y?“ . №,Na º “...,,,,… *ſ*. * ** ** **• • • • • • ) -- * * r -> s * * * * * * * *= * * * * · §§§Nº, №.§§§§§§§§§§```````````&&--~~~~§§§§§§§§§§§§§§ --~~~~'+'~^^>)<~~~~ ~~~~). (Nº, ¿ № №. 3. №, №. §§§§§§§§§§§§§ Chicago Public Schools —TT–T- A special Rep or t of the Department of C H I L ID STUDY AND P E D A G O Gº || C. INVESTIGATION ON Children Atten ding the Public Day-Schools for the Deaf in Chicago D. P. MacMILLAN, Ph. D., Director F. G. BRUNER, Ph. D., Assistant An Enlarged and Revised Edition of a Preliminary Report Submitted to the Board of Education and Printed on May 25, 1906 PUBLIC SCHOOLS A SPECIAL REPORT OF THE DEPARTMENT OF * l ...’, a -> ". 4' * - * * ~ * * -, -} f --- i. • *_ * > . . . *: f # * 1: { f* “A -- \, * *-** * * Child Study and Pedagogic Investigation ON CHILDREN ATTENDING THE PUBLIC DAY.SCHOOLS FOR THE DEAF IN CHICAGO D. P. MAC MILLAN, Ph. D., Director F. G. BRUNER, Ph. D., An Enlarged and Revised Edition of a Preliminary Report Submitted to the Board of Education and Printed on May 25, 1906, 433677 tº NIVERSI’, ¥ tº F chºicAGO LIBRAR}.S 266892 MARCH 1930 TABLE OF CONTENTS. Page. LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 HEARING . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 CLASSIFICATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II SPEECH . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I6 VISUAL ACUITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2I PHYSICAL CONDITION: Stature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ‘. . . . 25 Weight . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 Head Measurements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 Lung Capacity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 Strength of Grip . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Grip Inder . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 Motor Ability . . . . . . . . . . * * * * * * * * * * is e º ºs e º 'º e º ºs º e º e º e ºs 4 I Motor Inder . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 MENTALITY: Imitation and Suggestibility . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 Perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 “4” Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Shot-sizing Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Lifted Weights . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 Amenability to Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 SUMMARY AND SUGGESTIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 A d LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL. Chicago, October 12, 1908. Mr. E. G. Cooley, Superintendent of Schools— Dear Sir: I submit herewith a report on the deaf children of the public schools of Chicago, and ask that a sufficient num- ber of copies be printed at the earliest convenience. The subject- matter has been on our hands now for a long time, and on ac- count of the large number of requests for the information col- lected by us, as well as the intrinsic interest and value of this data for our teachers in service, I believe the whole subject ought to be put in available form for use. An account of our dealings with the special study of the deaf children is given in a formal way in this letter of transmittal to you. The attention of the Department of Child Study and Peda- gogic Investigation was first officially drawn to deaf children in the public schools of Chicago by action of the Board of Educa- tion at the opening of school in September, 1902. At this time the Superintendent of Schools placed the matter before the Committee on School Management, and on September 3, 1902, the minutes of the Board indicate the following course of action: “The Committee on School Management reports that there are in the School for the Deaf Mutes quite a number of children who might be more properly taken care of in the schools for the feeble-minded. As the task of separating the feeble-minded from the normal children in these schools is a delicate one, the committee recommends that the Superintendent be authorized to employ the teachers in the Child Study Department to aid him in determining whether they should be excluded from the public schools or not.” (Proc. 1902, Page 70.) A report on these children was submitted to the committee On October 2, 1902, which showed that a psycho-physical ex- amination had been given to 160 pupils in the rooms for deaf children, among whom were found I6 children who were notice- ably or markedly subnormal in mentality and were accordingly 4 LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL recommended for exclusion from the public schools. Since that time it has been the policy to have each child undergo a psycho- physical examination by the Department of Child Study, as soon. as the parents of the child make application for his admittance to these rooms for deaf children. In two or three instances the Supervisor had been prevailed upon to admit to these rooms, when the classes were exceptionally small, certain children who were practically deaf for ordinary school exercises and afflicted with speech defects, for the reason that they would gain excep- tional benefit from the kind of training which the teachers of these children alone could give. On account of the time-worn contention among parents of deaf children as to the relative superiority of the lip-reading or oral and the manual methods of teaching the deaf, and especially as to the alleged non-adaptability of the oral method to any children except those of more than ordinary brightness, and to those with some power of hearing, it so happened that the whole matter was again brought before the Committee on School Man- agement at the beginning of the School year 1904-1905. No definite action was taken by the Board until the following communication was received October 26, 1904, from one of its members which finally was effective in bringing about the more comprehensive and formal report herein embodied. “. . . Since the last meeting I have visited several deaf and dumb schools, and I found them very much neglected. In one case I found two children who were not deaf or dumb, and had no right to be placed with these children. I also found at least thirty or forty per cent of the children under normal, as well as deaf and dumb, and was informed that teaching these children the oral system was a waste of time on account of their impaired mental condition. I also found teachers with classes so large that it is impossible for one teacher to make progress with all the children—naturally more attention was given to the brighter children and the others were neglected. Therefore I think it the duty of this Board to thoroughly investigate the entire mat- ter and where there are children who cannot learn the oral sys- tem, the combined alphabet or the sign system should be taught in special rooms in schools other than where the oral system is LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL 5. taught. I also think more time should be given to the teaching of reading, writing and arithmetic, as these studies seem to me to be the most essential to a deaf and dumb person. . . I trust your honorable body will give the matter the most thorough con- sideration and investigation, as this department of the public School system is much neglected and should have a thorough reorganization, better rooms, more teachers and more attention in general given to the children afflicted by the loss of hearing and speech.” On January 18, 1905, a committee consisting of the Super- intendent of Schools, the Director of the Child Study Depart- iment and three medical inspectors was appointed to investigate the whole matter and to report its findings to the Board of Edu- cation. In accordance with the usual routine of such matters the whole inquiry was referred by the Superintendent of Schools to the Department of Child Study and Pedagogic Investigation, and on April 12, 1905, a brief summary of the results was sub- mitted in a preliminary report. The only action taken on the matter at this time was to refer it back to the investigators with instructions to present the data in detail. The final report herein submitted was placed in the hands of the Superintendent of Schools, subject to call from the Committee on School Manage- ment, which demand finally was made on May 25, 1906, where- upon it was ordered printed at once, and copies distributed among the members of the Board of Education and educators of the city. The original form in which it was printed by the Secretary of the Board was, in certain features, abbreviated and popular- ized and as such it fulfilled a temporary and local service, but owing to the large number of requests for copies and inquiries from school boards of other places, from educational governing bodies and from professional and general libraries in this and other countries, as well as the desirability of placing our re- sults in permanent form for our teachers, it seems highly advis- able to print the whole subject-matter in a form which will more adequately meet the larger demand. Very respectfully yours, D. P. MAC MILLAN, Director. EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES OF DEAF CH!LDREN. It is doubtful if more than a very small minority of persons who have not had intimate dealings with deaf children are familiar with the characteristics which differentiate this special group from hearing children. One meets with all sorts of opin- ions and notions concerning their capabilities or incapacities. It is held by some that the senses of deaf children, other than hearing, are exceptionally acute, and again the assertion is made that children who are defective in one sense are deficient in all the senses and for this reason they speak of the mind of the deaf child as of a distinctly lower type. This study is not, indeed, concerned with all the deaf child- ren in Chicago, nor with all deaf children of School age. It deals with deaf children attending the public day schools whom this department considered of high enough grade of mentality to be educable by the special class methods of the public schools for the deaf. These deaf children have been admitted into these special rooms of instruction upon recommendation, after being subjected to a careful psycho-physical examination to determine their mental status and the degree of hearing power which they possessed. The feeble-minded and those of extreme sub-normal mentality are not incorporated in our results. It was, then, our object to carefully examine and estimate the comparative physi- cal and mental status and to make an extended study of the progress and school conditions of the deaf children in the public day Schools of Chicago. Some three months were devoted to this study. Children were taken in groups of two or three in a private room in which conditions were similar to those of the classroom, and in all about one or two hours was given to each child. This length of time was found necessary on account of the difficulty of get- ting responses from this class of children, who are naturally so excessively timid and hesitant in manner. This it is natural to 3 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES expect on account of the difference in their social life, which is usually confined to intercourse with parents, teachers or children similarly afflicted. Experience has shown that one must have at least a half-hour's intimate association with any child before he can be reasonably sure that the responses secured are anything at all like normal, and that the estimate made of the child's mental efficiency is not totally erroneous. If this be true of children in general, it is so true as to be axiomatic when deaf children are the subjects of examination. To estimate the men- tal status or educational possibilities of the deaf child requires not only special knowledge of their habits and characteristics, but also considerable intimacy with their environment and social conditions. A cursory examination will very frequently lead one to adjudge a child as of sub-normal mentality, whereas a greater familiarity with the disparities and mental traits of this class of children will enable one to Secure surprising results. It was found advantageous, in order to overcome the reticence of these children, to make the first examination brief and preliminary, So that the children might become personally acquainted with the examiners and confident in all their modes of expression. In order to investigate the hearing and speech possibilities and progress of deaf children, it is indispensable that we have Some knowledge of the sensory, nervous and bodily conditions which are intimately related to both hearing and speech. Thus, it is necessary to know something of the sight, as good vision is necessary to good lip-reading. It is necessary to know whether the defect in hearing is local or whether it is due to gen- eral lack of sensitiveness. Likewise it is of value to know whether the deaf child is less or more sensitive to impressions of touch, weight, etc. In case these senses are obtuse, one might well conclude that the deficiency in aural acuity is primarily due to general dullness of the whole psychical system. If the other senses are acute, the defect is probably due to the organ of hearing and not to obtuse mentality. To acquire speech it is of the first importance to have good organs of speech. The nose and throat must be open and free. The naso-pharyngeal cavity must be examined