows the influence of the cervical sympathetic on the radiating fibers of the iris to effect the dilatation of the pupil. There should be harmony in the antagonism of these two muscles, for the purpose of properly regulating the quantity of the light that enters the eye. The iris also serves to cut off the peripheral rays of every cone of light, which enables the refractive media to make a focus sharper than it would be if spherical aberration were not thus prevented. MUSCLES OP THE IRIS. 627 In most cases the muscles of the iris do their work well. There are cases, however, in which either the sphincter muscle is too weak, because of poor develop- ment or slight innervation, or the pupil-dilator muscle is abnormally strong from hyper-development or over- stimulation; as a result, the pupil is too large under ordinary light and when shaded. Under the stimulus of strong light it contracts; but if such exposure is pro- longed, the contraction of the sphincter becomes painful. Any one of several symptoms of eye-strain may be caused by prolonged contraction of a weak sphincter of the iris. Such patients cannot endure, with any com- fort, prolonged exposure to bright light, whether nat- ural or artificial. The author has observed a fairly large number of such cases, and has had a fair degree of success in managing them. A weak pupil-dilator mus- cle is not likely to be the source of annoying symptoms. A patient with weak pupil contractors should have these muscles treated, regardless of what else must be done for the relief of other conditions that may cause strain. TREATMENT. Patients with large pupils, who suffer when exposed to bright light, may have their pupil contractors devel- oped and strengthened by exercise. The method is as follows : Before retiring at night, the patient should 628 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. sit before a fairly bright lamp, holding- in one hand a densely opaque cardboard which she should place be- tween her and the light at regular intervals of two or three seconds, looking all the time in the direction of the light. Thus there would be alternate exposure of the eyes to light and darkness, producing rhythmic contraction and relaxation of the sphincter ol the iris. For this exercise ten minutes will be sufficient; but should the e} T es be- come fatigued, the exercise should cease sooner. The exercise should be repeated every night for a long w r hile. In the absence of the exercise treatment, the only thing to be advised is the wearing of smoke lenses con- stantly, or whenever the eyes are to be exposed to bright light. These lenses allow the pupils to become more or less enlarged, which means that the sphincters are thereby rested. This method favors the muscle in its weakness, and is not to be preferred to the exercise treatment, for the latter means that the sphincter will be made strong eventually. MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. It was not known until 1846 that there were any mus- cular fibers in the ciliary body; but at this time Bowman discovered, with the microscope, muscular fibers which were parallel with the meridians of the eye, hence prop- erly called them "meridional." These he considered as MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 629 having" their origin in the anterior part of the ciliary body and their insertion in the anterior part of the choroid. He named this newly discovered muscle the "tensor of the choroid," and in it he thought he had found the secret of the power of accommodation. His explanation was that this muscle, by making tense the choroid, so compressed and changed the shape of the vitreous as to force the lens forward, thereby increasing the distance between the macula and the lens sufficiently, he thought, to account for the phenomena of accommo- dation. Before that time no theory of accommodation had included the idea that there existed, in the ciliary body, a muscle. So thoroughly satisfied was Bowman that he did not pursue his investigations further, else he would have discovered, also, the circular fibers which were discovered later by H. Miiller. Long before the discovery of either Bowman's muscle or Miiller's muscle (that there are two muscles will be shown farther on), Young had demonstrated an increase in the curvature of the crystalline lens in the act of accommodation. Miil- ler thought that, by contraction of the circular muscle discovered by himself, direct pressure, by the ciliary processes, on the periphery of the lens, caused the in- crease in convexity; but Helmholtz had taught that the increase in curvature was due to inherent elasticity of the lens, which was allowed to manifest itself because of 630 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. relaxation of the zonula, effected by the contraction of Bowman's muscle. It would seem that all authors are agreed that in the ciliary body there are two sets of muscular fibers one set running- parallel with the meridians, and another col- lection of fibers forming a circle in the anterior part of the ciliary body. There is universal agreement, also, that one or the other of these muscles, or both, is the active agent in accommodation. It is also universally conceded that the accommodative muscle is supplied by the third nerve. The microscope has revealed the fact that there are two muscles or, at least, two different arrangements of muscular fibers in the ciliary body; the existence of two independent muscles, each getting its nerve supply from a separate source, appears to be shown by physiology. Young, in 1801, demonstrated increased curvature of the lens in accommodation; Helmholtz accounted for this in relaxation of the zonula by a contraction of the muscle of Bowman the so-called "tensor of the choroid " when he could have accounted for it more easily in a contrac- tion of the muscle of Miiller, which, by making more narrow the zone between the ciliary processes and the periphery of the lens, necessarily relaxed the suspensory ligament of the lens, thus allowing the lens to manifest its elasticity. By becoming' more convex, the lens would MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 631 necessarily take up the laxness of the ligament. A fact strongly favoring the idea that the muscle of accommo- dation is Miiller's muscle is that Coccius has seen, in eyes on which an iridectomy had been done, a narrowing of the circumlental zone in the act of accommodation. It is easier to understand how the circular muscle (Miiller's) could relax the zonula, and thus enable the lens to become more convex, than it is to understand how Bowman's muscle could effect the same changes. In fact, it would appear that, if Bowman's muscle is the active agent in accommodation, the zonula would be made more tense, and would, therefore, prevent the lens from increasing in convexity; but, on the contrary, the lens would be compressed toward its periphery. If Bowman's muscle is the active agent in accommodation, it is supplied by the third, or mortor oculi, nerve, and the change of the lens would be more in position than in curvature; the change in curvature would not be an in- crease in spherical curvature, but it would be the forma- tion of a lenticonus. There, then, could be no possible use for the circular muscle of Miiller. If the Miiller muscle is the active agent in accommodation, it is sup- plied by the third, or mortor oculi, nerve, and the change in the lens would not be in position, but wholly in curva- ture; the change in curvature would be in an increase of the sphericity of the lens a shortening of the radius 632 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. of the anterior curve made possible by a narrowing of the circumlental zone. If this is the work of the Miiller muscle, there would still remain a work to be done by the Bowman muscle, which will be set forth farther on. The answer to the question, "Is the muscle of Bow- man or the muscle of Muller the active agent in accom- modation ? " must come from a study of the actual change that occurs in the lens in the accommodative act. If Bowman's muscle is the muscle of accommodation, Tscherning's views as to the condition of the zonula and the state of the lens in accommodation must be correct that is, the zonula must be on the stretch, and the ante- rior surface of the lens must assume the shape to which he has given the name "lenticonus." If in accommoda- tion the zonula is relaxed, a more rapid spherical curva- ture of the lens is possible, and these changes can be ef- fected only by the contraction of Miiller's muscle. Helm- holtz would be correct, at least in part; he would be wrong only in the thought that the narrowing of