3 &#C THE GENESIS AND DISSOLUTION THETACULTY Or SPEECH A CLINICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL STUDY OF APHASIA JOSEPH COLLINS, M.D. Professor of Diseases of the Mind and Nervous System in the New York Post-Graduate Medical School; Neurologist to the New York City Hospital, to the St. John's Guild Free Hospital for Children; and to the Post-Graduate Hospital; Attending Physician to the St. Mark's Hospital Bwaroefc tbe Slvarencia prise of tbc College of pb\>sfcians of pbilafcelpbia, 1897 Iftevv THE MACMILLAN COMPANY LONDON: MACMILLAN \- CO.. LTD. 1S9S All right rexerifd COPYRIGHT, 1898, BY JOSEPH COLLINS, M.D. TO MY TEACHER CHARLES L. DANA, M.D. AND TO MY FRTKM) LIGHTNER WITHER, Ph.D. TO WHOM I OWE MY INTELLECTUAL AWAKEMENT THIS LABOR OF A YEAR'S LEISURE IS AFFECTIONATELY DEDICATED PREFATORY NOTE. THE MS. of this monograph left the writer's hand in April, 1897. Since that time several important contri- butions have been made to our knowledge of aphasia. The author regrets that time and opportunity have not been granted him to give them the consideration and discussion they merit. Only verbal changes have been made in the text since its completion, but here and there a footnote has been added to call attention to some of the more im- portant facts and striking claims set forth by recent writers. 47 WEST THIRTY-EIGHTH STREET, New York, Christmas, 1897. c;- CONTENTS. CHAPTER I. PAGE DISORDERS OF INTELLECTUAL EXPRESSION, KNOWN AS APHASIA, . i CHAPTER II. HISTORY, ........... 17 CHAPTER III. AN ANALYSIS OF THE GENESIS AND FUNCTION OF SPEECH, . . 40 i. General Expressive Reactions ; Mimic Reactions. 2. Articu- late Speech. 3. Writing, 4. The Genesis of Percepts. 5. Re- marks on Visual Sensation ; Acoustic Sensation, and the Mecha- nism of Articulation. CHAPTER IV. CONCEPTION OF APHASIA, ........ 86 i. Remarks on the Anatomy of the Brain, z. _&Qne.f>f T.gflyuage. Site of Revival of Words in Silent Thought. 3. Evidence in Favor of and against the Existence of a Special Graphic Motor Centre. CHAPTER V. MOTOR APHASIA, .......... 153 i Motor Aphasia; General Considerations. 2. Cortical Motor Aphasia ; Articulatory Kinaesthetic Aphasia. 3, Subcortical Motor Aphasia. CHAPTER VI. SENSORY APHASIA, . . . . . . . . . . 216 i. General Considerations 2. Sensory Aphasia: Word Deafness, or Auditor}- Aphasia, and "\Vord Blindness, or Visual Aphasia. 3. Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. via Contents. CHAPTER VI.- Continued. PAGE SUBCORTICAL SENSORY APHASIA, ....... 2g5 CHAPTER VII. TOTAL APHASIA, 313 CHAPTER VIII. DIAGNOSIS OK APHASIA, 324 CHAPTER IX. ETIOLOGY, ........... 343 CHAPTER X. MORBID ANATOMY OF APHASIA, ....... 370 CHAPTER XI. REMARKS ON THE TREATMENT OF APHASIA, ..... 396 CHAPTER XII. REMARKS ON THE MEDICO-LEGAL ASPECTS OF APHASIA, . . 407 APPENDIX I. CONDUCTION APHASIA, .. \ ...... 414 APPENDIX II A CASE OF ARTICULATORY-KIN.KSTHKTIC APHASIA, . . ' . 422 Jlf^M A I THE FACULTY OF SPEECH. CHAPTER I. DISORDERS OF INTELLECTUAL EXPRESSION, KNOWN AS APHASIA. Introduction. THE possession of the faculty of speech distinguishes the human being from even those animals which stand next to him in biological relationship. Man is in con- sequence inclined to reflect upon its possession, and to speculate concerning its nature and origin. In days when philosophic speculation addressed itself principally to a-priori assumptions of the nature and interrelations of un- analyzed mental phenomena, the function of speech, and that, too, in its fully developed form, was regarded as an essential attribute or inherent faculty of the human mind, manifesting itself with the perfected completeness of a Minerva springing from the brows of Jove. From the vagueness, often the vagaries, of such speculation, there was little relief until attention was directed to the de- velopment of speech in the infant and to its dissolution from disease. Though of recent development, labor in these two fields of observation has extended our knowl- edge of the genesis of speech and thereby given a basis for- the interpretation of its significance. The genetic method 2 The Faculty of Speech has rendered relatively a small service compared with the pathological method. In this judgment I am not unmind- ful of the value of many painstaking observations, such as those of Preyer and of Baldwin, that have aided in de- termining the antecedent factors of voluntary articulation and other forms of mental expression ; but it has been chiefly from the study of those disorders of speech in- cluded under the term aphasia, made by many students of human language, physicians, physiologists, and psycholo- gists, that real knowledge of the faculty of speech has been acquired. In introducing the term aphasia, Trousseau first ap- plied it to the condition in which there was inability to express thought in words ; in this narrow sense it was used for some time. When the part played by language in the communication of ideas began to be studied, it was not long before it became apparent that this application of the term was wholly inadequate; an:l that a word was needed not only to connote restrictedly an inability to create phonetic symbols, but to include as well the inability to express ideas or to manifest states of consciousness by signs of all sorts, of which speech occupied only the first rank in importance. Disturbance of the power to express a condition of mind manifests itself in speech, writing, pantomime, gesture, drawing, instrumentation, symbols, coloYs, attitudes, etc. To this end the term asymbolia was suggested by Finkenburg. It was improved upon by Kussmaul, who substituted the word asemia, which ad- mitted easy paronymization and the adjective asemic, and which signified literally the meaning that was desired. Nevertheless, the term aphasia, sanctioned by time and Disorders of Intellectual Expression. 3 consecrated by usage, has not been supplaced by these technically better constructed terms. On the contrary, aphasia has been given a wider significance, a significance that attaches to it to-day, namely, the total inability or partial disability of an individual to make outward expres- sion of thoughts, feelings, or other states of conscious- ness, whether such disability result from interference with the formation of the mental content or in the emis- sion of it. Aphasia has gone through many evolutionary stages. After the first wave of its universal recognition had sub- sided, it came to be looked upon as a symptom with an established seat and an accompaniment of definite lesions. As the literature of the subject grew and the cases with anomalous symptom complexes became more numerous, it was seen that they did not fit in with the simple con- ceptions of those who stood sponsors for its original recognition. This, coupled with the apparent ambition of almost every one who wrote on the subject to adopt a new nomenclature, soon robbed the symptom of its attributed pristine simplicity, and showed it to be in reality one of the most complex subjects with which the physician and psychologist had to deal. Former and current nomenclature fostered the obscurity in which the subject of aphasia has been and is still en- shrouded. After aphasia had been recognized as a symp- torn indicative of localized lesion in the brain, the use of the words "ataxic" and "amnesic" aphasia, the one to in- dicate the aphasia of impaired articulation, the other to indicate loss of memory of the word, stood obstinately in the way of ready comprehension of the speech disturb- 4 The Faculty of Speech. ances attending given cases. Such classification, how- ever, did not offer material hindrance to those who desired to study the subject of aphasia seriously and scientifically, nor yet did the diagrammatic portrayal which held sway for a long time. As time went on, students of the anatomy and physiology of the brain began to unravel the intricacies of its archi- tecture and the mysteries of its functions ; and it became apparent that diagrammatic methods, which necessitated allocating certain functions to definitely and sharply local- ized areas and the connection of these areas, the one with the other, by individual fibres, as well as the representa- tion of such connection in the incoming and outgoing pathways, were inimical to the findings of science. It will be shown in the chapter on the psycho-physiological conception of speech, that the entire speech area, i.e., all that part of the cortex whose functional integrity must be preserved for the production of speech, whether it be con- cerned in the reception of impressions or in the emission of them, is practically a sensory area. Study of the an- atomy of the brain, particularly by the aid of the embryo- logical method, which Flechsig has made so uniquely his own, has thus been of service in interpreting the symp- tomatology of aphasia. A glance at the literature register appended to this monograph, 1 and a perusal of the chapter devoted to the history of the subject, will show that delay in establishing a satisfactory explanation of aphasia cannot be attributed 1 There is appended to the original manuscript of this essay, now in the archives of the College of Physicians of Philadelphia, a bibliography of aphasia which aims to indicate the literature of the subject up to January 1st, 1897. Disorders of Intellectual Expression. 5 to dearth of reports of cases. In fact, the bare report of a simple case of aphasia is to-day even considered by many writers to be a dignified procedure ; and, although it has been a sort of covenant among medical writers that, as soon as the symptom complex of a disease became uni- versally recognized, nothing was to be gained by putting on record bare reports of cases that offered not the slight- est difficulty in their interpretation and that contributed nothing to the elucidation of the problem to be solved, recalcitrant covenanters are numerous where .aphasia is concerned. To show that this is not an exaggeration, I may refer to an instance of recent date in which the presi- dent of a most learned and dignified European medical society cited a simple case of subcortical motor aphasia, which he referred to as "quite remarkable," as a contribu- tion to " A Discussion on Aphasia" which had been opened by a savant who is known in every medical community and who, in closing the discussion, disposed of the simple case in a few simple words. I would not be understood to say that the study of the simplest cases of aphasia is not of value. It is of para- mount importance, and especially study of the morbid anatomy. It is in such cases that the relation between cause and effect is most distinctly traceable. Bare clinical reports have made the literature vast and they have made it a literature that is not of very great value. Compara- tively few cases have been observed in which careful, ac- curate autopsies were made and intelligently reported. This, coupled with the varying nomenclature that has been used by different writers and the different value put upon words, has served to make the subject of aphasia appear 6 Tke Faculty of Speech. much more intricate than it really is, and the possession of a knowledge of it more difficult than the phenomena warrant. That comparatively few cases carefully observed, prop- erly reported, and accompanied by details of post-mortem findings, go far to unravel many of the apparent mys- teries of aphasia has been shown by the contributions of Wernicke, Dejerine, Vialet, Redlich, Wyllie, Elder, and of many others. Aphasia is a term used to indicate any_ disturbance or perversion of intellectual expression. The significance of the term has expanded from the time when its application was used to designate a defect in the verbal expression of an idea, until now it includes all defects or disorders of intellectual expression, whether such disorders be the re- sult of disarrangement or destruction of the receptive or of the emissive components of speech mechanism, or of anything which may be employed as the analogue of speech. Thus /a person who, despite the integrity of the peripheral speech mechanism, is unable to utter his own name, or to give expression to thoughts which arise in the usual way, has aphasia. If he is incapable of making known his thoughts by the employment of some equivalent of spoken words, such as writing of any sort or by expres- sive mimicry or pantomime, he likewise has aphasia. Moreover, a person has aphasia who, with the extra-cere- bral apparatus intact, is unable to understand the lan- guage in whose use he has been trained ; does not even recognize, although he hears, the sounds of the most familiar nature and words to which he has for a lifetime been accustomed, such as his own name ; although he may Disorders of Intellectual Expression. 7 be able to read, is unable to write voluntarily or from dic- tation, or to express his thoughts by words, by symbols, or by pantomime. Yet even these shortcomings do not comprise all that is meant by aphasia. If a person with normal ocular apparatus looks upon a printed or written page, and the symbols there with which he has previously been entirely familiar convey no meaning to him in the form of approxi- mate thought or idea, such person has aphasia, even though he may understand all that is communicated orally to him, and though he may himself be able to express his thoughts (incompletely and defectively however) by spoken and written words. Thus it will be seen that aphasia, in the broad usage of the term, may be the result of conditions by which the patient is unable to part with the expressive equivalent of an idea which has been properly formed. This failure is not confined to words, but includes all modes of expres- sion. Or it may be caused by any conditions that inter- fere with the reception of impulses or stimuli that enter into the genesis of ideas used in the construction of in- ternal or external language. As movement in some form is requisite for the manifestation of any and all expres- sions, defect of this is the condition to which the term motor aphasia is applied, a condition which is equally well expressed by the term aphasia of emission. In the second form of aphasia, the sufferer is unable to adapt receptive communications and make them fit the idea represented by the verbal symbol, auditory or visual ; that is, he has lost the faculty of adapting the complement of the word to his own idea. It matters not whether these words be 8 The Faculty of Speech. spoken or written, or communicated by some equivalent, such as music and pantomime. In a general way, this is the aphasia of reception, or sensory aphasia. Motor aphasia, or aphasia of emission, which was de- scribed by Broca as aphemia, and by many writers after him as ataxic aphasia, may be divided into as many forms as there are habitual avenues of exteriorizing thoughts. Ideas are usually exteriorized by spoken words, by written words, by symbols, and by pantomime. Thus, we have aphasia of articulation, or logaphasia; and aphasia of writing, agraphia or logagraphia; asymbolia, and asemia. Aphasia of reception, or sensory aphasia, is also made up of a number of constituents, the two great divisions being auditory aphasia or word deafness, and visual aphasia or word blindness. Each form of aphasia admits in turn of further subdivision. The understanding of the subject of aphasia depends upon a comprehension of the develop- ment of the powers of language and thought. It has seemed to me necessary, therefore, to say something of the conditions under which these are developed, before pro- ceeding to the clinical side of aphasia, although it would be manifestly out of place in a monograph of this kind to trace the evolution of speech in detail from the time of the child's birth to the period when the speech faculties have reached their highest development. Before proceed- ing to this psychological consideration, however, it seems to me desirable to present as succinctly as possible a con- sensus of the opinions furnished by physiology, psychology, pathology, and anatomy as to the location, interconnection, and relative importance of the different speech centres ; or, to be more explicit, the areas by virtue of whose integrity Disorders of Intellectual Expression. 9 a person is able to adapt sensations coming through the special senses to the idea represented by such sensations, or to adapt and produce words to ideas which are con- ceived by him. After a brief consideration of the history of aphasia I shall take up the subject in the manner in- dicated. I am aware that this mode of attack may not commend itself, at first sight, to the physician who seeks information of the clinical phenomena of aphasia alone, but I hope that it will to him who would understand the genesis of speech, as well as the phenomena of its disso- lution. Classification of Aphasia. The externalization of thought requires : I. () The production of movements through the muscu- lature of the thorax, the larynx, the tongue, and the lips, and the co-ordination of the respiratory, vocal-cord, lingual, and labial movements, (b} Integrity of the musculature in- volved in writing, generally the muscles of the right hand, but sometimes the muscles of the left hand, and very rarely those of any mobile part of the body, such as the foot, and hence positing specialized motor areas in the opposite cerebral hemisphere, (c) Integrity of the musculature of the face, arms, and hands, and to a lesser extent of other muscles of the body which produce pantomimic ex- pression. II. The sense organ of hearing, which is capable of being stimulated differentially by sounds that differ in timbre, pitch, intensity, and duration, and possessing a musculature which is reflexly stimulated so as to bring about a certain degree of accommodation or adaptation of io Tke Faculty of Speech. a sense organ to sounds that differ in the above fashion, and a conduction tract leading from the labyrinth to the oblongata, thence to the internal geniculate body, and to the cortex in the anterior region of the first temporal lobe. III. A sense organ of vision capable of differential re- sponse to form, to magnitude, and to distance, with a musculature possessing a high degree of accommodative adaptability to such different stimuli, and a conduction tract which passes directly to the external geniculate body, thence to the anterior quadrigeminal body ; a larger bundle, however, passing directly from the external geniculate body into the pulvinar of the thalamus, and thence to adjacent parts, ultimately reaching the cor- tex of the occipital lobe in the vicinity of the calcarine fissure. IV. Sense organs or peripheral sensory nerves in muscles, in joints, and in adjacent parts which are cap- able of differential stimulation by different bodily positions and by differently executed muscular move- ments, and afferent tracts leading from these peripheral parts to the cortex, certainly to the cortex of the Rolan- dic region, the so-called somaesthetic area, and possibly to other locations. V. Apperception, a term that is used to indicate the combination of central excitations with any incoming sen- sory stimulus before that stimulus arouses such excitation in the cerebral cortex as to bring into consciousness a completed perception. I deem it expedient here to say a few words concerning the use of the term apperception, which, although thor- Disorders of Intellectual Expression. i i oughly familiar to psychologists, is by no means so to physicians. Apperception is a factor that enters into all such processes as understanding, interpreting, identifying. It is not an element of consciousness, but a process that must intervene between the presentation of an object to consciousness and the disposition of such presentation. It cannot be maintained that there are locally separable centres of apperception and of sensation. Apperception involves past experiences acting on the cortex, leaving what are called memory traces. Every perception and every enunciation of speech involves the co-ordinate act of incoming sensory impulses and of these cortical mem- ory traces. The adult consciousness never has a simple sensation which could be supposed to be the resultant of the activity of the ganglion cells of a distinctive sensory centre ; but incoming impulses may evoke few or many of these memory traces, in some cases involving activities which represent memory traces that are the resultant of the actions of different sense organs. A study of aphasia teaches us that this central associating or co-ordinating power may become lost, and the patient's apperceptive powers be restricted, in some cases even to a state ap- proximating simple sensation. That an auditory impulse, passing up the acoustic nerve through the oblongata, through the temporal lobe, and thence radiating posteriorly, inferiorly, and dorsally, awakes in temporal succession these associated memory traces, cannot be doubted. Consequently at one point we may assume that we have a simple sensation. That point is certainly under normal conditions not higher than the initial auditory receptive cells in the middle portion of i 2 The Faculty of Speech. the superior temporal convolutions. Whether a simple sensation or any consciousness at all is produced by an activity lower down cannot be determined. ,A classification of aphasia is of much service in. con- tributing to a ready understanding of the symptom aphasia. I shall endeavor to give a simple, natural classification that shall be in harmony with the interpretation of aphasia that is expounded in the chapter devoted to " Conception of Aphasia." Personally, I am convinced that the time has come to make radical departure from the usual classification of aphasia. We should cease the use of such misleading terminology as ataxic aphasia, amnesic aphasia, etc., and discourage the attitude adopted by British writers, who use the term aphasia only for the condition that most writers call motor aphasia, and the term amnesia for sen- sory aphasia. To illustrate how confusing and mislead- ing the nomenclature of the Britishers may be, I cite an instance of very recent occurrence. A Colonial physician read an excellent paper on " Motor and Sensory Aphasia" before the Royal Medical and Chirurgical Society. On rising to discuss it, a member of the society said that he thought the term aphasia was hardly justified ! He would prefer to call the condition amnesia. It must be granted, I think, that a nomenclature of this kind has not sufficient in its favor to warrant us in retaining it. I believe that aphasia should be classified as follows : i. TRUE APHASIA. Aphasia of apperception. Due to lesion of any constituent of the speech region, the zone of language. It might be subdivided into (a] visual aphasia, due to lesion of the visual areas and centre ; (&) auditory Disorders of Intellectual Expression. 15 Associative or Transcortical Apliasia. I shall content myself with mere mention of this sub- division, as its symptomatology is in reality a part of sen- sory aphasia. Variation in the clinical picture is in ac- cordance with the location of the lesion between speech areas in the zone of language. The lesion may be : 1. In the habitual pathway traversed by impulses going from the auditory to the visual area (the patient can hear a name but cannot write it from hearing; cannot write it from dictation; he has paragraphia). 2. It may be in the habitual pathway of impulses going from the visual area to the auditory area (the patient can see an object, but he cannot call up its name, because this requires the mediation of the auditory area). 3. A lesion that interrupts the habitual pathway that impulses take when going from the auditory to the seat of phonetic memories in Broca's convolution (the patient can hear, can interpret from hearing, but cannot talk cor- rectly : paraphasia). 4. The lesion may interrupt the pathway taken by im- pulses going from the visual area to the auditory area (the patient is dyslexic, paragraphic, and slightly paraphasic). Thus it will be seen that the two great divisions of aphasia are sensory aphasia and motor aphasia. As I have said previously, many British writers, such as Bastian, use the term amnesia synonymously with sen- sory aphasia, and a greater number of writers use the term aphemia, originally employed by Broca, as the specific term for the motor form of aphasia. Both these forms of aphasia are so complex that it is nee- 1 6 The Faculty of Speech. essary to have recourse to a further subdivision ; the most satisfactory basis for which will have reference to the seat of the lesion that produces the speech disturbance. Thus we have cortical sensory aphasia when the lesion is of the speech centre itself, and subcortical sensory aphasia when the perversion of the function of speech is due to a lesion of the sensory tracts leading to it. The same subdivision is made of motor aphasia, i.e., cortical motor aphasia, and subcortical motor aphasia. The designations cortical sensory and cortical motor aphasia are often abbreviated to simply sensory aphasia and motor aphasia and the sub- cortical forms to pure sensory aphasia and pure motor aphasia (Dejerine, Mirallie, et the olfactory trigone and the adjacent part of the convolution of the corpus callosum, the anterior perforated space, the uncinate gyrus, which is in contact with the island of Reil, and the adjacent parts of the hippocampus. 3. The visual sphere. This is situated on the internal surface of each cerebral hemisphere, in the immediate neighborhood of the calcarine fissure, and fibres run from this tract into those adjacent areas of the occipital cortex which show the macroscopic stripe of Vicq d'Azyr. The centripetal fibres of the visual sphere come from the optic thalamus, the external geniculate body, and the anterior quadrigeminal body. Among these centripetal fibres, those that come from the external geniculate body ter- minate exclusively in the borders of the calcarine fis- sure, while those coming from the optic thalamus and the quadrigeminal tubercles terminate around this fissure. This distinction, according to Flechsig, is important, be- cause the external geniculate bodies receive exclusively the fibres coming from the macula lutea. 4. The auditory sphere. The fibres communicating auditory impulses to the cerebral cortex form the lateral fillet in the pons and are connected largely with the me- dian geniculate body. They terminate in the transverse temporal gyri, especially in the anterior one. This sphere io6 The Faculty of Speech. is united by centripetal fibres to the inferior quadrigemi- nal body and to the internal geniculate body, and by centrifugal fibres (the temporal cortico-protuberantial fas- cicle) with the nucleus of the pons. Flechsig con- siders these descending fibres as a motor pathway which has for its function the transmission of impulses ar- riving in the auditory sphere from the muscles that move the ear. In addition to these projection fibres uniting the sen- sory spheres with the inferior gray masses of the brain axis, there exists in each centre of projection a number of fascicles, composed of centripetal and centrifugal fibres, uniting the centre with the corresponding part of the thalamus. These centres of projection are in connection with all the peripheral organs by a double set of nerve fibres, one the ascending or centripetal fibres, which are sensory, the other a descending centrifugal set of fibres, which are mo- tor. These two sets of fibres end in the same cortical region. According to this there is no exclusively motor cortex, no exclusively sensory. The sensory spheres are thus in reality the sensori-motor regions of the cerebral cortex. All the impressions received by the terminals of the peripheral nerves and their sensory fibres are con- ducted to the tactile sphere of the cerebral cortex by a series of centripetal neurons. These impressions are transmitted to the cells of origin of the descending or motor fibres of this same region, which then descend by a series of centrifugal neurons as far as the peripheral muscles. It is the same for the olfactory impressions, the visual, and the acoustic. They are transmitted by the Conception of Aphasia. 107 centripetal pathways to the corresponding sensory spheres. The centres of projection, considered by themselves, are completely separated from the centres of association which surround them, and constitute thus a sort of nervous centre for reflexes of cortical origin. When these centres have become ripe, that is, when the fibres going to them and coming from them are medullated, only about one-third of the whole area of the cortex has been disposed of. Two-thirds of the cortex appears to have nothing to do with the periphery, but is reserved for other and apparently higher work. These areas are the so-called association centres of Flechsig. Before considering them it is well to draw attention to two statements of Flechsig regarding the projection areas. The somaesthetic area is much more extensive than are all the other areas combined. This disproportion should not be a matter of surprise. In it are represented all the forms of tactile excitation that acquaint us with the outer world, and from it start all impulses by which are externalized thought, feelings, desires. The olfactory sphere is very little developed, because the sense of smell is so rudi- mentary. The auditory sphere and the visual sphere re- produce in the brain, the one the sensitive surface of the organ of Corti of the internal ear, and the other the retina. These last three spheres inform us of the external world and are dependent for their activity on sensation coming from without. The second point made by Flechsig is that the sensory spheres are localized around the primary fissures, the visual sphere around the calcarine fissure, the tactile sphere around the central fissure, the olfactory io8 The Faculty of Speech. sphere and the auditory sphere around the fissure of Sylvius. The zone of the association centres is formed of three distinct centres : first, the posterior large association centre, which comprises a part of the lingual convolution, the fusiform convolution, all of the parietal convolutions, the inferior temporal convolution, and the anterior part of the external surface of the occipital lobe ; second, the median association centre, which corresponds to the island of Reil ; third, the anterior association centre, which is constituted by a portion of the superior frontal convolution and a large part of the median and third frontal convolutions. Flechsig believes that these asso- ciation centres represent arrangements which unite the activities of the central internal sense organs and build them up to higher limits. Sensory impressions of differ- ent qualities, visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory, and gus- tatory, are united, or, at any rate, the anatomical mecha- nism is afforded for their union. These association centres are completely independent of the inferior gray masses, and are the portions of the cerebral cortex which above all others are concerned in the higher intellectual mani- festations, judgment, memory, etc. The middle association centre is represented by the cortex of the island of Reil. This is the centre which unites all the convolutional areas bordering the fossa of Sylvius, the integrity of which is indispensable for the preservation of language. It belongs in part to the som- jesthetic area (especially to the region for the speech organs), partly to the auditory area, and partly to the ol- factory area. The large posterior centre of association is Conception of Aphasia. 109 the part of the cortex which puts us en rapport with the external world. It is that which unites the superior cen- tres for all the sensations, tactile, visual, and acoustic, that come to us from without. Flechsig points out that the association areas, the con- volutions which have no direct communication with the crus, central ganglia, or the corpus callosum, are those which are latest in the order of development, and on this ground alone might be supposed to be concerned in more strictly mental faculties, which are latest in their mani- festations. These areas constitute a striking difference between the human cerebrum and that of lower animals. The centres of projection exist in the lower animals to a very much more highly developed extent than in man. But in man, on the other hand, the centres of associa- tion are immeasurably greater, while as we descend the scale of animal life the centres of association become fewer and finally disappear. It has been noted that the centres of association are often interpolated between the centres of projection, so as to separate these last centres completely one from another. It would seem to be in accord with the general plan of construction of the nervous system and with what we know of mental operations, that these convolutions which are withdrawn, so to speak, from direct relation with the outer world should be the seat of the more purely intellectual operations. From a consideration of the morbid anatomy of some cerebral diseases, especially general paresis, Flechsig has come to the conclusion that the anterior association centre is the part that conditions consciousness of personality 1 10 The Faculty of Speech. (Personlichkeitsbewusstsein). Lesion of this area causes, in a word, loss of the faculty of abstraction. This extensive reference to the view of- Flechsig has seemed to me fitting not only because it is a most im- portant advance in the interpretation of the physiology of the brain, but also because it is of direct and signal service in the conception of aphasia as here outlined, although it is not in entire harmony with it. More- over, it has a direct bearing on many problems in the morbid physiology of the brain that confront the physi- cian to-day. In a way, Flechsig's contentions are not at all new. Views very similar- to them were expounded by Broadbent nearly twenty-five years ago, when he pointed out that the centres which Flechsig calls " association centres" were parts of the cortex that were neither in direct relation with peduncular fibres nor with those of the corpus callosum. These association areas are also in reality what Bastian has described as " annexes of the perceptive centres." Some of Flechsig's views, it seems to me, will be very slowly accepted, particularly those referable to the projec- tion systems of fibres. Already Monakow and Kolliker have taken serious exception to a number of his conten- tions and very recently Dejerine contests them. e of Language Site of the Revival of Words in Silent Thought. The zone of language is a part of the brain in which are carried on the processes essential to the facultas syna- trix. It is that part of the brain whose function is the Conception of Aphasia. \ \ \ necessary material substrate of conception and of com prehension and expression. By way of introduction, it may be said that when the expression "zone of language" is used I do not mean an area which can be mapped out on the surface of the brain by unvarying mathematical lines. Rather than have such interpretation put upon the use of the term, I should prefer to be understood to use it merely as an expression of convenience. This speech area or zone of language is not, in all prob- ability, strictly delimited. It varies in individual cases, and at different periods of life in the same individual, i.e., it is subject to phylogenetic variation and to ontogenetic variation as well ; the latter depending somewhat on the speech acquisition of the individual, arid on the range and number of avenues by which he receives or has schooled himself to receive information of objects. This area is a receptive and an emissive centre for all forms of stimuli or excitations that reach it, and which its individual de- velopmental metamorphosis has accustomed it to accept, to give tenancy to, and to elaborate into new forms of stim- ulation. It is receptive chiefly to auditory and visual stimuli, which it emits to other centres, and also to kinaes- thetic, olfactory, and gustatory stimuli. It is emissive to the frontal lobes and to the cortex of the Rolandic region, from which start the motor projection tracts and by which all thought externalization is mediated. The speech area or zone of language is an area made up of neurons, some of which send their axones into the Rolandic region and into the frontal regions of the brain, while others confine their distribution to the speech area i 1 2 The Faculty of Speech. itself, and, as they do not pass outside of this area, they may be looked upon as intercentral neurons. The zone of language has no projection fibres going directly into the motor projection tract ; it sends no im- pulses directly to the projection tract which carries down neural impulses to be externalized as speech. On the contrary, the zone of language sends impulses composed, in the illiterate, of auditory and articulatory memories of the word, and, in the educated, of auditory, visual, and ar- ticulatory memories, to the Rolandic cortex and to partic- ular areas of this region, depending on the manner in which the idea is to be externalized; that is, whether by spoken or written word or symbol, or by some form of mimetic or purposive action, which the judgment of the individual decides to be most serviceable in conveying the thought. If the idea is to be expressed by articulate speech, the impulses are sent to that area of the Rolandic region in which there is separate allocation for the move- ments of respiration, vocalization, lingual and labial ac- tion. This area is in the foot of the ascending central convolution, adjacent to the area in which are stored sen- sory memories of articulatory movements, Broca's area. From here the real motor impulses start. They go down through the motor projection tract, the axones of which en masse form the pyramidal tract, and the central motor projections of the cranial nerves, to the various muscles whose contraction produces articulate speech. There is reason to believe that these outgoing impulses are co- ordinated, given rhythm, force, and association, not in the cortex of the brain, but in stations situated in the brain ganglia, the cerebellum, and the pons-ob- Conception of Aphasia, 113 longata, the centres composing these stations acting automatically. When the idea is externalized in writing, in complex movements such as mimetic movements, or by a simple nod of negation or affirmation, a simple movement of beckoning, the genesis of the symbol or the pantomime is exactly analogous to that of articulate speech. They are all the result of internal language, and the person who writes an impassioned editorial on some subject that fires his patriotic spirit is as cognizant of the words ringing within him as the orator who enunciates them from the platform. The preparation of the language and the words is in both cases the same. They both re- quire the absolute integrity of the zone of language, the only difference being that in the second instance the fin- ished product is sent to the cortical area which is the cen- tre of the articulo-vocal musculature, and in the other to the Rolandic allocation of a much less complex motor mechanism, namely, to the cortical centre of the member that holds the pen, whether that member be the hand or other mobile part of the body. In the case of the speaker, the primary revival is in the auditory centre, and this in- fluences to activity both of the other speech centres the articulatory-kinaesthetic centre strongly, the visual centre slightly and the perfected word is externalized by the articulatory area/ o ihe Rolandic cortex. 1 In the case of a writer, it is probable that the visual centre is the seat of primary revival of the word, but it may be quite as le- 1 Some students of language believe that the primary revival of word images in the case of the speaker is in the kinresthetic articulatory centre, but I hold to the view as stated above. ii4 The Faculty of Speech. gitimately explained by considering that this takes place in the auditory area. The visual centre being then thrown into a state of vigorous activity, the articulatory-kinaes- thetic influenced very mildly, the finished product is sent to and executed by the arm area of the Rolanclic cortex. The direct association that may take place between the primary revival of auditory word images and the produc- tion of written symbols is seen in those rare cases of chil- dren born blind, who afterwards learn to write. As a rule, the visual centre conditions writing, both in its development and in its production. The action may be compared to that of a corporation which is accustomed to send simple commissions to one broker for execution, while other more complicated com- missions, requiring the concerted action of a number of people, are sent to a firm having facilities for the execution of such orders, who have accustomed themselves to such responsibilities. The first broker may execute offer- ings in a way very similar to the second, but his transac- tion is done in a different place and requires little or no concerted action, while the transaction of the latter is so complex that it requires the associate action of a number of individuals in different locations, perhaps in different cities. It is the same way in pantomime. Every one has felt the intensity with which some simple command, such as " Come back !" " Get out !" and the like, has been formed in internal speech on occasions when necessity or circum- stance compelled communication by signs. In such an instance the words are fully formed in the zone of lan- guage; we are cognizant of internal language, but our judgment informs us that it is wiser to attempt to sum- Conception of Aphasia. 115 mon to us a person who is beyond the reach of the voice by motioning of the hands or arms than by shouting. In such a case internal speech, though fully formed by the activity of centres in the zone of language, is externalized by a wave of the hand, the motor impulse of which starts from that portion of the cortex known as the arm-hand centre, and which is externalized by simple muscular movements. All thought or ideas are revealed through words, acts, and deeds, all of which are the immediate result of mus- cular action. This action is conditioned by influences operative on the Roland ic cortical area, for it is there and there alone that movements having differentiated func- tions have representation. It should be said here that this is in apparent contradiction to the claims of some experimentalists who teach that by excitation of the su- perior temporal convolution there results contraction of the musculature of the. ear, and by excitation of the oc- cipital cortex there follows movement of the eyeballs. It is by no means so on closer examination, and the theory here suggested explains more logically than does any other the slight muscular activity that results from such excita- tion. All the special senses are provided with a highly developed muscular mechanism whose function is to fa- cilitate the action of the special sense and to contribute to its perfection. The most typical example of this is the complex musculature of the eyeball, which is of inesti- mable importance in contributing to all of the welfare and pleasure that result to one through vision. The de- gree of complexity and the amount of musculature devoted to any one sense are conditioned by its needs, which always 1 1 6 The Faculty of Speech. stand in definite relations to the evolutionary stage or pe- riod of its possessor. In the human, the most advanced product of evolution, the musculature of the external ear is rudimentary, though not yet vestigial, because in man the sense of hearing has reached such a rare degree of perfec- tion, such an advanced .stage of development (it conditions speech), that he no longer requires a trumpet-like apparatus to turn reflexly in the direction from which sounds ema- nate, as do lower animals. It is the same with the other special senses, smell, taste, and touch. The sniffing move- ments of smelling, the smacking movements of tasting, the delicate movements that facilitate the tactile sense and which reach such a degree of acuteness in the blind, are all in evidence to show how important a part move- ment plays in contributing to the exquisite function of a special sense. Every time that the eyeballs move to look at an object, every time the ears are moved to contribute to the readi- ness and completeness with which the auditory excitation is obtained in the lower animals, there is stored up a memory not alone of the object seen or the sound heard, in their respective situations in the inferior parietal and superior temporal lobules, but a memory register is made of these movements, kinsesthetic memories, and it is highly probable that where the one is registered the other is registered also. It will then follow that excitation of such an area might produce movements similar to those the memories of which are there enregistered. Such movements would result on excitation of these areas by sending the impulses of excitation to the Rolandic area of the cortex ; thus the movement would be in reality an Conception of Aphasia. 117 indirect one along the route exactly analogous to that travelled by impulses from the zone of language to the Rolandic cortical area that result in articulate speech. I have said that all thought is externalized by words, by acts, or by previous acts called deeds. Articulate speech is the customary manifestation of thought, but the word speech is often used generically to include other forms of thought externalization, such as writing, paint- ing, instrumentation, pantomime, etc., which are specifi- cally called acts ; and I use it in such generic sense here. What is said for aphasia might as truthfully be said for amusia and for agraphia if it were wise to consider music and writing independent forms of expression, but it is not. Writing and other forms of symbolic notation are but media to which mankind attach a certain conventional significance in order to expedite and harmonize inter- course. The possession and utilization of such notation requires the integrity of the zone of language the zone of symbols and the production of internal speech. In order to use them, the memory images, visual, auditory, and articulatory, must be revived by stimuli coming from without, or spontaneously by the intelligence. In either case they are jutted into consciousness or not, as the case may be, after the completion of internal language, and they are externalized by motion in some form. I have used the phrase, "they are jutted into consciousness or not, as the case may be," advisedly, for articulated and written language may be produced without imperfection during unconsciousness. Normal sleep is the typification of unconsciousness. The time occupied by it is taken out of the cognizant existence of the individual as com- 1 1 8 7^ he Faculty of SpeecJi . pletely as if he were dead ; consciousness of the slightest degree is antipodal to normal sleep. Yet it is within the experience of every physician to have witnessed examples of pavor nocturnus, somnambulism, etc., in which articu- late speech, even to a considerable extent, was produced. In such cases the speech centres act harmoniously to an un- known excitant. The auditory images of the word are in some way revivified, and they in turn invoke the articu- latory images which send their impulses to the foot of the ascending frontal convolution, from which they pro- ceed to cause the movements of vocalization and of articu- lation. It seems very probable that, when speech im- pulses from the zone of language are deposited in the cortical motor area of articulation, the latter has no discre- tion or decision about what it shall do with them ; it must execute them at once. In the normal conscious state, however, the faculty of inhibition plays the part of moni- tor to this motor area of articulation and decides whether or not impulses sent there from the zone of language shall be externalized. If the judgment decides that it is wiser, more politic, more humanizing, not to externalize the mes- sages sent up from the speech area, it will annul them, but they are none the less vivid in internal language. It is not necessary, it appears to me, to dwell on this, for every reader must be in possession of numberless experi- ences conveying the truth of it. It is at trie basis of so many social amenities, the retort polite, the true psychologi- cal explanation of turning one cheek when the other has been smitten instead of making the reply that instantly arises in the zone of language, that I need dwell no fur- ther on this conscious inhibition of execution of all in- Conception of Aphasia. 1 1 9 ternal language. I wish only to emphasize that in states of unconsciousness the zone of language may be incited to activity and send its finished product to the Rolandic area cortex, which executes it, and executes it as it is sent there, without inhibition, addition, or deviation by the mandates of consciousness. In similar manner may be explained the aphorism " In vino veritas. " Alcoholic intoxi- cation brings about various degrees of intellectual abnega- tion, extending up to profoundest unconsciousness. There is a degree of this unconsciousness characterized by a ces- sation of the inhibitory influences of consciousness over the amount of internal speech that shall be externalized, and when this stage is reached the veritas vini is evident. The motion that externalizes ideas may be of the fingers in executing a musical fantasy; it may be of any complex action which " speaks louder than words." The more in- tricately co-ordinate the action by which ideas are exter- nalized, the more specialized is its representation in the Rolandic area, and the more liable is the localized lesion to cause disturbance of it. Articulate speech requires the co-ordinate action of a number of highly specialized peri- pheral parts. And it has been shown beyond parley that the larynx, tongue, palate, and lips have special Rolandic representation, while those for the execution of movements of writing have no other specialization in the Rolandic cortex than, that of the mobile part which holds the pen. Persistent repetition of the act of writing may bring about such great facility that the act is performed almost auto- matically ; but practice, though contributory to perfection, is not contributory to the development of a special centre; and the scrivener does not possess a special centre in 120 The Facility of Speech. which arc located the graphic motor memories any more than does the telegrapher a telegraphic centre, or the typewriter a typewriting-centre, or the pianist a special centre in which are represented the complex movements of the fingers. There is a special and very definite corti- cal allocation for movements of the hand and for move- ments of the fingers, probably for each individual finger; at least it would seem to be definitely proven that there is such representation for the index finger. The acquisi- tion of great dexterity from long practice in writing fa- cilitates the readiness with which such specialized motor centres functionate, the same as practice facilitates the execution of all acts, voluntary and involuntary. And this facilitation may become so great that the muscular movements required are made quite unconsciously, ap- parently automatically, as witness the movements of the telegrapher's fingers when they touch the keyboard, or of the violinist as he fingers the strings. Nevertheless, not- withstanding this facilitation, which may be dependent anatomically upon new association tracts, the essential constitution of the act is the same as it was in the begin- ning, and no short cuts have been formed nor has a new centre been developed. I shall return to the discussion of a special writing-cen- tre later on, but before doing so it seems to me desirable to cite some of the evidence that may be offered in sup- port of separate localization in the Rolandic regions for the movements of the larynx, palate, tongue, lips, and respiratory movements before taking up a discussion of the constitution of the speech area and the closer relation of the centres there situate to other parts of the brain. Conception of ApJiasia. 1 2 1 Because of the importance that I attribute to clinico-path- ological observation in comparison with the experiment- al, although fully cognizant of the great value of the latter, I shall cite first, as briefly as possible, a few cases that have been studied clinically and anatomically which seem to give unequivocal support to such localization. And before doing this, I shall call the reader's attention to the necessity of bearing in mind that in this mono- graph the term Broca's convolution is not used in the sense which many writers of the last generation would give to it to include the foot of the third frontal convo- lution, the lower part of the ascending frontal convolution, and the lower part of the ascending parietal convolution, and thus not only the entire frontal operculum but a part of the parietal as well. In this treatise the term Broca's convolution is used synonymously with " foot of the third frontal convolution," and its physiological synonym may be said to be " centre for articulatory kinaesthetic images." A case reported by Elder 1 is one of much importance. The essentials of this case are as follows : A man sixty years old, who had previously been healthy, suddenly developed difficulty in speaking. Speech became in- distinct and blurred, and saliva trickled from the mouth. On the following day he was so weak that he demanded admission to the hospital, and it was then found that there was paresis of the right side of the face, more marked in the lower part and at the angle of the mouth and not involving the orbicularis palpebrarum. The pa- tient was perfectly conscious, understood everything that was said to him, had no hemiplegia, and his only trouble 1 Elder : Edinburgh Hospital Reports, vol. :ii., 1895. 