for enlarged tonsils and adenoid growths. Something must be known of the hard and soft palate in their DEAIF CHILDREN 9 relation to middle-ear deafness and breathing power, as well as the condition of the tongue, both as to structure and mobility. The general nervous condition of children very markedly in- fluences their speech and mental ability. The same nervous con- 'ditions which induce such defects as stammering, stuttering, hes- itancy, lisping and lalling are as likely to occur in deaf children as in hearing children. Then, too, there are such nervous affec- tions as chorea, nervous inertia and general incoordinations. These especially influence mentality and speech. The report of the Child Study Department for the year 1899– 1900 showed that those children who were larger, stronger quicker and better nourished were mentally superior to those who were not so well developed. The same conditions which favor physical superiority are found to have a like or comparable influence on the mental side. It therefore to a degree not only affords a convenient but a serviceable cue to the mental status, to know how deaf children compare in size, strength, quickness, sensory and motor defects with hearing children of the same age. In the case of a child having no language, it is difficult to gain much information directly of his mental ability. It is, how- ever, possible to measure his efficiency in such simple processes as perception, and, in case a child possesses a few symbols, it is possible to learn something of his immediate sense memory. It was our aim to employ only simple tests to study all these various phases of the sensory, physical, nervous and higher mental life of these children, in order to secure cumulative evidence of the exact nature of their hearing defects and of their mental and physical conditions as these are related to the acquiring of speech and school training. HEARING. METHOD OF CONDUCTING THE TEST.S. Three instruments were used in the examination of hearing power: First: The Audiometer. The instrument used was an ordinary Seashore audiometer. This is, in brief, a simple elec- trical apparatus for producing a measurable noise, which is brought about by the making and breaking of a secondary cur- rent. This noise is transmitted to the ear of the person tested IO EXPERI MIENTAL STUDIES by a telephone receiver held tightly against the ear. The child is tested with his back to the examiner and with eyes closed. The instrument was devised for testing differences in aural . acuity of persons not ordinarily considered defective in hearing. In many cases its range is too limited for grading hearing power when this power is not of sufficient acuity to detect speaking tones of the human voice. The hearing power of children can be conveniently graded according to the norms for hearing acuity, worked out with normal children and already reported on. (See Report No. 2, Department of Child Study and Pedagogical In- vestigation.) Second: “Snapper.” This little instrument when snapped between the forefinger and thumb, makes a mechanical snap of sufficient loudness to be heard by the human ear of average acuity in a quiet medium at a distance of about 500 feet. It is held by the experimenter slightly behind the ear of the child examined, out of range of vision either direct or lateral, and the child is asked to count the clicks, or in case of the younger children, the starting of the body entire, and the super- ficial reflexes, especially the conjunctival, are observed. By this means the hearing power of certain children could be gauged who could not be reached by the more delicate instrument. Third : Differential Tube. This piece of apparatus consists of a tube of soft rubber whose internal diameter is 4 mm. and whose length is IOO cm., fitted with hard rubber ear pieces to be inserted into the ears, one end into the child’s ear and the other into the ear of the examiner. This ear piece was held firmly between the first finger and the thumb, and care was taken that the stimulation was not communicated through the sense of touch. A tuning fork of pitch “a” was struck on a hard sub- stance and its base applied to the tube. First, in order to famil- iarize the child with the sound, the fork was struck briskly on a hard object and its base placed on the child's front teeth. Only after the child became somewhat familiar with the sound thus conducted, was recourse had to air conduction. This instrument proved adaptable for testing hearing power of even lower grade than the snapper, owing largely to the fact that the stimulus had a greater duration, thus affording a better opportunity for the element of attention to enter. * DEAF CHILDREN II This work with instruments was further supplemented by tests made with the human voice in ordinary speaking tones and in shouting, taking care when within close range that the child was made as nearly as possible dependent upon the Sense of hearing. CLASSIFICATION. The hearing ability of this group of children somewhat con- veniently is divided into five classes. This division is purely conventional and practical inasmuch as it is formulated wholly on the basis of school room efficiency and the value which must be attributed to the power of hearing in its relation to the acqui- sition of oral speech. In the first column, Class I, are shown the pupils who are totally deaf-insensitive to the strongest Sound vibrations which we had any means of employing. In the second column, Class 2, are presented those who are practically deaf, i. e., only Sounds which are continuous and most intense can be heard. It is very probable that their hearing can never be developed sufficiently to hear the human voice even with the assistance of mechanical devices. In column three, Class 3, is given the number of those child- ren who possess a degree of hearing power—they hear loud sounds, but yet they have not sufficient sensitivity and control of hearing to understand vocal speech. The children of this class offer hope that with the assistance of mechanical devices for intensifying the voice, their hearing power may be developed So that advantage may be taken of this normal means of ac- quiring spoken language. Class 4. Those pupils who are deaf for Ordinary School conditions, are placed in column four of the table. In general, the pupils of this class hear only words spoken loudly and close to the ear and are therefore to be considered as deaf from the point of view of conditions that ordinarily prevail in the school- room. They do, however, possess sufficient hearing to assist them in acquiring speech when appeal is made to their under- standing through this sense avenue. In column five, Class 5, is listed a certain small number of hearing children who were temporarily in the rooms for the I2 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES deaf and who are greatly in need of the training in articulation which it is not possible for them to get in our School system, except from teachers who have had special training in the rudi- ments of voice culture. TABLE I. TPOWER OF HEARING. Class 2. Practically Class 3. Class 4. Class 5. Schools Class 1. Deaf. Hear Hear loud Deaf for ordi- Hearing chil- Totally only most in- |noises but can-| nary school | dren. In deaf Deaf tense continu-jnot hear speech purpoSeS rooms for speech. ous sounds training Holden. . . . . . . . . 2 1. 2 5 O Beidler. . . . . . . . . 2 0 0 1 0 Burr. . . . . . . . . . . 5 1 7 2 2 Clarke. . . . . . . . . 2 2 4 1 0 Darwin . . . . . . . . 10 4 8 3 2 Dore . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 0 6 I Froebel . . . . . . . . 4 1. 0 1. 0 Hammond. . . . . 3 2 3 0 O Kozminski. . . . . 1. 1 4 0 O Normal . . . . . . . . 2 1 3 1. 0 Ogden . . . . . . . . . 7 8 5 O 1 Seward . . . . . . . . 4. 2 0 1 O Yale. . . . . . . . . . . 11 8 17 5 2 Totals. . . . . . . 55 33 53 25 8 Total number examined. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174 Although thus classified, it must not be assumed that these children will always retain the exact relative order here pre- sented. With some the hearing power deteriorates, while the hearing power of others will show marked improvement owing to their general mental training in giving attention and in attaching significance to excitations of this sense organ. Even among those congenitally defective in hearing it must not be assumed that the hearing power will always remain stationary. Some children were in the deaf rooms who were regarded as born absolutely deaf and now at the age of fifteen or sixteen are able to hear conversation. This improvement usually accom- panies the great growth changes at the periods of second denti- tion and of puberty. There are certain children with varying degrees of deafness whose hearing has gradually improved; there are other cases of deafness in which there is apparently no improvement. The congenitally deaf, however, form, as far as we are able to determine, only about 50 per cent of the total number, but it is difficult to draw a hard-and-fast line of de- markation between congenital cases and deafness as one of the DEAF CHILDREN I3. sequelae of disease. Many parents assign very insignificant causes for this affliction. In many other cases it is impossible to deter- mine just when deafness did occur, especially since scarlet fever, spinal meningitis and other febrile diseases sometimes are ex- perienced during the first year of the child’s life before speech develops, and deafness is attributed to these, whereas as a matter of fact, it antedated them. The two most frequent. maladies which leave deafness as one of the sequelae in their wake, as suggested above, are scarlet fever and spinal menin- gitis. The significance of this is manifold in that very frequently some tendency to mental aberration is also present, together with nervous disturbances and inco-ordinations. Although not feeble- minded always, these children so afflicted would be far below the normal in mentality even were their hearing normal. It is a matter of observation that deafness which occurs as a result of disease is as frequently attributed to some catarrhal condi- tion of the middle ear, atrophy of the ossicles of this region, or degeneration of the typmanic membrane, as to some internal ear trouble or lesion of the brain. Sometimes these diseases cause absolute loss of hearing. In other cases there is but slight diminution in auditory acuity. In every case it is very desir- able to know just where the defect lies, as well as the degree of deafness, in order to classify the child and suit his school train- ing not only to his present status but also to his future needs. Reference to Table I will show the relative proportion of children in each of the five classes. In the first class there are 55; in the Second, 33; in the third, 53; in the fourth, 25. Were the number examined very large, extending into the thousands instead of 178, we might reasonably expect to find an equal number of children in each class; that is, experimental evidence goes to show that there are varying degrees of deafness, rang- ing all the way from slight and temporary impairment of hearing due to a cold, to the stage of absolute and permanent silence. This, of course, necessitates some practical standard by which one must judge whether a child's hearing is sufficiently poor to justify his being assigned to the the rooms for the deaf. Where the recommendation has been made by the Child Study Department, the criterion has been a practical one, based not I4. EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES alone upon the child's absolute power to hear, but also upon his inability to carry on the work successfully in the rooms with hearing children. However deaf a child might be, he was never recommended for the deaf classes as long as his progress in the rooms with hearing pupils was satisfactory. Likewise in those cases where the defect was more largely one of mentality in- stead of defective hearing it has been the policy of the Child Study Department to recommend that the child be excluded from the rooms for the deaf. The above criterion seems the only one usable in actual practice. Among normal hearing children, speech is an instinctive ac- quisition which shows itself at a relatively definite time in mental ripening. The activities and speech of all persons in the home are a constant appeal to the young and yearning listener, and so there come to be set up mental associations between his acts of hearing and his vocal responses. The activities of seeing, hearing, touching, etc., which at first Operate somewhat inde- pendently of each other, come to be firmly and constantly con- Inected, so that one sense organ-act stands for the activity of another sense-process or group of such processes. Seeing comes to stand for touching. Hearing means doing or going to do. The child hears his own prattle as well as the speech of others about him. Others attribute meaning to his spontaneous bab- blings and his special acts, and, in the initial steps at least, his acts are named for him. These activities in which he indulges and to which others give specific values and names, are at first gross and inchoate, with meanings as vague, but gradually become with his growth of consciousness, more definite and precise. In this way a certain word or name heard is associated with some object reacted upon and the child's natural babblings are made to conform to the word heard. Thus the child, through his Own hearing