the circumlental zone had been effected by Bowman's muscle. Is the change in the lens the formation of a lenticonus, according to Tscherning, or is it an increase in the curvature of the anterior surface, according to Young, Helmholtz, Bonders, and many others? One thing should be conceded at the outset that is, there is in accommo- dation a narrowing of the circumlental zone, the space MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 633 between the ciliary processes and the periphery or equa- tor of the lens. All writers agree that in the absence of accommodation the two surfaces of the lens are spher- ical. Now, suppose the eye of one individual to be em- metropic, and the eye of another person to be hyperopic. The emmetropic eye, without any accommodative effort, has vision equal ^x, and that through a lens whose sur- face must be conceded to be spherical. The person with the hyperopic eye (say of 3. D), by the aid of his accom- modative power, also has vision equal ||. Surfaces dif- ferently curved cannot refract light in the same way. If the emmetropic eye saw through a lens having a spherically curved surface, the hyperopic eye, in the act of accommodation for distant seeing, must have had also a lens whose surface was spherically curved. If ac- commodation of 3. D for distant seeing does not produce a lenticonus, is it possible that accommodation of 3. D for near seeing will do so? Again, suppose one person to be emmetropic and sixty years old, but still possessing a perfectly transparent lens; and suppose another person emmetropic and sixteen years old. These two persons will have equally good distant vision, without any accom- modative effort on the part of either. At a distance of thirteen inches the older person, not having any accom- modative power, cannot see to read; while the younger person, by the exercise of accommodation, sees every 634 MUSCLES OP THE CILIARY BODY. word and letter perfectly. On placing 1 a plus 3. D lens before the eye of the older person, everything 1 on the page becomes as clear and as distinct to him as it is to the young-er person. The lens of the older person has not lost its sphericity in changing* the point of view from twenty feet to thirteen inches, for he has no accommoda- tive power; but for him the plus 3. D spherical lens has made divergent rays parallel, and these parallel rays are easily and accurately focused on his retina by his own spherically curved crystalline lens. If the two spher- ically curved bodies the presbyopic lens and his crys- talline lens have made the old man see perfectly, has it been necessary for the surface of the crystalline lens in the young 1 person to become changed to a lenticonus in order that he might see distinctly at thirteen inches? Either the surface of the crystalline lens in the young person has not changed from a spherical curve, when there was no accommodation, to a lenticonus, in accom- modation, or a presbyopic lens should be a lenticonus, and not spherical. In conclusion, this statement will be made without fear of contradiction: Spherical refractive surfaces, with the peripheral rays cut off by a perfo- rated diaphragm, can reproduce perfectly, point for point, an image of the object from which the rays of light have come. For the reasons given above, the logical conclusion is MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 635 that in accommodation the zonula is relaxed by a nar- rowing- of the circumlental zone, that this laxness is taken up by an increase in the convexity of the elastic crystalline lens, and that these changes can be effected only by a contraction of Miiller's muscle. Since Miil- ler's muscle is the active agent in accommodation, it is supplied by the third nerve. Since Tscherning published his views concerning- ac- commodation, it has been demonstrated by Hess that the zonula is relaxed in accommodation; and Priestly Smith has claimed that it is possible for the anterior surface of the lens to assume the lenticonus shape, in accommoda- tion, when the zonula is relaxed. As already stated, there is a use for the muscle of Bowman, reg-ardless of the fact that it is not the active agent in accommodation. There is a reason for believ- ing- that this muscle, like the radiating- muscle of the iris, is supplied by the cervical sympathetic, through the medium of the ciliary ganglion. It is generally taught that at least some of the meridional fibers extend from the anterior part of the choroid through the ciliary bod}*, terminating in the part occupied by the circular muscle (Miiller's) in the region of the attachment of the zonula to the ciliary processes. They pursue such a course and are so related to the suspensory ligament that it appears highly probable that it is through the active agency of 636 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. these fibers (Bowman's muscle) that the crystalline lens is made to assume a mathematically correct position in its bed in the anterior part of the vitreous. This normal and mathematically correct position the position that it must occupy when there is no corneal astigmatism is such that the antero-posterior axis shall coincide with the visual axis, and that its equatorial plane shall be parallel with the equator of the eye. In the process of development the lens, in many instances, may passively assume the proper position; and, if so, there would be nothing" for Bowman's muscle to do; but in many in- stances the passive position assumed by this lens in its development would not be mathematically correct, hence the need of some active agent for readjusting. This agent could be, and doubtless is, Bowman's muscle, and the medium through which this muscle could effect the readjustment is the suspensory ligament the zonula. The agent calling on Bowman's muscle to do this work would be the guiding sensation of the retina, which is the taskmaster of all the ocular muscles, intrinsic and extrinsic. The time for effecting this readjustment would be after birth, for before birth the guiding sensa- tion must be dormant. The task of readjustment is, doubtless, fully accomplished in infancy, or, at the latest, early in childhood. It is not more wonderful that Bow- man's muscle should be endowed with this unvolitional MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 637 power than it is that Muller's muscle, likewise under the control of the guiding sensation, should be endowed with power to mathematically adjust vision for all dis- tances. If Bowman's muscle can readjust the lens in an eye whose cornea is free from astigmatism, so that its posi- tion shall be mathematically correct, is it not also pos- sible that the same muscle may be the active agent in producing a lenticular astigmatism at right angles to a corneal astigmatism for the purpose of neutralizing it? Suppose a case of corneal astigmatism of 2. D, with the meridian of greatest curvature at one hundred and eighty degrees; to correct this by a neutralizing lenticular astig- matism, an impulse would have to be sent to a collection of meridional fibers, either directly above or below the lens, a contraction of which would tilt the lens on its hori- zontal axis so as to increase the refractive power of its vertical meridian to the extent of 2. D. Thus would the corneal astigmatism be neutralized. It is well known that the tilting of a lens increases its refractive power at right angles to the axis of tilting. Tilting a lens forty- five degrees practically doubles its strength at right an- gles to the axis of rotation, without changing its power in line with this axis; and in this way a spherical lens is made to have an astigmatic effect. The strength of the crystalline lens is so great that a very slight tilting 638 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. would produce a considerable astigmatic effect. L/entic- ular astigmatism cannot be effected by a contraction of Miiller's muscle, but it can be easily effected by the ac- tion of individual fibers cf Bowman's muscle. How the guiding sensation can select and call into action a few fibers of Bowman's muscle, yet allow all the balance of this muscle to remain quiet, is not subject to expla- nation. The author does not have to suppose a case of corneal astigmatism that, in some way or other, was neutralized automatically, to a considerable extent, for many years. His own corneal astigmatism is 2.50 D, and has been so since 1889, when Swan M. Burnett, of Washington, meas- ured it with Javal's ophthalmometer. The first attempt at astigmatic correction by artificial means was made in 1882, under the influence of atropine. The correcting cylinder for each eye was plus .65. In 1885, again under a