122 TJie Faculty of Speech. apparently was incapacity to enunciate words, due to diffi- culty in moving the tongue, lips, and other muscles of artic- ulation as readily as was necessary. There was no aphasia. The voice was unimpaired. He could protrude the tongue, although it came out rather slowly, and there was diffi- culty in swallowing liquids. The patient grew weak very rapidly and died five days after the occurrence of the dys- arthria, the immediate cause of death being hypostatic pneumonia, coma, but no hemiplegia having preceded death. On examination of the brain, there was found in the lower part of the ascending frontal convolution, about half an inch from the Sylvian fissure, a blood clot which had pushed its way through the cortex and which could be seen before cutting into the hemisphere, so completely had it destroyed the cortical substance. On slitting the brain horizontally, the blood clot was found to contain about two drachms of blood, situated at the level of the lower part of the ascending frontal and the ascending parietal convolutions. It had destroyed almost entirely the cortical substance of the lower end of these convolu- tions. It did not involve quite the whole of the lower end of the ascending frontal, as there remained intact a strip of cortex adjoining the foot of the third frontal. The foot of the third frontal was quite intact. The ac- companying diagram shows the extent of the lesion, both as it appeared from a view of the uncut hemisphere and on horizontal section (Fig. n, A). Internally, the hem- orrhage extended inward and forward immediately about the level of the lenticular nucleus in a very thin layer for about two-thirds of the distance between the cortical sur- face and the internal surface of the hemisphere. Its far- Conception of Aphasia, 123 thest point internally was close to the anterior limb of the internal capsule and quite in front of the motor tract. The situation and extent of the lesion is shown in pip;, n, B. This observation is almost equal to a mathematical demonstration, so convincing is it that a localized lesion of parts of the cortex which are strictly motor, namely, the Rolandic-area cortex, ./i may when diseased be at- tended by inability to make the peripheral associations or movements necessary for speech. It is not the only one of its kind on record, but it is the most con- vincing one on account of the fact that there were no complicating lesion and, of course, no other symptoms. Then the strictness with which the lesion was con- fined to the lower part of the Rolandic cortex, i t s sharp delimitation from the third frontal convolution, and the rapidity with which death followed the occur- rence of the lesion, all tend to make it an ideal case in proof of the claim of separate localization for the peri- pheral speech mechanism. Fig. ii. A y Subdivision of Broca's convolution, according to Elder, a, Centre of psycho-motor speech im- ages ; b, centre for adduction of vo- cal cords ; c and ,*^ ' 203 -v^~ ^/ O Have you children ? A. (Holds up one finger.) Is it a boy or a girl? A. (Smiles and makes a nod of nega- tion.) I want you to tell me if your child is a boy or a girl ? " B' B' ba" (that is, he endeavors to say " boy," which is the correct answer, but he is unable to articulate it.) How old is he? (Holds up five fingers and then three fingers to indicate that he is eight years old.) What is your wife's name? A. (Reaches for pencil, and writes in distinct Hebrew characters the word " Rachel.") It should be said here that the patient is fairly well educated in the use of his own language. He has learned to write with the left hand. He spends his time in writing letters to his family and friends, and in put- ting on paper his memories, thoughts, and hopes. The Hebrew language readily lends itself to construction by the left hand, its continuity being from right to left, and this patient writes with great distinctness and ac- curacy. It would seem on testing him a great many times that he writes spontaneously with more ease, accuracy, and correctness than from dictation. When, how- ever, dictation is slow and he is not hurried, he repro- duces most commendably. Many of his written produc- tions he carries with him, and when they are read it is seen that not only the thoughts but the diction are very creditable. In response to simple questions or sugges- tions, such as, " How would you ask the nurse to get you a glass of water?" he fixes the nurse in his gaze, nods to him expressively, points first to a glass, then in the direction of the hydrant, and then to himself. After- ward he makes a movement of carrying the glass to his lips. When asked to put out his tongue, to close the eyes, to squeeze my hand the same number of times as V 204 J & of Speech. there are syllables in the word " Constantinople," he does so very intelligently and correctly. The nurses and order- lies in the hospital say that he never makes use of articu- late words. The sensory side of his speech mechanism is apparently intact. On being asked if he understands everything that he reads, he nods his head in the affirmative, and writes "Yes." On being asked to read a paragraph from a Hebrew newspaper, and then to write its significance or meaning he does so consistently, but uses nearly the same words that he has read. Numerous attempts were made with this patient to determine whether or not internal speech was intact. On every occasion he was able to in- dicate the number of syllables in a spoken word, the num- ber of letters in a word, to write quickly the names of objects. In response to the question if he hears in his ears the sounds of the words that he reads, he writes "Yes." As to his hearing them with the same distinct- ness as when they are spoken, he answers " No," but that he hears them within himself. It may be said that he hears whispers with the same ease as loud-spoken words and sentences. He readily interprets familiar sounds, and when his eyes are bandaged and a watch, a bell, or the like impinges its sound upon his ear, he smiles and then quickly writes the name. In the same way he interprets the names and uses of familiar things that make impres- sion upon his visual, tactual, and olfactory apparatus. When shown a handful of coins and requested to pick out all the five-cent pieces, or twenty-five-cent pieces, or cent pieces, he does so with accuracy. He likewise matches pennies, according to the heads or tails, dates, etc. When a twenty-dollar bill is taken in one hand and a number of bills amounting to eighteen dollars in the other, and the patient asked which he would rather Motor Aphasia. 205 have, there is no hesitation in his decision. Quality goes before quantity with him every time. On being given a number of bills and coins, and asked to tell how much they aggregate, he writes down the correct answer. Sim- ple columns of figures are summed up with accuracy but slowly, and other problems in mental arithmetic are per- formed in such a way as to indicate the workings of the associative faculties. He can hum the national anthem, can whistle in unison with another, and plays dominoes, cards, and penochle with a great deal of skill. On being asked if he would like to be able to speak again, he looks suspiciously at his interlocutor and smiles in a most dis- couraged sort of a way ; but after a moment he reaches for the pencil and writes "Yes." On being told that it is very probable that he could learn to speak some words, he quickly joins in the physician's effort to repeat after him simple sounds, such as " o, a, n," and when he is shown how to place the tongue and when the lips are fixed in the proper position for him he makes intelligent efforts to produce the sounds into words, but persistent effort to teach him to speak has not been rewarded by material progress. This is a case of subcortical motor aphasia. It is the purest and most uncomplicated that I have ever seen. It is not associated with agraphia or with amimia, and the sensory side of speech production seems to be absolutely intact. Whether or not it was formerly, before he came to the hospital, I am unable to say, for critical examina- tions of his speech defects had never been made. His case illustrates with great force the fact that in subcor- tical motor aphasia loss of articulate speech may be the only expression of inability to project outwardly states of mental content. He writes with accuracy, both sponta- 206 The Faculty of Speech. neously and from dictation. He expresses mental feel- ing by means of mimicry which is very easy to compre- hend, but with the one exception of the word " Yes" and a sound resembling " No" he makes no use of articula- tion. His writing shows no evidence of repetition, of transliteration, or of agraphia. It shows, however, an un- commonly accurate portrayal of cognitions and feelings, for one of his class. To summarize the clinical conditions in this patient, they are : Loss of volitional speech ; the images of articu- lation are preserved and he responds to the Proust- Licht- heim test. On account of inability to speak, he cannot repeat from dictation. He can hum the air of a song, but not the words. He is unable to read aloud. On the other hand, there is no alexia or dyslexia, there is no agraphia; he can write voluntarily, from dictation, and from copy. He comprehends spoken speech, written speech, and ges- ture language. The word memory is not disturbed, the intrinsic speech mechanism is intact ; none of the word memories, visual, auditory, articulatory, are in any way destroyed. The left motor area or the projection fibres going out from it are destroyed, and this is indicated by the spastic hemiplegia which the patient has of the right side of the body. The fact that there are complete inability to articu- late a sound and pronounced inability to co-ordinate lin- guo-buccal movements, as in whistling, without facial paralysis, leads me to the belief that the lesion is of the cortical Rolandic area to which are allocated the repre- sentation for the vocal cords, tongue, lips, etc., and not lower down in the projection tracts. For if the lesion Motor Aphasia. 207 were of the latter it is probable that there would not be such complete aphemia as there is, but that there would be pronounced dysarthria or stammering. In this case, it would seem that the word impulse representing the idea left the zone of language properly and completely formed, but, on being handed over to the cortical execu- tive mechanism, it could not be started down the emissive pathways, the projection tract, on account of destruction of that part of the cortex. The next case, in which I believe from analysis of speech the lesion is lower down, is as follows : Mrs. S , a Polish Russian, thirty-eight years old. There is nothing of interest or of import in her family or previous personal history, with the exception of the event with which her present illness is associated. From members of her family it is learned that she was in per- fect health until immediately after the birth of her last child, five years ago, when she was stricken with an at- tack of apoplexy, followed by right hemiplegia and inabil- ity to communicate thought by means of spoken language. Examination shows, as will be seen hereafter, that she is in possession of the most complete understanding of every- thing that is said to her, and that she has the faculty of making manifest such understanding and her wishes by means of the most expressive gestures. Her whole vocab- ulary is contained in two or three words, which she uses for every occasion. What is your name? A. Frieda, Frieda, Frieda- Perl. (With a look of discouragement) Ich kann nicht. How old are you? A. (In a sing-song tone.) Five, and five, and five, and five, and five, and five, and five (and then she holds up three fingers, at the same time smiling very knowingly). 2o8 The Faculty of Speech. How old did you say you were? A. (Starts to repeat it.) Are you thirty-nine? A. No. Are you thirty-eight? (Quick as a flash.) Yes (this being the number of years indicated by the use of the seven fives plus three, reckoned on the fingers). Are you married? (In a sing-song way.) Oh, yes, yes, yes. Then she begins repeating, " Five, and five, and five, and five" in a sing-song way, and then holds up one finger, all of which indicates that she wishes to say that she has been married twenty-one years. On being asked if she has been married twenty-one years, she responds very quickly and accurately, " Yes." How many children have you had ? (She holds up one finger.) One? A. Yes. What is your child's name? A. I can't say. Is it a girl or a boy? (Points to herself to indicate that the child is one of her own sex. ) How old is she ? A. Five, and five, and five ; then she holds up three fingers, which, of course, means that her daughter is eighteen years old. Is she married ? A. Yes. What is her husband's name? A. I can't say (ich kann nicht). Has she children ? In response to this question, the patient makes the most exquisitely intelligent and ex- pressive gestures that I have ever seen ; her face is wreathed in smiles, she places her. left hand over her heart, and her right hand a short distance from the floor, as if to lift up the baby, and then she carries the imagi- nary baby to her breast, caresses and kisses it ; then she puts it down and kisses her hand to it; and then she waves kisses in the direction of her previous home in the lower part of the city. After doing this, tears are seen Motor Aphasia. 209 welling up in her eyes, but this condition is not the lachrymose uncontrollable attacks of depressed emotion, such as are sometimes seen in hemiplegia. Interpreted, these gestures mean, in the most inimitable way, that she has recently become a grandmother; that the little child is just beginning to creep; that she would like to press it to her bosom ; that it is the embodiment of her maternal affection ; and her tears come because she is separated from it. If it is suggested to her that this is the proper interpretation of her gestures she gives the most appreciative and grateful assent. If some other interpretation is suggested to her, she laughs scornfully. Tell me about your sickness. Oh, yi, yi, yi, yi, yi, yi, yi, vergessen (forgotten), kann nicht (I cannot) friiher (before), weiss es nicht (I do not know), spater (later), in, vergessen (forgotten), Frieda Perl (her first name, to which she attaches the word " Perl," for what reason I have not yet been able to fathom), finger. All this is said in a singing tone. After she repeats these words over for a time, she ceases to speak, as if discouraged with her own efforts. She then makes most suggestive gestures of combing her hair and then of toppling over, which means that her attack of hemiplegia came on without warning in the morning five years ago, while she was combing her hair, that she toppled over, and that she was unconscious for quite a long time. On examination of the patient's physical condition, it is seen that she is a well-nourished woman, that she has the gait and station of one with partial right-side hemi- plegia, and that the remains of the paralysis are more marked in the right upper extremity than in any other part of the body. The patient is able to perform all the general movements of the tongue and lips, and there is no difficulty of swallowing, no dribbling of saliva, and 2io The Faculty of Speech. she goes about and takes care of herself as does any patient whose, mental integrity is undisturbed. As has been said, the language of her emotions is not at all im- paired, and she smiles, frowns, laughs, and cries, when- ever there is cause for any of these outward expressions, but she does not manifest them without cause. She under- stands all that is said to her, and detects with the great- est readiness unreasonable or ridiculous statements or speeches. The sensory side of her speech faculty is ap- parently intact. The patient has never been able to read or write, but she recognizes the letters of the Hebrew alphabet and words which form parts of familiar prayers, and she recognizes and puts the proper interpretation upon pictures. For instance, shown the picture of a girl, she smiles, and points to some young female patient in the ward, or goes through the gestures mentioned above to indicate that one of the kind in the shape of her little granddaughter is in her own family. When shown the picture of a cow she tries to pronounce the word, but when she cannot she reaches for a glass and by carrying it to her lips aims to indicate that the cow gives milk ; and so on, with all the objects or subjects with which she is vis- ually familiar. Sounds and words apparently make natural and proper impression upon her auditory peripheral and central ap- paratus, and the tick of a watch, the sound of a bell, the paring of an apple, the note of a pianoforte, are all heard and properly interpreted. As I have said, she is wholly uneducated from a scholastic standpoint, and unable to write. Naturally, therefore, it is impossible to say whether or not there is agraphia or paragraphia. We may infer, however, that had she been able to write there would have been no defect in the graphic representation of thought except that due to the immobility of the right hand. It Motor Aphasia. 2 1 1 is perhaps unnecessary to state, after what has been said, that the patient responds with alacrity to the ordinary commands, such as " Put out your tongue," " Close your eyes," " Bring me a piece of paper," and the like, and con- ducts herself in every way like a person in the fullest pos- session of her mental faculties. Repeated examination of this patient, week after week, shows no material change in the responses to various tests. The age of the patient, a consideration of her previous history, the physiological effort immediately antecedent to the attack, the mode of onset, duration, symptoms, and termination of the attack, and the degree of physical and mental recovery which followed it, all point unerringly to an embolus of one of the lenticulo-striate arteries as the cause of her apoplexy. It is not my purpose to pursue further a discussion of this question here, first, because it is not patent to the symptom complex of subcortical aphasia, which now interests us, and second, because the onset and course of the hemiplegia were typical of cere- bral embolism. A retrospect of her history shows that in the beginning there was complete aphemia, and that now, some five years later, the aphemia is still present, but is not ab- solutely complete. She can articulate a certain number of words; she can use properly the words "Yes" and "No;" and almost always the sentence, "I can't say." The few words of her vocabulary are used with conven- tional significance; but they are produced in a sing-song fashion, that is, the sounds are intonated and the words are repeated, and the rhythm of their emission is irregular. Really, the most striking and interesting side of her 2 1 i The Faculty of Speech. possessions in the way of projecting mental contents is her ability to use pantomime so expressively and intelligently. It is not necessary, in this connection, to say again that thought is expressed by movements. Movements are particularized in three ways, namely, by speech, by writ- ing, and by pantomime, and inability to express thought by any one of these three ways is known respectively as aphemia, agraphia, and amimia. In some cases of apha- sia, all of these defects are present, in others one may be complete, and the others only partial ; while in sub- cortical motor aphasia, of which this case is an example, one of these faculties, oftentimes two, are entirely spared, while the third is completely destroyed. In this patient it would seem that the projection areas of the cortico- nuclear neurons which serve motor speech production were interrupted by the vascular lesion that occurred five years ago, but that the cortico-nuclear neurons which conduct impulses that are manifest by pantomime have remained intact. Sufficient has been said in speaking of the exam- ination of the patient to show that the associative faculties which are the basis of intellectual activity have not been seriously impaired in this woman, nor has the internal language. Her mode of answering questions into which the element of time is interjected shows that she is capa- ble of reckoning. I might say in this connection that if she were asked to compute simple problems in mental arithmetic she would indicate the correct answer by hold- ing up successively the fingers of the hand, or by intonat- ing, " Five and five and five," and so on, until she ap- proached as nearly as possible to the correct figure by using the multiple ; then she would complete the answer Motor Aphasia. 213 by holding up one, two, three, or four fingers as the case required. The distinguishing features in these two cases are that in the first there is no agraphia and the patient responds to the Proust- Lichtheim test, and in the second there is no disturbance of internal language. If it be a fact that a lesion of any of the speech centres entails some perver- sion of internal language in every instance, these cases do not belong to the category of cortical or true aphasia. They differ one from the other by the fact that the first patient is absolutely speechless, and the second can say a few words but only in a sing-song manner. When she says, " Fiinf und fiinf und fiinf und funf," etc., she does not speak it, she sings it. The other words that she uses are pronounced very imperfectly and indistinctly. It is impossible to indicate this on paper, for she speaks the Yiddish jargon and it has to be translated into English that it may mean anything on paper; but as a matter of truth the words that she uses are very imperfectly articu- lated. It is this imperfectness of articulation of words that she can say and the sing-song way of making replies that lead me to believe that the lesion in her case is at a lower level than in the case first enumerated. That is, I believe that in the second case the words may get started down the projection tracts on their way to be externalized, but they meet with an interruption in the shape of the lesion at some lower level. Those fibres that are not en- tirely severed allow the transmission of the impulses that are externalized by the patient's few words. When the lesion causing subcortical motor aphasia is situated at a level as low as the internal capsule, then a 214 The Faculty of Speech. diagnosis can often be positively made, not alone from the employment of the Proust- Lichtheim test, but from study of the trouble of articulation. Such a patient has all the components of internal language absolutely intact, inclu- sive of the articulatory kinaesthetic images of words. He hears, sees, writes, mimics, and in other ways gives evi- dence of intellectual integrity. He is incapable only of causing the co-ordinate movements which subserve articu- late speech. He may not be totally devoid of power of articulation ; his incapacity may vary from simple slur- ring and elision of certain syllables and words, through dysarthria, dysrhythmia up to complete anarthria and arhythmia, and thus complete speechlessness. This partial loss of the capacity for articulate speech is well illustrated by the first case of Dejerine cited above. In the conventional use of the term, this condition is aphasia; but it is not true aphasia, for true aphasia occurs only with lesion of the area of language. Yet it simulates true aphasia so closely that a differential diagnosis can be made only after very careful study. The nearer the lesions to the cortex, the more difficult will this differ- entiation be for such cases. All the projection fibres coming from the executive articulatpry area are more likely to be involved, and with it there may be some func- tional perversion (possibly transitory) of the zone of lan- guage. The differentiation of cortical and subcortical aphasia sometimes becomes of inestimable importance from a medico-legal point of view. For instance, a patient who has the symptom complex of motor aphasia due to a sub- cortical lesion may be just as competent to make a will Motor Aphasia. 215 and dispose of his possessions as a man who has hemi- anaesthesia due to a central lesion ; but a man who has cortical motor aphasia, and thus a derangement of his in- ternal language, entailing some deviation from normal in every component of speech, be it in hearing, in seeing, or in expressing himself (including writing), may be quite incapacitated from such disposition, according to the interpretation of the law. Cortical motor and subcortical motor aphasia are both almost invariably associated with right hemiplegia, and are dependent upon the same lesion. 1 In the cortical form the hemiplegia is apt to be less complete, and the spasticity of the paralyzed parts great. Moreover, it usu- ally follows immediately after the stroke, although it may occur with epilepsy, tumors, abscess, foci of inflammation, or other conditions which slowly destroy Broca's area. 1 In Appendix II. a case of cortical motor aphasia is cited in which there was no hemiplegia. It may be of interest, or instructive perhaps, to read it in connection with this chapter. CHAPTER VI. SENSORY APHASIA. I. General Considerations. 2. Sensory Aphasia: Word Deafness or Audit ony Aphasia, and Word Blindness or Visual Aphasia. J. Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. As motor aphasia is used to designate those disturb- ances of speech expression in which the chief diffi- culty is in making speech, sensory aphasia, as a term, is applied to those cases in which imperfections of language, disability or inability to speak, are due to interference with the reception of speech forms ; that is, to lesion of the perceptive areas of the brain and the immediate in- coming and commissural pathways of such areas. The perceptive centres by whose functioning speech is onto- genetically developed are the auditory and the visual, and sensory aphasia is thus practically auditory and visual aphasia, and it is as such that I shall describe it, after a brief introduction to the subject. Sensory aphasia may be defined as loss of the under- standing of words, due to interference with the formation of associations necessary for complete perception. An- atomically speaking, cortical sensory aphasia might be de- fined as aphasia due to lesion of the posterior part of the area of language, and cortical motor aphasia as due to lesion of the anterior end of this zone. The subcortical Sensory Aphasia. 217 forms of each variety occur when there is lesion of the pathways which carry impressions into and away from the zone of language. When Wernicke first described sensory aphasia it was as a symptom complex characterized chiefly by loss of comprehension of words, heard and read, often associated with paraphasia and agraphia. Clinically speaking, this is what is understood by the designation, and although sensory aphasia is subdivided into auditory and visual, it is only that the subject may be easier of comprehension and more in accordance with the symptoms that our cases present. The separation of sensory aphasia into auditory aphasia and visual aphasia is quite natural, in view of the fact that physiology and pathology are in the fullest accord in granting separate allocation to the two functions, seeing and hearing, whose integrity is so essential to the de- velopment and maintenance of speech. As the cortical representation of these two functions is independent, it follows that one may be diseased without the other. In such case the resulting symptoms are predominantly of the centre diseased, but nevertheless there are always some symptoms referable to the centre not the seat of lesion, for none of the speech centres are autonomous and lesion in one entails perversion of the function of all the others. In other words, in order that speech may be per- fect, whether it be internal speech or external speech, it is necessary that the entire speech mechanism be intact. For the occurrence of a physiological reflex it is necessary that the afferent tract, the centre, the efferent tract, and the part in which the reflex act is manifest be intact ; and if any one of them be disorganized the reflex act will be cor- 2 1 8 The Faculty of Speech. respondingly abnormal. Each element entering into the constitution of the reflex arc is an entity, if one chooses to call it so, and each one of its parts may continue to discharge certain functions after another part is diseased, but it can no longer contribute its share to the perform- ance of a compound or a conjoint act after such disease. In the same way speech becomes disordered when one of the components entering into the speech mechanism is diseased. Nevertheless, when the auditory area is alone the seat of destructive process, there is no reason why the visual and articulatory kinaesthetic memories should not remain intact, and so they do ; but when memorial recalls from these centres are intercalated into the speech circle, the auditory link is missing, and the circuit cannot be completed ; the result is disturbance of speech, referable particularly to the hearing, because that is the element that is wanting. Moreover, as the primary revival of words takes place in the vast majority of peoples by the initia- tive of the auditory centre, when this is diseased internal speech is very defective. The blood-vessel supplying the zone of language is the left middle cerebral artery, the so-called Sylvian artery. Lesion of different parts of its course entails different re- sults. Disease of the anterior part and branches is apt to cause motor aphasia ; of the posterior part and branches, sensory aphasia. In the beginning the sensory aphasia may appear to be equally of both the auditory and the vis- ual centres, but, as time goes on and nature strives to over- come the lesion, the affection of one of these centres of lan- guage may be less serious than that of the other, and there- fore the symptoms referable to it will be soon cleared up. Sensory Aphasia. 219 The symptoms referable to the other sensory centre may be caused by lesion of a branch of the vessel going directly to that centre, and therefore these symptoms are continu- ous and persistent. Thus it is readily seen that a degree of auditory aphasia and visual aphasia, or word deafness and word blindness, as they are unfortunately called, are frequently coexisting phenomena. The predominance of the one, or the apparent individual occurrence of the other, depends upon the seat and the intensity of the lesion. Many cases of sensory aphasia, especially those due to vascular lesion, have in the beginning symptoms pointing to implication of both the auditory and the visual centres; yet after the condition has existed for a time the symptoms referable to one of these centres may be reduced or even become latent, while those referable to the other centre per- sist and dominate the clinical picture. Thus auditory aphasia or word deafness and visual aphasia or word blind- ness are differentiated remains of one and the same sensory aphasia. Sensory aphasia possesses certain very definite general features, which I propose to enumerate briefly before taking up the separate discussion of auditory aphasia and visual aphasia. These general features are in many ways materially different from the general features of motor aphasia, and they are explained by the location and rela- tionships of the different centres. Motor aphasia is al- most invariably associated with hemiplegia, while sensory aphasia is rarely accompanied by hemiplegia unless the lesion is a most extensive and severe one. In the case of motor aphasia this is easily explained by the proximity of the convolution of Broca to the motor centres in the 220 The Faculty of Speech. Rolandic region and to the motor projection tract. On the other hand, if the lesion be confined to the posterior part of the zone of language, the cortical motor area and the projection tract constituted by its central axones may be entirely spared. Compared with motor aphasia, sen- sory aphasia is relatively more often associated with lesions that are not primarily vascular in origin. In other words, it is more likely to occur with encephalitis, with tumors, with injury, and with certain degenerative diseases of the brain ; and thus its evolution is often very different from that of motor aphasia. Whereas the former is almost always abrupt, and consequent on an apoplectic stroke which usually entails more or less prolonged loss of consciousness, sensory aphasia not infrequently unfolds itself slowly, and even when due to a vascular lesion it oftener comes on progressively or in repeated accessions than does motor aphasia. Another very striking feature of sensory aphasia is that in the beginning it is the aphasia of comparative speech- fulness, while motor aphasia in the beginning is usually absolute speechlessness. Sensory aphasia is characterized by logorrhoea, motor aphasia by alogia. Then the career of sensory aphasia is most instructive. The unfortunate patient starts in with his senseless loquacity, and week after week, sometimes day by day, one notes the shrink- age of his useless vocabulary, through the stages of bab- bling, of lalling, and of echoing, down to absolute mutism as complete as that produced by total destruction of the articulatory kinaesthetic area. The course of a sensory aphasic may be compared to that of a runaway engine. Some accident opens the throttle during the absence of Sensory Aphasia. 221 the driver, and away it flies without regard to destination, to danger, to results. Gradually, as its steam becomes ex- hausted, its pace becomes slower, it becomes less boister- ous, it is more easily overtaken, until finally the last atom of steam is exhausted and it comes to a dead halt. There is nothing to stir it up and start it on. It stands ready, however, at any moment to be made a useful agency ; it requires but coals and water. If it remain unused for a long time, rust and other accompaniments of time so incapacitate it that, even if these elements be given to it, it is no longer a useful agent, and it stands a useless wreck of its former self, a wreck induced by loss of the agencies necessary to drive it, all dating back to the acci- dent that opened the throttle. It is the same with the patient who has had a lesion of his auditory speech centre which is sufficient to derange it without completely destroying it. From the beginning of the person's ability to speak this centre has exercised the influence of a director-general over the articulatory speech centre, inhibitory and excitatory. Now this in- fluence is taken away. The result is that all the inhibi- tory influence is destroyed by one blow, and the work of years of experience on the part of the articulatory centre in the endeavor to become a centre of the primary reviver of words begins to dissipate like a corundum wheel when breaking, and the result is logorrhoea. After this automatic storehouse (which is always small, even in the most expert linguist and most highly educated man) is ex- hausted, the vocabulary begins to shrink, and, as the centre which has always acted as the stimulator in the memorial recall of words is destroyed, the patient gradually approxi- 222 The Faculty of Speech. mates mutism, although the articulatory kinaesthetic centre is itself intact, ready to do its work on the reception of the proper incentive. But if this incentive has been long with- held, like the engine that becomes incapacitated by rust and other ravages of time, it will be incapable of doing it, even though such incentive should be given to it long after. This is but another bit of evidence in favor of the interactivity of the different speech centres and against the autonomy of any one of them. Patients with sensory aphasia are very rarely reduced to a condition of mutism by such lesion alone, because de- struction of the auditory centre is rarely complete. When the lesion of the auditory centre is slight, the most strik- ing abnormality in voluntary speech is the inability to use words with their proper signification, although the words that are used are articulated with as much clearness and distinctness as in the normal state. The patient may utter words that are entirely the opposite of those which he intended to use. No more striking example of this can be given than a reference to the history of a lady cited by Trousseau, ' who, arising to greet a visitor, would with courteous bow and apparent welcome say in the most matter-of-fact way, " Pig, brute, stupid fool," which when interpreted by one of her family meant, " Madame begs you to be seated." Happily the misuse of words does not often take this decidedly frappant form. It may be ap- parent only by a change of position of words in a sentence, or by the use of one word for another that has a somewhat similar sound or beginning. The condition is one that admits of ready explanation : the sensory centres being de- 1 Trousseau : "Clinical Medicine," fourth edition, vol. ii., p. 674. Sensory Aphasia. 223 stroyed, their images can no longer be evoked to act as a check or control on the articulatory centre, the images in which are always evoked before the thought that is to be externalized can be expressed. The defect of speech known as " jargonaphasia" occurs oftentimes with sensory aphasia. This may be considered a degree of paraphasia, although the latter is properly applied to a condition in which words are used in an incor- rect sense. Jargonaphasia consists of the production of a jumble of words all forged into one, the syllables of which may be articulated, but the words have no similarity to words as usually spoken. Indeed, so wholly dissimilar are they that some cases of jargonaphasia have been con- sidered evidence of a supernatural influence manifesting itself in the sudden acquisition of a language undecipher- able by the most profoundly versed in strange tongues. Reports of these cases not infrequently find their way into newspapers, and excite great attention and are much marvelled at by the laity. They are oftentimes investigated by committees made up of men imbued with praiseworthy scientific zeal, but oftener by persons who have exhausted their interest in things that conform to the established laws or courses of nature and who find inclination only in the supernatural. Jargon speech is occasionally an accom- paniment of disordered cerebration dependent upon altered states of consciousness attending acute sthenic and asthenic states. It is not alone in spontaneous speech that these perver- sions are manifest ; they are as evident when the patient attempts to repeat what he hears (indeed oftentimes very much more so, particularly if the patient is partially word 224 The Faculty of Speech. deaf). In singing also the condition of paraphasia and even of jargonaphasia is sometimes very striking. The patient remembers and is able to hum the air of a familiar tune, but the words that he attempts to fit to it have no sense, no propriety, no reason. The occurrence of para- phasia and of jargonaphasia on reading aloud is also very striking, but as this subject will be considered in more detail under the subdivisions of sensory aphasia auditory and visual aphasia I shall merely make mention of it here. Not secondary in importance to the information that may be obtained from a study of articulate speech is that which is to be had from an examination of the spontaneous, dictated, and copied writing. Patients with pronounced sensory aphasia are not usually hemiplegic, so that tests for defects of writing can be undertaken without trouble if the patient be made to comprehend what is wanted. Defects in writing are most striking when visual aphasia is the prom- inent feature of the sensory aphasia, although they occur in every case of sensory aphasia. The degree to which spontaneous writing may be preserved, or lost, in sensory aphasia varies with the patient, the seat, and the intensity of the lesion. Oftentimes there is preserved, even in cases of genuine visual aphasia, the ability to write a few words spontaneously, such as the patient's name, address, the name of some member of his family, and possibly a few other words ; but even in the production of these the patient gets the surname following on the family name, and in other ways shows the condition known as paragraphia. If the patient has destruction of the angul'ar gyrus, there will be practically total agraphia, because he cannot revive the visual memories that are necessary to be evoked in order Sensory Aphasia. 22=; that the mobile part of the body carrying the pen may be directed by the thought to inscribe them. If the lesion is principally a destruction of the auditory area, there will be absolute inability to write from dictation; and even though writing spontaneously may be preserved to a very limited extent, the words or sentences produced will be markedly disordered in their arrangement. Writing after copy is preserved in every case of genuine sensory aphasia, but the patient copies in a way that at once puts the stamp of his infirmity upon his work. He copies letters the way a beginner does a drawing, line for line, curve for curve, angle for angle, and makes an exact reproduction of what is before him in the same manner as does an engraver or a forger of a man's signature. These variations in writing will be discussed particularly under visual aphasia, in which variety of sensory aphasia they are most pronounced. I shall cite the following case as a typical example of sensory aphasia, even though the symptoms were predomi- nantly referable to that subdivision of sensory aphasia known as visual aphasia. It appears to me much more instructive to give accurate reports of interviews with such a patient than merely to state the results of such ex- aminations. These reports demonstrate more conclusively than can any description, the shortcomings of speech as they really exist. This case has been of great interest to me as one offering difficulties of interpretation, and I have many stenographic reports of interviews with the patient, two or three of which I shall now present. L. B , female, thirty-nine years old. Said to be married. Admitted to my wards in the City Hospital, 15 226 The Faculty of Speech. July 8th, 1896. History: One year ago on awaking in the morning she could not move the right side. The lower extremity was not so helpless as the upper. Since that time she has not been able to talk understandingly. About eight or nine months later she had an epileptic attack, and since then she has an attack about once a month. Practi- cally nothing is known of this patient's history. The facts just mentioned were obtained from the registrar of the hospital, to which she had been taken immediately after the beginning of her illness. These facts were given by the individual who brought her to the hospital. Since she has been in the City Hospital we have not been able to find out anything about her from herself, nor has any one been to visit her from whom such information could be obtained. In discussing the case I shall limit myself, therefore, to the results of examination. A general survey of the patient shows that she is able to walk without the aid of a stick ; she swings and drags the right leg and foot, not in a characteristic hemiplegic way, yet with a considerable paresis and spasticity. The right upper extremity is in a state of contracture and very nearly completely paralyzed. There is no involvement of the face or cranial nerves. To convey a proper idea of her speech disturbance, I shall give a stenographic report of a few examinations. Examination, September ^.th. What is your name? m'm'm'y name, m'name's Samp- son, my name's's I don't know. I can't tell that. You heard the doctor read the name ? No, that I know. Is your name Lucy Brown ? Yes. Is your name Lizzie Brown ? No. What is your name ? Lucy. Lucy what? I can't say. Sensory Aphasia. 227 Is it Lucy Black ? No. Is it Lucy Brown ? Yes. How old are you? I can't tell you, mu mother, my mother, my g'mother go. Are you twenty ? I wouldn't say. Are you more than twenty or less than twenty? Oh, yes; twenty-one, twenty-two; I had children you know. Are you forty ? Oh, no. You are seventy-five ? Yes, ma'm. Is your mother living? Yes. When did you see her last ? She comes on the first m'm'mth. Was she here the first of this month ? No, the last of the month. Are you older than your mother? I can't tell that much. Is your mother twenty ? (Putting hand to throat) I can't tell that much. Is she fifty ? Of course I can't boys got Are you married? Yes, I'm married about o'o'o' can't call it now. How long have you been married? This boy twenty- one boy. How long have you been married? I told you, I couldn't tell you I couldn't tell; sometimes my speech Why can't you tell me? What? (in surprised tone). Why can't you tell me? Married I tell you, please (pointing toward city) mother-care. Here she began to talk of her paralyzed side "My hand s'clame [lame] sometimes s'clame, sometimes strong." I do not know yet whether you had one child or twenty ? I have had two. 228 The Faculty of Speech. Do you understand all I say to you ? Oh, me. Why yes, ma'm ; I was sleeping, that's all. Where are your children ? Boy and a girl, boys, boys, girls. How big are they ? One boy is dead. How big is the other? Twenty-one. That is right, is it not ? Yes, sir, right. How long have you been in the hospital? Well, I think it is two months, I can't tell, Miss Buckley (the nurse) knows. Have you been here a year? Oh^ no, sir; no, sir. Where did you live before you came here? Its, its, its, in though its, its I can't tell it (in despair). Where do you live ? I can't tell you, but I can tell you tame. Do you remember how the house looked? Oh, yes, sir; yes, sir. Would you know it again if you saw it ? Yes, sir ; yes, sir. Can you remember the house next door? I don't know, I think so. Can you remember how your mother looked? Yes, ma'am ; my m'm'm'oth other was lighter woman. Can you remember the sound of her voice? Mercy, yes ; oh, yes. What was it like ? White woman. What was it like ? What do you mean ? My mother was a white, my mother was a white, light woman. Can you remember the scund of a bell ? Oh, I guess so. Can you imagine that you hear a bell now ? No. What is a voice ? Voice, voice. Yes; is my voice like the voice of that gentleman? (Smiles.) Voice, voice. Have you ever heard that word before ? No answer. Sensory Aphasia, 229 I then spell v-o-i-c-e and ask her to repeat it, but she cannot. When she tries she says p-u-c-c-b. You can say that pretty well ? Yes I can't once in a while, I have to suffer. Do you know the letters of the alphabet ? I can say them. Say them ! C, c, c, no, no, is that right ? C, u, t, v, u, c, o, n, u, p, e, u, t, m, n, o, t, c, t, o. Does the alphabet not begin, a, b, c, etc. ? Yes, that's it. Now let me hear you repeat the letters of the alphabet after me. A? u. B? c. C? u. D? w. E? w. F? i. G? c. H? h. I? i. J? j. K? k. L? o. M? i. N ? u. - O ? o. P ? u. O ? yaw. R ? r. S ? t. T ? a. U? t. V? u. W? w. X? x. Y? a. Z? c. You recognize that the alphabet starts off a, b, c. Yes., sir, I recognize but I can't say them, that's all. It is noticed that she watches very closely the lips of her questioner. Test for Recognition of Objects and Naming Objects. Holding up a watch. Clock. Chain? Clock, it's a clock. Key? It's a clock (said very hesitatingly and starts to correct herself after the word is partly out, but at last lets it go). Holding up another and more typical key? It's a key. Pencil? 'S'key, it's what's its name, can't tell it now. It's a clock, can't tell it now. Knife ? Knife. A roll of bills ? It's a dollar. A two-dollar bill ? I can't tell, a dollar. A twenty-dollar bill? A dollar. Tain't, can't do it now; I know it but I can't do it now. 230 The Faculty of Speech. Count on your fingers how many dollars this is (showing five-dollar bill). Doesn't understand and cannot be made to, and even after I show her how to do it and illustrate what I mean she cannot do it. Once she recognized and named a five-dollar note. Although she seemed to know the value of different denominations, she could not pick out the money when bills were called by face notation. Match box ? Match box. Cigar? P-p-five, pipe. Is it a pipe ? (With emphasis) No, sir. It is a cigarette ? Oh, no. It's a cigar? Yes, it's a cigar. What am I doing now (lighting a match) ? Match, turning it up, lighting. How old do you think I am? I don't know. Eighty- six? (Scornfully) Oh, no. How many eyes have I ? Two. What is the color of my beard ? No answer. Black? No. Yellow? No. Green? W'w'ee, I can call it, but I can't say the name. Is it red? Y-e-s (doubtfully). What day of the week is it ? Wesday (it is Sunday). What day? W-e-s , yes, sir, Sunday. What do you do on Sunday? Sleep. Are you tired ? Oh, no, no, sir ; oh, no. Can you move your right hand ? That one (pointing). Apparently she cannot tell right from left with ac- curacy. (Shown a picture of a man.) What's that ? (Hesitates.) Is it a cow ? Oh, no. Is it a woman ? It's a man. (Shown a picture of a woman.) Sat's boy. Why, that is a woman. Yes, I say it's a boy. (Shown some letters, words, a book.) Points at them Sensory Aphasia. 231 all indiscriminately, yet still she seems to recognize some difference between them. (Testing letters.) A? u. N? o. H? c. E? t. R? r. S? r. E? t. M? c. I? _. N? E? r. W? n. What did you have for breakfast ? (No answer, seems to be thinking.) Stewed kidney (most improbable) ? Oh, no. At what hour do you dine, as a rule, Lucy? (Greeted with laughter.) What do you like to eat? I'm not pertickler. Test for hearing. A tuning-fork held to the ear seems to annoy her, as if there was great hyperaesthesia of hearing. She can detect a bell and the jingle of coins in one ear as well as in another, but she cannot say what they are. She hears the lowest whisper and apparently comprehends, and if one says she does not she assures him that she does. Calls a watch a clock when put to ear. She could not tell the time, but seemed to recognize the error if one said it was a different hour than indicated by watch. (A cat walks across the floor). (Suddenly) Lucy, what is that? (Flabbergasted.) Is it a dog? Oh, no, no, sir. Is it a cat? Yes, sir. What kind of a cat? (Smiles.) Lucy, do you take in washing here ? No, sir, not here. Do you drink beer? Once in a while. Whiskey? No, sir. What is your husband's name? Lucy Brown. There is no tone deafness and she can hum tunes in unison when some one whistles ; can detect tunes but can- not say what they are. A knife, a pencil, a tuning-fork, a spool of thread, a book, a pair of scissors, a small bell and a penholder were put in a row in front of her, and she was asked to pick them up as their names were called. She did this with 232 The Faculty of Speech. considerable accuracy, particularly after she had done it once or twice. In the beginning there was some hesita- tion, a searching look directed toward me, as if to ask, "Is it right?" and a clumsiness in picking them up, but she soon selected them without mistake or appreciable uncertainty. Examination, September i^tli. What is your name? Well, tell him, m'u mame can't you (last addressed to nurse). What is your name? I can't, can't, can't tell it now. Why not ? I can't, I can't, my name is oh, o-, o-, o-, o-, I can' . How old are you? Ma' mother's name, my mawth' name is ma'name. How old are you ? My mother got it down. Can't you tell me? No, ma'am. Have you a father and mother? My father is dead, my mother is dead. What was your father's name ? Reilly. His first name? Can't tell that. Your mother's name? M'a mother, Margaret Reilly (bit off). Are you Irish? I don't know, but that is my name though. What is your name? M'm'ma name is I can't tell you. I can't call it. I know what it is though. Is it Lucy Brown? Yes, sir (slowly). Did you ever go to school ? N'o, not much ; I used to t'k washing. Could you read? Yes, ma'm (with a glib, cock-sure manner). Could you read ? Little bit. Could you read? Write, oh, no, sir; no, sir, no. Sensory Aphasia. 233 Are you married ? No, I told you yes. Were you married? Yes, sir; two children. What was your husband's name ? Sampson. His first name ? I can't call that name, but I can say it. Is he living? Yes, ma'am, but I don't know where he is tho'. How long were you married? Two children, I can't catch that. What were your children? Two boys. You told me the other day they were a boy and a girl. No, sir, I didn't. I beg your pardon, you did. I did not (beginning to cry). What was your first boy called ? Sampson, after his mother. The second boy ? Little boy was dead, was dead any- how. What do you mean by that ? I can't tell. Was he born dead ? Yes, sir. How long is it since you had a child? I can't say, I can't call ; fo', fo' years, I can't call it but anyhow my boy, ah, u, oh, ah (sort of a re very). Will you count for me? 12345 67? 9 1 1 i j_ _I can't. What is that figure (large 4) ? No answer. Same for other figures. (Showing coins) What is that? Money. How much is that? Five cents (right). That? Twenty-five cents (right). Please pick out a dime ? Selects a penny. Pick out a ten-cent piece ? Does it correctly. Pick out a five-cent piece? (Picks out a ten.) (Showing two-dollar bill) How much? I can't tell, I can't call it. 234 The Faculty of Speech. Is it money? Yes, but I can't call it. Is it a five-dollar bill ? Yes, I know it now, it's a one- dollar bill; no, it's a five-dollar bill, yes. (Holding up key) What's that? Key. (Penknife.) (After some hesitation) Knife. (Pencil.) Key isn't it ; no, was do you do with it ? Interlocutor recites first line of Longfellow's " Bridge," also Lord's Prayer, and asks her to repeat the first line, then half a line at a time. It is absolutely impossible, she makes the mouth go and occasionally makes an audible sound, but in no way repeats. Say birds ? Words. Rat? Cat. Mother? Mother. Steamer? Simmer. Kettle? (In amazement, and does not repeat.) Says wasin for basin, richer for pitcher, and in other ways simulates the sounds of words. A number of familiar things are spread out before her, and she is requested to pick them up as their names are called. Pick up the key? Picks up scissors. Spoon? Correct. Glass? Correct. Hair pin? Correct. Look- ing-glass? Pincushion. Pencil? Correct. Key? Knife. Lock? Bell. Thimble? Correct. Looking-glass? Fum- bles pin cushion, then picks up mirror. (This is in marked contrast to the successful efforts of the last ex- amination.) I then said to the house physician that she had mistaken" the pincushion for the mirror before, and then she began to say something very inco-ordinate, like " I said, you, well, some, befo'," etc., all of which might be construed as an explanation of her failure to pick things up properly. There was marked confusion of names of things. After she had picked them up once, she seemed to be able to do it more accurately the second time, but after picking them up a number of times she got more confused. Sensory ApJiasia. 