WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE AIKEN 820.5 729 RONALD A 1,020,944 PAAS MADUR S *** TAMALLEETSİMULA: Saša 11817 SCIENTIA ARTES OF THE VERITAS UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN LIBRARY UMIBU: PLUM olet **NOTETAA Paraparauma jam salarygo* parket MsAP mig at kontrar al die wat's allhane TUEBOR SIQUE AIS-PENINSULAM AMŒNAM” FIRCUMSPICE VAWAWAFANYA TYA BILUTE RPADLARTELENTK roy the Face Bangkok. LASTA #Sugarciënekst: WHY ENGLISH V SOUNDS CHANGE By JANET (RANKIN) AIKEN, PH.D. LECTURER IN ENGLISH, COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY друг Ⓡ THE RONALD PRESS COMPANY NEW YORK ! • ; Copyright, 1929, by THE RONALD PRESS COMPANY All Rights Reserved 1 PREFATORY NOTE The nature of this study has precluded extensive use of authorities. A few standard works on his- torical English have been quoted, in order to cite a prevailing, or more or less accepted, statement of linguistic fact or theory. The books so quoted are: Jespersen's "Language" and "Philosophy of Gram- mar," Krapp's "Pronunciation of Standard English in America," Wright's "Old English Grammar, Bloomfield's "Study of Language," and Wyld's "Short History of English.' 19 Thanks are due to Professors William Witherle Lawrence, Ashley H. Thorndike, A. V. Williams Jackson, and Louis Herbert Gray, of Columbia University, for reading and constructive criticism of the manuscript. Helpful suggestions have been made by Professor Harry Morgan Ayres. A greater debt of gratitude is due Professor George Philip Krapp, who not only has contributed largely to the theories themselves, but has most patiently and inspiringly supervised this book during the five years of its working out. JANET RANKIN AIKEN West Redding, Connecticut, September 3, 1929. 99 iii CONTENTS CONSONANTS AND VOWELS ACCORD Are They Generically Distinct? Basis for Vowel-Consonant Correlation Speech Sounds in Detail General Conclusions CHAPTER I Definition of Accord Approximation Accord Ablation Accord Mutation Accord Recurrence Accord CHAPTER II Accord in Other Fields Other Principles Operative • CHAPTER III OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE The Modulation of Sounds The Principle of Grading Types of Grading in English Tensity or Stress, not Quantity Types of Stress Grading The Principle of Conservation Spelling Influence The Intellectual Element Summary . • • • 3 6 10 18 22235 29 33 35 38 40 H*4ROBNON 41 43 45 49 50 55 57 60 62 PAGE 3 22 41 ... V vi CONTENTS CHAPTER IV ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS Few Instances Analyzed Elimination of Gutturals Other Languages Compared A Theory of Linguistic Development Palatalization of [k] Satem and Centum OE [g] Before Front Vowels Medial [S] and [ž] General Conclusions CHAPTER V ACCORD IN ENGLISH Vowels Theories of Sound Change Why Have Tense Vowels Become Raised? Slack Vowels Not Raised • The Influence of [r] Other Consonant Ablations. Other Exceptions Due to Contiguous Consonant Vowel Tensings Slackenings in Vowels Diphthongization General Conclusions • • CHAPTER VI GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS Newness of Accord Immediate Causes of Sound Change The Revival of Learning Change Also Continuous Time and Place Not Important More Than One Principle of Change Theory Re-stated Comments on General System • · • • • ***INNI?? 76 80 82 85 90 93 96 99 100 102 104 106 109 111 113 116 117 119 122 123 125 PAGE 65 85 111 CONTENTS vii CHAPTER VII ACCORD OF IDEAS Need for a Simplified Terminology Accord in Morphology Accord in Syntax Accord in Semantics APPENDIX: A FEW RULES STATED TABLES • • 127 131 135 138 I. Speech Sounds Classified According to Active Por- tion of Tongue II. Occurrence of Consonantal Sounds in English III. Approximation Accord IV. Ablation Accord V. Factors in Sound Change VI. Tense Vowel Changes VII. Frequency of Consonant Occurrence PAGE 127 141 9 12 26 31 282383 63 91 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE I CHAPTER I CONSONANTS AND VOWELS Are They Generically Distinct? To the remote inventors of the hieroglyphs, vowel sounds probably mattered little. At least they were not considered worth the trouble of recording. There are no hieroglyphic vowels, nor does the vowel appear in the early Semitic alphabets, which are supposedly related in some way to the Egyptian. Sanskrit has few symbols originally denoting vowels, and even today we use an alphabet so meagerly furnished with vowel symbols that each of the five we possess must be made to carry an enormous orthographic load. Today, however, the pendulum has swung, and the vowel has become the pride and joy of linguists, the consonant appearing comparatively uninteresting, or at least comparatively unregarded. The absence or paucity of vowel symbols in early alphabets by no means indicates that vowel sounds were absent or few in number. Contrary to the con- clusions of the older school of linguists, who assigned to Sanskrit, early Semitic, etc., only three vowel sounds in all, it is now generally agreed that early languages had their full proportion of vowels, prob- ably as many as we have today. True, we cannot reconstruct the systems, for lack of conclusive evi- 3 4 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE dence; but we can be reasonably certain that a linguistic enthusiast set down among the Akkadians or the Sumerians would have found plenty of vowel distinctions to analyze. It is not difficult to understand how vowels might easily be omitted in early alphabets. The consonant pattern, generally speaking, is the word. G--rg- W-sh-ngt-n w-s- gr--t m-n is simplicity itself to read, while -eo-e -a--i---o- -a- a --ea- -a- could hardly be read without further clue. The child spelling by ear will seldom leave out a sounded consonant, whereas vowels are frequently omitted, e.g., fnish, dinr, apl, RNSR (answer; A. A. Milne, Winnie-the-Pooh, page 46). The vowel, by the very nature of its articulation, is a weak, "open" tone, and acts largely as an accompaniment to the stronger consonant. At least so much must be said for the customary opposition of vowels and consonants in current lin- guistic writing, an opposition so hallowed by tradi- tion as to be practiced automatically in every study in this field. And yet a further question may be asked. Granting that vowels are weaker than con- sonants, that the distinction between them is fre- quently useful and has been apparent in linguistic practice since the very dawn of written language, yet may vowels and consonants properly be treated invariably as two distinct genera? Is the distinction between them one of kind, or one of degree? And if the latter, may not linguistics as a science have lost CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 5 something by its invariable separation and opposi- tion of the two? Consonants and vowels are frequently differen- tiated by the statement that vowels are the more open, less obstructed impulses of the breath; and as between [p], perhaps the most obviously obstructed of English speech sounds, and [a], the most open vowel, this distinction is easily apparent. Only one sound, [h], is as open as [a]. Yet, oddly enough, [h] is numbered among the consonants; and there we are, with two excessively open sounds, one a vowel, the other not a vowel. Again, it is asserted that only a vowel can carry a syllable; and yet phoneticians recognize syllabic n, chicken, l, little, and r, dinner. Also we have a few words such as sh, which contain no vowel. The consonants n, 1, and r are frequently called semi- vowels; but such a term in itself certainly does not support the notion of any generic distinction between the two groups. Such another instance as the word tune, whose pronunciation passes easily, almost imperceptibly, from [tiun] to [tjun] and thence to [tsun], seems to indicate some sort of affinity in the formation of [1], [j], and [s], at least in these particular vocal surroundings. Indeed, it seems impossible to find an exact line of demarcation between vowels and consonants such as those differentiating cats from tables or red from B sharp. Like the colors, the speech sounds appear to shade into one another in a 6 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE continuous series from obstructed to open, from strong to weak. And this being the case, it appears feasible to treat them as one series. The present study will first attempt an analysis of English speech sounds in one group instead of two, and on a single basis. Such an analysis, in bringing out likenesses and correlations rather than perpetuating differences, may perhaps cast light upon some linguistic problems. No other radical departure from traditional methods will need to be made; indeed, few if any novel facts will have to be sought. But perhaps simply the regrouping or re- analyzing of the known facts of English linguistic history may illumine some dark places. Basis for Vowel-Consonant Correlation The first problem obviously must consist in find- ing a common basis for classifying English speech sounds together as members of one system. To accomplish this, the usual descriptive categories must be modified, because these categories do not apply equally to consonants and to vowels. For example, consonants are ordinarily divided into voiced (media) and breath (tenues). This dis- tinction does not apply to vowels, which are all voiced. The consonants are likewise distinguished from one another by the character of the sound, as continuants (opens, spirants), stops (surds, explo- sives, plosives), nasals, fricatives, affricatives, sibi- lants, aspirates, and a well-nigh endless list besides. CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 7 These categories are meaningless for vowels, which are all open sounds. And on the other hand, there are vowel categories seldom or never applied to consonants, as tense- slack, rounded-unrounded, and high-mid-low. Upon what basis then may all speech sounds profitably be compared? Unquestionably the tongue is an important factor in sound modification, and it is the nature or place of a given sound's formation or modification by the tongue which will determine its place in the present system. To accomplish this, it will be necessary to take only a single step beyond the present practice, since vowels are already classified minutely according to the region of the tongue active in their formation, into back-front-blade, and high-mid-low; and con- sonants also are classified on the back-front basis, though not hitherto as high, raid, or low.¹ If then we use the accepted data for speech sounds as classified on the back-front system, and (for vowels) on the high-mid-low system, about three- fourths of the labor will be found already accom- plished, for a reclassification of English speech sounds, to show the part of the tongue active in forming each sound, and the height of that active It must not be supposed that the breath-voice, rounded- unrounded, etc., categories will be disregarded in this study. They are omitted from the table of sounds because it would be imprac- ticable to show them there; but they will be utilized frequently in making explanations of various linguistic facts. In general it may be said that all consonants are tense in comparison with vowels, and that most consonants (w an exception) are unrounded. 8 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE part. Thus the task becomes by no means a diffi- cult one. Table I shows 42 English speech sounds graphi- cally placed according to the portion of the tongue which is active in the formation of each, and in such a fashion as to correlate consonants and vowels. This table will form a basis for subsequent discussion. The sound positions which have been charted in this table represent what may be taken as the cardi- nal or normal, not the only, region of formation for a given English sound. Many or most sounds can be made or approximated with the tongue in other positions, and few are definitely stable in their region of origin. "There are as many varieties of [h] as there are varieties of vowels before which it may stand." (Krapp PSEA 15.) And the sound [k] can be produced at any point of a fairly large mouth area of contact between tongue and hard palate. The vowels, especially the long ones, are likewise unstable, shifting in endless variability. These are extreme examples, but it is not too much to say that no single sound in the speech of any individual carries at all times precisely the same value; and therefore the only question concerns the degree of variability, not the fact of its presence. Hence the chart of sounds in Table I must be taken as an approximation, not a definitive plan. Except where specifically stated otherwise, Amer- ican English of a Middle Western type has been used as standard. This is done for two reasons: first, the in that and CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 6 TABLE I. SPEECH SOUNDS CLASSIFIED ACCORDING TO ACTIVE PORTION OF TONGUE A. High B. High-mid... [p] [b] [m] [f] [v] C. Mid. D. Low-mid.. 1. Lip E. Low. 2. Point 3. Blade 4. Center (Front) [t] [d] [s] [z] | [s] [ž] [b] [8] [n] [1] [r] [1] [R] [i] [y] [I] [j] [k₂] [82] [e] [A] [9] [ɛ] [ə] [x] 5. Back [u] [U] [o] [W] [M] [ɔ] [g.] [k] [n] [a] [x] [3] Note: The sound [h] is omitted since its position varies approximately from B3 to E5. T 10 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE author knows this type of speech best from use and observation; and secondly, certain sound elements ([M], the inverted r, etc.) are preserved in this speech although lost in Standard British English. Use of this speech type of American English is a conven- ience, and should not affect the validity of any con- clusions which may be reached. With these considerations in mind, let us examine Table I in detail. The comments will relate largely to height, since the factor of comparative frontness is already well established. Speech Sounds in Detail B1. The lip consonants² [b], [p], [f], [v], [m], are all classified as of high-mid height, although as a matter of fact the height of the tongue is not the significant factor in their production, and may vary from mid (model, potter, father) to high-mid (pease, feeble, veer). They may be described as of a neutral high-mid tongue position, although on the whole they are to be classed with the high consonants. A2. The largest single group of sounds in Table I is, as might be imagined, those consonants formed by the active tip or point of the tongue. As a group, these are the most stable of English speech sounds. Only one, [1], varies in tongue position to any extent. By the very character of their formation they neces- sarily remain substantially the same in tongue posi- 2 The terms consonant and vowel will not of course be discarded on account of their inclusion into a single system. CONSONANTS AND VOWELS II tion, no matter what the contiguous sounds may be. Such variation as may occur (e.g., between s in stone and saw) does not materially affect the tongue position. The production of [1] varies somewhat more widely, e.g., in eel and awl. Point consonants (A2 and B2) not only are the largest single consonant group, but approximately two of every three consonant sounds used in actual speech belong to this group. This fact is set forth in Table II, an analysis of some 5,000 consonant speech sounds based upon four representative pho- netic transcriptions giving sounds as used in ordinary speech (Krapp PSEA 151-170). The transcriptions were analyzed in two separate groups and the results compared. As the proportions of the various sounds were found substantially the same in both groups, it is probable that the figures here given are fairly representative of the frequency of consonant occur- rence in general, although this could not be es'ab- lished definitely without more extensive investigation than is practicable here. Table II will not be discussed in detail at this point. The facts set forth will enter into subsequent discus- sion, but here it is sufficient merely to note the large preponderance of point consonants in English speech. B2. The point consonant group includes, besides the eight sounds placed as A2, two slightly lower sounds, the semi-vowel [r], and its attenuated echo [1]. These weak consonants are all that is left of the OE strong trill [R]. It is supposed that [R] was 12 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE TABLE II. OCCURRENCE of ConsonANTAL SOUNDS IN ENGLISH Lip.. .[b] 140 [p] 163 [f] 141 [v] 157 [m] 242 Total lip........843 Point.....[d] 393 [t] 580 [s] 441 [z] 244 [b] 28 [૪] 237 [n] 496 [1] 353 [r] 223 [1] 319 Total point.....3314 Blade.....[S] 104 [ž] 29 Total blade......133 Front.. Back³ Front.....[il Total front. Back³.....[g] 45 79 [k] 207 [w] 139 [M] 36 [n] 99 Total back......560 SUMMARY AND PERCENTAGES .. 45 Aspirate...[h] 118 Total aspirate....118 Grand Total....5013 Number Per cent of total 843 Lip... 16.8 Point.. .. 3314 66.1 Blade...... 133 2.7 45 0.9 560 11.2 Aspirate... 118 2.3 5013 100.0 3 The two sounds [g] and [k] are not divided into their so-called guttural and palatal values, represented here as [g], [k], and [ga], [kal, because this distinction was not made in the original transcrip- tions. If this were done, the percentage of front sounds would be considerably increased. CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 13 a point trill, and it is so placed here; but the details of its development to [r] are not entirely clear (see pages 96-99). The sound [r] is not stable even initially (compare real and raw) and except at the beginning of a syllable (rat, very) it has disappeared from the standard speech of parts of England and of the United States. In this respect [r] is like [h] and [w], both of which have become sounds so weak as to be audible only at the beginning of a syllable. A3. Slightly farther back in the mouth than the group of sounds just described are the pair [S] and [ž]. Neither of these is stable (compare Shah and she), but like all the other consonants, these are sufficiently uniform to give the effect of the same sound in various words. These two are perhaps slightly lower than, e.g., [t] and [d], where the tip of the tongue actually touches the roof of the mouth. B3. The tense vowel [i], a "long" sound, is the highest of the vowels, and is produced farthest front in the mouth. Its rounded equivalent is [y], found in OE and in modern French. The so-called "short" value of [i], namely [1], is slack where [i] is tense, and is slightly lower. A comparison of the vowel of deed with that of geek will reveal the range of variability of the pronunciation of [i], and [1] varies to much the same extent (compare king and did).4 4 As "long" and "short" vowels are in fact entirely distinct sounds, and are sufficiently differentiated by using separate symbols, the phonetic "long sign" [:] will not be used in this study. Į 14 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE A4. The final group of high sounds includes [j] (y in yet), [k2], and [g]. The first of these is a semi- vowel, and is very near to [i] and [1], both in placing and in pronunciation. Here we may see graphically how the word tune passes from [tiun] to [tjun] and thence to [tsun]. The three sounds [1], [j], and [s], are easily transferable, being all continuant sounds and all near one another in place of production. The word duty may follow a similar sequence, [diuti], [djuti], and [džuti]. The sounds [k] and [g] represent high or "palatal" k or g. These values occur in conjunction with high sounds. The k of keen, for example, would be [k]; that of cart, [k,]; geese has [g], garden [g.]. These sounds are both slightly farther back than [j]. High and low [k] and [g] will not always be differ- entiated in this study, as the distinction is often not essential to the argument. C4. A group of five vowel sounds falls within this area. First, there is the tense sound [e] (a in mate) and its slack companion vowel [e]. A slack vowel slightly back of [e] is [^] (u in up, nut, etc.). Also in this group we have [9], the vowel heard before [1] in the American "r-full" pronunciation of bird, word, etc. This "inverted" vowel is discussed at length in Chapter IV.5 And lastly, there is the slack, “neutral” vowel [ə], very like [^] in pronunci- 5 Phoneticians also recognize [a:], the vowel of the "r-less" pro- nunciation of hurt, bird, etc. This is not [A] lengthened but another, tense, vowel. As the present study is based on American "r-full” speech, it is not necessary to include [A:] in our table. CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 15 ation but shorter and slacker. This is unquestionably the most common vowel sound of English, since it may appear in place of any other vowel in unstressed position. Why is it that we find so many vowels centering in this area, just as we found so many consonants (obstructed sounds) whose obstruction was caused by the active tongue tip? It seems plain that these vowels all involve a position of the tongue nearly (in the case of [ə], quite) where it falls in a state of rest. In other words, the vowels center about the easiest position for emitting sound, the consonants about the easiest mechanism for obstructing sound. D4. Below these may be placed [æ], a sound of very frequent occurrence in English, and a variant with [a] in such words as grass, laugh; etc. In all the words where [æ] alternates with [a], professional speech experts (elocutionists) will advise pronouncing [a]. Indeed, the sound [æ] is regarded by such teachers as definitely distasteful and reprehensible wherever avoidance of it is found possible. So far as I know, no explanation of this avoidance has been attempted; but is it not reasonable to sup- pose that it is because of the facial expression in- volved in pronouncing [æ]? The nose is slightly elevated by the flattening of the lips, and the lip corners are drawn down, giving an expression almost precisely that which normally follows the apprehen- sion of a bad odor or some other unpleasantness. Compare with this the facial expression normal to 16 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE X [a], and I believe it can hardly be doubted that this difference causes the avoidance of [æ]. B5. The highest sounds in the back series are the tense vowel [u], and its slack companion [u]. If the vocal cavity be regarded as a pipe, with the vocal chords at one end and the lips at the other, then [a] would be called a "short-thick-pipe" sound, since in its formation the lips are flattened back against the teeth and the tongue held low, making a sharp contrast to the production of the "long-thin- pipe" sound [u], where the lips are protruded as if for whistling, and the tongue held high to produce an elongated, thin vocal cavity. All the rounded vowels, as well as the consonant w, have this char- acteristic, and hence are now and then called lip sounds." The consonants [w] and [M] show close affinity with the vowel [u]. A simple fashion of teaching foreign students the sound [w] is to substitute [u], saying, e.g., u-ater for water. The breath equivalent of [w], namely [M], is disappearing from standard British English, as the breath equivalents of n, r, and I have already done. Many Englishmen make no distinction between when and wen, which and witch, etc. This practice is frowned upon by lan- guage teachers in this country. Slightly lower than [w] is the tense rounded vowel "The back vowels u, o, etc., are classed as the first or frontmost series in Hebrew grammars, while w is called a lip consonant by Wyld and others. CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 17 /// [o]. In speech this vowel tends to diphthongize, giving note [nout], etc. This tendency, which applies to most of the tense vowels in British speech, is less strong in American English. D5. In the next mouth area fall three con- sonantal and one (or two) vowel sounds. The symbol [ɔ] in this study represents the tense vowel of law, fault. The same symbol is often used to represent the slack concomitant of [o], but there is an ambiguity in the use of this symbol, inasmuch as the long or tense [ɔ] is a sound different in quality from short or slack [o]. Since, however, American speech tends to use the tense sound in place of the slack in, e.g., God, frost, only the tense sound is represented in our table. To be exact, at least four values for k should be recognized, instead of two. The pronunciation of k differs perceptibly in key, coo, Coe, and caw. The k-region (which is also the g-region) is perhaps larger than that for any other sound except h. When like a hammer the tongue strikes the hard palate at any point within this area, a sound is produced which we recognize as [k]. The final sound in this group does not vary over a wide range. This is [n], a sound with the peculiarity that it does not occur initially in English. In words where n is followed by g or k, [n] is substituted for [n] (anchor, anger). E5. The low-back group contains as its only ModE member the open vowel [a]. The open char- that Zei 18 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE 9 X acter of [a] has long been exploited by physicians, singers, and others. It is not a sound of particularly frequent occurrence in ModE, unless we count its presence in the diphthongs [ar] and [au]. Then there is the pair of guttural continuants [x] and [3], which are absent from ModE but present in OE and ModG. These are both definitely ob- structed sounds, localized at the low and rear limits of the English speech-producing area. They may be called fringe sounds, falling as they do at the extreme limits of this area. Finally, it is impossible to classify [h], the most unstable sound of English. This sound may be pro- duced with the tongue in any position at all; its very indefinite character makes it practically inaudible except in initial position, although it may occur as a slight aspiration in other positions. General Conclusions Taking the table of sound positions as a whole, it is impossible not to be struck by the resemblance of the plan to a rough diagram of the vocal cavity with the teeth slightly apart and the tongue at rest. This fact increases the conviction of its general cor- rectness, since it would seem reasonable that sounds should distribute themselves according to the com- plete area of their modification. It may