mydriatic, L. Webster Fox, of Philadelphia, found the correcting cylinder for each eye to be plus .75. In 1887 a plus 1. cylinder for each eye was given. Again, in 1888, a plus 1.25 cylinder for each eye fully corrected the manifest astigmatism, a mydriatic again being used this time, homatropine, gr. viii. to water oz. i. Ten drops, one every five minutes, were placed in each eye, and at the end of half an hour the examination was made. From that time until 1895 more astigmatism became MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 639 manifest, but the stronger cylinder was not given until that date. The cylinder then given was plus 2. D for each eye. In 1900 the cylinder given for the right eye was plus 2.25, and that given the left eye was plus 2.50 practically a full correction of the corneal astigmatism, which during at least eleven years had remained the same, as shown by the Javal ophthalmometer. The author has thus related his own experience for the purpose of giving emphasis to two points: (1) There was a lenticular astigmatism that almost completely neutral- ized the corneal astigmatism up to the age of twenty- eight years; (2) the power that effected the neutralizing lenticular astigmatism was not suspended by the repeated use of mvdriatics, and this power, therefore, could not have been given through the fibers of the third nerve that come from the ciliary ganglion. The logical con- clusion is that the lenticular astigmatism was produced by fibers of Bowman's muscle, and that these fibers de- rived their power from the cervical sympathetic. A personal experience, similar to that through which the author has passed, has been related by Edward Jack- son, of Denver. What Jackson's explanation is, the au- thor does not know. Doubtless many others have had a similar personal experience; and it is not too much to say that every observer has had such cases in his prac- tice. Occasionally men of large experience have seen 640 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. cases of astigmatism to be accounted for only by an as- tigmatic condition of the crystalline lens. It may be that, in some cases, a much higher degree than 2.50 D of corneal astigmatism could be neutralized by a lenticular astigmatism. It is true, nevertheless, that, in many cases, even a low degree of astigmatism is not always thus neutralized, the Bowman muscle being unable, from some cause, to effect the necessary change in the lens. If a tilting of the lens by contraction of a single part of Bowman's muscle is not the cause of the lenticular astigmatism, the simultaneous and equal action of two opposite parts of Bowman's muscle, by making tense the corresponding parts of the zonula, could so compress the part of the lens intervening as to increase its refractive power, thus effecting lenticular astigmatism. It would appear that, in one of these two ways, the muscle of Bowman causes a neutralizing lenticular astigmatism. If this change is caused by a tilting of the lens, it is reasonable to suppose that the work of tilting could be transferred from one part of the muscle to another part directly opposite. For instance, in the production of a lenticular astigmatism for neutralizing a corneal astig- matism meridian of greatest curvature, vertical the lens could be tilted on its vertical axis by contraction of either the nasal or the temporal part of Bowman's mus- MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 641 cle; for the effect would be the same, whether the nasal part of the lens is thrown forward by contraction of the temporal part of the muscle, or thrown backward by a contraction of the nasal part of the muscle. Both of these parts would have to contract simultaneously and equally if the lenticular astigmatism is a result of change in curvature, and not a change in position. In either in- stance, the work done would be strain, and possibly one of the worst forms of eye-strain. After the foregoing study of the muscles located in the ciliary body, this chapter may be concluded with a study of the normal work required of each in some con- ditions of refraction, and the abnormal work required of them in other states of refraction. THE FUNCTION OF THE MULLER MUSCLE. No mus- cle of the eye is ever called on to do either normal or ab- normal work for any other purpose than the improve- ment of vision, and these calls are always made b} r the guiding sensation, which resides in the macula, or, at the farthest, within the retinal area of binocular fusion. The contraction of Miiller's muscle is intended only to increase the refractive power of the crystalline lens equally in all of its meridians. This change in the re- fractive power of the lens is effected by two agents, Miiller's muscle being the active agent and the elasticity of the lens being the passive agent. When the Miiller 642 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. muscle is at rest, the zonula is tense, and, by its pressure against the lens, suspends the elasticity of the latter, in which state it has its minimum power of refraction. When the guiding sensation calls for a sharper image on the macula, the response comes in an impulse sent through those fibers of the third nerve that end in the muscle of accommodation (Miiller's). At once this muscle contracts equally in its entire extent, relaxing the zonula, the laxness of which is at once taken up by an increase in the convexity of the lens by its own elas- ticity. The degree of contraction of the muscle is reg- ulated with mathematical precision, and the change in refractive power of the lens is only enough to satisfy the guiding sensation. EMMETROPIA. In this condition of refraction, Miil- ler's muscle is never called into action when the object looked at is in the distance; but when the point of fixa- tion is at thirteen inches, in response to a demand from the guiding sensation, an impulse is sent to this muscle sufficiently powerful to effect a contraction that will in- crease the refraction of the crystalline lens by 3. D. At nine inches a 4. D impulse must be sent to the muscle; but when the point of fixation is at the distance of 1 M, only a 1. D impulse is needed. Looking again into practical infinity any distance beyond twenty feet the Miiller muscle goes into a state of rest, to be aroused MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 643 into activity again only when the point of view is near by. In distant seeing 1 , not only is the muscle of accom- modation at rest, but all of the recti; and the obliques should also be in a state of rest, and they will be in this state if there is orthophoria. As shown elsewhere, the brain-center controlling- the ciliary muscle (Miiller's) is in some way associated with the third conjugate center ( convergence center), so that for every dioptre of ac- commodation there is a convergence of each visual axis of nearly two degrees of arc, which would mean about four degrees of prism a point not made clear in the study of pseudo-esophoria. In emmetropia and orthophoria everything is favorable for comfort in the use of the eyes. Such eyes, under or- dinary use, can give trouble only when in the emme- tropic eye there is a weak muscle of accommodation or when the orthophoria is asthenic. What should be done for the asthenic orthophoria has already been set forth; what should be done to strengthen a weak muscle of ac- commodation will be shown farther on in this chapter. In a normal condition, the strength of Miiller's muscle varies with the age of the patient, until advancing years deprive it of all power. The table showing the varia- tion in strength, as given by Jackson, is probably as nearly correct as any. The error, if any, is in the show- ing of too much ciliary power after the age of thirty 644 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. years. This table shows the relative or associated ac- commodation, and is much greater than would be shown if accommodation, unassociated with convergence, were tested with concave lenses. Jackson's table is as fol- lows: Accommodation Distance of Point Age. in Dioptres. of Fixation Inches. 10 14. 2.81 15 12. 3.28 20 10. 8.94 25 9. 4.4 30 8. 4.9 35 7. 5.6 40 5.5 7.1 45 4. 