and with very small effort, catches the cues which lead to his articulation of the words he has heard spoken by those about him. But with the deaf child, the process of acquir- ing speech is by no means so simple. No sounds are heard, or if sensed at all, are heard very indistinctly, so that the procedure becomes one in which intense and concentrated attention is nec- essary to succeed at all. DEAF CHILDREN I5 The most direct inherited associations are between speech and hearing, and not between speech and sight, as must be the procedure in the case of those whose power of hearing is ab- sent or very defective. It is always very difficult to divert na- ture's course, but this does not imply that it is never advisable as exigencies arise to modify her laws or methods of procedure, When limitations have been set to one group of associations, it is absolutely indispensable to form a new set of associations if training of any part is to take place, and this re-direction of an instinctive procedure is not accomplished without considerable effort. Intercourse between individuals is possible only by some language medium, and a language medium implies some Sort of symbols which refer directly to objects, activities or events, and it further implies that the symbols used have a commoir meaning to all using them. All articulatory symbols of intercourse are relatively arbi- trary and conventional, and their expression must occur either, as is commonly the case, by the natural way of imitating with the vocal mechanism what is received through the auditory appa- ratus—in which case the symbols are expressed spontaneously— or the meaning of these symbols must be consciously acquired and their expression laboriously worked out, as in the case of the deaf. In the latter there must occur a definite adjustment of mouth-parts and chest activity on the one hand, or the manipulation of the hands on the other, according as the vocal or manual language is used. One must be learned as well as the other. The selection and preference of one method over another, must be decided on other than psychological grounds, inasmuch as psychology properly defined, deals with a descrip- tion and explanation of all mental processes, and so every men- tal reaction, the true and serviceable as well as the false and trivial, must have its history as to its origin, characteristics and function. Both utility in social relations and facility or ease of acquirement must largely determine whether one or the Other method of training the deaf shall be used in the school- 1 OO111. There is the further question as to whether the manual method has certain limitations as compared with the oral in I6 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES developing the mind. But this is a question that must be de- cided, for the present at least, on theoretical and philosophical grounds. Thus far there have been no scientific investigations on adults, trained by these two methods, which might tend to show that the range of expressive thought is greater or less in one method than the other. The philosophical problem is one that does not concern us here, for it leads to nothing more than the expression of an opinion, and offers no hope for the solution of the question. SPEECH. There are found in the rooms for the deaf, children who had no speech when deafness occurred. There are others who had learned to speak a little, and again, there are a few others whose deafness appeared after five years of age and who possessed con- siderable power of speech. It obviously would not be just to group all these together. Therefore the children are distrib- uted into three groups on the basis of their capacity for appre- ciating and using speech, taking into account the time they have been receiving instruction. We have then as a mater of fact the following groups: (1) those whose speech is normal or nearly so; (2) those whose speech is defective either in the quality of tone of the vocal sounds or in the articulation of the consonants, nasals, labials, sibilants, etc.; and (3) those who have no speech or are incapable of uttering intelligently at least fifty words. Of course all children who had lost their hearing be- fore they had learned to talk would be in group 3 on first en- tering school. The problem, then, is that of determining the character of the speech that has been acquired for the different periods that the children have been in school. In keeping with this, the children in the various rooms have been distributed accord- ing to their efficiency in speech after they have been in school three, five, seven and ten years. It must be borne in mind that these groups do not include all the children in the rooms for the deaf, since obviously there is a large number of them who for various reasons have not been in school for as long a period as three years. Furthermore, not only must the length of time the pupil has been in school be considered but also his condition DEAF CHILDREN 17 on entrance into school. Under those three groups, therefore, of children whose speech is normal, whose speech is defective, and those whose speech is practically wanting, it was thought advisable to indicate the following (See Table II, p. 19): I. Children who have sufficient hearing to assist them in acquiring speech. II. Pupils whose hearing was lost after the age of five years—i.e., after quite a degree of facility in speech had been gained before deafness occurred. III. Pupils who lost hearing before five years of age—i. e., where an opportunity had been afforded for the loss of auditory imagery and the fading of muscular habits associated with articulation and phonation. IV. Those born deaf, or those in whom the loss of hearing occurred so early in life that speech had never been acquired. THREE YEARS IN SCHOOL. Of deaf children who have had speech training for no longer than three years, we can make the following summary: 24, speech normal, or nearly so. 23, speech more or less defective—i. e., have considerable speech, but the articulation or tonal quality of the voice is more or less defective. 7, Speech wanting—i. e., from the practical standpoint, little or no speech. It must be understood that where speech is herein spoken of as more or less defective, the child can usually be easily un- derstood by almost everybody, and in all cases can be under- stood by those who are accustomed to associate with him, though his voice, to be sure, is unpleasant and unnatural. Of these twenty-four whose speech was found to be normal, it is interesting to learn that six children possessed some usable remnants of hearing, while 18 were deaf. Of the 18 deaf at least IO had lost their hearing after the age of five years, leav- ing only 6 who were afflicted with deafness before the age of five and 2 congenitally deaf. That is, we may say, that normal speech had been taught to 8 of the 54 children by the indirect method during the brief period of three years. Turning to the 23, whose speech was understandable but I8 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES somewhat defective, the data show, that 3 had some hearing and 3 had lost their hearing after the age of five, 9 deaf chil- dren had lost their hearing earlier than the age of five and 8 were born deaf. Of the 7 who possessed little or no speech ability after three years in school, none had ever had hearing to be of any service in the acquisition of an articulatory language, 4 having been congenitally deaf and 3 having lost their hearing early in life. To summarize then if we exclude from consideration first, those who have sufficient hearing to assist them in acquiring speech and those who lost their hearing after five years of age, which two groups obviously form a separate class, we have remaining those who lost their hearing before the age of five and those who were born deaf. Of these we find : 8, normal speech. I7, speech more or less defective. 7, Speech wanting. FIVE OR SIX YEARS IN SCHOOL. Turning now to the children who have been in school five or six years, we find the cases distributed as follows: I3, speech normal. I4, speech more or less defective. O, Speech wanting. Considerable interest attaches to a comparison of the make up of the children of these three groups with those of three year School instruction. (See Table II.) Whereas, among the lat- ter but 8 of 24 with normal speech had lost their hearing earlier than five years, or were congenitally deaf, of those with 5 or 6 years of School instruction, IO of the 13 belonged to this class; all of those with somewhat defective speech had lost their hear- ing early in life; and no child of 5 and 6 years’ school instruction Was found without speech sufficient to communicate his simpler needs. Again to summarize the results, excluding as above, that special class comprising those who have some hearing and those who lost their hearing later than the age of five, we have those DEAF CHILDREN I9 classified as born deaf and having lost their hearing before the age of five—of these there are: IO, speech normal. I4, speech more or less defective. O, Speech wanting. SEVEN OR MORE YEARS IN SCHOOL. Taking up those children who have been in school seven or more years, we find the following distribution. (See Table II.) I3, Speech normal. 9, Speech more or less defective. o, Speech wanting. One boy among those with normal speech had hearing and was temporarily given a place in the rooms for the deaf to rem- edy his very defective articulation. Of the remaining I2, only 2 had lost their hearing later than five years, 2 were deprived of their hearing before the age of 5 and 8 were congenital cases. One only of the 9 with somewhat defective speech had lost his hearing later than 5, 3 before the age of 5 and 5 had been deaf from birth. TABLE II. ABILITY TO USE SPOKEN LANGUAGE. Table showing the power of speech possessed by children of different degrees of deafness, who have received speech training for three, five, seven, and ten years respectively. Hearing Deaf Character of Time in sufficient speech School to aid in Hearing Hearing Con acquiring lost after | lost before it * speech. the age of the age of '. # five years | five years €3. 3 years. . . . . . . . . . . . 6 10 6 2 Speech normal, or 5 or 6 years. . . . . . . 3 0 7 3 nearly So. 7 years. . . . . . . . . . . . 0 2 2 5 10 years or more... 1. 0 0 3 Speech somewhat 3 years. . . . . . . . . . . . 3 3 Q 8 defective, either in 5 or 6 years........ 0 0 4. 10 quality of tone or 7 years. . . . . . . . . . . . O 1. 3 5 articulation of sounds.'10 years or more... 0 0 0 0 w 3 years. . . . . . . . . . . . 0 0 3 4 Speech wanting, 5 or 6 years........ 0 O 0 O or less than 7 years. . . . . . . . . . . . 0 0 0 O 50 words 10 years or more... 0 0 O O Totals. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 16 34 40 Taking into consideration then the acquirement of speech as it is related to time of instruction, we find of the whole number of IO3 children who had had at least three years School training, 2O EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES that 96 succeeded in acquiring oral speech, of whom 50 possessed normal speech and 46 were able to interpret oral language, but to speak albeit with difficulty or slight defect. It will be seen, moreover, that of this whole number there are I3 children who possessed sufficient hearing to enable them to be materially as- sisted in acquiring speech under schoolroom conditions. Of the children who must be classed as deaf, I6 lost their hearing after the age of 5 years; 34 were deprived of their hearing before the age of 5 years, while there are 40 whose parents gave evidence regarding their early lives which indicated that in all probability these children were born deaf. Although 7 children were found whose speech was wanting after a period of training of 3 years, there were none after a school period of 5 or more years who were not able to express themselves more or less intelligibly. Not considering those who lost their hearing later than the age of 5, and those who have considerable hearing, 23 had voices which were unpleasant and unnatural after three years of school training, I4 after five years and 9 after seven or more years. These comparisons are made with reference to the voice of the normal child as a standard and are obviously unfair to a child working under the limitations of the deaf. It would no doubt be better to use a criterion of practical utility rather than one of esthetic value, but it is impossible in the case of the former to arrive at any norm for comparison, and hence we are forced to the use of the apparently less practical standard of reference, which, nevertheless, has, as a matter of fact, considerable signifi- C311Ce. The time that a child has been in school is not the only nor is it the most important, factor in effecting speech efficiency. The natural endowment of the child is an extremely important consideration. Next stands the environment, some of the most important factors of which are the character of the food which the child receives, and the regulation of his rest and exercise. It is further of significance whether the parents speak the lan- guage of the school and are thus able to give the child some drill in the exercise of his speech outside of school hours, or in case the parents do speak the language of the school, whether they have the time and inclination to thus co-operate with the DEAF CHILDREN 2I teachers. An attempt was made to evaluate all these factors in the study of the condition of each child, and formed part of the basis for the specific recommendations which were given to the child’s teacher at the time he was examined with reference to his school possibilities and the best line of treatment adapted to His particular needs. VISUAL ACUITY. TABLE III. VISUAI, ACUITY. Tables showing the distribution of deaf and hearing children, respectively, as regards the visual acuity of both eyes, whether the vision of the two eyes is the same or different. DEAF CHILDREN. 