likewise be noted that the vowels form, as it were, a layer beneath an outer or upper layer of consonants. This is equivalent to saying that the CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 19 vowels are of a more open nature than the consonants, and is again what we should expect to find in an accurate representation. In describing the sounds of this table, it has been mentioned frequently that a given sound is unstable, i.e., that it tends to accommodate itself to neighbor- ing sounds. It is this phenomenon which forms the subject of the present discussion, and to it I have ventured to give the name accord. Accord in speech sounds has long been given par- tial recognition by linguists, under the various terms combinative change, analogy, and assimilation. That its full significance has not been perceived and set forth is due in large measure to this confusion of cumbrous or misleading terms, a confusion which has effectually prevented clear thinking on the subject. The term combinative change is too vague and un- wieldy for scientific use. The term analogy is bad on many counts (see page 130) but chiefly in its implication of a conscious mental process favoring change. And as for the term assimilation, perhaps the most nearly exact of the three, let us examine in more detail its qualifications for illuminating the facts of sound accord. Assimilation may mean making like or similar; but is this what sound accommodations involve? We have mentioned the word keen as employing [k 2] because of the following high vowel [i]. But [k2] and [i] are no more like or similar than [k,] and [i]; they merely accord much more nearly, as Table I 20 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE graphically indicates. Evidently, assimilation should not be used in this sense to describe this phenomenon. Again, assimilation might conceivably refer to the swallowing up of one sound by another. In ME or early ModE night [nit], OE [x] has disappeared, and this disappearance might be termed assimilation. But the sound [x] has not been assimilated but aban- doned, as when a river makes a cut-off to straighten its course. The sound [x], between the high vowel [1] and the high consonant [t], was in an awkward posi- tion. It did not fit with either of these sounds. Its range of variation, less than that of [k], was not sufficient to enable it to harmonize with its sur- roundings. Finally it was simply replaced by [j], which then merged with the vowel to produce [i]. Can this loss of [x] be called an assimilation? G Enough has been said to show the unsatisfactory character of the term assimilation as compared with accord. In addition, the term accord has the advan- tage of being capable of wide application to many linguistic fields. Some of these wider applications will be suggested in Chapters II and VII. In addition to the lack of a suitable term, the significance of sound accordings has been largely hid- den by the lack of any basis for discussing speech sounds as members of a single system. Table I presents such a system graphically. It indicates clearly why the tongue is at three different levels in pronouncing the initial sounds of geese, goes, and gall. It shows how, e.g., gleam [glim], from OE glam CONSONANTS AND VOWELS 21 [glæm], is a change in the direction of sound accord. Indeed, if I am not mistaken, this simple process of correlating vowels and consonants will explain at least a large proportion of the sound changes of English phonological history. The detailed dis- cussion of these will be reserved for later chapters. Y ܬܝܐܦܐ ܝܚ 3 1 CHAPTER II ACCORD Definition of Accord In the foregoing chapter it was stated, what in- deed must be obvious upon consideration to every student of linguistics, that vowels and consonants are not separate and distinct speech groups but are parts of one continuous system. The universal separation or opposition of them, practiced by scholars, may possibly have operated to obscure linguistic problems. Accordingly the writer has attempted to construct a table which would show all English speech sounds as members of one system. It was seen that this classification could not be a hard-and-fast one; that many or most speech sounds vary in their pronuncia- tion, some slightly, others over a wide range. It was found that this variation is determined, in part at least, by the linguistic surroundings of a given sound, that is, by the other sounds preceding and succeed- ing it. To this variation was given the name accord. And it will now be necessary to complete the general definition of this term, and to indicate a few of its manifestations in the field of linguistics. Accord in speech may be defined briefly as that principle whereby a speaker tends to harmonize 22 ACCORD 23 speech sounds. In another aspect, it may be termed the tendency of spoken language to become pro- gressively more simplified, hence more efficient¹ in its sound patterns. More specifically, accord is that principle in language by whose operation sounds tend to repeat or to approach in manner or place of pro- duction, other sounds, whether immediately con- tiguous in utterance or more remote.. It must not be supposed that these vocal accom- modations which we are about to group under the term accord take place quickly. The human tongue is a most agile member, and when habituated to the feat, can accomplish the most barbarous combina- tions of sounds with no appreciable difficulty. Cus- tom, conformity to social standards, the desire to be understood, perpetuate the difficult sound combina- tions, and it is ordinarily only over long periods of time that any alteration is achieved. Linguistic change is not a river rushing onward; rather it is a glacier, whose motion is imperceptible to the casual observer, but which nevertheless follows its own definite laws of motion. Nor must it be supposed that accord works in the domain of conscious manipulation. The child who discards useless motion in learning to walk does not therefore understand and consciously use the principles of physics. The adult who says eight may realize that ModE has simplified OE eahta, but that I Not always more "easy." The relation of accord to an "ease theory" of speech development will be discussed on page 125. 24 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE : 1 ↓ realization is by no means a necessary concomitant of the process of simplification. In one group of in- stances, to be mentioned in this chapter, accord has emerged from the realm of the unconscious into that of conscious manipulation, but it has become thereby a somewhat different thing, and manifests less com- pletely the underlying principle. Accord in its narrower sense might be stated geometrically as a process of shortening the line be- tween two points, or, physiologically, of reducing tongue action. A simple instance may be given. When a modern speaker says sea [si] instead of OE să [sæ], he is pronouncing two sounds in each in- stance, but nevertheless he is shortening the line of sounds considerably. Let us analyze the change in detail, on the basis of Table I. In this table, the range of tongue-heights from high to low has been lettered from A to E, and the lip-back series has been numbered from 1 to 5, to facilitate reference. Thus the consonant s, a high- point sound, has the notation A2 in this system. The low-mid-front sound [æ] is described on the same plan as D4; while [i], a high-mid-blade sound, is represented as B3. The combination [sæ], OE sæ, will then be repre- sented by the sequence A2, D4. The combination sea, [si], will be in this notation A2, B3. Is it not obvious that the sounds in the ModE word have come to accord much more nearly than those of the older word? Here as always with vowels, the process 懼​。 ACCORD 25 of shortening has been a gradual one, [æ] having passed through the stages [e] and [e] before becom- ing [i]. Approximation Accord This change from să to sea is typical of what may be called the first or approximation type of sound accord. In such instances the line of sounds shortens, making a more efficient vocal pattern. This shortening may involve a change in the vowel, the consonant, or both, as will be seen from the further examples about to be given. Not without misgivings, the writer has had re- course to a graphic method to introduce the discus- sion of the workings of accord. By this method, the dots represent the various tongue positions, and a line drawn from one to another shows the sequence of these tongue positions in a given sound pattern. A glance at Table III will show how this works out for să and sea. The picture is merely another way of saying that the sequence in să is A2, D4, and in sea, A2, B3. The defect in this plan of graphic representation is that the line may be mistaken for a picture of an actual motion of the tongue, an inference which should not be drawn. In pronouncing sea, for exam- ple, the point of the tongue does not move from high-point to high-mid-blade. The line means merely that these portions of the tongue are active in sequence. If this fact is thoroughly understood, 26 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE I A B C D E AB C D E A B C D E A B C D w E TABLE III. APPROXIMATION ACCORD 1 2 3 4 5 OE sæ 1 2 3 4 5 OE bāt 1 2 3 4 5 OE top 1 2 3 4 5 OE sceap A B C D Ladud ABC D E A } B C D E A B B C D E 1 2 3 4 5 1 ModE sea 2 3 3 4 5 ModE boat 1 2 3 4 5 Mode tooth 1 2 3 4 5 1 ModE sheep ACCORD 27 the graphic method becomes a simple and effective means of presenting mathematically the facts under consideration. Three further examples of this type of accord are given in Table III. In bat-boat, the vowel has lifted nearly to the level of the lip consonant which pre- cedes it, and has also approached more nearly the level of the succeeding high sound. In top tooth, the same thing has occurred. The two high consonants have in- duced a raising of the vowel to the highest vowel level. At this point may be heard the objection that the words given in Table III are not individual in- stances, but are merely chosen examples of universal sound laws of English; that, e.g., the change from să to sea is only one of a large number of instances where [æ] changed to [i]. This is of course true; but it does not alter the further fact that this particular change is an according of sounds to each other. At present the task is merely to illustrate the various types of accord. Later the large currents of sound change will be considered. In the final instance given in Table III, scēαpt sheep, we have a change of a slightly different order. Here the vowel has undergone its usual development, being raised to a higher level. But in addition the consonant [] has been substituted for the two for- mer sounds [s] and [k]. The new sound is between 2 * It is, of course, possible that the change from [sk] to [S] may have been complete at the beginning of the OE period; but the exact chronology of the change is not essential to the argument here. } 28 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE the two older ones in region of production; both former sounds are continuants; hence it is natural that the one sound should replace the two. This forms an instance of another type of approximation accord, namely, substitution of sounds. • By the very nature of their formation consonants cannot shade into one another as can vowels. A singer may sing "ah - o - u" or "ee - ay - ah" but not "p - t - tk" without fresh breath impulses at each change. Consonants, therefore, experience substi- tutions where vowels approach near-by sounds. merely by gradual approximation. Another instance may be given. ModE yelp • derives from OE gielp. The initial sound of the older word undoubtedly was at one time [g], although by the OE period it may have been [g 2], or even [j]. It is certain that, at some time in the history of this word, [g] ceased to be [g] and became [j]. It was at this point that the substitution took place. Under this approximation type of accord should also be grouped cases where the sound alters, not in region of formation, but in some other respect. For example, it is well known that d in the termination ed is pronounced [d] after voiced sounds (buzzed, begged, sorrowed, drifted,³ handed, hummed) and [t] after breath sounds (dressed, picked, laughed). The d becomes a breath sound where necessary to accord 3 The vowel following t constitutes voice · ACCORD 29 with a preceding breath sound. The same thing occurs in the phrase have to, popularly pronounced [hæftǝ]. The [v] of have has become [f], a breath sound, to accord with the following [t]. Also round- ing may be contagious, as in water [wɔtr], from OE · water, which should normally have given the flat vowel [æ] in ModE, instead of the rounded vowel [ɔ]. All these instances of accord may be regarded as approximation types. Arbican Gernal Ablation Accord Far more numerous than examples of the approxi- mation type of accord are those of the second or ablation type. Under this heading fall all those words where the process of according has involved the loss of one or more sounds included in a former pattern. The enormous majority of word changes between OE and ModE involve such loss. More- over, changes of this type are more spectacular, showing in many instances a simplification quite astonishingly radical. The OE word eahta, already mentioned, is a good example. In OE this word actually contained five elements, ranging from high-point to low-back. Today its elements number but two, neither falling below mid-height.4 OE cneo and folc illustrate this 4 It is true that meticulous analysis of ModE eight would show three elements, [eit], the first two forming a diphthong. The second element of this diphthong, however, is in B3 position, or in the direct } 30 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE с same simplifying tendency, and a final instance is OE feoh, ModE fee, where two sounds now do the work of four previous ones. Each of these examples, shown graphically in Table IV, may profitably be considered in some detail. Let us first examine the instance folc-folk. Here a short-cut, so to speak, is made to avoid following a roundabout path. The l of folc is obviously an unnecessary complication, coming as it does between the two according sounds [o] and [k]. It is the accord between these two, [o] and [k], which operates to eliminate the extraneous [1].5 Note that we have here two non-contiguous sounds illustrating the principle of accord. Words such as hafoc-hawk, hlaford-lord, illustrate the same principle. In both these the lip consonant is at a far remove, mechanically, from the two con- tiguous low vowels, and finally becomes lost. The instance eahta-eight is much the same; the low-back consonant [x] is out of accord with its neighboring line from 4 to A2. Such slight diphthongal elements will frequently be disregarded in this study, which aims to confine itself to the larger essentials of an extremely wide subject. Moreover, the first element in eight is not universally pronounced as a diphthong. For discussion of diphthongization, see Chapter V. 5 Wyld (SHE 159), following Luick, says incorrectly, "lis retained initially and medially before vowels, and before point (and blade-point) consonants; it is lost before back and lip con- sonants." Here an understanding of sound accord would have avoided the error. This sound is lost before back and lip consonants only when the preceding vowel is also low-back or mid-low-back; that is, is lost only when low or low-mid sounds come both before and after it. It is not always lost before lip and back consonants, e.g., elm, elk, ilk, etc. ACCORD 31 A B C D E A B C D E AB B C D E A B C D E TABLE IV. ABLATION ACCORD 1 2 3 4 5 OE eahta 1 2 2 3 3 4 5 W1 OE cneo 1 2 3 4 5 OE folc 1 2 3 4 5 OE feoh A B C D E A B C D Lad E A B C D E A ܐ B с D E 1 2 3 4 5 Mode eight 1 2 3 4 5 ModE knee 1 2 3 4 5 Mode folk 1 2 3 4 5 ModE.fee 32 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE sounds, and is ablated accordingly. The second and third vowel elements are likewise lost, although these changes probably are due not to accord but to an increase of stress on the first element of the word (see page 51 below). The examples cneo-knee and feoh-fee require fur- ther elucidation. In one we have the ablation of an initial sound, in the other of a final sound. It may be asked, how can accord between sounds operate to secure the loss of sounds at the beginning or end of words, on the ablation or "short-cut" principle? The answer to this question becomes simple if we think of words, not as separate entities, but as com- ponent eddies in a "flow of speech." To consider feoh first, it is easy to perceive that only rarely would this word end a sentence. And if medial in a sentence, what kinds of sounds could possibly follow? Only sounds higher and farther front—there are no others in English speech. Thus, whatever the position of feoh in the sentence, an ablating ten- dency would generally be present, a tendency which finally compassed the loss of the [x]." This explanation indicates a solution for the word cneo as well. The initial low consonant was followed, and in practically every instance would be preceded, by a sound definitely higher in region of production. "It should be mentioned that the oblique cases of feoh already in OE had ablated the [x]. In these instances, of course, the guttural was always medial. The vowel loss and change in feoh is to be ex- plained on the basis of stress and approximation, as with eahta above. ACCORD 33 This condition finally brought about the disappear- ance of the [k]." It is interesting to note that as with the examples in Table III, each of the words listed in Table IV illustrates a general type of sound change. Thus, OE h [x] always disappears in ModE, as it does in eahta and feoh; OE cn when initial always becomes simply [n], as in cnēo; and OE / between low-back sounds always disappears, as in folc. Occasionally a word combines the ablation and approximation types of accord in the same change. Take for example OE bohte, ModE bought. Here the [x] has been eliminated as in eahta, but at the same time we may observe a lowering in the vowel [o], which after lengthening should normally give ModE [u]. It can scarcely be doubted that this lowering of [o] is an approximation to the low-back [x], which before its ablation effected this lowering of the pre- ceding vowel. Mutation Accord The third type of accord might well be deemed incredible if brought forward here for the first time. - How a sound in one syllable could be fancied to operate to alter the vowel in another syllable, after- ward itself disappearing entirely from the word, seems on the face of it quite incapable of rational 7 It may be asked why then did cnèo not simplify earlier? Why has it never simplified in German? These questions will be dis- cussed later, in Chapter VII. 34 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE demonstration. Yet this remarkable manifestation of accord, under the name umlaut, is an accepted fact of linguistic history, even though the rational basis for the phenomenon has not been pointed out. This rational basis is clearly found in our principle of accord. For umlaut works throughout in the direction of according sounds. Wright (OEG 30, 32) defines both palatal and guttural umlaut. The former he calls "the modification (palatalization) of an accented vowel through the influence of an i or j which originally stood in the following syllable." Palatali- zation, in the quaint vocabulary of linguistics, means a raising or fronting or both. Wright, therefore, here recognizes the nature of the change performed by umlaut, though he does not relate it to any gen- eral principle of sound change. That this principle is however involved is easily apparent from a few of his illustrations: Goth. andeis, OE ende; Goth. badi, OE bedd; Goth. bugjan, OE bycgan; and so on through a host of examples, all of the same general nature. Wright further defines guttural umlaut as "the modification of an accented vowel (a, e, i) through the influence of a primitive OE guttural vowel (u, o, a) in the next syllable, whereby a guttural glide was developed after the vowels a, e, i, which then combined with them to form the diphthongs * Something less than quaintness characterizes the application of the descriptive term guttural to u, a high-back vowel! ACCORD 35 ་ · ea, eo, io." Here again we may see clearly the oper- ation of accord. Examples are perhaps unnecessary, since guttural umlaut, according to Wright, is only of a partial nature and occurs only in certain dialects of OE. It seems to be a rule of linguistic history that fronting changes among vowels are more fre- quent and more thorough-going than retracting changes; and perhaps the reason for this fact may become apparent as we proceed. Umlaut might reasonably be classed as one form of approximation accord, since in all cases the first vowel tends to approximate the second. But as the sounds influencing each other are non-contiguous throughout, and as umlaut itself is already such a well-defined linguistic phenomenon, it seems better to distinguish it completely from the usual type of approximation accord. Other illustrations of the mutation type, besides umlaut, might possibly be found; but for the present we shall not search for them, but shall pass on to the final division, the recurrence type of linguistic accord. This final type is well known by the terms rhyme, alliteration, and assonance. Recurrence Accord All language appears to have an instinctive feeling for rhyme, alliteration, assonance, even for repetition pure and simple. In Ancient Hebrew, a tongue devoid of these as formal metrical devices, we find such fossilized rhyming phrases as thóhu wavóhu 36 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE duinitive bitta (Gen. 1:2); and alliteration, assonance, and repetition are likewise extremely frequent. The same thing is true of Greek and Latin, among Indo-European tongues; while Finnish and Bantu might be said to be constructed largely on these principles." In English we have pell-mell, helter-skelter, teeny-tiny, dry-as-dust, by-and-by, and a host of others. OE examples can scarcely be given, since this type of unconscious repetition of sound plays small part in highly formal literature such as that of the Anglo- Saxon period. Accord of this recurrence type, including rhyme, assonance, and alliteration, in their spontaneous or unstudied states, requires a more or less formal definition to establish its character as accord. This definition may be given as follows. In all these phenomena, the tongue slips easily back into a cer- tain position or pattern after a brief lapse of time, and this return gives a sense of ease and familiarity which in its turn produces æsthetic pleasure. Sound patterns thus come to accord with one another, not by the alteration or loss of their component parts, but by their repetition. Indeed, repetition pure and simple is the very first stage in this type of accord, and this is shown in dozens of nursery words, papa, mamma, by-by. Slightly more advanced is the stage of rhyme, bow-wow, and of assonance, tick-tock, 9 But see page 73, note. Accord of this type is extremely primi- tive, and is closely involved in the formation of inflectional systems. Or else, having become conscious, it becomes the basis of a sophis- ticated prosody. ACCORD 37 7 fot ধত while alliteration by itself is apt to represent an even more sophisticated stage, Simple Simon, Miss Muffet. In many languages the æsthetic pleasure of this type of accord finally becomes self-conscious, and in one or another of its manifestations is wedded to that other even greater æsthetic pleasure of accents -beats or strokes recurring regularly or irregularly. Then we have poetry of the English, Old French, or Anglo-Saxon types, depending on which aspect― rhyme, assonance, or alliteration-is chosen. Fre- quently, as in Hebrew, Greek, Latin, etc., the con- nection is never made, and then we have a poetry depending on accent or on quantity (another device to the same purpose) with the ornaments of rhyme, alliteration, assonance, and repetition entering only more or less casually. 1° IO Obviously it would be difficult or impossible to treat accord of this recurrence type within the narrow limits of English linguistic history, and the present writer will not attempt to do so, but will merely suggest the fact that the same principle which governs the Cockney when he says "'am and eggs," or, emphatically "ham and h’eggs," and the child saying "I hafta go," governs also the lofty sound patterns of poetry. The "concord of sweet sounds" of the IO 10 Note, however, that in tongues which do not use such “orna- ments" for meter, the inflectional system usually makes up the lack by providing plenty of sound accord. Thus, in such tongues, all these four types of recurrence accord are already embedded deep in the grammar. 