9.84 50 2.5 15.75 55 1.25 31.5 60 .5 75.74 65 .0 .00 The above table shows that the amplitude of accom- modation, which is the distance between the far and the near points, grows less with advancing years, until, at the age of sixty-five years, the near point has receded so far that it becomes one with the far point. The recession of the near point is due either to an ac- tual weakening of Miiller's muscle or to a gradual loss of elasticity of the lens, probably both. If the muscle could be kept strong, the elasticity of the lens would doubtless MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 645 be maintained longer. The method for keeping the cil- iary muscle strong will be set forth in answer to the question: ' ' Can presbyopia be deferred ? ' ' HYPEROPIA AND HYPEROPIC ASTIGMATISM. In ei- ther of these conditions of refraction the guiding sensa- tion will call for activity of Miiller's muscle in both dis- tant and near seeing. In hyperopia the accommodation, unassociated with convergence, must effect just such an increase of the refractive power of the lens, for dis- tant vision, as will render sharp the image of the object of fixation. This necessity for the exercise of the ac- commodation for distant seeing disturbs the harmony be- tween accommodation and convergence. For every di- optre of accommodation for distance there is developed nearly two degrees (of arc) of pseudo-esophoria, which would be measured by a prism of nearly four degrees. In the discussion of pseudo-esophoria this distinction be- tween arc and prism degrees was not made; hence the author would emphasize it here. Wherever the quantity of pseudo-esophoria is given in the chapter on esophoria, it should be read "of arc," or it should be multiplied by two to read "of prism." In manifest hyperopic astigmatism, simple or com- pound, the guiding sensation must be satisfied when Miiller's muscle has so accommodated as to place the focal interval on the macula, for vision of astigmatics is 646 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. best for all parts of an object when the anterior focus is just as far in front of the retina as the posterior focus is behind it. This is shown in the half-tone cut on page 434. However, the muscle can accommodate so as to place either the anterior or the posterior focus on the macula in the first instance, sharpening 1 lines at right angles to the meridian that is most curved; in the second instance, sharpening lines that are parallel with the most curved meridian. This irregular, zigzag con- traction of Miiller's muscle is what occurs when hyper- opic astigmatic eyes look at checked goods a very un- pleasant thing for them to do. This work of Miiller's muscle to sharpen images in hyperopia and hyperopic astigmatism is abnormal, and always develops a pseudo-esophoria. The pseudo-eso- phoria manifests itself in one of three ways: First, it shows an esophoria, when, intrinsically, there is ortho- phoria; second, it shows a greater amount of esophoria than really exists; third, it lessens an existing intrinsic exophoria. In either instance, if the muscle of accom- modation is not of subnormal development, the pseudo- esophoria will be the same in both far and near vision. In any case of hyperopia or hyperopic astigmatism, the error must be considered not wholly with reference to the muscle of Miiller, but the inherent condition of the lateral recti muscles must also be taken into consid- MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 647 eration. If there is lateral orthophoria, and especially so if there is an inherent esophoria, the abnormal work re- quired of the muscles of accommodation should be re- lieved by a full correction of the focal error, and the lenses should be worn for both far and near seeing. If there is inherent exophoria, it would be better, in many cases, especially those having- strong- muscles of accom- modation, to correct the hyperopic astigmatism with minus cylinders, and to leave uncorrected any low de- gree of hyperopia, in order that the pseudo-esophoria may neutralize, wholly or in part, the inherent exo- phoria. In every case of hyperopic astigmatism a full correction of all the error that can be made manifest should be given, using either plus or minus cylinders, as might be indicated by an associated study of the focal error and the intrinsic state of the muscles. If minus cylinders are used because of a complicating exophoria, the hyperopic astigmatism, which is worse, is converted into a simple hyperopia, which is better. How to deal directly with any inherent muscle imbalance, has already been set forth in preceding chapters. MYOPIA AND MYOPIC ASTIGMATISM. When either of these errors exists, the guiding sensation does not call on Miiller's muscle to do any work when the object of fixation is in the distance. If there is simple myopia of 3. JD, or more, the muscle of accommodation is not called 648 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. into action, even in the near use of the eyes; so that in such eyes the muscle remains inactive, whatever may be the location of the point of view, unless it should be brought nearer the eyes than their far points. If there is manifest myopic astigmatism, the accommodative mus- cle will be brought into activity only when the eyes are used in near vision and for the purpose of placing the focal interval on the macula. In compound myopic as- tigmatism, the myopia being 3. D or more, the muscle of Miiller will remain inactive, regardless of whether the eyes are being used either in the far or in the near. In nryopia of less than 3. D, with the page at thirteen inches, the guiding sensation calls only for such accommodative action as will increase the refractive power of the lens to correspond, in dioptres, with the difference between the number of dioptres of myopia and 3. D. This work of the ciliary muscle to improve vision in the near when there is myopic astigmatism, and to give the proper near point when there is a low degree of myopia is abnormal work, though less than is required when there is emme- tropia. These refractive errors interfere in no way with any intrinsic muscle condition, when the point of view is in the distance; but in the near use of the eyes there is a resultant pseudo-exophoria. This may show itself in any one of three ways: First, there will be an exophoria in the near, when, intrinsicall)', the lateral muscles may MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. be well balanced; second, the manifest exophoria will be more than the real; third, the pseudo-exophoria may serve to lessen an inherent esophoria. In any case, a full correction of a myopic error for the purpose of distant seeing- should be given, since such a correction will not excite into activity the muscle of ac- commodation, nor will it interfere with any existing state of the lateral muscles. There can arise no disad- vantage from wearing the correcting lenses for distant vision, and there is a very great source of pleasure given in making sharp and well defined objects that are re- mote. When the associated muscle condition is inherent orthophoria, and especially when there is exophoria in the near, myopes should wear their correcting lenses for both far and near seeing; but when there is esophoria, as will be shown in both the distant and the near tests, the myope will have added comfort by removing his lenses for all near work, in that the pseudo-exophoria will lessen the intrinsic esophoria. In all cases, myopic astigmatism should be corrected by a proper minus cyl- inder when there is lateral orthophoria or exophoria in the near, but by plus cylinders, to be worn only in read- ing or in other near work, when there is a complicating esophoria. So it appears that myopic errors must be studied and treated by taking into consideration the Miiller muscle and the lateral recti muscles. 