1.00 or 0.40 of nor- tº gº 0.67 of nor-0.50 of nor- e Vision better. Nor- mal. 20–50 Blind mal mal. 20-30 || mal. 20-40 or less The vision of both eyes being .. 225 35 21 2 0 The vision of one eye being 1.00. the other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 9 3 5 The vision of one eye being 0.67, the other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 4 O The vision of one eye being 0.50. the other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 S Total number examined, 353. E[EARING CHILDREN. 1.00 or 0.67 of 0.50 of 0,40 of #. Blind or Vision better, normal. normal, normal, "ºo * nearly Normal 20–30 20-40 20-50 or 1 SO e S The vision of both eyes being.. 972 82 11 27 69 0 The vision of one eye being 1.00, the other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 8 11 21 4 The vision of one eye being 0.67, the other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 18 20 1 The vision of one eye being 0.50, the other . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 7 0 The vision of one eye being 0.29, the other . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 0 Total number examined, 1,436. The Oral method of training the deaf implies not only the utterance of speech, but also speech-reading. With normal hear- ing children this is done through the medium of the ear, where- by even the slightest variation of tones and inflections are in- terpreted naturally and without effort, yet each carrying with it a significance that differentiates a shade of meaning. Though the eyes are always used in discerning the attitude of the speaker by the hearing child, the use of these organs plays a subordinate role. Not so with the deaf child. Every idea scommunicated to him, except the few he gets through touch, taste 22 EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES and smell, must reach him through his eyes. In interpreting speech he must observe the most minute movements of the mouth- parts, the eyes and facial muscles, to interpret their significance, translate them into ideas and observe shades of meaning by the slightest variations in muscular adjustments and movements. Hence the condition of the deaf child's vision becomes a very im- portant consideration. While there are many opportunities for relaxation of the eyes of the normal child, these opportunities are obviously less frequent in the case of those children who have to depend on their vision almost entirely. Defective vision, therefore, tends to produce greater limitations among the latter than the former. In Table III are given the distributions of deaf children and of normal hearing children, from comparable Social environments and home surroundings, as regards visual acuity. In this the emphasis is laid on the function of vision as a determining factor in acquiring school information. It is well known that a child may have very defective vision in one eye and yet because the other eye functions normally, the child may not be seriously handicapped in receiving impressions from his surroundings. Accordingly the acuity of each eye is registered separately and the number of children is recorded who have good vision with both eyes as well as the number of those who are visually acute with one eye and defective in the other, or indeed, those who have poor vision with both eyes. In the upper half of the table is recorded the distribution of 353 deaf children and in the lower half a similar distribution of I,436 hearing children. Looking at the upper half of the table for a moment (the part referring to deaf children) it is seen that in the first line, beginning at the left, are tabulated; first the number of children with normal vision in both eyes (225); then the number whose vision for both eyes is o.67 nor- mal (35); O.50 normal (21); and O.4O normal (2), respectively. In the second line is shown the number of deaf children whose vision for one eye only is normal, while that of the other is o.67 normal (22); O.50 normal (9); O.40 normal (3); and where the vision of one eye is wholly wanting (5). Then in the third line follows the number whose vision in one eye is o.67 normal, DEAF CHILDREN 23 as registered from the vision chart, while that of the other eye is O.50 normal (II); O.40 normal (4); and vision entirely want- ing (o). This will suffice to make the whole table easy of in- terpretation. From the distributions in the table it is thus seen that of the total number of deaf children from whom we have visual acuity records (353) there are 225 or 63.7 per cent whose vision is normal as judged by the standards of the optician or occulist. Two hundred and sixty-four of the 353 or nearly 75 per cent have vision of at least one eye normal according to the same standards, and nearly 90 (88.9) per cent have at least one eye whose visual acuity is O.67 normal. It is of interest and significance to compare the visual acuity of deaf children with that of the 1,436 hearing children in the lower half of the table. About 3 per cent fewer of the deaf have the vision of both eyes normal. Four per cent fewer have vision of at least one eye normal, but almost 2 per cent more of the deaf have a visual acuity of both eyes O.67 normal or bet- ter, as compared with their hearing companions. Certainly of considerable pedagogical significance is the fact, moreover, that the per cent of hearing children whose vision is very poor—o.5o normal or lower for one or both eyes—is nearly one-half greater than in the case of deaf children. Accordingly, it is clear that deaf children are not handicapped by visual defects, to any greater extent than are those children not afflicted with loss of hearing. By way of accounting for the relatively poor show- ing of deaf children among those whose vision is normal or nearly So, it is probable that resort must be made to some psycho- logical explanation induced by the deaf child’s inability to react to the test as efficiently as do hearing children. And owing to the paucity of numbers too much weight must not be attached to the apparently better showing of deaf children when com- paring those whose vision is very poor. As much as can be asserted with confidence is that it is extremely probable in the light of the experimental data exhibited in Table III, that deaf and hearing children, on the whole do not differ as regards vis- ion. A summary of the comparisons just made will perhaps show what has been said to better advantage. 24 EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES Whole number of children: Deaf, 353 ; hearing children, 1,436. Number. Per cent Vision, 1.00 (20-20) or better. * Deaf. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 225 63.7 €yêS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hearing child. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 972 66.9 Vision, 1.00 (20-20) or better. one; Deaf. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 264 74.8 eye at least. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hearing child... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1153 79.0 Vision, 0.67 (20–30) or better. Both | Deaf, . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 282 79.9 eyes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hearing child... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1123 78.2 Vision, 0.67 (20–30) or better. One } Deaf. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 314 88.9 eye at least... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hearing child... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1287 89.6 Vision, 0.50 (20–40) or less. Both J Deaf. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 9.3 €yêS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ! Hearing child... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188 13.1 In the foregoing statements of results and comparisons nothing has been said as to what constitutes defective vision. Of course it is well understood that defective vision from the educator's standpoint is quite a different consideration from visual defects as an oculist understands the matter. For in- stance, a child may have normal vision (20/20) in one eye, and the other may be only two-thirds, two-fourths, two-fifths, two- sevenths or even less in visual acuity. It will readily be granted even by oculists that if a child possesses sufficiently acute vision to be able to carry on his school work well he must be placed in a different category from those whose vision seriously impede their school labors. Very unfortunately the minimum degree of visual acuity that must be possessed to insure against eye strains, inefficient book- and black-board work, and certain physical ills which attend the functioning of weak visual organs, has not yet been determined. Indeed it is improbable that any hard and fast line can ever be set to mark off the functionally efficient from the inefficient eyes, and particularly not in terms of visual acuity alone. One frequently finds instances where the dioptric defect is seemingly grave, and yet the individual appears to see well and experiences no ill effects from the use of his eyes, and on the contrary a very slight irregularity in the organ of vision itself is frequently attended by very serious pains and strains. In the latter the trouble is induced by bodily or nervous depletion, primarily, and only secondarily is it to be attributed to irregularity in the structure of the dioptric media. * PHYSICAL CONDITION. Besides the foregoing data on the comparative sensitivity of the sight and hearing of deaf children, and their acquired power of speech after definite periods of School instruction, it is of im- portance to gain some definite idea of their general physical con- dition. Here again it is to be observed that so-called practical obser- vations and verbal descriptions of differences between this group of children and normal children have always led to exaggerated accounts and unwise statements regarding the deaf, sometimes to their advantage, but more often to their disparagement. There- fore the more positive and precise are the statements and the greater variety of aspects of the physical make-up investigated, the greater is the hope of our understanding the exact condi- tions that must be encountered in attempting to educate these children into citizenship. It is on this account highly desirable to present certain quantitative statements of the physical status and deficiencies of deaf children as are evidenced in the two more or less, closely related aspects of physical growth, viz., size or bulk and movement or control. In pursuance of this end, in the following tables are given the comparative stature, height-sitting, weight, lung capacity, maximum length and breadth of head, strength of hand-grip and voluntary motor ability, distributed according to age and sex throughout. (The greater number of cases (293) recorded in some of these tables is brought about by the fact that they in- clude measurements other than these made during the period of this special investigation.) STATURE. The stature records represent the net heights expressed in millimeters. (See Table IV.) It will be seen that deaf boys are not so tall on the average as normal hearing boys at any age with the exception of those at the age of Io years, at which time a slight difference is in favor of the deaf boys. This exception may be accounted for by 26 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES a class of boys who had been receiving instruction and unusual physical training since their kindergarten days and who pos- sessed especially favorable home surroundings: The deaf girls before the age of Io years are noticeably shorter than hearing girls and the differences are greater than the inferiority of deaf boys for their corresponding age—norms; but after this age the deaf girls are almost uniformly taller than their hearing companions. To be sure the numbers are small and the cases (75) of those years of apparent superiority may be exceptional in this respect, but the fact is indicative at least that for the deaf girls the disabilities which they experience in hearing or the causes of their affliction have not, during this span of years, impaired their growth as measured by their stature. The height-sitting measurements show (Table V) that deaf boys are uniformly inferior throughout the years represented. The girls on the other hand are inferior up to the age of II years; at the ages II, I2 and I3 they are taller sitting; at I4, I5 and 16 years they are again inferior, and thereafter the four cases show excess over the hearing girls. Taking the deaf children as a whole and comparing them with hearing children with regard to stature it is found that only Seventy-seven out of one hundred and seventy-seven, or 43.5 per cent, of deaf boys are equal to or above the average stature of hearing boys for equal age as against eighty-eight that would be found with the same normal hearing boys, and in height-sit- ting only sixty out of one hundred and sixty-seven boys, or 35.9 per cent, are equal to or above the average of normal hear- ing boys, instead of a possible eighty-three or eighty-four out of one hundred and sixty-seven in a normal group. Again we find only fifty-two out of one hundred and twelve deaf girls, or 46.4 per cent who are equal to or above the average statures for the normal hearing girls of their respective ages, and in height-sit- ting only forty-three out one hundred and ten, or 39. I per cent, are equal to or above the average of hearing girls for the same ages represented. Both deaf boys and girls, consequently, it is learned, are not quite so tall as are hearing children of corre- sponding ages, and the deaf of both sexes have shorter bodies. than do their hearing companions. • *-* , as * : º TABLE IV. STATURE. Boys Girls Age Deaf children, Hearing children, Deaf children, Hearing children, No. of average height. A. D. average height. No. of average A. D. average Yrs. Mos. C2S6S. Millimeters Millimeters C3 SCS height. height. Millimeters Millimeters 6. . . .6 10 1123.2 28.0 1132.5 9 1097.3 35.4 1125.1 7.... 