38 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE latter is in the former instances merely concord of sounds; but it is concord in both. Accord in Other Fields It might be possible to pursue the search for accord even farther; to pass from the mechanical realm of sounds to the psychological one of ideas, and to show that grammatical systems are based upon definite principles, one of which is this har- monizing tendency, this feeling for synthesis, which we have named accord. Accord in the field of ideas becomes a psycho- logical principle, thus approaching nearer the plane of conscious manipulation. Perhaps for this reason, its manifestations are more familiar to the average student, grouped as they mainly are under the two heads, grammatical agreement and analogy. It is easy to see that the feeling for synthesis of expression lies at the very root of all inflectional systems. Whether or not the inflectional elements themselves were ever independent words, their use and extension as such elements is due to accord of ideas. Ordinarily this accord of ideas is expressed through the use of identical or closely related sounds. In Hebrew, for example, the termination ah regularly or normally expresses the idea of the feminine gender, whether in noun, adjective, or verb. In English the related sounds [s] and [z] ([Iz]) express the idea of plurality in nouns. It is well to distinguish clearly the repetition or ACCORD 39 echoing of sounds which is based on accord of ideas from the repetition or approximation or ablation of sounds which is sound accord. The child who says by-by, ma-ma, is unconsciously utilizing this repeti- tion type of sound accord. The child who says sheeps, on the other hand, or heads, or mammas, is using accord of ideas. The final s in these words has no necessary companion or echo in the sentence. It is not an approximation to any contiguous sound. It is simply an unconscious echo of the general body of s-plurals already established in the child mind. According of ideas is also shown in grammatical solecisms like It is him, and, All but he had fled. II A 37 It is shown in "popular etymologies" such as sparrow- grass for asparagus. It is shown in most grammatical "mistakes," but it must not be concluded that its manifestations are confined to such mistakes, since it is shown in some degree in every "correct" locution as well. 12 The subject of accord of ideas will be sketched in more detail in Chapter VII. Here it is necessary I The double negative, among grammatical solecisms, I believe to be largely a matter of sound accord, or at most a combination of the two types. Jespersen, in his monograph on the subject, hints at the according character of such negatives when he says that they nearly always consist of some easily distinguishable sound element repeated in speech. Thus the phenomenon becomes sound accord of the fourth type, conditioned, however, upon the unconscious desire on the part of the speaker to emphasize the negation. 12 So also with other linguistic forces. Stress is present with . equal universality. As to ablaut, which may be a result of the operation of stress, Wright (OEG 100) says “Every syllable of every word of whatever part of speech contains some form of ablaut." 40 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE only to mention this type of accord as a division of our general subject. Other Principles Operative By this time the reader no doubt concludes that accord is to be the famous lion's den into which all went and from which none returned. To allay this suspicion, as well as to complete our definitions, let it be said that accord, potent and universal as it is, is by no means the only principle involved in sound change. In all, four forces or principles regulating sound change are recognized in this study. The first, accord, may be likened to a centripetal force in language, bringing sounds closer together through ablation, approximation, and so forth. But this principle can scarcely be made fully comprehensible apart from the three others which remain to be de- fined. It is frequently the interplay of these prin- ciples which brings about phonological change. Therefore, before considering in detail the appli- cation of accord as formulated in this chapter, to the large facts of the history of English, it will be neces- sary to describe these other producers or modifiers of sound change; and this description will occupy the next chapter. CHAPTER III OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE The Modulation of Sounds The principle of sound accord, as formulated in the previous chapter, is concerned wholly with the articulation. In other words, in order to explain any given sound change by this principle, it must be shown that articulations have thereby become more completely harmonized in manner or place of pro- duction. Such harmonization may have to do with the character of articulations as high or low in the mouth, as back or front, voiced or breath, rounded or unrounded, tense or slack; in every case it is some aspect of the articulations which becomes harmonized by a given change. But scarcely less important than the articulation itself is its modulation, or modification, by other factors which enter into all speech in a more or less determinative degree. Thus a sound may be high or low, not in region of utterance, but in pitch; and the pitch itself may be rising, falling, or even. A sound may be loud or soft, accented or unaccented, prolonged or brief. Not only does all song or poetic meter depend upon one or more of these types of sound modulation, but in the everyday speech of whatever tongue, each of them will be found to be 4I 42 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE employed, either casually or as an integral factor in the conveying of meaning. Thus in Chinese, pitch is often the only mark to distinguish between two separate meanings, and according to tradition, sound length sometimes played the same rôle in classic Greek. In English all three elements, pitch, accent or stress, and quan- tity, enter, though in widely varying degree, into oral language as determinative of meaning. The "rising inflection" of the question is an in- stance in point; thus yes? is a different word from yes, though the heard distinction is one of pitch only. Accent is not infrequently used to distinguish mean- ings from one another, e.g., project and project, differ and defér, perfúme and pérfume. And occasionally even quantity may be used to determine meaning; compare ye-e-es (I suppose so; perhaps) with yes (agreed). Differences in sound modulation may assist in producing large cumulative effects. The Shylock of Shakespeare's time spoke the same words as the Shylock of today; yet the effect was almost certainly opposite. To give another instance, the writer saw Shaw's "Pygmalion" as interpreted by the company of Mrs. Patrick Campbell and, some years later, by the Theatre Guild. The first production carried an atmosphere of geniality, of spaciousness; the second, of thin ill temper. Perhaps such differences are too pervasive to be analyzed in detail; still they are unmistakable, and are dependent largely upon the OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 43 modulation of sounds, as well as gesture, expression, and stage "business." Sound modulation may be described somewhat empirically as comprising all distinctions, other than articulatory, made between sounds. Its main as- pects are quantity, pitch, stress, and loudness. All these aspects are present in some degree in any spoken language, and one or more of them are likely to be found basic factors in any given speech. The standardized use of a certain form of sound modulation tends to produce sound change, often as radical as anything accord may effect. For example, it is probable that the loss of English inflectional terminations, certainly the most extensive single change in the history of this language, was involved with, if not entirely brought about by, an increased stress falling upon the initial syllable of English words. This change obviously has nothing to do with according articulations of sounds. It is a stress phenomenon, having to do with the modulation or modification of sounds. The Principle of Grading A second principle of phonological change must then be formulated, and from a superficial standpoint this principle might well be termed stress, since stress is its most common manifestation, particularly in English. But this would be as misleading as to call accord articulation. Stress is a cause or basis of the changes we are about to consider, and not their Y 44 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE essence. Moreover, stress is only one aspect of the modification of articulations, even though a thorough study of language in general might well place it as the most important. As the articulation is the basis for the principle of accord, so sound modulation is the basis for the second principle of sound change, a principle for which I venture to suggest the term grading. Grad- ing may be defined as that principle whereby phono- logical change is induced by the strengthening or weakening of sounds in standardized or habitual modulation of a given type.¹ It is a differentiating principle, strengthening and emphasizing on the one hand, suppressing and ablating on the other. A word should be said regarding the choice of the name grading. The term gradation is not new in linguistics, having already been employed as synony- mous with ablaut, to designate vowel permutation in the derivatives of a root, e.g., sing, sang, sung, or ride, rode, ridden. While the origin and cause of ablaut are obscure, it is frequently held to be a manifestation of the workings of stress, and hence a phenomenon of sound modulation, of much the same sort as those we are now discussing. To apply the name gradation to all such phenomena, however, might cause confusion and hence a related word has been chosen, defined with a widened scope to include ¹ It need hardly be said that many sound modulations are individual or eccentric, and it is only as modulation of a given sort becomes generally adopted that it becomes effective as a cause of sound change. OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 45 consonants as well as vowels, and matters of pitch, quantity, etc., as well as those of stress alone. A simple illustration of grading is the formation of doublets, too and to, off and of, Fr. me and moi, etc. Here stress modulation has suppressed a word in one set of surroundings and emphasized it in another, with the result that two separate words have become distinguished from each other. Additional illustra- tions might be multiplied; but since this study is concerned largely with accord in English phonology, and since the general subject of grading may well occupy a separate and extended analysis, we shall not linger over the general principle, but pass on to its specific applications in English. In the previous chapter accord was compared to a centripetal force, bringing sounds into closer har- mony. By the same analogy the principle of grading may stand for a centrifugal force, driving sounds apart from each other. But this should not be interpreted to mean that these principles oppose each other, or work to opposite ends. One of the commonest results of each is the ablation of sounds, though it is usually possible to distinguish between ablations of the two types. In general the principles interplay and balance rather than oppose; and the net result of each is usually a linguistic simplification. Types of Grading in English It will be admitted generally that while pitch is present in English speech, and at times is even 46 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE determinative of meaning, it has not been proved to have any basic importance in producing English linguistic change. We may therefore safely omit pitch grading from further discussion, and loudness, which is almost entirely unstandardized in English and which in any event may be regarded as a factor of pitch and stress, may likewise be disregarded. The situation is somewhat different when we con- sider quantity. It is the writer's belief that quantity plays no more significant part than pitch, in English speech; and yet the very terminology of linguistics appears to oppose this view. We do not ordinarily distinguish, it is true, long and short consonants; but we do have what are termed long and short vowels, and this distinction appears to be drawn as univer- sally as that between consonants and vowels. What justification may we find for seeking to eliminate quantity from English modulational types? Certainly our vowels do vary somewhat in time of utterance. In the two phrases That's THE thing and THAT'S the thing, the vowel of the varies in numerous ways-in length, in stress, in tongue- height, in tongue-tensity, etc. In the first illustration the vowel is [il, a tense high-blade sound; in the second it is [ə], a slack mid-front sound. Here a quantity distinction is only one of a number of differences which are present, and it is perhaps the least important of them. Let us vary the illustration. Take these two phrases: We meet John today and We met John today. OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 47 The vowel of meet is termed long, of met short. But is there any easily apparent (i.e., heard) difference of time length between the [i] of meet and the [ɛ] of met? Whether the verb or the object be stressed in both phrases, the vowels appear to carry much the same time value. Indeed, if John be emphatic in the first sentence and met in the second, it is observable that [e] will be a "longer" vowel than [i]. Linguists have long recognized that the ordinary distinctions of kindergarten "phonics" between long and short in vowels are invalid; that so-called "short e" [e] is not "long e" [i] shortened; that "long i" [ar] is a diphthong and "short i" [1] a high- slack sound. But the temporal terminology still lingers on, and is present, so far as I know, in all phonological studies. Why has it persisted, if the quantity distinction is indeed completely invalid? It appears to be the common view that while length is not a factor in vowel classifications in the former naïve sense, it must nevertheless be present, since there does exist a real distinction of some sort between the two vowel groups called "long" and "short." There is for example the [e:] of there (British pronunciation) and the [e] of met; and these are historically as well as experimentally distin- 2 2 Laboratory analysis has shown some extremely minute differ- ences in number of vocal vibrations, e.g. between [i] and [ɛ] in speech use; yet it is a question how much of the observed difference is really stress, and also how much weight a minute difference of this sort on either side should carry, since other effective modulational distinctions are easily apparent to "the naked ear.” 1 48 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE T guishable. ME mete [mɛ:tǝ] (ModE meat) had a different history from ME mette [metǝ] (ModE met). If this difference is not one of quantity, what is it? Wyld (SHE 45) suggests a correct answer, but it is the merest suggestion, not recalled at any later point in his treatise. In a general discussion of tenseness in vowels he recognizes, though he does not name, “two distinct series of vowels, one made with a tense tongue, the other with a slack, but differing in no other particular." In passing from one tense to another tense, no slack stage is present. "If, for instance, the tongue be slightly lowered from the High Tense, we do not get a Slack vowel, but merely a lowered Tense vowel, unless, of course, the tongue be deliberately slackened, which is not at all necessary.' It is astonishing that Wyld did not take the obvious next step, that of identifying "long" with tense, "short" with slack vowels. As a matter of fact his own tabular representation shows all English "long" vowels (except [a]) tense; all "short" vowels slack; where he includes long-slack or short-tense, the illustrations are taken from other tongues where this quantity distinction is presumably preserved. But nowhere does he appear to recognize the bearing of his own statements.³ This identity of "long" with tense, "short" with 3 The phonological table of the New English Dictionary shows the same reliance upon foreign words generally to furnish illustrations of long-slack and short-tense, its editors seemingly blind to the anomaly of requiring non-English illustrations for English pro- nunciation! OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 49 slack vowels, deserves more extended exemplification and proof than is possible here. Its validity is not necessarily wider than English. That is, many tongues undoubtedly employ real quantitative dis- tinctions between vowels. Classic Greek and Latin are traditionally held to have used such distinctions, and this fact suggests the reason for the importation of this nomenclature into English, so long held in the Procrustean bed of Latin linguistic terminology. Tensity or Stress, not Quantity It would add to the scientific accuracy of English phonology to discard altogether the modulational terms long and short as applied to the two series of vowel sounds, replacing them with some such articulatory terms as tense and slack. In other words, most of this matter of so-called quantitative distinctions must be removed from the field of modulation into the realm of articulation. 4 In the instances where what is regarded as quan- tity in sound is indeed a modulational manifestation, it is a manifestation of stress, not of quantity; or of quantity only as a factor of stress. In the illus- tration cited above, We meet JOHN today, and, We MET John today, the tense vowel [i] of meet is observably shorter than the slack vowel [e] of met. 4 The terms tense and slack are not to be insisted upon. If Russell's terms mellow and metallic carry more significance to any mind, they may be substituted. The important thing is to replace a quantity (modulational) term by an articulatory term. 1 50 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE But this length distinction arises out of the difference in stress and is dependent upon it. In other words, any stressed vowel in English will probably tend to be longer in time of utterance than the same or another vowel when unstressed. If the tense vowels appear to occur in stressed position more frequently than the slack, this also is explicable, as may appear later on (Chapter V).5 We are left then with a tense-slack and a stressed- unstressed distinction, as inheritors of what have been called quantitative distinctions in English." And so we finally arrive at the conclusion that every important or considerable manifestation of the prin- ciple of grading in English falls under the single head of sound change resulting from the operation of stress." Types of Stress Grading Sound gradings arising from the operation of stress may be grouped under three heads: first, 5 Complete analysis of this matter would contemplate also "checked" and "free" vowels, and would formulate the rules whereby tense and slack tend to coincide with stressed and unstressed respectively. A former illustration, that of [e:] and [e], may require additional comment. While for convenience the two sounds (compare [ɔ] slack and tense, page 17) have been given the same symbol, they are really distinct from each other, the difference lying precisely in the matter of tongue-tensity. The same distinction may be drawn between tense and slack [æ]. This tense-slack distinction is heard more easily with high than with low vowels, a fact which may account for the confusion. 7 Indications are not lacking that English is gradually altering in the direction of a more resolved stress, and such indications, with the possible causes for them, might well be made the subject of study. OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 51 ablations due to unstress; second, obscurations or suppressions from the same cause; and third, strengthenings due to stress. While it will not be possible to consider these types in detail here, a few examples of each may be given. 8 Under the first heading will fall what is unques- tionably the most extensive single linguistic change in English, namely, the elimination of inflectional terminations, in so far as this may be proved to be a manifestation of stress. Closely allied with such ablation is the loss of consonant elements from pre- fixes, e.g., OE onweg, onbutan, ModE away, about; or the loss of the prefix ge. It is noticeable that the weak consonant sounds such as [n], [j], etc., seem especially subject to such ablation, whereas the stronger sibilant [s] has more than held its own in suffix or prefix. Vowels, being even weaker sounds than the ablating consonants just mentioned, show an even greater tendency toward disappearance in unstressed position. It is quite possible that the loss of the second element in OE diphthongs is an instance of unstress ablation. Certainly this type of gradation played a part in the simplification of OE hlaford, hafoc, already referred to (page 30). And a host of ModE words like separate [seprit], beloved [blavd] or [blavid], show the same principle in force today. 8 Certainly stress would appear to be the main cause of these ablations; but just as probably the intellectual element (see page 60) was a factor. 52 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE · The second group of gradings based on stress includes instances of the suppression or obscuration of sounds. With regard to vowels, this tendency has long been formulated into a rule, which may be stated thus: In unstressed position, back vowels become levelled as [ǝ]; front vowels as [ǝ] or [1].9 This rule is such a commonplace of linguistics that it seems unnecessary to discuss it, except to point out that in every instance the articulation becomes or remains slack after the change. The rule thus might almost be formulated: Unstress tends to slacken a tense vowel. Likewise with regard to consonants, unstress has a definite effect, which may be summed up in the statement, Unstress in consonants tends to voice, stress to unvoice. The second half of this statement anticipates the next section, dealing with the strengthening of sounds by stress; but it is convenient to consider the rule as a whole. In the example have to [hæftǝ], given on page 29, grading undoubtedly reinforces accord in producing the breath sound [f], since the first syllable carries considerable stress. Just the opposite is don't do it, where the stress falls on the second syllable, with a resulting merging of the [t] of don't with the voiced [d] following. 9 The rule as here given differs slightly from, e.g., Wyld's state- ment (SHE 155) which does not contemplate the reduction of front vowels to anything but [1]. Yet we have statement [stetmənt], eagle [igǝl], travel [trævəl] or [trævl), etc. OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 53 It is somewhat difficult to exemplify this rule in OE and ME, since several letters (ƒ, s, þ, etc.) carried both the breath and voiced values, and it is difficult to prove which were used in any given instance. But in Mode the tendency is most clearly observed in just such instances, because here the spelling has less effect in determining the character of the sound. Perhaps the best instance is ModE x, which is pro- nounced both [ks] and [gz]. In execute, the first syllable is unstressed, and we have the breath value of x, namely, [ks]. In executive, the first syllable is unstressed, and we have the voiced value [gz]. We have exist, exhort, exonerate, with [gz], but exile, extra, axiom, with [ks]. 1° 10 It is probable that this alternation of voiced and breath consonants is far more prevalent than the average individual, whose ideas of pronunciation are involved inextricably with ideas of spelling, would admit. Thus it appears doubtful that there is any great distinction in popular speech between debtor and deader, deafen and Devon. But the distinctions. of spelling and meaning are so marked that this identity would scarcely be admitted without labora- tory proof. 10 Note that in all the above examples, x unstressed is followed by a vowel (voiced sound). When the first syllable is unstressed but a breath consonant follows the x, it is noteworthy that x has its breath value [ks]. This fact is to be explained as an according with the following consonant. Examples are extreme, extend, excess, express, etc. So far as I know there are no instances in English of unstressed x plus voiced consonant, but there is the German- American dialect form exdreme [1gzdrim]. 54 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE This rule, which covers stress as well as unstress, in consonants, takes us into the third division or group of English gradings, a group including those emphasizings or strengthenings caused by stress. And with regard to stress in vowels, also, a rule may be formulated. It may be stated thus: Stress in vowels tends to tense, unstress to slack. The second half of this rule has been exemplified in a previous paragraph. It is exemplified also in all those shortenings (i.e., slackenings) of vowels due to the compoundings of word elements in English, as wisdom, husband, bridal, stirrup, etc. The only difference between these vowels and those of the type of about [ǝbaut], lettuce [letis], is that in the compounds vowels are not reduced invariably to the neutral vowels [ə] and [1], but frequently show a slackening less thorough-going. The first part of the rule stated above, that stress in vowels tends to tense, concerns us here particularly. It has many exemplifications in English, but the outstanding one, in my belief, is the regular length- enings (i.e., tensings) of the vowel which took place in the first (open) syllable of OE dissyllables. If vowel slackening is admitted to be a phenom- enon of stress grading, then this tensing clearly belongs to the same group in general, as a reversed counterpart of the first type. In other words, if unstress be admitted to slacken a tense vowel, it follows reasonably that stress will tense a slack one. When OE mete [mete] (slack vowel) became ME OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 55 mete [metǝ] (tense vowel) and finally ModE meat [mit], the tensing undoubtedly took place because of the accession of stress on the first syllable, the very same factor which through unstress induced or hastened the loss of the final syllable. These tensings and slackenings will be considered again in Chapter V, in their relation to the workings of sound accord. They form the final example of the operation of stress grading which will be given here. The next task is to define the third general force or principle affecting linguistic change, a prin- ciple to which we may give the name conservation. The Principle of Conservation Thus far we have considered only forces making for linguistic change, and the question may properly be asked why, if these forces are so powerful, do they not work more quickly and more completely, to change the character of a language? Is there not a force, corresponding somewhat to inertia in physics, which to a large extent neutralizes and nullifies all the tendencies toward linguistic change? It seems apparent that such a force must exist, since in fact. language changes with extreme deliberation and only over long periods of time. 1 Does not the language itself, oral and written, constitute such a force? It seems apparent that it does; and so we shall recognize a linguistic principle of conservation, defining this as the influence exerted by the language itself, to impede and resist change. 56 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE : This influence is apparent in each of the two great aspects of language, namely, speech and literature. As with accord and grading, the operation of this conservative principle is for the most part uncon- scious or at least unreasoned. Take for example the popular distinction between "easy" and "difficult” in sounds. We call the Russian [sts] difficult or even barbarous; the Russian applies the same term to our [dž] and [b], and even our "easiest" sound [h]. What each means is that the sound does not conform to his particular linguistic habit, i.e., to the body of sounds actually in use in his linguistic group. Actual physical ease or difficulty does not figure in the situation. II Conservation in speech sounds may well be named linguistic habit, defining the term as indicating the body of sounds in use by a group of speakers at a given time. Linguistic habit is exemplified not only by our judgments of easy and difficult in sounds, but most widely in those changes incident to the taking of words into one language from another. Thus the dictionary itself is one mass of evidence for the potency of such habit. Change in sound values will come despite lin- "If one be disposed to argue that English sounds are really easier than, e.g., French, let him consider our word garage, pro- nounced popularly garidge, where an extraneous sound has been inserted into original [garaž], merely because English linguistic habit does not favor [ž] initial or final in a word. In the same way Russian will ordinarily replace [h] by xl, physically a much more complicated or difficult sound. [g] OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 57 guistic habit. English loses its [x] and [3] French its [b], German its [w]. English gains [S], and other tongues likewise acquire new sounds. But such changes take place only gradually and over long periods of time. They occur in spite of linguistic habit, not because of it. In a primitive tongue, or one without a written alphabet, this linguistic habit would constitute the only check on linguistic change. But in most modern languages, linguistic habit in sounds is powerfully reinforced by another influence which in my belief is scarcely less potent. This is the written word. Spelling Influence - A great literature (Greek, Hebrew) is a force toward stabilization of the vocabulary and grammar of a language. It is safe to assert that English today would be a perceptibly different tongue were it not for the plays of Shakespeare and the King James Version of the Bible. How far this stabilization affects the values of sounds, however, it is difficult to say. Probably when such a literature is orally transmitted the influence upon sounds would not be great. But where this literature, or indeed, any message at all, is transmitted through the written word the case is altered. For most important, as regards sound stabiliza- tion, is the influence of the written word, or spelling, an influence operative almost universally in modern languages by reason of the invention of printing and 58 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE } Į the extension of literacy. This influence, as affecting sound change, has been minimized by many linguists who regard tense vowels almost to the exclusion of other sounds. 12 For this reason, without attempting to enter deeply into the subject, I shall state my belief that the influence of spelling on sounds is extremely potent, and give a few illustrations of what I believe to be the fact. For example, it is interesting to note that where a single consonant represents both voiced and breath sounds, these alternate easily. Thus we have north [nɔr]] and northern [nɔrðårn], extra [ɛkstrə] and exist [egzist], caps [kæps] and cabs [kæbz], etc., etc. But where two separate symbols for voice and breath are employed, alternation is consciously resisted. To say debt [det], and debtor [dedor], would be regarded as barbarous by the very people whose pronunciation unconsciously approximates that very thing. Spelling influence may turn back the tide of an according change. How else shall we account for [Indiǝn], [odiǝs], where [Indžiən], [odžiəs] (Wyld SHE 160), were standard, recommended forms at the beginning of the nineteenth century? There is evi- dent a semi-conscious desire to keep as close as pos- sible to the symbol, which desire will prevent sound 12 The alterations in the tense vowels came about so gradually that this spelling influence was not felt. It is apparent in English only when we compare the spelling values of our vowels with those of French, German, etc. Alterations in consonants and slack vowels involve substitution of sounds, a much more radical change, from the spelling aspect. OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 59 accordings or other linguistic change involving a departure from spelling tradition, or even initiate disaccordings to approach such tradition. Thus one may hear religionists saying [kristiǝn] for [krist §än], purists saying [Isju] for [Isu], and the man on the street saying [ɔften] for [ɔfn]. ¹³ 13 A most amusing illustration of the potency of spelling is found in a little book by Robert Bridges. His "Tract on the Present State of English Pronunci- ation" is a product of the real agony this poet experienced on seeing for the first time, set down in uniform symbols, what English pronunciation really looks like. In terror lest this monstrosity supplant the spelling he loves, Mr. Bridges devises a new "phonetic" alphabet, into which one "eye spelling" after another finds its way, until he finishes with some 70 symbols which are to him "readable," that is, traditional. As Mr. Bridges was so rudely forced to perceive, traditional spelling does not express English pro- nunciation. Reluctantly he admits that something must be done, to ward off the approach of a scientific, phonetic alphabet. So he makes a compromise, keeping all the spelling illusions he possibly can, throwing a few to the wolves of science. 14 The book 13 Not all "purisms" are disaccordings. The purist pronunciation of rise (noun) as [rais] accords far better than the ordinary [raïz] in the phrase where it is commonly employed, give rise to. 14 One feature of Mr. Bridge's alphabet deserves unstinted praise. His new symbols are beautiful in themselves. If the phoneticians would abandon their upside-down e's, backward c's, and ugly j's, 60 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE may be recommended to all who would question this matter of spelling influence as a most appealing, even pitiful, human document, by a lover of English spelling. Its flavor is intensified by the fact that we all agree with the writer, at least in theory. We all love the spelling we have acquired at so much cost. Even the phoneticians have bowed to this idol. In constructing their graphic systems, no attempt ordinarily is made at any logical representation. Convenience would dictate, for instance, some uni- form mark to distinguish vowels from consonants, breath sounds from voiced, tense from slack, and so on. Sweet made some approach to a logical system in his "History of English Sounds," but his system, based on Ellis, rapidly became out of date, and happily so. It is probable that the practical advan- tages of a "spelling system" outweigh those of a logical system, so potent is the compulsion of the written symbol. The Intellectual Element Our fourth principle of sound change might be omitted entirely without great damage to the argu- ment. Conscious or directed thought plays so very small a part in linguistic change as to be, broadly speaking, negligible. When it does enter its effect is largely permissive; that is, it may facilitate the operation of one of the other principles, but seldom for symbols which possessed both beauty and practicality, a really phonetic spelling might come nearer adoption. OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 61 $ if ever will it initiate sound change. Two examples, which will now be given, are fairly typical. The loss of inflectional elements (terminations mainly) in English was unquestionably assisted by the fact that these inflections were not necessary in the conveying of meaning. Indeed, Jespersen seems to regard this as the main cause of such loss, associa- ting the simplifications with the growth of a fixed sentence order. But this can scarcely be the case, since a fixed sentence order is by no means peculiar to those tongues which have lost inflectional elements. The intellectual element here is to be regarded as permissive, as facilitating the operation of unstress grading. Secondly, the double negative shows the intel- lectual element operating in conjunction with recur- rence accord. The former tends to emphasize the negative, or at least to recur to it for more effective transference of meaning. Accord reinforces this tendency by adding its own tendency to repeat sounds, and the result, in all the periods of English development, has been the double, often the mul- tiple, negative. - In this instance we see the intellect in a double rôle, for having facilitated the development of the double negative it began to apply the thumb-rule of logic to it, calling I haven't got no cake a logical affirmative, through the cancelling of the negatives against each other. (One wonders, would three negatives be allowable, as adding up to a final 62 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE affirmative?) Again it appears probable that the real argument against the double negative is the unconscious tendency to eschew overemphatic modes of speech (compare the academic dislike of love for like, etc.) rather than any dependence upon abstract reasoning. It is difficult to find a term for this unconscious rationalization which will not seem to lift it into a plane of conscious tinkering with the language, and as we have just seen, such tinkering is frequently conscious so far as the precept is concerned. But seldom if ever are the reasons adduced for the use of certain locutions ("good English," "propriety in speech," etc.), the real reasons. Thus the intellectual element must be considered to lie wholly or at least largely in the realm of unconscious mental processes. Summary We are now ready to summarize the effective factors or principles of sound change, and this is done in Table V. Succeeding chapters will attempt to review the large or outstanding facts of English sound change and to show the application of these principles, particularly of accord, to them. It should be emphasized that each of these prin- ciples is universal in the sense that every radical change probably involves all of them to at least a slight extent. Thus when a change is referred to a single principle, this is not intended to mean that no other principle is involved. It is impossible as yet OTHER FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE 63 TABLE V. FACTORS IN SOUND CHANGE I. Accord (basis, sound articulation) 1. Approximation A. Shading of vowels B. Substitution in consonants 2. Ablation A. Vocalization of consonants B. Loss of consonants 3. Mutation (umlaut) 4. Recurrence A. Repetition B. Rhyme C. Assonance D. Alliteration II. Grading (basis, sound modulation) 1. Pitch (not important in English) 2. Quantity (not important in English) 3. Stress A. Ablation through unstress B. Obscuration through unstress C. Strengthening through stress III. Conservation 1. Linguistic habit (sounds) 2. Spelling influence (orthographic) IV. The Intellectual Element (largely permissive) 64 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE ! to trace accurately all the elements entering into all changes, but it is safe to say that accord, grading, conservation, even the unconscious operation of the intelligence, are universal causes of sound change. It is to be hoped that this analysis of sound change may lead to deeper and more far-reaching analyses of all the linguistic principles, so that phonology will finally abandon its present status as a descriptive science, and become an analytic one. Linguists have too long been at pains to confine themselves to descriptive research. The facts are available. It remains only to correlate them, in order to make linguistics rational instead of heterogeneous. CHAPTER IV ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS Few Instances Analyzed Ideally, a demonstration of the principle of accord, as formulated in Chapter II, should involve the reconsideration of every type of sound change in the history of the English language, to determine the extent to which accord is an efficient factor. Such an analysis is a task far beyond the aims of the present study. Moreover, in a majority of instances the according character of a change is obvious as soon as the prin- ciple is recognized. Take for example the progressive simplifications of OE hn, hl, hr, and hw. Originally, probably in primitive Germanic, these are believed to have been true consonant combinations, [xn], [x1], etc. Later the [x], a low-back consonant accord- ing badly with the sounds following, was weakened to [h], giving [hn], [hl], etc. Next the sounds were simplified or accorded yet further by losing the first element and becoming merely the breath semi-vowels [n], [1], etc. Finally [n], [1], and [R] became voiced, obviously to accord with the voiced character of the vowel which would follow.¹ ¹ An interesting phase of this development is the fact that OE hw, ModE wh, apparently lagged behind the others of this group, 65 66 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE Likewise accord is easily apparent in the progres- sive changes which ushered the new sound [S] into English phonology. In OE there was a consonant combination sc, in which the c [k], originally [k], had probably by now become [k]. This [k] still accorded badly with the preceding [s], and finally became ablated, at the same time causing a slight retraction of the [s] to [S]. The stages were in all probability [sk,], [sk₂], [sj], and [S].² Since most accordings are of so obvious a nature, it will be unnecessary, as well as impractical, to make the present study an exhaustive catalog of English sound changes. Therefore we shall consider in this chapter only especially significant or outstanding examples of consonant accord. Elimination of Gutturals Of these, one of the most interesting is the loss of the OE gutturals h [x] and g [3]. This ablation illustrates a wider aspect of accord than has yet been mentioned, and illumines the development of languages generally. The loss of these two OE so that today we still have the breath sound [w], or, as it is com- monly written, [M], in most speech areas of English. The explana- tion of this lagging would seem to be that w, being a back sound, accorded with [x] more nearly than did n, l, or r. Hence its simplifi- cation was less early and less thorough-going. That all these stages, or at any rate the first three, occurred at some time before the ME period is evidenced by the fact that words borrowed from ON (skip, skirt, sky, skull, etc.) do not exhibit this according, although these were early importations into English. Just why accordings should take place at one time rather than another is too broad a question to be considered here. It will be discussed in Chapter VI. ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 67 sounds bears upon such apparently unrelated matters as the loss of [w] in German, the elimination of gut- turals in Hebrew, and the typical "neutral" vowels in various tongues. It establishes accord as exem- plifying what may be termed the centripetal ten- dency in sound development, and on all these counts deserves to be considered in some detail. The fact of guttural elimination in English is familiar to every student of historical linguistics. This elimination must have been well under way by the OE period, since OE initial h is already [h], not [x] (OE hund, ModE hound; cf. ModG hund). OE initial g too had begun to change in early Germanic. Before front vowels it had become, at the beginning of the OE period, [g], [gz], or perhaps even [j] (OE geard, ModE yard); before back vowels this sound is thought to have remained [3], but only until the late period of OE, when it became [g] as at present (OE god [30d]; late OE god [g,od]; ModE good [g,ud]). Medially OE h [x] was very frequently found in high-front surroundings. There were many words, for example, in iht (niht, liht, gesiht, etc.). It is evident that [x] in these surroundings would com- pletely fail of accord, and hence it is not surprising that here [x] early fronted to [x2], then to [j], and then disappeared, leaving a lengthened vowel which later diphthongized. OE niht³ [nixt], ME night [nijt], then [nit], ModE night [nart]. 3 Should it be questioned how such an awkward and barbarous combination of sounds as [ixt] could occur, several replies may be 68 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE Words containing [x] after front vowels experi- enced the same ablation whether or not a t followed, as did also [x] when final (OE neah, ModE nigh; OE heah, ModE high). In the oblique cases of words containing final [x] the [x] had already been ablated, so that this sound in the nominative is itself merely a survival. The ablation of a word's final sound has already been explained, page 32. It would be gener- ally true that a word following final [x] would begin with a sound higher and more front, and such a sound would act to produce an ablation according, just as a t following within the word would do. When [x] was combined with back vowels, its ablation naturally occurred somewhat later, since in this event there was less pressure toward an accord- ing. When the ablation took place, it occasionally involved a slight lowering of the vowel, in addition to its tensing. And now and again the lip-rounding present in most of the back vowels induced a sub- stitution, giving the lip consonant [f]4 in place of the former guttural [x]. made. First, [x] may have been raised to [x2] or even to [j] before the vowel was raised to [i]. Second, as stated on page 23, the human tongue is an agile member, capable of almost any distortion, and with practice can bend itself into well-nigh impossible patterns and even call them easy. For a general discussion of such "disaccord," see page 122. 4 This replacement of lip-rounding by [f] illustrates the affinity between the lip and back sounds. It is not properly an according but rather what may be termed an "infantile substitution." The influence of accord is seen, however, in the fact that a high-front consonant, nearer the consonant norm, replaces a former low-back sound. ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 69 Mode shoe [Su] Mode bought [bɔt] ModE rough [raf] OE scoh (skox] ME shoo [So] OE bohte [boxte], [boux.tɛ] ME boughte [bɔtə] OE ruh [Rux] ME rough [ruj], [ruf] The OE sound g [3], the voiced equivalent of [x], disappeared under much the same conditions, except that [3] occurs less frequently in high-front sur- roundings. OE has few or no words in, e.g., igt; the termination ig had probably already [32] or [j]. The sound [3] occurred most often with back vowels or medially between vowels, but in whatever surround- ings, like [x], it underwent ablation. ME owen [ɔwǝn] OE agan [azan] Mode own [on] OE nægl [næ31] ME naile [næjlə] or [nælə] ModE nail [nel] Finally, then, we have a language from which the two sounds [x] and [3] have been eliminated, so com- pletely that it requires some practice for an English- speaking adult to pronounce properly German ich or tage, Scottish loch or nicht. It is easy to see why these two sounds should disappear. In the first place, they accord more or less badly in any sound pattern. Secondly, the motion of the tongue in producing them is scarcely an easy motion compared with that of the flexible tip or point in pronouncing [d] or [s]. Unlike the low-back vowel [a], which involves merely an opening of the throat, they require an appreciable muscular effort. And finally, they are on the extreme fringe of the sound-producing area, and their elimination would therefore tend to make the English speech 70 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE 1 mechanism more compact and hence more economical and efficient. Other Languages Compared If accord is a universal tendency in language, we should expect to find this phenomenon of guttural ablation in other dialects as well as in English; and in this we shall not be disappointed. It is illustrated to a greater or less extent in the history of all the Indo-European language families, as well as the Semitic and many other tongues. It is in all respects an according change, both in its narrower aspect as accord within the individual word or sentence, and in its wider application as a simplification of the entire system of speech production. The process might well be compared to centripetal action, in that it narrows the area of sound production. It might also be compared to the sloughing off of unnecessary muscular actions in the perfecting of such activities as learning to walk or to play the piano. Ablations such as the one just analyzed, whereby English has lost its gutturals, would be expected to take place normally at the rear of the speech area because, as has been pointed out, tongue action here is more difficult. The rear mouth area is not so well adapted to easy utterance as the tongue-tip region. But neither are the lips so facile in action as the tongue tip; [m], [b], [p], require appreciably more muscular effort than [t], [d], [s]. If the experiment be tried of reading rapidly with the mouth becoming ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 71. progressively more relaxed, it will be found that the lip sounds will be entirely lost even before such back sounds as [k] and [g] disappear. It must not be supposed, therefore, that in every instance the "fringe sounds" (see page 18) which are eliminated will be gutturals. German, which in several dialects has kept guttural sounds, has lost the consonants [w] and [m], with their strong lip action. Thus it has acquired a "center of gravity" slightly farther back in the mouth than English, which has lost its gutturals and retained its lip action. French, on the other hand, has no gutturals, but includes even more lip action (huile, oiseau) than English, thus exhibiting a "center of gravity" yet farther front. These observations concerning modern French, German, and English offer a fertile field for more detailed study. It would, I believe, appear from such analysis that German vowels, as well as con- sonants, are characteristically or preponderantly farther back than English vowels, and these latter than French. For example, where [ə] is our "neutral" vowel, in German the neutral vowel would be found to be slightly farther back, and nearer to [o] or [ɔ], and in French slightly farther front, and nearer to [e]. Where fronted [k] becomes [s] or [S] in French, and in English [t s], in German this sound does not front, but remains [k].5 5 It may be asked, why do not all mouths have a normal, standard center of gravity, if accord is a universal principle? Why, for 72: WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE A Theory of Linguistic Development In the widest aspect of all, these observations point to a whole theory of linguistic development. A primitive speech may be assumed to contain sounds drawn from a wider speech area, and more awkwardly put together, than will be the case after speech development has taken place. Such accord as exists. will be found mainly of the recurrence type. The necessity, on the part of an undeveloped intelligence, for being understood by other undeveloped intelli- gences, will lead in general to elaborate speech effort." As the speech area is wider in the primitive than in the mature stages of a given language, so also the sound combinations will be more awkward and cum- brous. The vocal aspect of language will exemplify inefficiencies analogous to those of which Jespersen wrote in his "Progress in Language." In addition to the verbal circumlocutions and repetitions there will be also maladjustments of sounds, sound patterns which completely fail of accord. But gradually the example, should German. "cohere" about a point farther back than English? If accord were postulated as the only principle of linguistic change, this question might constitute a real problem. But every language is the resultant of many forces or principles, which must of necessity lead to systems varying from one another. • This does not necessarily mean that sounds are more in number in a primitive speech, although this also may be the case; it means that their range, i.e., the speech area, is apt to be wider. If a child is asked to talk in a "new language" of his own, he will often "mouth" his sounds, making various facial contortions and in other ways widening the normal speech area, possibly even introducing inspir- ational sounds as well as the normal expirational ones. ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 73 influence of accord will be felt, against the weight of linguistic habit, and finally the language will become more compact and at least comparatively efficient. Primitivity in speech, Professor Krapp has said, is not a matter of chronology but of social experience. Thus progress in speech adjustments depends upon increased culture, wider national relationships, the movements of trade, science, invention, and so forth. As these develop and increase the intelligence of a people, several results accrue. The mechanism of the native speech becomes simplified and hence vastly more effective, but this advance is partially neutral- ized by the fact that the vocabulary increases enor- mously, introducing innumerable new elements to be assimilated into the existing body of speech. The system of grammatical devices, only slightly affected by vocabulary increase, becomes simpler and more unified. A mature language thus presents what may be called an enormous complexity of simplified ele- ments, elements of sound, inflection, syntax, even of word meanings.7 The course of this linguistic development may be seen traced in the history of nearly any language 7 Superficially it might seem that languages such as Finnish and Bantu, whose sounds accord to a large extent, contradict this theory, since the degree of social experience which they reflect could scarcely be called high, compared with the Indo-European tongues. But the seeming accord of these comparatively primitive tongues is not in fact simplification, but the manifestation of an (extremely rigid system of inflection, which itself is symptomatic of an undeveloped language. Recurrence accord (see page 36, note) is differentiated from the other types in that it represents generally an immature period of language structure. San Timo Cp??, note & for San Accordi P #7 14 ! po bester 74 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE • where sufficient records exist to make comparisons possible. In this sense languages might accurately be said all to be approaching the same end—that synthesis which we have named accord. Of course much remains to be done before such a theory as the one just propounded could be called established. Broad comparative studies should be made, embracing languages of different stocks, to study the direction of change. For example, a most significant study could be made of two entirely unrelated tongues such as English and Biblical Hebrew, each the fruit of long centuries of social development. It could be shown in detail how the linguistic expedients adopted by each tended to develop similarly, how the sound systems converged into the same path, how the inflectional devices, the syntax, even the semantics, have tended toward syntheses of remarkably similar types. All such syntheses are epitomized in the single most significant consonant change of English, name- ly, the ablation of the gutturals. We can allow our- selves here only a hint of the wider ramifications of this change, if we would avoid being led far beyond the limits of this study. Palatalization of [k] The next consonant change to be considered, only less important than guttural ablation, also has ramifications far beyond English, connecting as it does our tongue with all the so-called "satem" ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 75 group of Indo-European dialects. This change is the fronting of OE c. Superficially it might seem possible to refer this change to the same analysis as the one previously described. It might seem reasonable that when the gutturals [x] and [3] were lost, the next step would be the elimination of [k] and [g], the pair of consonants next most remote from the consonant norm. But no such orderly process can have taken place. These consonants were not eliminated save in front-vocal surroundings, and even this partial loss did not fol- low, but preceded, the loss of the gutturals. Hence the fronting of OE [k] and [g] is to be regarded as an instance of internal speech accord, rather than a narrowing of the speech area as a whole. The change from OE c to ModE ch is one of the trite, almost threadbare, examples of "assimilative" sound change. It is cited in every discussion of the subject. Ordinarily it is cited uncritically and with no detailed analysis. And yet this one change, with its analogs in other languages, presents a mass of problems, and corresponding opportunities for orig- inal investigation. Why should OE [k] have become [ts], whereas Sanskrit [k] became [s], and Latin [k], French [s] or [s]? Why did OE [k] change, but not OHG [k]? (At least a partial answer to this question has been suggested in the previous discussion of Ger- man as a "back" language.) Why has not [k] changed in ModE keen, key, ken, etc.? It were presumptuous to promise that all these questions 76 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE may be answered offhand by reference to the prin- ciple of sound accord; and yet perhaps some addi- tional light may be thrown upon them. It seems scarcely necessary to review the course of the c-ch development. According to the current statement, OE c [k], whenever it was followed by an original (i.e., un-umlauted) front vowel, was [k₂], and fronted to [ts]. If the vowel following the c had been fronted by undergoing umlaut or other such change, no fronting took place. OE cinn [k,in] OE ceap [k,eap] OE cene [k,enɛ] ME chin [tSin] ME cheap [tsepl ME kene [k,enə] Mode chin [tSin] ModE cheap [tſip] ModE keen [k,in] Ma Satem and Centum Changes regarded as similar to the c-ch develop- ment are found to be widespread in language gener- ally. All the branches of Indo-European are classified as “satem” or “centum" tongues, according as IndE [k] in words such as Sanskrit satem, hundred, has or has not been replaced by [s], [ts], [s] or [ts]. In the centum tongues the [k] has survived in all sur- roundings; in the satem tongues it has been replaced at least partially by point or blade consonants. Of course not all the centum dialects have remained centum. Thus French, from centum Latin, has had three varieties of k-development, exemplified by the three words corps [k], chanter [$], and civile [s]. Germanic is regarded as a centum family; yet in T ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 77 English [k] has been lost in instances such as those just noted. In Modern German, on the other hand, we have the curious spectacle of a language which has experienced probably more consonant shifts than any other IndE dialect, still clinging to the primitive Germanic k (probably [k]) before front vowels (kirche, church; kinn, chin). And this fact inten- sifies our impression of German as a "back" language. 8 But to return to the English c-ch sequence. Is this change a simple matter of substitution accord, analogous to the civilis-civile sequence? And if so, why do we have [ts] and not [s], a simple sound? The writer wishes to suggest that the determining factor in changing OE c to ModE ch [ts] rather than to [s] or [S] may reasonably have been a slight i-glide (what the French call a sound mouillé) after the [k]. The i-glide was present wherever OE c was followed by tense or slack i or by diphthongs. It was not present where c was followed by a single vowel sound other than i, but appears frequently to have been present with final c.9 8 Here is to be found a useful corrective to the attempts of some linguists to determine the "primitive" or "mature" character of a given language by some one or some few phonetic characteristics. If the gutturals only be considered, German is far more primitive than English; if the other consonants, it is far less so. As before stated, primitivity must be determined by criteria other than these. A given language shows its maturity by its general compactness and simplification, not in any one sound loss or survival. • In all discussions of final consonants it should be remembered that in most or in many uses the "final" consonant was followed by a vowel, the vowel of the inflectional termination. #.. ! 61 .7 78 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE An i-glide in combination with any variety of [k] will tend to change into [ts]. Take for example our modern word cart. It could not by any possi- bility pass directly into chart. But consider the word in its Southern dialect form cyart [k,jat]. It will at once be seen that this form could easily pass into chart, and might indeed have done so except for the restraining effect of the standard form cart. A little experimentation will demonstrate that the same thing holds for [k] plus front vowels. [kæt] could not become [tsæt], nor [ket] [tset]; but [kjæt] and [kjet] might conceivably be changed thus." Let us consider very briefly a few exceptions to the fronting and loss of OE c. First there are words like OE cynn, ModE kin, where the rounded vowel may offer some excuse for the exceptional develop- ment (cf. cinn-chin). Then there is swylc-such, an exceptional word on several counts; but the [ts] in ModE is probably explainable by the fact that a usual OE form was not swylc but swylce. But the mass of exceptions are words like cæg-key, cene-keen, centisc-Kentish. In all these the c is followed by a single vowel, not a diphthong nor i. A cursory examination seems to indicate that OE c combined with a single front vowel (except i) regularly does not front. This would seem evidence that the 10 Much the same reasoning should apply to languages where [k] becomes [ts]; but in French the change seems simply a matter of approximation accord (substitution). Note, however, the according shown in the fact that French [s] is used with the highest, farthest front vowels, while [§] is used with lower and less front sounds. ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 79 fronting of OE c is indeed dependent on some factor other than the mere frontness of the vowel. I The great mass of regular developments from OE c to ModE ch is exemplified by words like ceap-cheap, ceorl-churl, cinn-chin, birce-birch, stenc(e)-stench. In all of them there may have been present a slight vowel glide after the [k], a sound which we may represent by [j]. The change then becomes [ts] for former [kj], and we have two substitutions. There is first the substitution of [t] for [k], which latter had already fronted to [k] to accord partially with its neighbor sound. And next there was the substitution of [S] for the i-glide, an alteration precisely analogous to that involved in [tsun] for [tjun]. The popular pronunciation [tun] could not become [tsun], any more than [k,en] could become [tsin]. 12 To summarize, the presence of [ts] in these words seems to be conditioned upon the former presence of a slight vowel glide after the original c. The [S] fol- lowing [t] is not a direct substitution, but appears I I Wright (OEG 153-155) enters into what seem quite unneces- sary refinements of derivations, to separate "guttural" from "palatal" [k]. The simple fact is that it is easiest to enunciate a "palatal" [k] with a "palatal" vowel now, and that this condition probably obtained in OE also. 12 This analysis, if correct, separates English from, e.g., French, where direct substitution of [s] and [S] for former [k] has taken place. It is regrettable that the breadth of our subject prevents the elaboration or complete proof of many suggestions like this one, but it seems a better alternative to give an unelaborated outline of accord than to present a partial statement of the principle. Further investigation must establish or discredit many of the detailed suggestions. 80 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE after [k] has been replaced by [t]. The entire process, however, is an example of the operation of approxima- tion accord as manifested in substitutions of sounds. OE [g] Before Front Vowels Since [g] is the voiced equivalent of [k], it might be supposed that its history would be parallel with that of [k]; but this was not the case; OE [g] failed to become [dž], the voiced equivalent of [ts]. Late OE [g], as has been mentioned, had two values, [g], before back vowels, and [g] before [i], [I], [j], etc. This [g] became [j]. OE gear gives ModE year, OE giest, ModE yeast, etc. This develop- ment indicates that the i-glide, operative in words with [k], was so strong in the g-words as to swallow up the [g] sound entirely. This may be because [g], a voiced sound, is closer in character to [j] than is [k]. In French also, Latin g developed differently from Latin c. Where c had three resultants, g had but two, exemplified in the French words garrulité (L. garrulitas) and genou (L. genu). In other words, Latin g before back vowels remained; before front vowels it became [z]. This fact will be discussed further in the next section, which deals with [s] and [ž] in English speech. Medial [S] and [] The c-ch development helps to make easily understandable the very large group of ModE words where a former t, s, k, etc., has become [S] when ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 81 · followed by the i-glide already mentioned. A vol- ume could profitably be written on the accordings involved in the extension of the sound [S] in ModE, its various sources and orthographic representations. The "regular" development of this sound from OE sc has been sketched at the beginning of this chapter. But [S] in ModE is of more frequent occurrence in numerous groups of words which have entered the language since OE. In all these words, [S], a blade sound just behind and below the breath point con- sonants, is substituted for one of them (or for [k]) to accord more nearly with a following high vowel, [u], [i], [1], or [j]. To use the termimology of Chapter II, the first element in the sound sequences A2, B3, or A2, A4, or A2, B5, has been altered to A3, a posi- tion which makes for greater sound accord in every instance. 13 The words sure, ocean, sensual, social, mansion, anxious, mission, issue, and nation, (Krapp, PSEA 124 ff.) indicate the multiplicity of types under this heading. It will not be necessary here to analyze closely the accordings involved in each, as these are easily apparent upon a little consideration. While [ž] is the voiced equivalent of [S], it is a much rarer sound in English speech, occurring almost invariably in the combination [dž]. This sound [ž] has not arisen from any OE source as has [S], and 13 It is not always only the consonant which is lost; sometimes the vowel is lost (e.g., Christian) and sometimes both vowel and consonant are replaced by [S] (ocean, cf. oceanic). 82 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE its comparative rarity is therefore not a matter of accord but of linguistic habit (page 56). The English-speaking peoples are not unaccustomed to the sound [ž] medially (treasure, usual, vision) but its use is avoided in other positions, [dž] being sub- stituted for it. Thus the recent borrowing garage [garaž] becomes in popular speech [gæridž] or [gǝrádž]. This substitution might properly be called a disaccording due to linguistic habit; there is of course nothing inherently more difficult or less efficient about [garaž] than about [gæridž]. According is seen, however, in those words which do contain [ž], in that the originals from which such words (treasure, vision, gymnasium, etc.) were bor- rowed had [z], not [ž]. The high-point consonant has retracted, just as [s] and [t] were shown to have done, to accord with a following high vowel. The difference is that here, voice is present in both original and derivative sounds. General Conclusions One does not finish discussing the operation of accord in English consonantal changes; one merely stops doing so. There is no end to the accordings, individual and general, expressed in such instances as moths [mɔöz] beside deaths [debs]; as anchor [æŋkr] and anger [ængǝr], where the [n] becomes [n] to accord with the back sounds [k] and [g]; as the two popular pronunciations of fifth [fiþ] and [fift]; and there are innumerable other such examples. ACCORD IN ENGLISH CONSONANTS 83 This chapter has reviewed briefly merely a few of the consonant accordings of English phonological history. We have seen the general tendency of language to restrict its speech area, as exemplified in guttural ablation; the tendency of [k] and [g], both excessively unstable sounds, to pass their limits of variation and become different sounds, when im- pelled by the according tendency; the falling to- gether of such near-by pairs as [s] and [8], [t] and [j], and the consequent extension of the sound [S] in ModE. Linguistic habit has been shown to play a part in some consonant changes. Stress has not been found important here, or at least, it has not been found necessary to utilize the principle of stress grading in making explanations. The nearly uniform first-syllable stress from late OE well into the ModE period, has made stress a constant factor during these periods. In conclusion, let us return to the glacier simile employed in a previous chapter. The motion of a glacier, according to recent research, is not smooth- flowing like a river. Rather it is a somewhat spas- modic alternation of pressure and release; that is, pressure accumulates at a given point until it is sufficient to overcome a specific obstacle, or the in- ertia of the mass, or both; and then there is a burst. of forward motion, if anything so deliberate may be called a burst. Is there not here an analogy to the process of sound change? Deliberate in the extreme, nevertheless language does move toward greater } 84 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE accord, as the pressure of the unconscious urge toward ease in speech becomes irresistible. The glacier does not move all its mass equally; motion is slower along the edges. So in language, changes are seldom uniform or complete. Tenden- cies which are strong enough to operate in one in- stance fail to operate in another which seems nearly related to the first. OE c becomes [ts]; g becomes, not [dž], but [j]. ModE [t] plus [1] becomes [S]; ModE [d] plus [1] remains. 14 The scientist cannot yet predict precisely how far a given impulse will carry a glacier; and it is the same in language. No one, at least in the present state of linguistic knowledge, could have predicted beforehand that OE [n] would become ModE [n], while [m] would remain [m]. No one could have predicted that [S] would arise from OE [sk], while [ž] would remain a sound foreign to the genius of English speech. To vary the figure, sound change may be compared in this respect to a rudderless boat. When the engine moves, we know that the boat will alter its place, and we know why this occurs, and the general direction of the motion; but we cannot pre- dict the precise point at which the boat will bring up. That knowledge, with respect to sounds, may be approximated by further investigation. 14 That the according tendency was and is present is shown by the frequent pronunciations [odžas], [hidžas], [Indžan], for [odiǝs], [Indiǝn], etc. Again the presence of voice in the [d] may account for the failure of the tendency. CHAPTER V ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS Theories of Sound Change Attempts to express the general nature of sound change, or to state a reasonable cause or causes for it, have been surprisingly few and feeble. For exam- ple, it is sometimes intimated that sounds tend toward articulations progressively farther front in the mouth (palatalization). The analysis of English consonants made in the previous chapter, an analy- sis based on facts generally known and established, by no means bears out this theory. The consonants of English do exhibit a tendency toward compactness leading to the elimination of certain fringe sounds. Where substitutions take place, moreover, they generally involve a closer approach to the consonant norm, the point of the tongue. But on the other hand, instances have been adduced, such as [S] for [tj] or [sj], where a point consonant has suffered retraction toward the rear of the speech-producing area. And other changes, such as the voicing of hn, hl, and hr, have nothing to do with frontness. It seems apparent, therefore, that sound change is not primarily or consistently in the direction of fronting. Let us then examine another attempted 85 86 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE generalization, namely, that sounds tend toward articulations progressively higher in the mouth (also called palatalization). This generalization has been a common one; in- deed, so common that efforts have been made to explain the alleged rule, or to point out its causes. None of these explanations' has met with any general acceptance, and as I can see no weight of conviction in any of them, I shall notice them no more here than to run over several by name. There is, first, the occupation theory, by which the spread of more sedentary, more indoor, less vigorous occupations is alleged to make for a more "mincing," less vigorous style of speech, in which the mouth is not opened wide for a good volume of sound, but is semi-closed, emitting therefore the higher sounds. Second, there is the civilization theory, whereby the spread of culture and of cultivated, mannered society may be supposed to have discouraged the wide opening of the mouth (not a pretty sight at best) for a "narrower," hence higher, type of speech.² There is also the altitude theory, by which geo- graphical altitude is credited in some more or less occult fashion with a "raising" effect on sounds. 'The "imperfect transmission" theory is omitted here because it is not an attempt at explaining the direction of sound change, but merely at describing its character. The theory is noticed, e.g., on page 118. Perhaps this statement is incompletely fair to this theory, which contemplates also the nervous," "tensing" mental effect of civilization. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 87 And finally, there is the weather theory, whereby the artificial elimination of winter would tend to pro- mote "freer" articulation.3 As just intimated, no one of these theories is satisfactory to any large group of linguists. When one of them is adduced, it is usually as a more or less casual suggestion, rather than a convincing analysis. Their tentative character has been so obvious that linguists have appeared positively relieved to forget them in descriptive research. All these theories are conditioned upon the notion of sound change as a series of sound risings, and if this notion is without foundation in fact, they become irrelevant. Has the "rising" concept of sound change any justification? Certainly this concept is not supported by the facts of English consonant development. No single one of the changes described in the previous chapter is wholly a "rising" change, unless it be the substi- tution of [j] for former [g]. Indeed, if we contemplate merely the consonants, it is difficult to see how such a theory could have arisen, so completely does it fail to express the facts of their development. But when we turn to the vowels, that is, to the tense vowels, we can see at once how the "rising" theory became current. It is quite true that every 3 Note that this and the first two theories tend to neutralize each other. Obviously a man in a warm room can comfortably open his mouth wider than a man in a snowstorm. The "wide" sounds are the low ones. Therefore artificial summer should logi- cally tend to lower, not to raise, sounds. 88 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE regular English sound change where one tense vowel has become another tense vowel, has involved a rise in articulation. OE [a], lãd, which has the notation E5 on our diagram (page 9) became ME [5], lood, D5, and ModE [o], load, C5. OE [æ], D4, became con- secutively [e], C4, [e], C4, and [i], B3. It will be found convenient to summarize such changes as follows: TABLE VI. TENSE VOWEL CHANGES OE sound [a] lād [o] nōn [e] rēc [æ] sæ ME sound Low.... [] lood [o] noon [e] reke, reek [ɛ] sea Blade High..... [i] Mid..... لا [e] Front K [ε] [æ] ModE sound [o] load [u] noon [i] reek [e], [i] sea Back [u] [o] ↑ ↑ 7[0] [a] This system of risings certainly constitutes the most compact and striking body of sound changes in English. This is the central feature of the story of English phonological change, the citadel of its kingdom. All or most else is subordinate in interest to these surprisingly regular changes in the tense vowels. It is these rises in articulation, and prac- ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 89 tically these alone, which the phonologists have contemplated, when attempting to interpret the gen- eral nature of sound change. Considered by them- selves, they present a picture of change as based on rising articulation; and needless to add, it is this partial picture which has occasioned the generaliza- tion we are now considering.4 While the rising of the tense vowels constitutes the most striking example of English sound change, it cannot be regarded as typical of such change. The consonants, as we have just seen, do not exem- plify the same tendency in any regular fashion. Neither do the slack vowels. Where these latter do alter, it is not infrequently a lowering (OE pluma [pluma], ModE plum [plam]). The tense vowels themselves, in their so-called exceptional develop- ments, frequently result in a lowering. All slacken- ings (shortenings), as freond-friend, are of such a nature, also thought from OE [boxte]. And finally, how may we reconcile with this theory the regular diphthongizations (OE is [is], ModE ice [ais]) which have reduced the highest OE vowels to a combina- tion of low and high? The present study aims at the discarding of the "rising" generalization, together with the varied theories built upon it as attempted explanations. It 4 The inference is made even easier by the fact that similar or analogous tense vowel risings are to be found in most or all IndE dialects. The "iotacizing" tendency of Greek is an instance in point, but illustrations are very numerous. 90 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE aims at the establishment of another type of rational- ization, based largely upon the principle of sound accord, but contemplating also the influence of the other linguistic principles which have been set forth. Why Have Tense Vowels Become Raised? Since it is an indubitable fact that English tense vowels have actually become raised with much regularity, any theory of sound change, to be accept- able, must rationally explain this rising, as well as the other changes in consonants and vowels. Hence it is proper to ask at this point: does the principle of accord offer a reasonable explanation of the phenom- enon of vowel rising? If the vowel risings are to be admitted as accord- ings, we should find in sounds contiguous to these vowels (probably following them; see page 141) in- fluences exerted in the direction of higher articula- tion. In other words, if it can be shown that the long vowels were generally followed by higher sounds, then accord might be postulated. But is not precisely this the case? Let us recur for a moment to the analysis of consonant sounds in Chapter I. There, figures were given which show that high-front consonants (lip, point, and blade) comprise over 85 per cent, or 17 out of each 20, of consonant sounds used in ordinary speech. An additional analysis, covering approximately half of the 5,000 consonants included in the first analysis, indicates that an even larger proportion of high con- ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 91 sonants, over 90 per cent, or 9 out of 10, is found in post-vowel position. TABLE VII. FREQUENCY OF CONSONANT OCCURRENCE Point.. Blade.. Front. Back. Aspirate. Total... • Lip consonants 297 910 53 27 191 • Pre-vowel No. Per Cent No. Per Cent No. Per Cent 64 19.3 59.0 3.4 1.7 12.4 4.2 Post-vowel 1542 100.0 128 687 16 73 14.1 76.0 1.8 8.1 Total 904 100.0 425 1597 69 27 264 64 17.4 65.3 2.8. 1.1 10.8 2.6 2446 100.0 What better basis could be procured for referring tense vowel change to accord? The high consonant which follows the vowel in practically every instance, would by our theory induce a progressive rising of the vowel. The tenth instance, such a word as rēc-reek, may be explained as analogical in nature, with the additional consideration that the low-back consonants themselves are extremely unstable, and may have been raised, or "palatalized" already, through the influence of succeeding high sounds in word or sentence.5 5 That [e] and [æ] should both have risen to [i] is perhaps explained by rule 4, page 142. 92 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE If further proof be required, one has only to run over word lists such as those in Sweet's "History of English Sounds," and determine in what propor- tion of instances the rising of the vowel results in a distinctly more efficient vocal pattern. In practically every instance this will be found the case. It is reasonable, then, to refer the phenomenon of tense-vowel rising to the principle of sound accord, operating to produce a closer approximation of the vowel to contiguous sounds. And therefore accord will constitute the present explanation of this body of facts." But immediately other problems present them- selves. For example, the slack vowels were subject to "raising" consonants as were the tense; why then were they not raised? Likewise, not all tense vowels rose regularly, even under apparently favorable con- ditions; how can such exceptions be explained? And finally, how account for the most puzzling change of all, the diphthongization of OE [i] to ModE [ar], of [u] to [av]? These questions, taken in the order • In this matter of tense-vowel rising, we have an interesting illustration of the interaction of accord and grading. As we have seen (Chapter III) grading played a large part in the ablation of inflectional terminations. As the inflectional termination was lost, the final consonant of the stem came into the same syllable with the preceding vowel, and so was freer to act upon it in the direction of accord. This undoubtedly hastened the tense vowels' rising, most of which occurred in the late ME and ModE periods. For example, in words like OE na-ma, the vowel of the open syllable was first tensed, giving ME na-me [na-mǝ]. Then as the vowel termination became completely eliminated, giving [nam], the accord- ing influence of the high consonant [m] was felt, and the vowel gradually rose to [e], ModE name. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 93 just mentioned, will occupy the remainder of this chapter. Slack Vowels Not Raised As with the tense vowels, the facts of the slack vowels' development from OE to ModE are so familiar as hardly to require restating. Broadly speaking, the slack vowels have remained stable during the entire course of their history in English. The high blade vowel y [y], with its tense coun- terpart, became unrounded to the nearest vowel sounds, namely, [1] and [i]. Of the other OE slack front vowels, [1] and [e] have remained unchanged, while the change commonly attributed to OE [æ], that of first retracting to ME [a] and then fronting once more to ModE [æ], (OE at [æt]; ME at [at]; ModE at [æt]), appears dubious on the face of it, depending as it largely does for evidence upon the fact that the symbol æ disappeared from English orthography in the early ME period. It seems a much more probable hypothesis that the symbol altered with no change in the sound, a fairly frequent occurrence in linguistic history, than that the sound itself should undergo such arbitrary and undirected shifting. The slack back vowels may have altered some- what more than the front, from OE to ME. OE slack [a] has practically disappeared from the lan- guage, either tensing in ME, or falling in with the 94 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE slack [æ] group." OE slack [o] (which was spelled o, but may have been pronounced [o], [ɔ], or even [a], in various dialects of OE) has had a dual develop- ment, to ModE [o] or [a] (e.g., the two pronunciations of ModE God, OE god). And finally, OE u [u], if indeed it was so pronounced, frequently slackened yet further to ModE [A] (OE wulf, ModE wolf [wulf]; but OE hnutu, ModE nut [nat]). From this brief summary it will be seen that it is not necessary to admit that there has been any change at all in the English slack vowels, between OE and ModE, except the unrounding of y, and possi- bly the fronting of a, where this has remained a slack sound. All the others may credibly have been and remained just what they now are. If we cling to the former quantitative distinctions of long and short as setting apart the vowel groups from each other, we should be hard put to it to dis- cover why mere vowel length should neutralize the raising influence of the succeeding consonant and leave the "short" vowels unchanged, while "long" ones were raised. But the distinction of slack and tense throws a different light upon the whole prob- lem, and seems to bring it at least nearer to an explanation. First let it be said that the consonants (with the 7 The phrase Alma Mater illustrates the modern pronunciations of the symbol a. As pronounced by different individuals, or at differ- ent times by the same individual, it is either [alma mateı] or [ælmə mete]. Where a is slack, it is either [æ] or [ə]; where tense, it is [a] or [e]. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 95 315 possible exception of h) are all tense, as compared with the vowels. Thus they are much nearer in kind to the tense vowels, and as rule 6 (page 142) states, approximation accord, like physical cohesion, works most effectively when the units are close together in place or manner of production. (Otherwise accord is apt to work by ablation.) The tense vowel, being nearer in kind to the tense consonant, would nor- mally tend toward approximation accord, to a greater degree than the slack vowel. It should also be remembered, in seeking an explanation for the failure of the slack vowels to rise, that the tongue, in pronouncing, e.g., slack [æ] fol- lowed by tense [t], does not take two positions in sequence. The vowel element may be described as a state of the tongue, the consonant as an action. This consonant action can be initiated from a slack tongue state or from a tense one, but when the tongue begins by being slack, the syllable merges into one tongue action to greater degree than when the tongue begins by being tense, as is the case when a tense vowel is followed by a consonant. Thus a syllable containing slack vowel plus consonant is in more stable equilibrium, so to speak, than when the vowel is tense, the difference being at bottom one of muscular variation. We see, then, that the failure of the slack vowels to rise may be not incompatible with our theory of accord. Let us proceed to the second question, that of so-called exceptional or irregular vowel develop- 96 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE ments. If the principle is to be found operative, we should expect that these exceptions will likewise prove the rule; that apparent irregularities are to be explained along combinative lines. Can it be shown that accord is the principle not only of the regular but of the exceptional vowel changes? Exceptions to the regular process of vowel devel- opment are reducible in general to two types, and it is as types rather than individual instances that we shall consider them. One type has already been mentioned, in the previous chapter. This is the very large group where a consonant in vocalizing, or for some other cause, has affected the vowel. The second group comprises the multitudinous instances of lengthening (tensing) and shortening (slackening) of vowels. Let us consider these groups in their order. The Influence of [r] 8 One consonant sound has occasioned so many irregular or exceptional vowel changes that it deserves detailed consideration. This is the consonant r, which in modern speech has largely disappeared in the very positions where formerly it had most effect upon the vowel. 9 The usual description of the pronunciation of 8 Whether one prefers to call this r a vowel or a consonant is of no moment in the present discussion. • This ablation itself is an according like the elimination of l in back surroundings, since it eliminates an "inverted" sound accord- ing badly, not in place but in manner of production, with neigh- boring sounds. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 97 OE r is that it was a sound like Modern Scottish r, that is, a point trill, formed by allowing the point of the tongue to tap or vibrate against the gums. Without going into this question, let us note the description by Krapp (PSEA 23) of another variety of r, heard in the speech of Americans who "sound their r's." This r, says Professor Krapp, is "pro- duced by bending back the point of the tongue so far that if it actually came in contact with the roof of the mouth, it would strike about the middle of the hard palate." From this characteristic bending of the tongue, this r is described as an inverted sound. It is also necessarily a mid or low sound with reference to the main body of the tongue, even though the tip of the tongue may be high. If [a] be pronounced before this variety of r it will be seen that no new position of the tongue need be taken. The two sounds fit together. It is this variety of r which I believe to have been present, not necessarily in the OE period, but at the time when vowels were undergoing the modifications caused by the presence of r. An r of this type alone rationally explains the facts of the combinative changes of vowels plus r. A back sound with much tongue action alone could, in disappearing, have caused the mutations which actually took place. The vowel was unaffected by a final or intervocalic r. Here the force of the consonant was somewhat distributed and not confined entirely to a single 98 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE syllable. So we have OE här, ModE hoar, eare-ear, mōr-moor, ræran-rear, etc. But when was followed by a consonant, crowd- ing within the syllable permitted the maximum of effect upon the vowel, and in this event we have two possible lines of development. If the vowel was low, it became [a] and remained so. OE deorc, ModE dark; eart-art (vb.); earm-arm, deorling-darling, steorra-star.10 That is to say, all low or low-mid vowels tended to become levelled under [a], to accord with a following plus consonant. Where the vowel was high, a second variety of levelling took place. This time the vowel [9] was substituted for the former vowel. This new vowel [9], then, becomes present in words originally of such diverse vocalization as earth, bird, worth, herd, and church. It is worth while pausing to examine [9] a little more closely, and we may profitably quote Professor Krapp's description of it (PSEA 35). "The most characteristic quality of this vowel is due to the fact that the point of the tongue is lifted up and slightly inverted so that it is directed toward the roof of the mouth. In other words, the tongue posi- tion for is practically taken even while the vowel is being pronounced. Could a better vowel be devised, to precede this peculiar inverted consonant? As r was ablated the tongue naturally tended to "" 10 Compare also clerk, Derby, hearth, etc., where even a former [ɛ] has been lowered to accord with r in British English. With clerk compare cleric and clerical, where no such lowering is found, because the subsequent vowel divides the force of the r. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 99 smoothe from its inverted position, and we have the final stage in the r-modified vowel, i.e., tense [^]. This is a mid vowel in which the point of the tongue touches the lower teeth. It is the vowel pronounced in bird, word, etc., in England and in parts of the United States. But whether these words be pro- nounced as in "r-less" British or "r-full" American English, the vowels are conditioned upon the pro- nunciation, lost or present, of an inverted r. Other Consonant Ablations As indicated in Chapter IV, vowels such as that of thought [þɔt], OE pohte [boxtɛ], have actually low- ered from their former values, through an according with the low-back consonant [x]. This development is found in a number of parallel words, sohte-sought, brohte-brought, tæhte-taught, etc. It did not occur in the combination of a high vowel with [x]. Indeed, the vowel so affected seems to have been mainly o, and so it may be questioned whether o in such words may not have been already [ɔ] in OE. The high-mid consonant w affected o in a differ- ent fashion. The vowel of OE rowan, flowan, growan, etc., instead of rising to [u] as is regular with this tense vowel, remained [o]. The [w] disappeared, leaving however a perceptible u-glide ([rou], etc.). ¹¹ In any event the change is a matter of accord, since normally OE o should have risen to [u], and it was undoubtedly the low-back [x] which prevented this development. 100 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE The rounded character of [w] here exerted a stabil- izing influence which prevented [o] from rising. A few other accordings followed by consonant ablation, where exceptional vowel development has resulted, are OE bleow [bleow], ModE blue [blu]; strēaw-straw, græg-gray, etc. The accordings involved are not difficult to trace. In a few instances, how- ever, the vowel developed regularly despite an ablating consonant; slōg-slew, trēo(w)-tree, sāwol-soul. Probably the consonant sound in these words was not marked. Other Exceptions Due to Contiguous Consonant All the foregoing instances involve an alteration in the normal vowel development followed by the disappearance of the consonant sound which caused it. There are also many instances where a consonant has affected vowel quality without itself becoming ablated. These instances chiefly involve two con- sonants, w and 1.12 Where OE w is followed by the unrounded vowels [æ] or [a], these slack vowels, normally giving ModE [æ], change instead to the lowest rounded vowel, [ɔ]; water-water, wascan-wash, etc. 13 Accord is patent both in this "regular" exception and in the further 12 Note that it is the semi-vowels which cause most vowel change, another illustration of rule 6, page 142. 13 A curious instance in warm, OE wearm. Here r and w are mutually operative, and w prevails. Warm is not an exact rhyme for arm or harm. Note also that slack vowels alone are involved here. Tense vowels after initial w appear to develop regularly; wā-woe, wōm-womb; but compare wic-week. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS IOI exception to it, namely, words where the low-back sounds [k] and [g] follow the vowel, and no rounding takes place; wax, wagon. There is also a small group of words (twā-two; whā-who) where [w] appears to have "swallowed up" the original vowel (possibly by stress grading) sub- stituting for it the tense vowel nearest like itself, i.e., [u]. This has not taken place in the compara- tively unstressed word swā-so; here [a] has developed regularly to [o]. In all these and a few other words [sweoster-sister, sweord-sword) pre-vowel [w] has been lost. Probably individual factors enter here; but it is scarcely practicable to analyze closely such iso- lated cases. Somewhat less potent than the rounded consonant [w] has been the lateral consonant [1]. Here again an influence is exerted only when l is doubled or followed by another consonant. Under these circumstances, a low vowel preceding / tends to become the slightly raised and rounded vowel [o]; weall-wall, sealt-salt. Where I ends a syllable the vowel frequently is not affected (Alfred; but cf. altar). Here the influence of l is so distributed as not to be uniformly effective. Ablated I (walk, talk, etc.) appears sometimes to have had the same rounding effect on the vowel as has which has remained in pronunciation. ¹4 So much, then, for the exceptions in vowel devel- 14 It seems almost necessary to apologize for reviewing examples of sound change so well known to linguists. The writer's only excuse for repeating them is to make them understood better as connected phenomena, related to each other through general laws. . 102 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE opment caused by the influence of contiguous con- sonants, whether or not these were later ablated. The exceptional development frequently takes place as a preliminary to such ablation, and this likewise points to accord as a cause, since the very lack of accord which would lead to the final loss of the con- sonant might be expected first to bring about a par- tial accommodation of the vowel. We have seen that the exceptions to the rule of vowel change are not exceptional, if the nature of vowel change is correctly stated as accord rather than merely a raising of the vowel. Each change makes for greater harmony or efficiency of sounds. The other most striking group of "exceptions" to the rule of tense vowel change is to be found in the lengthenings (tensings) and shortenings (slackenings) which are so very frequent in the transitions from OE to ModE. It is the writer's belief that in this department of English phonological history we have phenomena of stress grading, not of accord. Here is to be seen clearly the same cause which may have effected the loss of English inflectional terminations, namely, the rise somewhere during the OE period of a strong first-syllable stress, which by the early ME period had intensified, operating in practically every word. Vowel Tensings Tensings most often occurred in the vowel of an open (initial) syllable of a two-syllable word. Thus OE nama [nama] became ME name [name] (tense ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 103 vowel), ModE name [nem]; OE mete, ME mete, ModE meat. The tensing took place because the progressive accession of stress on the first syllable tended to make tense the stressed vowel, and finally did so. Stress invariably tends toward tenseness, lack of stress toward slackness (page 54). + Accord is seen, however, in the minor fact that the tense vowel substituted for the slack would nor- mally be slightly higher than its predecessor, and in the more important fact that, once tensed, these vowels rose into closer accord with their succeeding consonants, just as vowels originally tense did. There is need for a thorough study of the influence of the consonant in closed syllables, as these became closed in ME through the loss of inflectional termi- nations (page 92, note). For example, we have just seen that the slack vowel in the open syllable tends to become tense. Alterations such as this obviously must occur while the termination was still effectively present, i.e., in late OE and early ME. Then accord, made more potent by the closing of the syllable, operates to raise the vowel, as in the word meat, just instanced. But in closed syllables there is also undoubtedly a slackening influence upon the vowel, since the consonant itself absorbs much of the tongue action of the syllable; and this fact probably led to slackenings such as breath, bread, to be discussed in the next section. 15 15 Such slackenings may be even more prevalent in other lan- guages. Compare French biftek for our beefsteak, etc. 104 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE Most linguists recognize, besides the tensing of the open vowel in dissyllables, certain "lengthening combinations" of consonants, which combinations, they assert, gave rise to the long vowels in words like child, comb, find, etc. Numerous exceptions make this analysis uncertain, and while it is impracticable to enter into the subject here, it should be said that the entire subject of lengthening and shortening com- binations needs a thorough reconsideration. Thus many "shortening combinations" are practically identical with lengthening ones. 16 Slackenings in Vowels We have seen that vowel tensings, or lengthen- ings, to use the ordinary term, fall into two groups: tensings in the open syllable, and so-called tensings by consonant combinations. The first is involved with the accession of first-syllable stress, while the second is of dubious validity. Vowel slackenings may also be treated under two heads. First, there is the stress (or rather, unstress) phenomenon of slackening in the elements of word compounds. As the elements of a compound were welded into one whole, generally stress became fixed upon the first syllable. In this event the vowels of subsequent syllables tended to slacken; if the stress 16 Wyld (SHE 76, 114) gives to OE cild, and its plural, originally a short vowel. He then asserts that this vowel was lengthened in both by the combination ld; but he also makes ld plus consonant a shortening combination and says that the ME plural childre had a short vowel through its influence. This is simply confusion. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 105 remained diffuse, both vowels might slacken. Thus we have wisdom from wis dom, hussy from hūs wif, Kingston from cyninges tūn, etc. The same tendency is well illustrated in double pronunciations, e.g., I [ar] and [ə], the [ði] and [ðə]. Another group of vowel slackenings, fully as im- portant, is exemplified by ModE words such as breath, dead, good, friend, all of which show by their spelling as well as by other linguistic evidence that a tense vowel must have persisted well into the ModE period. Indeed, most such words still retain a tense vowel in one or more regional dialects of English. It is possible that these slackenings may be the result of a tendency of a consonant or consonant combination to divert stress and therefore tensity from a preceding vowel, since in all instances they occur in a closed syllable. In this event they fall into line with what has been said in the previous section about the influence of a consonant in a closed syllable. They may possibly likewise be connected with an ob- servable tendency in ModE to a more diffused stress, since in late OE or early ME, strong initial stress worked in an opposite direction. But much remains to be done for the full elucidation of these slackenings. And now, what do we find? Except for a few individual instances such as briar, shall, merry, the explanation of which will not be attempted here, the discussion so far has taken cognizance of all the important types of vowel change since OE, always excepting the thorniest question of all, the problem 106 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE of diphthongization. Thus far the case for accord seems fairly clear in both regular and exceptional changes. The rising of the tense vowels exemplifies it, and except for so-called "quantitative" change, all the exceptional types fall under the head of con- sonant influence, a matter of accord in itself. Diphthongization The writer will not attempt to describe the sources of the OE diphthongs, to rationalize these, or even to analyze their disappearance. Falling diph- thongs, almost without exception, their history in general is that of their first (or stressed) element, and at that this study is content to leave them. The consideration of diphthongization will contemplate only those diphthongs present in ModE, and of these, will omit the rising diphthong [Iu], which has been mentioned before. It is a curious and significant fact that ModE diphthongs fall naturally into two groups. Where a ME or Mode vowel is diphthongized by the entrance of a sound previous to the main or true vowel, as fine [fain] from [fin], this extraneous or added sound is low and back, usually [a], less frequently [ɔ]. Where diph- thongization, on the other hand, works by the en- trance of a sound subsequent to the true vowel, it is generally a higher vowel sound, as note [nout] from [not], eight [eit] from [et], play [pler] from [ple].17 17 There are similar situations in other IndE dialects. There is classical Greek, where all diphthongs end in iota or upsilon, German ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 107 } The second type of diphthong is very easy to explain by our principle of accord. If the high con- sonants subsequent to the first vowels in such words or phrases as note, eight, play ball, play the game, etc., are operating to raise the vowel, what more likely than that the first indication of a rising should be a vowel glide in the direction of a rise? The sequence in the word note might well become [not], [nóut], [nóút], [noút], [nut]. But the other and very common type where a low-back sound appears preceding the vowel, is another matter. One of the most invariable rules in English phonology is that OE i [i] becomes ModE [a1], and OE u [u], ModE [au] (mīn-mine, hūs-house). How may this apparent anomaly be reconciled with our system of accordings? While it is true that a lesser proportion of pre-vowel than of post-vowel con- sonants are high (about 82 as compared with 92 per cent) still the rules of accord certainly do not explain the introduction of a low-back vowel glide just before the very highest vowel sounds. One thing at least is clear; the explanation must be sought outside the field of accord. Tenseness might conceivably enter as a factor, since undoubt- edly the higher vowels are progressively tenser than the low. The body of the tongue, which has become where ei is a diphthong but ie a single sound, etc. I do not say that these two English types apply to all languages, however. English linguistic habit appears to be conditioned upon an enormous pre- ponderance of point consonants. So far as I know other tongues have not been studied from this point of view. 108 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE E raised, may unconsciously be seeking lower, more normal levels. An explanation which appears more probable, and which yet is advanced only tentatively, is as follows: The sound [a] represents as nearly as any sound, pure voice unmodified and unobstructed. The sounds [i] and [u] represent such voice as little as any vowel sound may, being very close in quality and region to the consonantal [j] and [w]. But consonants alone cannot "carry" a syllable. They may be likened to the bony structure, which to function must be held together by the muscles or tendons of speech, i.e., the vowels. Hence as the high vowels become higher, accord- ing more and more closely with the high consonants, there comes a stage at which their vocal quality becomes attenuated so much as to endanger the carrying or voicing of the word or syllable, and it is at this point that pure voice, in the shape of a lower vowel-glide, enters. 18 This glide may become so strong as to "swallow" the vowel, as in Southern dialect [tam] for standard [taim], or Cockney [ladı] for former Cockney [laıdı]. In this event the whole raising process must begin anew. 18 If this introduction of voice was gradual, as the current state- ment holds, this fact does not affect the argument. All approxi- mation accord is made up of gradual approaches, and the sequence in the instance fine may well have been [fin], [fəin], [fain], with many intermediate steps as well. Of course the present statement requires much elaboration to be complete. ACCORD IN ENGLISH VOWELS 109 The process may be compared to the stretching of a rubber band. The vowels are, must be, based in voice. While there may be a straining away from this base, a long-maintained equilibrium of height, still when a change takes place, it is likely to initiate a return to pure voice. This points to the really basic difference between consonants and vowels, a difference of course well known to phoneticians, but obscured by a mass of non-essential and petty distinctions. The basic dis- tinction must remain that of voice, the essential in the vowel, the non-essential in the consonant. All other distinctions may profitably be swept away, but this one must remain. General Conclusions It is immediately apparent from the foregoing analysis of English vowel change, that principles other than accord operate with far greater frequency than is the case with English consonants. For ex- ample, stress grading enters here to a considerable extent. And yet the working of accord is also patent. Not only the main, regular English vowel changes are explicable in the light of this principle, but the so-called irregular developments reveal themselves for the most part as consistent manifestations of accord. Of course in this brief analysis the surface of the subject has scarcely been scratched. The great 110 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE system of umlaut, itself definitely according in char- acter, has been completely disregarded, as has all consideration of the influence of dialects and dialect variation. Studies along these and other lines are needed, in order to increase order in English phonol- ogy by referring its facts to understood principles. CHAPTER VI GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS Newness of Accord It is perhaps idle to put the question whether the principle set forth in the foregoing chapters is indeed a novel idea in linguistics. For generations linguists have recognized accord in a partial and unorganized fashion, under the term assimilation. But no such recognition, so far as the writer knows, has done more than point to a few vocal accommodations as phenomena more or less isolated in character. For example, Jespersen comes as near accord as any one, when he says (L 264), "In the great majority of linguistic changes we have to consider the ease or difficulty, not of the isolated sound, but of the sound in that particular conjunction with other sounds in which it occurs in words." But Jespersen does not in fact consider this in any large proportion of his instances. He does not even take the necessary first step of constructing a system upon which all speech sounds may profitably be compared. Bloomfield also makes an approach to a statement of accord. He says (SL 218), "We may suppose that every sound change is assimilative in nature, chang- ing some discordant element in the habits of pro- nunciation into an articulation harmonious with the III I12 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE • * total speech activities of the time." At first glance this statement appears to define accord very well. But then one asks "What are 'total speech activi- ties?'" The phrase betrays the essential vagueness of Bloomfield's concept. Moreover, he himself immediately qualifies the entire statement by saying that in so speaking he is "not naming its cause, but merely describing, in part, the general nature of sound change." "" Indeed, Bloomfield specifically denies that the fact of assimilation can possibly throw any light upon the causes of sound change, because, he says (SL 210), "the results do not indicate why the change took place when and where it did." The combination [kn], as in OE cneo, says Bloomfield, had been pronounced so from time immemorial. Why should it have been in England only (cf. ModG knabe, etc.) and during a specific century (the seventeenth, he infers) that this [kn] was simplified to [n]? Even if it were the fact that we cannot know why a given sound should change at one time rather than another, still this is no argument against assuming accord as a final cause, if not an efficient or imme- diate cause, of the phenomenon. The causes of earthquakes are fairly well known; yet it may be impossible to account scientifically for the time and place of any particular shrinkage of the earth's crust. We know why a dam bursts; but who can predict the exact time and place of its bursting? Even so GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 113 in language, accord is no less a cause because it has not yet been determined what is the immediate or moving force behind a specific according. We may say accurately that whenever these moving forces which make in general for change become actively operative, then change will take place, caused ultimately in large measure by the according tendency. What then are these immediate or moving forces? What, in language, corresponds to the rising water which bursts a dam, to the accumulation of pressure which impels a glacier? Immediate Causes of Sound Change Language being fundamentally a social institu- tion, it is reasonable to suppose that the immediate causes facilitating linguistic change will be the same as those favoring social change of other varieties. These causes have been traced in detail by historian and sociologist. Conquest, trade, invention, educa- tion, economic pressure, the mingling of diverse racial stocks, are a few of the forces which are behind the slow movement toward unification of society. If this hypothesis is correct, it is by no means a coincidence that the period of most rapid linguistic flux in England is likewise the period of most violent social upheaval. The eleventh and twelfth centuries were times of conquest and consequent social reorganization such as England has never seen since, and they also witnessed more sound changes than 114 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE any equal period of time since the first coming of the Anglo-Saxon tribes to England.¹ During this transition period Saxon, Norman, and Dane not only fought but lived together, the three cultures mingling on a basis not too unequal to admit of permeation. English had the determining advan- tages of numbers and tradition, French the weight of social and political domination, and Norse the facts of its family likeness to English and its posses- sion of a number of simplified, convenient pronominal and other forms.² One product of the middle period of English literature illustrates so well the results of this battle of tongues, that a detailed study of this aspect of it might well be made. The poem "Sir Gawain and the Green Knight" is believed to have originated in an aristocratic (French) level of society, in the west midland of England, a borderland between Scandi- navian and English. The vocabulary seems almost equally apportioned among the three tongues. The meter derives from French, the word order and syntax generally from English. Norse influence is C ¹ To perceive this fact clearly, one must take a broad view of sound change, not confining it to changes in the tense vowels, as seems the usual procedure, but including also the vocalization of consonants, levelling of inflectional terminations, etc. The tense vowels did not rise to any extent until after the root syllable had become closed by the loss of the termination. 2 Danish was the speech of many closely knit communities in England, and within these communities it has been very persistent. A Danish acquaintance told the writer that before she learned English she could understand much of the speech of the inhabitants of certain English west coast towns. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 115 less strong, but is seen in vocabulary and in some syntactical and inflectional forms. Old English, which at one time seemed almost submerged in this welter of tongues, finally emerged in the ME period a language simplified and har- monized, like a boulder which has been smoothed by wave action. If we compare any late ME poem (the Canterbury Tales, Piers Plowman) with a representative poem in OE, this simplification is most evident. The English of the later date is relatively unencumbered with inflections; it does not proceed by indirection, leaping, halting, veering in its ad- vance, but comparatively at least, each sentence progresses logically and steadily to its goal.³ The simplification of consonants is practically complete, and the rise of the tense vowels is under way. This simplification, it must be remembered, is qual- itative, not quantitative. Quantitatively the English language expanded enormously during this period, in vocabulary and in linguistic processes-idioms, syn- tactical devices, etc. Numerically, speech sounds may have increased also, though the elimination of the gutturals and voiced semi-vowels makes this fact uncertain. Simplification is shown in the more efficient adjustment of these multiplied factors to one another; in a word, by increased linguistic accord. 3 It should not be inferred that simplification originates from any one of the three linguistic ancestors of ME, that is, from OE, French, or Danish. ME has accorded far more than any one of them. The accord results from the mixing, not from any single ingredient. 116 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE The Revival of Learning One other period of English linguistic history is comparable with the period of rapid change just mentioned. This latter embraces the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries, which constitute the transi- tion from Middle to Modern English. Here we find a period of flux second only to that of the eleventh and twelfth centuries. Not only does the vocabulary of English increase enormously, but the processes of according and grading are unusually active. But do these facts bear out the theory that lin- guistic change is conditioned upon social upheaval? This later period presents no such picture of military conquest and mingling of races as does the former. Civil strife existed to a considerable extent, but this was distinctly subordinate to other social processes. The question is answered easily by the reminder that war and military conquest, so long assumed to constitute history, are in reality only a part of the forces making for social reorganization. War of itself will not operate toward linguistic alteration, as wit- ness the Anglo-Saxon conquest of the Britons, the European conquest of the American Indian, etc. It happened that the Norman Conquest, unlike these others, involved a real mingling of cultures, which latter was the important factor in inducing social as well as linguistic change. Conquest by itself would have effected little or nothing in these directions. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 117 The factors which in the Conquest made for lin- guistic accord were present in high degree in the second period, though to be sure with a difference. Where previously cultures had mingled in physical impact and continuous contact, now the mingling was less physical, more intellectual and spiritual. This latter is a period of mental and commercial exploration such as has never been surpassed in the history of the English peoples. We see a more peaceful penetration of outside cultures, together with the development of trade and industry, the reorganization of religion and government, and in the intellectual realm a depth of interest in classic and foreign models combined with an immense surge of creative expression. These were some of the many factors which, I believe, caused the linguistic flux. With the two great periods of linguistic change closely paralleling the great periods of English social reorganization, the hypothesis seems at least reason- able that such upheaval is indeed the genesis of sound accord. If this hypothesis can be established through further study and comparison with other tongues, then we have advanced a step nearer an understanding of the nature and cause of sound change. Change Also Continuous The foregoing analysis must not be interpreted as meaning that linguistic change takes place only in periods of upheaval. To a certain extent, sound 118 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE change is continuous. Take for example Iceland, which for the past six or seven centuries has been a fairly homogeneous, isolated community. The vocabulary, the grammar of Icelandic have altered so little in this period that an English student of Old Icelandic could read a modern Icelandic newspaper with no great difficulty. Yet the sounds have altered so much that this same student could not understand the spoken language without some practice. //// The reason for this undercurrent of continuous change appears to lie in the fact that social change is in some degree continuous, and in the further fact that language, like other social habits, undergoes a continuous process of transmission from one genera- tion to another. Children are apt to learn im- perfectly the most awkward sound combinations, and so, very gradually, the sound pattern itself be- comes simplified. An analysis of the patterns of "baby talk" would show clearly the operation of this simplifying tendency, which finally may affect even adult speech.4 If "baby talk" were merely a fortuitous substi- tution of sounds undirected by the according ten- 4 A somewhat analogous instance is the learning of English by foreigners. The changes of the Period of Conquest were doubtless facilitated by the fact that large groups learned English as adults. But here we must reckon also with the operation of linguistic habit. For example, the modern German's substitution of [v] for [w] is of course due merely to the absence of [w] in his native speech. Accord probably induces the choice of [v] (a lip sound nearer the consonant norm than w) instead of some other sound. And so with the results of other "foreign accents." GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 119 dency, no norm of baby talk could exist, and no effect on adult speech would be possible. Thus the very existence of this linguistic type is a possible proof of our principle. To summarize the discussion so far, we may con- clude that the final cause of sound according is the "unstable equilibrium" of awkward sound com- binations. The immediate causes are: first, the continuous shifting of the personnel of the language- using group; and second, the forces of social progress and reorganization, namely, war, education, inven- tion, trade, race amalgamation, and so forth. These latter may be called blind forces in that they do not determine the direction of change. This direction is determined in English almost wholly by the interplay of the two principles of accord and grading. Time and Place Not Important Is it necessary to a comprehension of linguistic change as conditioned upon these principles, to account for the time and place of each specific mani- festation of accord? Compare accord, if you please, to a most fixed human law, that of gravitation. Com- pare specific changes to the rolling of pebbles down- ward following the tilting of a plate. Let accord stand for the law, a period of social reorganization for the tiltings, and individual accordings for the motions of the pebbles. We may say that the rounder pebbles (i.e., unstable combinations) will move first; and that the more the plate is tilted the more pebbles 120 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE will move. But does it affect our acceptance of gravitation as a final cause, tilting as an immediate cause, if we admit that we do not know which pebble will move first, or the precise instant of its moving? Just so, we do not always know why one according antedates another in English sound development; we could not yet predict the precise time of its occurrence; but none the less we may know definitely its immediate and final causes. Gravitation may be reduced to predictable results under laboratory conditions, it is true; and the devel- opment of language, covering as it does centuries in time and continents in area, may yet be made sub- ject to analysis of this sort. But even should this be accomplished, it will in all probability merely verify our knowledge of the fundamental causes of sound change. It may be well to render more specific the state- ments just made, by analyzing in detail one par- ticular change. Let us select for this purpose the change from OE sc [sk] to ModE sh [S]. Any OE sound combination involving the back consonant [k] would be more or less awkward and hence susceptible to change. It is significant that OE [k] has survived initially only with back or low vowels (coal, calf), with the back consonant [w] (quick, queen), and with the semi-vowels [r] and [1], which as we have already seen (pages 98, 101) have an affinity for low-back vowels (claw, crab). Initial [k] has disappeared before a high vowel or vowel GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 121 glide (chin), before the point consonant [n] (know), and after the point consonant [s] (short). And these instances cover the use of this sound initially in English. Why OE sc should have simplified some centuries earlier than OE cn it is perhaps not possible, certainly not essential, to know. Suffice it to say that by the end of the OE period this [sk] had become [S]. The change marks a considerable advance in vocal efficiency. We now have one sound in place of two, and that one far better accommodated to speech sounds in general. In the early ME period there entered the lan- guage a considerable group of ON words in [sk], and these have remained unchanged in consonant pat- tern (skin, sky, skirt, etc.). The question may now be asked, why did OE sc change, but not ON sc? This question seems capable of a definite answer. It is reasonable to suppose that ON sc was prevented from altering by the very fact that OE sc had changed. It would not have been difficult for any hearer to grasp the fact that the initial sounds of, e.g., short and sky were entirely distinct, and being distinct, they naturally tended to be kept so. The speakers had no consciousness of any traditional con- nection between the [S] of short and the [sk] of sky. On the contrary, they had the very best aural proof of difference.5 5 To be sure, there are instances where sounds originally distinct have fallen together, i.e., where the according tendency has been 122 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE Finally, the question may be asked, is not the net result of these phonological developments a compli- cation rather than a simplification, since English is left with two phonograms for one? Where OE had only [sk], we now have both [sk] from ON and [S] from OE. The answer seems to be that a simplifica- tion has taken place, even though it has been incom- plete. In so far as [sk] has become [S], the language has simplified, even though all [sk]'s have not passed into [S]'s. More Than One Principle of Change Indeed, the aim of this study has not been accom- plished if the reader is left with any idea that all changes must be complete and in a single direction. Were this the case, languages everywhere would by now be mechanisms of super-human efficiency, in- stead of the makeshifts we find them. Sound change is frequently incomplete. More than this, we now and then encounter a change which can only be called disaccord, where the principle we have been describing is positively reversed! An excellent example of such disaccord is the diphthongization of the high vowels (page 108), where the low-back sound [a] enters between two high sounds, and even tends to swallow up the succeeding vowel. On the face of it, nothing could more com- able to overcome this tendency to keep separate sounds distinct from one another. But such instances do not make invalid the present analysis of ME [S] and [sk]. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 123 pletely deny the principle we have tried to prove operative. In like manner the law of gravitation is seem- ingly set aside, by a balloon, a feather, a kite, by muscular action, and by the slower process of physical growth. Every physicist knows that these excep- tions are apparent and not real, and can analyze the opposing factors which temporarily neutralize the law. So in linguistics we may admit the principle of accord in spite of disaccording instances. Such instances may be set down as due to any of a number of linguistic forces. Theory Re-stated To return, then, to a final statement of the gen- eral theory of language first set forth in Chapter IV. It seems a tenable assumption that a language in its earliest stages is "something rich and strange"-rich in inflections, in tongue contortion, in vocal action; and strange in all these. Sounds are apt to be drawn from a wide speech range, and to be most awkwardly put together into syllables. As social progress takes place, the principle of accord, which may be called the unconscious urge toward ease in speech, operates. operates. Continuously through the influence of new generations of speakers, and often radically through social amalgamation and general upheaval, changes occur, changes which are furthered, modified, obstructed, or even reversed by countless influences affecting now a single individual, 124 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE now the entire language group. The changes may conform to any one of a number of linguistic prin- ciples, among which grading and accord are easily pre-eminent in the history of English. Grading tends to separate and distinguish sounds, accord to har- monize and simplify them. In the long run, when we evaluate the alterations, we see that the accord- ings are probably the more numerous and radical. Frequently we can rationalize yet further, and see why a particular situation should arise in a given language. It is easy, for example, to conclude that the English linguistic "center of gravity" (page 71) is between those of French and German because the language itself is between the two, formed in large degree from their union. We cannot trace in detail the processes which made German a "back" language and French a "front" language; but this we can assert: in so far as linguistic pressure (through con- quest, trade, etc.) of a front phonological character is brought to bear upon, e.g., German, that tongue will tend to be modified in a fronting direction. And in any event its modifications, such as they are, will be explainable by our basic principles. This may not be all we could desire in the way of scientific in- sight, but it is something. With all this interplay of forces and principles it would be strange indeed if languages did not con- tinuously vary widely from one another in their ❝ Of course grading may and does also lead to simplifications, by its tendency to obscure and eliminate unstressed elements. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS 125 phonological systems. We need no explanation of such variation; it is natural and even inevitable. Theoretically one may construct an ideal system of according sounds, a harmonious divine whole toward which human sound creations might be said to move. But practically, such a system is far from realization. Comments on General System A few comments on the general theory just set forth may be in place. In the first place, it is observ- able that the total number of speech sounds is not important to our system. This number, indeed, is likely to remain much the same; that is, coincident. with the ablation of disaccording elements may occur the introduction of new sounds nearer the norm. But while this is true, it should be remembered that these new sounds are not necessarily or ordinarily substitutions for the old, and that the number of sounds in itself has no special significance to our theory. Secondly, it should be said that this is not an 'ease theory" of speech development, as the term is commonly used. The proponents of "ease" as an explanation of sound change contemplate at least to some extent the ease or difficulty of certain isolated sounds, a matter quite unessential to the argument here. While it is true that sounds do vary from one another in the amount of what we may call mouth distortion necessary to their production, still this 126 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE ! variation, difficult or impossible to measure, is not a factor in sound change, since habitual use will make a familiar "difficult" sound easier than an unfamiliar "easy" one. It is only with respect to sound combinations that the present analysis could be called an ease theory; it might better be termed an efficiency theory. But the phrase, theory of accord, best expresses the matter of this analysis, since it deals with sounds only in relation to each other, not with a sound, per se. The writer has been at pains to emphasize that we have as yet little more than a theory, an outline which must be filled in by both broader and narrower studies by many investigators before it can be re- garded as established. The fact of sound accord as a main principle of linguistic change I regard as demonstrated; its full statement must involve a great deal of further research. CHAPTER VII ACCORD OF IDEAS Need for a Simplified Terminology It will be observed that here and there through- out this study, statements which may apply to de- partments of linguistics other than phonology have been made. Especially was this true in setting forth a general theory of language development. Will the term accord then be found useful in a wider, general sense, to illumine phenomena of morphology, of syntax, even of semantics? On the surface, there seems grave objection to adding another to the already extended array of lin- guistic terms. Indeed, to the casual observer the terminology of linguistics might well appear in a hopeless state of mist and muddle. On the one hand, we have a single term such as spirant, which in Webster's Dictionary is given no fewer than four incompatible definitions, each backed by authorita- tive usage. On the other hand, we have a single construction such as the good in the good are happy, for which Jespersen (PG 106 note) lists nine terms in common use. In addition there are such words as velar, palatalization, dark and bright vowels, etc., which are either positively misleading, or cursed with a vagueness of intent which cannot but pre- A 127 128 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE clude scientific exactness. A new term, to contribute toward the resolving of this tangle, must have strong qualities of definiteness and simplicity. The great need of linguistics has been for a new and better terminology, and in phonology at least, this need is being met. Easy, definite terms, such as high, slack, back, voiced, etc., are replacing the former romantic or cumbrous ones, and have gained wide currency among those scholars who do not regard clarity as incompatible with erudition. The change is comparable to that from Roman to Arabic notation in mathematics. It becomes suddenly easy to perceive principles and relationships which were hidden under the old system. Throughout the present study I have been en- deavoring to establish accord as such an illuminating term, which by its very use must operate to clarify the phenomena it describes, a thing the older term, assimilation, failed to do. I now wish to suggest as a possibility that this same term, accord, may well be found useful in other departments of linguistics. To prove this and to indicate a few of the resulting clarifications, will be the aim of this chapter. It will be impossible to give more than the barest hints at a synthesis of this vast field, more than an outline to be filled in by further detailed investigation. It was said (page 22) that accord in speech is that principle whereby a speaker unconsciously tends to harmonize speech sounds. With the single verbal substitution of ideas for sounds, this definition will A ACCORD OF IDEAS 129 serve very well here. Accord in "grammar" is that principle whereby a speaker tends to harmonize ideas. It is the tendency of spoken or written lan- guage to become progressively more efficient, more synthesized, in expression. Several examples of this type of accord were given in Chapter II, pages 38 and 39. A simple one is the child's formation of a plural sheeps from sheep. With no conscious mental effort, the plural is con- formed or accorded to the vast body of s-plurals. I Sound accord will accompany accord of ideas. If the word to be pluralized ends with a voiced sound or a sibilant, the child will make the according sound adjustments, saying, e.g., pigs [pigz] or dishes [di siz], without in the least realizing that sound accord, as well as accord of ideas, is present. Of course the two factors are entirely distinct. The adding of a sibilant to pig is idea accord; while the voiced character of that sibilant is sound accord. At this point the comment may be heard: what you have been describing is entirely familiar to linguists under the term analogy. The child adds [s] to sheep by analogy between sheeps and the vast body of s-plurals. There is no reason for calling this process accord of ideas, with another term already established in usage. • The illustration sheeps should not be interpreted to mean that such accord of ideas is apt to produce a result grammatically incor- rect. In the great majority of words, of course, a correct result will be obtained by the according process, e.g., dogs, cats, houses, etc. 130 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE If it is true that the term analogy accurately and completely describes such phenomena, there is indeed no place for another term. But does it? In its general use outside linguistics, analogy con- notes a more or less conscious connection between two things (processes, sequences, etc.) of not too unequal rank or value. The term is largely used with reference to reasoning of a distinctly high order. Thus one would scarcely say "Jean bobbed her hair by analogy between her own and the vast array of bobbed heads in Chicago." The comparison is too unequal. Neither would one say, "A baby cries at birth by analogy between itself and other newborn babies." The process is too unconscious. One might, however, say, "There is a real analogy be- tween the process of invention and that of physical growth," or "No analogy exists between the vague hypotheses of astrology and the facts of astronomy." If this summary is correct, then the current state- ment that the child adds s to sheep by analogy between sheeps and the vast body of s-plurals, is bad English, a definite misuse of words. The process is entirely unconscious, the comparison wholly unequal. The situation is somewhat improved, it may be ad- mitted, if we rephrase the statement thus: sheeps is a formation analogous to that of the normal s-plural. Here the conscious reasoning is not so directly referred to the child who says sheeps, nor the com- parison phrased to appear so unequal. But obviously we cannot get along with the adjective analogous, ACCORD OF IDEAS 131 and so omit the misleading noun analogy from linguistic terminology. There are other reasons for objecting to analogy as a descriptive term for these instances. It is too pompous, too inflated, too vague, for this purpose. Just as front and high have replaced the older palatal, so will scientific exactness be served by substituting an easy, accurate, definite term for the cumbrous analogy. Accord is in itself a desirable term in this con- nection. It has the necessary qualities of ease and definiteness, and in addition it bridges the gap be- tween phonology and grammar, establishing in itself a synthesis between them.2 No further apology, then, will be made for the adoption of the term accord, or accord of ideas, in place of analogy. Let us proceed to a consideration of a few of its workings in the various departments of linguistics. Accord in Morphology This heading might almost be phrased "Mor- phology is Accord," so completely do morphological systems depend upon according of ideas. It seems reasonable to assume, with many linguists, that all or most inflectional elements were at one stage of 2 Were it not for this fact, the word agreement would do as well as accord; but agreement is also used in the specialized sense of formal grammatical agreement, e.g., between a noun and its verb, adjective and noun, etc. 132 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE J the language separate words, even though one may balk at the task of ferreting out just what those words were. In the nature of language, once such an element had merged its identity with the govern- ing words, it would in all probability disappear, leaving no independent survivor in speech. In a few instances, e.g., in the Semitic tongues, we are reason- ably sure of the progenitors of many of the inflectional elements, but even here the attempt to complete such information raises more difficulties than it resolves. Contracted forms such as I've, he'll, shan't, show stages in the paths taken by syntactical units in the process of becoming morphological elements. Such elements-ve for have, ll for will, nt for not—if found regularly in a given relation to other stronger sen- tence words, might easily, especially in a language without spelling, become merged with these words, either as suffix or prefix, depending on the sentence order and sentence stress characteristic of the language. In every instance the merging of linguistic ele- ments represents or reflects a union or according of ideas. This according process may be extremely active. It is probable that in early stages languages use far more and finer morphological groupings, just as some primitive tongues are said to have separate "genders" for long things, round things, hard things, and so forth. Possibly the strong and weak cate- gories in Germanic, the Latin and Greek declensions . ACCORD OF IDEAS 133 and conjugations, are survivals of some such group- ings. At present we have only a few traces of such former categories, but these are still numerous enough to keep most of our systems from complete uniformity. Thus we have the various "irregular” verbs, plurals like oxen, children, etc., and unaccord- ing systems of pronouns, where the recently altered it alone follows the usual (noun) plan of inflection, with objective like the nominative, and possessive formed by the addition of s. Minor according groups may even grow up within the limits of a larger system according generally with- in itself. An example of such development is the English plurals of fish and game words. Nouns indicating varieties of fish or game, quail, deer, salmon, bear, pike, trout, etc., have come to exhibit a plural identical with the singular, especially when used in the dialect of outdoor sport. This is not a traditional condition; e.g., OE fisc formed its plural regularly, fiscas. These words have accorded to a few such words with no separate plural form. In general, however, inflectional systems of accordings are remarkably uniform and universal within a given language, exhibiting a strong ten- dency toward obliterating any exceptions which may happen to exist. Whether these accordings are by terminations incorporated into the individual word, as illustrated by the third person singular of verbs (goes, comes) or in word phrases, as the possessive 134 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE pronoun plus a noun (my book, his table), they are none the less manifestations of the according prin- ciple. Chinese with its separate grammatical ele- ments, Hebrew with its combination of independent and compounded accordings, indeed, any language at all, exhibits similar syntheses. Perhaps it is somewhat easier to perceive accord of ideas in morphological "mistakes" than in "cor- rect" locutions, since in the former instances the according principle works in opposition to tradition rather than in consonance with it, and hence is easier to isolate as a factor. Take for example the words nearer and drownded, both traditionally mis- takes, and both illustrating the same type of error, that of a double according, where a second inflec- tional element has been added through an imperfect realization of the first one as actually added and not a part of the word. This imperfect realization has also tended to establish the form with a single in- flection element, near, drownd, as the primary word. In one of the two instances the tendency has been successful. Accord is here seen to have overridden tradition and to have established a new form as standard. 3 To re-state briefly the function of accord in 3 What part the repetition type of sound accord has played in this development it is impossible to say with any certainty; but I believe it to be very small, else why not higherer, sinneded, etc.? On the other hand, there is sounded, by tradition properly souned. Certainly there is a tendency to add to a final voiced point continu- ant [n], the corresponding voiced stop [d], to round off the close of a word. ACCORD OF IDEAS 135 morphology, we may say that accord of ideas is the principle active in producing ever more completely synthesized inflectional systems. Occasionally minor according groups, separate from the main body, are found; but in the long run these also tend to become absorbed. When a conflict of linguistic ideas occurs, accord may lead to locutions unaccepted by the standard speech; and in course of time many or most of these are received into the language. Worn-down tongues such as English, Danish, Hebrew, Aramaic, etc., best illustrate this morpho- logical according. Declensions and conjugations have merged and so become fewer in number. Former morphological distinctions have been obliterated. In general the language has approached nearer and nearer the Esperanto ideal. 4 Accord in Syntax In syntax, we have a type of accord appreciably more nearly in the realm of conscious mental activity than morphological accord. Grammatical "correct- ness" is not by any means always instinctively attained, even by native speakers of a language. It is one of the favorite pastimes of grammarians to 4 The part played by a fixed word order in facilitating the loss of inflectional distinctions has been mentioned by linguists. Its effect is by no means negligible, but may have been overrated. Highly inflectional languages frequently possess a fairly fixed word order; and on the other hand, the word order of ModE is if anything less rigid than that of German, a much more highly inflectional tongue. A stock example in English is the line "The plowman homeward plods his weary way," which may be transposed in surprising variety. ! 136 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE compile lengthy lists of every sort of grammatical solecism, committed by literary artists of the highest repute. Such lists should be well adapted to comfort and encourage lesser wielders of the pen, by demon- strating the unattainability of syntactical perfection. Where agreement is followed out according to the usual standards of grammatical correctness, the according is simple and obvious. I go, he goes, she herself, need no special analysis. Where a "mistake" has arisen, it will be apparent upon examination that a mental conflict exists, a conflict between an idea of form and an idea of content. Let us analyze a simple instance. In grammatical practice it is ordinarily found that the subjective case precedes, the objective case follows, the verb; e.g., John hit him, we sent to them, etc. And as English word order almost always places subject and verb at the beginning of a sentence, the rule in practice, that is, in the semi-conscious practice of the ordinary speaker, might be stated thus: Before the verb, use the subjective; after the verb, use the objective. But this levelling according tendency does not operate so uniformly as the speaker semi-conscious of his language might desire. For one thing, we are told that the copula is not a true verb but an expres- sion of identity, and hence must not take the ob- jective case. That is, the idea of identity expressed in It is I, should rule out the idea of standard form expressed in It is me. But can it in fact do so? In ACCORD OF IDEAS 137 this as in most cases, accord of form is strong enough to outweigh the idea of identity, and we have it is me triumphantly riding to general acceptance over the dead bodies of innumerable purists. Another less obvious instance of the working of the same rule may be given. The poet wrote: The boy stood on the burning deck Whence all but he had fled. “All but him,” immediately says the grammarian. But here is clearly a preposition, and prepositions govern the objective case. The phrase as written seems preferable, showing as it does a sound regard for the tacitly adopted rule of subjective before verb, objective afterward. If the rhyme had required: The boy stood on the burning deck Whence all had fled but him } no doubt it would naturally have been written that way. "Between you and I," almost invariably a pre- verb phrase, illustrates precisely this same tendency toward larger syntactical accord; and many other examples might be cited. These instances all indicate clearly the leveling nature of syntactical accord, which works toward grammatical unity even beyond that permitted by grammatical rules. One larger aspect of the rule set forth in the foregoing paragraphs is worthy of 138 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE note. The rule is one of sentence order. The older rules call for accord in the smaller grammatical units of prepositional phrase, or copula plus pronoun. The present practice therefore illustrates a tendency toward a larger synthesis which takes in the sentence as a whole, instead of in its minor parts; and this tendency is perhaps characteristic of progress in grammatical accord. Any particular rule of syntactical accord, working toward general acceptance in any particular language, is by no means universally applicable to language as a whole. We might postulate a syntactical norm or "center of gravity" in each language, a norm differ- ing in each just as phonological "centers of gravity" have been found to vary. In other words, the appli- cation of the according principle will in all probability be found peculiar to one language; the principle itself is universal. - Accord in Semantics Fascinating as it would be to delve deeper into the workings of syntactical accord, it would not be profitable in the present study, since most such accordings are either "regular" or of the type just analyzed, that is, proceeding from a conflict of grammatical ideas. But when we turn to the field of semantics, or the meanings of words, we rise at once to the plane of pure ideas relatively unhampered by form or system. Wherein does accord play a part in this ACCORD OF IDEAS 139 immense conglomeration? And what may be said of accord in its still wider reaches, of the endless shades and tones of blended meanings of words, which, woven with highest artistry, we call literary style? The English instructor who told his fair pupil, "Horses sweat; men perspire; but ladies merely glow!" had a clear perception of the fitness of words to one another. The lady who boasted to a friend, "You just can't realize the enormity of Mr. Smith's Cohen business!" likewise had a clear perception of what she wanted to express. We smile because her verbal pistol went off in the wrong direction. We have one connotation for the word enormity; she had another. All malapropisms are of this character; a word properly according with one set of meanings is wrenched into an incongruous environment, and the result is verbal humor. But the consideration of such misplaced words merely illumines the truth that our entire verbal system is a vast interplay of according ideas. The extent to which such accord is carried out efficiently constitutes clarity of style; the extent to which accord is manifested brilliantly constitutes style itself. Oliver Wendell Holmes speaks somewhere of words which have been betrothed from the cradle, but never mated until joined by the master hand. The highest literary art blends sound and sense so delicately as to afford exquisite enjoyment. Accord here has become to a great extent a self-conscious 140 WHY ENGLISH SOUNDS CHANGE I art, but it is not the less accord; and the highest art, for example that of Shakespeare, takes account of every variety of accord, from the harmony of sounds to the most lofty blending of ideas. The fields of semantics and of literary style have never been reduced to order by research. Students of the former have been somewhat like children pick- ing up pebbles on the seashore; each word is exam- ined in turn and abandoned. Studies in semantics have thus almost without exception been supplements to dictionaries; and significant studies in style have been few. Probably the highest literary expression will al- ways elude complete analysis; but countless more pedestrian subjects are in need of light. For example, many studies of recurrence accord as a factor in style might well be undertaken. A study of obso- lescence is needed, to cast light on the causes why words disappear from the language. Also verbal humor might well be studied along the lines just suggested. Indeed, the charting of accord in sounds appears a comparatively simple matter beside its analysis in these largely unexplored fields. APPENDIX A FEW RULES STATED At the risk of some repetition, it appears wise to assemble here certain rules of phonology, most of which have been formulated and discussed here or there in the body of this study, others of which have merely been assumed to be true, and will now be stated for the first time. These rules are brought together for convenience of reference, with no great pretense at completeness or logical arrangement. Several have to do with stress (grading), others with accord, others with neither. In general it may be said that all of them present what the writer believes to be facts important to the present argument, but facts largely undiscovered or disregarded in current linguistic writing. 1. In general, sounds tend to influence preceding more than succeeding sounds. This is a matter of ob- served fact which the writer will not attempt to explain. Its bearing upon the discussion of accord is obvious, since it means that we generally look to a succeeding sound for the explanation of a change in a preceding one. Perhaps the most radical example of this rule is in umlaut (page 33); but it is exempli- fied in practically all accordings. 2. For the terms long and short as used of vowels, 141 142 APPENDIX tense and slack should be substituted. For discussion, see pages 46 to 50. 3. Vowel sounds change more readily than con- sonants, tense vowels than slack. 4. Sound change in vowels normally occurs with reference to tongue height rather than tongue region. That is, the back vowels ordinarily stay in their own series without becoming front, and vice versa; mōr does not become meer, nor mate, moat. The motor picture of the active part of the tongue in making a given sound appears to be clear, but the exact height of that active part is not so clear. A violin player may finger a string slightly too far up or down, but he is not likely to pass from one string to another unknowingly. Just so, the tongue appears capable of more accurate action in regard to region than to height. 5. Accord in vowels works generally by approxi- mation. This is due to the unstable character of the vowel. 6. Accord in consonants works in general by sub- stitution when the according elements are near each other, by ablation when they are far apart in the mouth. Tune [tjun] passes easily by substitution to [tsun], [sjur] to [Sur]; but in niht, cealc, noth- ing but an ablation is likely to straighten the line of sounds. 7. An increase of stress on the initial syllable of words facilitated the leveling and final dropping of English inflectional terminations (page 51). A FEW RULES STATED 143 8. Stress in consonants tends to unvoice, unstress to voice (pages 52, 53). 9. Stress in vowels tends to tense, unstress to slack (pages 50-54). 10. Unstressed back vowels tend to become leveled as [ə], unstressed front vowels as [ə] or [1] (page 52). 11. Sound change is more likely to take place under stressed conditions. This is illustrated by the frequent alteration of initial consonants before, or by excep- tion to, medial or final ones. It is also shown to some degree by the fact that tense (usually stressed) vowels became raised, while slack vowels remained at the same level. 12. Linguistic habit consists of that body of sounds to which a people is accustomed (pages 55 to 57). 13. Accordings not involving spelling take place easily; others with greater difficulty (pages 57 to 60). Ablation accord, 29 ff., 63 Ablaut, 44 Accord, defined, 19, 22 ff., 39, 40 in morphology, 38, 131 ff. in semantics, 138 ff. in syntax, 39, 135 ff. in tense vowels, 90 ff. of ideas, 38 ff., 129 ff. types of, 25 ff., 63, 65 ff. Analogy, objections to term, 129 ff. Approximation accord, 25 ff., 63 in consonants, 65 ff. umlaut as, 35 Assimilation, objections to term, 19, 20 Bantu, 36, 73 BLOOMFIELD, quoted, 111, 112 Breath-voiced, INDEX of consonants, 6, 7, 28–29, 65 rule, 52 ff. BRIDGES, quoted, 59 ff. Causes of linguistic change, 113 ff. "Center of gravity" in languages, 71, 124 Chinese, 134 63 Consonants, accord in, 65 ff. categories applied to, 6 Consonants-Continued classified, 9 frequency of, 12, 91 influence on vowels, 96 ff. opposed to vowels, 4-6 Danish, 114, 135 Diphthongization, 106 ff. Disaccord, 68, 122 Ease theory, 23 note, 56, 125, 126 ELLIS, 60 Final sounds, change in, 32, 68 Finnish, 36, 73 French, 13, 45, 56, 57, 71, 75, 76, 78, 79, 80, 103 note, 114, 115, 124 Fringe sounds, 18, 69, 71 German, 57, 67, 71, 77, 106 note, 124 Grading, defined, 43 ff. types of, 45 ff., 63 Greek, 36, 37, 49, 89, 106 note Gutturals, elimination of, 66 ff. Conservation, principle of, 55 ff., Icelandic, 118 Hebrew, 35, 37, 38, 74, 134, 135 Intellectual element, the, 60 ff. JESPERSEN, 39 note, 61, 72, 111, 127 145 146 INDEX A KRAPP, 8, 11, 73, 81, 97, 98 Latin, 36, 37, 49, 75 Linguistic habit, 56-57, 63, 82 LUICK, 30 note MILNE, 4 Modulation, sound, types of, 41 ff., 63 Mutation accord, 33–35, 63 Pitch, 41-43, 45-46, 63 Point consonants, 9-13 Quantity, 42, 46 ff., 63 Recurrence accord, 35 ff., 63 defined, 36 Rounded-unrounded, of consonants, 7 note, 29 of vowels, 7 note, 99 ff. RUSSELL, 49 note Russian, 56 Sanskrit, 3, 75, 76 Spelling influence, 57 ff., 63 Stress, 44 ff., types of s. grading 63 SWEET, 60, 92 Tense slack, of consonants, 7 note of vowels, 7 note, 13 note, 47 ff. rule for vowels, 52 ff. slack vowel changes, 93 ff. slackenings in vowels, 104 ff. tensings in vowels, 102 ff. tense vowel changes, 88 ff. Theories of sound change, 85 ff. Transmission theory of language, 86 note, 118 ff. Vowels, accord in, 85 ff. categories applied to, 7 classified, 9 in early alphabets, 3, 4 opposed to consonants, 4-6 WRIGHT, 34, 35, 39 note, 79 note WYLD, 30 note, 48, 58, 104 !! * r . 3 :: : Cat Martin ( 1 2 WERE NE SE ---- ---- 1 UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN 3 9015 00577 0394 DO NOT REMOVE OR MUTILATE CARD Tanan kegaga M thông đ đến 3 ngư