650 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. WEAKNESS OF MULLER'S MUSCLE. As already shown, the amplitude of accommodation diminishes with advancing- years. Whether this failing accommodation is due to loss of power in the muscle, or whether it is because the lens, growing- more dense, continually suf- fers loss of elasticity, is still a subject for discussion. Probably the two conditions enter as factors in the de- velopment of presbyopia. The weakening- of the muscle of accommodation may favor the loss of elasticity, and the loss of elasticity may react on the weak muscle. Presbyopia shows itself in a recession of the near point until, finally, it has been removed inconveniently far. Before this, however, the muscle may have become so weak that its use in reading- or other near work bring-s on fatig-ue, headache, or other symptoms of eye-strain. In such cases one of two thing's should be done: The muscle should be helped by increasing its strength by exercise, or by supplementing its power. Can presbyopia be deferred ? What the author wrote in 1894, in answer to this question, so perfectly coincides with his present views, that he reproduces it here: ' ' That the leading cause of old sight is failure of cil- iary power we think may be proved; and if this is true, simple and scientific means for deferring the onset of presbyopia may be brought into use. Rhythmic exer- cise can be as readily effected in the ciliary muscle as in MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 651 any of the extra-ocular muscles. Will this exercise de- velop the power of these muscles that are under the direct control of the guiding sensation, the common master of all the ocular muscles ? "That involuntary muscular fibers can be increased in size and augmented in power as a result of effort to overcome obstruction is a matter of common accept- ance, so far as the heart and the bladder are concerned. In mitral stenosis it is well known that the walls of the left auricle become hypertrophied and more powerful, so that it may send the blood through the narrowed open- ing into the ventricle; in like manner, when there is obstruction at the aortic opening, the walls of the ven- tricle become hypertrophied and more powerful. "When the prostate gland is enlarged or there is stricture of the urethra, impeding the flow of urine, the muscular fibers in the walls of the bladder are increased in size, and become much more powerful, so as to be able to force the flow. It will be conceded that this muscle development in heart and bladder results from effort to overcome obstruction. "Displacement of images by prisms and blurred im- ages by means of concave lenses are obstructions which, if not too great, will be overcome by muscular action the former, by action of the recti muscles; the latter, by action of the ciliary muscles. Many observers have 652 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. already acknowledged that rhythmic exercise of the recti muscles, by means of prisms, and of the obliques, by means of cylinders properly placed, not only in- creases their power, but at the same time dispels the nervous phenomena associated with and dependent on their former weakness. If the ciliary muscles can be developed by rhythmic exercise, then concave lenses, not too strong 1 , used rhythmically, but not too long- at a time, may be the means of deferring old sight to five or ten, or even more, years beyond the now common age of its onset about the age of forty-five years. "As in developing the recti and obliques, so in the development of the ciliary muscles, the power to over- come obstruction should never be taxed to anything like its fullest capacity. Gentle contraction and relaxation, rhythmic in order, continued from five to ten minutes and repeated once or twice every twenty-four hours, must result in giving tone to the ciliary muscles. The time of life for beginning the exercise, and the strength of concave lenses to use, are matters that must be settled by observation and experience. As a preliminary to the treatment of failing accommodation, all existing focal errors should be corrected, and this correction should be worn behind the exercise lenses at each sitting. A mi- nus .50 D. spherical lens, properly centered, will be the most useful. The patient should be seated from fifteen to MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 653 twenty feet from a lighted candle, lamp, or gas jet, and should look at the same through the concave lenses five seconds, and then raise them for a period of five seconds, and so on to the end of the sitting. It is evident that, with the focal correction on when needed, the image of the flame is sharp, satisfying the guiding sensation without ciliary action. The moment the weak concave lenses are lowered the image is blurred, and at once the ciliary muscles are called into action, to again return to a state of rest the moment they are raised. Thus contrac- tion and relaxation are easily induced. In this way the nutrition of the muscle should be improved and its power enhanced or maintained. The age at which to begin the exercise, as a rule, need not be under forty nor over forty-three years, and it should be continued as long as the proper reading distance is preserved. "The question would naturally arise in the mind of the patient as well as that of the practitioner: ' Can any injury come from the treatment?' The nutrition of the ciliary body is from the blood that circulates in it. Nothing is more reasonable than that gentle, rhythmic, and periodic exercise of the ciliary muscle would im- prove the nutrition of the ciliary body, just as the nutri- tion of other muscles is improved by proper exercise. No harm, then, could come to the muscle as a result of the proposed gymnastic exercise. May we fear unfa- 654 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. vorable change in the lens as a consequence of this exer- cise of the ciliary muscle ? It is generally conceded that the lens gets its nourishment, by the process of osmosis, from the blood circulating in the ciliary body. It must be acknowledged that the better the nutrition of the lens, the more likely will it retain its two very impor- tant properties, transparency and elasticity. It cannot be denied that the better the nutrition of the ciliary body, the healthier will be the crystalline lens. We must conclude that, if the exercise would improve the condition of the ciliary body, it would at the same time have a tendency to improve the nutrition of the lens, whereby the latter would be only the more certain to continue both transparent and elastic. Thus not only may old sight be deferred, but also the development of cataract may be prevented." It is now about eight years since the above was writ- ten. During five of these years, beginning at the age of forty-three years, the author faithfully "took his own prescription," hardly a day passing that he did not exer- cise his ciliary muscles with concave spheres. When the exercise was commenced, there was already beginning presbyopia, interfering somewhat with the finding of foreign bodies in the cornea and with their removal. There was hesitating vision in the more delicate opera- tion on the eye. He commenced the exercise with a mi- MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 655 nus .50 sphere and soon experienced increase of ciliary power, as shown in both comfort and convenience experi- enced in reading 1 and operative work. After a few months a minus 1. sphere was substituted for the weaker lens, and with this the exercise was continued until the age of forty-eight years. Except for the hard work, by artifi- cial light, incident on the writing of this book, he would have continued the exercise longer, possibly until the age of fifty years. Even now, in his forty-ninth year, although he has been wearing a presbyopic lens (a plus 1. D) for the past six months, he can see to read this print easily at fifteen inches through his astigmatic cor- rection only. If the three necessary factors for carry- ing out this exercise time, patience, and perseverance - could be kept compounded, much might be accomplished, especially if the exercise were commenced before the age of forty years. If the exercise treatment is not resorted to, waning ciliary power should be supplemented by convex lenses, as soon as inconveniences arise in near work. There is no reason why plus .50 D spheres might not be first used, and as long as they give comfort. Whenever the ciliary muscles begin to call for more, the presbyopic correction should be increased by steps of .50 D. Finally, when all ciliary power has vanished, artificial aid must be sub- stituted for the lost ciliary power. To give an unneces- 656 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. sarily strong presbyopic correction at the beginning should be avoided, for by so doing the nutrition of both the ciliary body and the lens might be interfered with. Presbyopic lenses should always be correctly centered, and they should be set before the eye so that the visual axes would pass through the lenses at right angles. WEAKNESS OF MULLER'S MUSCLE IN THE YOUNG. Either from the want ol proper development of the muscles or because of faulty innervation, the power of accommodation in young people is often found below par. In association with convergence, the printed page may be seen, but prolonged use of the eyes for reading fatigues the weak muscles and brings on headache or other reflex nervous symptoms. If focal errors exist in these cases, they should be corrected as might be indi- cated by an associated study of the focal errors and in- trinsic condition of the lateral recti muscles. In many cases weakness of Miiller's muscle is the chief source of trouble; and if comfort is ever to be given such a suf- ferer, this condition must be treated. A diagnosis of such a condition may be made easily by means of the concave lenses in the refraction case, but more