6 23 1178.2 44.2 1183.9 15 1163.4 45.7 1182.2 8. . . .6 23 1185.4 55.6 1234.8 14 1201.1 26.7 1227.5 9.... 6 18 1287.1 51.2 1288.0 7 1250.7 31.7 1277.4 10.... 6 20 1335.4 55.0 1330.3 12 1326.2 55.3 1324.1 11.... 6 12 1357.0 66.3 1371.9 7 1403.0 63. 1 1383.0 12, ... 6 14 1370.2 57.5 1418.9 8 1484.3 51.8 1443.2 13. . . .6 15 1465.9 69.6 1490.9 11 1541.1 72.1 1510.4 14. ... 6 12 1528.1 69. 1 1547.4 7 1501.7 84.5 1562.4 15. . . .6 13 1505.5 68.1 1614.1 11 1577.1 61.9 1574.2 16.... 6 11 1655.9 75.7 1666.5 5 1637.0 74.8 1591.8 17. . . . 6 4 1637.3 32.3 1690.4 6 1606.0 32.3 1593.4 18. . . .6 2 1633.0 69.0 1734.1 : ] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ." . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . S 28 EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES WEIGHT. With reference to weight (See Table VI) it may be said that the deaf boys on the average are almost uniformly lighter than the hearing boys, and although there are exceptions to this at ages 7, 15 and 18, the numbers and the average deviations at these ages make the data non-significant. In the case of the girls there is greater irregularity in the data. This does not become evident, however, until after the age of Io years. The girls are, still, at no age, where the numbers are at all significant as inferior to the norms for their hearing sisters as are the boys. Again comparing the deaf children as a group with hearing children, it is found that in weight, there are only forty out of eighty-five boys, or 47 per cent, who are equal to, or above, the averages for hearing boys for the same ages, and with girls we find thirty-two out of a total of fifty-three girls of all ages, or 60.4 per cent, who are equal to or above the average of normal girls for their ages. While deaf boys are, on the whole, there- fore, slightly lighter than hearing boys, deaf girls are consider- ably heavier on the average than are hearing girls of compar- able ages. HEAD MEASUREMENTS.* In Table VII are recorded the head measurements of the deaf children, boys and girls by ages; the average maximum length and breadth of head of deaf children with the average deviation for each age group placed side by side with the corre- sponding figures for hearing boys and girls. (Note—See p 88.) It will readily be seen that almost uniformly the deaf boys have shorter heads than the hearing, and that in breadth of head the deaf boys are almost as uniformly wider than the aver- age of our norms from hearing children. Deaf girls show the same tendencies in head measurements as the deaf boys. The average maximum length of head of the deaf girls is less, while the average greatest breadth is greater, although indeed there are exceptions in breadth at ages 9, II, I6 and I7. *The Smedley cephalometer was employed to make these meas- urements. The details and methods with reference to the head meas- urements conform with those of our best American and German anthro- pologists. - TABLE V. HEIGHT SITTING. Boys Girls Age Deaf children, Hearing children, Deaf children, Hearing children, No. of average height A. D. average height No. of average height A. D. average height CaSCS sitting. sitting. CalSeS sitting. sitting. Yrs. Mos, Millimeters Millimeters Millimeters Millimeters 5.... 6 2 584.0 2.0 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 593.0 5.0 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6.... 6 11 622.8 13.8 635.4 10 600.8 33.1 629.0 7.... 6 20 642.1 22.9 657.8 14 647. 9 17.6 652.5 8.... 6 23 668.6 19.3 677.2 12 658.1 14.3 674.3 9.... 6 16 697.3 25.3 698.5 11 681.0 36.5 692.1 10.... 6 18 710.0 25.4 712.6 10 706.2 8.1 708.9 11.... 6 12 718.5 27.6 729.3 7 749.7 27.4 735.8 12.... 6 14 725.5 19.5 747.0 8 776.8 47.8 762.9 13. ... 6 15 759.3 25.9 777. 9 10 809.2 32.2 795.4 14. ... 6 11 792.3 41.4 806.4 8 804.9 33.6 824.3 15.... 6 12 797.5 30.6 836.8 11 825.8 40.5 839.9 16.... 6 10 859.0 45.8 871.7 3 829.3 21.0 850.9 17. ... 6 4 849.0 47.0 891.4 6 861.7 11.0 853.0 18. ... 6 1 914.0 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 914.6 2 865.0 23.0 857.2 19. . . .6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 855.8 31.8 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . § 3O EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES In addition to average lengths and breadths are given the averages of the products of length and breadth of the head of each child of both the deaf and the hearing children of the sev- eral ages (Table VII). By consulting the columns of the table which represent products it will be seen that in the case of the boys, the deaf are uniformly inferior to the hearing children, and this comparative status likewise obtains throughout with the girls with the exception of the age of I2, where the nine cases measured show a superiority on the side of the deaf. In these measurements again let us consider the deaf as a group and compare them with normal hearing children of com- parable ages. In length of head only sixty-seven cases out of a total of two hundred and seven, 32.3 per cent, are equal to or in excess of the average hearing child for each age group. In breadth, however, there are one hundred and twenty-four of the whole number of two hundred and seven boys whose heads are as broad or broader than the average—59.9 per cent. Practically the same ratio obtains in the same measurements with the girls. Only thirty-seven out of a total number of one hundred and twenty-seven girls, or 29.1 per cent, are equal or greater in length of head than the averages of our normal group and in breadth there are eighty-seven out of the whole number of girls, one hundred and twenty-seven, or 68.5 per cent, whose heads are as wide or wider in greatest breadth than the average of the normals for their ages of hearing children.f Deaf children have, as a whole, therefore, smaller heads than do hearing children, taking as a criterion the length, breadth product,” and they are more brachiocephlic, broad-headed. #These norms Of head measurementS have been taken from the Statistics on Growth by Franz Boas, published in Report of Com- missioner of Education, Chapter II, 1904. It is found in consulting the records of this Department that with reference to this particular physical measurement our cases are so largely atypical, that it is thought unsafe to adopt them in a comparative study of this character, and On this account recourse is had to the careful record of individual Fmeasurements in the report of Professor Boas. The standard devia- tions recorded in his tables are translated into the average deviation by using the formula A. D.—,7979ö. *It Would have been better, no doubt, to have rendered a comparison of head Size based on the three head dimensions, employing as the height measurement the maximum height of the head above the floor of the auditory meati, but it was impossible for us to get satisfactory records of the last named measurement. TABLE VI. WEIGHT. Boys Girls Deaf Children Hearing Children Deaf Children Hearing Children Yrs *. OS No. of cases Average 1bs. A. D. No. of cases |Average 1bs. | No. of cases | Average it. A. D. No. of cases Aºaze 6. . . . . . 6 5 46.4 2.6 227 45.6 5 42.2 2.6 204 43.8 7. . . . . . 6 Q 5.2.1 4.0 230 49.6 4 47.1 3.7 236 48.5 8. . . . . . 6 17 54.8 4.2 255 55.4 4 51.3 4.0 232 52.8 9. . . . . . 6 7 57.4 6.2 228 60.8 7 61.6 4.6 221 58.3 10. . . . . . 6 12 62.0 6.2 254 64.65 5 63.8 4.2 240 64.0 11 . . . . . . 6 6 65. 1 7.0 228 71.5 3 72.6 6.6 223 71.0 12. . . . . . 6 6 66.9 7.7 256 78.4 3 85.6 6.4 247 80.3 13. . . . . . 6 8 86.7 8.1 230 88.6 7 86.9 7.7 255 91.2 14. . . . . . 6 5 88.2 12.5 250 98.5 3 96.9 17.2 301 103.4 15. . . . . . 6 4 118.1 13.0 205 112.2 5 109.3 16.9 379 108.5 16. . . . . . 6 2 116.8 11.8 146 121.5 1 99.7 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 358 114.3 17 . . . . . . 6 2 122.1 6.6 92 130.3 4. 129.1 8.1 251 116.2 18. . . . . . 6 2 146.1 4.6 40 138.3 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19. . . . . . 6 2 128.9 5.7 23 141.6 1. 97.7 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 120.0 20. . . . . . 6 1 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 158.2 1. 107.4 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 118.9 21. . . . . . 6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1. 107.6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 120.5 24. . . . . . 6 : . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 116.6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . º 32 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES LUNG CAPACITY. The measurement of the lung capacity was taken by means of the ordinary wet spirometer (Narragansett) and simply in- dicates the greatest amount of air that can be expelled from the lungs after a forced inspiration. This is after everything is considered, perhaps also the best test of the amount of air that is available for use. It may not give a direct index of the actual or possible size of the lungs, but it affords in a way a convenient, safe and valuable criterion of the degree of activity to which the vocal apparatus has been subjected, or is capable of exercis- ing at the time the test is made, as well as the extent of the child’s general physical activity. It is on this account quite natural that we should find that the deaf children are markedly inferior in lung capacity to normal hearing children, and this palpable inferiority, even in the case of those deaf children who were given special physical and speech training, gives us an in- dication of the immense amount of physical exercise other than speech that normal hearing children naturally indulge in and from which the deaf are barred by reason of their sensory handi- caps. The intimate relation which exists between middle ear infirmities, high and narrow bone-palate formation with all the accompaniments and causes of obstructed breathing and dimin- ished lung capacity has doubtless been observed by every careful investigator of normal children with merely defective hearing. It has likewise been pretty well recognized that the amount of usable air in the lungs of normal adults varies, within limits, with the degree of general physical activity. But besides suffering the consequences of inertness during early years, the deaf children do not give their lungs anything like the same de- gree of exercise that normal hearing children indulge in, since indeed they vocalize so little, and hence the cumulative effects of this double handicap becomes palpable and hard to overcome even with persistent exercise. In no other particular are the deaf children so uniformly or markedly inferior to the hearing normal children as in lung capacity. This is seen clearly by reference to Table VIII. The deaf boys are inferior at every age. This inferiority, to be sure, is not so pronounced at ages II and I2 years, or at $3 TABLE VII. HiAD MEASUREMENTS.–BOYS. - - s Length-Deafs Length-Hearings” Breadth-Deafs Breadth-Hearings | Average pro- | Average pro- Age No. of -------- I - --- *ś ... . Ca,SeS and breadth- an rea. wº IIllin, A. D. IIll Il A. D. In 111, A. D. IIll Il. A. D. Deaf Hearing 6 Q 180.4 7.1 178.2 5.3 143.2 4.4 140.7 4.1 25768 25866 7 28 172.5 7.1 179.6 5.1 142.1 4.8 142.1 3.7 24776 25627 8 26 176.9 8.8 180.1 4.5 144.3 3.9 143.6 3.8 25562 26325 9 15 175.5 7.1 181.2 5.0 144.4 4.1 144.0 3.7 25119 26649 10 22 181.7 6.2 181.7 4.4 146.8 9.1 144.5 3.6 26750 26912 11 17 180.5 6.2 183.4 4.8 146.0 4.4 144.6 4.0 26364 26707 12 18 177.5 7.8 183.3 4.8 147.1 5.3 145.6 4.5 26098 27242 13 18 181.5 7.2 185.2 5.1 146.7 4.0 146.8 3.8 26643 27186 14 13 182.7 5.5 187.2 5.1 151.0 4.1 146.9 3.8 27253 273.71 15 17 181.9 7.0 188.3 4.9 148.5 5.3 148.1 3.7 27025 27832 16 11 184.6 5.2 190.8 6.1 147.6 5.6 149.2 3.8 27275 285.15 17 6 185.3 4.6 191.6 4.8 148.0 6.3 149.5 3.9 27456 2877 7 18 4 185.2 6.7 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147.7 4.2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27388 29088 FIEAD MEASUREMENTS-GIIRLS. Length-Deafs Length-Hearings Breadth-Deafs Breadth-Hearings Average pro- | Average pro- Age No. of duct of length duct of length CaSCS IIT IIle A. D. IIllſle A. D. Iſidºl, A. D. IILLIle A. D. and breadth- and breadth- Deaf Hearing 6 11 165.8 6.0 173. 