235 She seemed to have the faculty of tracing simple figures such as squares, circles, and crosses, but she could not copy simple printed letters, although she could trace them fairly well. When asked to make a square or a circle after she had been shown a drawing of one, or to make such figures from memory she failed in the attempt. To-day she seems to be able to match pennies with considerable accuracy. If I put down three pennies and ask her to put down an equal number, she does so. She also matches heads and tails very well. She cannot call the notation of money. She does not appear to be so well to-day as heretofore. She is emotional and several times is about to cry, and when starting to cry she says something about children. Examination, November ^.th, 1896. What is your name ? Lucy. Is that all of your name ? Lucy Reilly. Is it not Lucy Black? No, ma'am. Is it not Lucy Brown? Lucy Brown. What is your name ? Lucy Reilly, I told you (indig- nantly). You said it was Lucy Brown ? Me (in tone of aston- ishment) ? No, sir. Well, is it not Lucy Brown ? Yes, I told you it was. How can you have two names, Lucy Brown and Lucy Reilly? m'm'w son. What about your son ? He's dead, he is. Well, what about him ? Nothing at all. Then what did you mention him for? Oh, I don't know. Did you ever go to school ? Not much ; no, sir. Why not ? Don't want ; the doctor told me, washing all the time, my mother. 236 The Faculty of Speech. Can you read ? No, sir. Why not ? Cause I could. How old are you? Ma answere, mo amte got my age. How old do you think you are ? I can't tell. Are you a hundred ? Oh, no. Are you more than one hundred ? Yes, sir. Less than one hundred ? I don't know. (This is rather characteristic.) She apparently appreciated the ridiculous- ness of asking her if she were upward of one hundred, still in the very next breath she said that she was. Then she denied it, and if her questioner looked astonished or in- credulous she became embarrassed or indignant. One is almost sure that she does not understand the question, but she assures him that she does. Then the next moment she answers a question not only properly but very quickly ; for instance : How do you feel to-day? First rate, only I want to go home, that's all. Where do you live? I, I'v, I told, I can't tell you, I can't tell you now, but I know the name of the street all the same. Tell me how your sickness came on. She hesitates, and after a time, during which she seems to be getting ready to talk, she begins : " One day I had the morning I was on street swashing one day I was my mouth come she said I was so tizzy I, I, I, morning I forget the rest." All this is told in a long drawn-out way, as if she were telling a story that she had memorized. Test for Recognition of Money. (Handful of change, eighty-one cents.) How much? Fifty cents. (This after she picks the coins up in suc- cession, looks at them, and feels them with great de- Sensory Aphasia. 237 liberation). Then I ask again, How much? She hesi- tates. (Holding up twenty-five-cent piece) How much? Three, I told you. One cent ? Three cents. Ten? Three cents, f, f, f, f ( as if endeavoring to say fifty or five). Fifty? M'o, five, no. Is it fifteen ? No. Is it ten ? Yes (correct). Examination, November 2$th. How many fingers are held up? One, two, three, four, five, six, seven, seven, five, seven, five, five fingers (lat- ter correct). (Holding up seven fingers) How many? Nine. (It seems that she cannot tell without reckoning on her own fingers, and then not correctly.) I hold up five fingers, and she counts them slowly and correctly. I hold up ten fingers, and she responds "nine." Ten, wasn't it ? Yes, sis. She is now given a roll of money and asked to count it. She turns it over, separates the individual bills, then pays no attention to it until told to go on with it again. There are fourteen dollars in the roll. She counts the four one- dollar bills, but hesitates when she comes to the ten-dollar bill, and then says : Dollars, dollars, you know what I mean. Show me with your fingers how many dollars that bill stands for? She makes no reply. She is either unable to respond or she does not comprehend the question, and emotionally there is no evidence that she does understand. It is a twenty-dollar bill, isn't it? Yes, yes, sir. 238 The Faculty of Speech. It is a twenty-dollar bill ? No, sir. Well, you said it was a twenty-dollar bill ? (Exclama- tion of indignation and surprise.) I did? No, sor; no, sir; (mumbling) yes, sir; no, sir. Did you ever go to school ? Once in a while. Couldn't ; working all the time. Well, when I was a girl, playing all the time. Yes, sir, working. A cat walks into the room, and I ask her to tell me what it is. She replies quickly and promptly, That is a book. It is a little book? Oh, no, sir. Or, no. No. Well, you said it was a book. (Indignation) I ? Oh, no, sir. Well, what is it? (Hesitatingly) a child. A little girl. What color is it ? I can't call it. I kept urging her to name it, and finally she burst out, "It's a little cat." Is it the same color as that (holding up brown paper) ? A. No, sir. As you ? Yes, sir. She not infrequently uses " Yes, sir," for " No, sir," and vice versa, and sometimes there is a repetition of words " intoxication" by a word. What's that (pointing to bird in cage) ? Bird. What kind of bird ? I know but can't tell. It's a robin, isn't it? Yes, sir. It's a canary bird ? Yes, sir. I knew it was. A robin. What is it ? A robin? No, I tell you it's a canary. Yes, sir; I said it was a robin (indignant tone of voice). (Pointing to a pair of shoes.) What are those ? Ladies' shoes. (Pointing to an artificial orange.) Ladies' shoes. (Pointing to a mirror?) Ladies (hesitates). (Pointing to a purse.) Can't call it. Sensory Aphasia. 239 (Pointing to a book.) Bo', bo', bo', I can't call it, I can't. (Stops abruptly.) Lucy, tell me again about your sickness? One day wa' was washing, and all I tell you that, I don't know I tell you, but was, s s washing, com par my daughter, the girl me, and I should I can't, I can't, and then the girl, washing, oh, o-h, the boy other, some stuff, and I look, that's all, got me I said it, see? Well, tant, I can't tan o-h, I can't tell that. Tell me what you did yesterday. Warden, on I sway all looking warden names fighting, fighting, fighting, I'll go home anyhow. Se doctor names (it is thought that the patient is endeavoring to say that some of the other patients in the ward taunt and tantalize her, call her nigger, etc. , and when she is asked if this is so she responds promptly that it is.) I ask her to repeat the following sentence, " I am de- lighted to make your acquaintance." Way, m', me, me, m, once, me (continued to get more monotonous and lower). Lucy, what day of the week will to-morrow be? Pure, fure, not Friday, I can't call it. Friday? No, sir. Saturday? Yes, sir. (After a little bit, when her interlocutor makes no reply, she promptly says, " No, sir.) Thus she is absolutely unable to repeat. She is also completely unable to read words and letters, and I be- lieve unable to read figures, although I have never been able to convince myself of the latter. Sometimes she apparently recognizes the figures on a banknote. Cer- tain it is that she cannot read figures on a test card. She is unable to write a letter spontaneously or from dictation, and she is unable to copy, except the simplest lines. She is, however, able to trace. 240 The Faculty of Speech. It is now for the first time made quite certain that she has typical right-side homonymous hemianopsia. This was thought to be the case after the first examination, but the paraphasic answers on the part of the patient and the difficulty in making her comprehend what was de- sired of her when objects were brought into the visual field, made the test very uncertain. To-day objects thrust toward the right sides of both eyes, as if they were going directly into the eye, do not cause the slightest blink- ing. A candle brought toward on the right side is not perceived until it gets beyond the mid line. When a summary is made of the results of repeated ex- aminations of the speech defects of this patient, we find that the most striking defects of spontaneous speech are amnesia of words, particularly of nouns, and paraphasia. The patient can usually tell her name, but she cannot tell the names of members of her family or her residence. In fact, even to-day we do not know where she lived or the names of any of her friends. The articulatory images seem to be preserved. If she can call up what she wishes to say, there is no difficulty in saying it. It is impossible for her to repeat the simplest sentence after me. This is well illustrated by her effort to repeat the conventional phrase, " I am glad to make your acquaintance." All that she produces is an unintelligible mixture of sounds consti- tuting gibberish. There is relative preservation of the melody of some songs with inability to use the proper words. She can sing the airs of hymns and popular tunes, but she cannot get the words in properly. There is in- ability to read aloud or to herself. She cannot write spon- taneously or from dictation, and there is very great diffi- Sensory Aphasia. 241 culty in copying. In copying simple geometric figures, if the copy is removed for a moment she cannot go on with the delineation. There is word blindness, a loss of the comprehension of written and printed words. She is still capable of distinguishing one object from another, one color from another ; and she can match pennies with some certainty. Figures on a banknote are recognized but not mentioned. During one test she seemed to have little or no notion of the difference between a letter and a word, and in looking at a printed page she could not point out single letters as different units from the individual groups of letters constituting single words. There is right lateral homonymous hemianopsia. There is a slight degree of word deafness, which becomes more apparent the longer the case is studied. This word deafness does not seem to me to be due so much to a destruction as to a functional degradation of the auditory word centre. This functional degradation is the most patent cause of her am- nesia. The amnesia verbalis which she has, i.e., of per- sons, places, and things, is the usual kind of amnesia as- sociated with slight disorder of the auditory area. The symptoms which she presents are characteristic of sensory aphasia, and I believe it to be a case of sensory aphasia in which lesion of the visual centre and the subadja- cent white substance is responsible for the leading speech disturbances. Her capacity to speak and her vocabulary vary from day to day, and vary considerably with her dis- position and general tone. She has shown herself able to articulate with distinctness at times words presented to her consciousness by voluntary recall of past impressions, by auditory impressions, and by association ; i.e., those sug- 16 242 The Faculty of Speech. gested to her mind by an appropriate question. She fre- quently says, " I can't tell," and oftentimes in such situa- tions she manifests the state that is best termed amnesia verbalis. This amnesia is most marked for proper names and nouns, and very slight for verbs, adjectives, and pro- nouns. It is not easy to decide whether this be due to conceptual, to kinaesthetic, or to auditory amnesia, and no tests so far have positively settled which one of these important elements is most disturbed, but it is very prob- able that the amnesia is predominantly auditory. On the other hand she at times indicates that the word is just on the tip of her tongue. It appears as though the word were mentally formed. She gives every indication of know- ing what she wants to say and of having the feeling of making the proper innervations ; but the wrong word comes out. In other words there is paraphasia. This wrong word frequently presents interesting relationships with the correct word ; if she is asked to say B, she may say C in- stead. On one occasion when asked to say X, she called out I. Here there was a double relationship, first to the contiguous letter Y, and then to the similarly sounding let- ter I. Many contrast relationships manifest themselves, such as saying "yes" when she means "no;" "mam" instead of "sir." The wrong word was also frequently associated by similarity of sounds with the proper one. The influence of words just heard, or just thought of, also manifests itself occasionally, simulating an enforced echolalia. There are many instances, moreover, which suggest that the right word is just about to be spoken after a number of struggles, without any result; "wed" has been enunciated for " red," and through a series of approxi- Sensory Aphasia. 243 mations of this sort, the patient has frequently been able to arrive at the proper word or words. She seems to have hypersesthesia to tone vibrations, and they appear to produce a disagreeable tickling sensa- tion in the ears. This is mentioned because it has been associated by Freund and others with verbal perceptual deafness, without general acoustic deafness. The pa- tient, despite this intelligent hesitation in responding to questions, shows at times a faulty perception of the significance of the words of others. I say to her, " You said yesterday that you had three children," and she will say, " Yes, three. " I say, " What were they, "two boys and a girl?" She will say, "No, two boys." I then say, " You have three children, both of whom are boys?" She will say, "Yes," even repeating the state- ment after me. If I then say, '" You do not mean three children, you mean two," she will say, "Yes, two." In repeating words and letters her greatest difficulty seems to be on the articulatr/y side. Nevertheless, I feel cer- tain that there is some perceptual word deafness, but it is the slightest of her deficiencies. She seems to call up with a great degree of certainty the use and significance of objects, and even their names. At times, however, she shows marked deficiencies amounting almost to com- plete perceptual blindness. I have seen her overlook an object that was held in the hand, which I named with dis- tinctness and with which she had shown herself quite familiar. She tried to strike a match on the inside of a box, although she had just shown that she understood the significance of a match and understood what striking it meant, and it was not until I had told her that she should 244 '-The Faculty of Speech. strike it on the outside and move the box about a little that she seemed to perceive the side of the box, felt of it, and then ignited the match. The mental power that seems most persistently deficient is the ability to grasp ideas of number. If I say, " Match that penny," she will put the penny down, correctly match- ing it. If I say, " Put down as many pennies as I do," she is utterly at sea. She seems to have no notion of number. This is manifest also in regard to relative ages. She seemed only to be sure that her mother was older than herself. A fairly simple task she performs with ease ; but even a small complication throws her off the track. She matches one penny at a time with ease. Put down three pennies and ask her to match them with an equal number, even though the pennies are widely separated, she will put down all the coins in her hand, at the same time making no attempt to match them ; or, if she does, failing signally to do so. The case seems to present some symptoms of restriction of consciousness and of detachment of con- sciousness, such as are met with in cases of hysteria. Then again she shows, but only very occasionally, a blank stupidity, absent-mindedness, or mental vacuity and conse- quent confusion. Auditory ApJiasia. Verbal Deafness. Word Deafness. In discussing the localization of the various sensory and motor elements involved in the faculty of speech, we dis- tinguish three distinct speech centres : the articulatory, the auditory, and the visual centres. Strictly speaking, the latter centres, in so far as their functions concern the faculty of speech, are not sensory areas but areas of per- Sensory Aphasia. 245 ception, or, better still, of apperception. The so-called sensory aphasias do not present disorders of sensation, nor yet of simple perception, but disorders of the interpreta- tion or understanding of certain classes of perception. Destruction of these centres produces conditions in which memory significance of spoken or written speech is lost. Though the sound of the words and the images of the words may be clearly perceived, there is a loss of co-ordi- nation which prevents these simple receptive centres from arousing other elements in adjacent centres, whose activity is necessary in order to awake the memory impressions stored there as the result of past experience with the spoken and written symbols of speech. In this form of aphasia, first differentiated by Wernicke and given the name word deafness by Kussmaul, there is inability to understand spoken words. This is dependent apparently upon the total loss of auditory verbal memory images. It is one of the uncommonest forms of aphasia, and it rarely occurs individually, being frequently asso- ciated with some degree of visual aphasia or motor apha- sia. Strictly speaking, the defect is not word deafness at all, for not only do such patients hear spoken words, but frequently they show no diminution in the intelligent ap- preciation of the significance of simple, co-ordinate, or purposive sounds. Such patients, although word deaf, may be as alert as ever to the significance of a shrill blast of the whistle from an approaching engine. They detect sounds, even the slightest, as quickly as does the person whose auditory mechanism is intact, and they seek the origin and the sig- nificance of such sounds. In fact, because they are un- 246 The Faculty of Speech. able to interpret spoken words they are often apparently more keenly alive to sounds. It is true that an aphasic patient may also lose the significance of differentiated or purposive sounds but such an occurrence would needs be in a more pronounced case of aphasia than that which is typified by the term auditory aphasia. Such sounds are not usually complex and highly differentiated, therefore loss of the capacity to interpret their significance is rare. There are as many kinds of auditory aphasia as there are varieties of interpretation put upon symbolic sounds by our consciousness. The seat of lesion which causes auditory aphasia has already been pointed out to be the cortex of the middle and posterior portions of the first temporal convolution, extending over into the second tem- poral and upward into the supramarginal convolution, where it impinges upon the cortical area for visual verbal images. Lichtheim 1 has reported a case of so-called subcor- tical auditory aphasia in which there was word deafness with no disorder of speech. The lesion was thought to be of the auditory projection system, in the white substance within the left cerebral hemisphere. Wernicke and his pupil Freund * report cases of extracortical peripheral le- sion, located by them in the labyrinth, which manifested symptoms of word deafness, very similar to those reported by Lichtheim and others, as due to subcortical and trans- cortical aphasia. It would appear that pseudo-aphasia symptoms, which must be very carefully distinguished from those of true aphasia, may be due to actual lesion or to functional defect in the sense organ or in part of the subcortical receptive tract. 1 Loc. fit. 9 Loc. fit. Sensory Aphasia. 247 The symptoms of auditory aphasia are subjective and objective. If the aphasia is limited to simple word deaf- ness, the patient hears the voice in which words are spoken, but the words convey no idea to him and he has no more comprehension *what they mean than if they were spoken in a tongue which he never before had heard. He, however, recognizes the significance of other sounds, unless it be that the memory pictures for such sounds are also lost. Naturally there are different degrees of word deafness, depending upon the extent of the lesion or the destruction of the auditory area. In some cases the ex- tent is so great that the sound of the voice which speaks them is simply perceived as a sound, and such patients do not recognize the sound of their own name. In other in- stances they recognize the sounds of their own names and possibly the names of other members of their family, their places of residence, business, etc., words that have for them a much wider significance, and are more deeply imprinted in their memories than is the ordinary concrete word. In other cases the limitation of the lesion in the auditory area allows the patient to comprehend that he is being spoken to, and possibly to understand a word here and there. It is often very difficult when the patient gathers the sig- nificance of a single word to say just how complete the auditory deafness is, unless the physician be very careful in controlling suggestive gestures and facial expressions, for some patients quickly interpret in part the significance of what is spoken and guess the rest. If this form of aphasia exists for a long time, the patient may acquire con- siderable skill in lip reading. In the milder forms of word deafness it is often neces- 248 The Faculty of Speech. sary, in order to estimate correctly the degree of word deafness, to test trje patient carefully and repeatedly. For instance, if the patient is asked to protrude the tongue and he does so ; to extend the right hand and he obeys, it might lead one far astray if he were" to make a note, " The patient has not lost the comprehension of spoken words, for he obeys requests." Patients with word deafness are often keenly alive to their infirmity, in fact morbidly so, and they strive to conceal it by adopting any ruse that occurs to them. As protruding the tongue, extending the hand, etc., are customary accompaniments of a doctor's visit, they guess the meaning of questions which convey no ideas to them, and sometimes the responses are perti- nent, either by accident or clever gesture reading. In those cases in which the word deafness is slight, the pa- tient understands one or two words of a question, and pieces it out in his own mind accordingly. The simula- tion can be easily exposed by changing the sense of the question, while employing practically the same words or a number of the same words. It is then apparent that the patient does not recognize the different questions, for he answers as before. The concomitant accompaniments of word deafness, in- ability to write from dictation, defective comprehension of what is read, imperfect writing, paragraphia, etc., may be disposed of very quickly. Inability to write from dic- tation, which a patient with auditory aphasia presents, needs no explanation. If the sound of the words do not revive the memorial significance of these words in the auditory centre, no impulses inciting the visual centre to visualize the word proceed from the auditory centre. Sensory Aphasia. 249 Internal reading is disordered, because the primarily excited visual word centre, in transmitting the impulses to the auditory area, finds the latter disordered, and there' is in consequence defective revival of corresponding word memories and lack of comprehension of what is read. The paragraphia is an expression of the disorder of inter- nal language, which is always present in true auditory aphasia. A patient with word deafness, having a lesion that cuts him off from the significance of all that is said to him is practically rendered deaf. And as the catastrophe comes suddenly, after he has for years been accustomed to the delights of auditory sensations, he is naturally very much changed in manner, in appearance, and in demeanor. He is quiet and observant ; his glance betrays suspicion or fear, and his demeanor is often one of trouble and unrest. This change in demeanor and manner, combined with the paraphasia which is often strikingly manifest, the inability to repeat from dictation, and the profound diminution, even to complete absence, of spontaneous speech, have often led physicians and laymen alike to look upon these unfortunate patients as insane. It need scarcely be said that such an implication is unjust. As has already been said, word deafness rarely, if ever, exists alone. It is often associated with cortical mo- tor aphasia, and frequently, on account of the proximity of the auditory area to the visual area, with some degree of word blindness. Wernicke and Lichtheim claim to have seen cases in which there were absolutely no defects of articulate speech. If the first of these two conditions ex- ists, the patient is unable to communicate thoughts in 250 The Faculty of Speech. writing and is dyslexic, while if the last be present he is word blind as well. Examination of patients with word deafness reveals dif- ferent objective conditions dependent upon the degree and completeness of the word deafness. Even though the patient does not grasp the question that is addressed to him, he will endeavor to give answer, but the answer has no pertinency to the interrogation ; in the first place be- cause the patient does not understand the question, and in the second place because emissive speech is dependent upon the integrity of the auditory centre, that is, upon the revival of auditory memories and their transmission to the articulatory kinaesthetic centre. On account of the fact that the centre for articulatory memories is guided and correlated in action by the auditory centre, disease of the latter causes not only amnesia of words, but misuse of words, a condition to which the name paraphasia is given. I have said before that a patient in whom the primary re- vival of words has been through the auditory centre will be more incapacitated by a lesion that interferes with his auditory centre than will one who relies in part or largely on the revival of visual memories of words, or for the re- inforcement of the memory of words by visual images. Such individuals are rare compared with those who revive words through auditory memories, but nevertheless they ex- ist. An illiterate man who gets word deafness becomes al- most speechless, because the illiterate man is then deprived of his only mode of recalling words to mind. According to Hughlings Jackson, Ballet, Strieker, and others, words may be revived as motor processes, i.e., by stimulation of the centre of articulation (which they consider motor). It Sensory Aphasia. 251 is perhaps unnecessary to say here that the writer does not admit this, for it is one of the fundamental ideas in the conception of aphasia that he has put forth that Broca's area is not motor but sensory, as has been contended by Bastian for more than a quarter of a century. Moreover, in another place it has been pointed out that, developmen- tally, in the child the articulatory power is conditioned by audition. If the centre in which are stored the articulo- kinaesthetic memories of words that is, Broca's centre is not affected, articulation of words revived in thought by visual impressions may be but little impaired. This is especially noticeable in response to questions the signifi- cance of which the patient gets through the visual areas ; that is, questions addressed to him by writing and by pan- tomime. But in such replies the words may be transposed, because the patient is not in possession of the faculty to know whether or not they have the proper sequence, for he can hear his own words no better than he can those of others, and, the auditory centre being destroyed, the artic- ulatory centre is deprived of the directing control which the former exercises over the latter. It is very necessary that this statement should be as lucid as possible. If when one is speaking aloud a word is misplaced or a word is not used in its proper sense, if there be made what is called a lapsus lingua, the auditory area, which is keenly alive to the slightest misuse of words, quickly detects the error and communicates it to the intelligence or carries it into consciousness. This in turn calls up the articulatory image of the proper term, which is then articulated. The sound of every articulated word acts as a stimulus to the auditory centre for the next. If the auditory centre has 252 The Faculty of SpeecJi. been destroyed there is no such leader in the memorial order of words, and the frequent occurrence of lapsus lin- guae constitutes paraphasia. The patient is usually not cognizant of the mistakes that he makes in speaking, though sometimes when he is he treats them lightly and essays not to notice them, or he is loath to express him- self in words at all, providing he be in no way demented. Usually, however, the patient with partial auditory apha- sia is loquacious, the logorrhoea being devoid of sense or pertinency. Occasionally one encounters a patient with word deafness who prefers to answer questions in writing, because the intactness of the visual memories of words enables him to control his output. In a similar way the auditory centre guides the action of the articulatory centre in the employment of internal language. In other words, I do not believe that the motor centre of speech ever be- comes independent of the sensory centre which presided over its education, as is claimed by Bernard in his exten- sion of Charcot's teaching. If the visual area is coincidently diseased and the pa- tient has right-side hemiplegia as well, such a patient will be, objectively, as one without a mind. The important re- ceptive avenues of speech are closed to him and the emis- sive, likewise. He may be able to make known some of his thoughts or wants by means of pantomime, but just how much it is very difficult to say, except by the study of each individual case. As an instance of a case of this kind I may cite the following, which I saw with Dr. H. P. Hirsch : A. M , German, fifty-nine years old, married; by occupation a merchant. His wife has had four miscar- Sensory Apkasia. 253 riages and has borne one full-term child, which has since died. The patient, who had been a temperate, well-pre- served man, of a kind and lovable disposition, was in ex- cellent health until about three years before the onset of his present illness, when he had an attack of rheumatic sciatica. Since then he has had recurring attacks of " rheumatics" in the arms and legs. About two years after the attack of sciatica he had a mild attack of endocarditis. During the year preceding the present illness his entire disposition and temperament underwent a change. He became iras- cible, irritable, worried over trifles, and got fatigued more easily than formerly. He never complained of headache, nor were there symptoms that led to an examination of the urine. Attacks of the blues came oftener and stayed longer. On the afternoon of the 2 1 st of November the patient came home from business, apparently in his usual health. He sat in an easy chair and read a paper. Suddenly with- out warning his wife noticed his arms drop, his eyes roll up, his respirations become dyspnoeic. She applied re- storatives and in about five minutes, he recovered con- sciousness, asked his wife what she was troubled about, did not seem to recognize that anything had befallen him, averred that he was all right, and did not want a phy- sician. If he appreciated that he had had an attack of unconsciousness he did not say anything about it, and when dinner time came he went to the table as usual and partook of the meal. It was noticed by the members of the family that he did not use the right hand so much as usual, and he said that it felt somewhat stiff and heavy. Before retiring he took a hot bath, and on getting out of the tub he had another syncopal attack of very short dura- tion. He passed a restless night, and in the morning on arising he found that his right hand was very stiff, un- 254 Th e Faculty of Speech. wieldy, and lame. He seemed to be in other respects quite well. There were no noticeable change of facial ex- pression, no hesitation of speech, and no difficulty of getting about. On the afternoon of the 22d, Dr. Hirsch, a friend but not heretofore his physician, was sent for. He noted that the pulse was rapid and hard, the face flushed and anxious, and the patient evidently in a very anxious state. Examination showed a paralysis of the right arm which, although not complete, prevented the patient from using the arm except slightly. There was no involvement of the face or of the right leg. There were no sensory dis- turbances. The patient seemed to be in fullest possession of his faculties, talked distinctly, and explained rationally to the physician why he had been selected as the medical adviser ; told of his symptoms, showed that he was able to read understandingly, and in other ways demonstrated that he used words at their proper worth. The physician was sent for again about seven o'clock that evening. The patient had become unconscious while in the bathroom. He remained profoundly unconscious for upward of seventy-two hours, during which time his temperature was subnormal. He then recovered conscious- ness gradually. He was completely paralyzed on the right side, unable to speak, and apparently unable to understand anything that was said to him. For the next six weeks he was completely speechless ; then he began to say the words . " Yes" and " No" and some other monosyllabic words,. but he did not use them correctly. He has not been able to understand spoken or written words since the attack. Oc- casionally, if commands are repeated a number of times, such as "Put out your tongue," etc., he will obey, but almost always it is necessary to show him pantomimically what is meant. After the second month his newly ac- quired, limited, monosyllabic vocabulary began to shrink, Sensory ApJiasia. 255 and then he began to " babble" and to echo words that he heard. The babbling became almost continuous for a time and it was thought that the patient was insane. After a while it became less frequent and was produced only in response to or after a question. One day, about six weeks after the attack, he suddenly began to sing a German song, which he had often sung, " O Tannenbaum, O Tannenbaum, bau, bao, ba, ba, ba, bo, " ending in a laugh ; and on a number of occasions he es- sayed to sing when the piano was played, but he never got beyond a few words. Status, March i6th, 1897, nearly four months after the beginning of the present disease. There is hemiplegia of the right side of the body, the right side of the face being involved only to a very slight degree. The paralysis of the extremities is not pronouncedly spastic, but the ten- don jerks are very much exaggerated. The heart impulse is weak, about ninety times a minute ; there is a harsh systolic murmur, heard with greatest intensity over the aortic valve and the second sound is accentuated. The pulse is small and feeble. On being asked his name he begins to whine and intone sounds which may be expressed by the following words : " Nein, nein, na, no, no, nein, bettau, betta, tau, tau, nein, nein," etc., beginning in a moderate tone and then get- ting higher pitched, and ending in a babble. All the time the musculature of the face is in such a state that on looking at the patient one would suppose that he is crying, but in reality there are no tears. " How old are you ?" No response, but after a few mo- ments he begins the above senseless, articulated babble. " Are you ninety years old ?" Does not recognize that he is being spoken to. In fact, test as long and as completely as we may, it 256 The Faculty of Speech. is absolutely impossible to convey any meaning to him by spoken word. He is alive to the slightest noise, and turns his head when a person comes in the room or on the occasion of a slight noise the origin of which he does not understand. It is possible that there is right homonymous hemi- anopsia. It was difficult to get positive proof of this, as it is in all cases in which the patient cannot understand a word that is spoken to him and does not comprehend writ- ing. The wife, however, suggested its presence by insist- ing that when she approached anything to his mouth from the right side, such as in giving him a spoonful of medi- cine, or to wash his eye, he did not see her until after she had got it immediately in front of him. On bringing the finger or fist quickly toward the right side of the eye, the patient does not blink or make any movement to indi- cate that he recognizes the approaching object which is seemingly going directly into his eye. When the same test is tried on the other side, the eye blinks at once, and he even indicates that he sees objects, such as the light of a candle brought into the visual field from the left, but in no way gives heed to it when brought into the visual field from the right. There is complete alexia. A letter from his family, who are in Europe, is given to him, but although he takes it in the hand he does not recognize a word. It is the same with print. There is total agraphia; he cannot make a stroke with the pencil put in the left hand. When the hand is made to grasp it and the physician's hand guides it to shape a word, he takes no interest in the matter, apparently not having the slightest idea of what is being done, and begins to say, " Nein, nein, na, na, bateau," etc. There is in addition to this some mind blindness, but this does not seem to be so pronounced now as it was a . Sensory Aphasia. 257 few weeks before, when he many times endeavored to drink out of the urinal, and in other ways showed that he did not recognize the uses of objects. Nevertheless the apraxia is not complete, for when given his eyeglasses he places them astride his nose, in a particularly dexterous and intelligent way. This, however, may be an automatic act. Voluntary speech is entirely lost, except the words that we have given, which he uses on all occasions. Repeti- tion of speech is also lost, but sometimes he surprises the examiner and the family by echoing what is said, particu- larly if it be said a number of times and in a loud voice. For instance, if one says " Good by" or " Good morning" a number of times, he may respond like a parrot, " Good by, ba, ba, ba, ba," and he will take up the old refrain, " Na, nein, nein, betteau," etc. In this way he occasionally says, " Pfui" (first used by his wife, on one occasion when he was about to drink from the urinal) ; " Lieb, " after the question, " Liebst du mich ? Hast du mich lieb?" Without in the slightest manner understanding the question he may articulate with con- siderable clearness, " Lieb, lieb." He apparently has some recognition of music, for when his daughter plays a familiar air on the piano he is atten- tive and seemingly follows it. Examination of this patient eight months later reveals practically the same condition as above stated, save that the word deafness is, if changed at all, more complete. The hemianopsia is very difficult to demonstrate, and, if it exists, it is very slight. The only change of any im- port is a marked echolalia that he has developed. If one says, " How old are you ?" he repeats over and over, " You, you," with a rising inflection on the last letter. " How is papa?" "Papa, papa," repeated and repeated. Usually he takes the last word of the sentence that he hears and 258 The Faculty of Speech. echoes it, occasionally the last two words. Such as " Will you have an orange?" "An orange, an orange," he re- peats--the "an" with great vigor and clearness of enunci- ation and with a rising inflection on the last syllable of orange. Complex words he occasionally attempts to echo, but he does not succeed in so doing. There is still a de- gree of that condition known as mind-blindness, but it is not so conspicuous as when he was first seen. No general description of auditory aphasia can be given to cover every case, so great is the variation in individual instances. In some cases there is only inability to inter- pret a certain language, or a certain dialect, or a certain number of words. For instance, there are a number of cases on record in which persons seemingly as familiar with several languages as with one, have had word-deaf- ness for all of them except one, and that one the mother tongue, the one acquired first. Some years ago I had a patient who was employed as an official interpreter at the Immigration office. He developed a very slight right-side hemiplegia, which soon ameliorated so much that it was scarcely to be noticed unless examined for, but he had a marked degree of word deafness for all languages except Swedish his mother tongue a language which he had in later years rarely used, and never except when occasion or necessity compelled him. He was partially word deaf for Swedish. This language having been first acquired, it is logical to infer that it was most indelibly imprinted in his speech area and least easily destroyed. I have said before that there must necessarily be as many forms of auditory aphasia as there are distinctive symbolic sounds. Spoken speech is the most highly sym- Sensory Aphasia. 259 bolic ; the next most differentiated is music. To the form of aphasia in which there is deafness for musical notes, the designation tone deafness (musical deafness) is given. Amusia as an accompaniment or as an integral part of aphasia has been studied carefully by Edgren in recent years. In his article a number of cases are cited in which the auditory form of amusia has been subjected to criti- cal examination, and some of them were studied in the light of post-mortem examination. Musical deafness is almost always associated with word deafness, but there have been a few cases recorded in which it occurred apart from the latter. Such is an in- stance cited by Brazier, ' in which a famous tenor of the Opera Comique was suddenly seized during a performance of an opera with complete amnesia of words and music ; neither the orchestra nor his fellow-singers, who tried to prompt him, succeeded in reviving his memory. Another instance in which the tone deafness was not associated with word deafness was that of a man who suffered from attacks of ophthalmic megrim, during which there were passing attacks of motor aphasia, lasting from four to five hours. On one occasion there was no aphasia but he could not distinguish musical airs. The " Marseillaise," on being played by a military band, was not recognized ; although he could hear quite well, he did not know the tune. He knew only that it was a noise of brass. The clinical forms of amusia are strikingly analogous to the clinical forms of aphasia, and they generally accom- pany the latter, although the different varieties of amusia 1 Brazier: " Amusie dans 1'aphasie." Rev. Philos., October, 1892, p. 337, t. xxxiv. ' 260 The Faculty of Speech. have some clinical independence. The cases of word deafness and tone deafness reported by Serieux 1 and by Dejerine ? may be taken as classical examples. In the case reported by the first mentioned there was total loss of the conception of spoken words. The patient remarked that she could hear the words very well, but that she did not understand them. The most familiar tunes when played on any instrument were not recognized. " Au Claire de la Lune" was said to be a "dead march." Cafe chantant music was designated church music, etc. Lichtheim 3 has reported a very instructive example of amusia. His patient was a teacher and journalist, who became completely word deaf after a second attack of apo- plexy. Communication with the patient could be made only in writing. He heard when one sang or whistled, but he did not recognize the melodies. Concert singing by his children was most annoying because it was " so noisy." The most familiar melodies, such as " Rufst du mein Vaterland," were not recognized. This case is worthy of remark, in so much as the pa- tient wrote facilely and correctly, and understood every- thing that he read. These features, I venture to think, stamp the case as one of subcortical sensory aphasia. A still more striking example of subcortical sensory aphasia with notal amusia is that reported by Pick, 4 in which there was loss of musical recognition with preserva- 1 Serieux : " Sur un cas de surdite verbale pure." Revue de Medecine, tfyi, P- 733- 4 Dejerine : " Ce'cite verbale." Memoires de la Societe de Biologic, February 27th, 1892. 3 Lichtheim : " Ueber Aphasie." Deutsches Archiv f. klin. Med., vol. xxxvi., 1885, p. 238. 4 Pick : Archiv f. Psychiatric, 1892, p. 910. Sensory Aphasia. 261 tion of musical expression associated with word deafness. All other disturbances of speech and writing could be ex- cluded. When the brain was examined it was found that it had its usual normal configuration, save that the convolu- tions were rather small. The patient had had a left-side hemiplegia, and changes were found in the right hemi- sphere to explain its existence. In the left hemisphere there was a subcortical softening of the posterior half of the first temporal convolution and of the medullary sub- stance of the adjacent supramarginal convolution, which accounted for the preservation of internal speech associ- ated with word deafness and loss of musical recognition. Much evidence might be cited to show that there is a definite representation of musical memories, viz., auditory perception of notes, accords, and melodies, in the first and second temporal lobes of the left hemisphere. Edgren' believes, after careful weighing of the facts bearing on this allocation, that it is immediately in front of the area for word memories. The indications are that it is a part functionally and anatomically of the auditory centre. Visual Aphasia Verbal Blindness Word Blindness. This is a form of aphasia in which there is loss of the significance of written or printed words, although the words themselves can be seen with the usual distinctness. The designation "word blindness" or "verbal blindness" to in- dicate the inability to recognize words and letters, and in- terpret what they stand for, is a very unhappy one, because the term blindness has here a very different significance 1 Edgren : "Amusie." Deutsche Zeitschrift f. Nervenheilkunde, vol. vi., 1895, p. i. 262 The Faculty of Speech. than that given it in every-day use. In the form of apha- sia which is described under the caption of verbal blind- ness the patient can see the word perfectly, but he gathers no meaning from it. The peripheral visual apparatus is intact. A printed page of a language previously entirely familiar to the patient suffering from this form of aphasia conveys no more meaning to him than does a page of Greek or Hebrew to the illiterate, or a page of Chinese symbols to him who reads only English, although he sees with the customary distinctness the letters printed or writ- ten, and he may even be able to tell the handwriting of one person from that of another. As in word deafness, in the literal interpretation of the term, the defect is not word blindness but loss of the significance of words. Words seen do not arouse a corresponding content of consciousness. Word blindness may be classified according to the de- gree of its completeness and according to the kind of con- crete written or printed symbols which we associate with ideas, such as algebraic symbols, musical notes, geometri- cal figures, hieroglyphics, etc., that the patient is unable to recognize. When the unmodified term word blindness is used, it is understood that other forms of printed and written symbols than letters and words are seen and in- terpreted, and that they call forth corresponding ideas. Many cases are on record in which a patient absolutely word blind was able to have roused in his consciousness certain ideas or thoughts leading to efforts of judgment by such printed or written, verbal or numeral notation. De- jerine has described a man with complete word blindness who was able to interpret the markings on goods in his Sensory Aphasia. 263 shop, and to tell customers the price. In other words, written letters having an entirely different significance than that ordinarily attached to them quickly called up a content of consciousness which he was able to associate with previously acquired knowledge. I have already mentioned that blindness for words may be met with as an isolated condition. Broadbent in 1872 was the first to note such occurrence, but it was not until Kussmaul's discussion of it that it began to be carefully studied. The visual area is in the posterior lobe of the brain. It is made up of two more or less distinct centres : a visual perceptive centre and a centre in which are stored the visual memory of words and other symbols. The for- mer is situated on the mesial surface 9!' the occipital lobes in the enviromental area of the calcarine fissure ; the latter (usually known as the visual centre) is in the posterior portion of the inferior parietal lobule, the angular gyms, and the adjacent margin of the supramarginal convolution which curves over the posterior extremity of the fissure of Sylvius. Destruction of this centre produces a form of sensory aphasia in which there are inability to put interpre- tation on words seen and consequent inability to read the condition known as word blindness, alexia, but it causes no loss of visual acuteness. It will be seen later on that the primary visual area and the higher visual centre are fre- quently diseased simultaneously, but the symptoms pro- duced by each can be differentiated. In these cases it is to be understood that no lesion exists in the peripheral visual apparatus, although the condition known as homony- mous hemianopsia, which will be referred to hereafter in some detail, oftentimes exists. 264 The Facility of Speech. If cases of uncomplicated word blindness existed, the study and interpretation of their symptomatology would be a very simple matter ; but such cases do not exist. There is almost always some association of motor aphasia, agraphia, and word deafness, and these coincident occur- rences make the interpretation more difficult. In addition to the fact that word blindness is thus complicated with other conditions, it is to be remembered that word blind- ness due to lesion of the zone of language is some- times associated with right homonymous hemianopsia or concentric limitation of the visual fields, on account of the juxtaposition of the optic radiations of Gratiolet to the an- gular gyrus on their way to the cortex around the calcarine fissure. Or, to express this anatomically, there is fre- quently some destruction of the optic projection fibres con- stituting the radiations of Gratiolet and those connecting the area in which visual memories are stored, the angular gyrus, with the primary visual centre in the left occipital lobe, which mirrors objects in the right visual fields of both eyes. Recent and trustworthy observations prove beyond cavil that destruction of the higher visual centre in the angular gyrus and supramarginal convolution does not cause hemianopsia, nor does it cause concentric con- traction of the visual fields. In every case, therefore, in which hemianopsia and concentric limitation are present, the lesion must involve either the primary visual centre in the cunei or that band of white fibres which is the back- ward prolongation of the optic tract connecting the exter- nal geniculate body, the anterior quadrigeminal bo/ly, and the thalamus with the primary visual centre, and known as the radiations of Gratiolet. I am at a loss to Sensory Aphasia. 265 understand why Brissaud, 1 while admitting that there are cases of cerebral hemianopsia without verbal blindness, denies that cases of verbal blindness, and by that I mean literal verbal blindness, may occur without hemianopsia facts laid down by Prevost and substantiated by the work of Dejerine and Serieux. These observers have put on record a number of cases which lead them to conclude that lesion of the visual centre, the angular gyrus, causes word blindness and agraphia, but does not cause hemianopsia if the lesion is limited to the gray matter, and that a lesion of the primary visual centre in the left occipital lobe alone causes right homonymous hemianopsia, but if limited to that the higher visual centre remains intact and the pa- tient is not word blind so long as the latter is in connec- tion with the primary visual centre in the other occipital lobe ; but if a lesion should cut across the connection be- tween both primary visual centres and the centre in the left angular gyrus, the patient is word blind but not agraphic. Before proceeding to a discussion of visual aphasia I wish to record the following case of true sensory aphasia in which visual aphasia was the leading feature. I am under obligations to Dr. Joseph Fraenkel for opportunity to study the patient. R. M , a right-handed woman ; native of Russia ; forty-five years old. She is married and has borne several children. There is no history of miscarriages or of syphilis. She has now what seems to be an atypical form of Basedow's disease, the only attributable cause of which is emotional shock. She complains of fever and pain 1 Brissaud : " Traitc de Mcdecine," vol. vi. 266 The Faculty of Speech. around the heart. Objectively tachycardia, exophthalmos, and enlargement of the thyroid gland, more pronounced on one side, are very evident. A year or more ago she was quite melancholy ; then she got very much better, and remained fairly well for a few months. The symptoms from which she now suffers came on very abruptly, about eight months before she came to the hospital, and followed an attack of pneumonia. Shortly before she came to the hospital she had a transitory attack of right-side hemiplegia ; the right side of the face and the right arm were particularly in- volved, and the right leg very slightly. Admitted to the hospital, June i6th, 1896; died, October 24th, 1896. The notes of the house physician previous to my first examina- tion are as follows : " Futile attempts to get a history, patient being aphasic, this condition having come on, it is said, on the way down from the ward to the office. She appears to understand when spoken to, but her answers show paraphasia and mo- tor aphasia. She cannot read ; does not understand written words ; laughs frequently ; is very active ; soils the bed ; has dyspnoea. " Examination : Weight, seventy-five and three-quarter pounds; pulse 148, arhythmical and irregular ; respiration, 42; temperature, 99.5 F. ; skin hot and dry. Patient fidgety ; slight bed sores ; thyroid gland enlarged ; has Basedow's disease. When asked to do simple things, such as 'Put out the tongue/ 'Give me your hand,' etc., she seems to understand, and does it quickly and hastily. Favors the left hand ; the right arm hangs down from the body. " July 2Oth, 1896 (one day later). Aphasia exists ; con- siderable psychical bewilderment ; her vocabulary, com- pared with that of yesterday, is diminished. There is well- marked motor paralysis of the right upper extremity, and Sensory Aphasia. 