easily and rapidly by means of two parallel bars containing a series of concave spherical lenses, varying in strength from .50 to 3. D, the difference in strength between any two adjacent lenses being .50 D. These MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 657 bars should be so connected above that the distance between them may be regulated to suit the distance between the pupillary centers of the eyes to be tested. The minus .50 should be below; the minus .3D sphericals, above. Beginning with minus .50 sphericals before the two eyes, the patient all the while looking at the Snellen letters made to be read at twenty feet, each pair of lenses should be passed down in front of the eyes so long as the patient is still able to see xx- If the muscles of ac- commodation are weak, ciliary power, unassociated with convergence, will be less than 3. D; in many cases it is not more than 1. D. The standard of ciliary power, when unassociated with convergence, may be placed at 3. D, for the reason that a ciliary muscle that can overcome a minus 3. D, when not converging, can easily accommodate for the reading distance, in association with convergence. Not much help, from a diagnostic standpoint, can come from a test of the relative power of accommodation. The test apparatus just described may be called a "cili- ometer. " Except for the trouble and inconvenience, the lenses from the trial case could be used for making these tests. Lucien Howe has devised a more complicated appa- ratus for testing ciliary power, which he exhibited before the Section of Ophthalmology of the American Medical Association two or three years ago. As the author re- 658 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. members the device, he thinks it would be easy to use and fully worthy of trust. Every protracted illness must weaken ciliary power, which time and tonics will relieve. Such patients should abstain from near work until the general health has been fully restored TREATMENT. The treatment of weakness of Miil- ler's muscle is by exercise, after the method set forth in the answer to the question: "Can presbyopia be de- ferred?" The result of the exercise will depend largely on the faithfulness with which it is carried out. In cases of congenital weakness of the ciliary muscles, strychnia, electricity, and other tonics can accomplish but little. Rhythmic exercise by means of minus .50 to minus 1 D spherical lenses will cure, usually in a few months. The giving of convex lenses to supplement the weak ciliary power should not be considered, for it would be making the patient old while yet young. BOWMAN'S MUSCLE: ITS NORMAL AND ABNORMAL WORK. In non-astigmatic eyes, when the position of the lens is ideal from the processes of development, the Bowman muscle can have nothing to do, unless it be to aid in steadying the lens in the act of accommodation by Miiller's muscle. Such action, if it occurs, must be of the entire muscle. If a lens, as the result of the processes of develop- MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 659 ment, is not ideally placed in a non-astigmatic eye es- pecially if its equatorial plane is not parallel with the equator of the eye, and probably if its antero-posterior axis does not coincide with the visual axis the task of correction of these errors must fall on Bowman's muscle, and the work must be accomplished through the guiding sensation of the retina, most likely in the earlier months of infancy. This regulation of position must be effected by action of a definite portion of the muscle, and it must be kept up unless the suspensory ligament should un- dergo such change as, in itself, would fix the lens. A failure on the part of the muscle to act would give a permanent lenticular astigmatism. Such work required of Bowman's muscle would be abnormal, but essential. When there is corneal astigmatism, there is necessity for abnormal action of Bowman's muscle in a localized part. The adjustment of the lens must be such as to increase its refractive power in the part at right angles to the plane of the meridian of greatest curvature of the cornea. This can be effected in one of two ways first, by a contraction of one single part of Bowman's muscle, so as to rotate the lens on an axis that lies in the plane of the meridian of the cornea that is most curved; second, by the simultaneous and equal contraction of opposite parts of the muscle so as, by pressure on the lens through the tension of corresponding parts of the suspensory 660 MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. ligaments, to increase the curvature of that part of the lens at right angles to the plane of the most curved cor- neal meridian. While, in either instance, the corneal as- tigmatism would be neutralized, in part or wholly, the author inclines to the view that it is effected by the tilt- ing- of the lens. This work is abnormal, rarely ever perfectly effective, especially in the higher degrees of astigmatism, and is probably one of the worst kinds of eye-strain. As the patient grows older this astigmatic accommodative power is less able to accomplish its work, and a greater amount of the astigmatism becomes mani- fest. There is no known drug that will suspend astig- matic accommodation, else at the first examination the whole error could be found, and should be corrected. There are but two ways to deal with astigmatism: First, correct only the manifest error, increasing the strength of the cylinder from time to time, as the latent error becomes manifest; second, give at once a full cor- rection of the corneal astigmatism, as shown by the oph- thalmometer, and thus force the suspension of the ac- commodative astigmatic power, which would doubtless occur in a short while. The production of artificial astigmatism, unless indi- cated by insufficiency of the superior obliques, should always be avoided. To avoid thus calling into abnor- mal action any part of Bowman's muscle, the correction MUSCLES OF THE CILIARY BODY. 661 of corneal astigmatism should be perfect, both as to strength of cylinder and location of its axis; and when minus spherical lenses are required for the correction of myopia, or plus spherical lenses are given for the cor- rection of hyperopia and presbyopia, they should be so placed that the plane of each lens shall be parallel with the equator of the eye by which it is to be used. APPENDIX. In the original Cyclo-phorometer, depicted on page 262, the graduated semicircle is below. Those now made have the degrees marked in the semicircle above. In the former instrument, when the index stands in the lower- nasal arc, in order to make the two streaks of light paral- lel, the condition is plus cyclophoria; in the latter instru- ment, when the index stands in the upper-temporal arc, the condition is plus cyclophoria. In either instrument, when testing for cyclophoria or cyclotropia, the upper- temporal and lower-nasal arcs refer to the superior obliques, and the upper-nasal and lower-temporal arcs refer to the inferior obliques. The duction arcs for the superior obliques are the upper-nasal and lower-temporal; for the inferior obliques, the upper-temporal and lower-nasal. INDEX. ANTAGONISTS AND SYNERGISTS, tables of 59, 62 ADVANCEMENT OPERATION, to only alter tension 261 to alter tension and change plane 266 marginal 267 for esophoria 312 suggested by Querin 501 of Lagleize 269 ABEA, RETINAL, of binocular fusion, and how measured 175 ANGLE GAMMA 528 how measured 532 Axis OF CARDINAL ROTATIONS, how to find 16 AXES OF OBLIQUE ROTATIONS, are only two 44 ACTION, principal and subordinate, of a muscle 52 ASTIGMATISM, oblique, why more annoying than the vertical 446 parallel 450 phenomena of, shown by experiments 459 metamorphopsia in, through correcting cylinders 462 hyperopic, how to correct 647 myopic, how to correct 649 (662) INDEX. 663 ASTIGMATIC ACCOMMODATION 637, 638 two points about 639 ANISOMETROPIA, a cause of compensating hetcrotropia 492 ANTIPATHY to binocular single vision, explained 520 ACCOMMODATION AND CONVERGENCE 279 Axis OF VISION 6, 8 Axis OF ROTATION 8 ACCOMMODATION, change of lens in 629 Helmholtz' theory of 630 muscle of 631 Miiller's or Bowman's, which? 