1 3.7 140.1 3.1 138.0 2.8 23750 23984 7 12 169.1 4.1 174.7 4.8 142.5 4.6 139.1 3.3 23986 24571 8 15 172.7 4.4 175.0 4.7 141.6 2.7 140.3 3.4 24533 24639 9 11 170.0 3.8 176.3 5.0 135.9 3.7 140.2 3.8 23573 24782 10 13 174.1 6.7 177.8 4.7 144.9 3.4 142.1 3.6 25256 25429 11 11 173.6 5.1 178.2 4.3 141.4 4.8 142.1 3.9 24587 25542 12 9 180.5 6.2 180.0 4.3 147.0 2.6 143.2 3.7 26574 25564 13 12 179.0 5.9 181.7 5.4 146.1 5.5 144.0 3.9 26167 26386 14 7 182.0 5.1 182.5 5.1 144.5 4.7 144.0 4.2 26339 26431 15 13 179.6 6.1 184.3 4.1 146.0 3.8 145.4 3.6 26247 26370 l6 6 175.8 4.1 183.7 4.5 143.0 5.6 144.6 3.9 25172 26206 17 7 179.0 6 184.8 4.4 143. 4.0 145.2 3.4 25709 26791 *Among the hearing children the figures represent the average measurements of approximately 100 children of each sex for each age group. 34 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES the particular epoch in their lives at which we notice that some of the other measusements showed an equality or even superior- ity to the norms for hearing children. The same inferiority is apparent in the case of the deaf girls, and here again the great- est inferiority is shown at those years, Io, II and I2 years, at which we previously saw a lack of uniformity. Here again it should be noted that because of the relatively small number of children examined at each age it is probably a safer criterion to take the total number of deaf children whose lung capacity is equal to or above the average for hearing child- ren of the same age. In counting over the cases with this object in mind we find among the one hundred and forty-seven deaf boys, of all ages examined, that only eighteen, or I2.2 per cent, are equal to or above the average for hearing boys, and that in the case of the deaf girls thirty-two out of a total of ninety-five of all ages, or 33.9 per cent, are equal to or above the average for hearing girls, instead of 50 per cent, which figure would indicate them as normal. It is interesting to note the position of deaf children with reference to lung capacity in the per centile group of hearing children.” These are shown in Table IX, in which is indicated the per centile group of hearing children into which the average of the records of the deaf of each age fall. In a parallel column is given also the average for each age, of the ratios which the in- dividual lung capacity records of deaf children make with the average lung capacity of hearing children of the same age. Thus it is seen that the average lung capacity of deaf boys, and girls as well, of the mean age of six and one-half years, falls in the *For an explanation of these percentile groupings, see Report of the Child Study Department, Public Schools, Chicago, 1901, p. 13. In brief, the method of procedure was as follows: the four or five hun- dred records secured for the children at each age were distributed according to rank. The minimum measurement gives the zero per- centile for the whole group. To determine the ten percentile, ten per cent of the number of cards is removed, beginning with the minimal record, and the highest measurement On the card SO removed is re- Corded as the maximal limit of the ten percentile group. A similar placing and selecting of the next highest ten per cent of records with their minimal and maximal limits gives us the twenty percentile group, and so on, for the others. TABLE VIII. LUNG CAPACITY. Boys Girls Deaf Children Hearing Children Deaf Children Hearing Children . Age No. of Average A. D No. of Average No. of A verage A. D. No. of Average Yrs. Mos CalSeS Cll, CII1S, Ca. SeS Cºl. CITIS. C3 SeS Cl1, CIIlS. tº sº-ºº º CalSeS Cll, CII1S, 6. . . . . . 6 9 833 181 227 1098 8 631 114 204 1008 7. . . . . . 6 17 1003 174 230 1240 6 800 200 236 1121 8. . . . . . 6 21 1164 172 255 1388 9 983 148 232 1215 9. . . . . . 6 11 1282 169 228 1549 10 1283 168 221 1360 10. . . . . . 6 19 1466 146 254 1659 8 1394 84 240 1456 11 . . . . . . 6 12 1604 195 228 1799 7 1571 153 223 1587 12. . . . . . 6 12 1704 138 256 1956 9 1706 349 247 1729 13. . . . . . 6 17 1868 186 230 2246 11 1828 285 255 1924 14. . . . . . 6 7 2171 367 250 2527 5 1990 352 301 2117 15. . . . . . 6 9 2239 290 205 2858 9 2106 341 379 2225 16 . . . . . . 6 6 2817 277 146 3363 5 2150 380 354 2306 17. . . . . . 6 2 2250 755 92 3570 6 2.192 325 251 2304 18. . . . . . 6 3 3067 843 40 3700 3 2133 310 137 2351 19. . . . . . 6 2 3200 150 23 3872 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 2441 20. . . . . . 6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1. 2300 | . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 21. . . . . . 6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2150 | . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 2250 • * * * * , * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * , , | § 36 EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES * º ; : : ; TABLE IX. LUNG CAPACITY. Boys Girls Age Percentile Group | Average of the Percantile Group Average of the e of Normals in ratios of of Normals in ratios of Yrs. Mos. which deaf deaf children to which deaf deaf children to children fall normal hearing children fall normal hearing 5....6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 0–10 74 6. ... 6 0–10 .81 0–10 62 7.... 6 10–20 .82 0–10 77 8.... 6 10–20 .83 20–30 83 9.... 6 90 10–20 82 10.... 6 20–30 88 40–50 96 11.... 6 20–30 91 60–70 1.03 12.... 6 10–20 .85 10–20 85 13. ... 6 10–20 .81 20-30 88 14. ... 6 10–20 .77 10–20 84 15.... 6 10–20 .75 40 96 16.... 6 10–20 .79 40–50 98 17. ... 6 Be! ". f .63 40–50 98 elow limits o 18. ... 6 normals -48 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19. . . .6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30–40 95 ten per centile group of hearing boys and girls of that age, and on the average, deaf boys have a lung capacity .81 as large as hearing boys, and deaf girls .74 as large as hearing girls, at this age. The table presents the data so clearly that further comment is unnecessary. The striking inferiority of deaf children of both sexes at all ages, almost uniformly, stands out in this table even more convincingly than in Table VIII. STRENGTH OF GRIP. To determine to what degree the strength of the grip of deaf children is inferior or superior to normal children of the same age and school conditions, an adjustable grip dynamometer was used, which registers the strength of grip in kilos (I kilo= 2.2 lbs.). The gross results of these tests are given in Table X. The ages, number of pupils of each age tested, grip of right and left hand are given in parallel columns for boys and girls respectively, and the norms of the grip of normal children (taken from Child Study Report No. 2, 1899–1900), placed in columns adjoining for ready comparison. By consulting Table X we find that the deaf boys are slight- ly superior in right hand at ages 7, 8, 9 and II. After the age of 12 years they are uniformly inferior, and this inferiority in- creases with age. The strength of left hand is superior with: DEAF CHILDREN 37 TABLE X. STIRENGTH OF GRIP.-DEAF BOYS. Right Hand Left Hand Deaf Hearing Deaf Hearing Age No. of children, children, children, children, CalSeS average | A. D. a Verage average A. D. Average. #Yrs. Mos. grip, grip, gT1D, g|T1 p, kgs. kgs. kgs. kgs. 5.... 6 2 6.5 .5 ! . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.5 .5 7. 12 6. ... 6 7 9.7 1.9 9.97 8.7 1.7 9.29 7.... 6 28 12.7 2.7 11.51 11.7 2.0 10.92 8.... 6 24 15.8 2.5 13.31 14.6 2.8 12.43 9.... 6 20 16.4 2.4 15.37 15.6 3.0 14.51 10.... 6 23 19.5 2.8 17.67 17.8 2.1 16.66 11.... 6 17 21.7 3.5 20.03 19.5 2.4 18.80 12.... 6 18 21.7 2.8 22.45 20.7 3.0 20.62 13. ... 6 18 23.4 3.0 26.43 21.1 2.2 24.40 14. ... 6 14 28.9 8.1 30.40 28.5 6.5 28.40 15.... 6 14 28.1 4.9 36.38 26.3 6.3 33.73 16.... 6 11 38.6 7.2 42.35 37.2 6.7 39.40 17. ... 6 6 37.7 2.3 47.14 37.4 1.6 42.87 18.... 6 4. 35.5 7.0 51.43 37.5 10.0 47. 16 19. . . . 6 1. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . sº 20: . . .6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . STRENGTH OF GRIP.--DEAF GIRLS. Right Hand Left Hand Deaf Hearing Deaf Hearing Age children, children, children, children, No. Of average | A. D. average average | A. D. average Yrs. Mos C3 SeS grip, grip, grip, grip, kgs. kgs. kgs. kgs. 6. . . . . . 6 10 8.8 2.2 9. 12 8.10 2.10 8.49 7. . . . . . 6 7 12.9 1.9 10.65 9.80 2.50 9.99 8. . . . . . 6 14 11.1 2.3 11. 67 10. 60 2.00 10.96 9. . . . . . 6 11 12.6 1.6 13.88 12.20 1.10 12.99 10. . . . . . 6 13 15.4 1.7 15.43 14.70 2.30 14.46 il . . . . . . 6 11 16.8 3.3 17.65 15.60 3.10 16.58 12. . . . . . 6 Q 19.3 2.3 20. 19 16.70 2.70 18.97 13. . . . . . 6 12 24.2 4.2 23.49 22.90 4.58 21.80 14. . . . . . 6 10 23.7 3.5 26.10 22.40 3.12 24.03 15. . . . . . 6 14 27.5 3.8 27.91 25.40 4.07 25.80 16. . . . . . 6 5 35.2 3.4 29.50 30.10 2.80 27.31 17. . . . . . 6 7 32.0 3.1 29.63 29.00 2.85 27.55 18. . . . . . 6 1 33.0 | . . . . . . 29.87 28.00 | . . . . . . . . 27.77 ; is a tº a e º ; 4. 39.0 2.0 | . . . . . . . . . . . . 32.25 5.25 | . . . . . . . . . . . - the deaf boys at ages 7, Io, II, I2 and I4 years and inferior at ages 6, 8, 9, 12, 13, 15, 16, 17 and 18. The deaf girls are weaker in right hand grip than the norms for hearing girls at ages 6, 8, 9, Io, II, 12, 14, 15 and slightly stronger at ages 7, 13, 16, 17 and 18. Practically the same com- parative status obtains in grip of left hand of these deaf girls. Again because the number of cases of the deaf is so small, a comparative estimate of the children as a whole may better be 38 EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES made from the number of children of the group who are equal to or superior to the average of our normal children in this test of strength. In actual numbers it is found that ninety boys out of two hundred and four, or 44.1 per cent, are equal to or Superior to the averages for hearing children. With the left hand, as a group, the deaf boys stand higher than with the right hand, in- asmuch as one hundred out of two hundred and four, or 49.7 per cent, are equal or superior to the average of children of the ages included in the norms. In the case of the deaf girls there are fifty-five both left and right hand out of the total number of one hundred and twenty- four, or 44.4 per cent, who stand equal to or Superior to the norms of hearing children for the ages represented. It appears, therefore, that the deaf boys as a group more nearly approach the norms of their hearing companions than the girls with the left hand, while the advantage is slightly in the favor of deaf girls as a group in respect to the right hand. GRIP INDEX. The observation has quite frequently been noted * that left- handedness is found more frequently among deaf children than among the hearing. Various reasons have been assigned for this peculiarity, although all agree, however, in supposing that there is some intimate connection between speech and this dif- ferentiation in hand power or selectiveness in manipulation. In Report No. 2 (1900) of this Department, the data with reference to changes in growth of hand strength of normal children were presented. Moreover, it will be remembered that in Prof. J. Mark Baldwin's studies of his two children ++ there was an account of a noticeable preference for one hand Over another in grasping for attractive objects which made its initial appearance about the time of speech beginnings. This apparent correlation of the two modes of expression at the time of their origin seems to be again in evidence at the time of the great **Mental Development in the Child and the Race, 1896, chap. I. *Hartmann, Ueber TaubStumme, Deutsch med. Wochenschr, III, 1877. Deaf-Mutism—(trans. Cassell), 1880. J. Kerr Love; Deaf- Mutism, Chap. I, Glasgow, 1896. St. Hilaire: La Surdi-Mutite, p. 251, Paris, 1900. DEAF CHILDREN 39 transformations of life. An instance of this occurs during the pubertal epoch. In the reports of this department attention was called to the fact that the greatest difference between the strength of the hands occurred at early adolescence contempo- raneously with the greatest changes of voice. Another group of facts somewhat akin in import relates to speech defects. It was found in our work that by far the greatest number of minor speech defects was discovered among children who were pro- nouncedly left-handed or who acquired ambidexterity by reason of the teacher’s persistence and the parents’ training. This whole line of reasoning may be merely a matter of coincidence. It seems much more reasonable to suggest that all cases of minor speech defects correlate directly with inferior intelligence, as TABLE XI. MANUAL INDEX.* GRIP. Boys Girls Deaf Hearing Deaf Hearing A ge Yrs. Mos. No. Of Index | No. of | Index No. of Index No. of | Index cases per cent cases | per cent CaseS per Cent CaSeS per Cent 6. ... 6 7 88.8 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 89.6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7.... 6 28 90.0 8 Q3.1 7 85.0 9 90.0 8. ... 6 24 87.0 29 92.0 14 93.0 28 92.9 9. ... 6 19 86.0 44 93.1 12 91.0 30 91.5 10.... 6 23 89.0 42 93.04 13 92.0 41 91.7 11. ... 6 16 94.0 44 95.4 11 94.0 45 92.1 12. ... 6 18 92.0 40 91.6 9 86.0 47 92.4 13. ... 6 18 91.0 48 91.1 12 95.0 45 93.6 14. ... 6 14 94.0 36 91.8 8 92.6 64 93.1 15.... 6 14 93.0 31 92.5 14 91.0 47 91.7 16.... 6 11 93.0 20 90.9 6 93.16 39 90.6 17. ... 6 5 94.2 90 91.8 7 90.85 51 89.9 18. ... 6 4 93.2 37 89.4 4. 83.0 75 89.9 *NOTE.