267 the forearm is kept in a semiflexed position. The knee- jerks are nearly equal on both sides, and they show the peculiarity of being increased with a flaccid condition of the lower extremities. There is distinct hemianaesthesia on the right side, but it is difficult to ascertain of what nature this is, on account of the jabbering character of the responses. Examination of the urine reveals the presence of albumin, the specific gravity being 1.028." Since she has been in the hospital she has had one or two recurrences of the hemiplegia, but like the other at- tacks they were transitory. The most striking feature of the case in looking at her is her intense restlessness and the rapidity with which every movement is performed. She is either continually moving and agitated, or talking strings of words that are wholly unintelligible. She gives the impression of a person who is on the verge of bursting into an attack of acute mania, but who still has some coh- trol of herself. On my first examination, I found the patient to be an emaciated, excited-looking woman, who, on account of the bulging of the eyes, the never-ceasing physical activity, and the verboseness, presented a strik- ing picture. At this time there was no trace of the pre- vious hemiplegic attacks, the last of which occurred only a few weeks before the examination. The patient was sitting up in bed ; she held continually a folded handker- chief or a towel against the face and mouth with the right hand, for what purpose I could not learn, but there was no drooling. Her speech possession is best indicated by the following stenographic report : What is your name? Tanes tanes tanes. (Then she smiles, looks distressed, turns abruptly and reaches for the card which hangs over her bed and on which her name, age, period of admission, etc., are written. This she holds out to her interlocutor and laughs.) 268 The Faculty oj Speech. How old are you? Sex sex vier vier fiinf fiinf (then, with the same rapidity as before, points to the card). Are you married ? Sure sure. Are you one hundred years old? Sure, sure. (It must be marked here that the lower class of Polish Jews give wider significance and usage to the word sure than does any other race, and it is necessary to bear this in mind in this patient, with whom the word " sure" seemed to be a recurring utterance.) How many children have you? Four. Sure, sure, sure. (All this with the greatest facial and bodily activ- ity and emotional display smiling and laughing.) What is the name of the first ? Sexel. What is the name of the second? Vier. What is the name of the third ? Fickel. (These are possible Hebrew words, but they have no appropriate- ness.) What is your husband's name ? Fickel. What is your mother's name? Finckel. Now, Mrs. M , quiet yourself (it should be said here that the patient is very verbose, continually emitting a string of words which have no sense or meaning), and tell me slowly all about your sickness. My husband my husband. He will say, explain. I can't. Sure, sure. Yesterday morning early (then she repeats a string of words of which it is impossible to make a report, which here and there can be recognized as elements of Hebrew jargon, but the larger number of them are not a constitu- ent of any language, nor have they any connection. Their production is accompanied by great motor activity, rest- lessness, gesticulation, and occasionally the word "sure.") (Holding up a spoon) What is that ? A book. Is it a spoon ? Yes. (Says it with pleasure.) Sensory Aphasia. 269 (Holding up a cup.) A cup. (It is probable that she heard the word cup.) (Holding up a watch.) A thing to look for the hour. (Holding up a match.) To rub. I understand all. (Holding up a pencil.) To write. (Pointing to some bread.) To eat. (After hearing the word " bread" uttered, she says it very quickly and with avidity.) (Holding up a key.) Pickle (German, Schliissel). (Then replies) What one opens with. (Holding up a knife.) (Making a motion as if to open it) To eat. Is it a knife? Sure (repeats jargon). She is given a handful of coins and asked to select all the five-cent pieces, but she is entirely unable to do so. She starts in as if she fully understood what she was requested to do, but she is unable to do what is asked. How much is there there? Twelve, six, four (and then she begins to laugh). (Shown a two-dollar bill) How much is that? Six, six, six, six. \Yill you please count for me? Six, six, seven, eight, one (then gets rather emotional). On being asked to repeat the line, " Aus tiefem Schlaf bin ich erwacht," she was unable to do so. She started with " aus" and then poured forth a jargon interspersed with " sure," all the time smiling, grimacing, gesticu- lating. Can you read ? Yes. (She is given the text of a very familiar Hebrew prayer, and asked to read it, but she cannot repeat a word correctly. She is unable to de- cipher either words or letters.) When an interpreter who is very familiar with her jar- gon asks her her name, she repeats, " Weitl, sceitl, heitl, 270 The Faculty of Speech. weitl." (It is thought that she is endeavoring to say " Rachel." All this time she is talking rapidly and ges- ticulating.) How old are you ? Six and four, and two, and sure, and sure. Are you married ? Sure, sure. How long? Sixty-four, sixty-five. I can't remember. Now try and count again for me ? four, six, seven, six, seven, five, six, eight, seven, seven, eight, eight, sixteen, forty-six, six and four and five and six. Can you repeat the letters of the alphabet ? She begins to say the same figures as above. Her people say that she was formerly able to read and write. She is now given the printed page of text that she should be familiar with if she had been able to read a word. She cons it very studiously and then begins, " Six and four and four and six," and so on in the most mixed-up fashion. She then takes an individual line and points the words out with the finger, and as she points to a word and makes some articulate sound she looks up at the physician inquiringly as if for corroboration. It is impossible to say from examination of the vision whether or not there is hemianopsia. It is oftentimes very difficult to say just how much she recognizes through the visual sense. I ask her to look at my watch and tell me what hour it is. She looks carefully and says it is nearly five (correct time, 4 130). Is it ten minutes after five? Yes. And sometimes on being given a number of coins and requested to match them she does it very accurately. It is absolutely impossible for her to repeat a sentence or a few connected words. On being handed a pen and asked to write she makes a show as if she were about to write (that is, she seems to recognize fully the use of the pen or pencil), but nothing Sensory Aphasia. 271 in the shape of writing results. There is complete agraphia. Of course it must be kept in mind that she has a very severe tremor; but this is not sufficient to prevent the formation of letters. She can copy, but in the most laborious and servile way. The efforts at copying figures and numbers are somewhat more successful, but she cannot call the numerals that she is trying to write. A tuning-fork, a watch, etc., held to the ear she heard, but she apparently did not associate them with any distinct source or sound. It is impossible to test her for possession of associative faculties, for aside from the fact that she does not indulge in foolish actions, there are no means of judging. In order to show the slight variation in her symptoms I append a protocol of an examination made some weeks later on the 24th of September : What is your name? Heitem, weitel, sure. Can't say it. (All the time she is talking some sort of jargon of which neither I nor any of the attendants can make out a syllable, and she gesticulates at the same time.) How old are you? (Apparently does not understand the question.) Question repeated, and she looks inquir- ingly at the nurse and at me, and then says, " Six and four and four and four," and then all at once as if she suddenly interpreted the question she points to her card. Are you married? Sure. How long? Six and four and four and six. I can't tell, I can't remember; sure. (Always after an apparent reply she goes on with considerable jargon, then smiles, laughs, looks around. She keeps the corner of her shawl, or a napkin, or whatever she may have in hand, up to the right side of the mouth.) How long have you been married ? Six and four, sixty- five, I can't say it. 272 The Faculty of Speech, She does not comprehend questions that are addressed to her in writing, and although she essays to read such questions the answers are not at all a propos. Interro- gated in this fashion she looks at the writing eagerly, quickly, and knowingly, but when she starts to answer it is always the same, " Six and four, weitel," etc., etc. If one smiles incredulously, he is pretty apt to hear " Sure," etc. Tests to make her repeat after her interlocutor resulted the same as determined previously. Sense of smell seems acute, but whether she detects the individual substance held to her nostril is not clear, for she cannot name it properly. She seems to know the use of things. Autopsy (Dr. Fraenkel) : Asymmetry of the skull, shown by bulging of the right parietal boss. Dura and sinuses normal. Pia of the convexity normal. The Sylvian vessels as well as their branches are normal. Along the sulcus of the right insula there are three yellowish-white papules on the surface of the cortex, each about three millimetres in diameter. These lie either in the pia or upon the ex- treme surface of the brain, or in both places. In the left hemisphere there is seen at the posterior portion of the inferior parietal lobule a soft pultaceous yellowish mass, which is slightly depressed beneath the surface of the brain cortex. This measured four centimetres in diame- ter. Where this softened region joins the surrounding brain cortex it is less yellow and more firm. The pulta- ceousness is most pronounced at the centre. It occupies the angular and supramarginal gyri, not completely effac- ing both, and has slight impingement on the superior temporal in its posterior portion (Fig. 12). The depth of this softened region is so great that it extends through and involves the outer portion of the posterior arm of the Sensory Aphasia. 2/3 internal capsule just as these fibres enter the basal ganglia. The occipital lobe is slightly encroached upon. The right hemisphere shows a recent softening of the posterior por- tion of the third frontal convolution. This grayish mass is about two centimetres in diameter. The areas of softening in the right side of the brain were of very recent date apparently, and they, like the FIG. i2.-Shaded Area Indicates Spot of Softening. one on the left side, can probably have their origin traced to emboli coming from the hypertrophic and thrombotic auricular appendix, and the chronic endocarditis, probable, moreover, that the lesion on the right side of the brain had nothing to do with determining either 1 aphasia or the paralysis, and the case is one of true sensory aphasia, the word blindness being the most pronnn feature. The comparatively slight degree of word dea. ness that the patient presented was dependent upon t impingement of the area of softening in the inferior pai tal lobule upon the posterior end of the superior temporal. 1 8 274 The Faculty of Speech. The total agraphia and alexia are of course "significant of destruction of the angular gyrus, and the paraphasia and jargonaphasia are likewise most interpretable in the light of the autopsical findings. Word blindness in its simplest form entails alexia, in- ability to read, or inability to get any information from written or printed symbols. Naturally there are various degrees of intensity of word blindness. The patient may be unable to read words, and yet retain the faculty of rec- ognizing letters; or, on the other hand, this may also be lost, constituting literal as well as verbal blindness ; or he may be able to recognize letters and unable to join them in syllables (asyllabia). For instance, Mierze- jewski ' has described a case as a form of caecitas sylla- baris et verbalis sed non literalis, and Badal, 2 in his mono- graph, mentions another case which was carefully studied by the author. The patient studied by Badal could read individual letters, but he could not combine them or keep them long enough in memory to form a word. This, it will be readily seen, is merely a difference in degree and not a difference in species. It has been noted in excep- tional instances that a patient who has verbal but not lit- eral blindness is able, if his auditory centre is intact, to have the meaning of the word made evident to him by spelling it out. For instance, though he cannot read the word " cat" he can read c-a-t and comprehend what the letters stand for. This conservation of the ability to read 1 Mierzejewski : " Ein Fall von Wortblindheit. " Neurologisches Cen- tralblatt, p. 750, 1890. 2 Badal : " Contribution a 1'etude des cecites psychiques ; alexie, agraphie, hemianopsie inferieure, trouble du sens de 1'espace." Arch. d'Ophthal., p. 97, 1888. Sensory Aphasia. 275 individual letters when the sight of words calls up in mind no corresponding ideas is not difficult of explana- tion. It is dependent upon the mode of education of the individual. Most children, and indeed all until recent years, learned to read, that is, learned to attach certain significance to printed and written words, by first learning to recognize the individual letters of the alphabet and to differentiate them one from another. They are not sup- posed to associate any significance with such acquisition. Recognition of the letters of the alphabet is attained as the result of prolonged and tedious effort. Later, the child joins a number 'of letters together to form words, which he may or may not have previously heard, but which have a visual and auditory individuality and which give rise to a distinct content of consciousness, and the visual and auditory memory of them leaves its impress upon the cortex of the angular gyrus and the first temporal con- volution of the left hemisphere in right-handed persons; of the right hemisphere in left-handed persons. The early acquisition of letters, and the primitiveness of their registration, explains the greater tenaciousness of their possession and the greater difficulty of their disintegration. At the present day, one of advanced pedagogic en- lightenment, children are no longer required to learn let- ters before syllables and words. It is probable that in a person thus educated there would be letter blindness co- incident with word blindness. A condition somewhat analogous to that of variation in intensity of word blind- ness is that in which a patient becomes unable to read the letters and words of one or more languages that he had previously been able to read and speak perfectly, 276 The Faculty of Speech. while still retaining the capacity to read other languages. Cases of this kind are by no means common, but a num - ber of them have been reported, particularly by Charcot and Pitres. It is not often that suc*h a condition is the only disability, object blindness and some degree of mind blindness are generally associated with it. In the case cited by Charcot, for example, there was loss of visual memory for form and for color, for objects and for places. The monuments, houses, landmarks, streets, etc., of the town in which the patient had lived for many years, and with which he had been very familiar, all seemed new to him. Moreover, he did not recognize the members of his own family ; they seemed to him like strangers. Some writers make the very grave error of confounding these cases of object blindness and mind blindness with cases of word blindness. In many cases of visual aphasia, or word blindness, the patient, although absolutely unable to recognize anything else, still tells his own name when he sees it written. But unless he has been accustomed to see it in print, he will not recognize it. The only explanation for this fact that can be offered is that the individual's name, because of his long experience in seeing it, in writing it, in hear- ing it, is more deeply printed on the specialized sensory area of the brain to which it has been carried by the hearing and seeing apparatuses. In such cases it is under- stood that the angular gyrus is not completely destroyed. Oftentimes the patient will preserve a recognition of a number of other words, particularly of names with which he has been for a long time familiar, such as those of the members of his family, his business, his place of resi- Sensory Aphasia. 277 dence, the church of which he is a member, etc., and for precisely the same reason that he recognizes his name. Occasionally cases are met with in which the verbal blindness is so very slight that it requires careful and persistent examination to reveal it. This is particu- larly true of cases in which the symptom of word blindness unfolds itself slowly, and of cases in which there has been a considerable degree of recovery. In such cases the patients may be able to recognize one or two words of a sentence, especially the substantives, and from them they gather the sense of the phrase. If the pa- tient is not an educated person, the examiner may be misled at first by the readiness with which the patient essays to read, but there is that about his actions and demeanor that will suggest to the experienced physician that the patient is guessing. He very rarely reads right along unhesitat- ingly, as one usually does. He watches the expression of the person for whom he is reading, and at the end of every sentence or line he looks up and asks, " Is that right?" heaves a sigh of relief if it is, and turns to again. It is a well-known fact that the acquisition of, and the memory for, figures and numerals are a different process than that for letters, and it is also known that the memory for the latter may be preserved and the former lost. In- deed, it is not so very rare for a patient to be word and letter blind but not figure blind. A very instructive case is one recently reported by Hinshelwood, in which a man although absolutely letter blind could read figures quickly and with the greatest readiness. 278 The Faculty of Speech. The term dyslexia is one that was first used by Berlin, 1 of Stuttgart, in 1886, to indicate a form of word blindness which differed materially from the ordinary forms. The first patient described by Berlin was a man sixty-six years old, who had been forced to abandon his occupation be- cause the reading of printed and written characters had become quite impossible. He was not word blind in the usual sense of the term. He could pick up a paper and read five or six words quite correctly and get full appreci- ation of their meaning, and then he would have to stop, as the words had no longer any meaning for him. After he had rested for a short time he could go on and read a few words more. There was precisely the same difficulty with letters of every size. On being asked why he could not read he gave no satisfactory explanation. The letters did not become dim or confused, he simply could not read them. Efforts to read were most obnoxious to him, so keenly alive was he to his infirmity. Later, the patient developed other, cerebral symptoms and eventually died from apoplexy. Berlin' looks upon the symptom as a special form of word blindness due to interruption in the conductivity of the connecting fibres of the visual centre, the angular gyms. It does not seem to me necessary to look upon the symptom as an exclusive " aphasia of con- duction," and I see no reason for not believing that it may be due to a partial impairment of the molecular function of the centre for visual memory, a condition that may re- sult from injurious agencies that pervert the function of 1 Berlin : " Weitere Beobachtungen tiber Dyslexic mit Sectionsbefund." Berliner klinische Wochenschrift, p. 522, 1886. 4 Berlin : Archiv f. Psychiatric, pp. 289-292, 1887. Sensory Aphasia. 279 this area without causing anatomical destruction of it. In such a condition the centre is capable of being aroused for the memorial recall of letters and words for a short time, then it becomes exhausted. After it has time to rest, a brief period of excitability follows. I am in- clined to this view of it particularly from the fact that the symptom is not accompanied by symptoms pointing to impaired functioning of the auditory centre, which would be the case if the disturbance was in the internuncial fibres connecting these two centres. This view would seem to receive further corroboration from the fact that the symptom has been observed a number of times in alcoholic patients, and it has disappeared when the use of the toxic agency was abandoned. This view is in har- mony with the genesis of alcoholic visual hallucinations which are so often of an extraordinarily vivid character, and with the occurrence of alcoholic retrograde amnesia. Hinshelwood ' has published a very instructive example of dyslexia due to alcohol. His patient was a tailor, forty- nine years old, who before his present trouble was a very capable workman. Latterly, on starting a piece of work, he forgot how to proceed, and every step had to be -pointed out to him as though he were a beginner. Even then he would make the absurdest mistakes, and after he had sewed parts together they had to be ripped. The greater part of his time in the shop was spent in looking for things that he would put out of his hand, such as the thimble, needle, glasses, etc., so that finally he had to be dismissed. A rather remarkable feature of the case was that he frequently lost his way in the most familiar 1 Hinshelwood : Lancet, December 2ist, 1895. 280 The Faculty of Speech. parts of the city, and he could find his way home only with the greatest difficulty, even over a route that he had travelled for years. The difficulty of reading was very similar to that described by Berlin. The patient could read a few words and then he would completely lose the capacity to proceed. The letters, though seen with dis- tinctness on first endeavors to read, would lose all mean- ing when he continued in his attempts to read. There was no blurring or running together of the letters. There was no disturbance of speech or deterioration of mental power, while memory for past events was not at all im- paired. The patient recovered under tonics. This case is one that admits of interpretation along the lines already suggested. The effects of the alcohol would seem to have been first and particularly upon the centre for the memorial recall of printed and written words, which were followed later by affection of those parts in which are stored the visual memory of objects, and finally, and to a comparably slight degree, upon the primary visual area which reflects the images of things. Hinshelwood con- tends that memories for form, color, etc., are stored up in both occipital convolutions, and the symptoms of his case would lend credence to this view. It would seem warrant- able to suppose, in this case at least, that the seat of the disturbance was the entire visual area, the primary cen- tres, and the centre for the visual memories of words. The integrity of speech and the fact that there was no loss of memory for past events show that the auditory word memories could be revived with their customary vividness and transmitted to the articuiatory kinaesthetic centre. It is to be regretted that careful tests were not made to de- Sensory Aphasia. 281 termine the patient's capacity to write, for the auditory centre, acting with its customary force, should send im- pulses, which have a marked influence in arousing the memory of graphic images, to the visual centre. If the angular gyrus is completely destroyed, the fac- ulty of writing is lost with it, or, perhaps I should say, if the power of visual memorial recall is lost, then writing is no longer possible. In those cases in which voluntary writing is preserved, the lesion, involves the primary visual centre, and, as this lesion is so often associated with right homonymous hemianopsia, the patient begins to write at the extreme left side of the sheet and stops in the middle of the page, unless he takes the trouble to inform himself by mediation of the tactual sense that there is still room on the line and pushes the sheet toward the left in order that it may be brought into the visual field. These pa- tients, being unable to read what they have written, are totally unconscious of any errors, of spelling or phrase- ology that they may make, although they may put the words on paper in as orderly a fashion as they were able to do before the development of the aphasia. If the auditory centre is uninjured, the patient is able to comprehend what is read to him, and if his own hand- writing is read he may be able to detect errors of se- quence, of diction, and of spelling, but he is unable to take a pen and correct them, because he does not recognize and appreciate the value of a word or a letter. Patients who have word blindness will occasionally take a paper, a book or written matter and essay to read aloud, but naturally what they produce has no connection with the subject matter before them. Why they do this is difficult 282 The Faculty of Speech. of explanation. In many instances it has been the reason for considering the person to be demented. Patients with word blindness are sometimes able to read written or printed words and sentences by tracing the word (which, it is to be remembered, they see with cus- tomary acuteness) with the end of the index finger or with a pencil. This is a most instructive phenomenon, and, although it is not often elicitable, it is by no means very rare and it should be studied carefully in every case. Such patients by utilizing kinaesthetic stimuli excite pre- vious kinaesthetic memories, which in turn react upon or act conjointly with auditory and articulatory memories to revive the mental concept of the word, the idea that it represents. In other words, the kinaesthetic apperceptive area acts vicariously for the visual, and if it acts more slowly and with less certainty it is because it is neither ontogenetically nor phylogenetically intended for that pur- pose, while the visual area is. This should not be con- strued, I think, as a revivification by "motor processes," as taught by Strieker and others. The idea of the word is revived by kinaesthetic stimuli which consist of sensory impressions coming from the muscles, joints, and skin of the hand and upper extremity. Patients who practice perseveringly this method of conveying to their minds the meaning of written symbols, often acquire great facility with this mode of interpretation. The principle involved does not differ materially from that by which the con- genitally or accidentally blind acquire ideas of form, of space, of size, of quality, and even of color, in which association becomes established between the auditory centre and various parts of the somaesthetic area. Sensory Aphasia. 283 The fact that such patients interpret by tracing written characters more readily than by tracing printed, indicates that muscular sense and the centre of graphic motor co-or- dination are the elements that mediate the connection of the word and the concept, for in every person the training of the hand has been to trace written and not printed sym- bols. These patients read the movements of which the letters are the tracing, the usual pathway between the word and the concept in the revivification of visual images being destroyed. In some cases there is complete inability to read printed or written characters, while the recognition of musical notes, of figures, and of various other expres- sive symbols is preserved. In other words, there is a dif- ferentiation of graphic symbols, just as we have seen a differentiation of auditory symbols, and the more highly differentiated these symbols are the more completely and easily are they lost. The denomination of money and its representative value may be quickly and thoroughly grasped; the patient may retain any skill which he may have possessed in playing cards and in giving perception to symbolic formula other than those represented by let- ters, all of which tends to show that the storage of written symbols is not the same as it is for other forms of nota- tion. It should not, however, be understood that the patient may not be blind to all forms of notation, graphic and symbolic representation. Thus, there may be sensory amusia, sensory asymbolia, sensory amimia, etc. The oc- currence of these will vary with the composition and char- acter of the patient's intellectual possessions. A musician who has been accustomed to carrying simple or most in- 284 The Faculty of Speech. tricate themes by means of musical notation may gaze on scales without their having any other signification for him than they have for a person who has never seen a page of music. He may also watch the movements of a leader's baton while they give no more orientation for the execu- tion of a piece of music which he may be able to play by revivification of auditory memories than would a person who had never heard of tempo. Musicians, by virtue of the acquisition of a unique method of eye externalization, are in possession of special- ized sensory images, visual and auditory, entirely divorced from the memory of letters, of words, and of figures, which probably posit for their possession different cells or asso- ciation tracts, and the one may fail to be revivified when the eye falls on it, as it appears on paper, while others are called up by gazing on their like. It is my desire to make as clear as possible that the patient with visual aphasia, word-blindness, sees with per- fect distinctness the letters in his visual field. This is done by virtue of the primary visual area in the occipital lobe, but the reflection of letters by the primary visual area does not convey anything to the intelligence, it does not call up any memory pictures, for the area in which they have been stored is destroyed. The primary visual area reflects letters and words as a mirror does an object held up before it, except that there is no reversal, and as reflec- tions by a mirror make no impression which remains after the object reflected is taken away, neither do words make any impression because of the impingement of their images on the primary visual area. Destruction of the primary visual area produces blindness in the literal sense Sensory Aphasia. 285 of the word; there is loss of perception of luminous ob- jects, virtually the same kind of blindness as when the retime are destroyed. When the primary visual area of FlG. 13. Course of the Optic Fibres. EGB, External Reniculate body; AQB, anterior qiuulrigeminal body ; P of 7\ pulvinar ; J'C, visual centre ; 11','C, half-vision centre. one side is destroyed, the blindness that follows is of half of each retinal field, and on account of the partial decussa- tion in the chiasm this blindness is homonymous, that is, of the same half. This visual defect is shown graphically 286 The Faculty of Speech. by the adjoining figure, which is based upon recent knowl- edge- of the course of the primary optic neuron. There are two or three subdivisions of visual aphasia, to which I shall refer. The first is a form in which there is loss of the perception of the word, not only of the visual impressions corresponding to the word, but a loss of the value of the symbol in arousing the idea of which it is the written representation. This form of sensory visual aphasia is entitled to the name loss of word visualization, verbal amnesia, or psychic blindness of words. Unlike word blindness, the patient interprets letters as letters and words as words. They are not simply marks on paper, and he can read them and copy them, but they convey no meaning to him after he has read them. When they are pronounced before him he hears and interprets them very readily ; but he has no idea that they are the same words that he has been reading or copying unless he is so in- formed. It has been suggested that psychic blindness of words is a phenomenon exactly analogous to that which occurs on reading a book when our attention is absorbed with something entirely apart from the subject matter be- fore us ; we read on, line after line, we reach the bottom of the page, and not until the necessity for turning the page arouses us do we find that although the page has been faithfully read not a word has entered into con- sciousness, nor can we cite a single fact that the writer attempted to convey. It is unnecessary to dwell upon the difference between this and letter blindness and word blindness. It is read- ily seen that the lesion which produces such a condition must be one that interferes with the pathway that conveys Sensory Aphasia. 287 the sensation from the printed word or object to the idea, or to where the idea is formed ; and we are warranted in saying that such interference is nearer the seat of con- sciousness, wherever that may be, than it is to the seat of memory images of words. A second subdivision is that in which the patient on looking at an object, which he has previously seen and used is unable to call up its name, although he is in condition to utter the name if he could call it up. This is the condition to which the name optic aphasia has been given by Freund. The striking symptom is the inability to name things. This inability is not difficult of interpre- tation. It depends upon an interruption of the pathways that unite the seat of cortical visual representation and the seat of cortical auditory memories. It is necessary in order to enunciate a name to influence articulatory images through the auditory centre ; in other words, the impulse that starts the externalization of a name travels along the intercentral (or internuncial) auditory-articulatory pathway, and if auditory images are properly revived, and there be no dis- turbance of the articulatory kinaesthetic area, the name will be uttered. If, however, something prevents the im- pulse from passing through the visual apparatus by which one gets a visual concept of the object and thus from reacting on the seat of auditory speech images, then calling objects by their names, when such are presented to the peripheral organ of the visual apparatus, will be entirely impossible. On the other hand, if the object is of such a nature that it can appeal directly to the audi- tory apparatus, the auditory mechanism being intact, the name may still be produced. One or two simple examples 288 The Faculty of Speech. will suffice to show this clearly. If a photograph be held before an individual and he is asked to designate it by name, an impulse travels from the percipient ocular ap- paratus to the primary visual centres and to the higher visual centre, and the impression thus produced will excite the auditory centre, to which the articulatory kinaesthetic area is subservient, and the result will be the enuncia- tion of the word "photograph." On the other hand, if the visual mechanism is destroyed, and the photograph is held to the ear, the patient will not be able to name it, although the auditory mechanism is intact. The photo- graph has no qualities that appeal to the ear. If, how- ever, the object used be an apple, a patient with loss of visual memories, but with auditory memories intact, may still be able to name the word '' apple" if the object be pared and sliced within his hearing. The occurrence of this condition, with its sharply denned symptomatology, has led certain writers to allocate special portions of the speech area as a "concept centre" and a "naming centre." Broadbent was the first to advance this view, in a paper published in 1872.' In his own language: "There is a primary or rudimentary perceptive act in which the exter- nal cause of a given set of sensations is recognized as such, and in which the simple attributes, as of form, color, hardness, etc., are perceived. And there is a higher de- gree of elaboration in which, by the combination or fusion of perceptions derived from the various organs of sense, a conception or an idea of an object as a whole is obtained. This is a new and distinct process, and is usually accom- panied by the affixing of a name to the object. The higher 1 Loc. cit. Sensory Aphasia. 289 elaborations and the fusion of various perceptions together, and the evolution of an idea out of them, will be accom- panied not by radiation from one perceptive centre to all the others, but by convergence of impressions from the various perceptive centres upon a common intermediate cell area, in which a process analogous to the translation of an impression into a sensation and of a sensation into a primary perception will form a part of the supreme centre, and will be situated in the superadded convolutions which receive no radiating fibres." In the further elabo- ration of this theory, Broadbent postulated a " proposition- izing" centre, in which words other than substantives were registered and in which words were arranged orderly pre- paratory to being uttered. It seems to me unnecessary to point out how completely at variance this is with the conception of speech that I have attempted to portray in another chapter, and how contrary it is to the idea that the various speech centres have an integral part in the conception of words, the sym- bols of thought. The existence of such a centre is, I be- lieve, contrary to the conception of the psychogenesis of speech. The reasoning on which its existence is posited is fallacious, and the cases that have been cited to estab- lish its autonomy are made to .support it only by putting unwarrantable interpretation upon accompanying symp- toms. If such a centre existed, it is reasonable to suppose that cases of aphasia would have been described before this in which the symptoms pointed to the destruction of this area, especially considering the autonomic activity that its sponsors give it. I have been unable to find any such cases ; that is, no case in which the speech disturbance 290 The Faculty of Speech. could not be explained by a lesion of one of the speech centres or of its afferent and efferent pathways. The case cited by Mills 1 in support of this contention is reported in such an incomplete and fragmentary fashion that I am astonished that one of his clinical astuteness and erudi- tion should consent to offer it in evidence. The follow- ing is a n'sum^ of his case : A woman, forty-five years old, complained of numbness in the back of the head and neck, vertigo, and vomiting. Soon after this it was noticed that she did things differently from her usual custom. She hung upside down a certifi- cate of membership in a society without realizing her mis- take. She had to forego her occupation, that of dress- making, because she no longer got the parts together properly, etc. In othenwords, she developed optic apJiasia. (If she had been tested there would have been found, in all probability, dyslexia or alexia, but no information is given concerning the reception, interpretation, or emis- sion of speech.) About three years after these symptoms were first noticed, she had an epileptic fit, and after this forgetfulness of words became very evident. Examina- tion showed left lateral homonymous hemianopsia, word blindness, but not letter blindness, inability to name ob- jects, it mattered not through what receptive avenue she got information concerning them, although she knew their uses. I infer there was Considerable agraphia, although nothing 'is said about it, for a fac-simile of the patient's signature shows the last name unfinished. There was slight paraphasia in spontaneous and repeated speech, but the former soon became limited to the use of " Yes" and " No, " which she used properly. About ten weeks before death there developed slowly a right-side hemiplegia. On 1 Mills : Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, p. i, 1895, Sensory Aphasia. 291 autopsy, which is most meagrely reported, there was found, on cutting into the temporal lobe, a hard tumor, yellowish- brown in color. Its hardest and apparently oldest part was in the middle of the third temporal gyrus, but a firm, nodulated, more or less hemorrhagic condition extended backward as far as the white matter of the occipital lobe and forward toward the anterior ends of the second and third temporal convolutions. It was thought that the dis- ease started in the third temporal convolution, at a point in line with the posterior extremity of the horizontal branch of the fissure of Sylvius. I find it difficult to convince myself that the gradually developing optic aphasia and word blindness in this case were due to anything else than a gradually progressive lesion which encroached upon the projection tracts of the visual fibres, the optic radiations, which lesion was mani- fest by such infallible evidence as homonymous hemianop- sia, and which later severed the connections between the area of visual and auditory speech memories, the invoca- tion of which by the former is necessary before concrete words can be used. (Mills reports that the patient had left homonymous hemianopsia, which I take it refers to the retina, and not to the visual field; in other words, blindness of the right side of the fields.) The patient showed optic aphasia by her inability to recognize that a certificate was hung upside down, and by her inability to go on with dressmaking. This sympt in all probability, coincided with disturbance between the cortical area for visual reflection in the cuncus and the area in which visual memories are stored up in the angu- lar gyrus. That the interruption became more profound 2cj2 The Faculty of Speech. and extensive is shown by the development of word blind- ness and homonymous hemianopsia ; that it never became absolute is shown by her continued ability to recognize letters; that her sensory speech area became gradually destroyed is shown by the fact that she became almost, if not absolutely, unproductive of voluntary speech. The area of articulatory images was cut off from the centres that excite it to activity and control, and the result was almost the same as if there had been no articulatory kinaesthetic centre a locomotive with steam but without a driver. Moreover, the patient was not wholly agraphic, although she wrote with great difficulty. Now, if there is any one fact substantially proven in sensory aphasia, it is that the cases of aphasia in which the patient is word blind, but still not .agraphic, are dependent upon a lesion in the white matter of the occipital lobe which severs the fibres connecting the higher visual centre with both of the primary visual centres in the two hemispheres. The lesions found on post-mortem examination in this case show, so far as they indicate anything, that they severed the connection of the auditory centre with the visual cen- tre, and the connections of the latter with the primary visual areas. In addition to the kind of visual aphasia in which there is loss of memory for written and printed letters, and to which the name verbal amnesia or psychic blindness of words has been given, there is a form of less common but more striking occurrence, known as psychic blindness or mind blindness, the " Seelenblindheit" of the Germans, '' Cecite psychique" of the French, a condition not infre- quently associated with the ordinary form of word blind- Sensory Aphasia. 293 ness and letter blindness. In this condition the patient not only does not recognize the significance of letters, but he loses the power to differentiate between familiar ob- jects or persons and to distinguish the use of things. He looks at a member of his own family and sees, apparently, a stranger. He does not recognize the house in which he has lived for years, neither from the outside nor from within, and frequently asks when lying in his own room when he is to be taken home. Such patients do not rec- ognize a washbasin from a drinking-glass, and drink as readily from one as from another. They have no more conception of the use of a fork or of a spoon than has an aborigine. The condition known as apraxia, the inability to com- prehend the usage of ordinary objects and things to which one had been accustomed, is analogous to this. Its oc- currence and misinterpretation in former times were often responsible for getting the unfortunate possessor into an asylum. This is not at all surprising when we consider that a very similar symptom often occurs during the very early stages of general paralysis. A patient with the latter disease, now under my care, first showed symptoms of mental illness by persistent and repeated refusals to enter his own house, insisting that the one in which he lived looked entirely different. Even before any other symptoms of mental disorganization were noted, he showed himself quite incapable of finding his house or the street in which it was located, or of recognizing it when brought to it. A similar condition is often seen during the oc- currence of very severe illness, such conditions being looked upon as the visual hallucinations of delirium. 294 The Faculty of SpcecJi. The nature of the condition is probably an abolition of the visual memories of objects, which memories have been stored up in the higher visual area, a condition analogous to that of word blindness. The patient who has this condition may see the object; that is, he sees it objec- tively, but he does not see it subjectively. When we are as yet small children, we have no conception of the uses of a great many things that impinge themselves on our visual apparatus, which afterward become very familiar to us. In the acquisition of this familiarity there is stored up in the higher visual centre not only an image of the object itself, but with it a memory picture of its use. This latter may be a combined kinaesthetic-auditory-visual one, the latter being for most objects by far the most important one. When the visual image is lost, the mem- ory picture cannot be rehabilitated. As Wyllie has re- marked, the imprintation or storage of the images of objects does not require such close attention as the im- printation or storage of words, and the images of objects and things are stored in both hemispheres of the brain. This would account in a measure for the infrequent occur- rence of this condition even in cases in which the sensory speech area is very largely destroyed. It would also lend itself to the interpretation of the occurrence of this symp- tom in the early stages of general paresis, in which the lesion is a widespread degeneration of the cortex. CHAPTER Ml. (Continued-]. SUBCORTICAL SENSORY APHASIA. THE subcortical forms of sensory aphasia, pure sensory aphasia of Dejerine, are analogous to the subcortical forms of motor aphasia. In discussing subcortical motor aphasia, it was said that the symptom complex attending that con- dition was the result of a lesion that prevented the idea, properly and completely formed, from being externalized in a word or words. In similar fashion the lesion of sub- cortical sensory aphasia is one that interferes with the passage of the spoken and written word to the idea of the word or to where the idea is formed, it being understood that the structures by whose functioning the idea of the word is formed are intact. The real components of sensory aphasia are visual aphasia and auditory aphasia, and it follows that the visual cortical area and the auditory cortical area are the parts by virtue of whose activity one gets a visual and auditory idea of words. Therefore, there may be a subcortical in- terruption of the visual and auditory pathways which causes a subcortical visual aphasia and a subcortical audi- tory aphasia. I shall not attempt to discuss these in extcnso here, as I have already referred to them in dis- cussing the true forms of sensory aphasia. The symptoms of subcortical or/;/;r visual aphasia will be readily understood if it be borne in mind that the vi- 296 The Faculty of Speech. sual centre itself is intact and ready to functionate and that it only awaits the impulses inciting it to function which proceed from the half-vision centres. In subcortical visual aphasia connection with the primary visual centres is severed, and consequently the higher visual centre, although retentive of its anatomical integrity, is perverted in its physiological ability. The connection of the visual centre with the other speech centres is not disturbed, ex- cept in so far as the latter are not properly and customarily influenced on account of the fact that the visual centre it- self does not itself receive customary stimuli. This ac- counts for the occasional occurrence of shortcomings of speech (slight paraphasia, such as noted by Bramwell), which might otherwise seem paradoxical. The symptoms of subcortical visual aphasia vary some- what with the seat of the lesion, i.e., with its proximity to the left angular gyrus. Generally speaking, they are verbal blindness, always associated with right lateral homony- mous hemianopsia, as the lesion is either of the primary visual area, in the cortex of the occipital lobe bordering on the calcarine fissure, or of the optic radiations connect- ing the occipital lobes with the left angular gyrus, the higher visual centre. The patient looks at printed and at written letters and sees only black marks on a white sur- face ; he is wholly unable to interpret them, because the visual impulse is prevented from reaching the left angular gyrus, in which such impulses are interpreted by compar- ing them with the residua of other impulses the signifi- cance of which have been registered in consciousness and which are known as memory pictures. The fact that the centre in which are stored such visual memory images is Subcortical Sensory -Aphasia. 297 intact, and the images are therefore preserved, can be shown in a variety of ways. In the first place, they can be re- vived through kinaesthetic stimuli. If the patient has been long habituated to writing, it may be possible for him to interpret the significance of written letters and words by tracing them with the finger end. Printed sym- bols remain unintelligible to him, because he has not been accustomed to tracing printed characters, and he has no kinaesthetic memories and association tracts for such im- pulses which can proceed to the angular gyrus. It is not always possible even in one accustomed to writing to re- vivify visual images in this way. In the second place, patients with subcortical visual aphasia retain the capacity to spell. Spelling consists, in most persons, of the memo- rial recall of auditory images which are sent to the other two speech centres, the articulatory and the visual. As these centres are intact, capacity to spell is undisturbed. Patients with subcortical visual aphasia are able to copy, but they delineate without deviation from the copy before them, and copy print in print, script in script. These are the prominent symptoms. Spontaneous speech, except occasionally the slight paraphasia already referred to, and capacity to repeat are intact. The pa- tient is able to write voluntarily and from dictation, but he cannot read what he has written any more than he can read what some one else has written, except in those in- stances in which the sense can be gathered from tracing each letter with the finger tip. The patient understands readily what is said to him and can reply intelligently and correctly. Subcortical visual aphasia may or may not be accom- 298 The Faculty of SpeecJi. panied by a degree of optic aphasia manifested by inability to name objects. Whether or not these patients have any difficulty in spelling needs further investigation. In all probability they do not, save in rare instances. Bramwell has recorded an example of this form of aphasia in which the faculty of spelling was preserved to such a remarkable degree that the patient became the speller for the family. As an example of subcortical verbal blindness which is every way typical I shall cite the following instance studied in my clinic at the Post-Graduate Medical School and later in the City Hospital. 1 The patient is a male, fifty-eight years old, by occupa- tion an artisan, whose life has been one of uniformity. Although he has not had syphilis, rheumatism, or gout, his blood-vessels show well-marked arterial sclerotic change. His present infirmity dates back five months. He had been complaining somewhat of not feeling well, of headaches, and of some vertigo, when without other warning he became unconscious and lay in a semi-uncon- scious, semi-delirious condition for about three weeks. There was no evidence of hemiplegia. He recovered his mental balance after three weeks, but has not since that time been able to return to his occupation. His wife says that this incapacity is the result of bodily weakness, forgetfulness, and because he does not remark anything. He comes to the clinic on account of a stupid feeling in the head, and because he is unable to work. Exam- ination shows that voluntary speech is without notice- 1 I regard the case as particularly valuable because of the corroboration which the examination of the brain, thus far made, gives to the statements made from clinical data alone. The patient died six months after the monograph had left the author's hand, and, as will be seen on further peru- sal, the autopsy fully bears out the diagnosis. Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. 299 able defects. He talks in a somewhat more confiding way and with a little more emphasis on words than one usually does, and occasionally he misplaces a word. His wife again comes to our aid and says that he misuses words at home, although I must say that I have not been able to remark any considerable paraphasic speech dis- turbance, although if he is not understood quickly he seems to get impatient and with a gesture significant of impotence he turns to his wife. When his wife is asked to cite specific examples she says there are many, but she has difficulty in recalling one. Finally, she says that if she sent him to a shop for soap he would be quite likely to bring back flour. This surely cannot be cited as a paraphasic manifestation. In -repeating sentences there is occasionally sight misplacement of words, but no more, I think, than would be expected in one of his intellectual attainments. Pantomime and mimicry he does not in- dulge in. He understands what is said to him, although he is not quite so alert as formerly. He says that he has to be attentive to what is said to him, and that if the sen- tence is very long he loses the connection. Examination of the eyes shows a complete lateral homonymous hemi- anopsia, which is shown by the accompanying chart. The patient is unable to read, that is, there is complete word blindness. Occasionally he can make out a letter here and there, but there is almost complete letter blindness. He sees the letters very distinctly and is able to tell how many letters there are in a word, but he is totally unable to mark off a word into syllables or to take a number of detached letters and construct a word or syllable out of them. There is no object blindness. He recognizes things and calls them by their right names. Voluntary writing is preserved, but, with the exception of his name and address and a few words, he is not productive. After ;oo 77/6' Faculty of Speech. writing a few words, he foregoes further efforts with some despairing remark. He writes badly to dictation after the first few words, the writing from dictation showing only two striking features : first, he is apparently unable to retain in mind a sentence sufficiently long to write it ; and, second, he stops the line before he gets to the right side of the page. Writing from copy is very defective ; what he copies he does laboriously and servilely, the let- FIG. 14. Complete Lateral Homonymous Hemianopsia. ters being an exact reproduction of the copy. Script is copied as script and print as print. There is no optic aphasia or mind blindness. The patient is quite as intel- ligent as are most people of his age and social position ; he is able to compute mentally and to spell, and is in fair possession of his associative faculties. In the examination of his eyes, it was interesting to note that when a lighted candle was brought into the right visual field the light of the candle was not seen until after it had passed the median line, although the patient detected at once that the atmosphere was brighter as soon Snbcortical Sensory Aphasia. 301 as the candle was brought into the space of a normal visual field. In other words, as soon as the illumination struck the retina he said, " It's brighter," but he did not see the light. A specimen of his voluntary writing is reproduced here. It is an attempt on his part to tell me in writing ' FIG. 15. Specimen of Voluntary Writing. how his sickness came on to tell me all about it. The words have no significance. So far as they can be made out they are : " Dare durg [the last letter is really nothing] akenbeit ist es gekommen ist mochte lesen, ist kann nicht. Die Krankheit ist," etc. When I ask him to read what he has written he begins pointing to each word in turn " Ich mochte gern lesen, ich mochte gern lesen" all this slowly and with some emphasis. On being asked to spell the individual letters entering into the formation of each word he says, pointing to the first, second, third, and so 302 The Faculty of Speech. on, " That is e, that is also e, that is e, that is also e," and so on, until he seems to become tired. I repeated slowly the first lines of Schiller's poem, " The Casting of the Bell" : " Fest gemauert in der Erden Steht die Form aus Lehm gebrannt ; Heute soil die Glocke werden," etc. and asked him to write them. Although this was re- peated line by line and he could say it himself quite well, the following is a fac-simile of what he produced when he endeavored to write it from dictation (Fig. 16) : The letters are formed, and some of the words are real words ; there is no sense in them or any suggestion of the original. He does not give the slightest heed to com- mands addressed him in writing, such as, "Put out the tongue," although he responds quickly when told to do so in spoken words. In this case the symptoms are, briefly, word blindness, alexia, right lateral homonymous hemianopsia, inability to copy, and defective writing from dictation. Spontaneous speech and repeated speech are not disturbed, nor is there noticeable defect in the interpretation of spoken words. These symptoms and possessions point to a lesion between the angular gyrus and the half -vision centres in the occi- pital lobes, situated in the posterior end of the left hemi- sphere. The fact that he does not write facilely either spontaneously or from dictation, leads me to believe that the lesion is situated close to the angular gyrus and is perhaps encroaching upon it. The lesion, which was originally either a hemorrhage or a thrombosis, must have so completely implicated the white matter of the occipital lobe that it cut across the- fibres Siibcortical Sensor \ . \plictsia. 303 passing from both occipital lobes to the left angular gyrus, involving the optic radiations of Gratiolet. The fact tu FIG. 16. Specime 304 The Faculty of Speech. that there are word blindness, left hemianopsia without agraphia, but no object blindness, shows that the lesion cannot be either in the primary visual centre or in the higher visual centre. For if it were in the primary visual centre there would be object blindness; the lesion to pro- duce hemianopsia would not cause word blindness, because in such a case the higher centre would still be in connec- tion with the primary occipital area of the other side. The lesion is not in the higher visual centre, because if it were there would necessarily be agraphia and pro- nounced disturbance of internal language, while in reality neither of these exists. The general mental infirmity is no greater than would be expected from an intracranial lesion so grave as this must be, and in a patient who pre- sents the striking manifestations of vascular degeneration that this one does. So much for the clinical deductions. The patient was admitted to the writer's wards in the City Hospital, where repeated examinations showed no material depart- ures from the above-stated findings. Although appar- ently a docile, tractable individual, it was found that he had but slight control of his temper, and when aroused, oftentimes even when not excited, he indulged in profane and vituperative language, which was directed against his attendants and those about him. His ability to create internal language and to externalize it in spoken words was fully evidenced by numerous letters sent to his family which were dictated to a fellow-patient. His health was considered to be in fairly satisfactory condition until November 8th, 1897. On the afternoon of that date while sitting on a bench in the garden, he fell over suddenly Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. 305 and had a more or less generalized convulsion accompanied by frothing at the mouth. He was got into bed imme- diately and seen a few minutes later by the house physi- cian, who reported that the patient did not utter a word or apparently recognize any one after the attack, but grad- ually sank into unconsciousness, and meanwhile a right- side hemiplegia developed. The pupils were uneven, owing to contraction of the left pupil ; the face was flushed and showed a right-side palsy ; the axillary temperature was 98.2 F. and alike on both sides ; the respirations were ster- torous, and 32 per minute; the heart was working labori- ously, while the pulse, beating 84 times a minute, was very hard and firm. Both knee jerks were absent. The uncon- sciousness deepened, the patient vomited copiously, and soon the left side of the body ceased to indulge the continu- ous movements that were first noticed. Two hours after the apoplexy he became quadriplegic, and died two hours later, about four hours after the onset of the symptoms. An autopsical examination was made a few hours after death, and, aside from the changes in the brain about to be described, there was no noticeable abnormality save an advanced degree of arterio-sclerosis, most noticeable in the heart and large blood-vessels and in the kidneys. The skull is of the customary thickness. The veins and ctfploe are well filled. The dura is easily detached and not thickened, and the brain in situ looks normal. The pia is smooth and glistening and its vascular arborizations are very distinct. Even before the brain is removed from the calvarium, but more conspicuously after removal, a most striking abnormality is seen at the posterior pole of the left hemisphere. The entire occipital lobe looks reddish- The Faculty of Speech. yellow, is extremely soft to the touch; in fact, the lobe is replaced by a cystic formation, all save the posterior extremity, where there is a slight cortical mantle, evidently not yet implicated. When the fluid of the cyst escapes the cortex of the occipital lobe sinks in, and the superior surface of the left cerebel- lar hemisphere juts prom- inently into view. The same yellowish-red color and the same pultaceous- ness to the touch are ap- on the internal surface of the brain, where the destruction of tis- sue involves nearly FIG. 17. Shaded Area Shows Extent of Lesion on n <-u v 1 Mesial Surface. a11 the "DgUal gy- rus and the cu- neus, except a very small thin surface at the posterior pole of the latter, which seems to have retained a fairly normal appearance to the naked eye. In short, the cystic formation involves the whole pos- terior part of the left hemisphere, save the very apex of the cuneus and particularly the inner surface, being limited on the mesial surface (Fig. 17) anteriorly by the pajieto- occipital fissure, and on the external surface by the pro- longations of the same fissure. The gyri involved are the first, second, and third occipital, the cuneus and lingual gyrus. The remainder of the left hemisphere is apparently normal to the view and to the touch. The accompanying illustrations, made at the time of the autopsy, show the Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. 307 location and the extent of the lesion. It will be seen that the destruction of tissue extends anteriorly as far as the posterior limits of the inferior parietal lobule, the pre- cuneus, and the angular gyrus, but spares these absolutely. The temporal lobes are likewise quite unimplicated. The depth of the lesion, originally a hemorrhage which had undergone cystic transformation, was almost through FIG. 18. Shaded Area Represents Seat of Lesion. the entire thickness of the white matter and into the roof of the ventricle, so that the optic radiations were completely cut across. When the isthmus of the encephalon was separated by a horizontal cut of the pons anteriorly to the apparent origin of the trigeminal nerve, the cross-section showed a distention of the aqueduct with sanious fluid. When the two hemispheres were separated both lateral ventricles were found filled with clots, the left much dis- tended, the foramen of Monro and the middle ventricle also distended, the primary fatal hemorrhage apparently 308 The Faculty of Speech. being in the left lateral ventricle. The hemispheres were then cut according to the method of Flechsig and the brain was placed in Miiller's fluid for hardening prepara- tory to further study. This observation needs no further remark, save to say that the symptoms which had been present were so accu- rately substantiated by the post-mortem findings so far determined that it amounts almost to the exactness of a mathematical demonstration. Subcortical word deafness, or pure word deafness of Dejerine, is characterized especially by inability to un- derstand spoken words, and naturally by inability to write from dictation, because the spoken word of another cannot get to the part of the brain in which the idea of the word is interpreted. It is of much less frequent occurrence than subcortical visual aphasia. The feature that distin- guishes it from cortical auditory aphasia is the fact that spontaneous speech is preserved, there is no amnesia verbalis, the patient is able to read aloud, to write volun- tarily, to copy, and to read understandingly what he and others have written. As in every other form of subcorti- cal aphasia, aphasia in which the zone of language itself is not diseased, every constituent of internal language is intact. It is rarely if ever associated with paralysis of the extremities. A classical example of this form of aphasia is that cited by Lichtheim. 1 A journalist had an attack of apoplexy which was accompanied by pronounced sensory aphasia, from which he made a partial recovery. Five years later he had a second attack and again recovered, but this time 1 Loc. at. Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. 309 more completely than before, so that now there was no difficulty in speaking, in reading, or in writing. He went on with his work as a journalist. He was, however, absolutely word and tone deaf, although hearing remained quite undiminished. It has been mentioned casually in another connection that recently Bleuler, 1 Freund, and other writers have con- tended that the customary conception of s.ubcortical audi- tory aphasia is entirely too narrow, confined to too limited an area, and that it should be enlarged to include disease of the extracerebral neural structures whose functioning conditions audition ; at least, that disease of the peri- pheral auditory neuron, including the termination of the nerve in the organ of Corti, thus taking in labyrinthine diseases, should be included. Freund, 2 in a recent monograph on this subject, cites the following case, in which bilateral disease of the laby- rinth following epidemic cerebro- spinal meningitis caused a form of word deafness : A youth, twenty-two years old, by occupation a watch- maker, developed after prolonged physical effort, followed by rapid chilling from lying on the cold, damp earth, symptoms that led to the diagnosis of epidemic cerebro- spinal meningitis. The disease was of the severe type, and the patient did not convalesce until the sixth week. It was then noticed that, in addition to vertigo and pro- found disturbance of equilibration, hearing was very much impaired. Soon after this he was treated in the otological clinic of Professor Gruber for deafmutism. 'Bleuler: " Zur Auffassung der subcorticalen Aphasieen." logisches Centralblatt, 1892, No. 18. 2 C. S. Freund : " Labyrinthtaubheit und Sprachtaubheit." ^ 1895. 310 The Faculty of Speech. An examination made five months after the beginning of the illness showed : Loquaciousness ; speech rasping and of harsh intonation ; no paraphasia ; no optic aphasia. The patient appeared with a pad and pencil for the use of his questioner, as he claimed to be wholly unable to hear spoken words. There was no disturbance of mo- tion, sensation, or vision. There were slight vertigo and difficulty of maintaining equilibrium. Otoscopic exami- nation of the ears did not reveal any departure from the normal. Examination of the sense of hearing showed ab- solute deafness on the left side, while on the right side a few vowels, such as a, e, could be heard when they were shouted into the ear. Aerial conduction of notes of a tuning-fork was entirely unperceived. Bone conduction of the same was unperceived, although the patient was sensible of the mechanical vibration. There was no dyslexia or agraphia. His speech was interfered with only in so far as understanding of spoken words was entirely lost, and naturally there was inability to repeat spoken words and to write from dictation. All acoustic impressions impinging on the right ear were, when not too low, perceived by the patient, but they were not properly interpreted. In order to determine accurately what degree of com- prehension he had for articulatory sounds, the patient was subjected to a painstaking examination. The vow- els and consonants were shouted separately in the right car, and the patient was apprised in writing how to indi- cate when he had heard them, what he had heard, and made to understand fully the test that was to be made. The examination showed that the patient, who was ap- parently completely speech deaf, possessed the capacity to grasp during the examination a few words correctly and others very nearly correctly. As he expressed it, " I hear Subcortical Sensory Aphasia. 311 only the tone of the words, and I then must think what they can be." Perception of rhythm was well preserved, and he had good conception of the number of syllables in the uttered word and the intonation that was given to them. When asked to differentiate between the words "December" and "June," he did so with readiness, al- though he was often unable to differentiate between words that sound somewhat alike, such as December and Sep- tember, or June and July. The patient's intact, spontaneous speech confirmed the opinion that he was in possession of his auditory word images. Examination of his music - perception ability showed that he did not hear musical sounds so clearly as before his illness ; for instance, he was unable to tune a violin, but sounds coming from the street, the beating of horses' hoofs, the lumbering roll of heavy wagons, the sprightly sound from light vehicles, the jingle of the tram-car bells were heard and differentiated. He could differentiate whistling, the snapping of fingers, the cluck- ing of the tongue, the clapping of hands, etc., and he very seldom erred in naming the source of such sounds. He could likewise indicate the origin of tones from a violin, a trumpet, a piano, a mouth harmonica, etc. En- tire tones he differentiated with greater readiness than half-tones, and for high tones there was striking impair- ment. In response to the question whether he could hear him- self talk, he remarked that he could hear his own words but not clearly and distinctly, and the more trouble he had in hearing them the louder he spoke. When eating he always heard ( ?) the grinding noise of the teeth. The absolute deafness in one ear, the diminished bone conduction and loss of perception for high tones in the other, the normal condition on examination with the oto- 3 1 2 The Faculty of Speech. scope excluded absolutely middle-ear disease and bespoke a disease of the auditory percipient neural mechanism, due to bilateral labyrinth disease following epidemic cerebro- spinal meningitis. I have quoted this case in detail because it may be said that no unsurmountable objection can be raised to the admission that word deafness similar clinically to that found in subcortical sensory aphasia was present. The only question is whether one is willing to admit that the peripheral auditory neuron can be normal for the conduc- tion of ordinary sounds and diseased for the conduction of sounds having highly differentiated significance. Person- ally I see no objection to entering such cases as this in the category of subcortical sensory aphasia. CHAPTER VII. TOTAL APHASIA. OCCASIONALLY cases of aphasia are encountered in which there is a disturbance of all forms of intellectual expres- sion, involving disturbance in the reception of stimuli that condition mental states preparatory to speech, and dis- turbance in the emission of such mental states. To such cases the name total aphasia is given because it includes the phenomena of both motor and sensory aphasia. If the location and the relationships of the speech cen- tres be kept in mind, the existence of such a condition will not be at all surprising. The speech area is dependent for its blood supply, and therefore for its functional integ- rity, upon the artery of the fissure of Sylvius, and a lesion of this artery that interferes with the circulation is apt to show its malign consequences in every part dependent upon the artery for nutrition. In individual cases the area supplied by one of its branches may be more pro- foundly affected than another. Thus in cases of total aphasia we sometimes see one or more components of lan- guage less wholly submerged than others, and, on the other hand, cases are seen in which the aphasia in the be- ginning is total, yet after a time the symptom complex becomes so modified that one of the modes of language may be partly recovered. 314 The Faculty of Speech. As an example of total aphasia, 1 may cite the follow- ing instance : F. C , thirty-eight years old, a native of Italy, by occupation a bank clerk. He is married and the father of two unhealthy children. His wife has had two miscar- riages. There is no way of determining whether he has had syphilis. He has been a steady drinker for many years. After he had suffered from a condition which the family physician called influenza and which kept him in bed for about two weeks, he became very dizzy, and ex- perienced the sensation of falling while passing from one room to another. Soon after it was noticed that the right upper extremity was very unwieldy and that the right side of the face was somewhat drawn, and that he rapidly became unable to articulate. In fact, save for complaint of a severe pain shooting through the head im- mediately after the occurrence, of the vertiginous spell, he did not utter a word. His wife says that there was not the slightest loss of consciousness, but that the patient was " out of his head" up until a few days before I first saw him, which was six weeks after the onset of his sick- ness. The slight hemiplegia from which he had suffered disappeared in a few days. His wife, furthermore, states that he has not uttered a word that could be understood ; that he has not understood what was said to him ; and that he has not essayed to read since the beginning of his ill- ness. She says that he is in his right mind, that he does nothing foolish, except to watch her or any other person who may be in the room with embarrassing persistency. Examination shows that voluntary speech is wholly lost, and that he makes not the slightest effort to re- peat when he is instructed to. He never indicates wishes, desires, or other mental states by pantomime, and his wife tells us that he has not done so since the begin- f | Total Aphasia. 3*5 ning of the illness. When he is given a pencil and asked to write he makes a few up-and-down strokes, but frames no letters or words. On being requested to take off his coat he looks intently at the one who gives the command, watches his lips most carefully, and grunts (nothing ap- proaching articulation), " Sih, sih." After the request is repeated a number of times, each succeeding time in a louder voice, it would seem that he gets some idea of what is wanted, for he grasps the lapel of the coat, but makes no further effort to remove it. When I suggest my request by aid of pantomime by throwing off my own coat and then pointing to his, he obeys. To the command, "Put out your tongue," a number of times repeated, he closes the eyes. When he is asked to take the doctor's hand, he does not apparently have any conception of what is desired. Under further questioning and commands, he becomes very restless, gives vent to inarticulate sounds, moves the lips as if counting or mumbling, twists the fingers, and then starts to go out of the room as if he wished to go home. To test for visual perception he was shown the following request, first in writing and then in print: "Take out your watch and show me the time." No response. On the contrary, after adjusting his glasses, he reaches forward, takes the pencil from my hand, and begins to copy the words in script. On endeavor- ing to interpret this action on the part of the patient, I learned that some days previously he had been given a letter addressed to his employer which had been prepared by his brother and which he was requested to copy. This letter set forth that the patient was not very ill, was not out of his head as it had been reported to his employer, and to show that his mental faculties were unimpaired, and that his position should be kept open to him, this letter written by his own hand was offered in evidence. The 316 The Faculty of Speech. deception, however, did not work, as the concocters of it had never been able to get the patient to do the part allotted to him. He could copy, however, and he copied script in script and print in print. To the written ques- tion, "How old are you?" first in printed and then in written characters, he takes a pencil and points to the individual words, at the same time making this inarticu- late " Sih, sih, sih," and then he starts to copy it, without, however, giving the slightest intimation that he gets the purport of the question. It is impossible to say positively that there is no hemianopsia, but probably there is not, as thrusting the finger abruptly into the visual field causes prompt blinking. There is no optic aphasia nor is there mind blindness. The patient fully recognizes the use of things. On being handed a watch, he opens it, looks at the time, compares it with the clock on the mantle, and returns it. On being given a tuning-fork, he sets it in vibration and holds it to the ear. He feeds himself, dresses himself, and goes out alone, and in a general way deports himself as does a person in the possession of his faculties. Repeated examinations showed no considerable depar- ture from this condition for some time, but after a num- ber of weeks he became able to articulate " Yes" and " No," to write his name, that of his father, and of his wife, and to give evidence that he understood some things that were said to him, particularly if they were repeated a number of times. Voluntary speech, repetition of speech, still remained wholly submerged. Six weeks after the first examination, on being asked to put out the tongue, he did so ; but when he was asked to go into the next room he deported himself as one unaware of any request. Still, from this time onward, it was noted that he took greater heed of commands and requests directed to him Total Aphasia. 317 through the hearing, and a general improvement of the word deafness followed. On being asked to read a simple sentence, he takes the pencil and points to each individual word, accompanying the indications with the utterance of the sounds eh, fa, eh, fa, eh, la, fe, eh, la, fe, and contin- ues to do this throughout an entire page, pointing to each word. If the sentence embodies a simple request, he takes no notice of it. Frequently, after going through a sentence in this way he looks up at the examiner inquir- ingly, then shakes his head, assumes a look of distress, draws a line under the word, and goes on. He is now able to copy fairly well, but copies line for line, word for word, as in a drawing. In the same way he is able to copy simple figures and designs. He is totally unable to write from dictation or from written instructions. If he is given a pencil and left alone, he proceeds to write his name, then the surname of his father, and then that of his wife. On being asked where he lives, he writes his father's name ; to a second request, he writes that of his wife. If the question is asked him in writing, he proceeds to copy the question. He is able to add up a column of figures, but the results are not usually correct ; in fact, they are more often incorrect. He begins at the top of a column, points to the figures, and often says, " Feh, lah, feh, feh, lah," giving a variable intonation to each one of these utter- ances, then scratches his head, looks disturbed, goes again to the top of the column, and begins all over. His wife says that he is able to play cards, and that it tickles him very much to win. He apparently recognizes the numerals on banknotes, and he can tell time. He never essays to read a paper, and on being given a letter from his parents commiserating with him in his illness he takes no inter- est in the matter. To recapitulate briefly, this man has (i) loss of spontaneous speech; (2) inability to repeat 318 The Faculty of Speech. after dictation ; (3) inability to indicate desires or feelings by means of mimicry; (4) inability to write spontaneously (except latterly he has regained the ability to write his own name and that of his wife and of his father) ; (5) in- ability to write from dictation ; (6) letter blindness, word blindness, alexia ; (7) partial (in the beginning complete) word deafness he neither understands nor obeys spoken requests. He is able to copy, but copies exactly the words or letters that are put before him. He can interpret re- quests and commands made to him by pantomime ; he can play cards and other games ; he recognizes the use of things and the places and relations of objects; he can look at a watch and transfer to paper by means of figures the hour indicated ; there is no trace of hemiplegia, unless it be a somewhat lessened activity in the muscles of the right side of the face. This case shows, therefore, that there was disturbance of all the modes by which one person communicates with another, both receptively and emissively. The only in- terpretation to put upon it is that after the tenancy of an acute infectious disease a thrombosis formed in the left Sylvian artery, which robbed the speech area of the blood necessary for its functioning. That this pathological process did not cause a serious disturbance of other func- tions of the brain is shown by the fact that there was no loss of consciousness. That it did not involve the pyramidal projection is shown by the very slight and transitory hemiplegia which left no sequelae, as organic hemiplegia invariably does. That there was some involve- ment of the motor cortex, however, is shown by the slight paresis of the right side of the face and the right arm, which, it would seem, was dependent upon disturbance of Total Aphasia. 319 function of the ascending frontal convolution adjacent to the frontal operculum. After the thrombosis or whatever lesion may have existed was partly removed, or at least after the circulatory disturbance which it produced was in part compensated for, some of the speech centres regained in a very slight way their functions, and this was particu- larly true of the auditory centre, for it was the word deaf- ness more than anything else that showed improvement. To what extent improvement will still go on cannot be prophesied. A very important and instructive case of complex aphasia has recently been reported (although not yet in detailed form) by Bastian. 1 A right-handed man had an attack of right hemiplegia with loss of speech three months before coming under observation. Eighteen years after the first apoplectic attack a thrombosis of the right middle cerebral artery caused death. When the patient was first seen he was incompletely paralyzed on the right side, and there was also incomplete hemianaesthesia on the same side. For six years after the first apoplectic stroke he had slight convulsive attacks from time to time. There was then an interval of twelve years, but during the last year of his life he had three severe fits, each of which was followed by a temporary aggravation of the paralysis of the right side. Within two months of his coming under ob- servation his speech defects assumed the form which they maintained during the next eighteen years with remarka- ble constancy. The striking features of the man's condi- tion were as follows : i. Voluntary speech limited to a few words. 1 Bastian: Lancet, vol. i., 1897. 320 The Faculty of Speccli. 2. Could repeat words that he heard. 3. Understood everything that was said to him. 4. Inability to read aloud. (The reporter says that he spent much of his time in reading, and undoubtedly un- derstood what he read, yet many patients with sensory aphasia spend much of their time in reading, without comprehending a word. ) 5. Inability to write a word from dictation. 6. Inability to name objects at sight. As no mention is made of hemianopsia it is to be inferred that it was not present. The autopsy showed a complete atrophy of the convolu- tions in the territory supplied by the left middle cerebral artery. The atrophy had extended inward so as to lay open the lateral ventricle, and the whole of this region was occupied by a large pseudo-cyst. The supramarginal and angular gyri, as well as the posterior two-thirds of the upper temporal convolution, were included in the parts that had completely disappeared. The case is regarded by the author as " a very remarka- ble one ; first, because of the complete destruction of the supramarginal and angular gyri and the posterior two- thirds of the upper temporal convolution without the oc- currence of word blindness or word deafness." This would be very remarkable indeed if it were so, and would revolu- tionize in a measure the teachings not alone of aphasia but of cerebral localization. " It is presumed," says the author in attempting to explain these remarkable anom- alies, " that the atrophy of these convolutions must have occurred at some unknown period during the patient's ill- ness ; that it must have occurred gradually, and that func- Total Aphasia, 321 tional compensation must also have been gradually brought about through the further development of the correspond- ing centres in the opposite hemisphere." The constancy of the speech defects during a long scries of years and the very exact way in which the original defects were main- tained after some of the functional activity of the lost parts had been transferred to the opposite hemisphere, the writer asserts, are worthy of attention. Appreciating the great disadvantage that one is at in endeavoring to put interpretation upon a case from a very scant report of the leading symptoms, I hesitate to do more than to mention this case, which I believe will become an important one in the annals of aphasia when it is prop- erly published (so far it has only been reported to a medi- cal society). Nevertheless, it seems that a very different interpretation can be put upon some of the conditions than that mentioned by Bastian. In the first place, he says there was no word blindness, but the fact that the patient had inability to write from dictation (although he fully un- derstood words), the fact that he could not read aloud, and the fact that he could not name objects at sight seem to me to indicate that he had loss of the visual images of words. If he could repeat words that he heard, there is no other lesion that will explain inability to read aloud except loss of the visual memories of words and naturally the inability to invoke such memories from seeing a word. The fact that he could utter words voluntarily and that he could repeat words after they were said to him shows that the auditory images were capable of being aroused, and that they in turn were able to react upon and evoke the articulatory images; otherwise the patient would not have 322 The Faculty of Speech, been able to say a word. But the most convincing feature in leading us to the assumption that the patient had loss of visual memories that are stored up in the angular gyrus of the. left side is his total inability to write. Dejerine has shown that spontaneous writing is the result of arous- ing the visual images here stored and the copying of them by the motor apparatus that holds the pen, while writing from dictation is accomplished in exactly the same way, save that the visual images are evoked in this instance through the auditory. Furthermore, this patient retained the ability to copy, a possession which shows that the visual centre that reflects words, viz., the visual centre in the occipital lobe around the calcarine fissure, was intact ; the patient saw the letter, the word, or the design, and copied just what he saw. The fact that he was unable to name objects at sight, although he saw them, recognized their uses, etc., is explained in exactly the same way inability of the patient to resuscitate the visual memory of the name. Although willing to grant that the uneducated right hemisphere became in eighteen years somewhat edu- cated and functioned vicariously for the part of the speech area that was destroyed on the left side, I am not inclined to believe that this vicarious functioning played a part of such paramount importance in the status of the patient's speech as Bastian would have us believe that it did. It seems to me the interpretation to put upon this case is as follows : The original apoplectic attack was due to a thrombosis of the artery of the fissure of Sylvius, the direct continuation of the internal carotid. The imme- diate result was a partial hemiplegia of a cortical nature, due to the deprivation of blood from the cortical branches Total Aphasia. 323 of the middle cerebral artery going to the Rolandic region ; a partial hemiansesthesia, which is to be explained by the fact that the middle cerebral artery supplies in part the posterior limb of the internal capsule. The thrombus in the Sylvian artery caused defective activity or complete overthrow of functions in all the speech centres, and until I hear to the contrary I am prepared to believe that in this patient, before he came under Bastian's care, there were some word deafness, latent or manifest, loss of capacity to read mentally, and much more extensive aphemia than there was three months after the accident. After the substance occluding the calibre of the vessel was removed, or partly removed, some of the speech centres (let us say the audi- tory, for the temporal lobes are less dependent upon the integrity of the middle cerebral than is the parietal), the area in which are stored auditory images, nearly recovered itself, and there were no gross accompaniments of word deafness, but the loss of visual memories remained in part to the end as did the loss of articulatory images. The successive attacks of thrombosis expended themselves in causing softening and pseudo-cystic formation in the area from which blood had been deprived by the original thrombus. Thus it seems to me that this is a good example of complex aphasia, in which the symptoms, although per- haps very nearly indicative of complete aphasia, or total aphasia, clear up in one or more modalities of speech as time elapses. CHAPTER VIII. DIAGNOSIS OF APHASIA. To unravel the intricacies of aphasia is at no time an easy task, but it can be made immeasurably more difficult than it is in reality by approaching the examination of a case, and the analysis of the findings, in an improper or unmethodical way. Therefore, the first step in attempt- ing the diagnosis is a simple method of eliciting and asso- ciating the different symptomatic constituents. One who takes up the examination of a patient with aphasia should keep in mind that which has been said pre- viously concerning the constitution of the speech faculty that it consists of two parts, the receptive and the emis- sive, and that either of these two parts may manifest the predominance of the aphasic symptoms, but that in true aphasia, that is, aphasia dependent upon lesion of the speech centres, neither can be the sole medium of mani- festation of the speech defects. It should further be re- membered that emissive speech is manifest by articula- tion, by writing, and by pantomime, and that integrity of the receptive side of language is commensurate with the interpretation of visual and auditory stimuli. It should not be forgotten that the attitude, the de- meanor, the conduct, of the patient may be of the greatest service in orienting the physician, from the very begin- ning of the examination. Though recognition and inter- Diagnosis of Apkasia. 325 pretation of information thus obtained is usually consid- ered aquale of clinical insight, it is one of the cultivatable forms of this desirable qualification. The demeanor and expression of one with auditory aphasia are frequently those of a person who has lost all interest in his surroundings, and his attitude is that of a deaf person who is slightly de- mented. The same is true, though to a lesser degree, for the patient with visual aphasia. But the latter is more fre- quently of the restless, active kind, such as the case on page 265, who was so continuously moving and shifting that she gave the impression of one on the verge of acute mania. Moreover, patients who have this form of aphasia are often garrulous, and on the slightest provocation, or without provocation, emit a string of articulate or gibberish sounds that convey no meaning to those about them. This is es- pecially true of cases of not very protracted duration. Patients with cortical motor aphasia and with subcortical motor aphasia, on the other hand, present a very different aspect. They are often absolutely silent but watchful, and the intensity with which they hold every move of the persons surrounding them is often very striking. This is well shown in the case of John Masterson (Case No. 2). His wife repeatedly said that the feature of his progres- sive recovery that impressed itself on her memory more than anything else was the assiduity with which he watched her every movement. This intentness of obser- vation is particularly to be marked in cases of subcortical motor aphasia in which the patient is absolutely speech- less yet capable of the fullest understanding of all that goes on about him and within his hearing and vision. If these facts be kept in mind, the difficulties encoun- 326 The Faculty of SpeecJi. tered by the beginner in examining a patient with aphasia will be reduced and the chances of reaching warrantable and legitimate conclusions as the result of the examination will be greatly increased. Before a case can be recorded in such a way that the symptomatic findings can be used legitimately in establishing, or demonstrating, certain claims, it is necessary that the examination be conducted in a manner that searches every centre which must func- tionate for the production of normal speech as well as their connections. To do this it should be systematic, but it need not be done in conformation to any special formula or according to any hard-and-fast system. A number of schemes have been devised to facilitate the ex- amination of aphasic patients, but I have found the follow- ing simple plan most serviceable : After securing a gen- eral history of the patient's life and of his previous illness from some member of the family, and in this way getting information of the character of the disease of which the aphasia is a symptom, the patient's ability to express ideas, to receive and interpret information should be in- quired into. The mental processes apart from the mani- festation of mental states and the capacity for the re- ception of sensory stimuli should then be examined. Although a number of these may be determined simulta- neously, it is best to take each one separately. In approaching a patient with aphasia it is natural that the endeavor be made to elicit information by speaking to him. It becomes necessary, therefore, to determine if the patient takes note of what is said to him orally, and, secondly, if he understands what is said. In other words, does spoken speech awaken in his auditory centre corre- Diagnosis of Aphasia. 327 spending memories ? This can be done ordinarily by ask- ing some simple question, such as, " How long have you been sick?" or by addressing to him some simple com- mand, such as, "Give me your hand." Care must be taken not to employ too conventional questions or com- mands, such as, "What is your name?" "Put out the tongue," etc. The patient may have lost the auditory apperceptive faculty and still, oftentimes, make reasonable reply to such questions, merely from association or habit. Naturally the patient should get no information of what is being asked . through any other avenues than those of hearing. Such patients are quick to grasp, particularly if they have been aphasic for some time, the significance of even slight emotional expression or pantomime on the part of the interlocutor. If the patient does not reply to such questions or commands, there may be trouble with the receptive or with the emissive speech faculties. If he is word deaf, that is, if the trouble is one that prevents the sound of the word from reaching the centre in which the memories of previous word sounds are stored up, the patient will not endeavor to respond byword or act, though in some instances he does so. Nor will the face show the slightest response or indication of comprehension. If he does respond the diagnostic feature is that his answer, even though it be made up of articulate words, has no per- tinency or bearing on the question. If the patient is not word deaf, he will make some movement, be it of the head, hand, or features, to indicate that though he understands he cannot reply. Generally this gesture is very signifi- cant. It consists of a despairing expression of the coun- tenance and a touching of the lips or the throat with the 328 The Faculty of Speech. fingers. Oftentimes the question can be decided very quickly, if there remains some doubt even yet, by asking some absurd or ludicrous question, and noting how the patient receives it. If, in reply to the question, " Are you one hundred years old?" he solemnly says, "Yes," or if he does not see the ludicrousness of a request to turn a somersault when he is so obviously paralyzed, it is rather convincing proof that such speeches do not awaken the proper responses in his mind ; and if there be no demen- tia it is suggestive evidence that the patient is word deaf, and the examination should then proceed from that stand- point. Although other of the speech centres may be simultaneously disorganized, the symptoms attributable to the first one will dominate the character of the speech defect. If the examination so far seems to suggest the existence of word deafness as the leading feature of the sensory aphasia, it should then be determined to what degree of completeness this exists, and the extent and kind of disturbance that it causes in the externalization of language. The amount of diminution of the patient's vocabulary, the degree of inappropriate usage of words, the imperfections of sequence and rhythm, should all be noted. The patient should be tested for his power of recognition of simple words, short sentences, and long sentences. It has already been noted that, perhaps, he may react to conventional questions, such as, " Put out the tongue," etc. Uncommon requests, such as, " Touch the nose with the tip of the index finger," or, " Stand on the chair," should be made. The ability of the patient to interpret sounds should then be noted. Do sounds evoke previous memories of similar sounds and do they incite Diagnosis of Aphasia. 329 the auditory centre to revive the name of the object from which such sounds proceed ? When a bell is sounded, or a watch is held behind the ear and apart from the stimula- tion of any perceptual avenue other than hearing, can the patient say, "Bell" or "Watch"? Finally, the existence of any disturbance of bone or aerial conductivity should be demonstrated or excluded. If word deafness can be excluded, and the patient still makes no reply, that is, if he remains completely speech- less, the examination should be to determine whether or not internal language is defective, for it must be readily seen that the question has then narrowed itself to a deter- mination of whether or not the aphasia is cortical motor (kinaesthetic word-image) aphasia, or whether it is sub- cortical motor aphasia. In other words, is the inability to speak due to a lesion of the storehouse of kinaesthetic memories of articulated words, Broca's area, or is it due to a lesion of the neurons that conduct the motor word impulses from the Rolandic area to the parts that exter- nalize the word ? The essential thing then is to determine if the patient is in the full possession of internal lan- guage! If internal language in any of its components is disordered, then the patient has true cortical motor aphasia. If, on the other hand, there be no such disturbance, the lesion is elsewhere than in the zone of language. In some patients the differentiation will be an easy one. They will show, as did Case No. I, that they have the proper idea of words and that they can evocate them promptly, by the ease and rapidity with which they write, or by the exquisiteness of pantomime, as did the patient just referred to. On the other hand, however, the task is 330 The Faculty of Speech. oftentimes an extremely difficult one. It is particularly so because the test to determine if the legitimate idea of words can be evoked in the internal language, the test of Proust and of Lichtheim, is not one of universal application, be- cause in the first place many of our hospital and dispensary patients are not of sufficient scholarship to know anything of syllables, or of counting the number of letters forming them, and word construction is an exercise that has never been indulged in. In the second place, there is very often associated with aphasia, and a concomitant of the disease giving rise to the latter, a degree of deficiency in the associative faculties that amounts to a slight degree of dementia. In such patients it is oftentimes extremely difficult to make them understand just what is meant by telling them to press the physician's hand as many times as there are syllables in the word Constantinople, or some other equally resonant and polysyllabic word. Nor is the substitute suggested by Dejerine, of asking the patient to make voluntary expiratory efforts as many times as there are syllables or letters in a word more applicable. But even when we cannot get the patient to respond to these tests, there is a general atmosphere about the patient with subcortical motor aphasia that one cannot be long in with- out recognizing that the patient is in full possession of his intellect and internal speech. The only shortcoming of the subcortical motor aphasic is inability to articulate. He understands everything that is said to him ; he inter- prets information received through the visual sphere; he is capable of expressing his thoughts fully, facilely, and correctly, by writing and by pantomime, or, at least, he would be were it not that the right half of the body is Diagnosis of Aphasia. 331 usually paralyzed and he is obliged to portray mental states by the pantomimic activity of the left, the less dextrous half of the body. Physicians oftentimes find some difficulty in properly assigning cases of cortical motor (articulatory kinaesthetic) aphasia, because the patient is still able to articulate some words. I have often been made aware of this by con- versation with my house physicians, who work apparently with the following formula : " If the patient can think of the word and is unable to say it, he has motor aphasia; but if he cannot think of the word, though he is able to say it, then he has sensory aphasia. " If one had to choose between this formula, and nothing at all, it might be well to choose the formula, although it is only half the truth. If it be kept in mind that the patient with cortical motor aphasia (articulatory kinaesthetic) need not be absolutely deprived of the power to articulate words ; that he fre- quently retains the ability to say one or several words, which he uses at all times and under all conditions, perti- nent and impertinent alike ; and that frequently these words take the form of recurring utterances ; that there is always agraphia, which may be very evident or which may be difficult to bring out because the patient pleads paraly- sis of the right hand as an excuse for not making an effort to write ; that the agraphia is usually proportionate to the aphasia ; that it is manifest in voluntary writing and in writing from dictation, but not in writing from copy ; and that the patient in copying copies print in script and script in script, showing that the copying is not a mechani- cal but an intellectual act ; and that there is defective in- ternal speech, as shown by the test of Proust and Licht- 332 The Faculty of Speec/i. heim then the diagnosis of articulatory kinaesthetic aphasia will not be a difficult matter. After voluntary speech has been satisfactorily exam- ined, tests should be made to determine the patient's capacity to repeat. There is inability to repeat in both sensory and motor aphasia, and if word deafness has been excluded there will be no difficulty in interpreting this in- ability which is co-existent with loss of voluntary speech in articulatory kinaesthetic aphasia. Particular attention should be given, in every case of aphasia in which the symptoms point to destruction of Broca's area, to the faculty of writing. Following De- jerine, it has been maintained, and I venture to hope con- sistently, that lesion of this area causes agraphia. Re- cently Bastian has reiterated the statement that the agraphia that sometimes accompanies articulatory kinaes- thetic aphasia is not dependent upon lesion in Broca's area which prevents the patient from getting the correct notion of the word. He contends that it is an occasional phenomenon only, and when it occurs it is due (like alexia) to temporary or more or less permanent disable- ment of the visual centre. I readily admit that in some instances this does occur, but this in no way invalidates the explanation of the occurrence of agraphia, which, I believe, occurred in every case of cortical motor aphasia that has been given. The fact that such contradictory beliefs are held as to the occurrence of agraphia with motor aphasia demands the very careful examination of such cases in the future. - Of course, when a patient who has had articulatory kin- aesthetic aphasia has partially recovered and has regained Diagnosis of Aphasia. 333 quite an extensive vocabulary, it will require care and re- peated examinations satisfactorily to establish the diag- nosis. The one suggestion that I have to make in such cases is that there will always be found some degree of every one of the symptoms enumerated as occurring with this form of aphasia, if sufficiently careful and patient search be made for them, and if the physician is trained to recognize these slight defects they are of great service in orienting him. After having tested the patient's capacity to perceive and interpret words through the auditory apparatus, he should be examined with the view of determining if there is any disability of acquiring and interpreting information through the visual apparatus. To do this requires patience and circumspection. In the first place it should be estab- lished that the patient has no trouble with the peripheral visual apparatus. This can be done by an ophthalmo- scopic examination. Tests should then be made to deter- mine the existence of hemianopsia. This is not an easy matter to do if the patient is aphemic or if he has word deafness ; in fact, it is extremely difficult to do satisfac- torily. With a patient who can understand what is said to him and who can indicate when he perceives the en- trance of an object into the visual field, who can tell when the indicator of a perimeter passes beyond the range of vision, testing for hemianopsia is a very simple matter. If the patient is word deaf and if he has visual blindness, which of course he is apt to have if he has hemianopsia, one finds himself unable to convey to the patient by writ- ten or spoken word that which he wishes him to do or to observe. In such cases one must content himself with the 334 The Faculty of Speech. information that is to be derived from forcibly and sud- denly thrusting some object into the visual fields, from the right side (for right-handed patients invariably have right lateral homonymous hemianopsia when they have any), and taking note whether or not the patient blinks, as he should do if the object be perceived. If he does not it is rather certain that he has hemianopsia. Each eye should be examined separately and the findings noted on a chart. The form of hemianopsia that may be found will be read- ily interpreted if one has clearly in mind the course of the optic nerve and the tract to the cortex (see Fig. 6). A destructive lesion of the cortex in the vicinity of one cal- carine fissure, or of all the optic fibres leading to it, the radiations of Gratiolet, produces blindness on the opposite sides of the visual field. If the left one is destroyed there will be right hemianopsia, and as the hemianopsia is on relatively the same sides of the visual field, that is the right temporal and the right nasal, the hemianopsia is called homonymous. If the right cuneus is destroyed just the same condition will prevail, only it will be mani- fested on the other side ; and if both cunei are destroyed there will be true cortical blindness. If these facts be kept in mind there can be no difficulty in properly inter- preting the occurrence of hemianopsia. If there is de- struction of the cortex in the vicinity of the calcarine fissure, that is, destruction of a half-vision centre, there will be lateral homonymous hemianopsia, but not word blindness, as the higher visual centre is still in intact connection with the half- vision centre of the other cuneus. If there is lesion of the bands of Gratiolet connecting both half -vision centres with the primary visual centre and one Diagnosis of Aphasia. 335 cuneus with homonymous parts of both visual fields, then there will be hemianopsia and word blindness, complete alexia, merely because the patient cannot send impulses coming in from both half -vision centres to the angular gyrus where they are interpreted, although the latter is intact and shows this intactness by the undefectiveness of inter- nal language and the ability to write spontaneously and from dictation. Such a patient copies mechanically, print in print, script in script, as one traces a drawing. This, and one other fragment of knowledge concerning hemi- anopsia, are all that is necessary properly to interpret its occurrence. Sometimes a lesion that destroys the angular gyrus extends sufficiently deep to sever the optic radia- tions of Gratiolet, which are immediately subjacent on their way to the occipital lobes; in such cases there will be true word blindness with all its entailment of disturb- ance of internal language, disturbance of intellection, agraphia, etc., plus hemianopsia, but the latter symptom is merely an accident, a superadded phenomenon, and it never occurs with destruction of the angular gyrus, except in some such way as I have indicated. In testing the patient to determine the integrity of the visual mechanism one may begin by showing him familiar objects. If he does not recognize them, or show by act or deed that he comprehends their use or purpose, if he looks upon them as does one who sees them for the first time, then he has object aphasia and the lesion is of the occipital cortex. Such an individual may obtain informa- tion through the medium of other special senses, such as the tactile, gustatory, etc., that will enable him to recog- nize the object, the person, or the thing. If he is shown 336 The Faculty of Speech. familiar objects and he recognizes them, knows what they are for, but cannot name them, then he may have either an interruption in the pathway leading to the higher visual centre in the angular gyrus, or there may be lesion of the angular gyrus itself. If it be of the former, inter- nal language* will be preserved and spontaneous speech may be intact, although there is usually some paraphasia and possibly jargonaphasia, and this preservation is shown most conclusively by the retention of ability to write. He may write easily and moderately well, not only volun- tarily but from dictation, but the patient is unable to read what he writes. If it be of the latter and complete, the patient will be absolutely agraphic. This agraphia is to be considered a part of the disorder of internal lan- guage, loss of the visual image, the visual idea of the word. There is inability to arouse the visual image of the word. In such a case, as arousal must precede the transmission to the part of the Rolandic cortex that in- nervates the member holding the pen, there is complete agraphia. There is no more strikingly illustrative case of this on record than one communicated to the Royal Medi- cal and Chirurgical Society in 1872, which was one of the first, if not the first, in which subcortical word blindness was the striking symptom. Although this case was used by Broadbent then, and is still to-day, to bear evidence in favor of a naming centre, it must be apparent to him who interprets the genesis of speech in the light of our present knowledge that in reality it is a most recalcitrant witness in behalf of Broadbent's claim. The history of the case, if space permitted us to quote it in detail, would be very instructive to show the typical picture of subcor- Diagnosis of Aphasia. 337 tical sensory aphasia. " The patient after an acute cere- bral attack (?) showed absolute inability to read printed or written words (except his name), while he wrote cor- rectly from dictation and spontaneously." There was in- ability to recall the name of the most familiar object pre- sented to his sight, while he conversed intelligently, employing an extensive and varied vocabulary, making few mistakes, but occasionally forgetting names of streets, persons, and objects. There is no note of whether or not hemianopsia existed, but it may be taken for granted, I think, that it did, for in every published case, without ex- ception, in which this condition was examined for it has been found. In regard to Broadbent's statement that it bears evidence in favor of a naming centre, it need only be said that the case shows that there was a lesion that severed the visual percipient centre from the visual interpreting centre, and if he prefers to call the latter a naming centre no adequate reason has ever been offered why he does so. One then proceeds to examine to see if the patient has word blindness; that is, can the patient read (i) print, (2) script, (3) figures and other forms of notation. I have already described the significance of inability to do all or one of these. It can need no repetition to show that in- ability to read is not of itself an important localizing symptom ; it may be produced by lesion in many parts of the optic projection and of the parts that the optic projec- tion goes to. It is the association of the inability to read that facilitates localizing the lesion. If, for instance, a person's primary visual centres are intact, he sees the written or printed word, the figure or symbol, in all its details ; it merely has no significance for him. This bears 338 The Faculty of Speech. testimony that the lesion is centralward to the primary visual area. He can decide at once whether it be far enough centralward to be in the angular gyrus, the higher visual centre, or not, by determining if there be disturb- ance of internal language. If there is not (and as a mat- ter of fact one may say if there be no agraphia the lesion is not in the zone of language), then the word blindness is the result of a subcortical lesion. One word of caution must be sounded for the inexperienced in determining visual blindness. Some patients who have word blind- ness, particularly those in whom the symptom is depen- dent upon lesion of the higher visual centre, on being asked to read, take up a book or paper, or whatever is handed to them, and essay to read it understandingly. If they are made to read it aloud it will quickly be seen that they cannot read a word, that they " make up," as children do, as they go along. Oftentimes the assurance with which they take hold of a paper or letter and apparently read it deceives even an experienced examiner. It cannot do so, however, if the physician will show them writing or print embodying a request set forth in such a way that cannot escape their recognition if they read it, such as : " If you are able to read this then put your left hand in mine." Another test often employed is for the physician to read aloud the last lines of a page and request the patient to turn the page at the proper time. Naturally, he is unable to do so, as he cannot read, although if he is not at all word deaf, that is, if he can appreciate the quantity and amount read, he may very closely approximate the time when he should turn. In cases of complete aphasia the examination is very Diagnosis of Aphasia. 339 difficult and to one not accustomed to such a task it seems very unsatisfactory, as he is apparently unable to communi- cate with the patient or receive any information from him. Oftentimes it is thought that such patients have not re- covered consciousness completely. I have such a patient at the present time under observation : A married man, fifty-six years old, of good habits, was seized one day with a feeling of numbness and beginning powerlessness in the right leg, which after a few minutes showed itself in the right upper extremity. In less than a quarter of an hour he became unconscious and completely hemiplegic. There had been no premonitory symptoms save recurring head- ache, which, his wife now recalls, had been complained of for several weeks previous to the apoplexy. On the third day after the attack signs of returning consciousness began to show themselves, and at the end of a week there was apparently complete restoration of consciousness. The hemiplegia continued complete, the face being least severely involved. The following notes of the examination show the com- pleteness of the aphasia in this case : What is your name? No response; looks at me blankly and staringly. Is your name ? No reply, slight shifting or rest- lessness of the patient. Are you seventy years old ? No reply. Would you like to go home with your wife ? No reply, no interest. Then these same questions were addressed to him in writing and in print, but he took no more heed of them when endeavor was made to bring them to his notice in this way than when they were spoken. It was impossible 34-O The Faculty of Speech. to establish with any degree of certainty the existence of hemianopsia. The impression, as the result of repeated and careful testing, was that it did not exist. There was apparently no object, no mind blindness. The patient took a piece of candy that his wife brought, carried it to the mouth, and ate it with apparent relish. When given a pencil he grasped it and handled it in a familiar way, likewise a watch, a key, and other common objects. He recognized members of his family, and when his wife came to see him he would grasp her hand and carry it to his lips. He would try to detain her from leaving, and manifested appreciation of visits. Persistent efforts to get him to write a word are wasted. He grasps the pencil as if he were about to write, but instead of proceeding to form a letter he scratches the paper, just as an infant does who is given a pencil for the first time. If I guide his left hand which holds the pencil to outline his name, or the names of familiar objects, and then show them to him, he does not understand the one or recognize the other. If I guide his hand over the letters of a word to trace it, he may trace the next word alone, but he does it mechanically and takes not the slightest interest in it. When he is given a number of bills of different denominations, it is impossible to say that he understands the difference in value of them, for he takes with the same readiness a twenty-dollar bill and a two-dollar bill. He does not utter a word or the semblance of a word. He does not indulge in any form of pantomime or mimetic action. There seems to be ab- solutely no way of communicating information to him or of receiving wishes or desires from him. In other words, he has total aphasia. In another connection, it may be recalled, it was stated that sometimes such cases of aphasia clear up on one side of language, the receptive or Diagnosis of Aphasia. 341 the emissive, and the residue of the aphasic symptom com- plex constitutes sensory or motor aphasia. In this patient it would seem that the middle cerebral artery had ruptured and that the speech area has suffered so severely that restitution will not follow, as sufficient time has already elapsed to have it show itself if the vital forces were sufficient to do so. The fact that he has an advanced degree of chronic interstitial nephritis, and the fact that, despite most careful dietetic and vigor- ous medicinal treatment, he is continually emaciating, augurs ill for him. Sufficient has already been said of dyslexia in the chap- ter on " Sensory Aphasia" to make reference to it here, as a factor in diagnosis, unnecessary. I am disposed only to reiterate, particularly since reading the first of Bastian's recent lectures on problems in aphasia and other speech defects, which appeared after this monograph was written, the necessity of the most painstaking examination to re- veal latent defects of internal reading in every case of aphasia. After the condition of the functional state of the different speech centres and the immediate conducting tracts leading to and away from them have been inquired into, and a general survey has been taken of the patient, two things remain to be clone ; namely, an examination of the patient's capacity to externalize mental contents by pantomime or mimicry, and a study of the manifestations of emotion in melody, instrumental music, profanity, in- terjections, and gestures. It is believed that sufficient has already been said of these to make reconsideration of them unnecessary. It is necessary to say one word concerning the time of 342 The Faculty of Speech. examination of patients with aphasia. The complexity of symptoms that may be determined one or two weeks after the restoration of consciousness following the apoplectic insult, or the confusion and delirium which may be the introductory symptoms of the aphasia, may be quite dif- ferent from that found when the examination is made later. The morbid vascular changes that go on secondarily to the lesion, whatever it may be, will subside in part after the acute manifestations of the disease or accident have disap- peared, and if they have not been of sufficient severity to cause destruction of the parts the area that was for a time obscured will again functionate with more or less integ- rity. It is well, therefore, to compare the results of the clinical status made early with those made after the symp- toms have continued for a time, and thus to establish the permanent degree as well as the kind of aphasia. CHAPTER IX. ETIOLOGY. BEFORE enumerating the individual organic diseases or functional conditions of which the different varieties of aphasia may be a symptom, I wish to direct attention very FIG. 19. Vascular Supply of Cortex. briefly to the boundaries and blood supply of the zone of language, which contains the centres of auditory, visual, and articulatory (kinaesthetic) images. The locations of these centres in this zone of language are now definitely assigned. Their relative position on the surface of the brain is shown in Fig. 7, page 99. Their boundaries and interrelations are considered in Chapter IV., " Con- ception of Aphasia." 344 The Facility of Speech. A comprehensive knowledge of the vascular supply to this portion of the brain is necessary for A full under- standing of the mode of development and the lesions of aphasia. Such knowledge can best be obtained by a study FIG. 20. Cortical Blood-Vessels of Foetal Brain. of the fresh brain. The accompanying illustration (Fig. 19), taken from the well-known work of Duret, will convey an idea of the distribution of the blood-vessels to the exter- nal surface of the brain. It shows the middle cerebral artery, which supplies the side of the cortex in all its ramifi- cations. It will be seen that this one artery supplies through its trunk, its principal branch, and its terminals Etiology. 345 the zone of language and the three centres lying therein. Fig. 20 shows the cortical vessels of the foetal brain par- ticularly in their relation to the fossa of Sylvius. When one considers the importance of the middle cerebral artery to the zone of language, it is no longer surprising that we do not oftener encounter cases of aphasia whose symptoms can be interpreted as due to lesion of an individual centre. The middle cerebral artery, the largest and most important branch of the internal carotid, in fact, the continuation of the latter, supplies other parts than the zone of language, and enumeration of these parts facilitates interpretation of the accompanying symptoms in many cases of aphasia. The antero-lateral arteries which are given off from the middle cerebral immediately after leaving the carotid artery pass through the foramina of the anterior perforated space to the base of the corpus striatum and form the most impor- tant supply of that region. The lenticular branch sup- plies the inner and middle segments of the lenticular nucleus and the internal capsule. The lenticulo-striate arteries supply the outer segment of the lenticular nucleus and external capsule and the caudate nucleus, and the lenticular-optic arteries supply the outer part of the optic thalamus. The vessel then passes to the cortex. The cortical branches of the middle cerebral artery, or the Sylvian artery, the continuation of the main trunk of the internal carotid, are the interior external frontal, dis- tributed to the outer part of the orbital surface of the hemisphere and adjacent frontal convolutions; the ascend- ing frontal, distributed to the convolution of the same name and to the root of the middle frontal convolution ; the ascending parietal to the parietal convolution and to 346 The Faculty of Speech. the forepart of the superior" parietal lobule; and the parieto-temporal, which runs backward in the posterior limb of the fissure of Sylvius and ramifies upward over the angular gyrus and downward over the superior and upper part of the middle temporal convolution. Contrasting the distribution of this artery on the cortex with the relative position of the different speech centres, it will be seen that a lesion of the trunk at its entrance into the fossa of Sylvius, and particularly a lesion of the first branch, will be very apt to destroy Broca's convo- lution, in which are stored the kinaesthetic memories of articulation. If the lesion is not of sufficient sever- ity to destroy the centre it may materially pervert its functions, and this perversion may be transient or lasting. Simultaneously with the occurrence of such a lesion the integrity of the circulation in the posterior parts of the vessel, in the terminal branches, the one bending up over the angular gyrus and the other down over the temporal convolution, may be disturbed, though to a very insignifi- cant degree compared with the disturbance of Broca's centre, but yet sufficient to add a sensory element to the aphasic symptom complex. Thus, in the beginning of some cases of aphasia, the symptoms may indicate a mixed form, but the slight sensory or motor element, as the case may be, may disappear, leaving the other dominant to con- stitute the form of aphasia. It is not in place here to point out that the aphasic symptoms vary not alone with the seat of the lesion, but with the intensity of the lesion, the rapidity of its progress and development, or, in other words, with its nature. This may be inferred from what has just been said. Etiology. 347 With the exception of destruction of the speech areas that are the result of injury and new growth, organic dis- ease of the zone of language is almost always the result of vascular lesion. These vascular lesions are rupture of the blood-vessels and occlusion of their calibres, whether from embolus cr thrombus, and the consecutive changes dependent thereon. The lesions of the blood-vessels may, however, be due to inflammatory conditions of the vessels, but even then it is not at all improbable that the pathogene- sis of the lesion is the direct result of a septic or infec- tious process that causes infectious emboli and thrombi. The traumatic conditions that may produce aphasia are bullet and stab wounds, depressed fractures of the skull, and injuries producing meningeal hemorrhage. Etiologically, aphasia may be classified into organic and dynamic. The principal organic forms have just been enumerated. Under the dynamic forms may be included those in which no organic lesion is responsible for the development of aphasic symptoms. The term dynamic is used merely as a convenience in preference to the conven- tional " functional." The dynamic variety includes aphasia occurring with neuroses and psychoses which are not yet proven to be dependent upon recognizable brain lesion, of which epilepsy, neurasthenia, and hysteria may be taken as examples. It also embraces most of the cases of aphasia occurring with toxihaemia, such as uraemia, diabetes, and gout ; although aphasia in some of these cases, especially aphasia occurring with uraemia, is often dependent upon organic vascular lesion of the cerebral blood-vessels. Aphasia caused by the vegetable poisons, santonin, bella- donna, tobacco, etc., is almost invariably of the dynamic 348 The Facility of Speech. form. The aphasia that sometimes occurs in individuals who have been poisoned by lead, copper, etc., may be of the dynamic variety, or it may be a focal manifestation of the encephalopathy that these poisons occasionally cause. The dynamic aphasias also include the aphasic speech disturbances occurring with neuralgic affections of a migrainous order, those occurring with forms of insanity that have no known anatomical basis, and, finally, the comparatively insignificant number which are attributed to fright, anger, so-called reflex causes, such as intestinal worms, and the transitory aphasias from loss of blood. Ordinary etiological factors, such as age, sex, occupa- tion, etc., have no bearing on the causation of aphasia, because it is itself a symptom, and it results only when the diseases of which it is a symptom occur or are prone to occur ; but as aphasia is so often associated with cere- bral apoplexy, and as cerebral apoplexy occurs usually in late maturity and advanced age, it follows that aphasia is seen oftener in people beyond fifty years of age. Never- theless, it would be misleading to leave this statement unmodified, for the reason that three diseases which not infrequently have aphasia as a symptom, namely, uraemia, acute hemorrhagic encephalitis, and tuberculous menin- gitis, are particularly liable to occur in the young. More- over, aphasia sometimes develops in the wake of the infec- tious diseases, typhoid, diphtheria, and pertussis, and, as these occur more frequently in youth than at any other time, it follows that the aphasias of this variety will be seen oftenest at this time of life. It is my intention to enumerate the more prominent diseases of which aphasia is an important symptom, and Etiology. 349 to dwell upon a few of these only. In addition to those already mentioned, it may be said that aphasia of any kind may be referable to some such disease of the skull as exostosis and bony tumor; in short, to any condition of the skull bones or the meninges, such as pachymeningitis, that produces pressure on or irritation of the speech area. It is an occasional symptom of thrombosis of the dural sinuses, particularly thrombosis of the lateral sinuses. Aphasia is one of the most important and constant symptoms of acute non-purulent encephalitis. A case re- cently reported by Leva 1 is a good illustrative example. The patient had pronounced sensory aphasia, total alexia, and agraphia, but no articulatory aphasia. At the autopsy there was found diffuse encephalitis, with softening in the first and second insular gyri of the left hemisphere, in the adjacent convolutions of the inferior hemispherical convolutions, and some disintegration of the first temporal gyrus adjacent to the insula. In the right hemisphere similar areas of softening were found in the right temporal gyrus. Depending upon the locality of the abscess, a variety of aphasia is frequently an important symptom of purulent circumscribed encephalitis, and especially the form com- plicating purulent disease of the middle ear. In fact, in cases of brain abscess it is oftentimes a localizing symp- tom of most exquisite value. This is well illustrated by a case recently published by Zaufal : 2 1 Leva : Virchow's Archiv, vol. cxxxii., part ii. 5 Zaufal and Pick : " Otitischer Gehirnabscess im linken Temporallap- pen, optische Aphasie, Eroffnung durch Trepanation. Heilung." Prager med. \\~ochenschr. , Xos. 5, 6, 8, 9, 1897. 35<3 The Facility of Speech. A young woman, twenty-five years old, complained of headache, pain in the left ear, nausea, and vomiting. Eight days later there was a discharge of pus from the left ear. Examination revealed acute suppuration of the middle ear, inequality of the pupils, progressive stupor, and vomiting. Shortly after this, the patient was unable to name objects shown to her, although she knew them well and could describe their appearance ; that is, there was optic aphasia, pointing to a lesion severing the con- nections of the primary visual area with the area of word memories; in other words, a pure, or subcortical visual aphasia. The lesion was localized by Pick, and an opera- tion revealed an abscess of the size of a hen's egg in the region indicated, namely, in the left second and third tem- poral convolutions far back and in the white substance. The patient made a complete recovery. In this case it is to be noted that there was no hemianopsia, which showed that the primary visual area around the calcarine fissure, as well as the radiations of Gratiolet were not affected. Another most instructive instance of the same kind, but in which the outcome was not so gratifying, is that of a case related by Lannois and Jaobulay. 1 In this case there were, in addition to symptoms of ear disease and brain abscess, alexia, agraphia, word blindness, right-side hemianopsia, and slight facial paralysis. The patient was operated on but no pus was found. A second operation was made three weeks later, and a collection of pus was evac- uated. Eleven days later the patient died, and on autop- sical examination a large abscess was found in the centre of the left occipital lobe. There were also a number of foci of diffuse encephalitis, some of them purulent, proba- 1 Lannois and Jaboulay : Revue de Medecine, August, 1896, p. 659. Etiology. 351 bly metastatic, in other parts of the left hemisphere. In this instance, the symptoms were of such absolute localiz- ing significance, pointing to destruction of the left occi- pital lobe and of the radiations of Gratiolet, that it would have been fully justifiable to lay open that part of the brain instead of endeavoring to draw off the pus by punc- ture. Of the other intracranial inflammatory conditions, that of meningitis of the convexities and meningeal tubercu- losis are the two diseases sometimes accompanied by aphasia. In purulent leptomeningitis secondary to optic disease aphasia often develops, and, were it not for the teachings of Hugenin, it is probable that aphasia would not be considered of infrequent occurrence in tuberculous meningitis. To show the extent and complexity of the aphasic symptoms in tuberculous meningitis, I need only refer to a case communicated by Carriere, 1 in which a tuberculous meningitis (the lesion predominating in the posterior part of the fissure of Sylvius) was accompanied by verbal blindness, ageusia, anosmia, and, finally, total blindness. This case is interesting also for the reason that a spot of recent softening was found in the hippo- campal gyrus. The anosmia was probably dependent upon this lesion, as this would tend to corroborate the claims of Jackson, Beevor, Carbonieri, ct al., who place the cortical centre of smell in the hippocampal gyrus. Aphasia may occur as a symptom of direct injury to the brain, and in times of war and riot such cases are of com- mon occurrence. A remarkable instance of destruction of 1 Carriere, G. : Archives Cliniques de Bordeaux, 1^96, p. 135. 352 The Faculty of Speech. Broca's convolution by a foreign body is shown by a case of Simon. 1 The patient had been injured by a fall from a horse. After death a splinter of bone, which had been detached from the inner table of the skull, was fountt in the left third frontal convolution. Aside from the cases in which there is solution of continuity of the skull, it may result from injury to the head, such as from a blow, a fall, a kick, unaccompanied by fracture of the skull. Whether or not the lesion in some of these cases is a dynamic one cannot be said. Usually the aphasic symp- toms in such cases are neither so complete nor so contin- uous as is aphasia depending upon vascular lesion. This is shown by an instance published by Cameron. 2 A young man fell a distance of fifteen feet and struck on the head. He was unconscious for a time and later very restless. On the seventh day after the accident he had a series of. mild convulsions, manifest on both sides of the body, but with special involvement of the mouth and eyes. After this the stupor became greater, but from the eighteenth day there was a gradual clearing up of consciousness. On the nineteenth day he was able to say " Yes" and "No." On the twentieth day there were aphasia, alexia, and agraphia. These symptoms disappeared gradually, and about six weeks after the accident the patient was quite well. In a case of this kind, it is much more likely that there was no considerable organic lesion, and that the anatomical condition may be compared to that which is supposed to exist in some cases of traumatic neurosis. 'Simon: "A Case of Aphasia." Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin, Baltimore, 1889-90, i., p. 48. * Cameron : " Notes of a Case of Traumatic Aphasia." Glasgow Medi- cal Journal, August, 1896, p. 126. Etiology. 353 Therefore the aphasia is properly considered to be of the dynamic variety. The dynamic aphasias may be dependent on functional disorders of the brain, that term being used in its widest sense to cover not alone states of nervous exhaustion, but to include such diseases as epilepsy, hysteria, and migraine. They, as well as the aphasias of toxihaemia, are character- ized by the variability of their manifestations, the inter- mittency of their course, the transitoriness of their dura- tion, but especially by their favorable outcome. The form of aphasia that occurs with the neuroses varies, and we are not in position to s.tate the conditions governing such variation. In some instances, the aphasia will be typical articulatory kinaesthetic aphasia, while in others the sensory elements will predominate. The most common symptom of the aphasic speech disturbance attending migraine would seem to be paraphasia and inability to name objects. Not long ago, a hard-working physician, about forty years of age, who had been for a time under my treatment for epilepsy, developed under the auspices of an acute infec- tious influenza a profound status epilepticus, which lasted nearly sixty hours. He then gradually recovered con- sciousness, but for the next five days had aphasic symp- toms characterized particularly by loss of articulatory motor memories, as he has since then decided with me. After he recovered consciousness he was wholly unable to speak, although there was no word deafness, no word blindness, nor was there the slightest trace of disturbance of motility in any part of the body. He was at this time agraphic, although able to copy. I labored with him for some time to get him to pronounce the simple words " Yes" or " No" 23 354 The Facility of Speech. in response to questions which he apparently understood ; showed him how to say them by fixing his lips and by example, but all to no purpose. He regained ability to speak fluently and correctly within a week. Another case of epilepsy, but in which the aphasic symptoms were the result of bromide intoxication, has been for me an instructive example. The patient is a young woman of social position, who desires above every- thing else not only to remain free from the convulsive attacks of her infirmity but to keep her friends ignorant of it, and to do this she is willing to take very large doses of bromide. Occasionally if hydriatic and other restorative procedures necessary to keep up the tone of the nervous system are at all neglected, she develops symptoms of sensory aphasia, characterized especially by verbal am- nesia and mild degrees of word blindness and object blindness. I have had her maid make notes on numerous occasions of the mistakes of utterance that are noticed at these times, and I quote here a few of them. Wishing to say, " I am going to my room," she said, " I thick, think, that's the stick thick." For " I am going down stairs," she said, " I am going down town." When she desired the maid to hand her a cup, she said, " Will you give me that window ;" and at table, desirous of living up to the mandates of her physician, in refusing dessert she said, " I do not care for interest." She says that oftentimes she finds herself conning a printed page or a letter trying to make out what it means, to decipher the words, and then, all at once, after looking at them for a time, it will quickly dawn upon her that they are symbols, letters, and words with which she is familiar. It has also been noted Etiology. 355 that at such times, on preparing to go for a walk or a drive, she will insist that she cannot go without her hat, while all the time the hat rests on her head and she may be apparently looking at it in a mirror. There is no de- mentia or trace of insanity ; her conduct and her actions are in keeping with her breeding; and when the bromides are diminished in amount and restorative measures applied vigorously the aphasic symptoms disappear. The genesis of aphasia occurring with epilepsy is not an easy matter to interpret, but it should not be forgotten that some cases of epilepsy of which it is a symptom are dependent upon organic disease, such as a tumor, and vascular lesion, and in every case of epilepsy in which aphasia occurs a very careful examination is demanded. A case recently reported by Hay prompts me to this state- ment. A man, thirty-nine years old, free from syphilis, suffered three attacks of influenza in rapid succession, and immediately afterward complained of great weakness, headache, depression, which brought him to a very dis- tracted state. Shortly afterward a condition of status epilepticus developed, and when this terminated he had aphasia of a sensory type and agraphia. Six months after the beginning of the symptoms status epilepticus devel- oped a second time, and from this he did not recover. The autopsy showed a spot of softening in the left tem- poral convolution. It does not need the recitation of individual examples to show that aphasia in some of its forms, and particularly sensory aphasia, characterized by verbal amnesia and par- aphasia, is a common attendant upon states of mental ex- haustion, especially when associated with physical fatigue, 356 The Faculty of SpcecJi. and upon preoccupation. Almost every one who has been overcome by the former is in possession of a personal ex- ample. Naturally I do not mean to say that preoccupation produces aphasia. What I mean is that a person's cogni- tive areas maybe so intent upon/subjects that engross him that the zone of language is temporarily ungeared. A very good illustration of this is related of Emerson. It is well known that this immortal transcendentalist was wont to accompany himself with the traditional New Eng- land umbrella in his walks. On occasions when more engrossed in absorbing thought than usual, he would hesi- tate on going out, search various corners, the hatrack, etc. , where the cotton rain guard was usually to be found, and not finding it he would stand, solemnly tap the tem- ple or the brow, and say, half to himself and half to a by- stander, if there happened to be one, " Oh, where is , where is, where is, my, oh where is that thing that honest people take or borrow and never think it necessary to return ?" When the word umbrella was suggested, " Yes, my umbrella." Although this has been utilized as a contribution to " Umbrella Pleasantries," it reminds one of the patient described by Trousseau, who, desiring the same article, was wont to say, " Where is my u u u sacrt matin ! " " Your umbrella ?" " Yes, my umbrella." Genuine aphasia is a very rare accompaniment of hys- teria, as rare correspondingly as mutism is frequent. Why this should be so, I am at loss to understand. It appears to me that the most reasonable interpretation of most, if not all, hysterical phenomena is one that posits the partial abolition or perversion of function of one or more of the cortical areas. When the involved cortical Etiology. 357 area is the somaesthetic or Rolandic area, there result perversions of sensibility and motility; when of the pri- mary visual areas, we have hysterical blindness ; when of the auditory area, we have hysterical deafness ; and when of the frontal lobes, we have diverse and protean psychi- cal manifestations. Thus it would seem to me that a dynamic perversion of the zone of language analogous to the condition that forms the basis of the above-mentioned phenomena might produce aphasia; but as a matter of fact there are very few examples of hysterical aphasia on record. Hysterical aphasia is usually less transitory than are other forms of dynamic aphasia, but, unlike hysterical aphonia, it does not extend over months and years. It may be the only major symptom of hysteria or it may be associated with hysterical hemiplegia and contracture or with contracture in other parts of the body. It is more frequently seen accompanying traumatic hysteria, than with the so-called idiopathic form. Aphasia analogous to the hysterical form has been artificially produced in per- sons by hypnotism. In studying the aphasic speech disturbances which sometimes accompany neuralgic affections of the migrai- nous order, it is well to keep in mind the genetic relation- ship existing between migraine and epilepsy. Usually the aphasic disturbances of migraine are of a sensory character and very transitory. In discussing the relation of amusia to aphasia, I have cited one or two examples of the aphasia of migraine, and shall here refer only to one example recently published by Pick. 1 A young physician 1 Pick : Berliner klinische Wochenschrift, 1894, No. 47. 358 The Faculty of Speech. who had lived a very irregular life developed suddenly symptoms of ophthalmic migraine, with which were asso- ciated motor aphasia, word deafness, and echolalia, all of which disappeared with the attack. It has been suggested that the genesis of migrainous aphasia is in reality a vascular one, a contraction of the cortical blood-vessels of the left hemisphere, which causes a perversion of function of the zone of language. This is a very plausible supposition, considering the very striking evidences of vasomotor instability in other parts of the body that occur with migraine, but it is easily understood that there are insurmountable difficulties in the way of adducing proof of this hypothesis. There is an amount of very convincing evidence on record to show that certain drugs, such as santonin, bella- donna, tobacco, etc., of the vegetable poisons, sometimes produce aphasia when given in toxic doses. There is nothing especially characteristic of such aphasia to aid us in differentiating it from other dynamic forms, and the aphasia can be suspected to be of such origin only when it is known that the patient has exposed himself to one of these poisons. If the cause can be discovered and re- moved, the aphasic symptoms soon disappear, and this, more than anything else, stamps their origin. This leads me to speak of other toxic conditions that iviay cause aphasia, particularly uraemia, diabetes, and gout, as well as more uncommon forms of poisoning, such ag snake bite, etc., and of the aphasia which sometimes occurs with Raynaud's disease. Aphasia is an extremely uncommon complication or coincident symptom of Ray- naud's disease, symmetrical gangrene. I have been able Etiology. 359 to find but two references to it in the literature, one by Weiss; ' another by Osier. 2 Personally I have seen one case. A young man was brought into the hospital suffer- ing from symmetrical gangrene of both feet, slight cyano- sis of the tip of the nose and the upper part of both ears, and with the general-collapse symptoms attending the advanced stage of this disease. He was thought to be demented or amented by some members of the house staff who saw him, because it was impossible to extract any in- formation from him. He could not tell his name, age, residence, occupation, or anything about his illness, neither could he be got to write or read. Ability to articulate words was preserved, for he would occasionally use words, but they could not be understood or interpreted, i.e., they were without sense. Under tonic and stimulat- ing treatment, including large doses of nitroglycerin, he improved slowly, and eventually the sensory aphasia dis- appeared. The patient with Raynaud's disease described by Osier had three attacks of transitory right-side hemiplegia and aphasia, and died six months after the third attack. In my patient there was no trace of hemiplegia. It is not at all unlikely that the aphasia in these cases is dependent upon vascular conditions in the brain similar to those in the extremities that become blanched and then cyanosed. Unless the disease is very severe the aphasia is not com- plete, and may manifest itself only in paraphasia and diffi- culty in interpreting spoken and written language. When the affection of the cerebral blood-vessels is so profound 1 Wiener Klinik, 1882. 8 American Journal of the Medical Sciences, vol. cxii., 1896. o 60 The Faculty of Speech. as to cause hemiplegia the aphasia is usually total. In my patient the disturbance of speech was mainly on the receptive side of language, and there was no disturbance of locomotion except that conditioned by the peripheral manifestations of the disease and the asthenia which is always such a profound symptom. Of the toxic aphasias, that produced by uraemia is by far the most common. It may be a mistake to include it in the dynamic aphasias, for it must be classed with the cerebral symptoms of a paralytic order, provoked by a toxic substance in the blood. Uraemic aphasia is seen oftener in the old than in those of middle age, and oftener in chil- dren than at any other time of life. The frequency of ursemic aphasia in children is accounted for by the fact that post-scarlatinal nephritis and nephritis following other infectious diseases are more common at this time of life. During the years of fullest maturity, uraemic symp- toms are more liable to be dependent upon cirrhotic kidney than upon any other form of kidney disease. Clinically, uraemic aphasia is more commonly of a mixed form than it is visual, auditory, or articulatory, but of course this de- pends, as does the determination of the variety of aphasia in any given case, upon the part of the zone of language that is predominantly affected. In cases of transitory aphasia, accompanying grippe, pneumonia, etc., the aphasic symptoms frequently come on after the patient has been " flighty" or delirious, and usually the motor form predominates. A case of this kind is reported by Isager. 1 A child ceased to speak after 1 Isager : " Et Tilfalde af Afasi i Tilslutning til en krupos Pneumoni " Hosp. Tid., 1894, p. 42. Etiology. 361 the crisis of a croupous pneumonia. He understood fully what was said, he could not speak voluntarily, he could not repeat, nor could he read aloud. There was no paral- ysis, and after a week he began to use individual words, and in the course of a few days the faculty of speech was quite restored. It is not at all unlikely that cases of aphasia occurring with lead poisoning are in reality dependent upon a lead encephalopathy, and the aphasic symptom complex may be looked upon as a focal or localizing symptom. This leads me to say another word anent the aphasic speech disturbances of the specific fevers. A great deal of atten- tion has been devoted to this subject, and it is by no means settled that the majority of cases belong to the one form of aphasia, organic, or to the other, dynamic. It is likely that some are of the first kind and others are of the second. It is known that the infectious diseases predis- pose very materially to septic states of the blood, to phle- bitis, to endocarditis, and to other conditions directly causative of embolism and thrombosis, and thus in- directly to organic aphasia by producing an obstruc- tion of the middle cerebral artery and its branches which supply the zone of language, and which leads to softening. The aphasic speech disturbances that are sometimes, though rarely, associated with chorea, primary degenera- tive tic, and with different forms of peripheral irritation, usually called reflex, such as from intestinal worms, phi- mosis, etc., are very uncommon, although the existence of cases of this kind seems to be well substantiated. They do not call for particular discussion. The aphasia is to 362 The Faculty of Speech. be looked upon as the result of reflex inhibitory phenom- ena and purely dynamical in constitution. Aphasic symptoms occasionally develop after a consid- erable loss of blood, such as from epistaxis (Berthold), rupture of varix (Storp), post-partum hemorrhages, or sim- ilar conditions causing acute anasmia. Hallervorden 1 de- plores that so little attention is given by writers on the etiology of aphasia to acute anaemia. As a matter of fact, unless the loss of blood causes a marantic thrombus in some cortical branch of the left Sylvian artery, genuine aphasia is an excessively rare symptom of such hemor- rhages. Sudden partial exsanguination is often accom- panied by dysarthria, sometimes by anarthria, but these are bulbar symptoms, just as the syncopal attacks that often occur from the same cause are bulbar manifesta- tions. True aphasia, the result of loss of blood, is usu- ally partial, and may be manifest through both the recep- tion and the emission of speech. It is usually transitory and is one of the first exhaustion symptoms to disappear. There remain to be discussed in the etiology of aphasia two of the most important organic diseases of which aphasia is a symptom. These are tumor of the brain and cerebral apoplexy. Aphasia as a localizing symptom of new growth in the brain is one of the most important and trustworthy guides. As a rule, tumor presents the ideal lesion to cause a strictly confined destruction of one of the centres in the zone of language. At least, I mean to say, that it is so at the beginning of the tumor. Tumors begin in the great majority of cases in one of two ways : by a proliferation of the tissue of the part which is pathologi- 1 Hallervorden : Deutsche med. Wochenschrift, vol. xxii., No. 5, 1895. Etiology. 363 cal from the beginning, or by the appearance of a tissue that is foreign to the part. In the beginning the abnor- mality is small, perhaps microscopical. It maybe strictly confined to an area that has such highly individualized function as the angular gyrus, the first temporal convolu- tion, or to a definite part of the motor area, and at such time it will produce symptoms of almost mathematical precision. These symptoms may occur before the symp- toms of brain tumor, that are so consecrated by time that they are called cardinal, develop. If the physician takes proper recognition of them and gives proper interpretation to them, such perspicuity on his part may be rewarded by the life of the patient. It is not necessary to point out in detail what these varicus symptoms are. If one has in mind the topography of the surface of the cortical sub- stance, and the functions allotted to it, as well as an un- derstanding of the connecting pathways of these cortical areas, it is unnecessary to make explicit recitation of such symptoms. Aphasia may be a prominent symptom of a recoverable form of tumor, gumma, and therefore the importance of recognizing it and applying appropriate treatment is ap- parent. A most instructive case, showing not alone the amenability to treatment of very profound forms of sensory aphasia when due to syphilitic lesion, but also the accu- racy with which such a lesion can be localized from the clinical data, is one recently reported by Bramwell. 1 A young widow, who had had several miscarriages and abor- tions, complained after repeated exposure to the sun's rays, while working in the fields, of severe headache, worse at 1 Bramwell : " Illustrative Cases of Aphasia." Lancet, 1897. 364. Tke Faculty of Speech. night ; mental stupidity and loss of memory (she stated that she felt as if she was in a misti ; and dimness of vision. On examination there were found slight optic neu- ritis, right-sided homonymous hemianopsia, almost com- plete word blindness, complete agraphia. She could not write spontaneously, from dictation, or from copy. There was inability to name objects correctly, and when she tried to name she nearly always applied the wrong name, al- though she recognized when others called things by the wrong name. The auditory speech centre was practically normal, as was likewise the articulatory speech centre. Soon after iodide of potassium was prescribed the symp- toms began to be ameliorated, the headache was relieved, the mental condition cleared up, the word blindness and the agraphia disappeared, and the fields of vision gradually enlarged until at the date of her discharge they were quite normal. She regained possession of the modes of speech in the following order: First, ability to name objects; then ability to read letters and words, but at this stage she was completely unable to write spontaneously, to copy letters, or to write from dictation; then she gradually found herself able to read a little more each clay, and in the course of a few days to write a few letters. It is worthy of remark that before she regained the ability to write letters she seemed to have an idea of the letters, that she wished to write ; in other words, she could evoke them in her mind. This is shown by the fact that when she was asked if she did not know the letter a she said that she did, that it was an o with a crook. Finally, she regained the power of writing fully, and at the time she was discharged from the hospital every one of the aphasic Etiology. 365 symptoms, without exception, had yielded to antisyphilitic treatment. I have recently had a very similar experience. A young married woman whose manifestations of constitutional syphilis were repeated abortions and exfoliating necrosis of the palatal region of. the upper jaw began to complain of headache, nervousness, flightiness, depressed states of mind with frequent crying-spells, and of trouble in see- ing. Later, she complained of a subjective feeling of numbness and unwieldiness in the right hand. This was followed after a few weeks by a convulsive attack begin- ning in the right hand and involving the entire extremity, without loss of consciousness. After the convulsion ceased she was unable to speak and to write for several hours. There was no hemianopsia, apparently no disturb- ance on the receptive side of language, and she could under- stand what was said to her. She had three attacks simi- lar to this, and after the last of them I saw her. The twitching of the right upper extremity had ceased and there was no evidence of hemiplegia, but the patient had well-marked symptoms of cortical motor aphasia, which gradually disappeared after several hours. She made a complete recovery under treatment by mercury and the iodides. All the symptoms, including a slight degree of choked discs, had disappeared at the end of two months' energetic antisyphilitic and restorative treatment. This, then, shows clearly that the etiology of aphasia, and that, too, of organic aphasia, has a most important bearing on the course and outcome of the symptom and on determining the treatment. In Bramwell's case the lesion, although it did not destroy the higher visual centre 366 The Faculty of Speech. in the angular gyrus, made such inroads upon the latter that it determined its functional overthrow, as indicates the agraphia, writing in all of its modes having been lost, while the homonymous hemianopsia and the alexia, the in- ability to apply the proper names to objects, showed that the way from the primary visual centres in the gray mat- ter of the occipital lobes around the calcarine fissure to the higher visual centre was severely encroached upon. The efficaciousness of prompt and energetic treatment directed against the luetic meningitis or the gummatous encroachment upon the cortex is shown very strikingly by the disappearance of the symptoms in my own patient. Of the large number of examples of cases of brain tumor whose existence was directly pointed by the pres- ence of aphasic symptoms, I shall cite but one example, and that recorded by Walton. The patient was a man, forty years old, who suffered from gradually increasing attacks of headache, at first of brief duration, later continu- ous, together with difficulty in speech ("ataxic aphasia"). He complained of red spots in the field of vision, and ex- amination showed word deafness, word blindness, and right-side hemianopsia, in addition to double optic neuritis and paralysis of the left abducens. The patient was tre- panned, but after the skull was opened it was decided that the tumor was too deeply seated and too extensive to war- rant interference. The patient died two months after the operation. The autopsy showed a more or less lobulated pear-shaped mass, occupying the region of the posterior parietal and anterior part of the occipital lobe on the left side, loosely attached in some places to the brain sub- tance, infiltrating it in others. The temporal lobe was Etiology. 367 pushed downward, the occipital backward. The growth reached practically to the mesial surface, and on micro- scopic examination it was found to be a glioma. In this case, therefore, the symptoms were most path- ognomonic and suggested with great accuracy the locus of the growth. A lesion that will produce word blindness, left-side homonymous hemianopsia, and word deafness can be situated only in the angular gyrus, extending far enough centrally to sever the radiations of Gratiolet, and downward to impinge upon the auditory centre in the first temporal convolution of the left side. This case furnishes opportunity to say another word concerning the laxity in the use of terms by even those most worthy of the name of cultured neurologists. In this case it was said that there was ataxic aphasia. Now, in addition to the fact that no designation of medical terminology is more indefi- nite than ataxic aphasia, it may also be said that for the reader who would judge of the speech defects in the case from the published report there was nothing to point the existence of articulatory motor aphasia ; nor was there any lesion found on autopsy that would point to the existence of such a condition. Moreover, it is difficult to say from the autopsy report alone whether or not the cortical sub- stance of the angular gyrus was completely destroyed. One infers that it was not, because the tumor was appar- ently out of the reach of the surgeon and could not have, therefore, forced its way to the surface ; otherwise a part, at least, of it would have been removed. I wish hence to reiterate that, in the report both of the clinical side of a case and of the anatomical side of a case, those who would contribute to the real study of aphasia and thus to the 368 The Faculty of Speech. tomes of physiology, psychology, and biology, should be mindful ever of the need of accurate statements and of detailed description. So frequently is aphasia an accompaniment of apoplexy, a term which I use to include rupture of a blood-vessel, obliteration of its calibre, and acute softening, that in the minds of many physicians the word aphasia suggests apoplexy. The genetic and anatomical relationship exist- ing between the zone of language and the middle cerebral artery has already been mentioned, and, as the subject will be considered in extenso in the chapter on " Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia," it is referred to here in the briefest manner. Nor do I think it necessary in a chapter on the etiology of aphasia to remark on the causative factors of cerebral apoplexy. Of the organic mental diseases that aphasia may be a symptom of, general paresis is the most important. In the beginning of this disease, which is pathologically a widespread degeneration of the cortex, the most prominent symptom may be aphasia. This probably coincides with a beginning degeneration in some part of the zone of lan- guage. Usually the aphasia is of the sensory type. There remains but one form of aphasia to be mentioned, and this chapter is finished, and that is the form known as congenital aphasia, or congenital limitation of speech de- velopment. These cases are not very uncommon. Kuss- maul described a number of instances more than a quarter of a century ago, and recently Gutzmann has studied the subject very carefully. Congenital aphasia may be partial or it may be complete. I do not include in this catesrorv lack of speech development dependent upon porencephalia Etiology. 369 or other gross lesions of the cerebral hemisphere. Aside from the disturbances in the organs of perception which are a part of idiocy, there is a not inconsiderable number of cases in which, without any disease of the peripheral percipient sensory apparatuses, or of the central sensory perceptual areas, there is a condition of speechlessness, and that, too, entirely divorced from any apparent disturb- ance of intelligence. It is to these cases that Coen 1 gives the name of auditory dumbness in contradistinction to deafness and dumbness. Although these children do not present gross mental or psychical defect, usually careful examination shows that there are always a certain amount of abulia, tardiness in learning to walk, slowness in ac- quiring skill of any kind, and '/arious other manifestations of psychic and somatic degeneration. A very remarkable feature, and one that should be borne in mind when in the presence of such cases, is that in about one-third of them there have been found adenoid vegetations. The only other etiological factors that can be enumerated are that this condition has been met with more frequently in males than in females, and that there is almost always a neuro- pathic heritage, particularly from the father's side, and that, although a great many of these patients acquire con- siderable speech facility, it is often later in life associated with stammering. 1 Coen : Wiener klinische Rundschau, 1893, No. 6 24 CHAPTER X. MORBID ANATOMY OF APHASIA. WERE one to write the morbid anatomy of aphasia in detail, he must needs consider the various disease processes forming the basis of all the organic and functional dis- eases, enumerated in the chapter on etiology, with which aphasia may be associated symptomatically. Such de- scription would carry us far beyond the limits put upon this work. Indeed, I doubt if such consideration could be made to serve any useful purpose in elucidating the intri- cacies of aphasia. Never for a moment should we forget that aphasia is a symptom; and, although 'it is the most comprehensive symptom that a physician has to deal with, it nevertheless always remains a symptom. It has already been said that one associates in his mind almost invol- untarily the symptom aphasia with the disease apoplexy, because it is with apoplexy, using this term generically to include hemorrhage, occlusion of the cerebral blood- vessels, and acute softening, that aphasia most frequently occurs. Notwithstanding this, a discussion of the various forms and stages of cerebral hemorrhage and cerebral soft- ening cannot be attempted here. In reality aphasia due to lesion of the speech centres, true aphasia, is not often dependent upon cerebral hemor- rhage, and this for the reason that cerebral hemorrhage limited to the cortex is an extremely rare condition. On Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 371 the other hand, acute softening following the occlusion of one of the cortical vessels is not so rare. Aphasia is rarely dependent upon hemorrhage in the basal region, for rupture of the trunk of the middle cerebral artery before it gives off the ganglionic arteries is such a serious con- dition that its occurrence is rarely consistent with the continuance of life. In young persons, however, the main trunk of the Sylvian artery may be occluded at any level beyond the branching ganglionic branches, and yet no softening take place. This accounts for the transient- ness of aphasia in young people and for the rapidity and completeness of their recovery. The first of the author's cases cited in this monograph is an excellent example of such speedy recovery, even in a very severe form of intracerebral vascular disease. A more convinc- ing example, because accompanied by an autopsical report, has been published by Ross. 1 A young girl, while suffer- ing from endocarditis, had, first, embolus of the femoral artery, and in rapid succession embolus of the kidneys, spleen, and left Sylvian artery, occlusion of the latter being evinced by an attack of right-side hemiplegia and combined motor and sensory aphasia. The patier\t died from exhaustion about a fortnight after the attack of aphasia. There was found in the main trunk of the Sylvian artery, a little outside the anterior perforated space, but proximally to any of the cortical branches, a very firm occlusion. Not the slightest amount of soften- ing could be discovered, although Ross states that it is very likely that the nerve cells supplied by the Sylvian 'Ross: "Aphasia." Wood's "Medical and Surgical Monographs," vol. vi.. No. I, 1890. 372 The Faculty of Speech, artery had undergone fatty degeneration. There can be little doubt, judging from the outcome of analogous cases, that, had the patient lived, the nutritive and functional activity of the tissues of this area would have been grad- ually restored. Such an outcome should not be at all sur- prising when we consider that the cortical branches of this artery, unlike the ganglionic branches, are not ter- minal ones. Although the study of the morbid anatomy of aphasia really began with Broca when he made an autopsy on his first aphasic patient, Leborgne, previous writers on the subject of speech and its defects were not without anatom- ical data to fortify their contentions, and of these may be mentioned Bouillaud ' and Dax," who based their infer- ences on not an inconsiderable pathological experience. Broca's first case was one that is usually looked upon as a typical example of cortical motor aphasia, but to-day it would, I believe, be considered one of subcortical motor aphasia, as the lesions found were widely distributed on the surface and in the substance of the left hemisphere. Although one of his cases showed extensive involvement of the auditory area, the father of aphasia persisted in the belief that the seat of lesion causing aphasia was in every instance in the posterior part of the third frontal convolu- tion on the left side. It would be supererogatory to enumerate even a part of the great number of autopsies to show that destruction of Broca's convolution is attended by symptoms constituting the symptom complex of cortical motor or articulatory 1 Bouillaud : " Traite Clinique et Physiologique de 1'Kncephalite," 1823. 2 Dax : Gazette Hebdomadaire, .April, 1895, No. 17. Republished. Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 373 kinaesthetic aphasia. This is something that every one admits. In the chapter on etiology of aphasia the rela- tion of the area of Broca to the first cortical branch of the middle cerebral artery has been alluded to, and likewise the liability of this part of the cortex to be diseased. When it is diseased, a complexity of symptoms is produced that varies with the completeness of destruction. The lesions that are found there, constituting the organic basis of cortical motor aphasia, circumscribed encephalo- malacia, localized gummatous meningitis, tumors, abscess, purulent leptomeningitis, penetrating wounds, were also discussed in the chapter on etiology and need no further enumeration here. Future study of morbid changes accompanying motor aphasia should be particularly with two ends in view : first, to separate closely subcortical or pure motor aphasia from motor image aphasia, due to destruction of the area of Broca ; and second, to show that when the disease process is limited to the latter area there is absolutely no second- ary degeneration in any projection tract such as has been described by Pitres, Raymond, Brissaud, and many others as occurring in the inferior pediculo-frontal fascicle. To do this microscopical study of every case is necessary. In- vestigation of this kind can do for the motor side of aphasia what the microscopical studies instituted by De- jerine have done for the sensory side of aphasia. One cannot overestimate the importance of such microscopical study until he examines the literature and finds how easy it is to put different interpretations on the same case, if only the gross lesion is described. Incomplete anatomical investigation must account in some instances for the in- 374 The Faculty of Speech. terpretations of some of the cases that have been cited by authors to prove the existence of a graphic-motor centre, such as that of Henschen, 1 for example. The patient suf- fered from word blindness and agraphia, and after death there was found not only destruction of the foot of the second frontal but also a softening of the angular gyrus. 2 1 Henschen : " Klin. u. anatom. Beitrage zur Pathologic des Gehirns," Upsala, 1890-94. 2 I am constrained to mention again, at the risk of wearying the reader beyond justification, the fact that just so long as writers on aphasia refuse closely to differentiate cortical from subcortical forms of motor aphasia, just so much longer will it be before the question, " Does destruction of Broca's convolution entail agraphia?" is settled to the satisfaction of .every one. I am prompted to these remarks from an examination of the first of Bastian's recent lectures, which are now appearing in The Lancet. No one can be more profoundly cognizant of the fact, that, as a writer on speech disturb- ances, Dr. Bastian should rank with the Fathers of Aphasia, with Broca and Wernicke. It is not hazarding the truth to say that he has contributed more to the elucidation of the genesis of speech than has any English writer. My astonishment is therefore the greater that the following case, quoted from Wadham, should be offered in evidence to negative the claim that cortical motor aphasia always entails agraphia. I venture to state that if there has ever been a case published which is a mirror held up to the symptom complex and morbid anatomy of subcortical motor aphasia, it is the one just mentioned and which I now proceed to quote. " A youth, aged eighteen, left-handed and ambidexterous, became partly hemiplegic on the left side and completely speechless after long exposure to cold. Twelve days later, on being given a slate and pencil, he wrote readily the word 'orange,' and when asked his name wrote it correctly with the right hand, although his mother asserted that she had never previously seen him do so. He and four of his brothers were left-handed. About a week after this, being still absolutely speechless, when asked whether he tried to speak and was unable, he wrote, ' Yes.' Asked if when well he wrote with his left or right hand, he wrote ' Both,' and then added, ' Fight with left.' In six weeks' time the hemiplegia had much diminished, but he still never had spoken a word, and continued to write all his wishes on a slate. His manner gave the impression of a very intelligent and rather facetious young man. At the expiration of three months he left the hos- pital, and when seen at his home later it was noted that the boy repeated after his mother's dictation various words and sentences with the intonation of one who endeavors to speak without moving the tongue. This power gradually increased until he was able to talk with sufficient distinctness to Morbid An atomy of Aphasia. 375 The clinical history of Bastian's case stamps it as one of subcortical motor aphasia. Personally I should have been willing to make the diagnosis on one fact alone, viz., the patient's ability to write. I appreciate, however, that many physicians doubt the momentousness of this posses- sion as a diagnostic indicator, and to those it might be said that a patient with cortical motor aphasia of such severity that it causes absolute speechlessness, and who is " very intelligent and rather facetious," and who passes through the stammering stage in learning to talk, and finally talks like one whose tongue is fixed in the mouth, has yet to be recorded, if the present writer's excursions afield into the literature of aphasia have been properly interpreted. A person to be facetious must be in the possession of inter- nal speech at least, and if one of his speech centres is de- stroyed the integrity of internal speech is destroyed with it. The autopsy record puts the case in the right light, " a lesion in the white substance beneath tJic Rolandic area." That is what the writer of these pages means by the term subcortical ; therefore his astonishment that the case has been cited by Bastian in support of the statement that cortical motor aphasia does not entail agraphia. We are in the early stages of positive knowledge con- cerning the exact limitations and seat of the lesions that produce aphasia, and of the conclusions that can be drawn from such lesions. If any one thing is needed to make the data of aphasia more reliable in the future and more be perfectly understood by those accustomed to him. He subsequently suffered from necrosis of the jaw and died. At the necropsy a large area of softening was found in the right hemisphere, involving part of the white substance beneath the Rolandic area and the island of Reil. The left hemisphere was normal." 376 77*6* Faculty of Speech. utilizable as scientific evidence in support of, or in behalf of, certain theories of the genesis of speech and its locali- zation, it is that the patient be studied methodically and carefully, and when the case comes to autopsy that the findings be recorded in definite, conventional, and scien- tific language, particularly the seat, extent, and nature of the lesions that are found, and that there be depicted on a chart which represents the usual convolutional relations and fissuration of the brain the seat and extent of the lesion as it manifests itself on the cortex. A study of the subcortical lesions, to be of any considerable value, must be done with the microscope after the tissues are properly hardened and stained. In the conduct of an autopsy of this kind a number of points are to be noted. After the removal of the calva- rium, the condition of the pia should be particularly re- marked, as knowledge to be obtained from its color, its adherence, and its consistence may be of much help in interpreting the nature as well as the duration of the lesion in the brain, especially if the lesion be of the cortex. Not infrequently aphasic patients will have a complexity of symptoms that indicates the destruction of one or more of the speech centres, and yet before death such a patient may develop symptoms that are to be ex- plained only by positing disease of other parts of the brain. Observation and study of the macroscopic appear- ances of the lesions in such a case, their color, consistence, etc., will allow us to say with much positiveness that one lesion is so recent that it could not have been the cause of symptoms of long standing, and that another lesion bears the imprint of ancientness, and therefore must account r Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 377 for the symptoms of similar duration. To be noted, then, are the color of the softening, the consistence, the exact location, particularly as contrasted with the undiseased hemisphere of the other side, the extent and number of the lesions, and the state of the blood-vessels. After this the consistence of the parts surrounding the area of soft- ening should be carefully determined in order that one may estimate how extensive the secondary changes are which, though not sufficiently advanced to produce soften- ing, may yet have reached a pathological state that robs the area of its function. It should be particularly noted if the lesion limits itself to areas of the brain to which have been allocated by phy- siologists and clinicists special centres for language, or if a number of such areas are involved, thus constituting the anatomical foundation of mixed or compound aphasia. After the surfaces of the hemispheres have been carefully examined and any lesions found there specifically noted, then the degree to which such cortical lesions have ex- tended into the substance of the brain and the subcortical location and extent of the lesions should be determined. This is by far the most difficult part of the autopsy. It is especially these subcortical lesions that should be most carefully studied, both in their extent and in their relation to, and separation from, the superambient cortex. Sev- eral cases of subcortical motor aphasia have been carefully studied, but the authors of such studies have interpreted them on the basis that the symptoms were due to the destruction of fibres of projection going from the foot of the third frontal convolution of the left side; that is, lesion of the fibres which are described as constituting the 378 The Faculty of Speech. inferior pediculo-frontal fascicle. Now, if the existence of this fascicle is denied, that is, if the third frontal convolu- tion has no projection fibres, then the aphasic symptoms resulting from destruction of the parts of the cortex sub- jacent to it must be explained in another way. I believe they can be more convincingly interpreted in a way that has already been indicated. The differentiation of subcortical motor aphasia from cortical motor aphasia has been most serviceable in widen- ing our conception and in expanding our knowledge of the entire subject of aphasia, but much that is desirable can still be done in this direction. The lesions accompanying subcortical motor aphasia have been considered to some extent in the chapter on motor aphasia, so that it is un- necessary to enter very fully into the subject here. To show the distance that such lesions may be from the cortex in cases of clinically typical subcortical motor aphasia, I may refer to an instance published by Banti. 1 A man, sixty-two years old, who had never been able to read or to write, developed immediately after an attack of apo- plexy hemiplegia of the right side and inability to talk. He understood questions that were asked him, but when he endeavored to respond the only sound that came forth was a confused unintelligible sound resembling " ti ti ti ti ti." He died five years after the first attack, there being in the mean time no essential change in his condi- tion. At the autopsy there was found an apoplectic cica- trix of a brownish-yellow color at the level of the internal capsule between the lenticular nucleus and the thalamus ; that is, in the anterior part of the posterior segment of the 1 Banti : Loc. tit. Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 379 capsule. Dejerine 1 has published an observation very similar to this. The patient was a man sixty-seven years old, who had right-side hemiplegia and aphasia of a number of years' standing, and who was absolutely unable to speak aloud. He could, however, whisper some words that were recognizable. There was no agraphia or para- graphia. He died eleven years after the beginning of the illness, and'on autopsy there were found three small foci of softening situated in the interior of the hemisphere, one in the middle of the internal capsule, another in the caudate nucleus, and a third in the fibres of the white sub- stance subjacent but some distance removed from the foot of the third frontal convolution. Although some writers, such as Brissaud, are unwilling to admit this case to the category of true subcortical motor aphasia, there would not have been any objection to its admission if all fibres coming from the articulatory areas of the cortex had been involved. The plan suggested by Dejerine for the conduct of the autopsy after the superficies of the cortex has been studied is probably the most useful one, although no hard-and-fast rule can be laid down for one's guidance in this matter. Dejerine's plan is the one that was followed by Vialet and Mirallie, whose labors have done so much to put sensory aphasia, cortical and subcortical, on a satisfactory founda- tion. The exact position and extent of the lesions having been noted, a division of the brain should be effected that leaves the diseased region intact. The usual method of division is that recommended by Flechsig, which consists of a horizontal cut passing through the head of the cau- 1 Dejerine : Loc. fit. 380 Tlie Faculty of Speech. date nucleus and the median part of the thalamus. It is done in the following way : After the skull cap has been sawed in the usual way it is left in place, and a knife introduced between the borders of the severed bone cuts the brain horizontally, en bloc, from without inward. This method of section facilitates particularly study of the central masses and of the internal capsule. The brain is then hardened and a plaster cast taken of the two pieces of the hemisphere. This gives an exact representation of the convolutions and is of the greatest service in orienting one later when the microscopical sections are made. The further handling of the tissues to render them susceptible for coloration is the same as that for ordinary brain tissue, and does not call for special description. Naturally, a special microtome is necessary to make the sections, which are cut horizontally through the length of the hemisphere, and particular care is called for in hand- ling these large sections. Mirallie has pointed out that, to use the method systematically and regularly and to get the greatest service from it, it is necessary to take every twentieth or thirtieth section and make a counter-drawing of the surface of the section on a piece of polished glass. This counter-drawing, used in connection with casts of the cortex that have been taken, makes it very easy to tell just what levels and what parts of the cortex the various numbered sections represent. The investigations that have been carried out in this manner by Dejerine, by Vialet, Mirallie, Redlich, Wyllie, and others have been the means of advancing our knowl- edge of sensory aphasia, and especially of subcortical sensory aphasia, to a degree that is not easy to overesti- Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 381 mate. In fact, it must be said that the reliable ana- tomical data of sensory aphasia have come from the investigators whose names have just been mentioned, although we do not mean to say that those of other pathologists and clinicians, such as Bastian, Seguin, Uenschen, Pick, Wilbrand, and others, are not of the greatest importance. The monograph of Vialet which appeared in 1893, em- bodying the report of several cases of sensory aphasia which the author had studied personally and with Dejerine, marked an epoch in the knowledge not only of the cere- bral centres of vision and the intracerebral visual mecha- nism, but also of sensory aphasia. These cases and a few others which I shall presently mention are of such im- portance in exposing the pathological anatomy of sensory aphasia that every writer who essays to describe the mor- bid anatomy of aphasia must cite them. I shall first refer to the findings in one of Vialet's patients, in whom the symptom was left homonymous hemianopsia, pure cortical hemianopsia. On removal of the brain an ancient spot of softening was found in the anterior one-fourth of the cuneus. Serial sections, made in the manner described above, showed that destruction of tissue was very much more extensive than the appear- ance of the lesion on the cortex indicated. The softening involved the anterior two-thirds of the cuneus, the an- terior part of the calcarine fissure and the tissue in which the parieto-occipital fissure pushes its way, and the foot of the cuneus reaching as far as the foot of the hippo- campus. In short, there was softening of the entire area supplied by the anterior branch of the occipital artery. 382 The Faculty of Speech. The consequent secondary degeneration involved the optic radiations of Gratiolet and the interhemispherical asso- ciation fibres of the fibrae callosae. The internal genicu- late body, the thalamus with the exception of the outer part of the pulvinar, the anterior quadrigeminal tubercle, and the entire internal capsule, as well as the foot of the peduncle, were entirely spared. In a case of this kind there was naturally no aphasia, and no disturbance of reading except that conditioned by loss of vision. It is for this reason that I refer to the case here. The disability of such a patient to read is entirely the same as that which might result from destruction of one-half of the retina or other peripheral defect, and it is grossly misleading to speak of it as " disturbance of read- ing." One of the cases described by Dejerine and Vialet was one of pure word blindness. The patient had right homony- mous hemianopsia, associated with inability to read letters and words. He could write voluntarily and from dicta- tion, but he could not copy correctly. The autopsy showed a focus of softening evidently of long standing, situated in the base of the cuneus and the posterior por- tion of the lingual and the fusiform gyri. Examination of microscopical sections of the brain showed that the softening did not confine itself to the cortex, but that it could be traced in the depths of the white substance from the calcarine fissure to the ependyma of the ventricle, where there was complete destruction of the tapetum, the optic radiations of Gratiolet, and the inferior longitudinal fascicle at the level of the lower wall of the occipital horn. In addition, there was found secondary degeneration of Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 383 the radiations of Gratiolet, that pass beneath the field of Wernicke in the auditory area. Another case was one of complete sensory aphasia, the patient being both word blind and word deaf. The area of softening involved the posterior parts of the first and second temporal gyri, the angular gyrus, the larger part of the external surface of the occipital lobe, the pole of the lobe being spared. In this case microscopical examina- tion showed that not only the cortex but the white sub- stance of the parietal and occipital lobes was affected. The three layers of fibres which border the external sur- face of the lateral ventricle were degenerated. The in- ferior longitudinal fascicle and the optic radiations were only partially destroyed. In fact, the inferior longi- tudinal fascicle was remarkably well preserved in all its in- ternal parts and in the interior walls of the ventricles. The lesion destroyed the cortex of the posterior part of the first temporal and all the inferior parietal, reaching as far for- ward as the foot of the ascending parietal and the pos- terior part of the island. The posterior part of the in- ternal capsule from its beginning in the lower part of the ovale, as far as the inferior^part, was degenerated. The optic radiations were matted by the first lesion to the upper part of the thalamus, and this seemingly had served to preserve the integrity of these fibres at lower levels. In contradiction to case three, this case showed that the anterior part of the field of Wernicke was degenerated. The fifth case was one of true sensory aphasia, word blindness. On autopsy a spot of softening the size of a silver dollar was found in the angular gyrus. Microscopi- cally it was seen that the lesion began at the point of the 384 The Faculty of Speech. ventricle which penetrated the softened mass, and the de- generation involved the bundle of the white sagittal sub- stance, the radiations of Gratiolet in varying degrees of severity, the fibnis callosre, and the long occipito-temporal association fibres. The two forceps were intact. The fibres situated on the external border of the ventricle suffered particularly. In the upper part of the thalamus there were two kinds of lesion : the posterior one very marked, extended into the zone of the optic radiations ; the anterior occupied the corona radiata at the level of the posterior part of the thalamus, the secondary degener- ation following along the course of the inferior longi- tudinal fascicle and the optic radiations, although the tapetum was preserved. A communication concerning a case of pure word blind- ness, reported by Hoisholt ' as a case of word blindness and music blindness without agraphia, is accompanied by a report of the autopsy, which shows how a lesion beginning in the posterior pole of the brain and producing symp- toms of subcortical or pure sensory aphasia may extend forward until it implicates some of the speech centres themselves. Hoisholt's patient was a musician, sixty-three years old, addicted to alcohol. Entirely cognizant of time and place, he was somewhat confused and his memory of recent events was impaired. His language was coherent, and speech was normal, both in form and arrangement. The intelligence of the man, and the comprehension of what was spoken to him, likewise seemed to be normal. He 'Hoisholt: " A Case of Pure Word Blindness." Occidental Medical Times, vol. vii., p. 483, 1893. Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 385 was able to spell words correctly, and also to write properly from dictation his name and a number of short English words, but there was an inability to read what had been written, even his own name. He would generally read the letters of the alphabet correctly, but was unable to read the smallest words. The ability to see and to recog- nize objects at a distance was preserved. He played from memory the most difficult passages without a fault, but when requested to play by note he tried to do so and failed, hesitating and playing something not before him. He was unable to name correctly any written notes. There was no trace of paralysis. Hearing was impaired and there was complaint of imperfect vision. Careful examination showed that there was left homonymous hemianopsia. For a time there was some improvement in his general condition, but finally the visual defect became more pronounced, the fields of vision becoming more and more contracted until there was total blindness, while the pupils grew larger and failed to react to light. Death resulted from cystitis. Upon post-mortem examination the whole occipital lobe of the left side of the brain pre- sented a yellowish-green color, and, viewed from above, appeared to be depressed below the general level. The convolutions of this area were reduced in size. These changes extended forward and upward as far as the angu- lar and supramarginal gyri, upon which they impinged, and inward along the median surface of the occipital lobe, the tip of which was considerably softened. The posterior part of the right hemisphere was of a yellowish-red color, from the occipital lobe upward and forward, a little beyond the limits of the change of color upon the left side. The con- volutions were flattened, but not so narrow or contracted as those of the opposite side. The cortical substance around the posterior extremity of the temporo-sphenoidal 25 386 The Faculty of Speech. convolution (angular gyrus) was somewhat depressed below the level of the surrounding surface, and presented several hemorrhagic spots varying in size from that of a pinhead to a pea, some of them extending through the whole thick- ness of the cortex. Smaller hemorrhages were also visi- ble on the median surface of the occipital lobe in the fusiform lobule. Although it is out of place to discuss here the clinical features of this case in relation to the anatomical lesion, it is appropriate to call attention to the fact that in the beginning the lesion of the posterior cerebral artery mani- fested its destructive effect in the interior of the occipital lobe and as the lesion extended posteriorly and anteriorly it caused respectively true cortical anopsia and true word blindness. A case of sensory aphasia reported by Dejerine and Mirallie is accompanied by the results of a most carefully conducted autopsy and microscopical examination of the diseased focus and its consequent secondary degeneration. The patient had presented typical symptoms of sensory aphasia, associated in the beginning with right hemiparesis, which soon disappeared. The course of the disease was attended with considerable amelioration of the word deaf- ness, but the complete alexia, total agraphia, paraphasia, and jargonaphasia were very pronounced and remained until the end. There was no optic aphasia or mind blind- ness, but on account of the difficulty of communicating with the patient it had been impossible to determine the absence of hemianopsia. The autopsy showed a lesion of the supramarginal and of the angular gyrus, and also of the part immediately ad- Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 387 jacent to the latter in the inferior parietal lobule of the left side. On microscopical examination a large focus of softening was found surrounding the marginal fissure of the island, and destruction of that part of the inferior parietal lobule known as the angular gyrus. In the white substance of the ascending frontal convolution there were three foci of degeneration. There was also descending, retrograde degeneration in the optic radiations, the inferior longitudinal fascicle, the pulvinar, the external geniculate body, and of the internal capsule and the foot of the peduncle. In this case the three distinct and primitive foci were : i. A focus, by far the most important, situated in the posterior marginal fissure of the island, which had severed the base of the inferior parietal convolution just at the point of junction with the island. This focus comprised all the supramarginal gyrus, and destroyed the white fibres of this convolution and the cortex as far forward as the fissure of Sylvius. 2. A focus of softening occupying the base of the Rolandic operculum at a point where it is continuous with the island. 3. A focus measuring only a few millimetres in diameter, just at the crest of the angular gyrus, and which caused a degeneration very strictly confined in the middle part of the white substance of this convolution. These foci had each caused secon- dary degenerations, the principal one of which had sev- ered completely the retrolenticular segment of the internal capsule, the radiations of the thalamus, and the inferior longitudinal fascicle. Behind this point the degeneration followed the external face of the lateral ventricle and reached to the occipital lobe. The fibres of the corpus 388 The Faculty of Speech. callosum which turn about the posterior extremity of the lateral ventricle were degenerated, forming at this level a zone of translucent fibres, standing out in contrast to the normal fibres of the corpus callosum. Anteriorly the de- generation penetrated the posterior part of the thalamus. The pulvinar was much atrophied, and its radiating fibres had, disappeared. The external geniculate body and the white fibres surrounding it showed slight departures from normal. In this case, the original lesion occupying the posterior marginal fissure of the island had separated the angular gyms and all its connections from the zone of language. Although the visual images of words which are stored in the angular gyrus were in a measure preserved, they were no longer accessible to the zone of language, and this iso- lation of the angular gyrus had caused from the standpoint of its function bearing on internal language the same re- sults as a direct lesion of the centre for the visual images and words would have. Mirallie, in reporting the case, says very truthfully that this is the first time that a case of this kind had been studied microscopically, and the scientific accuracy with which the lesion and its subse- quent secondary degeneration were depicted allows of true interpretation of the symptoms of the case, for without such microscopic examination the totality of the symptoms would have been much less intelligible. The anatomical findings in a case of sensory aphasia under personal obser- vation have been detailed in connection with the history of the case, and need not be repeated here. The morbid anatomy of a case of subcortical sensory aphasia of the verbal type (pure verbal blindness of De- Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 389 jerine, subcortical alexia of many writers) has recently been studied with great care by Redlich. The patient, a sixty-four-year-old man, had had for a considerable time loss of visual acuteness, dependent upon optic atrophy, but this did not prevent him from discharging the duties of a scrivener. Later he developed a right-side motor and sensory hemiparesis, right-side hemianopsia, and word blindness. The hemiparesis was transitory. After the first fleeting disturbances following the shock there was total literal and verbal alexia, but no mind blindness. Writing was undisturbed, both voluntarily and from dic- tation. At the autopsy there was found a spot of softening in the left occipital lobe immediately around the calcarine fissure in the lingual and fusiform lobules, extending as far forward in the medullary substance as the posterior horn of the ventricle. The splenium of the corpus cal- losum, the posterior part of the thalamus, and the tail of the caudate nucleus were softened. Microscopical exami- nation showed destruction of all the optic radiations ; and the forceps major as well as a part of the forceps minor were degenerated. The inferior longitudinal fascicle was also the seat of degeneration. The cortex of the angular gyrus was entirely intact. The left fornix, the anterior portion of the cornu ammonis, and the tapetum showed some spots of softening, which, according to Redlich, had interrupted the connection between the right visual area and the left visual centre, the author believing with H. Sachs that this pathway is through the tapetum. On ac- count of the interruption of the inferior longitudinal fascicle, the connection between the left visual centre and 390 The Faculty of Speech. the zone of language was interrupted. This case is in reality one of the most important that have yet been con- tributed to the fund of exact knowledge concerning the pathology of aphasia, and although its reporter draws what I believe to be some unwarrantable inferences, which are criticised in the chapter on " Sensory Aphasia," particularly concerning the neural basis of writing, it nevertheless puts beyond doubt the existence of this form of aphasia. Redlich is of the opinion that cases of this kind would be better designated by the term intercortical aphasia than subcortical. There are a number of other observations that have been of great importance in corroborating the conclusions of Dejerine and his pupils in regard to sensory aphasia. Wyllie has published the report and autopsy findings of a patient with subcortical visual aphasia, the two promi- nent symptoms having been word blindness and homony- mous hemianopsia. The lesion was found to be a soft- ening of the white matter in the floor of the posterior horn of the left lateral ventricle. It was due apparently to oc- clusion of one of the branches of the posterior cerebral artery. The softening confined itself strictly to the white matter, and, though the gray substance of the convolutions immediately over it was in a somewhat sunken and atrophied state, there could be no doubt that this was merely a condition consequent to the destruction of tissues underneath. Examination of the sections of the brain showed that the atrophy extended from the tip of the under surface of the occipital lobe, where the dilated posterior horn of the ventricle reached to within an eighth of an inch of the sur- Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 391 face forward as far as the middle of the crus cerebri. That is to say, the convolutions affected were the lingual and fusiform, together with the posterior half of the gyrus hippocampus. There was no involvement of the angular gyrus. In this case the disease of the white matter of the occipital lobe had involved the fibres connecting the an- gular convolution with the right and left primary visual areas, thus causing word blindness ; and on account of the interruptions of the radiations of Gratiolet from the pri- mary centre for vision in the left occipital lobe there resulted hemianopsia. A second case recorded by the same writer is no less interesting. A man seventy years old developed a slight motor and sensory hemiplegia with well-marked right lateral homonymous hemianopsia. The hemiplegia soon almost completely disappeared, but the hemianopsia and total word blindness continued. There were a slight de- gree of word deafness and amnesia of nouns. At the autopsy the zone of language macroscopically was entirely normal. The chief morbid appearances were found on the under surface of the occipital lobe, involving princi- pally the fourth temporal lobe, bordering the occipital con- volution, the hippocampal convolution, the lingual con- volution, the anterior part of the cuneus, and the calcarine fissure. On section of the brain, it was seen that beneath the thin cortex there was such marked atrophy of the white matter that the cortex was in direct connection with the ependyma of the ventricular horn. At this region of greatest atrophy, i.e., the under surface of the occipital lobe, about the middle of the inferior temporal occipital or the fourth temporal convolution, there was a marked 392 The Faculty of Speech. depression on the surface of the brain about two inches in length and an inch in breadth. This depression marked the situation of a cyst-like cavity where the gray matter and the subjacent white matter had been almost entirely destroyed. Serieux has recorded a case of word blindness with agraphia caused by destruction of the angular gyrus, and, although the report is not accompanied by the details of a microscopical examination, the strictness with which the lesion was confined to the inferior parietal lobule makes it a very important and valuable case, more, however, as substantiating the allotted function of the angular gyrus than as a contribution to the morbid anatomy of aphasia. The same writer has also published a detailed account of a case of mind blindness associated with word blind- ness. The patient, a woman, sixty-two years old, had a stroke followed by transient paralysis, word blindness, and agraphia, mind blindness, word deafness, and paraphasia. The patient's condition had bettered somewhat, when she died suddenly from an intercurrent pneumonia. Unfor- tunately, the lesions in the brain were multiple, there being oh the left side a softening in the inferior parietal and also a limited focus of softening in the posterior ex- tremity of the first and second temporal convolutions. On the right side of the brain a softening, somewhat more extensive, was found in practically the same areas. A very similar observation to this has been communi- cated by Bruns, 1 except in this case the clinical phenomena 1 Bruns : Neurologisches Centralblatt, Nos. 17 and 18, 1888; Nos. i and 2, 1894. Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 393 were those characteristic of complete sensory aphasia, with right homonymous hemianopsia. The autopsy showed that the greater part of the white substance of the first tem- poral convolution and the adjacent parietal convolution were in a state of advanced softening. There are few better cases to illustrate' the lesions of sensory aphasia, type of word deafness, than a case pub- lished by Leva. 1 The symptoms pointed most unerringly to a lesion of the auditory centre, and on autopsy there was found a yellowish, fluctuating sunken-in area of soft- ening in the middle segment of the left upper temporal convolution and almost exactly two millimetres from the upper border of the second temporal convolution. The softening was found to be a cyst rilled with milky, cloudy fluid. It measured in every diameter about three centi- metres. Anteriorly it extended to one centimetre from the tip of the first frontal gyms and posteriorly to two centimetres from the posterior end of the same gyrus. Internally it approached the two external segments of the lenticular nucleus. In the second frontal convolu- tion of the left side there was another small focus of softening. Although there are on record many cases of aphasia ac- companied with more or less autopsical details, I shall have to content myself with the relation of one more case taken from the literature, and that a case of subcortical word deafness, published by Pick. The patient was a typical case clinically. She could understand neither spoken 1 Leva : " Localisation der Aphasien." Arch. f. path. Anat. und Phys. und f. klin. Med.. Berlin, Mai, Bd. cxxxii. (Folge xiii., Bd. ii.), H. 2, p. 333, 1893. 394 The Faculty of Speech, speech nor melodies. On removal of the brain, no de- parture from normal could be made out save that the con- volutions seemed to be a little sunken. Examination of the superior gyri of the temporal lobes showed that they were somewhat abnormal in consistence and color. On the right side the first temporal and a large part of the second temporal, the island of Reil, and the adjacent parts of the anterior central convolution and of the inferior frontal convolution were transformed into a whitish-yellow firm mass. The environment of these softened parts was more dense. The lateral ventricle of this side was slightly dis- tended and contained some yellow serum. Section of this hemisphere made after the plan of Pitres showed that the cortex of the affected convolutions and the white substance were the seat of yellow softening. This softening com- prised, in a section made through the ascending frontal convolution, the region just in front of the external cap- sule and most external part of the lenticular nucleus. The globus pallidus and the internal capsule were intact. In the left hemisphere the posterior part of the first tem- poral convolution and the supramarginal gyrus were softened, the same as on the right side. Frontal sections of this hemisphere showed that the softening was super- ficial and affected no part of the external capsule or the central nuclei. The island of Reil was intact. The softened areas in this hemisphere had a more gelatinous aspect and the substance of the adjoining convolutions was firmer. On account of the meagreness of the ana- tomical details in this case, and on account of the exten- siveness of the lesion, it bears only indirect testimony in behalf of the exact localization of the aphasia lesions. Morbid Anatomy of Aphasia. 395 In a word, the pathology of true aphasia is the pathology of a lesion that injures the zone of language, and of sub- cortical aphasia a lesion of the immediate incoming and outgoing pathways by virtue of whose integrity the speech centres manifest their function. CHAPTER XI. REMARKS ON THE TREATMENT OF APHASIA. I SHALL discuss the treatment of aphasia very briefly, from the standpoint of the physician and surgeon, and from the pedagogue's point of view. Unhappily neither the physician nor the pedagogue can be of considerable assistance to the vast majority of aphasic patients. The medicinal treatment depends entirely upon the nature of the lesion that causes the aphasic symptom complex. If the lesion be a focus of encephalomalacia, then all that can be expected of medicinal treatment is to assist nature to prevent further destruction of tissue, and par- ticularly to assist in preventing a repetition of the imme- diate exciting cause of the softening. On the other hand if the lesion be a gummatous meningitis, or an isolated gummatous formation, in the zone of language or the sub- cortical speech tracts, and these can be diagnosticated as such, medicinal treatment is of the greatest value. A case of sensory aphasia recorded by Bramwell and cited in another chapter is in evidence. This patient had the profoundest symptoms of sensory aphasia, yet 'she fully recovered under the influence of antisyphilitic medica- tion. Another case in which the results of antisyphilitic treatment were most gratifying, even though the symp- toms did not completely yield to medication, has very Remarks on the Treatment of Aphasia. 397 recently been published by Mantle. 1 The difficulty in cases of this kind is oftenest with the etiological diagnosis. Usually the patient is not in condition to vouchsafe any information concerning himself, and as his family is, as a rule, ignorant of such matters, the physician is compelled often, if he has not been familiar with the patient's his- tory, to make a diagnosis of previous syphilitic infection on less satisfactory data than are ordinarily considered es- sential. Personally I am inclined to suspect a luetic origin in every case of aphasia coming on abruptly that occurs before the fifth decade of life, when valvular trouble of the heart, the^rjeeeTTf^possession of acute disease, and injury can be excluded. It would be a work of supererogation to repeat in detail the treatment applicable to the different forms of aphasia, for it will occur to every one who has in mind the various causes of aphasia. The treatment for aphasia in one pa- tient may be just as different from the treatment appli- cable to the next one as the causes are different. For in- stance, in the beginning the treatment of a uraemic patient is venesection if the patient has not an organic form of renal disease ; yet this kind of treatment would be fatal to a patient whose aphasia was dependent upon autoch- thonic thrombosis. When aphasic symptoms develop slowly without fever and with symptoms of increasing intracranial irritation and pressure, then tumor and abscess must be thought of. In making the diagnosis and the differentiating diagnosis one must be guided by the general rules applicable to the solution of these problems. When there are grounds for 1 Mantle : British Medical Journal, February 6th, 1897, p. 325. 398 The Faculty of Speech. the belief that the lesion is of a luetic nature, then the ad- ministration of mercury and iodide of potassium cannot be carried out with too great promptness and attention. Syphilitic lesions that develop some years after the pri- mary infection are, it is universally conceded, more amenable to the iodide of potassium than to all other measures combined. If, however, the date of the primary lesion is not very remote, then the administration of iodide should be simultaneous with the mercury, or the one should follow the other in the shape of a course of the ope and then of the other. The treatment of aphasia dependent upon organic dis- ease such as tumor, abscess, purulent meningitis, and focal disease of any nature, does not differ from the treatment of these conditions when aphasia is not present. When their presence is attended by symptoms which seem to indicate that they are amenable to surgical treatment their removal should not be delayed. In fact, the aphasia is oftentimes the localizing symptom that makes diagnosis positive and operation possible. The case which I have cited of Zaufal and Pick, an abscess of the brain successfully treated by operation, is in evidence. To enumerate the symptoms caused by focal, cortical, or subcortical disease that may cause aphasia would be a repetition of much that has been said in the chapter on " Diagnosis," and elsewhere. The seemingly widespread belief that aphasia is almost exclusively an on-hanger of the apoplectic state seems to necessitate, however, emphasizing the fact that some manifestations of the complexity of symptoms constituting aphasia are of great diagnostic im- Remarks o?t the Treatment of Aphasia. 399 portance in nearly every disease affecting the brain. It is a common symptom in the recently recognized and de- scribed disease acute hemorrhagic encephalitis ; it is per- haps the most constant symptom of abscess of the brain, on account of the pathogenetic relationship of disease of the middle ear and cerebral abscess, and it is not uncommon at some stage in the career of general paresis and of mul- tiple, insular, and diffuse cerebral sclerosis; while its occurrence after injury which may cause localized inflam- mation of the meninges or of the brain itself, hemor- rhage, depression of bone and spicules, is not unusual. Naturally, in order to produce aphasia, these factors must manifest their injurious activity on the speech centres, their interconnections, projections, or the imme- diate pathways leading to them ; to be less specific, on the left hemisphere in right-handed persons and vice versa. The form of aphasia that any of these diseases and accidents may cause will depend upon the location of the lesion and not on its nature. The only variety dependent upon any of the above-enumerated conditions that is very uncommon is the subcortical form of motor aphasia ; while the subcortical sensory variety is correspondingly frequent. This is readily understood, if we recall that the part of the brain which must be diseased to cause subcortical motor aphasia is well protected from injury and has no par- ticular relationship to the important factors that condition abscess of the brain. Oftentimes a careful consideration of the symptom aphasia in these diseases will be the most important factor in determining whether or not an operation shall be done, i.e., whether the lesion is sufficiently localizable to warrant 400 The Faculty of Speech. advising the surgeon to trepan the skull and attempt to remove the materies morbi. Taking it all in all, the question of the medicinal treat- ment of aphasia never comes up for consideration. The question that does present is : How shall we treat the con- dition of which aphasia is the symptom ? To answer that question satisfactorily requires an intimate knowledge of the therapeusis of all the diseases, functional and organic, that have been enumerated in the chapter on " Etiology" with which aphasia may be associated. Treatment may consist of such a simple matter as the interdiction of alcohol in a case of toxic dyslexia, or it may require the combined skill of the physician and surgeon to diagnosticate and re- move an abscess or tumor. The treatment of the dynamic aphasias is a different matter from the treatment of the organic aphasias. In the former all that is necessary is to remove the cause and the symptom will disappear, while in the latter the cause may be removed and the pathological condition which it has excited still continues and with it the aphasia. The pedagogical treatment of aphasia is a matter of re- cent development. It has been the legitimate result of an inquiry into the physiological and psychological antece- dents of articulate speech, and of clinical observations that when a young person became aphasic, even though the lesion was a very severe and extensive one, the faculty of speech was restored to him. Moreover, almost from the very beginning of the history of aphasia, it has been recognized that, even when the so-called " speech centre," meaning Broca's area, was completely destroyed, the patient regained occasionally some capacity to speak individual Remarks on the Treatment of Aphasia. 401 words or a number of words. Various hypotheses have been formulated to explain these occurrences, the most widely accepted apparently being that of Jackson, who suggested twenty years ago that the " uneducated centre" of the opposite side is in a way related to conventional, emotional, and other forms of what he terms " degraded" speech, in contradistinction to intellectual speech. This is the theory accepted by many writers to-day. Recently Wyllie has framed a theory along somewhat the same lines on the " overflow of education into the opposite hemi- sphere;" the hemisphere that contains the zone of lan- guage takes up all that it can in the way of education, and that which it is not equal to taking up flows over into the other hemisphere. As I have said in a previous chapter, the entire subject of the repossession of the speech faculty in patients in whom it has been lost must needs be looked at to-day from another standpoint than it was a few years ago, when the various forms of subcortical aphasia had not been satisfactorily differentiated. It seems to me that in the light of our present knowledge of aphasia it must be granted that not only do the areas of the opposite hemisphere sometimes under the stress of education un- dertake, in a very incomplete way, the speech function of the destroyed area of the hemisphere phylogenetically and ontogenetically prepared to carry on the speech faculty, but that the immediate environmental areas of the speech centres of the left hemisphere may take up the function in part ; secondly, that the opposite hemisphere, the one that has the zone of language ontogenetically developed, is not an uneducated hemisphere at all, but that it is in one sense just as much educated as the hemisphere in which the 26 4O2 The Faculty of Speech. zone of language is situated. It must needs be admitted that there are a general auditory area, a general visual area, and a general kinaesthetic area in the right hemisphere as well as in the left hemisphere, and that in-coming stimuli make a similar impression on it as they do on the so-called " educated" hemisphere. These impressions are bilateral in reception but unilateral in interpretation. This uni- laterality of interpretation is determined by commissural fibres of the corpus callosum. Now the same factors that determine right-handedness determine also that the left hemisphere shall be the executive speech side, but the elementary work is done on both sides. It seems to me that so far every one who is willing to accept the sugges- tions of experimental physiology must go. How many are willing to admit that the execution of speech is an auto- matic act and requires no conscious preparation, if process of anatomical completion is not considered " preparation," is another matter. Those who believe that the execution of speech is an automatic act find it easier to explain how an approach to or an unfinished automatism can be as- sumed by the opposite hemipshere which is educated but which is not intended to be automatic, and especially in young children, in whom the habit of automatic activity has not become fixed by continued practice. I do not think it at all improbable that if a healthy child should be kept mute until it was from five to six years of age, that is, until such a time as the neuro-muscular appa- ratus subserving speech was fully developed, he would go through the lalling and other stages of speech imperfec- tions as do children who begin " to learn" to talk be- fore the executive parts are fully developed. The words Remarks on the Treatment of Aphasia. 403 that such a child used (which would, of course, depend upon the words that he had heard) might, I believe, be perfectly formed. In other words, the execution of speech would be as automatic as breathing, and that in mankind speech is an endowment more than an acquirement. Furthermore, the factors that determine the seat of this automatic activity are the conditions that we have here- tofore supposed determined the education of the left hemisphere. A most remarkable case bearing on this matter has re- cently been published by Bastian. The patient was a boy, twelve years old, who had been subject to epileptic fits at intervals. The first of these occurred in infancy, when the patient was about nine months old. Toward the end of the second year the fits seemed to have ceased. The hear- ing was good and the child appeared to be of average intelli- gence to be well, in fact, in all respects, except that he did not talk. When nearly five years old the little fellow had not spoken a single word, and about this time two eminent physicians were consulted in regard to his " dumbness." But before the expiration of another twelve months, on the occasion of an accident happening to a favorite toy, he suddenly exclaimed, " What a pity !" although he had never previously spoken a word. The same words could not be repeated, nor were others spoken, notwithstanding all entreaties, for a period of two weeks. Thereafter the boy progressed rapidly and speedily became most talkative, and spoke without the least sign of impediment or defect. One other point that has previously been mentioned. A number of the cases that have been reported to show the assumption of speech function by the opposite hemisphere 404 The Faculty of Speech. have, I hope, been conclusively shown to be dependent upon a subcortical lesion and not upon destruction of a speech centre, and the partial or complete recovery of speech was commensurate with a disappearance of the conditions that had determined the partial interruption of the conducting fibres. In these cases recovery of speech has gone on pari passu with disappearance of other symptoms, such as hemi- plegia, for instance. In other cases in which the lesion - has been of the speech centres the partial repossession of speech has been due to the fact that the entire speech centre, which in the beginning of an aphasic attack was completely overthrown, has in a slight measure righted it- self after the exudative and -occlusive conditions have sub- sided. Then the patient finds himself in possession, to a very insignificant degree, of his previous speech endow- ment. In other cases there can be no question that the educated areas of the other hemisphere develop some ex- ecutive capacity. This is determined artificially, i.e., by education, and not ontogenetically as it is normally, except tp-the very slightest degree. \In^ brief, then, the education of an aphasic patient should consist in endeavoring to cause the centre or cen- tres in the left side of the brain that are not destroyed by the lesion which causes the aphasia to take the in- itiative in the primary recall of words, and complete the " circuit" necessary for speech by forcing the educated opposite side to supply a centre similar to that which has been destroyed^ For example, if the articulatory kinaes- thetic centre is detroyed, the primary revival of the word that should be spoken is through the auditory centre, and this calls up in temporal coincidence or succession the Remarks on the Treatment of Aphasia. 405 visual and the articulatory centres. The articulatory cen- tre, being destroyed, the speech impulse of the formed word cannot be completed, and the kinaesthetic articu- latory centre of the opposite side is acted upon through commissural fibres in just the same way as the articulatory centre of the left side was through intercentral fibres, r~~~^ in the beginning. [TJ^- process of education is very slow and must be given artificial aid in the way of showing the patient what movements to make in order to get the variety of kinsesthesis of which it is desired to store up memories.! The utilization of this suggestion is in reality at the bot- tom of educating patients with cortical motor aphasia to speak. In these cases in which it is desired to supply the articu- latory kinaesthetic memories, everything is to be gained by the use of the physiological alphabet, educating the patient to master the letter sounds. Even the briefest considera- tion of the physiological alphabet would require more space than can be given, and I prefer to say nothing in the way of explanation of it rather than give a faulty and imperfect exposition of the subject. To those who would have a most readable article on the subject, the first chapter of Wyllie's "The Disorders of Speech" is recom- mended. When the auditory centre is diseased, then the task is to get a primary revival of the idea of words in the visual or the articulatory centre. This is a very much more difficult matter, because in the vast majority of peoples the primary revival takes place in the auditory centre, and when this is destroyed the patient is stranded, from a speech standpoint. The plan of education is in 406 The Facility of Speech. reality that which is used for deaf-mutes, who are taught to think by the revival of the word impulse by the visual centres, the revivification of visual symbols prompted by hand or lip movements. In case of those born deaf and blind the primary revival is in the articulatory kinaesthetic centre, which, in cases like that of Laura Bridgman, is con- ditioned by the tactile sense. In fact, it is in all those defectives who learn to read aloud by the use of raised type. 1 Patients with the auditory form of sensory aphasia should be patiently taught to repeat words the meaning of which is conveyed to them through other senses, the visual, tactual, and olfactory. In this way it is believed that generally the auditory area of the same side that is not destroyed or of the opposite side may develop some executive capacity. The treatment of sensory aphasia conditioned by destruc- tion of the visual centres is most unsatisfactory, and very little can be done to ameliorate the condition of such pa- tients, even though all modes of education be assiduously employed. An effort should be made to teach the patient the recognition of forgotten symbols in connection with the arousal of other memories of them, the auditory and the articulatory. In short, the pedagogical treatment of aphasia embraces the methods of the kindergarten and the methods for the instruction of those defective in one or more of the special senses. Even with their aid but little can be done. 1 I am aware that these cases are somewhat opposed to the contention that the primary revival of words is never in the articulatory centre, and possibly the position I have taken may have to be modified or altered by further investigation: CHAPTER XII. REMARKS ON THE MEDICO-LEGAL ASPECTS OF APHASIA. APHASIA is so frequently a symptom of mortal disease, and it is of so much more frequent occurrence in the aged than in the young, that its presence and occurrence often give rise to exigencies necessitating disposition of posses- sions in such a way as to satisfy later tribunals of justice. On the other hand, patients with aphasia are oftentimes so changed in demeanor, in conduct, and in appearance, and they respond to environmental conditions in a manner so different from that habitual to them when in health, that they are adjudged insane by the laity, and, unfortunately, occasionally by physicians as well. These two facts ne- cessitate a discussion of the testamentary capacity and the mental status of patients with aphasia. The literature bearing on the testamentary capacity of aphasic patients is not very extensive, and, by way of intro- duction, it may be said that much of it is valueless. A quarter of a century back the subject was discussed by Legrand du Saulle, 1 by Gallard/ by Bateman 3 and others, but, as these discussions were held before the subcortical 1 Gazette des Hopitaux, June and July, 1868, and idem, vol. lv., 1882. '-' ( 'Unique Medicale de la Pitie, reviewed in Le Journal de Medecine et de Chirurgie Pratiques, vol. xlviii., pp. 377-3 8 - cit. 408 The Faculty of Speecli. forms of sensory and motor aphasia were separated clini- cally and established on a firm anatomical basis, they are of comparatively slight value. For example, a case re- ported by the last-named of the trio mentioned above : An elderly man, about to be married, was stricken with right hemiplegia attended with aphasia. He, in anticipa- tion of death, desired to make a will for the benefit of his fiancee. The instrument was prepared by his physician, with whom the testator communicated by means of signs which the former understood. The testator put his corrob- oration on communications to the physician by making an affirmative or negative gesture when the latter repeated them after having written down what he understood the deponent to say. For instance, he held up the hand and extended five fingers ; then closed the fingers and extended them again ; and repeated this performance three times to indicate thirty. On being interrogated if he meant " thousand" he nodded affirmatively. He was then asked if he wished }\\^> fiancee to have .30,000 ; he again nodded assent. When asked if it was his wish that she should have this sum absolutely, he made a nod of negation. When asked if she was to have it during her life and then to have it revert to his own family, he signified assent. He signed the document by making his mark, but owing to the non-satisfaction of a technicality of the law the will was not admitted to probate, although it must be evident to every one that the mental faculties of the patient were intact and that he had subcortical motor aphasia. As I have already hinted, it is around the differentiation of the sub- cortical and cortical forms of aphasia that this entire ques- tion of testamentary capacity revolves. If it were neces- Medico-Legal Aspects of Aphasia. 409 sary to define the status of the aphasic patient's testamen- tary capacity dogmatically and in a few words, I should say that, although every case is a law unto itself, no one is of sound and disposing mind who has true aphasia aphasia due to lesion of the zone of language; while a person who has any form of subcortical aphasia, be it motor or sensory, may be, and usually is, capable of indulging in civil transactions, although there are exceptions to the rule. If the integrity of the primary speech centres con- stituting the zone of language is necessary for the full and legitimate genesis and notion of the word, then disease of any of them must be attended by disorder in the conception and in the idea of the word. And as it is necessary to employ words or their motor equivalent in the making of a will, it will readily be seen that the patient with true aphasia cannot do this in a way to satisfy the law. Yet a person may have had true aphasia, and have recovered sufficiently, either by the education of the opposite side of the brain, or by the assumption of function by unde- stroyed portions of the diseased centre, to know the cor- rect application of words and to use them rationally and intellectually. It is in these cases that the observations of the physician and the completeness with which he has examined and studied the case should have the greatest weight in deciding as to the patient's testamentary capacity. Given a person with subcortical aphasia, the matter is very different. In a case of uncomplicated subcortical motor aphasia, that is, not associated with lesion of other parts of the brain that might interfere with intellectual functioning of the brain, the individual may be in full- 410 The Faculty of Speech. est possession of his faculties, including internal speech, and have simply a supreme inability to externalize his mental content. For purpose of contrast he may indeed be compared with a man who is bound and gagged. Probably no one would contend that the latter is incapa- ble of making a will, although the inability to do so with- out outside aid must be very apparent. The testamentary capacity of patients with subcortical motor aphasia may, however, be impaired by coincident lesion of a nature similar to that causing the speech defect, occurring in other parts of the brain. For instance, a thrombus of lue- tic origin located subjacent to the executive motor speech centre may cause symptoms of subcortical motor aphasia, while coincident luetic disease of the blood-vessels in other parts of the brain may cause a degree of dementia inconsistent with the making of civil contract. But, as I have said, such superadded deficiencies must be detected or eliminated by the examination of the physician. Subcortical sensory aphasia, be it of the auditory or visual kind, offers a more serious obstacle to the testa- mentary capacity of the patient than does the subcortical motor form, because in the former the indifferent attitude and the inattentive and unnoticing demeanor, which these patients so often have, lead those about them, and others with whom they may come in contact, to look upon them as insane. The testimony of these of the laity before a Surrogate, or before twelve of their peers, often has great influence even though it is contradicted by testimony which has the misfortune to be called "expert." But if the aphasic symptoms be wholly of a subcortical nature and uncomplicated, the patient has no defect of internal Medico-Legal Aspects of Aphasia. 4 1 i speech, and because of the shortcomings of internal speech he should not be forced into the category of those of un- sound mind because he is obliged to borrow another's eyes or ears. It would be just as legitimate to contend that a highly myopic or deaf person is incapable of mak- ing a will because he cannot lay hands on his glasses or his trumpet. As has been said before, the analogy is over- drawn, because uncomplicated forms of subcortical sensory aphasia are extremely uncommon, and it is the complica- tions or coincident symptoms that add to the complexity of the question. Thus it will be seen that my position in this matter is materially different from that of many writers on this subject. For instance, I cannot agree with Cowers, who says : " Word deafness is incompatible with will-making, because it is impossible to know whether the testator really understands what is said to him." If the auditory pathways were the only ones through which a patient could be communicated with, this would be true ; but word deaf- ness may be complete without involvement of the higher auditory centre and with the central visual mechanism in- tact, and therefore the patient can be communicated with by writing. Even in a case of word deafness and word blindness there might still be no optic aphasia and the pa- tient could be communicated with by pantomime, sign language. Neither can I agree with Diller, 1 who says by way of introduction to a brief discussion on the medico- legal aspects of aphasia : " In such a study neither the site of the lesion nor the particular division or subdivision of aphasia present need be considered." For my part, I be- 1 Journal of Mental and Nervous Disease, May, 1894. 412 The Faculty of Speech. lieve that the determination of the variety of aphasia that the patient has is the most important thing in determin- ing the patient's capacity to make contracts, wills, checks, and indulge in other civil matters. No more can I sub- scribe to the statement that " Motor aphasia does not of necessity incapacitate the patient in will-making, etc. " If by motor aphasia is meant aphasia due to lesion of the cen- tre in which are stored the memories of articulation, then such aphasia does incapacitate the patient, for in every case of this kind there is some disturbance of internal speech, not to speak of external speech. In fact the ma- jority of cases that have become famous in this country on account of the litigation connected with them, in which physicians have given testimony of the testamentary capacity of the patient, such as the Beven case, analyzed by Hughes; 1 the Parrish case, referred to by Ray; 2 and a case recorded by Clark, 3 will be found on close scrutiny to be cases of subcortical aphasia, and it was because of the symptoms of the latter that such testimony could be given, although at the time when some of the cases occurred the subcortical forms of aphasia had not been separated. The need of making such differentiation in every case of aphasia when it is necessary to determine the testa- mentary capacity, or to make a determination of the mental status for other purpose, is well illustrated by the pertinent words of Hughlings Jackson on the subject: " Such a question as ' Can an'aphasic make a will ? ' cannot be answered any more than the question, ' Will a piece of 1 American Journal of Insanity, January, 1879, p. 410. 3 " Medical Jurisprudence of Insanity." 3 American Journal of Insanity, 1892-93, p. 291. Medico-Legal Aspects of Aphasia. 4 1 3 string reach across this room ? ' The question should be, can this or that person make a will ?" And the determina- tion of the variety of aphasia that he has will do more to answer this question than will anything else. Physicians are rarely called upon to decide the question of the responsibility of an aphasic in criminal processes ; but when they are, the same precepts should guide them in estimating the patient's mental responsibility as were laid down to determine his testamentary capacity. It should never be forgotten that the majority of cases of organic aphasia occur with diseases of the brain that put great inhibition upon the unfortunate victim's passions and emotions, and for the indulgence and manifestations of these he should not be held responsible in the same way as a normal man is. The physicial infirmities of patients with aphasia alone, especially the motor forms, usually spare them from crimes against person and State; while the speech shortcomings of sensory aphasia are of such a nature that patients must be cared for by others, and are therefore kept from indulging any such tendencies that might be prompted. Personally, I believe that a patient with subcortical aphasia, it matters not of what form, is as capable of determining between right and wrong, mctim and tuum, as a normal person, coincident disease of the brain that might impair his faculties being excluded. The same cannot be said for a patient who has lesion of the zone of language ; but in cases of this sort the even- tual determination must be reached from personal study of the case and not from its conforming to any hypothetical conditions. APPENDIX I. CONDUCTION APHASIA. IN the body of this work I have limited myself to a dis- cussion of the varieties of aphasia that have been substan- tiated by morbid anatomical changes found post mortem. No one doubts the reality of the forms of aphasia discussed in the text, although different interpretations have been put upon the symptomatic accompaniments of each of them by different writers, but all are in accord as to their occurrence. A number of writers, prompted mainly by theoretical considerations, have described several varieties of aphasia dependent upon lesions situated between the different speech centres. To these varieties they have given different names, according to the posited seat of the lesion. The most plausible of these subdivisions is one described originally by Wernicke, to which he gave the name " Leitungsaphasie," conduction aphasia (inter- central aphasia, connection aphasia). 1 Personally, I be- lieve that theoretically there are very good grounds for the differentiation of this form of sensory aphasia, but as 1 Bramvvell distinguishes two varieties of conduction aphasia: (i) con- nection aphasia due to interruption of the connection between the different cortical speech centres; and (2) commissural aphasia, due to interruption of the commissural connections between corresponding speech centres in the opposite hemispheres. He remarks that in the present state of our knowl- edge it is impossible to distinguish these commissural forms. The second distinction seems to me scarcely warranted. Conduction Aphasia. 415 yet there is no convincing anatomical evidence to prove its existence. Therefore it is referred to in an appendix, instead of in the body of the work. I shall cite briefly, and without critical comment, some cases that have been contributed to establish the reality of this form of aphasia. Even the most casual reader of the chapter " Conception of Aphasia" will recognize that the explanation of the occurrence of the one symptom of conduction aphasia, viz., paraphasia, given by those who have written on the subject is not in harmony with what has been contended for in that chapter. The cases of conduction aphasia that have been reported can all be explained as symptoms of sensory aphasia, and the variations in the clinical picture depend on the different locations of the lesion in the zone of language. Every one must admit the possibility of the existence of a lesion in the zone of language between the auditory centre and the articulatory kinaesthetic centre, without de- struction of either of these centres. On the other hand, the proximity of the visual and auditory centres and sub- jacency of the radiations of Gratiolet make it very im- probable that a lesion could exist between these two centres and not implicate either of them. A number of the most reliable writers on the subject of speech disturbances have given this form of aphasia ex- tensive consideration. Wernicke, Lichtheim, Pick, and other writers have recorded examples in support of its oc- currence and differentiation. Personally the writer is of the opinion that conduction aphasia can rarely be differen- tiated from sensory aphasia, of which it is a part. It is possible that such differentiation may be made when the 416 The Facility of SpeecJi. lesion is of the island, but careful observations are needed before this can be decided. When the lesion is of the island, the connection between the auditory centre and Broca's convolution may be only partially interrupted and some impulses sent from the former reach the latter. All of them do not. This remaining partial anatomical con- nection has been taken by some writers (Ziehen) to explain the occurrence of paraphasia in conduction aphasia. Be- fore reciting the symptomatology that has been attributed to conduction aphasia, it should be stated that if the artic- ulatory centre is dependent absolutely upon the excitatory influence of the auditory centre, as so many physiologists believe it to be, then an intercentral lesion (a Leitungs lesion of Wernicke) should produce as complete inability to speak spontaneously or on repetition as does destruc- tion of the area of Broca itself. In the conduction aphasia of Wernicke the lesion which is posited as the cause of the aphasia is one that inter- rupts the conducting fibres that unite the centre for audi- tory and articulatory word images. Anatomically such a lesion is generally in the floor of the fissure of Sylvius or in the island of Reil. In this form of aphasia the speech centres themselves are intact. It is only their connections that are interfered with. Thus neither will such a patient have true word deafness nor will there be inability prop- erly to speak out the words ; but, as the connection be- tween these two centres is interfered with, the controlling influence which the one, the auditory, has upon the other, the articulatory speech centre, will be lost, and therefore there will be defect in the proper use of words. Wernicke assumes that the images stored in Broca's centre are re- Conduction Aphasia. rived directly by the centre in which primary revival takes place from the object representation. The speech disturbance which such a lesion gives rise to is techni- cally known as paraphasia and occurs in repeating as well as in spontaneous speaking. Patients with this form of aphasia understand and appreciate everything that is said to them. In fact they are responsive to all forms of audi- tory stimulation, and such auditory stimulation calls up in the natural way the proper auditory image and helps to give rise to the proper percept. On the other hand, the patient is not prevented from attempting to communicate his ideas and thoughts, and the thoughts which are formed for communication are, it is said, the correct ones. When, however, he endeavors to embody these in words the pri- mary revival of the auditory centre cannot send a stimulus to the articulatory centre, because the conduction is inter- rupted. This being impossible, the motor centre en- deavors to do the work unguided and the result is a mis- use of words. The patient may himself be cognizant of the shortcomings in his speech and endeavor to correct them, and to convey his meaning by pantomime. These cases are very rare, and when they occur they are to be diagnosed principally by the absence of symptoms point- ing to lesions of the individual speech centres. Occasion- ally paragraphia has been noted in this form of aphasic speech disturbance. When the latter occurs, the explana- tion that has been suggested i-s that such patients have been accustomed in writing to transcribe the word which has been revived primarily by the articulatory centre and passed on by the auditory. Thus anything which prevents the association between the last two 27 4i 8 The Faculty of Speech. causes disturbances in writing analogous to those of paraphasia. The diagnosis of these cases is led up to more by the absence than by the presence of symptoms. If there are paraphasia and absence of word deafness, and nothing more, the diagnosis of conduction aphasia is said to be justified. An abstract of the case of Lichtheim, which was re- ported as one of conduction aphasia, is as follows : A man, forty-six years old, with incomplete right-side hemiplegia. No history could be obtained. Examination showed that the patient understood spoken, written, and printed speech. The most remarkable feature of the case was paraphasia, which was so great that spoken speech was quite unintelligible. He was aware of the mistakes in his production and tried to assist himself by panto- mime. Writing was very imperfect ; he disarranged the order of the letters and words, and it was difficult to get him to make efforts of writing. The same defects were manifested in attempting to repeat as when he endeavored to speak voluntarily. He retained the ability to copy. The autopsy showed extensive lesions, the chief one, according to the writer, being of the island and of the floor of the Sylvian fissure. The extensiveness of the lesion robs this case of the weight it might otherwise have in contributing to a sub- stantiation of the diagnosis of conduction aphasia. The following is, in brief, the history of a case that Pick has recently recorded as a contribution to the study of insular aphasia : A woman, Franciska Dillkins, sixty-six years old, had suffered from infancy with epilepsy. Otherwise she Conduction . \phasia. 4 1 9 had been well until her sixty-fourth year, when she de- veloped symptoms of insanity; spoke falsely; would not .work ; was very restless at night, and during the day was absolutely uncommunicative. On admission to the hos- pital she answered the general questions correctly, in monosyllables, but often made use of the same answer. The countenance was staring and unexpressive, and she made the impression that she did not understand questions and commands addressed to her. After being taken into the hospital, she remained most of the time in bed, and when things were shown to her she took no notice of them or held them unobservingly in the hand. At other times she seemed to recognize things and to name them cor- rectly, i.e., objects presented to the visual and tactile senses. Once she designated money, " motarische Sprache" (the words " motorische Sprache" had been used a short time before). Physical examination showed slight pupil- lary inequality, incomplete right-side hemiplegia with- out facial involvement, increased patellar reflex, but no ankle clonus. ILvamination. What is your name ? Franziska. Your last name? Franziska. Are you not a Dillkins? Yes. How old are you? Answered correctly and also the ques- tion as to her last residence, where she had lived for four years. In response to the question if she had suffered a stroke, she raised the right arm and said : " There is where the stroke affected me." In response to the question if she could read she said : " Formerly I could read but not now." Shown some money and asked what it was, she said: "That is a watch." Is it not money ? No. Shown paper money. "That is two gulden." Shown a glass. "That is two gulden." Then she took it in the hand and said : " From that one drinks water." What is it called? " From that one drinks water." Shown a watch. Looked 420 The Faculty of Speech. at it for a long time and then said slowly : " I do not know what that is." When the name was said in her hearing, she said : " Yes, that is a watch." Shown a ring. That is a watch." For what purpose is it? " A watch." Is it a ring? "Yes, that is a ring." Although she was shown the ring immediately afterward she again re- sponded " That is a watch. Shown a knife. " That is a watch." Then she took it in her hand and said in re- sponse to the repeated question : " Yes, I know what one does with that," but she gave no evidence that she under- stood what it is for. A burning match. " That is ein Masche (a mesh?)." She did not concern herself when it was brought close to her face. Afterward she made several inarticulate sounds which had no significance for those who heard them. Ability of repetition was markedly impaired, and what she said in endeavoring to repeat could not be understood. Oftentimes she would harp on the last word that was spoken. Tests for writing and reading were not made, as she said that she could not read (formerly she could). When requested to point to the scissors, she pointed to a key. Almost all of the objects surrounding her she called " A watch." When her attention was called to pictures on the wall she called them "A watch," but apparently was not satisfied and said " Watches." Soon after this she was seized with general convulsions, which began with conjugate deviation of the head and eyes toward the right. The pupils were widely dilated and reactionless. Immediately after the attack there was deviation of the eyeballs toward the left, and the knee- jerks were not elicitable. Later the knee-jerks were ex- aggerated. After the convulsive attacks the patient was very mute. The defectiveness of speech became more apparent. The hemiparesis was followed by profound Conduction Aplictsia. 421 hemicontracture. After another convulsive attack she developed an extensive bronchitis and soon died. Autopsy. The left hemisphere (opened after the method of Pitres) showed in the lenticular nucleus, ex- tending so as to involve the claustrum and the internal capsule, an old focus of softening of the size of a walnut. The micioscopical examination showed that the entire island in its total transverse diameter was interrupted by the focus. In conclusion Pick remarks that he does not offer this as a pure case of insular aphasia, because of the fact there were other changes, including a universal atrophy of the brain. In spite of this he says the case may be cited as a " negative case," particularly in reference to the patient's capacity to repeat. These cases are both so in- completely reported clinically and anatomically that it is unwise to attempt criticism of them. APPENDIX II. A CASE OF ARTICULATORY-KIN^STHETIC APHASIA. IN the text only as many cases were cited as was thought absolutely necessary to illustrate different types of aphasia. The clinical findings in the patient whose case I am about to relate mirror so completely, however, the exact conditions found in true cortical motor aphasia, articulatory-kinses- thetic aphasia, that I am prompted to make brief note of them here. Moreover, I believe that the case is of no small importance in substantiating the claim made in the text, that Broca's convolution sends no projection fibres directly to the internal capsule or to the capsular irradia- tion, and that it is in reality a sensory area. Mrs. X , a widow, sixty-three years old, the mother of eight children, has had a vigorous, active life, free from ill health, save that twelve years ago she suffered severely from attacks of renal calculi. During the past year or two she has complained of indigestion and more recently of a dull, aching sensation in the back of the head and neck, with occasional attacks of very severe pain in the left temple. For a few weeks previous to the beginning of her present symptoms she suffered from insomnia, from irritability, nervousness, and forgetfulness. Her son, a physician, gives the following account of the onset of her aphasic symptoms. One week before consulting me she discovered, while making a call, that her speech had be- Articulatory-Khucsthctic Aphasia. 423 come, without warning, very much embarrassed. She could not finish the sentence that she had started to speak. She forgot what she wanted to say. She chafed under this impotence and got very much excited. She returned home in a street car, and was much astonished to discover on looking at the signs with which the cars are lined that she was quite unable to comprehend their significa- tion. She could see the letters and words, she knew that they were letters and words, but they conveyed no mean- ing to her. When she got home she tried to tell her family about her disability, but was able to say only a few words, and these were entirely disconnected. After trying to speak for a time she became excited and began to cry. On the following day, when she awakened, she could say only " Yes" or " No," but as the day wore on her vocabulary became somewhat larger. It was particularly remarked that when she was excited or very emotional sometimes words would flow out of her mouth in an astonishing manner. From that time until I saw her there had not been very much change in her capacity for speech pro- duction. The following is a stenographic report of the exami- nation to determine disorder of voluntary speech. In response to the question to tell me all that she could concerning the onset and course of her symptoms, she said : " Well, mem-mem three weeks, m-m-em feel-m-em- em sometimes [prolonged pause, seems to be thinking] couldn't thought no thought forget but eh last Fri- day [another prolonged pause] am no noticed they I couldn't eh I [prolonged pause] I couldn't tell, am, I don't, I can't, can't express [explosively] I can't tell I cannot [points to her head and looks weary]. It seems, I can't, last Monday, con-con-nects sentence, two or three 424 The Faculty of Speech. words gone. Was gone, blank, didn't know. Can't think, was gone, forget forget everything. Couldn't, couldn't, can't." To test her capacity to repeat, I asked her to say after me: "I stood on the bridge at midnight." Her reply was : " I stood the night," said with great effort, and with apparent endeavor to repeat each word as quickly as they fell from my lips. " Still sits the schoolhouse by the road ?" "Forget yes the the s' s' s' forget road." " Waterloo was a battle of the first class, won by a cap- tain of the second." " The bat-tie, ah, me, ah me, ah. " It is particularly noticeable that when I speak she en- deavors to say the words after me very rapidly one word after another, but it is quite imposible for her to repeat more than a word or two. The patient is an English lady who formerly was able to speak German very fluently, but when I recite the first verse of Schiller's " Bell," begin- ning, " Fest gemauert in der Erden," etc., she is not able to repeat a word of it. I then ask her to repeat the Lord's Prayer. She assures me by nod of the head that she can- not do so, but when encouraged to try she says : " Fa' a' ther our fa ' [gets excited and I believe tries to convey to me that she was unable to repeat it last evening]. I then ask her to say it to herself. She again indicates that it is entirely impossible. I ask her if it is impossible, and she says " Yes." It is interesing to note that when I encourage her to say it to herself after I have told her that I am going to say it in my internal language to determine if we reach the end simultaneously, she adopts the conventional attitude and manner, probably thinking that they will prompt the recalcitrant words, but A rticula tory-Kincestketic Aphasia. 425 all to no purpose. She is quite unable to read, either in a loud voice or to herself, although she can say a word of what she reads here and there, but words and sentences convey no meaning to her. She takes up the newspaper, cons it carefully, then puts it down with an expression of dissatisfaction and disgust. In other words, there are manifest verbal blindness and profound alteration of men- tal images. There is no trace of hemianopsia. When I request the patient to write her name, she does so promptly. When I ask her to write the name of her son, she does so; likewise the street and number where she resides. She is absolutely unable to write spon- taneously. Her capacity to write from dictation Is tested by asking her to write, " When in the course of human events," but she is absolutely unable to do so. The only word that is produced after numer- ous attempts and repetitions of the sentence is the word "When." Writing from copy is done without trace of hesitation or error; and when she is asked to copy printed letters in writing she does so with great readiness. She comprehends spoken speech, but oftentimes it is necessary to repeat before the meaning of what is said fully dawns upon her. In other words, although there is no word deafness, there seems to be some difficulty in calling up auditory images quickly and readily. She has no trace of hemiplegia, unless we call a slight asymmetry of the angles of the mouth an indication of defective cortical innervation, as the right angle of the mouth seems to be a trifle lower than the left. There is no ataxia or inco-ordination of the extremities ; the knee- jerks are lively and of equal intensity on both sides; the pupils react to light and shadow ; there is no tone deaf- ness or object blindness. The urine contains albumin 426 The Faculty of SpeecJi. and casts ; the pulse is regular, the arteries are hard and incompressible, and the second sound of the heart is very much accentuated. In other words, she has extensive arterio-capillary fibrosis. The interpretation which I put upon the case is as fol- lows : Pathological diagnosis, general arterio-capillary fibrosis, with consequent encephalomalacia of Broca's convolution. Clinical diagnosis, true cortical motor aphasia, articulatory-kinaesthetic aphasia. I need not re- peat that the elicitable symptoms parallelize in every de- tail those which have been proven to be typical of this form of aphasia. The chief deficiency of internal lan- guage seems to be an inability to evoke the articulatory- kinjesthetic images of the word, and this constitutes a gap in the circuit of internal-speech impulses. No more illus- trative case could be cited to show that spontaneous writ- ing and writing from dictation are disordered commensu- rately with voluntary speech in this form of aphasia. Moreover, the case shows with uncommon clearness that a striking degree of verbal blindness occurs with cortical motor aphasia. It is probable that in time this at pres- ent manifest verbal blindness will become latent, and, if the pathological process on which the symptoms are dependent does not progress, that a future careless exami- nation might fail to reveal it. In the text I have stated that hemiplegia is almost con- stantly associated with cortical motor aphasia, and that it is dependent upon extension of the lesion to the Rolandic cortex. This case is an exception to the rule, and I ven- ture to believe that it is an important exception, going to demonstrate that Broca's convolution has no direct repre- Articiitatory-Kin&stketic Aphasia. 427 sentation in the capsular irradiation, and finally that the products of its activity are not sent directly to be exter- nalized, but are sent to the Rolandic representation of the speech musculature and to the area of representation for other modes of language communication. NDEX. Abstract thought, development of, 55 Acoustic sensations, 74 Agraphia, S early explanation of, 179 localization of, 26 Amimia, in cortical motor aphasia, 165 Amnesia, articulatory, 173 verbal is, 175 Amusia, 259 clinical forms of, 259 Anatomy of the brain, remarks on, 86 Aphasia, articulatory kinsesthetic, 160 as a localizing symptom, 362 associative or transcortical, 15 classification of, 9, 12 conception of, 86 definition of, 3, 6 diagnosis of, 324 dynamic forms of, 353 etioiogical classification of, 347 etiology of, 343 history of, 17 medico-legal aspects of, 47 morbid anatomy of, 370 of emission, 7 of reception, S pathology of, pedagogical treatment of, 406 testamentary capacity in, 407 treatment of, 396 Aphasic patient, education of, 404 Aphemia, 84 Apperception, definition of, 10 Apraxia, 293 Articulate speech, dependency on, 8 1 during sleep, 117 Articulation, mechanism of, 78 Articulatory kimvsthetic aphasia, 160 a case of, 422 Articulomoteur, 44 Asemia, 2 Association centres of Flechsig, 107 Association fibres, diagram of, 101 Association pathways of the brain, 99 Asymbolia. 2 Asynergia verbale, 21 Asynergia verbalis, 1 75 Auditif. 44 Auditory aphasia, 244 symptoms of, 247 variations of symptoms in, 258 Auditory apparatus, constitution of, 75 Auditory centre, symptoms of lesion Of, 222 Auditory pathway, development of, 5i Auditory sphere. Autopsy, conduct of, 376 Brain, fissures of, 88 Broca, dictum of and hypothesis, 19 original contributions of, i ~ Broca's convolution location of, 91 430 Index. Ciphers, acquisition of memory for, 277 central representation of, 63 Color, perception of, 73 Concept centre, 288 Conduction aphasia, 414 Congenital aphasia, 368 adenoid vegetations in, 369 Connection aphasia, 414 Cortical and subcortical aphasia, as- sociations of, 215 differentiation of, 214 Cortical motor aphasia, 160 disorder of writing in, 176 essential accompaniments of, 164 Diagnosis of aphasia, 324 Dynamic aphasia, 353 Dyslexia, 278 due to alcohol, 279 in motor aphasia, 182 occurrence of, 132 Epilepsy, relation of, to aphasia, 354 Etiology of aphasia, 343 Expressive' reactions, 40 Fascicular anarthria, 192 Fissure of Sylvius, kind and appear- ance of, 88 relations of, 90 Flechsig's method of dividing the brain, 379 Flechsig's theories of localization, 102 Fossa of Sylvius, 88 Frontal convolutions, 90 Frontal lobes, functions of, 46 General paresis, aphasia in, 368 Graphic motor centre, existence of, 136 Hemianopsia, homonymous, in vis- ual aphasia, 264 Hemiplegia and cortical motor apha- sia, 426 Hysteria, relation of, to aphasia, 356 Infant, cries of, 50 Inferior pediculor-frontal fascicle, 102 Intellectual expression, disorders of, I Intercentral aphasia, 414 Internal capsule, constitution of, 84 reading, disorder of, in aphasia, 131 Island of Reil, 89 Jargonaphasia, 23, 223 Kinsesthetic memories, registration of, 116 Laryngeal muscles, cortical repre- sentation of, 125 Larynx, central representation of, 1 20 Light, 71 Lips, central representation of, 120 Localization of speech centre, onto- genetic importance of, 128 Logagraphia, 8 Logaphasia, 8 Medico-legal aspects of aphasia, 407 Memory residua, 43 Migraine, relation of, to aphasia, 353 Mimic reactions, 40 Mind blindness, associated with word blindness, 392 Morbid anatomy of aphasia, 370 Motor aphasia, emotional language in, 171 general considerations, 153 Index. Musical deafness, 259 Musical memories, allotment of, 93 Musical sounds, constitution of 76 Naming centre, 288 Nerve fibres of the hemispheres, myelination of, 87 Noises, constitution of, 76 Object, perception of, 43 Olfactory sphere, 105 Opercula, divisions of, 89. Optic aphasia, 287 Optic nerve, intracranial course of, 94 Palate, central representation of, 120 Pantomime, genesis of, 112, 114 Paramnesia verbale, 21 Pathology of aphasia, 395 Pedagogical treatment of aphasia, 400 Percepts, genesis of, 57 Physiological alphabet, 405 Projection centres, connections of, 1 06 Propositionizing centre, 289 Proust-Lichtheim test, application of, 155 Psychic blindness of words, 286 Respiratory muscles, cortical repre- sentation of, 126 Responsibility of aphasic patients, 413 Revival of words, site of, no Reynaud's disease, aphasia in, 359 Righthandedness, determining causes of 97 relations of, to speech centres, 95 Seelenblindheit, 292 Sensory amimia, 283 Sensory amusia, 283 Sensory aphasia, components of, 295 definition of, 216 disorder of reading in, 249 general considerations, 216 general features of, 2 1 9 subdivision of, 217 Sensory asymbolia, 283 Sensory pathways, time of medulla- tion of, 102 SomEesthetic area, 51, 103 Speech, analysis of the genesis and function of, 40 Speech faculty, unilateral represen- tation of, 169 Speech mechanism, constitution of, 54 diagrams of, 45, 47, 49 Spelling, psycho-genesis of, 297 Subcortical aphasia, distinction of, from cortical, 180 Subcortical motor aphasia, 192 location of lesion of, 198 symptoms of, 194 Subcortical sensory aphasia, 295 Subcortical visual aphasia, symptoms of, 295 Subcortical word deafness, lesion of, 393 Syphilis in aphasia, 365 Tactile sphere, 103 Temporal convolutions, enumera- tion of, 92 Testamentary capacity in aphasic patients, 407 Tone deafness, 259 Tongue, central representation of, 120 Total aphasia, 313 Toxic aphasir., 358 Trr.nsitory aphasia, 360 43 2 Index. Treatment of aphasia, remarks on, 396 Verbal blindness, 261 Verbal deafness, 244 Visual aphasia, 261 disorder of writing in, 224 Visual area, location of, 263 Visual centre, location of, 93 Visual mechanism, component parts of, 70 Visual memories for form, 74 Visual sensation, 70 Visual sphere, 105 Visuel, 44 Word blindness, 261 Word blindness, first use of, 25 intensity of, 274 Word deafness, 244 accompaniments of, 248 associations of, 249 first use of, 25 Words as phonetic phenomena, 84 Writing, development of, 64 loss of capacity for, in sensory aphasia, 281 Zone of language, 55, no anatomical basis of, 94 blood supply of, 218 boundaries and blood supply of, 343 shape of, 98 IN1VERS// 3 1158 00369 7355 A 000354164 6