632 Tscherning's views of lens-changes in 632 presbyopic correction compared with 633 of the hyperope 633 conclusions about 635 normal power of, how determined 657 ABDUCENS 597 ADDUCTION, the normal 153, 158, 197 ABDUCTION, the normal 153, 158, 197 ADVERSION 169, 173, 201 ABVERSION 168, 172, 201 ADJUSTMENT OF CYLINDERS 486 BAXTEB'S CYCLO-PHOROMETER 161 BLACK 268 BREWER'S TORSIOMETER 161 BEARD 2G8 BERGER . G2 1 664 INDEX. BIFFI 625 BINOCULAB SINGLE VISION 104, 105 line (curve) of 99, 102 surface of 99 antipathy to 520 BINOCULAB ROTATIONS, in the four cardinal directions 7, 21 in oblique directions ^ 58 BINOCULAB FUSION FIELD 174 BINOCULAB FIELD OF VISION 125 BINOCULAB FIELD OF VIEW 125 BINOCULAB SPACIAL POLE 124, 135 BINOCULAB SPACIAL MERIDIANS 126, 131 BINOCULAB SPACIAL PARALLELS 130 BRAIN CENTERS FOR BINOCULAR ROTATIONS, how they control 77,78 demonstration of their existence 78-81 individual fusion centers 81 BROWN, MANNING 99 BOWMAN'S MUSCLE 628 nerve supply of 635 function of 636 how it may act 640 its normal and abnormal work 658 its work of adjusting the lens, after birth 659 its work in neutralizing corneal astigmatism 659 BURNETT, SWAN M 185, 402, 638 CARDINAL ROTATIONS 169 CATAPHORIA, treatment of, by prisms 233 < See " Hyperphoria.") INDEX. 665 CATAPHORIA, DOUBLE, causes of 353 treatment of, by prisms 376 by exercise 378 by operations 378 CATATROPIA, DOUBLE, without cyclotropia 581 with plus cyclotropia 581 the operations for 585 with minus cyclotropia 581 CENTERS, the conjugate 69, 70 the fusion 71, 80-91 CHECK LIGAMENTS 503 CHECKED GOODS, why annoying to astigmatics 646 CILIARY MUSCLES, normal and subnormal 281 super-normal 282 CILIARY BODY, muscles of 628 Bowman's 628 Miiller's 629 CILIOMETER 657 CHISOLM, J. J 329 CRITCHETT 502 CONJUGATE CENTERS, disease of sixth, seventh, eighth, and ninth 612 disease of first, second, third, fourth, and fifth 614 COLLINS 624 Coccius . 631 666 INDEX. CONJUGATE INNERVATIONS 69, 70 CORRESPONDING RETINAL POINTS 451 CONVERGENCE, angle of, and formula for calculating 140 angle of, and pupillary distance 141 size of angle of, how to find 144 CONVERGENCE AND ACCOMMODATION 279 CORNEA, decentration of 493 imperfect images caused by 494 how to detect 494 CROSS-EYES 497 CULBERTSON, H 401, 407 CYCLO-PHOROMETER 161 how to use 162 CYCLO-DUCTION 163, 167, 198, 396 CYCLOPHOHIA, history of 383 varieties of 384 causes of 384 tests for, by Maddox prism 194, 389 by single prism 194, 392 by rotary prism 392 by the Stevens clinoscope 195, 393 by the cyclo-phorometer 195, 395 symptoms of 400 treatment of, by exercise cylinders 237, 401 by rest cylinders 402 how to place axes of cylinders given for correction of astigma- tism 405 by operations 248, 414 INDEX. 667 CYCLOPHOBIA, operations for, first suggested 402 uncomplicated, the operations for 414 complicated by double hyperphoria, the operations for 414 complicated by double cataphoria, the operations for 414 complicated by right hyperphoria and left cataphoria, the operations for 414 complicated by sthenic esophoria, the operations for 414 complicated by sthenic exophoria, the operations for 414 complicated by esophoria, right hyperphoria, and left catapho- ria, the operations for 415 complicated by exophoria, right hyperphoria, and left catapho- ria, the operations for 415 CYCLOTROPIA, compensating, history of the study of 419, 438 caused by oblique astigmatism 418 how retinal images are displaced 439 how the displaced images are fused 442, 458 treatment of, by correcting cylinders 466 annoyances following treatment of 466 why annoyances vanish sooner in some cases than in others. . . 467 Lippincott's method of applying correcting cylinders 469 the kind of cases requiring this method 469 the kind of cases not requiring this method 471 the gradual correction of, by displacing the axes of the fully correcting cylinders 471 Steele rule for displacing cylinders 472 Steele rule corrected . 472 668 INDEX. CYCLOTBOPIA, comitant 587 parallel and non-parallel 588 parallel, causes of 588 plus, causes of 589 minus, causes of 589 how detected and measured 590 plus, complicated by double hypertropia, the operations for 591 complicated by hypertropia of one eye and catatropia of the other, the operations for 592 uncomplicated, the operations for 593 CYLINDERS, adjustment of 486 distortion by displacement of 484 DANIEL, PBOF. JOHN 48 DEBCUM AND PABKEB'S EXPERIMENTS 210 DESCHWEINITZ 221 DEADY 231 DECENTBATION OF LENSES 302 DEGREES. of arc and of prism compared 643, 645 DEVIATIONS, primary and secondary 525 DJPLOPIA, Nature's two methods of preventing 66, 498 DISTORTION BY CYLINDERS, arcs of 474, 484 plates illustrating 475, 480, 485 a knowledge of, necessary 486 INDEX. 669 DlEFFENBACH 499 BONDERS 279, 509 DOAK, R. S 363 DUANE 187 DUNN, JOHN 566 DUCTION POWER 197 standard of 176 value of 199 how determined by the Wilson phorometer 153 how determined by the monocular phorometer 157 EXTERNUS. plane of action of 53 correct attachment of 53 high attachment of 53 low attachment of 53 operation on (see " Exophoria " and " Esophoria.") EXERCISE, ceiling-to-floor and wall-to-wall 177 EMMETROPIA, Miiller's muscle in 642 convergence and accommodation in 643 with orthophoria 643 EXOPHORIA 314 pseudo, causes of 318 treatment of, by concave lenses 328 treatment of, by under-correction of hyperopia 329 intrinsic 315 causes of 315 sthenic and asthenic 317 why variable 317 tests for, by exclusion 322 670 INDEX. EXOPHORIA, 314 by red glass 323 by double prism 323 by single prism 323 by Maddox rod 324 by photometer 324 abduction, and abversion in 325 adduction and adversion in 326 complications of 326 symptoms of 327 treatment of, by prisms for constant wearing 330 by exercise prisms 230, 334 by candle exercise 228, 332 by Gould or Deady method 232 how to adjust rest prisms in 331 sthenic, uncomplicated, the operations for 336 complicated by hyperphoria and cataphoria only, the operations for 337 complicated by plus cyclophoria only, the operations for 337 complicated by hyperphoria and cyclophoria, the operations for 338 asthenic, uncomplicated, the operations for 338 complicated by hyperphoria and cataphoria only, the operations for 339 complicated by cyclophoria only, the operations for 339 complicated by hyperphoria and cyclophoria, the operations for 339 ESOPHOBIA, pseudo, cause of 278 how it manifests itself . . 279 INDEX. 671 ESOPHOBIA, treatment of 297 intrinsic or inherent, conditions that cause 273 sthenic, how determined 277 asthenic, how determined 278 tests for, when unreliable 284 by exclusion 286 by red glass 286 by double prism 287 by single prism 287 by Maddox rod 288 by the phorometer 291 the same, far and near 281 variable, greater in the far 280 complications of 293 symptoms of 294 treatment of, by convex lenses 297 by rest prisms, and how to adjust 299 by exercise prisms 233, 304 sthenic, uncomplicated, the operations for 308 complicated by hyperphoria and cataphoria only, the operations for 308 complicated by cyclophoria only, the operations for 309 complicated by plus cyclophoria and hyperphoria of one eye and cataphoria of the other, the operations for 310 asthenic, uncomplicated, the operations for 310 complicated by hyperphoria and cataphoria only, the operations for 311 complicated by plus cyclophoria only, the operations for 311 672 INDEX. ESOTROPIA, comitant 513 time of occurrence, and why 513 causes of 514 esophoria as a cause 515 hyperopia 516 hyperphoria and cataphoria 517 low visual acuity 519 faulty connection of the maculas with the brain 520 varieties of 524 tests for, by phonometer 520 by perimeter 531 by tape 533 by linear method 535 by Hirschberg method 535 symptoms of 536 amblyopia 537 disfigurement 538 secondary deviation 539 headache and other reflexes 536 treatment of 539 by convex lenses 539 why these should be given only 540 by atropine in the good eye 543 by flap before the good eye 543 by the stereoscope 544 by the amblyoscope 545 by bar reading 548 the object of treatment 544 operative treatment of 549 by complete tenotomies, never 549 why Panas' complete tenotomy is safer than any other 550 by advancements 551 the two extremes in operating 551 by partial tenotomies, advancements, and shortenings, the ideal operations 552 INDEX. 673 ESOTROPIA, the two effects that may be accomplished by any operation. . . . 552 comitant, uncomplicated, the operations for 553 complicated by hypertropia and catatropia only, the operations for 555 complicated by plus cyclotropia only, the operations for 555 complicated by plus cyclotropia and hypertropia of one eye and catatropia of the other, the operations for 556 complicated by plus cyclotropia and double hypertropia, the operations for 558 EXOTROPIA, comitant, causes of, myopia 561 exophoria 561 defective third innervation center 561 the obliques may cause 562 traumatism (bad surgery on an internus) 562 is a binocular trouble 563 complications of 563 symptoms of 564 a case of non-comitant, with symptoms 562 comitant, uncomplicated, the operations for 569 complicated by hypertropia, the operations for 570 complicated by plus cyclotropia only, the operations for 571 complicated by plus cyclotropia, hypertropia, and catatropia, the operations for 572 complicated by plus cyclotropia and double hypertropia, the operations for 574 simple, the Fox operation for 575 674 INDEX. EYE, the ideal 27-35 the non-ideal 27-35 FBAMES FOB EXERCISE CYLINDERS 411 FIXED PLANES OF THE HEAD 72 FIXATION, LINE OF 528 Fox's OPERATION FOB EXOTROPIA 575 FUSION, RETINAL AREA OF 175 GBAEFE 184, 500, 520 GAMMA, ANGLE 528 GOULD 231 GUIDING SENSATION 175, 641 GBTTNHAGEN 624 HALE, G. W 215 HELMHOLTZ 10, 15, 16, 25, 96, 630 HELMHOLTZ' FUNDAMENTAL ERRORS 14, 34 HELMHOLTZ AND THE AUTHOR, eight points of disagreement 32-34 HELMHOLTZ' METHOD of finding the fixed axis of a rotation 10-13 HESS 635 HETEROPHOBIA, vertical 180 lateral 181 oblique 181 causes of 181 pseudo and intrinsic 188 tests for 188, 189 symptoms of 203 INDEX. 