-The measure of the Index is in each case, the per cent which the weaker hand is of the stronger. The degree of right- or left-handedness is not therefore indicated. indeed our work with subnormal pupils would seem to indicate, and that ambidexterity is one of the marks of inferior mental- ity instead of the converse, as further work with reform school boys gave evidence of. (See General Reports II and III). This feature of the whole matter is held in reserve for another series of studies. It must not be forgotten, however, that this matter of the 4O EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES number of deaf children who are left-handed is a relatively sec- Ondary problem in comparison with the larger aspects of growth in hand strength which center about the differences in hand power between the right and left hands of deaf children through- out the years of growth. This growth in specialization is indicated in Table XI, in terms of manual index. The measure of the manual index is in each instance the per cent which the weaker hand is of the Stronger, and for convenient comparison the index of normal hearing children is placed in adjoining columns, both for boys and girls. There seems to be greater difference—that is, the manual in- dex is less—between the strength of grip of the hands of deaf boys as compared with hearing boys at the ages 7, 8, 9, Io and II years. Beginning with the age of I2 years and from 12 to 18 years, both inclusive, there is less difference in the strength of the hands of deaf boys as compared with their hearing mates of corresponding ages. In comparing the younger deaf boys with those older in years it is found that, in general, the differ- ence between the strength of the hands tends to diminish, that is, whereas, in the early years the manual index is less than ninety, it goes up to ninety-three or ninety-four in the older years. As the deaf boys increase in age there is less and less difference between the strength of their hands, less specialization or selectiveness in manual power. This is the direct opposite of what we find among hearing boys, among whom we find that the strength of hands is more equal in the early years, and that the difference between the hands increases perceptibly as they grow older.” This may be only another indication that deaf boys are later in maturing than hearing boys, or that the lack of activity of all sorts and especially of constructive manipulation of ma- terials is So great an environmental handicap as to practically make the deaf boys a species by themselves. There is by no means so great uniformity found in the differences of hand power of deaf girls as compared with their hearing companions of equal ages. There is less difference between the strength of *Compare Binet. Quotation from Dr. St. Hilaire, “that grip of left Jhand differs less from light hand than normals.” Also, Kilian. DEAF CHILDREN 4 I the hand of deaf girls at ages 8, Io, II, 13, 16 and 17 years; and greater difference between the hands of deaf girls than in the case of hearing girls at ages 7, 9, 12, 14, 15 and 18 years respectively. The general tendency, however, so far as this can be read from the data at hand, seems to show that up to the age of 12 years there is less difference between the strength of hands of deaf girls than hearing girls of equal ages—the direct oppo- site of the relation that was found to obtain in the case of deaf boys. In comparing the younger deaf girls with those of older years we find that the differences between the hands increase as they grow older, the opposite of what we found prevailing among deaf boys, and more in line with the trend of develop- ment of normal hearing girls. Again this may be largely due to the fact that the difference in activity which is found between deaf and hearing girls is not so great as the difference between deaf and hearing boys. To summarize, the nature of the data is such as to make it unwarrantable to assert that any differences exist in relative strength of the two hands of deaf girls as compared with their hearing sisters, but with reference to deaf boys the data seem to indicate that for younger years, during pre-adolescence, more differentiation in the strength of the two hands, respectively, ob- tains than among the hearing, and for the adolescent years, less differentiation prevails than among children with normal hear- ing—that is, the deaf are more ambidexterous. MOTOR ABILITY. In examining the voluntary output of energy one should take into account not only the gross and spasmodic total result of effort usually called strength and represented in one instance as hand-grip (Table X), but also the factor of the quickness or speed of voluntary movement. This factor of voluntary motor ability is recorded herein as simple tapping-time, and in our tests registers the number of taps that can be made with an electric tapper during thirty seconds. The two most important elements in a test, namely, precision of movement and constancy in effort and execution unfortunately we shall have to ignore entirely, and hence there is presented only the total number of taps made during thirty seconds. 42 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES TABLE XII. MOTOR ABILITY-DEAF BOYS. Right Hand Left Hand Deaf Hearing Deaf Hearing Age No. of children, children, children, children, cases average no. A. D. average no. average no. A. D. average no. Yrs. Mos. of taps, of taps, of taps, of taps, 30 secs. 30 Secs. 30 secs. 30 Secs. 5.... 6 2 76.1 15.0 l. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72.0 12.0 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6. ... 6 7 117.8 19.5 ! . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100.5 2.6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7.... 6 20 120.1 13.5 ! . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97.7 13.8 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8.... 6 22 129.2 18.6 147 105.8 22.6 117 9.... 6 22 137.9 10.8 151 115.0 12.3 127 10.... 6 22 • 138.0 19.5 161 114.6 19.0 132 11.... 6 17 146.8 16.1 169 124.1 16.1 141 12.... 6 18 163.3 15.5 170 141.2 14.4 145 13. ... 6 17 165.3 16.9 184 145.0 20.0 156 14. ... 6 14 174.0 21.5 184 145.4 18.7 155 15.... 6 17 176.4 26.4 191 152.8 16.6 169 16.... 6 12 182.0 21.5 196 159.5 20.1 170 17. ... 6 7 162.4 9.9 196 146.3 18.6 174 18. ... 6 4 164.5 12.5 197 150.6 25.7 183 19. . . . 6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20. . . .6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . MOTOR ABILITY-DEAF GIRLS. Right hand Left hand Deaf Hearing Deaf Hearing A ge No. of childen, children, children, children, cases average no. A. D. average no. average no. A. D. average no. Yrs. Mos. of taps, of taps of taps, of taps, 30 Secs. 30 Secs. 30 secs. 30 Secs. 5.... 6 1 115.0 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105.0 l. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6. ... 6 8 112.9 11.1 ! . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95.1 26.0 [ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7.... 6 13 109.9 14.5 ! . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103.7 21.3 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8. . . . 6 14 122.5 12.2 146 93.8 17.0 117 9.... 6 10 134.9 14.5 149 116.5 13.8 118 10.... 6 13 141.5 17.2 157 128.2 12.1 129 11.... 6 10 148.2 22.4 169 132.2 13.0 139 12.... 6 9 153.7 20.6 169 131.4 20.2 140 13. ... 6 12 156.9 18.1 178 137.1 26.9 153 14. ... 6 7 157.7 21.1 181 133.3 14.0 157 15. ... 6 12 174.0 24.1 181 158.7 21.0 159 16.... 6 6 177.1 12.8 188 161.1 10.5 167 17. ... 6 7 175.7 14.9 184 163.4 11.9 162 18. ... 6 1 178.0 | . . . . . . . . 193 159.0 [ . . . . . . . . 169 19.... 6 3 188.0 18.0 [ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 171.0 9.3 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20.... 6 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .''. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . In Table XII are given in parallel columns the following: (a) The number of children tested at each age. (b) The average number of taps made in thirty seconds by the children. (c) The average deviation of the records from the average. For ready comparison the averages for normal children for cor- responding ages are placed beside those of the deaf. The rec- ords are given for both boys and girls for the right and left hands. DEAF CHILDREN 43 By going over this table somewhat in detail one sees that the deaf boys are with the right hand notably and uniformly slower than normal hearing boys, and that although not so quick at any age with the left hand, they are not in this case so in- ferior, and at the age of twelve years almost equal to the hear- ing boys. Moreover, this relative slowness seems to increase as they grow older. In the case of the girls we find that they too are much slower at all ages with the right hand, and with the left, though slower than hearing girls, they are at no age as much slower relatively as with the right hand. Indeed, at the ages fifteen to seventeen, they are almost equal in quickness to hearing girls with the left hand, and at the age of seventeen years they are actually quicker on the average. Moreover, the inferiority of the girls, unlike that of the boys, appears to decrease with age, and this may be only another indication of the increased degree of general physical activity in which they come to indulge, as well as of other environmental factors. Again on account of the paucity of numbers at each age it is advantageous to take the deaf boys and girls as a group and compare them in quickness directly with the hearing boys and girls for the corresponding ages represented in each deaf group. It is thus found that of all deaf boys examined, only forty-one out of one hundred and seventy-two, or 23.8 per cent, are as fast or faster than the averages for hearing boys of their ages with right or left hand, and that only twenty-nine of a total of one hundred and one, 23.8 per cent, deaf girls are, with the right hand, equal to or above the average for normal hearing girls of corresponding ages, while only thirty-four out of ninety- nine cases, 37.3 per cent, recorded for quickness of left hand, equal or surpass the averages of hearing girls for the same ages. The relative standing of the deaf, all in all, when compared with hearing children in the cases of both boys and girls, is therefore found to be, even less favorable, when rapidity of movement is involved, than was found in the measurements of stature and weight, and only slightly better than the relative standing of the deaf in lung capacity. The facts are diagramatically exemplified in the accompany- 44 EXPERI MENTAL STUDIES ing figure, which indicates the positions occupied on a per centile scale drawn from a large number of hearing children of each age, the average standing of deaf boys and girls of correspond- ing ages. From this it is seen for example, that the average standing of seven year old deaf girls is lower than the average of the lowest Io per cent of the records selected from the performances of hearing girls. In one instance, only, I2 year old deaf girls, does their average tapping rate exceed the aver- /967, º 222222' % * £6% 70% 40% 3.2% #9% 30% 20% /ož o/ THE HORIZONTAL NUMERALS REPRESENT THE AGES OF THE RESPECTIVE GROUPS– THE PERCENTS ON THE WERTICAL LINE SHOW THE RATINGS OF THE DEAF BOYS, AND GIRLS FOR THE DIFFERENT YEARS. age of hearing girls, and in the averages for three age groups only among boys do their performances equal the average of hearing children. Further, in the tests of motor ability as with the hand- strength there is an advantage and interest in giving the manual index. This index of manual quickness (See Table XIII) measures in each instance the fraction which the slower hand DEAF CHILDREN 45 is of the faster. In comparing then the manual index, as deter- mined by relative quickness of movement, of deaf boys with hearing boys, it is seen that there is practically no difference observable until after the age of eleven and one-half years. After this age, however, the manual index is higher among the deaf boys, showing that the difference in motor control of the right and left hand is not so great as among hearing boys of the same ages, or that specialization in this method of ex- pression has not attained as high a degree among the deaf as the hearing, that is, the deaf tend more to ambidexterity than do ordinary hearing children. TABLE XIII. MANUAL INDEX.”—MOTOR ABILITY. Boys Girls Deaf Hearing Deaf Hearing A ge Yrs. Mos.' No. of No. of No. of No. of C3 SeS Index C3 SeS Index CalSeS Index CalSeS Index 6. ... 6 6 79.3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 85.5 | . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7.... 6 25 79.9 8 79.0 12 83.9 11 80.1 8.... 6 22 81. 18 33 81.4 14 79.0 8 80.32 9.... 6 21 83.7 60 82.6 10 83.6 44 79.4 10.... 6 22 81.9 47 82.3 12 86. 0 48 82.4 11.... 6 17 82.5 49 83.0 10 88.2 46 83. 1 12.... 6 18 87.1 44 82.5 9 86.1 49 80.7 13. ... 6 17 85.5 50 84.6 11 85.5 44 86.7 14. . . .6 14 83.2 40 82.08 7 85.1 68 86.1 15.... 6 16 86.1 35 86.6 11 87.2 48 86.7 16.... 6 11 91.7 21 87.1 6 91.1 40 88.0 17. ... 6 7 88.9 13 88.7 7 92.1 51 88.1 18. ... 6 3 86.2 3 93.3 1. 89.0 # 5.5 *NOTE.