675 HETEROPHOP.IA, headache 204 vertigo and nausea 206 confubion of thought 206 chorea 207 epilepsy 208 catalepsy 212 hysteria 213 neurasthenia 213 visceral disturbances 214 asthenopia 216 treatment of, by rest prisms 217 by rest cylinders 219 by exercise prisms 220 by operations 239 HETEROTROPIA, compensating 489 caused by anisometropia 489 caused by prisms and decentered lenses 492 comitant, varieties of 497 history of 499 Panas' operation for 503 classes of 510 treatment of (see " Esotropia," etc.) how distinguished from paralytic heterotropia 512 HlRSCHBERG 538 HOROPTER 35-39, 94 HOTZ 419 HOWE 657 HYPERKIXESIS AND HYPOKIXESIS . .187 676 INDEX. HYPEBPHOBIA AND CATAPHOETA, inherent, causes of, malformation of orbits 342 too high attachments of the lateral recti 349 other causes 350, 354 tests for 358 proof tests for 361 duction and version tests 366 complications of 366 symptoms of 367 tilting of head in 363 treatment of, by prism exercise 233, 372 by rest prisms 375 uncomplicated, the operations for 379 complicated by cyclophoria, the operations for 380 HYPEBPHORIA, DOUBLE, causes of 351 tests for 361 proof tests for 193, 361 treatment of, by prisms 376 by straight-forward-to-floor exercise 377 by operations 377 HYPEBTBOPIA AND CATATROPIA 581 with no cyclotropia 582 with cyclotropia 582 the causes of 583 symptoms of 583 treatment of 584 uncomplicated, the operations for 586 INDEX. 677 HYPEKTEOPIA AND CATATROPIA 581 complicated by plus cyclotropia, the operations for 586 complicated by parallel cyclotropia, the operations for 597 HYPEBTBOPIA, DOUBLE, without cyclotropia 581 with minus cyclotropia 581 with plus cyclotropia 581 uncomplicated, the operations for 584 complicated by plus cyclotropia, the operations for 585 complicated by minus cyclotropia, the operations for 585 INTERNUS, correct attachment of 52 high attachment of 52 low attachment of 52 plane of 52 operations on (see " Esophoria " and " Exophoria.") INFERIOR OBLIQUE, plane of 53 INNEBVATIONS, THE CONJUGATE 69 what they do 147 IBIS, muscles of 624 nerves of 626 function of 626 weak sphincter of, how to treat 627 ISOGONAL CIRCLES, PRIMARY 104 ISOGONAL CIRCLES, SECONDARY 105 ISOGONAL CIRCLE 94, 97, 102 678 INDEX. ISOGONAL SURFACE 99, 102 IK SUFFICIENCY OF THE OBLIQUES 461 JACKSON, EDWABD 302 JOHNSON, W. B 538 JULER 625 KNAPP 26, 501 LAW, of monocular rotations 57 of binocular rotations 109 of corresponding retinal points 64 of direction 65, 94 of direction subordinate to the law of corresponding retinal points 456 LAGLIEZE'S OPERATION 269 LAGOPHTHALMOS 618 causes of 620 symptoms of 618 treatment of 622 LANDOLT 502, 554 LAWRENCE 535 LE CONTE 35, 38, 94, 95 LENS, changes of, in accommodation 632 effect of tilting of 637 elasticity of, how suspended 642 how to set for presbyopia 656 LISTING'S PLANE 75 law 43 LINES OF DIRECTION 35 where they cross 31 where they do not cross 27-30 INDEX. 679 LIPPINCOTT 469 LOWBY 430 MACULA, THE ROTATING POINT 6 MADDOX 46, 101, 102, 187, 302 MADDOX ROD 159 its legitimate use 160 single in testing for oblique astigmatism, but multiple when testing for cyclophoria 465 MAY 303 MAUNOIE 624 MAYO 625 MACULA, " new-formed," so-called 496 MEBIDIANS, retinal 17 spacial, monocular 17 spacial, binocular 126, 131 METRE-ANGLE OF NAGEL 141 variable with pupillary distances 142 value of, in degrees 143 METAMOEPHOPSIA THROUGH CORRECTING CYLINDERS, cause of 462 why it disappears more quickly in some cases than in others. . . 462 MICHEL, CHARLES E 226 MONOSCOPTEB 94 MOTOR NERVES, the third 595 the fourth 597 the sixth 597 MUSCLES ARRANGED IN NINE PAIRS . 68 680 INDEX. MUSCLE INDICATOR 113 MUSCLE PLANE, of the internus 52 of the externus 53 MULLEB'S MUSCLE 629 nerve supply of 635 function of 641 loss of power in 643 table showing 644 in emmetropia 642 in hyperopia 645 in hyperopic astigmatism 645 in myopia 647 in myopic astigmatism 648 weakness of, in the old 650 weakness of, in the young 652 diagnosis of 656 treatment of 658 NAGEL 141 NETTLESHIP ....'.... 407 NODAL POINT 27-35 NOMENCLATURE OF THE MUSCLES 185 NOTES 222 OPHTHALMOLPLEGTA EXTERNA. symptoms of 603 causes of 603 OPERATIONS ON THE MUSCLES 240 to lessen tension 241 to lessen tension and change plane 242 to increase tension 243 to increase tension and change plane 244 objects of all operations 310 (See the various " phorias " and " tropias.") INDEX. 681 ORBITAL MALFORMATIONS 182 OBLIQUE MUSCLES, history of the study of 184 simple function of 450 complicated function of 452 OBLIQUES, INFERIOR, once thought to be advertors 504 possibly cut by Taylor 507 OPTIC Axis 10, 32, 528 ORIENTATION 59, 74 ORTHOPHORIA 146 lateral, tests for 149, 152, 157 vertical, tests for 150, 152, 3 57 oblique, tests for 154, 161, 163, 250 sthenic 176 asthenic 177 treatment of 177 PLANES OF REFERENCE 73 median fixed plane 73 horizontal fixed plane 73, 182 PLANE OF ROTATION, how to construct 51 PLANE OF ROTATION OF INDIVIDUAL MUSCLES 51-57 PAN AS' OPERATION 503 PARALYSIS, of the third nerve 600 symptoms of 600 causes of 598 treatment of 616 of the fourth nerve 602 causes of 598 symptoms of , 602 682 INDEX. PARALYSIS, treatment of 616 of the sixth nerve 602 causes of 598 symptoms of 602 treatment of 616 of the seventh nerve 618 PARALYSIS AND PARESIS, diagnosis of 603 rules for finding the affected muscle 604, 605 of a right-vertor 606 of a left-vertor 607 of a sub-vertor 608, 610 of a supervertor 609, 610 pose of the head in 611 PARALYSIS, of motion, and not of muscle 611 symptoms of 615 PERIMETER, for making version tests 168 PERRY, C. H 398, 428 PETIT 625 POLES 27-33 POLE, POSTERIOR 10, 17, 19, 20, 24, 25, 94 POLE, ANTERIOR 10, 19, 20, 25, 27-35 POLE, SPACIAL, MONOCULAR 17 POLE, BINOCULAR 124, 135 PRESBYOPIA, causes of 644, 650 can it be deferred? 650 treatment by exercise 652 treatment by correcting lenses 655 INDEX. 683 PStUDO-ESOPHOBIA, how manifested 646 when desirable 647 when harmful 647 PSEUDO-EXOPHOBIA, how manifested 648 PBISMS, a cause of compensating heterotropia 492 PBICE'S CYCLO-PHOBOMETEB, the first made 160 PBICE, GEOBGE H 253 PHOBOMETEB, the Stevens 147 its capabilities 148 why objectionable 148 method of using 149 the Wilson, its capabilities 152 why objectionable 148 method of using 152 the correct principle of construction of the 155 the Monocular 156 QUEBIN 511 RANNEY 209 RETINAL POINTS, law of corresponding 64 RISLEY 182 ROTATIONS, cardinal 7, 2i oblique 58 monocular 39-63 binocular . 63-110 684 INDEX. ROTATIONS, law of 57, 59 axis of i, 2, 5, 10, 44, 45, 51 plane of l, 2, 5, 13, 19, 51 by a single muscle 51 in the four cardinal directions 41, 77 in any oblique direction 41, 78, 79 ROTATING LINE 2, 4, 5, 6, 17 ROTATING POINT 4, 5, 6, 20 ROTATING POINTS, Two 7 RHYTHMIC EXERCISE OF THE MUSCLES 226 STRABISMUS 497 STEVENS' NOMENCLATURE 146 phororaeter 147 clinoscope 160 description of 164 tropometer 169 STEVENS 182, 184, 209, 225 STEELE, N. C 472 SMITH, PRIESTLEY 533, 635 SHORTENING OPERATIONS, advantages of 257 to only increase tension 258 to alter tension and change plane 262 marginal 262 STROMEYEH 499 SUB-DUCTION, THE NORMAL 153, 158, 198 SUPERDUCTION, THE NORMAL 153, 158, 191 SUPERVERSION 169, 171, 202 SUB-VERSION 169, 172, 202 SQUINT ......... 497 SUPPRESSION, MENTAL 66 INDEX 685 SYNERGISTS AND ANTAGONISTS, tables of 59, 62 old table 59, 60 new table 62, 63 TAYLOR, THE SQUINT QUACK 505 TESTS FOR HETEROPHORIA 189 TENOTOMY, partial, central, to lower tension 251 marginal, to lower tension and change plane 256 TENDONOMETER 254 TSCHERNING 632 TORSION, measured 47 table of 48, 50 how prevented 46 TROPOMETER, description of 169 THORINGTON 303 VERSION POWER, standard of 174 VERSION TEST, value of 199 by tropometer 200 by perimeter 200 VISUAL Axis 8, 528 VISUAL LINES 14, 31, 35 WILSON, HAROLD 420 WORTH 545 YOUNG 629, 630 ZONULA, how relaxed . 631 Date Due CAT. NO. 23 233 PRINTED IN U.S.A. A 000510439 WWU o S263 o 1911 Savage, Giles C Ophthalmic myology wwUoo S263 o 1911 Javage, Giles C Ophthalmic myology . . . MEDICAL SCIENCES LIBRARY