-The measure of the Index is in each instance the per cent which the slower hand is of the faster. Again in running our eye over the data in each column to determine differences due to age among deaf boys, it will readily be seen that in general there is not so great a difference between the younger and the older deaf boys as is found to obtain in the case of the younger normal hearing boys and the older hear- ing boys. With respect to the manual index of deaf girls it is seen that at nearly every age there is not so great a difference in the control of the hands as is found to prevail among the hear- ing girls of comparable ages. This difference between the deaf and hearing girls is very pronounced at some ages, notably at 46 EXPERIMIENTAL STUDIES ages, seven, nine, ten, eleven, twelve, fifteen, sixteen, and seven- teen years and not so marked or even slightly less at ages, eight, thirteen, fourteen, and eighteen years. It appears safe to war- rant the statement, however, that there is less pronounced dif- ferentiation or selectiveness in motor manipulation among deaf girls than is found in the case of their hearing sisters. One fact stands out prominently in the tests of motor abil- ity, notably that both deaf boys and deaf girls of I2 years and older more nearly approach the standing of hearing children than do those of the younger years. That is, as deaf children increase in age, they become more nearly like normal hearing children. There seems to be no other casual factor to which this relative increase in efficiency with age can be attributed ex- cept that lack of definite motor control is intimately related to mental maturity. Since in their early years, deaf children have had few experiences as compared with hearing children, this increase in motor ability would be more rapid with mental train- ing than is that of the hearing child. This suggestion of the very close correlation between mentality and the development of the nervous mechanism is likewise illustrated and exempli- fied by work which has been done on primitive people. These show that while not greatly inferior to the normal average white people of civilization in strength for their stature, there is a marked inferiority in their quickness of response. This differ- ence in moving time is apparent even in the doing of things with little or no meaning, as well as in the performance of acts that require a more complex mental factor to initiate and control them. It may be said that responses are the result of Stimula- tion, and that the nervous mechanism, subject to the more varied and greater number of sensations appreciated, other things being equal, will respond the more rapidly. This cor- relation suggests the very interesting psychological problem of the inverse influence of the psychical on the efficiency of the motor mechanism. The question of the influence of bodily more- ments in giving rise to mental content has long been recognized. The influence of the psychical on the physical means that an unused mind, no matter from what caused, is accompanied by sluggish bodily action. It suggests also, the influence which School training of the purely mental type has in affecting an in- crease in manual productivity. MENTALITY. The problem of determining the mental status of deaf chil- dren is infinitely more complicated than that of estimating the psychical efficiency of hearing children. This is to be attributed in no small measure to the fact that their symbols for express- ing themselves and making known to us the contents of their minds are few and inadequate. On account of the paucity of symbols, too, it becomes exceed- ingly difficult to devise any tests which may be used with the same degree of facility and certitude on the deaf and hearing children alike, and thus make a quantitative statement of the differences of mentality between a deaf and hearing child. This does not mean, of course, that the mental condition of a deaf child cannot be determined with a fair degree of certainty by one who has had special training and has acquired special skill in bringing out and interpreting responses which are indicative of the deaf child's mental power. In testing the mentality the aim was more specifically that of examining definite functions rather than of evaluating proficiency in any particular line of School training. - Proceeding from the simplest to the more complex processes, simple tests were given to determine the following functions of the child: (a) Imitative ability; (b) Suggestibility; i. e., his ability to follow directions; (c) Perceptive ability; i. e., the child’s efficiency or skill in discriminating sense differences in degree and kind. (d) Capability in reproducing immediately (at once) a con- text which is relatively without connection with other meanings, i. e., the immediate sense memory. (With the deaf children the reproduction of things heard is eliminated.) (e) Ability to think, i. e., to intelligently use such con- cepts as cause and effect, part and whole, and the like. With reference to the general power of thinking, it should be stated that in speaking of such a highly complex mental activ- 48 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES ity as the power of forming good judgments, it is always necessary to tell in advance the particular field in which this activity is operative. It is a common observation that persons may exercise good judgment in dealing with one phase of human life, and yet show exceptionally poor judgment with another group of facts. A man, for instance, may be sound in Science but most eccentric and unsafe in business and religion. Hence it is unwise to use any arbitrary tests for general comparison of individuals or groups, who may happen to have had diverse interests and different training. In this test of the deaf chil- dren, an estimate was made merely of each child’s ability to size up situations and in general to interpret and communicate meanings. In addition it should be noted that the simpler tests were used for diagnostic purposes, and no effort was made to state them in quantitative terms. The question of primary import- ance was: Has the child a mind which is well balanced or sane, and susceptible of training, or is he feeble-minded ? Imitation. To determine whether the child had sufficient mental abil- ity to imitate, the following tests were employed: The child was placed at a distance of four or five feet facing the experi- menter. He was encouraged to observe attentively and execute the same acts which the experimenter performed, such as (I) placing the feet together, heels and toes; (2) standing erect, with head and shoulders thrown back; (3) raising one or both hands to a horizontal position at the side, and in front or per- pendicularly above the shoulders; (4) with the hands extended horizontally in front, the child spread apart his fingers, follow- ing the experimenter, holding now one, and now two, together. Suggestibility. To determine the child's suggestibility, two tests were em- ployed. In the first the child performed certain acts which he had just seen the experimenter execute. He made three, four or five, or any number of steps, after watching the experimenter perform the same, or he tapped a certain number of times in response to the same number performed by the experimenter. This implies that an idea or image of the performance or end to DEAF CHILDREN 49 be attained exists in the mind of the child, and that he has the ability to work out the meaning of the image or idea in some definite act or series of acts. There is also involved in a sug- gestion test the concept or translation from one sense to an- other. In order for the child to take three steps as a result of having seen three hand movements, it is necessary that he have definitely formed the concept of three, and this is by no means a simple function. In the second test for suggestibility packs of specially pre- pared cards were used, and the child was required to sort them in groups of their likes and unlikes. Three of such packs were employed, each containing forty cards. On the cards of the first series were pasted small circles of eight elementary colors. After the cards had been thoroughly mixed the child was requested to Sort them into piles, according to color. On the cards in the second series were pictures of familiar objects instead of colors; in the third simple geometrical forms. The procedure in all three parts of the series was practically identical. How- ever simple and elementary these tests for imitation and suggest- ibility may seem, experience with large numbers of children in the Child Study laboratory has shown that feeble-minded chil- dren are unable to execute even such simple operations. Perception. As tests which largely involve simple perception three were employed; one calling for sense discrimination by sight, the sec- ond by the sense of touch and the third bringing into play the muscular sense. To test the first function—sense discrimination by sight— use was made of a special form of what is customarily known as the “A” Test. (See form of this test, pp. 50 and 51.) The test is one which has done service in the Child Study Laboratory of the Chicago Public Schools for a number of years, and was invented and devised largely for diagnostic purposes. As is seen, the test is composed of two parts. In the first, one hundred “a’s” (18 point size) are arranged symmetrically in rows, so as to involve the perceptive factor to a negligible degree, in mark- ing them out, while in the second half of the test the one hund- 4 & – 3 red “a’s” are scattered promiscuously among the remaining let- ( 5O EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES 63c3 ∞c3 63&3 c3c3 636:3 c3G3 63c3 c3c3 63&3 c3c3 c363 636:3 ∞63 c3∞ 6363 c363 G3c3 g3§3 (C. S. & P. I. 10) - EOARD OF EDUCATION CITY OF CIHICAGO Drºp'ARTMENT of CEIILD STUDY AND E*EDA GOGIC INVESTIGATION Lskse evavy dawma kzaje xatzs cmjaf aruja ahalv iwige flºakp xmvwa parka Sugho rvsth yraea Zaqwb qiajc mygpa ehaih angao ymljd frazd Wolaks gsbnc azeqa grhng mayvk Zlaah spova iaucfakiwr hkdaf tafta puwaa rayxe yoirr wbuyx laehu Qnhra kaqul. Vaiso rnhay thcma biezw uvaru oajba jmzao tutzb zkapd lhnaf batxz xslac dhzrd feqagogaxl gunka Onawz fatdg. Plexw mefra ifano pivka xawye cirga laxoe adgfb linam asduh himſa csamy jekax ejwoa mpadu typia easvy nzyaz diuno ebola watma majtj abeta jaafb ahuys. Oqaql iumwa kaqpy baxap skaob cmfaz qatay aqizsigabg narbq wogad alcbg bpagx vyxpa mafwe vtjat kpjea 52 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES ters of the alphabet, so as to form one hundred groups of letters of five each. The first part of the “A” test was designed with two ob- jects in view. By taking the time required by the child to place a stroke with a pencil through each of the one hundred “a’s” when he has been requested to do it as rapidly as possible, a measure is afforded of a certain type of motor ability. But in addition to this there is also afforded, which is more particularly to the purpose of the test in hand, a measure of the physiological, or motor time consumed in marking one hundred “a’s.” After completing part one, the child was instructed to place a stroke through the a's in the page print (see p. 51), as quickly as possible, being sure to mark out every “a”; the instructions be- ing uniformily as given above for deaf and hearing children alike. A measure of the time required to perform this test was also taken and a record made of the number of “a’s” not marked. It is readily apparent that at least two factors enter into the selection and marking of “a’s” as called for in this test; the one a mental factor concerned in the several acts of discrimina- tion in finding the “a's,” and the second, physiological, having to do with the motor task of making the marks. To disinte- grate these factors and arrive at a measure of the purely men- tal time actually involved in perception, is the point of chief moment, to make the test of value in stating quantitative differ- ences between individuals and groups. And this is acomplished approximately by subtracting the motor time gotten in the first part of the test from the total time required to perform the sec- ond part. This latter we shall call the “Perception Time” (P. T.) in the discussions and tables. There is one factor in connection with the “A-Test” which makes the results awkward to handle. If every child marked out all the “a's,” differences in perception might be stated directly in terms of the perception time. The fact is, however, that many children fail to find all the a's, there being sometimes as many as forty that the child does not see and mark out. Here, con- sequently, are two variables, which thus far there has been found no satisfactory way of equating in terms of each other. Thus t TABLE XIV. PERCEPTION TEST.-“A” TEST. DEAIF CHILDREN Motor time, is average żrej do đ0 § „S,!!!!: : gĖ | eu º ecº eq eº:§: ; ; È È Ě || ~ ~ ~č r-<+ cn eñº e º : 3-4 •: (1) &=& -4-9 º ºg <ſ> <∞ → <> • H || ~º ~º ~º ~º ~º ~º ~º ~º ~º ~º : : : 6N OOc^ o^ o^ u^or)č ğ85 g§§§§§ęſęgęş ; vej35 º :ſì, „r3 ©● }g | sº cº r* * e * oº * sº º ; ; ; ºſ;�©N \,) C^ \O → OO OO vO vO O • �3 • º | ~ ~ ~ ~ ~º ~º ~º ~º ~* · * * * * * !!!<∞Oo $ $ | 33333$$$$$ $ º ‘8 %ur) (*) №e (*) oo CN<+ $ ${}{}{333}{3&ş; tº Z Ű º wej Z go ğ º „ºd &{}} | uqº !! !! + cºuņoº * * * ſqr: §§ ğgºoooº -- + en en ºyunun * •, GD šºs 8 �•C)!={ c^© :-)© ®© Cº <+> <ſ> eN, OO ^o CN <+ ON OOur) Oo ^• „€.. | cſ & & & &•ð oò ~ ~ ob s oòn, Q Ll- | O | |?- 3 9015 07042 RSITY OFM lili nºſūTETETTII) S se, N. Y. PAI, JAN, 21, 1908 yracul !!!!=bºssa-ººrs:::::: „……………….……=…=.=…=.=…=::::::::::::::::-- , , ; , , §<!, sae !!!!!!!! №t!!!-|- - , , , ,·- ·· -· №~~ ~~·- · <!--****************!!!!!、、。- -- · -----<!--','<-, º-vºs!!!!!!!!!!! ^|- ·--------±∞∞∞--~· ---------- ,,,,,,,,=,<>**************|----- ·· -¿?¿? ſuae*= *ae · -¿№:、、 ,,,,,,,…)· -(~~~~, ,¿¿.*¿№Ē№rº~~' ſ ºzºț¢”. ſº ºs gael~~ ™***...', ∞. ſaei - *.x); №š: