Poca t i a } aly re pk yi tc rset : CORNELL UNIVERSITY LIBRARY THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC SERIES VOLUME LXIV BY THE SAME AUTHOR. The Origin of Civilization and the Primitive Condition of Man, Mental and Social Condition of Savages. With Mlustrations. 8vo. Cloth, $5.00. Prehistoric Times, as illustrated by Ancient Remains and the Manners and Customs of Modern Savages. Cloth, $5.00. Ants, Bees, and Wasps. A Record of Observations on the Habits of the Social Hymenoptera. With Colored Plates, 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. The Pleasures of Life. 12mo. Cloth, 50 cents; paper, 25 cents. THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC SERIES. Each book complete in One Volume, 12mo, and bound in Cloth, 1. FORMS OF WATER: A Familiar Exposition of the Origin and Phenomena of Glaciers. By J. Tynpau1, LL. D., F.R.S. With 25 llustrations. $1.50. 2. PHYSICS AND POLITICS; Or, Thoughts on the Application of the Prin- ciples of “Natural Selection’ and ‘‘Inheritance”’ to Political Society. By WALTER Bacenor. $1.50. 3. FOODS. By Epwarp Smurru, M.D., LL. B., F.R.S. With numerous Illus- trations. $1.75. 4, MIND AND BODY: The Theories of their Relation. By ALEXANDER Batn, LL.D. With 4 Illustrations. $1.50. THE STUDY OF SOCIOLOGY. By Hrrprrt Spencer. $1.50. THE NEW CHEMISTRY. By Professor J. P. Cooxz, of Harvard Univer- sity. With 31 Illustrations. $2.00. ON THE CONSERVATION OF ENERGY. By Batrovur STewanrt, M.A., LL.D., F.R.S. With 14 Illustrations. $1.50. 8. ANIMAL LOCOMOTION; or, Walking, Swimming, and Flying. By J.B. Pettigrew, M.D., F.R.8., etc. With 130 Illustrations, $1.75. . RESPONSIBILITY IN MENTAL DISEASE. By Henry Mavpstey, M.D. $1.50. 10. THE SCIENCE OF LAW. By Professor SHELDON Amos. $1.75. 11. ANIMAL MECHANISM: A Treatise on Terrestrial and Aérial Locomotion. By Professor E. J. Maney. With 117 Illustrations. $1.75. 12. THE HISTORY OF THE CONFLICT BETWEEN RELIGION AND SCI- ENCE. By J. W. Draper, M.D., LL.D. $1.75. 13. THE DOCTRINE OF DESCENT AND DARWINISM. By Professor Oscar Scumipt (Strasburg University), With 26 Illustrations. $1.50. 14, THE CHEMICAL EFFECTS OF LIGHT AND PHOTOGRAPHY. By Dr. Hermann Vocet (Polytechnic Academy of Berlin). Translation thoroughly revised. With 100 Dlustrations. $2.00. 15. FUNGI: Their Nature, Influences, Uses, etc. By M. C. Cooks, M.A., LL.D. Edited by the Rev. M. J. Berkeley, M.A. F.L.9. With 109 Illustrations. $1.50. 16. THE LIFE AND GROWTH OF LANGUAGE. By Professor WILLIAM Dwicut WHITNEY, of Yale College. $1.50. eet = oO New York. D. APPLETON & CO., 1,8, & 5 Bond Street. 5 The International Scientific Sertes—(Continued.) 1%. 18. 19, 20. 21. 22, 26. 27. MONEY AND THE MECHANISM OF EXCHANGE. By W. STANLEY Jzvons, M.A., FL.R.S. $1.75. THE NATURE OF LIGHT, with a General Account of Physical Optics. By Dr. Evcene Lommex. With 188 Llustrations and a Table of Spectra in Chromo-lithography. $2.00. ANIMAL PARASITES AND MESSMATES. By Monsieur Van EENEDEN. With 88 Tlustrations. $1.50. FERMENTATION. By Professor ScuptTzENBERGER. With 28 Illustrations. $1.50. THE FIVE SENSES OF MAN. By Professor BERNSTEIN. With 91 Illus- trations. $1.75. THE THEORY OF SOUND IN ITS RELATION TO MUSIC. By Pro- fessor Pierro BLAsERNA. With numerous Illustrations. $1.50. STUDIES IN SPECTRUM ANALYSIS. By J. Norman Lockyer, F.R.S. With 6 Photographic Ilnstrations of Spectra, and numerous Engravings on Wood. $2.50. . A HISTORY OF THE GROWTH OF THE STEAM-ENGINE. By Pro- fessor R. H. Tourston. With 163 Llustrations. $2.50. . EDUCATION AS A SCIENCE. By ALexANvER Bary, LL.D. $1.75. STUDENTS’ TEXT-BOOK OF COLOR; Or, Modern Chromatics. With Applications to Art and Industry. By Professor OepEN N. Roop, Colum- bia College. New edition. With 130 Illustrations. $2.00. THE HUMAN SPECIES. By Professor A. DE QUAT?EFAGES, Membre de l'Institut. $2.00. . THE CRAYFISH: An Introduction to the Study of Zodlogy. By T. H. Huxuey, F.R.8. With 82 Dlustrations. $1.75. . THE ATOMIC THEORY. By Professor A. Wurtz. Translated by E. Cleminshaw, F.C.S. $1.50. . ANIMAL LIFE AS AFFECTED BY THE NATURAL CONDITIONS OF EXISTENCE. By Karu Semper. With 2 Maps and 106 Woodcuts. $2.00. . SIGHT: An Exposition of the Principles of Monocular and Binocular Vision. By Joszra Le Conte, LL.D. With 132 Illustrations. $1.50. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLES AND NERVES. By Professor J. RosenTHAL. With 75 Llustrationsa. $1.50. ILLUSIONS : A Psychological Study. By James SuLLy. $1.50. THE SUN. By C. A. Youna, Professor of Astronomy in the College of New Jersey. With numerous [lustrations. $2.00. New York: D, APPLETON & CO., 1,3, & 5 Bond Street. The International Scientific Series—(Continued.) 3 386. 3%. 38. 39. 40. 41 43. 44, 46. 4M. 49, 50. 51. 52, . VOLCANOES: What they Are and what they Teach. By Joun W. Jupp, F.R.8., Professor of Geology in the Royal School of Mines. With 96 ll- lustrations. $2.00. SUICIDE: An Essay in Comparative Moral Statistics. By Henry Mor- SELLI, M.D., Professor of Psychological Medicine, Royal University, Turin. $1.75. THE FORMATION OF VEGETABLE MOULD, THROUGH THE AC- TION OF WORMS. With Observations on their Habits. By Cuarizs Darwiy, LL.D., F.R.S. With Illustrations. $1.50. THE CONCEPTS AND THEORIES OF MODERN PHYSICS. By J.B. STALLO. $1.75. THE BRAIN AND ITS FUNCTIONS. By J. Luvs. $1.50. MYTH AND SCIENCE. By Tito Vienou. $1.50. DISEASES OF MEMORY: An Essay in the Positive Psychology. By Tu. Rreot, author of “ Heredity.’ $1.50. . ANTS, BEES, AND WASPS. A Record of Observations of the Habita of the Social Hymenoptera. By Sir Joun Lugzzocx, Bart., F.R.8., D.C. L, LL. D., etc. $2.00. SCIENCE OF POLITICS. By SHELDON Amos. $1.75. ANIMAL INTELLIGENCE. By Groner J. Romans. $1.75. . MAN BEFORE METALS. By N. Jouy, Correspondent of the Institute. With 148 Illustrations. $1.75. THE ORGANS OF SPEECH AND THEIR APPLICATION IN THE FORMATION OF ARTICULATE SOUNDS. By G. H. von MEYER, Pro- fessor in Ordinary of Anatomy at the University of Ziirich. With 47 Woodcuts. $1.5. FALLACIES: A View of Logic from the Practical Side. By ALFRED Srpewror, B.A., Oxon. $1.75. . ORIGIN OF CULTIVATED PLANTS. By ALPHONSE DECANDOLLE. $2.00. JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. Being a Research on Primitive Nervous Systems. By Grorcz J. Romanes. $1.75. THE COMMON SENSE OF THE EXACT SCIENCES. By the late Wai1- 1am Kinepon Cuirrorp. $1.50. PHYSICAL EXPRESSION: Its Modes and Principles. By Franors War- NER, M.D., Assistant Physician, and Lecturer on Botany to the London Hospital, etc. With 51 Ilustrations. $1.75. ANTHROPOID APES. By Rozert Hartmann, Professor in the University of Berlin. With 63 Illustrations. $1.75. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1,8, &5 Bond Street. The International Scientific Series—(Continued.) 53. 54. 5s 5 5%. 58. 59. 60. 61. a 62. oy 2S THE MAMMALIA IN THEIR RELATION TO PRIMEVAL TIMES. By Oscar ScumiptT. $1.50. COMPARATIVE LITERATURE. By Httcuxson MacauLay PosneETT, M.A., LL. D., F.L.8., Barrister-at-Law ; Professor of Classics and English Literature, University College, Aukland, New Zealand; author of “* The Historical Method,” etc. $1.75. EARTHQUAKES AND OTHER EARTH MOVEMENTS. By Joun MILNE, Professor of Mining and Geology in the Imperial College of Engineering, Tokio, Japan. With 38 Figures. $1.75. MICROBES, FERMENTS, AND MOULDS. By £. L. Trovessart, With 107 Dlustrations. $1.50. THE GEOGRAPHICAL AND GEOLOGICAL DISTRIBUTION OF ANI- MALS. By ANGELO HEILPRIN. $2.00. WEATHER. A Popular Exposition of the Nature of Weather Changes from Day to Day. With Diagrams. By Hon. RatpH ABE .CROMBY. $1.75. ANIMAL MAGNETISM. By Atrrep Binet and CHartets FERS, Assistant Physician at the Salpétriére. $1.50. INTERNATIONAL LAW, with Materials for a Code of International Law. By Leone Levi, Professor of Common Law, King’s College. $1.50. THE GEOLOGICAL HISTORY OF PLANTS. With Illustrations. By Sir J. Wiutiam Dawson, LL. D., F.R.8. $1.75. ANTHROPOLOGY. An Introduction to the Study of Man and Civilization. By Epwarp B., Tytor, D.C. L., F.R.8. Illustrated. $2.00. TBE ORIGIN OF FLORAL STRUCTURES, THROUGH INSECT AND OTHER AGENCIES. By the Rev. Gzorce Henstow, M.A., F.L. s., F.G.S. With 88 Illustrations, te THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC SERIES ON THE SENSES INSTINCTS, AND INTELLIGENCE | OF ANIMALS WITH SPFCIAL REFERENCE TO INSECTS BY Sir JOHN LUBBOCK, Barr. M.P., F.R.S., D.C.L., LL. D. AUTHOR OF ‘‘ ANTS, BEES, AND WASPS ;"’ ‘‘ PREHISTORIC TIMES,” ETC, WITH OVER ONE HUNDRED ILLUSTRATIONS NEW YORK D. APPLETON AND COMPANY 1888 ® PREFACE. —+02-—_- In the present volume I have collected together some of my recent observations on the senses and intelli- gence of animals, and especially of insects. While attempting to understand the manners and customs, habits and behaviour, of animals, as well as for the purpose of devising test experiments, I have found it necessary to make myself acquainted as far as possible with the mechanism of the senses, and the organs by means of which sensations are transmitted. With this object I had to look up a great number of memoirs, in various languages, and scattered through many different periodicals ; and it seemed to me that it might be inte- resting, and save others some of the Jabour I had to undergo myself, if I were to bring together the notes I had made, and give a list of the principal memoirs consulted. I have accordingly attempted to give, very briefly, some idea of the organs of sense, commencing in each case with those of man himself. vi PREFACE. Mr. John Evans, Dr. M. Foster, and my brother, Dr. Lubbock, have been so kind as to read through the proofs, and I have to thank them for many valuable suggestions. Lord Rayleigh also has becn so good as to look at the chapters on Hearing. Hicgu Exas, Down, Kent. CONTENTS. CHAPTER IL Introductory remarks—Difficulty of the subject—The life of a cell—Possible modes of origin of sense-organs—Origin of eye and ear—The sense of touch—The organs of touch— Nerves of touch—Sense of temperature—Cold points—Heat points — Pressure-points—Organs of touch among lower animals — Medusxz — Annelides—Mollusca—Crustacvca—In- sects—Sense-hairs—Tactile hairs oe oe ae 1 oe PAGE CHAPTER II. ‘The sense of taste—Taste-organs of man—Mammalia—Birds— Reptiles—Taste-organs of the lower animals—Crustacea— Insects—Sense of taste in insects—Organs of taste in insects—The bee—Humble bee—Wasp—Fly—Individual differences ... ii a ove eee ww = 19 CHAPTER III. Thesense of smell—Protozoa and Coelenterata— Worms— Mollusca —Insects—Seat of the sense of smell—Different theories as to the seat of the sense of smell in Insects—Experiments with Dinetus — Hydaticus — Silpha — Stag-beetle — Ants— Seat of the sense of smell partly in the palpi, partly in antenne—Orgaus of smell—Lceydiy’s olfactory cones— Organs of smell in Crustacca—Centipedes—Olfactory cones in insects—Olfactory hairs—Olfactory pits—Ol- factory organs of fly—Antenna of Ichneumon—Olfactory organs of wasp—Antennal organs of insects—Complex structure of the antennz— Various uses of antenne os = 82 vill CONTENTS. CHAPTER IV. PAGE The sense of hearing—Organs of sound—-Mollusca—Crustacea— Insects — Locusts — Grasshoppers — Crickets — Cicadas — Beetles—The bombardier beetle—Paussus—Death-watch— Burying beetles—Weevils—Cockchafers—Variety of organs of sound among beetles—Diptera—Hymenoptera—Ants— Bees—-Sounds produced in flight—Power of varying sound —Butterflies—Moths—Centipedes—Spiders—Power of hear- ing in insects—Sense of hearing in insects oe ww. ~=—60 CHAPTER V. The organs of hearing—Structure of the human ear—The organ of Corti—Mode of action of auditory organs—Organs of hearing in the lower animals—Meduse—Auditory hairs— Mollusca—Annelides—Crustacea—Use of grains of sand as otolithes—Ear in tail of Mysis—Mode of hearing—Organs of hearing in insects—Seat of the sense of hearing in insects —Different seats of organs of sense—Ears in legs of crickets —Ear of grasshoppers—Structure of ear—Auditory rods— Ear of locusts—Peculiar structure in leg of ant—Origin of ear—LEar of fly—Peculiar sense-organs— Auditory rods in bectles—Position of auditory rods—Chordotonal organs— Auditory hairs of antenne of gnat—Sympathetic vibrations —Organs of hearing in various parts of body wt ae AE CHAPTER VI. The sense of sight—Three possible modes of sight—Different forms of eye—The vertebrate eye—Structure of the eye— The retina—The rods and cones—The blind spot in the eye —Inversion of the rods—The pineal gland—-The rudimentary median eye—The median vertebrate eye—The organs of vision in the lower animals—Color-spots—Echinoderms— Worms—Molluses—Cuttle-fish—Compound eyes in Molluscs —Arca—Spondylus—Pecten—Onchidium—Sensc-organs of Chiton tee vas fas a aia +. 118 CHAPTER VII. The organs of vision in Insects and Crustacea—Ocelli—Compound eyes— Cornea—Crystalline cones — Retinula — Pigment— Different forms of eyes—Structure of the optic lobes—Eyes On On CONTENTS. of Crustacea—Structure of eye—Mysis—Corycwus—Copilia —Calanella—Limulus—Scorpions—Light-organs of Eu- phausia—Mode of vision by compound eyes— Miiller’s theory of Mosaic vision—Images thrown by the cornea—Objections to other theories—Position of the image—Absence of power of accommodation—Absence of retina- Summary—On the power of vision in insects—Experiments on vision of inseets —On the function of ocelli—Difficulty of subject—Experi- ments—Short sight of ocelli—Ocelli of cave-dwelling spiders —Probable function of ocelli ... pes toe eee CHAPTER VIII. problematical organs of sense—Muciferous canals of fish— Deep-sea fish—Light-organs—Living lamps—Problematical organs in lower animals—Medusee—Insects—Crustacea— Difficulty of problem—Size of ultimate atoms—The range of vision and of hearing—Unknown senses—The unknown world ae wis CHAPTER IX. bees and colors--Experiments with colored papers—Dr. Miiller’s objections—Reply to objections—Preferences of bees—The colors of flowers... aa eee wee CHAPTER X. ‘the limits of vision of animals—Ants and colors—The ultra-violet rays—The limits of vision in ants—Supposed perception of light by the general surface of the skin— Experiments with hoodwinked ants—Confirmation of my ‘experiments on ants—Experiments with Daphnias—Daph- nias and colors—Preference for yellowish green—Experi- ments—Limits of vision of Daphnias—Perception of ultra-violet rays—Objections of M. Merejkowski—Suggestion ‘that Daphnias perceive brightness, but not color—Further experiments—Evidence that Daphnias perceive differences of color aye one na te one toe CHAPTER XI. On recognition among ants—Experiments with intoxicated ants —Evidence against recognition by means of a sign or pass- word—Experiments with ants removed from the nest as 1x PAGE 146 182 194 202 x CONTENTS. PAGE pupz and subsequently restored—Experiments with drowned ants—Recognition after a year and nine months—Supposed recognition by scent—Recognition by means of the antenne 232 CHAPTER XII. On the instincts of solitary wasps and bees—Instinct of render- ing victims insensible—Origin of instincts—Habits not invariable—Change of instinets—Bembex—Odynerus—Am- mophila—Modifiability of instinets—Differences under different circumstances—Origin of the habits of Sphex— Race differences—Limitation of instinct—Toleration of para- sites—Cases of apparent stupidity—M. Fabre’s experiments —Limitation of instinct—Instinct and habits—Inflexibility of instinct — Different habits of males and females—Arrange- ment of male and female cells—Power of mother to regulate the sex of the young ... wee ose tas ve «242 CHAPTER XIII. On the supposed sense of direction—Experiments with bees— Whirling bees—Behaviour of bees if taken from home— Mode of finding their way—Experiments with ants—Mr. Romanes’ experiments—No evidence of separate sense of direction ... one ee aaa ees jon. 262 CHAPTER XIV. On the intelligence of the dog—Education of the deaf and dumb —Laura Bridgman—Application of the method followed with the deaf and dumb to animals—My dog Van and his cards—Use of cards with words on them, “ food,” “ water,” | “tea,” etc.—Recognition of the separate cards—Association of the card with the object—Reualization that bringing a card was a request—Attempts to convey ideas—Arithmetical powers of animals—Previous observations—Supposed powers of counting—Mr. Huggins’s experiments—Couclusion .., 272 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS. FIGURE 1. a “. ATO oP 10. 11, 12. 13. 14, 15. Diagram to illustrate possible origin of asense-organ. c¢, Cuticle; A, cellular or hypodermic layer se o . Diagram to illustrate possible origin of a celle beats C; utes ; A, cellular or hypodermic layer . Diagram to illustrate possible origin of a sense- are) C, Cuticle: ; h, hypoderm; n, nerve oe . Diagram of further stage in the origin of a sense-organ . Diagram illustrating a second possible origin of a sense-organ . Diagram of further stage in the origin of a sense-organ . Section through the simple eye of a young Dytiscus larva. A, Hypoderm ; 7, lens; 0, optic nerve; g, p, modified hypodermic cells; vr, retina .. . as ne . Auditory vesicle of Cntochis a avout to the optic, is the thickest in the 150. fr, tr, The tw as 3 trachen’ ar, the'awae, body—divides soon after entering a. the tibia into two branches; the one forming almost immediately a ganglion, the supra- tympanal ganglion, to which I shall refer again pre- sently; the other passing down to the tympanum, where it expands into an elongated flat ganglion, known after its discoverer as the organ of Siebold (Fig. 65), and closely applied to the anterior trachez. STRUCTURE OF EAR. 103 It is well shown in Fig. 65, taken from Graber. At the upper part of the ganglion is a group terminating below in a single row of vesicles, the first few of which eeese ‘Se SN Y ve PI [5k : leg Po ‘a Tr ay Fig. 65.—The trachee and nerve-end organs from the tibia (leg) of a grasshopper (Ephippigera vitium); after Graber. ERI, Terminal vesicles of Siebold’s organ ; AT, binder tympanum ; Sp, space between the trachee; AFr, hinder branch of the trachea; SN, nerves of the organ of Siebold; go, supra-tympanal ganglion ; Gr, group of vesicles of the organ of Siebold; vN, connecting nerve-fibrils between the ganglionic cells and the terminal vesicles ; So, verve terminations of the organ of Siebold ; v7, front tympanum ; v7’, front branch of the trachea. are approximately equal, but which subsequently diminish regularly in size. Each of these vesicles is connected with the nerve by a fibril (Fig. 65, vN), and contains an auditory rod (Fig. 66). 104 AUDITORY RODS. One of these auditory rods is shown in Fig. 66, and the general arrangement is shown in the suljoined diagrammatic figure (Fig. 67). The rods were first described by Siebold, who con- sidered them to be auditory from their association with the stridulating organs. They have since been discovered in many other insects, and may be re- garded as specially characteristic of the acoustic organs of insects. They are brightly refractive, more or less elon- gated, slightly club-shaped, hollow (in which they differ from the retinal rods), and terminate, in Graber’s opinion,* in . J _ a separate end-piece (Fig. 66,0). In "ea of a ene. different insects, besides being in some Vnuissimus Caner cases more elongated than in others, Graber, Fig. 90). ° < : fl, Auditory rod; they present various minor modifica- *o, terminal Piece tiong in form, but are nearly uniform in size—about ‘016 mm.; being as large, for instance, in the young larva of a Tabanus (2 mm. long) as in much larger insects. They are, as we shall see, widely distributed in insects, but as yet unknown in other animals. At the upper part of the tibial organ of Ephippigera there is, as already mentioned, a group of cells, and below them a single row (Fig. 65) of cells gradually diminishing in size from above downwards, One can- not but ask one’s self whether the gradually diminish- ing size of the cells in the organ of Siebold (Fig. 66) may not have reference to the perception of different * Graber, “Die chordotonalen Sinncsorgane und das Gehor der Insekten,” Arch. fiir Mic. Anat., 1882. POSITION OF AUDITORY RODS. 105 notes, as is the case with the series of diminishing arches in the organ of Corti (ante, p. 80) of our own ears. I have already alluded to the supra-tympanal ganglion; this also terminates in a number of vesicles Fig, 67.—Diagram of a section through the auditory organ of a Grasshopper (Meco- nema). ¢, cuticle; u.r, auditory rod; a.c, auditory cell; tr, trachea. containing auditory rods, which are said to be somewhat more elongated than those in the organ of Siebold. The arrangement of the organ is very curious, and will best be understood by reference to Fig. 68. The great auditory nerve, as already mentioned, bifurcates almost immediately after entering the tibia, and one of the branches swell into a ganglion: from this ganglion proceed fibres which enlarge into vesicles (Fig. 68), each containing an auditory rod; and then again contract, approximate into a close bundle, and coalesce with the hypoderm (inner skin) of the wall of the tibia. The supra-tympanal organ of the crickets closely resembles that of the grasshoppers, while, on the other hand, they appear entirely to want the organ of Siebold (Fig. 65). This is a very remark- able difference to exist in two organs otherwise so similar. There appear to be two ways in which the atmospheric 106 EAR OF LOCUSTS. vibrations may be communicated to the nerve: either the vibrations of the tympanum may act upon the air in the trache, and so upon the auditory rods, or the air in the trachee may remain passive, and tlie vibrations may act upon the auditory rods through the fluid in the anterior chamber of the leg. The fact that the auditory rod is turned away from the trachee would seem to favour this hypothesis. Fig. 68.—Outer part of a section through the tibia of a Gryllus viridissimus (after Graber). hk, Hind surface of leg; p, wall of trachea; F, fat bodies; Su, suspensor of the trachea; vIV, tracheal wall; ZV, nerve; gz, ganglionic cells; rB, tissue connecting the ganglionic cells; £.Sch., end tubes of the ganglionic cells, each containing an auditory rod; fa, terminal threads of ditto. In the true Locustidee (Acridiodeze of Graber) the organ of hearing is situated, not in the anterior tibia, but in the first segment of the abdomen ; externally it is marked by a glistening appearance, and it is oval, orin some cases nearly ear-shaped. It was first noticed by Degeer. Behind the tympanum is a large tracheal sac, as in the families already described, and the tension of the tympanum is regulated by one, or in some cases by two muscles. The tympanum also presents two chitin- EAR OF LOCUSTS. 107 ous or horny thickenings, a small triangular knob, and a larger, somewhat complicated piece, consisting of two processes—a shorter upper, and a longer lower one, making a broad angle with one another. As in the preceding families, so also in the Locustidee, the acoustic nerve is in close connection with the trachez ; it swells into a ganglion, which con- tains in some species as many as 150 auditory rods, and then, as in the supra-tympanal organ (see p. 105), con- tracts into a tapering end, which is attached to the small chitinous knob. ‘The auditory rods differ in no respect, as yet ascertained, from those already described. For many years no structure corresponding to the tibial auditory organ of the Orthoptera was known in any other insect. In 1877, however, I discovered * in ants a structure which in some remarkable points resembles that of the Orthoptera, and which I described as follows :—“The large trachea of the leg (Fig. 69) is considerably Fig. 69.—Tibia of yellow ant (Lastus flavus), x 75. 8,8, Swellings of large trachea; rt, small branch of trachea; x, chordotonal organ. swollen in the tibia, and sends off, shortly after entering the tibia, a branch which, after running for some time parallel to the principal trunk, joins it again. “Now, I observed that in many other insects the * Lubbock, “On the Anatomy of Ants,” Microscopical Journal, 1877. 108 PECULIAR STRUCTURE IN LEG OF ANT tracheee of the tibia are dilated, sometimes with a recurrent branch. The same is the case even in some mites. I will, however, reserve what I have to say on this subject, with reference to other insects, for another occasion, and will at present confine myself to the ants. If we examine the tibia, say of Lasius flavus, we shall see that the trachea presents a remarkable arrange- ment (ig. 69), which at once reminds us of that which occurs in Gryllus and other Orthoptera. In the femur it has a diameter of about ;!55 of an inch; as soon, however, as it enters the tibia, it swells to a diameter of about =4, of an inch, then contracts again to g},, and then again, at the apical extremity of the tibia, once more expands to =3,;. Moreover, as in Gryllus, so also in Formica, a small branch rises from the upper sac, runs almost straight down the tibia, and falls again into the main trachea just above the lower sae. «The remarkable sacs (Fig. 69, 8, S) at the two extremities of the trachea in the tibia may also be well seen in other transparent species, such, for instance, as Myrmica ruginodis and Pheidole megacephala. « At the place where the upper tracheal sac contracts (Fig. 69) there is, moreover, a conical striated organ (a), which is situated at the back of the leg, just at the apical end of the upper tracheal sac. The broad base lies against the external wall of the leg, and the fibres converge inwards. Indications of bright rods may also be perceived, but I was never able to make them out very clearly.” This closely resembles both in structure and position the supra-tympanal auditory organ of the Orthoptera. Graber has entirely confirmed this account and dis- covered some insects in which the structure is more ORIGIN OF EAR. 109 clearly visible than in any which I had examined. Fig. 70 represents part of the tibia of Isopterya apicalis. These organs do not, however, appear to be univer- sally present. In some very transparent species no trace of them can be found. But though so similar in structure, and probably in Fig. 70.*—Part of the tibia of Isopteryx apicalis (after Graber). | Se, Auditory organ 5 ef, terminal filament; Cu, cuticle; @, ganglion cells; ef, terminal filaments; tr, trachea; n, nerve. function, it may be doubted whether this tibial organ in the ants can be traced to acommon origin with that of the Orthoptera. According to Graber, the direction of the rods is reversed in the two cases, which he regards as clear proofs that they have arisen independently. He is even of opinion that the tympana themselves have originated independently in the different groups of Orthoptera. Moreover, Graber has found this organ in certain insects not only in the anterior, but also in. the two other pairs of legs. Indeed, rods of the same character have been found in other regions of the body. * In this, as in one or two of the other figures, the explanation of some of the lettering appears to be omitted in the original. At least, I have been unable to find it. 110 EAR OF FLY. As long ago as 1764 Keller * observed that the base of the curious club-like “halteres,’ or rudimentary hind-wings of flies, “est garnie de poils trés courts, ou la tige a le plus d’epaisseur pres du corps; elle est Fig. 71.—One of the haltcres of a fly (after Lowne). permit him to inflexible, et presque garrotté par en haut de plusieurs nerfs; en un mot, elle est faite de maniére que l’on peut juger par sa force par les dehors.” This observation remained unnoticed, and no further description appears to have been given of the organ until it was redis- covered by Hicks in 1856, and more fully described in 1857.4 He found that though in the Diptera (flies and gnats) the hind wings are reduced to two minute, club-shaped organs, they still receive a nerve which is the largest in the insect, except that which goes to the eyes. This proves that they must serve some important function, and renders it almost certain that they are the seats of some sense. He also found at the base of the balteres a number of “ vesicles,” arranged in four groups, and to each of which the nerve sends a branch, though the mode of pre- paration which he adopted did not see the finer structure of the nerves, which he figures as mere fine, hard lines. He describes the “vesicles” as “thin, transparent, hemispherical, or * “Geschichte der gemeinen Stubenfliege,” 1764. I have not seen the original, and quote from Hicks’s paper. t Transactions of the Linnean Society, vol. xvii. PECULIAR SENSE-ORGANS. 111 more nearly spherical projections from the cuticular surface,” and as placed in rows. The number and arrangement differ in different species: the blowfly (Sarcophaga carnaria) has ten rows, Syrphus luniger as many as twenty. These organs have recently been again examined by Bolles Lee.* The vesicles are, according to him, un- doubtedly perforated, contain a minute hair, and those of the upper groups are protected by hoods of chitine. He inclines to correlate them with the similar antennal organs, which he regards as olfactory. His view of the minute structure of these rods differs from that of previous authors, and the subject requires further study. He finds, moreover, that the sense-organ containing the rods has nothing to do with the vesicular plates, but that they are attached to the cuticle in a different place, and where it presents no special modification. The numerous small membranes in the halteres of insects seem to bear somewhat the same relation to the single tympanum of, say, the locust, as the many- faceted eyes do to those with a single cornea. The head of the halteres is divided into two separate spaces by a membrane composed of elongated hypo- dermal cells. The upper part contains a number of large vesicular cells, like those which are in connection with the ends of the trachee. It does not appear to contain any special sense-organ, and, in fact, the large nerve is almost entirely devoted to the sense-organs at the base. M. Bolles Lee suggests that it perhaps serves principally to regulate the pressure on these delicate structures. * «Ties Balanciers des Dipteres,” Recueil Zool. Suisse, 1885. 7 112 AUDITORY RODS IN BEETLES. Special sense-organs occur also on the wings of other insects. Hicks found them * most perfect in the Diptera, next so in the Coleoptera, rather less so in the Lepidop- tera, but slightly developed in the Neuroptera, scarcely at all in the Orthoptera (though this assertion may be hereafter modified), and that only a trace of them exists in the Hemiptera.” They are similarly constituted and equally developed in both sexes. Hicks regarded them as organs of smell. Leydig,* on the contrary, considered them as auditory organs. His mede of preparation dis- played better the structure of the nerves, and he found that they end in peculiar, club-shaped rods (Stdbchen oder Stafle), closely resembling those in the ears of Orthoptera. He observes that, as in the case of the ubial auditory rods of Orthoptera these rods are of two sorts, which are arranged separately, those in one part of the organs being shorter and blunter, those in another more pointed and elongated. Bolles Lee, on the contrary, considers that the supposed existence of two forms, pointed and rounded, is merely due to an optical deception, aud that in reality they are all similar. Leydig also observed in some cases that the rods were thrown into fine ridges. He found also somewhat similar papille on the front wings of certain insects, but could not detect in them the characteristic nerve-ends. It must be confessed that the base of the wing would not seem a convenient place for an organ of hearing. The movements of the wing, it might well be supposed, would interfere with any delicate sensations. till, this objection would apply to almost any sense being thus placed. “ Auditory rods” are now, moreover, known to occur * Miiller’s Archiv., 1860. POSITION OF AUDITORY RODS. 1138 in other parts of the body; for instance, they have been discovered in the antennz of a water-beetle (Dytiscus) and of Telephorus by Hicks, Leydig, and Graber, and in the body segments of several larvze by Leydig, Weiss- mann, Graber, Grobben, and Bolles Lee. In the larva of Dytiscus, indeed, they have been observed in the body, antennz, palpi, under lip, and levs. Moreover, while, as we have seen, in the tibiew of Orthoptera and the halteres of flies they are numerous, in some of these cases they are few, sometimes, indeed, only a single rod being present, as discovered by Grobben in Ptychoptera.* Nevertheless the evidence that they are really acoustic organs is, in the case of the Orthoptera, so strong, their structure is so peculiar, and the gradation of these organs from the most com- plex to the most simple is so complete, that it seems reasonable to attribute to them the same function. Moreover, as regards the very simplest forms there is another consideration pointing to this conclusion. We have seen that in the Orthoptera the terminal filaments close up, and are attached to the skin. Now, it seems to be a very general rule, in reference to these organs, that they are attached to the skin at two points, between which is situated the attachment of the nerve. These points, moreover, are so selected as to be main- tained at the same distance from one another, thus pre- serving an equable tension in the connecting filament. Fig. 72, for instance, represents part of one segment of the body of the larva of a gnat (Corethra). This larva is as transparent as glass, and very common in ponds, a most beautiful and instructive microscopic object. EG is the ganglion; a is the nerve in question, which * Sitz. der K. Akad. der Wiss, Wien, 1876. 114 CHORDOTONAL ORGAN OF GNAT-LARVA. swells into a little triangular ganglion at g; from g the auditory organ runs straight to the skin at e¢, and contains two or three auditory rods (not, how- ever, shown in the figure) at the point Chs; in the opposite direction, a fine ligament passes from g to the Fig. 72.—Right half of eighth segment of the body of the larva of a gnat (Corethra plumicornis); after Graber. EG. Ganglia; ¥, nerve; g, auditory ganglion; gb, auditory ligament; Chs, auditory rods; a, auditory nerve; e, attachment o. auditory organ to the skin, b, attachment of auditory ligament to the skin; hn, hn’, termination of skin-nerve; tb, plumose tactile hair; A, simple hair; tg, ganglion of tactile hair; Im, longitudinal muscle. skin at b. Hence the organ ge is suspended in a certain state of tension, and is favourably situated to receive even very fine vibrations.* There are, as we have seen, a large number of observations which point to the antenne as organs of hearing, and many more might have been given. When we come to consider, however, the anatomical provision which renders the perception of sound * Similar organs occur in other insects, as, for instance,in Ptychoptera. AUDITORY HAIRS ON ANTENN4 OF GNAT. 115 possible, we are met by great difficulties. The evidence is, I think, conclusive that the antenne are olfactory as well as tactile organs, and I believe that they serve also as organs of hearing. There are, moreover, as shown in the last chapter, various remarkable structures in the antenne, and I have given reasons for thinking some of them to be the seat of the sense of smell. Which, if any, of the remainder convey the sense of sound, it is not easy to determine. I have suggested that Hicks’s bottles (Fig. 43) may act as microscopic stethoscopes ; * but they occur, so far as we at present know, only in ants and certain bees. Fig. 73.—Head of gnat. That some of the antennal hairs are auditory can, I think, no longer be doubted. Johnson, whose figure I give (Fig. 73), suggested in 1855 that the hairs on the antenne of gnats serve for hearing. Mayer also,t * Tam glad to see that Leydig, who, however, does not appear to have read either Hicks’s paper or mine, also regards these as chordotonal organs (Zool. Anz., 1886). + Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science, 1855. ft American Journal of Science and Arts, 1874. 116 SYMPATHETIC VIBRATIONS. led by the observations of Hensen, has made similar experiments with the mosquito, the male of which has beautifully feathered antenne. He fastened one down on a glass slide, and then sounded a series of tuning- forks. With an Ut, fork of 512 vibrations per second he found that some of the hairs were thrown into vigorous movement, while others remained nearly stationary. The lower (Ut,) and higher (Ut,;) harmo- nics of Ut, also caused more vibration than any intermediate notes. These hairs, then, are specially tuned so as to respond to vibrations numbering 512 per second. Other hairs vibrated to other notes, extending through the middle and next higher octave of the piano. Mayer then made large wooden models of these hairs, and, on counting the number of vibra- tions they made when they were clamped at one end and then drawn on one side, he found that it “ coincided with the ratio existing between the numbers of vibrations of the forks to which co-vibrated the fibrils.” It is, interesting that the hum of the female gnat corresponds nearly to this note, and would consequently set the hairs in vibration. Moreover, those auditory hairs are most affected which are at right angles to the direction from which the sound comes. Hence, from the position of the antenne and the hairs, a sound will act most intensely if it is directly in front of the head. Suppose, then, a male enat bears the hum of a female at some little di.tauce. Perhaps the sound affects one antenna more than the other. He turns his head until the two antenne are equally affected, and is thus able to direct his flight straight towards the female. The auditory organs of insects, then, are situated in ORGANS OF HEARING IN VARIOUS PARTS OF BODY. 117 different insects in different parts of the body, and there is strong reason to believe that even in the same animal the sensitiveness to sounds is not necessarily confined to one part. In the cricket, for instance, the sense of hearing appears to be seated partly in the antenn, and partly in the anterior legs. In other cases, as in Corethra, the division appears to be carried still further, and a “chordotonal” organ occurs in each of several segments. No doubt the multiplication of complex organs, like our ears, arranged as they are to appreciate a great variety of sounds, would be so great a waste that any theory implying such a state of things would be quite untenable; but with simple organs, such, for instance, as that of Corethra* (gnat; Fig. 72), the case is different, and there would seem to be an obvious advantage in such organs occurring in different parts of the body, ready to receive sound-waves coming from different directions. Moreover, the different organs exist; they do not appear to be organs of touch, yet they are clearly organs of sense, and that sense, what- ever it be, whether hearing or any other, and though it may well be simple, and even perhaps confused, must be seated in various parts of the body. The fact of their being so distributed does not make it more improbable that they should be organs of hearing, than of any other sense. At the same time, it is an interesting result of recent investigations that the auditory organs of insects are not only situated in various parts of the body, but are constructed on such different principles. * Where, however, the number does not approach to that in certain Medusz (see ante, p. 84). CHAPTER VI. THE SENSE OF SIGHT. Ir might at first sight seem easy enough to answer the question whether an animal can see or not. In reality, however, the problem is by no means so simple. We find, in fact, every gradation from the mere power of distinguishing a difference between light and darkness up to the perception of form and colour which we ourselves enjoy. The undifferentiated tissues of the lower animals, and even of plants, are, as we all know, affected in a marked manner by the action of light. But to see, in the sense of perceiving the forms of objects, an animal must possess some apparatus by means of which—firstly, the light coming from different points, a, b, ¢, d, e, etc., is caused to act on separate parts of the retina in the same relative positions; and secondly, by means of which these points of the retiua can be protected from the light coming in other directions. There are three modes in which it is theoretically possible that this might be effected. Firstly, let S 8’ be an opaque screen, with a small orifice at 0. Let abede be a body in front of the THREE POSSIBLE MODES OF SIGHT. 119 screen. In this case the rays from the point ¢ can pass straight through the orifice 0, and fall on the retina of an eye, or on a flat surface atc’. There is no other direction in which the rays from ¢ could pass through 0. In the same way, ¥ the light from a would fall on the point a’, that from b on U’, from d on d’, and e one. ¥ es The results which would be ? Cet given in this way would be, e 4 a however, very imperfect, and, -¢ a’ as a matter of fact, no eye con- structed on this system is sf known to exist. Secondly, let a number of transparent tubes or cones with opaque walls be ranged side by side in front of the retina, and separated from one another by black pigment. In this case the only light which can reach the optic nerve will be that which falls on any given tube in the direction of its axis. Fig, 74. Fig. 75. For instance, in Fig. 75 the light from a will pass to a’, that from b to 0’, that from ¢ to c’,and so on. The light from ¢, which falls on the other tubes, will not 120 DIFFERENT FORMS OF EYE. reach the nerve, but will impinge on the sides and be absorbed by the pigment. Thus, though the lght from ¢ will illuminate the whole surface of the eye, it will only affect the nerve at ¢’. In this mode of vision, which was first clearly explained by Johannes Miller, the distinctness of the image will be greater in proportion to the number of separate cones. “An image,” he says,* “formed by several thousand separate points, of which each corre- sponds to a distinct field of vision in the external world, will resemble a piece of mosaic work, and a better idea cannot be conceived of the image of external objects which will be depicted on the retina of beings endowed with such organs of vision, than by comparing it with perfect work of that kind.” There is, it will presently be seen, reason to suppose that the compound eyes of insects, crustacea, and some molluscs, are constructed on this plan. Thirdly, let L (Fig. 76) be a lens of such a form Fg. 76. that all the light which falls upon its surface from the point a is re-collected at the point a’, that from b at 0’, from ¢ at c',and so on. If now other light be excluded, * « Phys. of the Senses,” by Johannes Miiller, translated by Dr. Baly. THE VERTEBRATE EY#. 121 an image of ac will be thrown on a screen or on a retina at a’ b’'c'’. The image, it will be observed, is necessarily reversed. This is the form of eye which we possess ourselves: it is, in fact, a camera obscura. It is that of all the higher animals, of most molluscs, the ocelli of insects, ete. Fig. 77, taken from Helmholtz, will give an idea of the manner in which we see. Hy = Z =, f ie Fig. 77.—G, Vitreous humor; Z, lens; W, aqueous humor; ¢, ciliary process; d, optic nerve; ¢ e, suspensory ligament; kk, hyaloid membrane; f 7, h h, cornea; 9 9, choroid; %, retina; 11, ciliary muscle; m/, nf, sclerotic coat; p p, iris; s, the yellow spot. The eyeball is surrounded by a dense fibrous mem- brane, the s«lerotic coat, or white of the eye, mf, nf, which 122 STRUCTURE OF THE EYE. passes in front into the glassy, transparent cornea, f f, hh; the greater part of the centre of the eye is occupied by a clear gelatinous mass, the vitreous humor, G, in front of which is the lens, L ; while between the lens and the cornea is the aqueous humor, W. The sclerotic coat is lined at the back of the eye by a delicate, vascular, and pigmented membrane—the choroid, g g, so called from the great number of blood-vessels which it contains ; in front this membrane joins the iris, p p, which leaves a central opening, the pupil, so called from the little image of ourselves, which we see re- flected from an eye when we look into it. The iris gives its colour to the eye, its posterior membrane con- taining pigment-cells; if these are few in number, it appears blue, from the layer behind shining through, and the greater the number of these cells the deeper the colour. ee, is a peculiar membrane, which serves to retain the lens in its place. The optic nerve, d, enters at the back of the eye, and, spreading out on all sides, forms the retina, 2, of which one spot, s, the yellow spot, is pre-eminently sensitive. The action of the eye re- sembles that of a camera obscura, and, as shown in Fig. 76, the rays which fall upon it are refracted so as to form a reversed picture on the back of the eye. The retina (Fig. 78) is very complicated, and, though no thicker than a sheet of thin paper, consists of no less than nine separate layers, the innermost (Figs. 78, 79) being the rods and cones, which are the immediate recipients of the undulations of light. Fig. 79 represents the rods and cones isolated and somewhat more enlarged. The number of rods and cones in the human eye is enormous. Ata moderate computation the cones may THE RETINA, 123 be estimated at over 3,000,000; and the rods at 30,000,000.* i Ati Voy vn i vn ca Fig. 78.—Section aineieat the retina (after Max Schultze). Beginning from the outside, 1, limitary membrane ; 2, layer of nerve-fibres ; 3, layer of nerve~ cells; 4, nuclear layer; 5, inner nuclear layer 3 6, intermediate’ nuclear layer; 7, outer nuclear layer ; 8, posterior membrane ; 9, layer of smal) rods and cones 5 io, choroid. * Sulzer estimates the cones at 3,360,000; Krause places the cones at 7,000,000, the rods at 130,000,000; but Professor M. Foster tells me that he thinks the latter figure is too high. 124 THE RODS AND CONES. It will be observed that the nerve does not, as one might naturally have expected, enter the eye and then spread itself out at the back of the retina, but, on the Fig. 79.—4, Inner segments of rods (s, $, 8) and cones (z, 2’) trom man, the latter in connection with the cone-granules and fibres as far as the external molecular layer, 6. In the interior of the inner segment of beth rod and cone fibrillar structure is visible. X< 800. contrary, pierces tle retina and spreads itself out on the front, so that the cones and rods look inwards, and not outwards—towards the back of the eye, and not at the object itself. In fact, we do not look outwards at the actual object, but we see the object as reflected from the base of our own eye. From the arrangement of the rods in the eyes of verte- brata, then, the light bas necessarily to pass through the retina, and is then re- flected back on it. This involves some loss of light ; on the other hand, it perhaps secures the advantage that the sensitive terminations ot the rods and cones can be more readily supplied with blood. I do not propose to enter into the reason fur this peculiar arrangement, which is connected with the development of the eye. But it is so different from what might have been expected, is in itself so interesting, and makes so important a THE BLIND SPOT IN THE EYE. 125 contrast with the form which is general, though not universal among the lower animals, that I think it will not be out of place to mention a very simple and beautiful experiment by which every one can satisfy himself that it is so. One result is that we have in each eye a blind spot, that at which the nerve enters. Turn the present page, so that the white circle is in front of the left eye and the small cross in front of the right. Then close the right eye, look steadily across at the cross with the left, and move the book slowly backwards and forwards. At one particular distance, about ten inches, the white circle will come opposite the blind spot and_ will instantaneously disappear. Across an ordinary room, if a man stands in front of a screen, his head may in the same way be made entirely to vanish. The ordinary vertebrate eye consists of two main : divisions: the refractive Fig. 80, part, which is a modified portion of the skin; and the 126 INVERSION OF THE RODS. receptive part, which arises from the central nervous system; and the inverted arrangement of the rods is, we can hardly doubt, connected with the develop- ment of the eye, though it is not yet, I think, satis- factorily explained. There is, however, another eye in vertebrates, with reference to which T must say something, and which, though now rudimentary, is most interesting. Our brain contains a small organ, about as large as a hazel- nut, known, from its being shaped somewhat like a cone of a pine, as the pineal gland. Its function has long been a puzzle to physiologists. Descartes suggested that it was perhaps the seat of the soul; and though this idea, of course, could not be entertained, no suggestion even plausible had been made. So matters stood until quite recently, when a most unexpected light has been thrown upon the question. As long ago as 1829, Brandt, describing the skull of a lizard (Lacerta agilis), pointed out that in the centre of the top of the head was a peculiar spot, one of the scales being quite unlike the rest. Leydig * subsequently observed that on the head of the slow- worm (Angus fragilis) there is a dark spot surrounding a small unpigmented body immediately over the pineal gland. Rabl-Riickhard,f in 1884, again called atten- tion to this structure, and suggested that it might serve for the perception of warmth. Ahlborn,t in the same year, was the first to suggest that it was a rudimentary eye. De Graaf§ has the merit of dis- * «Die Arten der Saurier.” t “Entw. des Knochenfischgehirn,” Bericht der Sitz. naturf. Freunde. Berlin: 1882. } “Ueber d. Bedeutung der Zirbeldriise,” Zeit. fiir Wess. Zool., 1884. § “Zur. Anat. und Ent, der Epi. b. Amphilieun und Reptilien,” Zool. Anz., 1886. THE PINEAL GLAND. 127 covering that in the slow-worm the pineal gland is actually modified into a structure resembling an inver- tebrate eye. This remarkable structure has since been examined in various Reptilia by Mr. Spencer.* It appears to be more highly organized in Hatteria than in any other form yet studied; but the retrogression of the different structures has not proceeded pari passu, in some cases the lens, in some the retina, in others the nerve, having been most modified, or having dis- appeared. In Hatteria and Varanus the eye is very distinct; the interior parts being more perfect in the former; while in the latter it is externally most con- spicuous, standing out prominently from its creamy whiteness. The lens is cellular in structure, aud thins away rapidly at the sides. The “rods” are well developed, and embedded in pigment. Spencer describes the various modifications of the organ in the iguanas, chame- leons, flying lizards, geckos, etc. Fig. 81 represents the ex- ternal aspects of the eye-scale in a small lizard (Calotis), with the transparent cornea in the middle, through which the eye is seen; and the diagram Fig. 82 a section through the eye-scale of a small lizard (Lacerta). Fig. 81.—Pineal eye-scale on the A very interesting pom In Geet GF o small Masta (Calotis) 3 connection with the pineal eye consists in the fact that the optic nerve does not penetrate the retina, and then spread out on its outer * Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science, October, 1886. 128 THE RUDIMENTARY MEDIAN EYE surface, as in the lateral eyes of all vertebrates, but, on the contrary, is distributed over its exterior surface. It is, therefore, as De Graaf pointed out, formed in this respect on the type of the usual invertebrate eye; so that we have the remarkable fact that in the same Oph Fig. 82.—Diagram of a section through the skull and pineal eye of Lacerta viridis. C, Cuticle; 7a, parietal bone; Hp, epidermis; ZL, lens; Pig, Pigment; R, rete muscosum ; CH, cerebral hemisphere; W, nerve; E.p, epiphysis; OpL, optic lobe of brain. vertebrate animal we find eyes formed on two different types. Not only so, but the development is dissimilar, the lens of the pineal eye being formed out of the walls of the neural canal. So that the lens of the pineal eye is a totally different structure from that of the lateral eyes. Spencer observed no effect whatever when he threw a strong light on the pineal eye. In fact, he does not believe that in any of the species examined by him the organ is at present in a functional condition. Tndeed, in some cases the cornea is quite opaque, and in others the nerve to the brain is not continuous; so that there can be no vision. At the same time, it seems to be established that this organ is the degraded relic of what was once a true eye, From the size of the pineal orifice in the skull of THE MEDIAN VERTEBRATE EYE. 129 the huge extinct reptiles, such as Ichthyosaurus and Plesiosaurus, it has been, I think, fairly inferred that the pineal eye was much more developed than in any known living form. In living fish and Amphibia, so far as they have been yet examined, the organ is even more rudimentary than in reptiles. But in the fossil Labyrinthodonts the skull possesses a large and well-marked orifice for the passage of the pineal nerve. This orifice is, in fact, so large that it can scarcely be doubted that the eye in these remarkable amphibia was also well developed, and served as a third organ of vision. In birds the organ is present, but retains no re- semblance to an eye. It is solid and highly vascular. In mammals it is still more degenerate, though a trace is still present even in man himself. The larval Ascidians, which present so many points of resemblance to the lowest vertebrates, and especially to the Lancelet (Amphioxus), have hitherto been re- garded as differing from them in the possession of a central eye. It now, however, appears that the verte- brate type did originally possess a central eye, of which the so-called pineal gland is the last trace. It seems, then, very tempting to regard the pineal eye as representing the central eye of Amphioxus; but Spencer points out that the two organs differ greatly in structure, and he himself doubts whether the pineal eye is really the direct representative of the central eye in the Tunicata. Béraneck* also regards the pineal as entirely different from the central eye of the Tunicata. Indeed, he considers its differentiation as an eye to be a * “Ueber d. Parictal Auge der Reptilien,” Jenaische Zeit., 1887. 130 ORGANS OF VISION IN THE LOWER ANIMALS. secondary modification, and considers that it had previously served some other function. However this may be, it cannot be doubted that the pineal gland in Mammalia is the representative of the cerebral lobe which supplies the rudimentary pineal eye of Reptilia, and this itself is probably the degenerate descendant of an organ which in former ages performed the functions of a true organ of vision. Tut ORGANS oF VISION IN THE LOWER ANIMALS. Mere sensibility to light is possible without any optical apparatus. Even plants, as we know, can well distinguish between light and darkness; and though it seems that in our own case the general surface of the skin has lost ils sensitiveness to light, still, in many of the lower animals, light seems to act generally and directly on the tissues. Some microscopic vegetable forms even, as, for in- stance, Englena (Fig. 83), possess a red spot,* which appears to be specially sensi- fey tive to light. The lower animals are, in we a great many cases, very al transparent. Light passes Fig. 88.—Rnglena viridis. easily through them, and, a Een: except in so far as it is ab- sorbed, can hardly be supposed to produce any effect. The most rudimentary form of a light-organ, then, may be considered to be a coloured spot. In the first chapter I have endeavoured to show how * The moving zoospores of certain algw also possess a red spot, which may perhaps have special reference to light. COLOR-SPOTS. 131 it may be possible to trace an almost complete serics from such a mere spot of colour in the skin up to a complex organ of vision, such, for instance, as- that of a snail; indeed, in the development of the eye in the individual animal may be traced some of the same stages as have probably been passed through by the ancestral forms of the animal itself in long bygone ages. We must not, however, suppose that all eyes can be traced back to one and the same origin, or have been developed in the same manner. There are even cases in which an organ fulfilling a different function appears to have been modified into an eye. Look, for instance, at the organ of touch of Onchidium* (Fig. 16). The cuticle (see p. 14) is thickened into a biconvex, almost lens-like body; the epithelial cells are elongated, and below is a mass of cells, to which runs a nerve. A very little change would make this an organ capable of distinguishing light from darkness, and some of the eyes of On- chidium appear, indeed, to have thus originated. Compare with this, for instance, the ocellus of the young larva of a water-beetle (Fig. 84), as figured by Grenacher. The eye-spots of Me- "S-Ssnpiytiscus lnten (after Gronacler. dusce were first noticed heerlen trols timng eres by Ehrenberg in 1836, “"™ and the lens was discovered many years afterwards by de Quatrefages. It is, in fact, by no means universally * A slug-like genus of molluscs. 182 ECHINODERMS. present; the eye, if so it can be called, in many species consisting merely of a coloured spot, while in others it is entirely absent.” Fig. 85.—Eye-spot of Lizzia (after Fig. 86.—Eye-bulb of Astropecten (after Hertwig). oc, Ocelluss; 2, lens. Haeckel). In the Echinoderms, the eyes, which were discovered bv Ehrenberg, have been described by Haeckel,t Wilson;} Lange, and others.§ They are in some cases situated, as in Astropecten, on a pear-shaped bulb (Fig. 86). They consist of a lens (Fig. 87), supplied with a nerve, and lying in a mass of pigment. In Solaster or * Allman, “ Mon. of the Hydroids,” Ray Society, 1871. t “Ueber die Augen und Nerven der Seesterne,” Zeit. fiir Wiss., vol. x. t Transactions of the Linnean Society. § Lange, “Beit. z. Anat. und Hist. der Asterien und Opkiuren,” Morph. Jahrbuch, 1876. WORMS. 133 Asteracanthion the lenses look like brilliant eggs, “each in its own scarlet nest.” In some species there are as many as two hundred eyes; but there appears to be no retina, so that they ean do little more than dis- tinguish between light and darkness. It is quite possible that in some of the lower animals, where the eye-spot is sup- posed to consist merely of a layer of pigment at the end of a nerve, a lens may here- after be discovered. ; . In the Turbellaria* the Boekel 6 Gonmies qeptkiellan, eyes, which were first noticed Cae ers by de Quatrefages, are numerous, and lie immediately under the epithelium (skin). They consist of a certain number of crystalline rods and corresponding retinal cells, resting on a cup-shaped bed of pigment, and con- nected with a nerve. There is often a group on each side of the head, immediately over the brain. In species which possess tentacles the eyes are generally combined with them ; in others they are scattered over the whole periphery of the body, and look in all direc- tions. They differ greatly in size, and in the number of rods and retinal cells—the larger tentacular eyes having several; the small, scattered ones, which are gs more deeply situated, even as few as two or three. * “Die Polycladen,” Fauna und Flora des Golfes von Neapel, 1884. Carrigre, “ Die Augen von Planaria,” Arch. fiir Mie. Anat., 1882. 134 WORMS. In most of the Annulata (worms), the eyes, so far as they have yet been described, are very simple, and probably in most cases not capable of giving more than a mere impression of light. In some species the eye- spot is merely a group of pigmented epithelial cells. In many (Fig. 87) there is, besides the pigment, a well-marked lens. At the same time, it is probable that in some cases this supposed simplicity is more apparent than real. The dioptric part is often cellular, consisting sometimes of one cell, sometimes of several. They are generally, but not always, situated on the .head. The Polyophthalmians (Fig. 90), as already mentioned, have a series along the sides of the body, in pairs from the seventh to the eighteenth segments. I agree with Carriére that there is no sufficient reasun for considering the supposed “eyes” of the leech as organs for the perception of light, but other species of the same group (Clepsiue) possess well-marked, though rudimentary eyes.* Certain leeches—for instance, Piscicola respirans—in addition to the pigmented spots on the head, have also some on the posterior sucking disc. These somewhat resemble the supposed organs of touch, but are larger, and surrounded by pigment. ‘There is no lens, but the large cells are very transparent. It is not supposed that they give any distinct image, or can do more than distinguish light from darkness—as Leydig says, “feel” the light. Still, I must confess that the deter- mination of these curious organs as eyes seems to me very doubtful. Fig. 88 represents the anterior extremity of a small freshwater worm (Bohemilla). “ Graber, “Morph. Unt. iiber die Augen der frei-lebenden Borsten- wiirmer,” Arch. fiir Mic. Anat., 1880. WORMS. 135 Fig. 89 represents an eye-dot of Nereis. In this genus there are two pairs of eyes, which differ some- Fig, 88.— Anterior extremity of a freshwater worm (Bohemilla comata); after Pane’ es ve brain, ¢, cuticle; Ap, hypoderm; Jb, tactile hair; what in structure, the lens in the anterior pair being flatter, that in the posterior more conical. In Hesione the difference is even more matked.f In Polyophthalmus, besides the eyes in the head, there is, as already mentioned, a series along the sides of the body, which differ some- Fig. 89.—Eye-dot of Nereis (after what in structure from those Pune. ob ao 'ad to show in the head. ere Asa general rule, in the Annelids each eye contains a single lens, but the cephalic eyes of Polyophthalmus, according to Mayer, contain three. * “Sys. und Morph. der Oligochsten.” t Graber, “Morph. Unt. iiber die Augen der frei-lebenden Borsten- wiirmer,” Arch fiir Mic. Anat., 1880. 8 136 WORMS. St. Str! ve WL We Fig. 90.—Tke first twelve segments of Polyophthalmus pictus, seen from below (after Mayer). The Roman numerals indicate the segments. S¢, Papille on the head; KS, head; au, head cye; s.au, side cyes; Ol, upper lip; Ul, under lip; v.ph pharyngeal vein; V.subinta, anterior ventral vein; Vud.l'—', veins connecting the superior lateral and vessels ; sept'—*, intersegmentary membranes ; m.ocs.1, lateral muscle of the wsophagus; V.ann, pulsating circular vessel; Md.d7, stomach- glands; V.v-l, vein connecting the inferior and lateral blood-vessels ; Md, stomach; Bm, muscles of the hairs; G, brain; jfl.o, ciliated organ; gm, transverse muscle. MOLLUSCS. 137 The most highly organized eyes in Annelids appear to be those of the Alciopide, which have been described by Krohn,* de Quatrefages,f and especially by Greef t and Graber.§ The Alciopide are small sea-worms; they live principally in the open sea, and, like many other pelagic animals, are extremely trans- parent. It is, indeed, often difficult to see more of them than the two very large eyes, red or orange, and a pair of dark violet dots (the seg- mental organs) on each ring. The principal parts of their eyes are —(1) the outer integument, the whole of which is so transparent that it needs scarcely any modification ; (2) the so- called “eye-skin,’ as to the true nature of which there is still much difference of opinion; (8) the lens; (4) the “corpus ciliare ;” (5) the vitreous humor; and (6) the retina, which again is composed of four layers—(a) the rods; (b) pigment layer; (c) granular layer ; (d) fibrous layer. In Mollusca the eyes are variously ac! (he 4 y tn US “iy (a , fi “HNN a Cie RY oa ‘Wng = aes Fig. 91.—Alciope (after de Quatrefuges). situatel; being, for instance, either placed on the pos- terior tentacles ; or between the feelers, as in the fresh- water species; or on a short stalk at the side of the * «Zool. und Anat. Bemerk. iiber die Alciopeden,” Wéiegmann’s Arch., 1845. + “ Etudes s, 1. typ. Inf. de ’emb. des Annelés,” Ann. Sci. Nat., 1850. t “Unt. iiber die Alciopiden,” Nova Acta Acad. Leop. Carol., vol. xxxix. 11, 1876. § Arch. fiir Mic. Anat., 1880. 138 MOLLUSCS. feelers, as in the Prosobranchiata; or on the back. In some cases they are deeply sunk, even into the brain. Ze Fig. 92.—Perpendicular section through the eye-pit of a limpet (Patella); after Carriére, 1, Epithelial cells; 2, retina cells, 3, vitreous body. The mussels are generally deficient in eyes; and some which are, ag larvae, provided with an eye, lose their eyes when mature. Fig. 93.—Eye of Trochus magus (after Hilger).* G2, Vitreous body ; Nv, nerve. In the limpet (Patella),* on the outer side of the tentacles, where the eyes are situated in more highly organized species, are certain spots, which may be * “Fraisse. Ueher Molluskenaugen,” Zeit. fiir Wies. Znol., 1881. + “Beit. zur Kennt. der Gastropodenaugen,” Gegenbaur’s Morph. Juhrbuch, 1885. MOLLUSCS. 139 regarded as a very rudimentary organ for the per- ception of light. The skin is thrown into a pit, within which the epithelial cells are elongated and pigmented. In the sea-ear (Haliotis), and in Trochus (ig. 98), the arrangement is similar, but the depression is deeper, the mouth is very much restricted, and the interior is filled by a vitreous body. In Murex (Fig. 94) the eye is still further developed, and is entirely closed in, a lens being present. , Fig. 94.—Eye of Murex brandaris (after Hilger). 2, Lens; Gl, vitreous body; No, nerve. In the snail (Helix) the eye is still more highly organized. It consists of a cornea, which lies imme- diately below the skin; a Jens, behind which is the- retina, consisting of three layers, (1) the rods, (2) a cellular layer, (3) a fibrous layer. This, indeed, appears 140 CUTTLE-FISH. to be a very general arrangement in the Mollusca. The power of sight given by such an eye can be but small. Indeed, it is probable that it does little more Fig. 95.—Eye of Helix pomatia (after Simroth).* ct. Cuticle; a, epithelium ; b, cornea ; e, envelope of the ey; d, cellular layer; ¢, fibrils of the optic nerve ; f, feeler cell; na, nerve of the tentacle; no, optic nerve. than distinguish degrees of light. According to Lespés, a Cyclostoma only perceives the shadow of a hand at a distance of five inches, and a Paludina of eight. It is interesting that, as Lankester first showed, the eye of Mollusca, in its gradual development, passes through the stages which we find are the permanent conditions in Patella and Haliotis, commencing as a depression, which grows deeper and deeper, and gradually closes over. Even in the Nautilus the cornea leaves an opening, * Simroth, “Ueber die Sinneswerkzeuge uns. einh. Weichthiere,” Zeit. fiir Wiss. Zool., 1876. t “Obs. on the Dev. of Cephalopoda,” Quarterly Journal of Micro- scopical Science, 1875. COMPOUND EYES IN MOLLUSCS. 141 throngh which the water has free access to the interior of the eye. In the higher cuttle-fishes (Cephalopoda) the eye is very complex, and the optic ganglion is in some cases the largest part of the brain; but, while we find the same parts, as, for instance, in Hélix, though in a higher state of development, there does not seem sufficient reason to regard the two organs as homologous, but it appears possible that the eye of the cuttle-fish had an independent origin. Certain bivalves (Lamellibranchiata) possess bright spots round the edge of the mantle, or on the siphon, which some naturalists maintain to be eyes, while others deny them this character, leaving their true function, however, undecided. But though there is much doubt in some cases, there are other eye-spots which are certainly true eyes. Of these there are two distinct types—those of Spondylus, Pecten, etc., on the one hand ; of Arca, Pectunculus, etce., on the other. The latter present several features of the compound insect’s eye. This was first noticed by Will,* and they have since been more fully described by Carriére f and Patten.{ They are composed (Fig. 96) of large conical cells with the points turned inwards. Pigment is deposited in the periphery of the cells. The outer surface is arched, and forms a biconvex lens. These cells pass gradually into those of the ordinary epithelium. It will be most convenient to consider the mode in which these compound eyes act when we come to * “Ueber die Augen der Bivalven,” Frorieps Notizen, 1844. +t “Die Sehorgane der Thiere,” 1885. + “ Eyes of Molluses and Arthropods,” Miit. Zool. Stat. Neapel, 1886. 142 ARCA—SPONDYLUS. consider those of insects, where they are more highly developed. The eyes of Pecten and Spondylus are, again, formed oa a totally different plan. It has been already observed that there is an Fig. 96.—Perpendicular section through an eye of Arca Noe (after Carriére). 1, Epithelium of the edge of the manile; 2, ceils uf vision; 3, lens; 4, 5, convective tissue; 6, section of one of the cells. essential difference between the typical vertebrate and the typical invertebrate eye; in that while in the former, the optic nerve (Fig. 77) penetrates the retina and then spreads ont on the anterior surface, so that tue “rods” point away from the light; in the normal invertebrate eye, on the contrary, the nerve spreads out on the back of the retina, so that the rods point towards the light. Krohn,* however, made the remark- able discovery that in the genus Pecten the rods, like those of the vertebrates, are turned away from the light. In this case, however, the optic nerve does not enter the retina directly from behind, but runs round it and passes, so to say, over the lip of the cup. Here, then, we get a remarkable approach to the vertebrate eye; but the similarity is still greater in * Miiller’s Arch., 1840. See also Hensen, “ Ueber das Auge einiyer Lanellibranchiaten,” Zeit. fiir Wiss. Zool , 1865. PECTEN, 143 Onchidium (a genus of slugs, widely spread over the Southern Hemisphere), in which Semper has shown * that the nerve actually pierces the retina as in verte- Fig. 97.—Diagram of eye of Pecten (after Hickson). a, Cornea; 6, transparent base- ment membrane supporting the epithelial cells of cornea; c, the pigmented epithelium ; d, the liuing epithelium of the mantle; e, the lens; J, the ligament supporting the lens; g, the retina; h, the tapetum; &, the pigment; m, the retinal nerve; ”, complementary nerve, brates. That this distinctive character should thus reappear in so distant a group is very interesting, and it is also remarkable that Onchidium possesses two kinds of eyes: some on the head, which are constructed on the same type as those of other molluscs; while the peculiar eyes just mentioned are scattered over the back, and their nerves arise, not from the cephalic, but from the visceral ganglion. Moreover, they differ in number, not only in the different species, some having one hun- dred, some as few as twelve, and others none at. all, bnt even in different individuals of the same species. Indeed, they are continually growing and being re- absorbed. But while thus resembling a simple verte- brate eye, the dorsal eyes of Onchidium have a totally * “Ueber Schnecken Augen am Wirbelthier typus,” Arch. fiir Mic. Anat., 1877. 144 ONCHIDIUM. different development, arising, except the nerve, entirely from the integument; on the contrary, in the vertebrate eye, while the cornea and lens are formed from the skin, the retina is an outgrowth from the brain. Semper does not suppose that the Onchidia perceive any actual image with their dorsal eyes, and thinks that they are merely ab’e to distinguish differences in the amount of light. They are shore-living molluscs, and are preyed on by small fishes belonging to the genus Perophthalmus, which has the curious habit of leaving the water and walking about on the sand in search of food. The back of the Onchidium contains a number of glands, each opening by a minute pore; and Semper suggests that, when warned by the shadow of the fish, the little slugs eject a shower of spray, drive off their enemy, and save themselves. This is not quite so far-fetched as might at first sight appear, for we know that there are many other animals, the sepia, many ants, the bombardier and other beetles, etc., which defend them- selves in a similar manner. It seems difficult to understand why the Onchidia should be endowed with so many eyes. The irrelative repetition of organs meets us, however, continually in the lower animals. Mvreover, in the present case Semper has thrown out a plausible suggestion. The organs of touch (see ante, p. 14) curiously resemble eyes in structure, and a very slight change might make them capable of perceiving light. It is possible, then, that some of them may undergo a change of function, and that this may throw some light on the variability in number. In the Chitonide, where dorsal eyes have recently SENSE-ORGANS OF CHITON. 145 been discovered by Moseley,* they are even more numerous. Chiton itself, indeed, has none; but in Schizochiton there are 300, and in Corephium more Fig. 98. Schematic representation of the soft and some of the hard parts in a shell of a Chiton (Acanthopleura), as seen in a section vertical to the surface, and with the margin of the shell lying in the direction of the left side of the drawing. a, Conical termination of sense-organ ; b, b’, ends of nerve; c, nerve; f, calcareous cornea; g, lens; h, iris; &, pigmented capsule of eye; m, body of sense-organ cut ACross ; M, nerve of eye; p, nerve of sense-organ ; 7, rods of retina. than ten thousand. As in Onchidium, they probably arose as modifications of the organs of touch, and are supplied by the same nerves. They possess (1) a cornea, (2) a perfectly transparent and strongly biconvex lens, and (3) the retina, which presents a layer of short but well-defined rods. It is interesting that they point towards the light, and not, as in Onchidium, away from it. * “On the Presence of Eyes in Shells of certain Chitonide,”’ Quarterly Journal of Microscopal Science, 1885. 146 BYES OF CRUSTACEA AND LNSEOTS. CHAPTER VIL. THE ORGANS OF VISION IN INSECTS AND CRUSTACEA. I now pass on to the eyes of insects. In most species of this group there are two distinct kinds: the large compound eyes, which are situated one on each side of the head; and the ocelli, or small eyes, of which there are generally three, arranged in a triangle, between the other two. Speaking roughly, the ocelli of insects may be said to see as our eyes do; that is to say, the lens throws on the retina an image, which is perceived by the fine terminations of the optic nerve. One type of such an eye in a young water-beetle (Dytiscus) is shown in Fig. 84, p. 131. This illustrates the mode of develop- ment of an ocellus, which has been already referred to (ante, p. 131). The structure of fully formed ocelli is shown by Fie. 99. In details, indeed, they present many dif- ferences, and it is remarkable that in some species this is the case even with those of the same individual; for instance, in those of one of our large spiders, Epeira diadema (Fig. 99). In this case the eye B would receive more light, and the image, thereture, would be brighter; but, on OCELLI. 147 the other hand, the image would be pictured in greater detail by the eye A. Ce. ap. ve oe oes Fig. 99.—Long section through the front (A) and hinder (8) dorsal eyes of Zperra diadema, (after Grenacher). .A, Anterior eye; B, posterior eye; Hp, hypoderm ; cular ‘Soren de a, Grone eeelions of Gites Se es oot Oe Cieec ca, L, lens , Gk', vitreous body; Kt, crystalline cones; Rt, retina; Nop, optic nerve. Speaking generally, an ocellus may be regarded as consisting of— 1. A lens, forming part of the general body covering. 2. A layer of transparent cells. 3. A retina, or second layer of deeper lying cells, each of which bears a rod in front, while their inner ends pass into the filaments of the optic nerve. 4, The pigment. From the convexity of the lens it would have a short focus, and the comparatively small number of rods would give but a very imperfect image, except of very near objects. But though these eyes agree so far with ours, there is an essential difference between them. It will be at 148 COMPOUND EYES. once seen that the pigment is differeutly placed, being in front of the reds, while in the vertebrate eye it is behind them. Again, the position of the rods them- selves is reversed in the two cases. Passing on to the compound eye, Fig. 100 gives a section of the eye of a cockchafer (Melolontha), after Strauss-Diirckheim. The separate facets of such an C _ v Yy Fig. 100.—Section through the eye of a cockchafer (Melolontha) ; after Strauss- Diuckheim. eye act themselves as lenses, and give a very perfect image. As regards the number of facets, Leeuwenhoek caleu- lated that there were 3180 facets in the compound eye of a beetle which, however, he does not name. In the house-fly (Musca) there are about 4,000; in the gadfly (distrus), 7,000; in the goat moth (Cossus), 11,000; in the death’s-head moth (Sphina atropos), 12,000; in a butterfly (Papilio), 17,000; in a dragon-fly (Aischna), 20,000; in a small beetle (Mordella), as many as 25,000. STRUCTURE OF THE COMPOUND EYE. 149 The size of the facets seems to bear some relution to the size of the insect, but even in the smallest species none have been observed less than g,y4 of an inch in diameter. Butterflies, which fly in the day, have the facets smaller than those of moths, which are generally evening insects. ; : weak ato The facets are in most cases similar, six-sided, and Fig. 101.—Section through the eye of a fly (after Hickson), 6.m, Basilar membrane ; ¢, cuticle; c.op, epioptic ganglion; m.c., nuclei; 7.¢.s., nerve-cell sheath; Nf, decussating nerve-fibres ; op, optic ganglion ; pe., pseudocone ; pg, pigment cells; p.op, perioptic ganglion; 7, retinula; h., rhabdom; %, trachea; t.a., terminal anastomosis; Tt, trachea; ti, tracheal vesicle. very regular. In locusts, however, they vary a good deal both in form and size. In some flies (Diptera) and dragon-flies (Libellulidz) those in the upper part of the eye are larger than the lower ones, and the junction of the two often forms a well-marked, curved line. 150 CORNEA— CRYSTALLINE CONES. The wonderful complexity is well shown in the pre- celing figure, which represents a s-ction through the eye of a fly, after Hickson.* In illustration of the finer structure, I may take the eye of the bee (Apis) (Fig. 102), as described and figured by Grenacher in his beautiful work.t Fig. 102, the general accuracy of which has been confirmed recently by Dr. Hickson, represents two of the elements of the faceted eve. The structure of the eyes varies considerably in different groups. They may be said to consist of the following principal parts :— 1. The cornea (Lf, Fig. 102). 2. The crystalline cones (Kk), of which there is one immediately behind each facet. The development of the crystalline cone has been carefully Fig 102—Two sepa- gtudied by Claparede. It consists of rate clements of the ‘ ar? farted vye of a bee from four to sixteen original, but com- (afler Grenacher). If. Cornea; n. nu- pletely combined segments, secreted cleus of Semper; Se eee by cells which lie immediately behind cells. #7, retinwla; each facet, but of which, when the eve Rm, rhabdum. : ‘ is completely developed, only the nuclei, known as Semper’s nuclei (7), finally remain. 3. Next comes the retinula (rl), which stands in more or less intimate connection with the pointed inner end of the crystalline cone. It is generally composed of seven, but sometimes of as few as four, or as many * «The Eye and Optic Tract of Insects,” Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science, 1885. + “Untersuchungen tiber das Schorgan der Arthropoden.” 1879. RETINULA—PIGMENT. 151 as eight, originally separate, but closely combined cells. They converge on the optic lobe, and form an outer nucleated sheath, enclosing a strongly refractive, generally quadrangular, rod (the rhabdom, Rm), the relation of which to the filaments of the optic nerve is not yet well understood. 4. The pigment (Pq). Between each separate eyelet (ommateum, or omma- tidium, as it is termed by Hickson), is—at least, in some insects—a long, tubular, thin-walled trachea. These are difficult to see in prepared specimens, but have been mentioned by several observers. They were first, I think, figured by Leydig,* and more recently by Hickson. Finally, the eye is bounded by a basilar membrane, which is perforated by two sets of apertures, a series of larger ones for the passage of the tracheal vessels, and of smaller ones for the nerve-fibrils, The erystalline cone is not, however, always present, and Grenacher divides the compound eyes of insects into three types: acone eyes, in which the crystalline cone is not present, but is represented throughout life by distinct cells ; pseudocone eyes, in which there is a special conical and transparent medium ; and, lastly, eucone eyes, with true crystalline cones.” f * “Zum feineren Bau der Insekten,” Miiller’s Arch. fiir Anat. u. Phys., 1855. + Acone eyes oceur in Nematocera (gnats), Hemiptera (bugs), For- ficula (earwigs), and those Coleoptera (beetles), which have less than five tarsal joints. Pseudocone eyes occur in the true flies (Muscidz), Eucone eyes prevail among other insects: Lepidoptera, Hymenoptera, Neuroptera, Orthoptera, Cicadide, the Coleoptera with five tarsal segments, and among Diptera the single genus Corethra, which, more- over, is remarkable as possessing compound eyes, even in the larva and pupa, 152 DIFFERENT FORMS OF EYES. The last form differs principally from the two first in that the elements which constitute the crystalline cone and the retinula have become completely coalesced and solidified. The differences are, no doubt, im- portant, but I need not enter into them at length here. Even the eucone eyes differ considerably, as may be seen from the following figures, representing (Fig. 103) an eyelet from the eye of a cockroach (Periplaneta), and (Fig. 104) one from that of a cockchafer (Melolontha), both taken from Grenacher. of aaa ee i eae Py” rmen Fig. 103.—Eyelet of cockroach (after Fig. 104.—Eyelet of cockchafer Grenacher). if, Cornea; kk, crys- (after Grenacher), Jf, Cor- talline cone; pg’, pigment cell; rl, nea; kk, crystalline cone; retinula; rm, rhabdom. pg. pg’, pigment cells; ri, Tetinula ; rl’, rhabdom. With some few exceptions (Corethra, Libellula, ete.), the larvee of insects do not possess faceted eyes; indeed, as a general rule their powers of vision are very limited, or they are altogether blind. Most caterpillars have STRUCTURE OF THE OPTIC LOBES. 1538 on each side of the head five or six eye-spots, contain- ing each a crystalline body, but, as we shall presently see, they can probably do little more than distinguish between light and darkness. I do not propose to attempt to give here any detailed account of the structure of the insect brain, but I must say a few words on the subject. Between the brain proper and the eye itself there are, in, for instance, the blow-fly (Musca vonutoria), three distinct ganglionic swellings, which Hickson, a copy of whose beautiful figure I have given (Fig. 101), terms the “opticon” (op), epiopticon (cop), and periopticon (p.op). It will be seen that the nerve-fibrils do not pass in a direct course, but actually decussate, or cross from one side to the other, three times, once between each two ganglionic swellings. ‘The optic lobes of the two sides are also con- nected by a fibrous bundle. ‘he structure of the three nervous swellings is also very complex. It consists of a fine granular matrix, traversed by a meshwork of very minute fibrille, and, at least in the periopticon, is col- lected into a series of cylindrical masses. It is entirely beyond our present range of knowledge to explain the origin or purpose of these complex arrangements, though we cannot doubt that they do serve important functions. It is remarkable that these arrangements, though apparently very constant in individual species and genera, differ greatly in different groups of insects ; for instance, Hickson asserts that in the water-scorpion (Nepa), there is no decussation, and Carriére makes the same statement as regards Libellula; but it seems very extraordinary that this arrangement should be present in some insect eyes, and absent in others furmed apparently on so nearly the same plan. 154 RELATIONS OF OCELLUS, AND EYE. On tHE RELATION OF THE EvyE To THE OCELLUS. In considering the relation of the eye to the ocellus, it is obvious that we cannot regard either as derived from the other. They are, as Grenacher says, “ sisters,” and derived from a common origin. The ocellus consists of a single lens in front of a larger or smaller number of visual rods. The com- pound eye consists of a number of facets, each in front of a single rod; which is produced by from four to sixteen cells: in some cases each c¢ell at first produces a separate rod, and these then subsequently coalesce more or less completely. Starting, then, from a simple form of eye consisting of a lens and a nerve-fibre, which would be capable of perceiving light, but would give no picture of the external world, we should arrive at the compound eye by bringing together a number of such eye-spots, and increasing the number of lenses, while the separate cells beneath each com- bined to form a single cone and rod; while, on the other hand, by increasing the size of the lens, and multiplying the nervous elements behind it, we should obtain the ocellus of an insect, or the typical eyes of a vertebrate animal. There is, indeed, no need to suppose that these two eyes are derived from a common origin. We know that, while very similar eyes occur in distant groups of animals, on the other hand nearly allied species often differ greatly in the structure of their eyes; that, indeed, eyes of very different types often occur even in the same animal, so that we have strong reasons for assuming that they had an independent and separate origin, OCELLI OF SPIDERS—MYRIAPODS. 155 The spiders have simple ocelli only, the higher Crustacea compound eyes only, while many of the lower Crustacea and of the gieat class of the insects possess both eyes and ocelli. It would seem probable, therefore, that the ancestral stock must have possessed both, though not perhaps in so perfect a form as that which has now been attained, and that the spiders have lost the compound eyes, while, on the contrary, in the higher Crustacea the ocelli have disappeared. Moreover, though the ocellus of a spider at first sight closely resembles the eye of a Scolopendya, the internal structure is, according to Grenacher, altogether different. In the ocellus of a spider or an insect we find, at a greater or less distance behind the lens, a retina consisting of a receptive surface, extended con- centrically with that of the lens, and consisting of -a number of more or less rod-like perceptive elements so arranged that the light falls on their ends. On the contrary, in the eyes of Myriapods there is, he says, either a single element behind the cornea, or where there are many such elements, they are arranged with their longer axes perpendicular to the direction of light; so that any separate perception of the rays of light coming from different points seems to be an impossibility. In the eye of Lithobius, behind the biconrex lens, he states that the cells lining what I may call the tube of each separate eye, terminate in filaments, between the free ends of which is left a narrow passage, down which the light must pass to reach the end of the optic nerve. Such a structure is certainly very remarkable, and seems entirely to preclude the possibility of the formation of a true image. Altogether the account given by Grenacher, both as to ihe mode 156 EYES OF CRUSTACEA. of action of the eyes of the Myriapods and as to their internal structure, differs entirely from that of Graber. Fig. 105.—Leptodora hyalina. Tne Kyres or CRUSTACEA. The eyes of many Crustavea are highly developed. In the higher families (thence named Podophthalmata, STRUCTURE OF EYE 157 or stalk-eyed) they are situated on more or less elongated pedestals. In some of the lower forms, though less complex, they are very large, occupying, as in the curious Leptodora (Fig. 105) of our deep lakes, the whole front of the head; while in Coryczus Fig. 106.—Eye of Mysis (after Grenacher). m, Nuclei; Lf, facets; Kk, crystalline cones; n', cells of the retinula; Rl, retinula; Rm, rhabdom; Cp, blood-vessels ; N, fibres of the optic nerve; N', V"', N', N', decussations of the fibres of the optic nerve; G, G', G", G'"', ganglia; 2, muscles for the movement of the eye-stalk; Am', Km'', nuclei. (Fig. 107) they extend to more than one-half of the whole leneth of the body. The higher Crustacea possess no ocelli. In the lower species, on the contrary, a central ocellus is often present, especially in the young state. 158 MYSIS—CORYCHUS—COPILIA. Tn illu-tration of the compound eyes of Cru-tacea, I give a figure of an eye of Mysis (Fig. 106). In the higher Crustacea the nervous elements of the eye are, moreover, very complex. There are no less than four optic ganglia (Fig. 106), and there is a chiasma, or decussation of fibres (N+, NU, N41, N44), between each. The eyes of lobsters and of crabs offer a curious difference. In the former, the crystalline cones are very long, and the retinule comparatively short ; while in the crabs, on the contrary, the crystalline cone is short, and the retinule long. The eye of Coryczus (Fig. 107) is very interesting. It is extremely large in proportion to the size of the Fig. 107.—Coryceus (after Leuckart), a, b, The eye. animal, extending from the front of the head to the beginning of the abdomen. The perceptive part of the eye (0) is, therefore, far removed from the lens (a). The eye of Coryczeus appears to represent, in fact, a single element of a compound eye. The eye of Copilia is also very remarkable, the retinula being, at about the end of the first third of its length, bent at a right angle. Here also the eye is about one-third as long as the body. The ocelli of Crustacea have not been much studied with reference to their microscopic structure. Those CALANELLA—LIMULUS. 159 of Calanella are very remarkable, and, indeed, but for their position and the presence of pigment, would hardly be recognized as eyes. They are three in number,and together form an X-shaped body (Fig. 108), supplied by a large nerve (N.op.), and consisting of three groups of large nerve-cells, embedded in pig- ment. There are eight in each of the two side groups, and ten in the central. In form they are pear-shaped, with the narrow end turned towards the nerve. The organ contains no lens nor rods, Fig. 108.—Eyes of Calanella Mediterranea (after Gerstarker) Pg., pigment cells; Nfr., frontal nerves; .Vop., nervus opticus. The numbers show the numbrs of the cells. The eyes of the king crab (Limulus) have been described by Grenacher and by Lankester and Bourne.* The two lateral eyes form a polished, kidney-shaped protuberance on each side of the great shield. ‘The outer side is smooth, but on the inner surface it is produced into a number of conical processes (Fig. 109), * «On the Eyes of Scorpio and Limulus,” Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Srience, 1883. 9 160 LIMULUS. each of which forms a special lens. Underneath each of these secondary lenses is a group of large, elongated pigmented cells, arranged round a central space, and touching the lens with their outer ends, while the Fig. 109.—Diagram of a vertical section through a portion of the lateral eye of Limulus polyphemus, showing some of the conical lenses, and corresponding retinule (after Lankester and Bourne). a, Cuticle; bd, cuticular lens; ce, hypoderm; Rn, retinula; m, nerves. inner ones are continued into the optic nerve. These nerve-end cells form the “retinula,” while their sides, which face one another, are thickened, and coalesce into a rod, the rhabdom, which is hollow at the end nearest the lens, but solid towards the nerve. The central eye is very different. It possesses a single lens, like that of an ordinary ovellus, underneath which is a layer of cells not differing much in appear- ance from those of the hypoderm, and below which again is another layer of large nerve-cells, which, how- ever, are so irregular as to suggest the idea that the central eye of the king crab may have partially lost its function. The king crab, then, so remarkable in other ways, is also very interesting in reference to the peculiar SCORPIONS—LIGHT-ORGANS OF EUPHAUSIA. 161 structure of its eyes. These can hardly be regarded as homologous with the compound eyes of insects and Crustacea, but appear to have originated independently. They have, indeed, hardly anything in common, except that of being compound eyes. Lastly, I may allude to the eyes of scorpions, which, though very different from those of Limulus in appearance, in Lankester’s opinion approach them more nearly in essential constitution than any other known eyes. Before quitting this part of my subject, 1 must mention the curious eye-like organs of Euphausia. Euphausia (Fig. 110)—a shrimp-like crustacean, be- Fig. 110. —Euphausia pellucida (after Sars). 1.0,, Luminous organ. longing to the same group as Mysis—and some of its allies, are remarkable for possessing at the base of some of the thoracic legs, and on the four anterior abdominal segments, luminous eye-like organs. They form small bulbs, each containing a vitreous body, some pigment, a lens, and a fan-shaped bundle of delicate fibres, and are very conspicuous from their beautiful red color and glistening lustre. 162 LIGHT-ORGANS OF EUPHAUSIA. Claus * regards them as true accessory eyes. Sars,t on the contrary, considers that they have no power of sight, but are highly differentiated luminous organs. He admits that they present a deceptive resemblance to true eves, but has convinced himself by observations of the living animal that they have no power of Vision. The fibrous fascicle (Fig. 111, f) he finds to be the chief light-producing part,t and the lens-like body in front serves, as he supposes, for a condenser, producing a bright flash of light, the direction of which the animal, by means of its muscles, is able to control. The anterior pair (Fig. 112, do), which differ some- what in structure from the rest, are situated on the Fig. 111.—Luminous organ of Euphausia (after Sars). f Fibres ; ¢, lens. Fig. 112.—Eye-stalk of Euphausia (after Sars). lo, Luminous organ; a, lower eye. evye-stalks, and appear to serve as “ bull’s-eyes” to the true organs of vision. Sars considers that the luminous organs do not serve as eyes, on the grounds * “Ueber einige Schizopoden und niedere Malacostraceen,” Zeit. fiir Wiss. Zool., 1863. + “On the Schizopoda,” “Clallenger Reports,” vol. xiii. t Valentine and Cunningham, in a memoir just published (Quarterly Journal of Microseopien! Science, vol. xxviii.) deny this, and attribute it to the inner surface of the reflector. MODE OF VISION BY COMPOUND EYES. 163 that the nerve which supplies them is but small; that the structure is not really analogous to that of a true eye, and that the position would be very unsuitable, one of them being actually situated on the stalk of the compound eye. The question does not, however, seem to be by any means clearly solved, and it must, I think, be admitted that, with the exception of the anterior pair, if the position does not seem suitable for true eyes, neither is it that which one would expect in light-urgans. On THE MODE oF VISION BY MEANS OF Compotunp Eves. Johannes Miller, in his great work on the Physiology of Vision,* was the first to give an intelligible explana- tion of the manner in which insects see with their com- pound eyes. According to his view (see Fig. 75), those rays of light only which pass directly through the crystalline cones, or are reflected from their sides, can reach the corresponding nerve-fibre. The others fall on and are absorbed by the pigment wiich separates the different facets. Hence each cone receives light only from avery small portion of the field of vision, and the ravs so received are collected into one spot of light. The larger and more conves, therefore, is the eye, the wider will be its field of vision; while the smaller and more numerous are the facets, the more distinct will the vision be. In fact, the picture per- ceived by the insect will be a mosaic, in which the number of points will correspond with the number of facets. * «Zur vergleichenden Physiologie des Gesichtsinnes.” 164 MULLER’S THEORY OF MOSAIC VISION. This theory was at first received with much favour. In 1852, however, Gottsche * attacked MiNer’s view, pointing out that each separate cornea of a compound eye can, and in fact does, give a separate and distinct image. This had, indeed, long previously been ob- served by Leeuwenhoek, who said, “ When I removed the tunica cornea a little from the focus of the micro- scope, and placed a lighted candle at a short distance, so that the light of it must pass throngh the tunica cornea, I then saw through it the flame of the candle inverted, and not a single one, but some hundreds of flames appeared to me, and these so distinctly (though wonderfully minute) that I could discern the motion of trembling in each of them.” t Of this, indeed, it is easy to satisfy one’s self. It is only necessary to look at a candle through the cornea of an insect, and then slightly draw back the micro- scope, when a thousand small images of the candle, each formed by one of the lenses, will be plainly seen. Tf, then, in such cases there was a retina placed at the proper distance, a true image would be formed, as on the retina in our own eyes. This paper of Gottsche’s threw great doubt on Miiller’s explanation, which, indeed, was, in Dors’s words, “ abandonnée par tout le monde.” t It is one thing, however, to see that the lenses throw distin :t pictures, but quite another to understand how such pictures could be received on the retina, or com- bined into one distinct image. * «Beit, zur Anat. und Phys. der Fliegen und Krebse,” Miiller’s Arch., 1852. t¢ A. Van Leeuwenhoek, “ Select Works,” translated by S. Hoole. t “De la vision chez les Arthropodes,” Ar. des Sci. Phys. et Nat. Geneva: 1861]. IMAGES THROWN BY THE CORNEA. 165 It must, moreover, be remembered that in our eyes the whole field of vision is reversed, so that different objects remain in the same relative position. In the case of insects, however, it would be the image thrown by each facet which would be reversed, and hence the general effect would be altogether false. We must not attach too much importance to the mere presence of an image. Any lens-like object, even a globule of fat, will give one. Moreover, as Miller and Helmholtz have shown, the lenses of the cornea would be an advantage on the theory of mosaic vision, by assisting to condense the rays of light on the termination of the nerve. Gottsche’s observation was made on the eye of the blow-fly (Musca vomitoria), and, as a matter of fact, the fly is one of those insects which do not possess a true crystalline cone. It is, therefore, probable that the image which he saw was that of the cornea. Moreover, as is shown by his figure, which I give below (Fig. 113), he states * that the image was formed at , while the retina is far away at y. He suggested, indeed, that the so-called optic ganglion really corresponds with the retina of our own eye; but this would not remove * His words are—“An der hintern Flache der Crystallkérper im Fliegenauge kehrt sich sicher das Bild um, weil das Bild dem object in der Lage gleich ist, und da das Mikroskop das Object einmal umkehrt, so muss hier eine doppelte Umkebrung stattfinden, einmal durch das Mikroskop und vorher durch den parabolischen Orystall- kérper. Entsteht nun bei « (Fig. 113) ein umgekebrtes Bild, so ist die Frage, wird das ganze Bild von # durch den Stiel zur Retina und zur Perception bei y hingeleitet oder wirkt dieser dine Stiel gleichsam wie ein Diaphragma und giebt er nur einen Theil des Bildes bei # nach y” (Guttsche, “ Beit. zur Anat. und Phys. des Auges dcr Krebse und Fliegen,” Arch. fiir Anat. Phys. und Wéss. Medicin., 1852). 166 MOSAIC VISION. the difficulty, because, if any definite picture is to be formed, the sensitive rods, cones, or other structures must lie in the plane of the image, and this is not, in fact, the case. Dor suggested that the crystalline cones are nervous structures, and cor- respond to the ruds of the vertebrate eye (Fig. 79). He admits, however, that, as a matter of fact, the image is not formed at the anterior surface of the crystalline cones.* And yet in his final summary, having shown that the image is formed, not at the anterior surface, but deep down in the crystalline cones, he expresses quite a different view, y compares the crystalline cone to Fig. 113.—One of the the vitreous body, and considers that elements of the eye 3 ° ie of a fly (afer the true retina is to be found in an Gottsche). kk, Ciys- “ talline cone; 2, posi- elvvelope which surrounds the cone. tion of the image; . * s, rod; se, sheath; Plateau f regards the mosaic theory Pi eee ae of Muller as definitively abandoned, serene but rather seems to have had in his mind that of Gottsche. At least, he states that, accord- ing to Miller, the mosaic is formed by a number cf partial images, each occupying the base of one of the elements composing the compound eye. This, how- ever, is not Miller’s theory. * “Ta cornée avee sa convexité postérieure correspond & la cornée et au cristallin des vertébrés, le corps cristallin (avec le soi-disant corps vitré) et la fibre nerveuse qui s’y attache & la couche des batonnets, enfin le ganglion optique ® celles des couches de la rétine, qui sont composées des granulations, des cellules, et des fibres nerveuses.” t Rech. Exp. sur la Vision des Arthropodes.” Bruxelles: 1887. OBJECTIONS TO OTHER THEORIES. 167 On the other hand, Boll,* Exner,t and Grenacher seem to me to have proved that the compound eyes of insects cannot act as ours do; that the theory which assumes that each facet acts as a separate eye and projects an image on a retina, is physically untenable. In the first place, there are cases—for instance, Forficula, Dytiscus, and Stratiomys among insects ; Ligia and many others among Crustacea—where the cornee are nit sufficiently arched to give any distinct image. But even where an image is thrown by the cornea, it would be destroyed by the crystalline cone. In certain Crustacea the crystalline cones are elongated and curved; this, which Oscar Schmidt ¢ regarded as fatal to Miller’s theory, is, on the con- trary, as Exner has pointed out, quite compatible with it, but, on the contrary, cannot be reconciled with the theory of an image. There are few beetles in which the cornea give better images than in the firefly (Lampyris splendidula). On the other hand, the crystalline cones entirely destroy these images. If the eye is looked at through a microscope, and the erystalline cones are left in situ, the field of view appears perfectly black, with a bright spot of light at the end of each cone. No trace of an image can be any longer perceived. In fact, the images seen by Leeuwenhoek and Gottsche are thrown by the cornea only. In most cases, then, it would appear that the image formed by the cornea is destroyed by the crystalline * «Beit. zur Phys. Optik,” Arch. fir Anat. Phys. und Wiss. Medicin., 1871. + “Ueber das Sehen von Bewegungen und der Theorie des zusammengesetzten Auges,” Sitz. K. Akad. d. Wiss. Wien., 1875. $ Ibid., 1876. 168 POSITION OF THE IMAGE. cone. This does not, indeed, always occur; but even in such cases the image docs not coincide with the posterior end of the cone. Grenacher repeated the experiment of Gottsche with moths. Here the crystalline cones are firm, and are attached to the cornea. Thus he was able to remove the soft parts, and to look through the cones and the cornea. When the microscope was focussed at the inner end of the cone, a spot of light was visible, but no image. As the object-glass was moved forward, the imave gradually came into view, and then disappeared again. Here, then, the image is formed in the interior of the cone itself. Exner had endeavoured to make this experiment with the eye of Hydrophilus (the great black water-beetle), but the crystalline cones always came away from the cornea. He, however, calculated the focal length, refraction, etc., of the cornea, and concluded that, even if, in spite of the crystalline cone, an image could be formed, it would fall much behind the retinula. In these cases, then, an image is out of the question. Moreover, as the cone tapers to a pont, there would, in fact, be no room for an image, which must be received on an appropriate surface. In many insect eyes, indeed, as in those of the cockchafer (Fig. 100), the erystalline cone is drawn out into a thread, which expands again before reaching the retinula. Such an arrangement seems fatal to any idea of an image. Moreover, for definite vision by the formation of an image, it is necessary that the eye should possess some power of accommodation for ditierent distances. It is obvious, from Fig. 76, that no distinct vision would be given unless the receptive surface follows the line a’ b' c. But the position of this surface will ABSENCE OF POWER OF ACCOMMODATION. 169 depend upon the distance of abe from the lens. As a matter of fact, Leydig * and Leuckart t thought they had discovered, between the cornea and the crystalline cones, certain muscular fibres which might regulate the distance between the two, and thus effect this object. Subsequent observers, however, have failed to detect these fibres. Again, it will be seen, from a glance at Fig. 76, that in an eye constituted like ours, on the principle of a camera obscura, the retina must follow a regular curve. If it is brought at all too far forward, or furced the lea-t too far back, the image is at once blurred. Hence, in our own case the frequent need for spectacles, and hence it would seem that a conical retina is a physical impossibility. Plateau, indeed, adoptst a suggestion made by Grenacher that the absence of any means of adaptation may be rendered unnecessary by the length of the cones, the rays coming from distant objects acting on the anterior end, those from nearer ones at a greater or less depth. This, I confess, seems to me inadmissible. In the first place, the light must surely act immedi- ately it impinges on the organ of perception ; and, in the second, the cones are, as a general rule, abso- lutely transparent—the light passes unimpeded through them. Again, if insects see with their compound eyes as we do with ours, they must, of course, possess a retivua. No such structure, however, has been as yet shown to * “Zum feineren Bau der Arthropoden,” Miiller’s Arch. fiir Anat. und Phys., 1855. “-‘t “Carcinologisches,” Wiegmann’s Arch., 1858. t “Rech. Exp. sur la vision chez les Arthropodes,” 1887. 170 ABSENCE OF RETINA. exist. Wagner,* indeed, observed that in some cases the optic nerve embraces the end of the cone, and he supposed that it thus forms a sort of retina, for which, however, its form is little suited. I ought also to mention that Max Schultze t con- sidered that he had, in some few cases—for instance, in Syrphus—been able to observe that the termina- tion of the nerve does divide into a number of fibres. Patten,t more recently, has also maintained the existence of numerous nerve-fibrils, which, however, subsequent observers—for instance, Kingsley § and Beddard ||—have been unable to discover. Even, how- ever, if we admit the perfect correctness of Schultze’s observation, these cases are exceptional, and the fibres so few that they can hardly, I think, affect the general conclusion. ‘To give anything like a distinct vision, a very large number would be required. A last objection is the extreme difficulty which would exist of combining so many different images into one idea, though it must be admitted that at first sight this difficulty (though to a minor degree) exists even in the case of simple eyes, the number of which varies considerably. Spiders have six to eight; some aquatic larve twelve; while the Oniscoide (1ood-lice), assuming that their eyes are aggregates of simple eyes, as Miller supposed, have as many as twenty to forty. * Hinige Bemerk. tiber den Bau der zus. Augen,” Arch. fiir Nat., 1835. t “ Unt. iiber die zus, Augen der Krebse und Insecten,” 1868. } “Eyes of Molluscs and Arthropods,” DMitth. Zool. St. Neapel, 1886. § “On the Divisions of the Compound Eye,” Journal of Morphology, 1887. || “On the Structure of the Eye in Cymothoide,” Trans. Roy. Soc. Edin., 1887, SUMMARY. 171 These, however, tuke in different parts of the field of vision. The principal reasons, then, which seem to favour Miller’s theory of mosaic vision are as fullows :— (1) in certain cases—for instance, in Hyperia—there are no lenses, and consequently there can be no image ; (2) the image would generally be destroyed by the crystalline cone; (3) in some cases it would seem that the image would be formed completely behind the eye, while in others, again, it would be too near the cornea ; (4) a pointed retina seems incompatible with a clear image; (5) any true projection of an image would in certain species be precluded by the presence of im- penetrable pigment, which only leaves a minute central passage for the light-rays; (6) even the clearest image would be useless, from the absence of a suit- able receptive surface, since both the small number and mode of combination of the elements composing that surface seem to preclude it from receiving more than a single impression ; (7) no system of accommoda- tion has yet been discovered. finally, (8) a combina- tion of many thousand relatively complete eyes seems quite useless and incomprehensible. On THE PowER oF VISION IN INSECTS, ETC. As regards the practical vision of insects, our know- ledge is still very imperfect. No one, indeed, who has observed them can doubt that in some the sight js highly developed. It is impossible, for instance, to watch a dragon-fly hawking over a pond,—to see the rapidity and accuracy of ifs movements, and doubt that it can see well. 172 ON TUE POWER OF VISION IN INSECTS. On the other hand, Claparéde asserts that at a distance of twenty feet a hive bee would be unable to see any object which was less than eight or nine inches in diameter, and even at a distance of a foot he says that each facet would correspond to an inch and a third. To determine how far a faceted eye could see, he takes the breadth of a facet, the radius of the eye- sphere, and the smallest angle of vision, and the dis- tance in centimetres at which the facet would cover a centimetre, and finds for the bee, for instance, 6°7 centimetres. He then proceeds to inquire at what distance from the faceted eye the image is as clear as in the human eye, and he thinks this would be about a millimetre, from which it would rapidly diminish, being only 7, at a centimetre, and at a metre no distant vision being possible; so that at a very little distance such eyes would be as good as useless. “In the human eye, for example, the distance between the @entres of two adjacent cones is only io Mm., but in Musca the distance between adjacent ommatidia is >}, mm. In fact, the picture, as received by the nerve-end cells of the Vertebrate eye, is much more complete in itself than it can pogsibly be in any Arthropod eye, and consequently the latter possesses a much more elaborate and complete translating appa- ratus in its retina than the former pessesses.” * Claparede arrives ut this conclusion by taking the average carvature of the whole eye, as b ing true for each part. This, however, is nut the case, and in the central region of the eye the adjacent facets * §. J. Hickson, “The Eye and Optic Tract of Insects,” Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science, vol. xav., new series, 1885, p. 242. EXPERIMENTS ON VISION OF INSECTS. 1738 make but a small angle with one another. Lowne has calculated that wasps, humble bees, dragon-flies, etc, would, at a distance of twenty feet, be able to distinguish objects from half an inch to an inch in diameter. Thus a dragon-fly would see an object twenty feet from its eye in the same detail that a man would perceive it at a distance of a hundred and sixty feet. Moreover, when Claparéde * observes that bees will return from a considerable distance straight to the door of their nest, and that, under Miller’s theory, the door would at such a distance be absulutely invisible, he forgets that the bee first probably guides itself by the known position of the door in relation to some tree or other large object, then with reference to the hive itself, and that it is quite unnecessary to assume that the door is actually seen from a distance. With reference to the power which insects possess of determining form, Plateau f has recently made some ingenious experiments. Suppose a room into which the light enters by two equal and similar orifices, and suppose an insect set free at the back of the room, it will at once fly to the light, but the two openings being alike it will go indifferently to either one or the other. That such is the case Plateau’s experiments clearly show, and, moreover, prove that a comparatively small increase in the amount of light will attract the insect to one orifice in preference to the other. It occurred then to Plateau to utilize this by varying the form of the opening, so that the light admitted being * «Zur Morph. der zus. Augen bei den Arthropoden,” Zeit. fiir Wiss. Zool., 1860. + Bull. deVAcad. Roy. de Belgique, t. x., 1885; Comptes Rendus de la Soc. Ent. de Belg., 1887; “ Rech. Exp. sur la Vision chez les Arthropodes,” 1887. 174 EXPERIMENTS ON VISION OF INSECTS. equal, the opening on the one side should leave a clear passage, while that on the other should be divided by bars large enough to be easily visible, and sufficiently close to prevent the insect from passing. His experiments were conducted in a room five metres square, lighted by two similar windows looking to the west. It was on the first flour, and looked out on to fields. Moreover, he had the glass of the windows’ slightly ground, so that, while the light penetrated, nothing outside could be seen. He then covered up the windows, leaving only two orifices, one of which was simple and square, while the other was divided by cross-bars, To secure equality of light, the latter was left somewhat larger than the other, and the equivalence of the two was determined by a Rumford’s photometer. The insects were set free on a table at the back of the room, exactly between the two open- ings, and at a distance of four metres. He states that a very slight difference in the intensity of the light determined the flight of the insect to either one or the other opening; while, if the amount of light was as nearly as possible equal, they flew as often to the one as to the otlier. Omitting the cases when the light was not equal, the numbers were as follows :— Clear Trellised opening. opening. Musca vomitoria (the blucbottle) ... Sep ge 8 nae ME On the other hand, they were—for Eristalis tenax (the bee fly) was 4 8 Vanessa urtice# (tortoiseshell butterfly) . 5 13 20 In fact, then, the insects seem to have gone more EXPERIMENTS ON VISION OF INSECTS. 175 often to the trellised opening. M. Piateau concludes that insects do not distinguish differences of form, or can only do so very badly (“I]s ne distinguent pas la forme des objects ou la distinguent fort mal”). I confess, however, that these experiments, ingenious as they are, do not seem to me to justify the conclu- sions which M. Plateau has deduced from them. Unless the insects had some means of measuring distance (pf which we have no clear evidence), they could not tell that even the smaller orifice might not be quite large enough to afford them a free passage. The bars, moreover, would probably appear to them somewhat blurred. Again, they could not possibly tell that the bars really crossed the orifice, and if they were situated an inch or two further off they would constitute no barrier. I have tried some experiments, not yet enough to be conclusive, but which lead me to a different conclusion from that of M. Plateau. I trained wasps to come to a drop of honey placed on paper, and, when the insects bad learned their lesson, changed the form of the paper, as I had previously changed the color. It certainly seemed to me that the insect recognized the chanve. M. Forel has also tried similar experiments, and with the same result. We know, however, as yet very little with reference to the actual power of vision possessed by insects. On THE FuNcTION OF OCELLI. Another interesting question remains. What is the function of the ocelli? Why do insects have two sorts of eyes? 176 ON THE FUNCTION OF OCELLI. Johannes Miiller considered that the power of vision of ocelli “is probably confined to the perception of very near objects. This may be inferred partly from their existing principally in larva and apterous insects, and partly from several observations which I have made relative to the position of these simple eyes. In the genus Empusa the head is so prolonged over the middle inferior eye that, in the locomotion of the animal, the nearest objects can only come within the range. In the Locusta cornuta, also, the same eye lies beneath the prolongation of the head. ... In the Orthoptera generally, also, the simple eyes are, in consequence of the depressed position of the head, directed downwards towards the surface upon which the insects are moving.” From these facts, he considers himself justified in concluding that the simple eyes of insects are intended principally for myopc vision. The simple eyes bear a similar relation to the compound eyes, as the palpi to the antenne. Both the antenne and compound eyes are absent in the larva: of insects.” * Lowne observes f that “the great convexity of the lens in the ocellus of Eristalis must give it a very short focus, and it is manifestly but ill adapted for ihe formation of a picture. The comparatively small number of rods must further render the production of anything like a perfect picture, even of very near objects, useless for purposes of vision. I strongly suspect that the function of the ocelli is the perception of the intensity and the direction of licht rather than of vision in the ordinary acceptation of the term.” * « Physiology of the Senses,” translated by Baly. ft “On the Modification of the Eyes of Inscets,” Phil. Trans., 1878, DIFFICULTY OF SUBJECT. 177 Réaumur, Marcel de Serres, Dugés, and Forel also have shown that in insects which possess both ocelli’ and compound eyes, the ocelli may be covered over without materially affecting the movements of the animal; while, on the contrary, if tlhe compound eyes are so treated, they behave just as if in the dark. For instance, Forel varnished over the compound eyes of some flies (Musca vomitoria and Lucilia cxsar), and found that, if placed on the grouud, they made no attempt to rise; while, if thrown in the air, they flew first in one direction and then in another, striking against any object that came in their way, and being apparently quite unable to guide themselves. They flew repeatedly against a wall, falling to the ground, and unable to alight against it, as they do so cleverly when they have their eyes to guide them. Finally, they ended by flying straight up into the air, and quite out of sight. It seems, indeed, to be a very general tule that insects of which the eyes are covered, whether they are totally blinded, or whether the ocelli are left uncovered, fly straight up into the air—a very curious and significant fact of which I think no satisfactory explavation has yet been given. Plateau * regards the simple eyes, or ocelli, as rudi- mentary organs of scarcely any use to the insect. Forel also states, as the result of his observations, that wasps, humble bees, ants, etc., find their way both in the air and on the ground, almost equally well without as with the aid of their ocelli. I confess that I am not satisfied on this point. In such experiments great care is necessary. M. Forel’s interesting experiments with ants, whose compound eyes * Bull. deV Acad. Roy. de Belgique, t. a., 1885. 178 EXPERIMENTS he had covered with opaque varnish, might almost, for instance, be quoted to prove the same with reference to the compound eyes. “Mes Camponotus aux yeux vernis,” he says, ‘attaquaient et tuaient aussitét une Formica fusca mise au milieu d’eux, la saisissaient presque aussi adroitement que ceux qui avaient leurs yeux. Ils déménageaient un tas de larves d’un coin de leur récipient 4 l’autre avec autant de précision qu’ avec leurs yeux.” * On the other hand, Forel goes so far as to say that if the compound eyes are covered with black varnish, insects cannot even perceive light (“Cela prouve quelles ne voyaient plus méme la lueur”). In fact, the use of the ccelli seems a great enignia, at least when the compound eyes are present. We must remember that some other Articulata— spiders, for instance—possess ocelli only, and they certainly see, though not probably very well. Plateau has made some ingenious observations, from which it appears that spiders are very short-sighted, and have little power of appreciating form. He found they were easily deceived by artificial flies of most inartistic construction; and he concludes that even hunting spiders do not perceive their prey at a greater distance than ten centimetres (about four inches), and in most cases even less. Scorpions appeared scarcely to see beyond their own pincers. I have also made some experiments on this point with spiders (Lycosa saccata). In this species, which is very common, the female, after laying her eggs, collects them into a ball, which she surrounds with a silken envelope and carries about with her. I captured a * Pecucil Zool. Suisse, 1887, SHORT SIGHT OF OCELLL 179 female, and, after taking the bag of eggs from her, put her on a table. She ran about awhile, looking for her eggs. When she became still, I placed the ball of eggs gently about two inches in front of her. She evidently did not see it. I pushed it gradually towards her, but she tonk no notice till it nearly touched her, when she eagerly seized it. I then took it away a second time, and put it in the middle of the table, which was two feet four inches by one foot four, and had nothing else on it. The spider wandered about, and sometimes passed close to the bag of eggs, but took no notice of it. She wandered about for an hour and fifty minntes before she found it—apparently by accident. I then took it away again, aud put it down as befure, when she wandered about for an hour without finding it. The same experiment was tried with other individuals, and with the same results. It certainly appeared as if they could not see more than half an inch before them —in fact, scarcely further than the tips of their feet. I may also mention that they did not appear to recognize their own bags of eggs, but were equally happy if they were interchanged. On tbe other hand, it must be remembered that the sac is spun from the spinnerets, and the Lycosa had perhaps actually never seen the bag of eggs. Hunting spiders certainly appear to perceive their prey at a distance of at least several inches. Plateau has shown, in a recent memoir, that cater- pillars, which possess ocelli, but no compound eyes, are very short-sighted, not seeing above one to two centimetres.* * «Rech. Exp. sur la Vision chez les Arthropodes.” Bull. de "Acad. Roy. de Belgique, 1888. 180 OCELLI OF CAVE-DWELLING SPIDERS. Lebert has expressed the opinion * “that in spiders some of their eight eyes—those which are most convex and brightly coloured—serve to see during daylight ; the others, flatter and colorless, during the dusk.” Pavesi has observed ¢ that in a cave-dwelling species (Nesticus speluncarum), which belongs to a genus in which the other species have eight eyes, the four middle eyes are atrophied, Tuis suggests that they serve specially in daylight. Returning for a moment to the ocelli of true insects, it seems almost incredible that such complex organs should be rudimentary or useless. Moreover, the evidence afforded by the genus Eciton seems difficult to reconcile with this theory. The species of this genus are hunting ants, which move about in large armies and attack almost all sorts of insects, whence they are known as driver ants, or army auts. They have no compound eyes, but in the place of them most species have a single large ocellus on each side of the head, while others, on the contrary, are blind. Now, while the former hunt in the open, and have all the appearance of seeing fairly well, the latter con- struct covered galleries, and seek their prey in hollow trees and other dark localities. Insects with good sight generally have the crystalline lenses narrow ond long, which inyolves a great loss of light. The ocelli are specially developed in insects, such as ants, bees, and wasps, which live partly in the open light and partly in the dark recesses of nests. Again, the night-flying moths all possess ocelli; while they are entirely absent in butterflies, with, accord- * “Die Spinnen der Schweiz.” f “Sopra una nuova Specie di Racui.” PROBABLE FUNCTION OF OCELLI. 181 ing to Scudder, one exception, namely, the genus Pamphila. On the whole, then, perhaps the most probable view is that, as regards insects, the ocelli are useful in dark places and for near vision.* Whatever the special function of ocelli may be, it seems clear that they must see in the same manner as our eyes do—that is to say, the image must be reverred. On the other hand, in the case of compound eyes, it seems probable that the vision is direct, and the diffi- culty of accounting for the existence in the same animal of two such different kinds of eyes is certainly enhanced by the fact that, as it would seem, the image given by the medial eyes is reversed, while that of the lateral ones is direct. Forel, in his last memoir, inclines to this opinion, CHAPTER VIII. ON PROBLEMATICAL ORGANS OF SENSE. In addition to the organs of which I have attempted in the preceding chapters to give some idea, and to those which from their structure we may suppose to perform analogous functions, there are others of con- siderable importance and complexity, which are evi- dently organs of some sense, but the use and purpose of which are still unknown, “Tt is almost impossible,” says Gegenbaur,* “to say what is the physiological duty of a number of organs, which are clearly sensory, and are connected with the integument. These enlargements are generally formed by ciliated regions to which a nerve passes, and at which it often forms enlargements. It is doubtful what part of the surrounding medium acts on these organs, and we have to make a somewhat far- fetched analogy to be able to regard them as olfactory organs.” Among the structures of which the use is still quite uncertain are the muciferous canals of fishes. The skin of fishes, indeed, contains a whole series of organs of whose functions we know little. As regards the * «Elements of Comparative Anatomy.” MUCIFEROUS CANALS OF FISH. 183 muciferous canal, Schultze has suggested * that it is a sense-organ adapted to receive vibrations of the water with wave-lengths too great to be perceived as ordinary sounds. Beard also leans to this same view. However this may be, it is remarkably developed in many deep- sea fish. In some cases peculiar eye-like bodies are developed in connection (though not exclusively so) with the muciferons canal, Leuckart,t by whom they were discovered, at first considered tlrem to be accessory eyes, but subsequent researches led him to modify this opinion, an] to regard them as luminous organs. Ussowt has more recently maintained that they are eyes, and Leydig considers them as organs which approach very nearly to true eyes (“welche wirblichen sehorganen sehr nahe stehen”), Whatever doubt there may be whether they have any power of sight, there is no longer any question but that they are luminous, and they are especially developed in the fishes of the deep sea. e These are very peculiar. The abysses of the ocean are quite still, and black darkness reigns. ‘he pressure of the water is also very great. Hence the deep seas have a peculiar fauna of their own. Surface species covld not generally bear the enormous pressure, and do not descen to any great depth. The true deep-sea forms are, however, as yet little known. They are but seldom seen, and when * “Ueber die Sinnesorgane der Seitenlinie bei Fischen und Amphibien,” Arch. fiir Mic. Anat., 1870. + “Ueber muthmassliche Nebenaugen bei einem Fische.” Bericht iiber die 39 Vers., Deutscher Naturforscher, Giessen, 1864. t “Ueber den Bau der sog. augenalimlichen Flecken einiger Knochenfische,” Bull. Soc. Imp. Moscow, 1879. 10 184 DEEP-SEA FISH. obtained are generally in # bad state of preservation. Their tissues seem to be unusually lax, and liable to destruction. Moreover, in every living organism, besides those usually present in the digestive organs, the blood and other fluids contain gases in solution. These, of course, expand when the pressure is diminished, and tend to rupture the tissues. The circumstances under which some deep-sea fish have occasionally been met with on the surface bears this out. They are generally found to bave perished while endeavouring to swallow some prey not much smaller, or even in some cases larger, than themselves. What, then, has happened? During the struggle they were carried into an upper layer of water. Immediately the gases within them began to expand, and raised them higher; the process continued, and they were carried up more and more rapidly, until they reached the surface in a dying condition.* It is, however, but rarely that deep-sea fish are found thus floating on the surface, aud our knowledge of them is mainly derived from the dredge, and expecially from the specimens thus obtained during the voyuge of the Challenger. In other respects, moreover, their conditions of life in the ovean depths are very peculiar. The licht of the sun cannot penetrate beyond about two hundred fathoms; deeper than this, complete darkness prevails. Hence in many species the eyes have more or less completely disappeared. In others, on the ‘contrary, they are well developed, and these may be said to be a light to themselves. In some species there are a number of luminous organs arranged within the area * Giinther, “ Introduction to the Study of Fishes.” LIGHT-ORGANS. 185 of, and in relation to, the muciferous system; while in others they are variously situated. These luminous organs were first mentioned by Coveo.* They have since been studied by Ginther, Leuckart, Ussow, Leydig, and Emery. Lastly, they have been catefully deseribed by Giintber, Moseley, and von Lendenteld in the work on “ Deep-Sea Fishes,” in vol. xxvii. of the “Challenger Reports.’ The deep-sea fish are either silvery, pink, or in many cases black, sometimes relieved with scarlet, and, when the Juminous organs flash out, must present a very-remarkable app: arance. We have still much to learn as to the structure and functions of these organs, but there are cases in which their use can be surmised with some probability. The light is evidently under the will of the fish. It is easy to imagine a Photichthys (Fig. 114), swimming Fig. 114.—Photichthys argenteus (‘' Challenger Reports,” vol. xxvii.). in the black depths of the ocean, suddenly flashing out light from its luminous organs, and thus bringing into view any prey which may be near; while, if danger is disclosed, the light is again at once extinguished. It may be observed that the largest of these organs is situated just under the eye, so that the fish is actually provided with a bull’s eye lantern. In other cases * Nuovi Ann. dei Sci. Nat., 1838. 186 LIVING LAMPS. the light may rather serve as a defence, some having— as, for instance, in the genus Scopelus—a pair of large ones in the tail, so that “a strong ray of light shot forth from the stern-chaser may dazzle and frighten an enemy.” * In other cases they probably serve as lures. The “sca-devil,” or ‘angler,’ of our coasts has on its head three long, very flexible, reddish filaments, while all round its head are fringed appendages, closely resembling fronds of seaweed. The fish conceals itself at the bottom,in the sand or among seaweed, and dangles the long filaments in front of its mouth. Other little fishes, taking them for worms, unsuspect- ingly approach, and themselves fall victims. Several species of the same family live at great Fig. 115.—Ceratius bispinosus (*‘ Challenger Reports,” vol. xxvii.). depths, and have very similar habits. A mere red filament would, however, be invisible in the dark, and therefore useless. They have, however, developed (Fig. 115) a luminous organ, a living “ glow-lamp,” at * Giuther, “Challenger Reports,” vol. xxvii. PROBLEMATICAL ORGANS IN LOWER ANIMALS. 187 the end of the filament, which doubtless proves a very effective lure.* These cases, however, though very interesting, throw little light on the use of the muciferous system in ordinary fish, which, I think, still remains an enigma. In some of the lower animals, the nerves terminate on reaching the skin at the base of rod-like structures similar, in many respects, to the rods of the retina, or the auditory rods of the ear, and of which it is very difficult to say whether they are organs of touch or of some higher sense. Round the margin of the common sea-anemone is a circle of bright blue spots, or small bladders. If a section be made, there will be found a number of cylindrical organs, each containing a fine thread, and terminating in a “cnidocil (Fig. 14);” and, secondly, fibres very like nerve-threads, swelling from time to time with ganglionic expansions, and also terminating in a enidocil. These structures, in all probability, serve as an organ of sense, but what impressions they convey it is impossible to say. Some jelly-fishes (Trachynemadz) have groups of long hairs arranged in pairs at the base of the tentacles (Fig. 116), which have been regarded as organs of touch, ‘and it is certainly difficult to suggest any other function for them. They are obviously sense-hairs, but I see no reason for attributing to them the sense of touch. The so-called eyes of the leech, in Leydig’s f opinion, * Giinther, “ Study of Fishes.” + “Die Augen und neue Sinnesorgane der Egel.,” Reichert’s Arch., 1861. 188 MEDUSA—INSECTS—-CRUSTACEA. which is confirmed by Ranke,* are also developed from the supposed special organs of touch. The latter are much more numerous, as many as sixty being developed Fig. 116.—Edge of a portion of the mantle of Aglaura hemistoma, with a pair of sense- organs (after Hertwig). v, Velum; k, sense-organ; ro, layer of nettle cells; ¢, tentacle. on the head alone. They are cylindrical organs, lined with large nucleated refractive cells, which occupy nearly all the interior. A special nerve penetrates each, and, after passing some way up, appears to terminate in a free end. I may also allude to the very varied bristles and cirhi of worms, with their great diversity of forms. Among Insects and Crustacea, there are a great number of peculiarly formed skin appendages, for which it is very difficult to suggest any probable function. The lower antenne of the male in Gammarus, for instance, bear a very peculiar slipper-shaped organ, situated on a short stalk: this was first mentioned by * “Beit. zu der Lehre, von den Uebergangs Sinnesorganen,” Zeit. fiir Wiss, Zool., 1875. DIFFICULTY OF PROBLEM. 189 Milne Edwards, and subsequently by other authors, especially by Leydig.* The short stalk contains a canal, which appears to divide into radiating branches on reaching the “slipper,” which itself is marked by a series of rings. Among other problematical organs, I might refer to the remarkable pyriform sensory organs on the antenne of Pleuromma,t the appendages on the second thoracic leg of Serolis, those on the maxilli- peds of Eurycopa, on the me- tatarsus of spiders, the finger- shaped organ on the antennze of Polydesmus, the singular pleural eye (?) of Pleuromma, and many others. Fig. 117.—Sense-organ of leech There is every reason to (am cers ster Haake) hope that future studies will oa throw much light on these in- teresting structures. We may, no doubt, expect much from the improvement in our microscopes, the use of new reagents, and of mechanical appliances, such as the microtome ; but the ultimate atoms of which matter is composed are so infinitesimally minute, that it is difficult to foresee any manner in which we may hope for a final solution of these problems. Loschmi'lt, who has since been confirmed by Stoney and Sir W. Thomson, calculates that each of the * Zeit. fiir Wiss. Zool.,” 1878. ee + Brady, “On the Copepoda of the Challenger Expedition,” vol. viii. 190 SIZE OF ULTIMATE ATOMS. ultimate atoms of matter is at most sy,golo,qyo0 Of an inch in diameter. Under these circumstances, we cannot, it would seem, hope at present for any great increase of our knowledge of atoms by improvements in the microscope. With our present instruments we can perceive lines ruled on glass which are gy hyo of an inch apart. But, owing to the properties of light itself, the fringes due to interference begin to produce con- fusion at distances of 7445;, and in the brightest part of the spectrum, at little more than 55479, they would make the obscurity more or less complete. If, indeed, we could use the blue rays by themselves, their waves being much shorter, the limit of possible visibility might be extended to ,golo5y3 and, as Helmholtz has suggested, this perhaps accounts for Stinde having actually been able to obtain a photographic image of lines only ,oo'sou of an inch apart. This, however, would appear to be the limit, and it would seem, then, that, owing to the physical characters of light, we can scarcely hope for any great improvement so far as the mere visibility of structure is concerned, though in other respects, no doubt, much may be hoped for, At the same time, Dallinger and Royston Pigott have shown that, as far as the mere presence of simple objects is concerned, bodies of even smaller dimensions can be perceived. According to the views of Helmholtz, the smallest particle that could be distinctly defined, when associated with others, is about gpba5 of an inch in diameter. Now, it has been estimated that a particle of albumen of this size contains 125,000,000 of molecules. In the case of such a simple compound as water, the number would be no Jess than 8,000,000,000. Even then, if we could THE RANGE OF VISION AND OF HEARING. 191 construct microscopes far more powerful than any we now possess, they could not enable us to obtain by direct vision any idea of the ultimate molecules of matter. The smallest sphere of organic matter which could be clearly defined with our most powerful micro- scopes may be, in reality, very complex; may be built up of many millions of molecules, and it follows that there may be an almost infinite number of structural characters in organic tissues which we can at present foresee no mode of examining. Again, it has been shown that animals hear sounds which are beyond the range of our hearing, and that they can perceive the ultra-violet rays, which are invisible to our eyes.* Now, as every ray of homogeneous light which we can perceive at all, appears to us as a distinct color, it becomes probable that these ultra-violet rays must make themselves apparent to the ants as a distinct and separate color (of which we can form no idea), but as different from the rest as red is from yellow, or green from violet. The question also arises whether white light to these insects would differ from our white light in containing this additional color. At any rate, as few of the colors in nature are pure, but almost all arise from the combination of rays of different wave- lengths, and as in such cases the visible resultant would be composed not only of the rays we see, but of these and the ultra-violet, it would appear that the colors of objects and the general aspect of nature must present to animals a very different appearance from what it does to us. These considerations cannot but raise the reflection * « Ants, Bees, and Wasps.” 192 UNKNOWN SENSES. how different the world may—I was going to say must —appear to other animals from what it does to us. Sound is the sensation produced on us when the vibra- tions of the air strike on the drum of our ear. When they are few, the sound is deep; as they increase in number, it becomes shriller and shriller; but when they reach 40,000 in a second, they cease to be audible. Light is the effect produced on us when waves of light strike on the eye. When 400 millions of millions of vibrations of ether strike the retina in a second, they produce red, and as the number increases the color passes into orange, then yellow, green, blue, and violet. But between 40,000 vibrations in a second and 400 millions of millions we have no organ of sense capable of receiving the impression. Yet between these limits any number of sensations may exist. We have five senses, and sometimes fancy that no others are possible. But it is obvious that we cannot measure the infinite by our own narrow limitations. Moreover, looking at the question from the other side, we find in animals complex organs of sense, richly supplied with nerves, but the function of which we are as yet powerless to explain. There may be fifty other senses as different from ours as sound is from sight; and even within the boundaries of our own senses there may be endless sounds which we cannot hear, and colors, as different as red from green, of which we have no conception. These and a thousand other questions remain for solution, The familiar world which sur- rounds us may be a totally different place to other animals. To them it may be full of music which we cannot hear, of color which we cannot see, of sensations which we cannot conceive. To place stuffed birds and THE UNKNOWN WOKLD. 193 beasts in glass cases, to arrange insects in cabinets, . and dried plants in drawers, is merely the drudgery and preliminary of study; to watch their habits, to understand their relations to one another, to study their instincts and intelligence, to ascertain their adaptations and their relations to the forces of nature, to realize what the world appears to them; these constitute, as it seems to me at least, the true interest of natural history, and may even give us the clue to senses and perceptions of which at present we have no conception, CHAPTER IX. ON BEES AND COLORS, In my book on “ Ants, Bees, and Wasps,’* I have recorded a number of observations which seemed to me to prove that bees possess the power of distinguish- ing colors—a power itaplied, of course, in the now generally accepted views as to the origin of the colors of flowers, but which had not up to that time been proved by direct experiment. Amongst other experiments, I brought a bee to some honey which I placed on a slip of glass laid on blue paper, and about three feet off I placed a similar drop of honey on orange paper. With a drop of honey before her a bee takes two or three minutes to fill herself, then flies away, stores up the honey, and returns for more. My hives were about two hundred yards from the window, and the bees were absent about three minutes, or even less; when working quietly they fly very quickly, and the actual journeys to and fro did not take more than afew seconds. After the bee had returned twice, I transposed the papers; but she returned to the honey on the blue paper. J allowed her to continue this for sume time, and then again transposed the papers. She *« Ants, Bees, and Wasps,” International Scientific Series. Kegan Paul, Trench & Co. EXPERIMENTS WITH COLORED PAPERS. 195 returned to the old spot, and was just going to alight, when she observed the change of color, pulled herself up, and without a moment’s hesitation darted off to the blue. No one who saw her at that moment could have the slightest doubt about her perceiving the difference between the two colors. I also made a number of similar observations with red, yellow, green, and white. But I was anxious to carry the matter further, and ascertain, if possible whether they have any preference for one color over another, which had been denied by M. Bonnier. To test this 1 took slips of glass of the size used for slides for the microscope, viz. three inches by one, and pasted on them slips of paper of the same size, coloured re- spectively blue, green, orange, red, white, and yellow. I then put them on a lawn, in a row, about a foot apart, and on each puta second slip of glass with a drop of honey. I also put with them aslip of plain glass with a similar drop of honey. I had previously trained a marked bee to come to the place for honey. My plan then was, when the bee returned and had sipped for about a quaiter of a minute, to remove the honey, when she flew to another slip. This I then took away, when she went toa third,and soon. In this way, as bees generally suck for three or four minutes, I induced her to visit all the drops successively before returning ' to the nest. When she had gone to the nest, I trans- posed all the upper glasses with the honey, and also moved the colored glasses. Thus, as the drop of honey was changed each time, and also the position of the colured glasses, neither of these could influence the selection by the bee. In recording the results, I marked dows successively 196 BXPERIMENTS WITH COLORED PAPERS. the order in which the bee went to the different coloured glasses. For instance, in the first journey from the nest, as recorded below, the bee lit first on the blue, which accordingly I marked 1; when the blue was removed, she flew about a little, and then lit on the white; when the white was removed, she settled on the green, and so on successively on the orange, yellow, plain, and red. I repeated the experiment a hundred times, usine two different hives—one in Kent and one in Middlesex—and spreading the observations over some time, so as to experiment with different bees, and under varied circumstances. I believe that the precautions taken placed the colors on an equal footing, and that the number of ex- periments is sufficient to give a fair average. More- over, they were spread over several days, and the daily totals did not differ much from one another. The result shows a marked preference for blue, then white, then successively yellow, red, green, and orange. The red I used was a scarlet; pink would, I believe from subsequent observations, have been more popular. I may also observe that the honey on plain glass was less visited than that on any of the colors, which was the more significant because when I was not actually observing, the colors were removed, and some drops of honey left on plain glass, which naturally gave the plain glass an advantage. Another mode of testing the result is to take the number of times in which the bee went first to each color, for instance, in a hundred visits she came to the blue first thirty-one times, and last only four; while to the plain glass she came first only five times, and last twenty-four times. It may be worth while to add that I by no means expected such a result. DR. MULLER’S OBJECTIONS. 197 A recent number of Aosmos contains a very courte- ous and complimentary notice of these observations by Dr. H. Miller, which, coming from so high an authority, is especially gratifying. Dr. Miller, however, criticizes some of the above-mentioned experiments, and remarks that, in order to make the test absolutely correct, the seven glasses should have been arranged in every possible order, and that this would give no less than 5040 combinations. I did not, however, suppose that I had attained to mathematical accuracy, or shown the exact degree of preference; all I claimed to show was the existence, and order, of preference, and I think that, as in my experiments the position of the colors was continually being changed, the result in this respect would have been substantially the same. Dr. Miller also observes that when a bee has been accustomed to come to one place for honey, she returns to it, and will tend to alight there whatever the color may be; and he shows, by the record of his own experiences, that this has a considerable influence. This is so. Of course, however, it applies mainly to bees which had been used for some time, and were accustomed to a particular spot. I was fully alive to this tendency of the bees, and neutralized it to a considerable extent, partly by frequently changing the bee, and partly by moving the glasses. While, how- ever, I admit that it is a factor which has to be taken into consideration, I do not see that it affords any argument against my conclusions. The tendency would be to weaken the effect of preference for any particular color, and to equalize the visits to all the glasses. This tendency on the part of the bees was, as my experiments show, overborne by the effect produced upon them 198 REPLY TO OBJECTIONS. by the color. So far, then, from weakening my con- clusions, the fact, so far as it goes, tends to strengthen them, because it shows that notwithstanding this tendency the blue was preferred, and the honey on colorless glass neglected. The legitimate conclusion to be drawn seems, I confess, to me, not that my mode of observation was faulty, but rather that the pre- ference of the bees for particular colors is even some- what greater than the numbers would indicate. Next, Dr. Miller objects that when disturbed from one drop of honey, the bees naturally would, and that in his experiments they actually did, fly to the next. Asa matter of fact, however, this did not happen in mine, because, to avoid this source of error, when I removed the color I gave the bee a good shake, and so made her take a flight before settling down again, According to my experience, bees differ considerably in character, or, I should rather perhaps say, in humour. Some are much shyer and more restless than others. When disturbed from the first drop of honey, some are much longer before they settle on the next than others. Much also, of course, depends on how long the bee has been experimented on. Bees, like men, settle down to their work. Moreover, it is no doubt true that, ceteris paribus, a bee in search of honey will go to the nearest sonree, ‘ But, as a matter of fact, in my hundred experiments Thad but very few cases like those quoted above from Dr. Miller. ‘his arose partly from the fact that my bees were frequently changed, and partly because, as already mentioned, I took care, in removing the color, to startle the bee enough to make her take a little flight before alighting again. Dr. Miiller says that in PREFERENCES OF BEES. 199 his experiments, when the bee did not go to the next. honey, it was when he shook her off ¢oo vigorously. I should rather say that in his observations he did not shake the bee off vigorously enough. ‘The whole objection, however, is open to the same remark as the last. The bee would have a tendency, of course, like any one else, to go to its goal by the nearest route. Hence I never supposed that the figures exactly indi- cate the degree of preference. The very fact, however, that there would naturally be a tendency on the part of the bees to save themselves labour by going to the nearest honey, makes the contrast shown by my observations all the more striking. I have never alleged that it was possible, in the case of bees (or, for that matter, of men either), to get any absolute and exact measure of preference for one color over another. It would be easy to suggest many con- siderations which would prevent this. For instance, something would probably depend on the kind of flower the bee had been in the habit of visiting, A bee which had been sucking daisies might probably behave very differently from one which had been frequenting a blue flower. So far, however, as the conclusions which I ventured to draw are concerned, I cannot see that they are in any way invalidated by the objections which Dr. Miller has urged, which, on the other hand, as it seems to me, rather tend to strengthen my argument. I may perhaps be asked, If blue is the favourite color of bees, and then pink, and if bees have had so much to do with the origin of flowers, how is it there are so few blue and pink ones? The explanation I believe to be that all blue flowers 200 THE COLORS OF FLOWERS. have descended from ancestors in which the flowers were rer, these from others in which they were yellow, while originally they were all green—or, to speak more precisely, in which the leaves immediately surrounding the stamens and pistil were green; that they have passed throueh stages of yellow, and generally if not always red, before becoming blue. It is, of course, easy to see that the possession of color is an advantage to flowers in rendering them more conspicuous, more easily seen, and less readily over- looked, by the insects which fertilize them ; but it is not quite so clear why, apart from brilliancy and visibility at a distance, one color should be more advantageous than another. These experiments how- ever, which show that insects have their preterence, throw some light on the subject. Where insects are beguiled into visits, as is the case especially with flies, they are obviously more likely to be deceived if the flowers not only, as is often the case, smell like decaying animal substance, but almost, re- semble them in appearance. Hence many fly flowers not only emit a most offensive smell, but also are dingy yellow or red, often mottled, and very closely resemble in color decaying meat. There remains another case in which allied flowers, and species, moreover, which are fertilized by very much the same insects, are yet characterized by distinct colors. We have, for instance, three nearly allied species of dead nettle—one white (Lamium album), one red (Lamiwn maculatum), and one yellow (Lamium galeobdolon or luteum). Now, if we imagine the existence in a single genus of three separate species, similar in general habit and THE COLORS OF FLOWERS. 201 appearance, and yet mutually infertile, it is easy to see that it would be an advantage to them to have their flowers differently colored. The three species of Lamium above mentioned may be growing together, and yet the bees, without difficulty or loss of time, cun distinguish the species from one another, and collect pollen and honey without confusing them together. On the other hand, if they were similarly colored, the bees could only distinguish them with comparative difficulty, involving some loss of time and probably many mistakes, I have not yet alluded especially to white flowers, They seem to stand in a somewhat special position. The general sequence, as I have suggested, is from green, through yellow and red, to blue. Flowers normally yellow seldom sport into red or blue; those normally red often sport into yellow, but seldom into blue. On the other hand, flowers of almost any color may sport into white. White is produced by the absence of color, may therefore appear at any stage, and will be stereotyped if for any reason it should prove to be an advantage.* * The genesis of the color is a large and interesting question. It may be due to various causes, and is by no means always owing to the presence of a different coloring matter. For instance, as Professor Foster has observed to me, many species of Iris occur in blue and yellow forms. The ycllow is largely, or wholly, produced by chroma- toplacts, the purple or blue to cell-sap, and if the latter is absent the yellow becomes apparent. CHAPTER X. ON THE LIMITS OF VISION OF ANIMALS. ANTS AND CoLoRs. I HAVE elsewhere * recorded a series of experiments on ants with light of different wave-lengths, in order, if possible, to determine whether ants have the power of distinguishing colors. For this purpose I utilized the dislike which ants, when in their nest, have for light. Not unnaturally, if a nest is uncovered, they think they are being attacked, and hasten to carry their young away to a darker and, as they suppose, a safer place. I satisfied myself, by hundreds of experiments, that if T exposed to light the greater part of a nest, but left any of it covered over, the young would certainly be conveyed to the dark part. In this manner I satisfied myself that the various rays of the spectrum act on them in a different manner from that in which they affect us; for instance, that ants are specially sensitive to the violet rays. But I was anxious to go beyond this, and to attempt to determine whether, as M. Paul Bert supposed, their limits of yision are the same as ours. We all know that * « Ants, Bees, and Wasps.” THE ULTRA-VIOLET RAYS. 2038 if a ray of white light is passed through a prism, it is broken up into a beautiful band of colors, known as the spectrum. ‘To our eyes this spectrum, like the rainbow, which is, in fact, a spectrum, is bounded by red at the one end and violet at the other, the edge being sharply marked at the red end, but less abruptly at the violet But a ray of light contains, besides the rays visible to our eyes, others which are called, though not with absolute correctness, heat-rays and chemical rays. These, so far from falling within the limits of our vision, extend far beyond it, the heat-rays at the red end, the chemical or ultra-violet rays at the violet end. I made a number of experiments which satisfied me that ants are sensitive to the ultra-violet rays, which lie beyond the range of our vision. I was also anxious to see how two colors identical to our eyes, but one of which transmitted and the other intercepted the ultra-violet rays, would affect the ants. Mr. Wigner was good enough to prepare for me a solution of iodine in bisulphide of carbon, and a second of indigo, carmine, and roseine mixed so as to produce the same tint. To our eyes the two were identical both in color and capacity ; but of course the ultra-violet rays were cut off by the bisulphide-of-carbon solution, while they were, at least for the most part, transmitted by the other. I placed equal amounts in flat-sided glass bottles, so as to have the same depth of each liquid. I then laid them, as in previous experiments, over a nest of Formica fusca. In twenty observations the ants went seventeen times in all under the iodine and bisulphide, twice under the solution of indigo and carmine, while once there were some under each. These observations, therefore, show that the solutions, 204 PERCEPTION OF LIGHT though apparently identical to us, appeared to the ants very different, and that, as before, they preferred to rest under the liquid which intercepted the ultra-violet rays. In two or three cases only they went under the other bottle ; but I ought to add that my observations were made in winter, when the ants were rather sluggish. I am disposed to think that in summer perhaps these exceptional cases would not have occurred, Professer Graber, however, while admitting the accuracy of my observations, has attempted to prove that the perception of the ultra-violet rays is not a case of sight in the ordinary acceptation of the words, but is due to the general sensitiveness of the skin, It has long been known that some of the lower animals which do not possess eyes are, nevertheless, sensitive to light. Hoffmeister,* in his work on earth- worms, states that, with some exceptions, they are very sensitive to light. Darwin, perhaps, experimented with a different species (for there are many different kinds); at any rate, his specimens seemed to be less keenly affected, though it one was suddenly illumi- nated it dashed “like a rabbit into its burrow.” He observed, however, that some individuals were more sensitive to light than others, and that the same indi- viduals by no mewns always acted in the same way. Moreover, if they “were employed in dragging leaves into their burrows or in eating them, and even during the shoit intervals when they rested from their work, they either did not perceive the light or were reeard- less of it.”t He observes, however, that it is only the * “Familie der Regenwiirmer,” 1845. + Darwin's “ Earthworms.” BY THE GENERAL SURFACE OF THE SKIN. 205 anterior extremity of the body, where the cerebral ganglia lie, which is affected by light, and he suggests that the light may pass through the skin and acts directly on the nervous centres. Lacaze-Duthiers, Haeckel, Engelmann, Graber, Plateau, and other naturalists have abundantly proved the sensitiveness to light of other eyeless animals. There has, indeed, long been a vague idea that blind people have some faint perception of light through the general surface of the skin. So far as I am aware there is not the slightest evidence or foundation for this belief; nor, indeed, has it been advocated by any com- petent authority. It seems @ priord improbable that an animal with complex eyes should still retain a power which would be almost entirely useless. On the other hand, it is unquestionable that light can, and often dues, act directly on the nerve termi- nations without the intermediate operation of any optical-apparatus. Some of them might, perhaps, be open to criticism. The effect of heat may not have been always sufficiently guarded against. Again, it is quite true that, as Plateau observes “Lorsque les Myriapudes chilopodes aveugles ou munis d’yeux, déposés sur le sol, s’introduisent avec empressement dans la premiere fente quw’ils rencon- trent, cet acte n’est pas déterminé par le seul besoin de fuir la lumiere, ces animaux cherchent en méme temps un milien huwide et avec lequel la plus grande partie de la surface de leur corps soit en contact direct.” * But though this is no doubt true, and though, perhaps, the moisture may be some help, still, whatever be their * Plateau, “ Rech. sur la perception de la lumiére par les Myriapodes aveugles,” Jour. del Anatomie, etc., T. xxii. 1886, 206 PERCEPTION OF LIGHT object, we can hardly doubt that the absence of light is the principal guide. Professor Graber,* in his interesting memoir on this subject confirms the observations on ants and Daphnias, in which I showed that they are sen-itive to the ultra-violet rays, by similar observations on earth-. worms, newts, etc. It is interesting, moreover, that the species examined by him showed themselves, like the ants, specially sensitive to the blue, violet, and ultra- violet rays. Graber, however, states that he differs from me inasmuch as I attribute the sensitiveness to the ultra-violet ravs exclusively to vision ;—that it is “ausschliesslich durch die Augen vermittelt.” I am not, however, of that opinion as a general expression, though I believe it to be true of ants, where the opacity of the chitine renders it unlikely that the light could be perceived except by the medium of the eyes or ocelli. Graber has shown in earthworms and newts, and Plateau t in certain Myriapods, that these animals perceive the difference between light and darkness by the general surface of the skin. But more than this. Graber seems to have demonstrated that earthworms and newts distinguish not only between light of differ- ent intensity, but also between rays of different wave- lengths, preferring red to blue or green, and green to blue. He found, moreover, as I did, that they are sensitive to the ultra-violet rays. Earthworms, of course, have no eyes; but, thinking that the light might * “Fundamental Versuche iiber die Helligkeits und Farben Em- pfindlichkeit augenloser und geblendeter Thiere,” Sitz. Kats, Akad. d. Wiss. Wien: 1883. t Journ. de VAnatomie et de la Physiologie, 1886. BY THE GENERAL SURFACE OF. THE SKIN. 207 act directly on the cephalic ganglia, Graber decapi- tated a certain number, and found that the light still acted on them in the same manner, though the differ- ences were not so marked. He also covered over the eyes of newts, and found that the same held good with them. Hence he concludes that the general surface of the skin is sensitive to light. These results are certainly curious and interesting, but even if we admit the absolute correctness of his deductions, I do not see that they are in opposition to those at which I had arrived. My main conclusions were that ants, Daphnias, etc., were able to perceive light of different wave-lengths, and that their eyes were sensitive to the ultra-violet rays much beyond our limits of vision. His observa- tions do not in any way controvert these deductions; indeed, the argument by which he endeavours to prove that the effect is due to true light, and not to warmth, presupposes that sensations which can be felt by the general surface of the skin, would be still more vividly perceived by the special organs of vision. In connection with this subject, I may add that I do not at all doubt the sensitiveness to light of eyeless animals. In experimenting on this subject, I have always found that though the blind woodlice (Platy- arthrus), which live with the ants, have no eyes, yet if part of the nest be uncovered and part kept dark, they soon find their way into the shaded part. It is, however, easy to imagine that in unpigmented animals, whose skins are more or less semi-transparent, the light might act directly on the nervous system, even thongh it could not produce anything which could be called vision. 11 908 EXPERIMENTS WITH HOODWINKED ANTS. Forel, in some recent experiments, varnished over the eyes of fifteen ants (Camponotus Iigitperdus) and pnt them with fifteen others, which were left in their normal condition, in a flat box with a glass top and divided in the middle into two halves by a cardboard division, which, however, left room enough underneath for the ants to pass freely from one half to the other. After some other experiments, in the course of which one of the varnished ants was accidentally killed, at 1 p.m. all the varnished ants and thirteen of the un- varnished were in the right half of the box, and two unvarnished in the left. He then placed over the whole box two flat bottles containing water to inter- cept heat-rays—over the right half a piece of cobalt (violet) glass; and over the left, a flat bottle containing a solution of esculine, which is quite transparent, but cuts off the ultra-violet rays. At 1.55 the result was as follows :-— Under the esculine. Under the cobalt. 5 varnished. 9 varnished. 13 normal. 2 normal. The esculine and cobalt were then transposed. At 2.3 the position was— Under the cobalt. Under the esculine. 4 vainished. 13 varnished. 8 normal, 12 normal. The esculine and cobalt were again transposed, and one normal ant was accidentally wounded and removed. At 3.8— Under the esculine. Under the cobalt. 3 varnished, 12 varnished. 11 normal, 3 normal. EXPERIMENTS WITH HOODWINKED ANTS. 209 The esculine and cobalt were once more transposed, and at 3.13 there were— Under the cobalt. Under the esculine, 8 varnished. 11 varnished. 1 normal. 13 normal. Thus the number of ants which followed the esculine and moved from one half of the box to the other at each transposition of the esculine and cobalt, was as follows :— Varnished. Normal. First change .., aa 5 ‘ li Second ,, ne ae ae ar ax «310 Third ,, a ie aaa OU Nast ate 8 Fourth ,, xe a0 ee Das oa - 20 6 40 And the number remaining under the cobalt and esculine respectively was— Under the cobalt. Under the esculine. Varnisned. Normal. Varmshed. Normal First experiment ... we OD 2 5 13 Second ,, pai me S 10 12 Third » ae won 12 So a6 aie 3 11 Fourth ,, S62 sae fad Te ae aun 32 13 28 9 30 49 These experiments clearly showed that, while the normal ants moved from side to side so as to be under the esculine and consequently protected from the ultra- violet rays, those in which the eyes had been varnished remained unaffected by the transposition of the esculine and the cobalt, showing that the difference was per- ceived, not by the general surface of the skin, but by the eyes, and that when these were covered the ants were unaffe:ted by the change. 210 CONFIRMATION OF MY EXPERIMEN'’S ON ANTS. It might be suggested that possibly the ants had been injured or stupefied by the varnishing. M. Forel accordingly, on the following day at 8 a.m., placed over one half of the box a layer of water six centimetres deep, and on the other a piece of red glass, which, while intercepting some of the light, allows almost all the heat to pass through. At 9.25 there were— Under the red glass, Under the layer of water. 8 varnished. 11 varnished. 12 normal. 2 normal. Here, it seems that the ants which could see pre- ferred the shade, even though they were rather too warm; while the hoodwinked ants went under the cool water. This indicated that the varnished ants remained sensitive to heat, though not to light. Indeed, Forel states that they were just as lively, just as sensitive to currents of air, as the normal ants.* These experiments, then, entirely confirm those I had made. “C'est une confirmation entiére,” says Forel, “des resultats de Lubbock +” and he sums up as follows :—The ants “ paraissent percevoir Vultra-violet principalement avec leurs yeux, c’est-i-dire qu’elles le voient, car lorsque leurs yeux sont vernis elles s’y montrent presque inlifférentes; elles ne réagissent alors nettement qu’a une lumiére scolaire directe ou moins forte. Les expériences ci-dessus semblent in- diquer que les sensations dermatoptiques sont plus faibles chez les fuurmis que chez les avimaux étudiés par Graber.” From these and other experiments M. Forel comes * Loe. cit., p. 167. ¢ Ibid., p. 174. EXPERIMENTS WITH DAPHNIAS. 211 to the same conclusion as I did, that the ants perceive the ultra-violet rays with their eyes, and not as suggested by Graber, by the skin generally. It is very gratifying that my experiments and conclusions should thus be entirely confirmed by an observer so careful and so experienced as M. Forel. Fig. 118.—Dophma pulex. a, Antenne; b, brain; ¢ eyes h, heart; m, muscle of eye; 7m, nerve of eye; 0, ovary; ol, olfactory organ; s, stomach ; y, three eggs deposited in the space between the back and the shell. EXPERIMENTS WITH DAPHNIAS. The late M. Paul Bert made some very interesting experiments on a small fresh-water crustacean belong- 212 DAPHNIAS AND COLORS. ing to the genus Daphnia (Fig. 118), from which he concludes that they perceive all the colors known to us, being, however, especially sensitive to the yellow and green, and that their limits of vision are the same as ours, Nay, he even goes further than tls, and feels justi- fied in concluding, from the experience of tivo species —Man and Daphnia—that the limits of vision would be the same in all cases. His words are— 1. “Tous les animaux voicnt les rayons spectraux que nous voyous.” 2. “Ils ne veient aucun de ceux que nous ne voyons pas.” 3. “ Dans létendue de Ja région visible, les différences entre les youvoirs éclairants des différents rayons colorés sont les mémes pour eux et pour nous.” He also adds, “Puisque les limites de visibilité semblent étre les mémes pour les animaux et pour nous, ne trouvons-nous pas 1& une raison de plugs pour supposer que le réle des milieux de l’ceil est tout a fait secondaire, et que la visibilité tient a limpression- nabilité de Pappareil nerveux lui-méme ?” These generalizations would seem to rest on a very narrow foundation. I have already attempted to show that the conclusion does not appear to hold good in the case of ants; and I determined, therefore, to make some experiments myself on Daphnias, the results of which are here embodied.* Professor Dewar was kind enough to arrange for me, at the Royal Institution, a spectrum, which, by means of a mirror, was thrown on to the floor. I then placed some * These observations were published in the Journal of the Linnean Suciety for 1881. PREFERENCE FOR YELLOWISH GREEN. 213 Daphnias in a shallow wooden trough fourteen inches by four inches, and divided by cross partitions of glass into divisions, so that I could isolate the parts illumi- nited by the different coloured rays. The two ends of the trough extended somewhat beyond the visible spectrum. I then placed fifty specimens of Daphnia pulex in the trough, removing the glass partitions so that they could circulate freely from one end of the trough to the other. Then, after scattering them equally through the water, I exposed them to the light for ten minutes, after which I inserted the glass partitions, and then counted the Daphnias in each division. The results were as follows :— Noumper or Dapunias. In the In the Beyond Beyond redand_ greenish yellow In the In the the the red. yellow. and green, blue. violet. violet, Obs. 1 0 20 28 2 0 0 55 02 1 21 25 3 0 0 8 2 21 24 3 0 0 go 1 19 29 ] 0 0 ee 0 20 27 3 0 0 4 101 183 12 0 0 I may add that the blue and violet divisions were naturally longer than the red and green. May 25.—Tried again the same arrangement, but separating the yellow, and giving the Daphnias the choice between red, yellow, green, blue, violet, and dark :-— Dark, Violet. Blue Green. Yellow. Red. Exp. 1 ~ 0 8 39 5 3 oD . 0 1 2 37 7 3 we 8 . 0 0 4 a1 10 5 » & . 0 1 5 30 8 6 » Oo . 0 1 4 83 6 6 ) 3 18 170 36 23 214 EXPERIMENTS. Of course, it must be remembered that the yellow band is much narrower than the green. J reckoned as yellow a width of three-quarters of an inch, and the width of the green two inches. Again— Dark. Violet. Blue. Green. Yellow. Red. Exp. 1 » 0 0 4 30 6 10 ey 2 a 0) 1 3 25 8 13 » 3 . 0 0 2 24 9 15 a of - il 0 3 25 8 13 » O . 0 1 2 24 7 16 1 2 14 128 38 67 Adding them to- — _— — — — _ gether, we get 1 5 382 298 74 90 M. Paul Bert observes (loc. czt.) that in his experiments the Daphnias followed exactly the brilliance of the light. It will be observed, however, that in my expe- riments this was not the case, as there were more Daphnias in proportion, as well as absolutely, in the green, although the yellow is the brightest portion of the spectrum. In fact, they follow the light up tea certain brightness; but, as will be seen presently, they do not like direct sunshine. I then arranged the trough so that the yellow fell in the middle of one of the divisions. The result was— Nemper oF Daruntas. Upper edge. Ultra-red of red, Greenish and yellow, and blue and Ultra- lower red. lower green. blue. Violet. violet. Hxpel Ga aia 18 38 4 0 0 5 ee ata 36 5 0 0 ds tit we 8 39 3 0 0 25 113 12 0 0 May 18.—In order to test the limits of vision at the LIMITS OF VISION OF DAPHNIAS, 215 red end of the spectrum, I used the same arrangement as before, placing the trough so that the extreme division was in the ultra-red, and the second in the red. I then placed sixty Daphnias in the ultra-red. After five minutes’ exposure, I counted them. There were in the—. Red. Ultra-red. Exp.1 ... eee oe = vee 5 a Dei rece? hte’. oem *DB ~ 4 T now gave them four divisions to select from—dark, red, ultra-red, and dark again. The numbers were— Dark. Red. Ultra-red Dark. Exp.1 ... ie owe «=S 47 6 2 dD Tete ake can) te) 41 7 3 I then shut them off from all the colors excepting red, giving them only the option between red and ultra-red :— Red. Ulira-red. Exp.1 aw. one we «= 46 eee oes 4 We Dee, Meare ai YAES Coe. cade 8 Bde, caw. wie (ER. Sa Gea 36 I then left them access to a division on the other side of the red, whieh, however, I darkened by interposing a piece of wood. This enabled me better to compare the ultra-red rays with a really dark space :— Dark. Red. Ultra-red. Expl. ote a ae 43 3 49 2 ee was wea oe «38 45 2 These observations appear to indicate that their limits of vision at the red end of the spectrum coincide approximately with ours. I then proceeded to examine their behaviour with reference to the other end of the spectrum. In the first place, I shut them off from all the rays 216 PERCEPTION OF ULTRA-VIOLET RAYS except the blue, violet, and ultra-violet. was as follows :— Nompber or Dapuntas. Ultra-violet. Vivlet. Blue. Exp lo... ow. ou. 1 9 38 vy Bake eos we «4 6 38 53 8! wee aoe sieve 2 46 5 17 122 The result 9 a x @ | row This shows that they greatly prefer blue and violet to darkness or ultra-violet. I afterwards gave them only the option of ultra-violet, violet, and darkness :— Ultra-violet. Violet. Exp.1 a... a's ues oe 8 48 ag AEE) ohm oa nee se 6 48 5 3 wwe 12 47 a nr we 1S 42 » 9 om tse fe oon 4 53 45 238 Dark, S it ! a | wo 09 et OD He They preferred the violet; but there were many more in the ultra-violet than in the dark. I then tried ultra-violet and dark. The width of the violet was two inches; and I divided the ultra-violet portion again into divisions each of two inches, which we may call ultra-violet, further ultra-violet, and still further ultra-violet. The results were— NcmsBer ofr Daruntias. Still further Further ultra-violet. ultra-violet. Ultra-violet. Exp.1 ... 0 6 52 wi 2 aes 0 5 52 3 - 5 0 6 50 i 0 4 53 ” 5 tae sae 0 4 54 Raiea eee Da: 5 k. | no eo wm oe BO —_ > PERCEPTION OF ULTRA-VIOLET RAYS. 217 Tn this case the preference for ultra-violet over dark was very marked. May 18.—I again tried them with the ultra-violet rays, using three divisions—namely, further ultra-violet, ultra-violet, and dark. The numbers were as follows, viz. under the— Further altra-violet. Ultra-violet. Dark, Expl ow. wow 6 50 4 3 Dome. x a 8 55 2 9 105 6 To my eye there was no perceptible difference be- tween the further ultra-violet and the ultra-violet portion; but slightly undiffused light reached the two extreme divisions. It may be asked why the still further ultra-violet division should have been entirely deserted, while in each case two or three Daphnias were in the darkened one. This, I doubt not, was due to the fact that, the darkened division being next to the ultra- violet, one or two in each case straggled into it. Not satisfied with this, I tried another test. There are some liquids which, though transparent to the rays we see, are quite opaque to the ultra-violet rays. Bisulphide of carbon, for instance, is quite colourless and transparent: it looks just like water, but it entirely cuts off the ultra-violet rays. If, then, we place the trough containing Daphnias, as I had previously done my nest of ants, in the ultra-violet part of the spectrum, and then place over one half of it a flat bottle contain- ing water, and over the other half a similar bottle con- taining bisulphide of carbon, both halves will seem equally dark to us, but the ultra-violet rays reach one half of the vessel, while they are cut off from the other. 218 PERCEPTION OF ULTRA-VIOLET RAYS. To our eyes both, as I say, are equally dark, and so they would be to the Daphnias if their limits of vision were the same as ours. As a matter of fact, however, the Daphnias all collected in the part of the trough under the water, and avoided that under the bisulphide of car- bon, showing that this, therefore, was to them darker than the other. I varied the experiments in several ways, but always with similar results. Bichromate of potash is also impervious to the ultra-violet rays, and had the same effect. Not satisfied with this, I tried to test it in another way. I took acell, in which I placed a layer of five-per- cent. solution of chromate of potash less than an eighth of an inch in depth, and which, though almost colourless to our eyes, completely cut off the ultra-violet rays. I then turned my trough at right angles, so that I could cover one side of the ultra-violet portion of the spectrum with the chromate and leave the other exposed. The numbers were as follows :— Side of the ultra- violet covered with Side chromate of potash. uncovered. Dark. Exp.1 ou. ine ve OS ase 55 ae 0 T now covered up the other side. Exp.2 ..0 00 ee Bone OER ee 0 Again covered up the same side as at first. Pepe Gao ca. aye ae SS ee UO Again covered up the other side. iid ate ae a 2 oe SE ae May 19.—I again tried the same arrangement, re- ducing the chromate of potash to a mere film, which, OBJECTIONS OF M. MEREJKOWSKY. 219 however, still cut off the ultra-violet rays. Ithen placed it, as before, over one half of the ultra-violet portion of the spectrum ; and over the other half I placed a similar cell containing water. Between each experiment I reversed the position of the two cells. The numbers were— Under the film of Under the chromate of putash. water. Exp.1 4... oa sc 8 sie a 52 gi Bae te Aes eee 88 ie Bak) ake Gee’ “fe gen. G50 jg EE oes deat aes aes 53 Evidently, then, even a film of chromate of potash exercises a very considerable influence; and, indeed, I doubt not that, if a longer time had been allowed, the difference would have been even greater. It seems clear, therefore, that a five-per cent. solution of chromate of potash only one-eighth of an inch in thickness, which cuts off the ultra-violet rays, though absolutely transparent to our eyes, is by no means so to the Daphnias. These observations seem to prove, though I differ with great reluctance from so eminent an authority as M. Paul Bert, that the limits of vision of Dapbnias do not, at the violet end of the spectrum, coincide with ours, but that the Daphoia, like the ant, is affected by the ultra-violet rays. Since these observations were published, M. Merej- kowski has experimented on the subject, and come to the conclusion that the Daphnias are attracted wherever there is most light, that they are conscious only of the intensity of the light, and that they have no power of distinguishing colors. It is no doubt true thatin ordinary diffused daylight the Daphnias generally oo DAPHNIAS SUPPOSED TO PERCEIVE congresate wherever the light is strongest. Their eyes are, however, so delicate that one would naturally expect, & priori, that there would be a limit to this; and, in tact, direct sunshine is somewhat too strong for their comfort. For instance, I took a poreclain trough, seven and a half inches long, two and a halt broad, and one deep, and put in it some water containing fifty Daphnias. One half I exposed to direct sunlight, and the other I shaded, connting the Daphnias from time to time, and trans- posing the exposed and shaded halves. The numbers were as follows:— In the sun. In the shado. At 1040 a.m. i ss 46 59, LO. 555 8 42 ve A yy E 43 gx gg © 43 7 OU! 5 4 16 ee ee wee di a 7 BO «5 sit sie ie ob 16 ger BNO ys fie 5 415 n £0 4 aah oP mie, wee 43 » £380 ,, eae wae ve ot wee 416 n8 417 This seems clearly to show that they avoid the full sunlight. I believe, then, that in some of my previous experi- ments the yelluw light was too brilliant for them; and the following experiments seem to show that, when sufficicntly diffused, they prefer yellow to white light. M. Merejkowsky, however, denics to the erustacea any sense of color whatever. His experiments were made with larvie of Balunus and with a warine eope- pod, Dias longiremis. ‘These, if I understand him correctly, have given ilentical results. He considers BRIGHTNESS, BUT NOT COLOR. 22't that they perceive all the luminous rays, and can dis- tinguish very slight differences of intensity; but that they do not distinguish between different colors. He sums up his observations as follows :—- “Tl résulte de ces expériences que ce qui agit sur les Crustacés, ce n’est point la qualité de la lumieére, c’e-t exclusivement sa quantité. Antrement dit, les Crus- tacés inférieurs ont Ja perception de tonte onde Zumz- neuse et de toutes les différences, méme trés légéres, dans son intensité; mais ils ne sont point capabies de dis- tinguer la nature des ondes, de différentes couleurs. Ils distinguent tres bien Vintensité des vibrations éthérées, leur amplitude, mais point leur nombre. II y a done, dans le mode de perception de la lumieére, une grande différence entre les Crustacés inférieurs et 1 Homme, et méme entre eux et les Fourmis; tandis que nous voyons les différentes couleurs et leurs différentes intensités, les Crustacés inférieurs ne voient qu’une seule couleur dans ses différentes variations d’intensité. Nous percevons des couleurs comme couleurs; ils ne les pergoient que comme lumiére.” * It is by no means easy to decide such a question absolutely ; but the subject is of much interest, and accordingly I made some further experiments, as it did not seem to me that those of M. Merejkowsky bore out the conclusion he has deduced from them. Professor Dewar most kindly arranged the apparatus for me again, He prepared a normal diffraction-spec- trum, produced by a Rutherfurd grating with 17,000 lines to the inch; the spectrum of the first order was thrown on the trough. In this case the distribution of * M. C. Merejkowsky, “ Les Crustacés inférieurs distinguent-ils les couleurs?” 223 FURTHER EXPERIMENTS. luminous intensity has been shown to be uniform on each side of the line having the mean wave-length, «.e. a little above the line D in the yellowish green of the spectrum. I then took a long shallow trough in which were a number of Daphnias, and placed it so that the centre of the trough was at the brightest part of the spectrum, a little, however, if anything, towards the green end. After scattering the Daphnias equably - I left them for five minutes, and then put a piece of blackened cardboard over the brightest part. After five minutes more, there were at the green end, 410; in the dark, 14; at the red end, 76. Here the two ends of the trough were equally illuminated; but the preference for the green over the red side was very marked, I then took five porcelain vessels, seven and a half inches long, two and a half broad, and one deep, and in each I put water containing fifty Daphnias. One half of the water I left uncovered; the other half I covered respectively with an opaque porcelain plate, a solution of aurine (bright yellow), of chlorate of copper (bright green), a piece of red glass, and a piece of blue glass. Every half-hour I counted the Daphnias in each half of every vessel, and then transposed the coverings, so that the half which had been covered was left exposed, and vice versi. I also changed the Daph- nias from time to time. Here, then, in each case the Daphnias had a choice between two kinds of light. It seemed to me that this would be a crucial test, because in every case the colored media act by cutting of certain rays. Thus the aurine owes its yellow color to the fact that it cuts FURTHER EXPERIMENTS. 223 off the violet and blue rays. The light beneath it con- tains no more yellow tays than elsewhere; but those rays produce the impression of yellow, because the yellow is not neutralized by the violet and blue. In each case, therefore, there was less light in the covered than in the uncovered part. After every five experiments I added up the number of the Daphnias; and the following table gives twenty such totals, each containing the result of five observa- - tions, making in all one hundred, My reason for adding one vessel in which one half had an opaque cover was to meet the objection that possibly the light might have been too strong for the Daphnias; so that when they went under the sheltered part they did so, not for color, but for shade. I was not very sanguine as to the result of this arrangement, because I had expected that the preference of the Daphnias for light would overcome their attachment to yellow. The numbers were as in the following table (p. 224). The result was very marked. The first two columns show the usual preference for light. If the covered half had been quite dark, no doubt the difference in numbers would have been greater; but a good deal of light found its way into the covered half. Still the result clearly shows that the Daphnias preferred the lighter half. The numbers were 2048 in the dark to 2952 in the light; and it will be seen that the preference for the light was shown, though in different degrees, in almost every series. The result in the blue gives, I think, no evidence as to color-sense. The numbers were respectively 2046 against 2954, and were therefore practically the same EVIDENCE THAT DAPHNIAS. 224 FeGS oF0G | FEES oF | BLOe S26L | FO6I 9608 BLS SFOS fT TRIO, SL¥FL LEOL OFeL OUET COFL TOOT 0Z0T OSFI GOOT TGS StL LET col StL eG 601 EST LG ae Gee ap eoL LIL oe TIT FU aT ISL GO) PRE) Se agir GCL C6 OoE 08 99 FOL 006. « OG |e = ee StL COL Gol Tél GL OL TSE 9 tee eee pw TFL 90 Tel 96 GOT TOL OT 6G ee gr StL col Skt GUL Olt LIT IGl gg | ee, * O@L ut eI LET SIL 1G ot ht deo ISL Go IIL 6ST FI ol ol TFL gor jes Step CLL gh FFL 90T OOT 98 TOL Sel GIT CLT 16 SIT Ler FIT LG eel GLUT er [ot TL 4d ISFL GUL | Feel OFIT L36 F838 9I19T CECT LSIT TFL 90T Lat 9IT aI get Tal 9% ae ECT 1c 18 SIT aL NBL TOL 8 OLY cI GS ECL O6L 89 est VBE F6L fen 5 CFL Sot a NBL cll Cel Sul ab mest Gees Oct #4 OLT 08 19 ESI eal LIL sé SOL as FOL 9b Sh GLE OFL Olt fot SEs OFT OIL 18 99 9L FLL StL Ginko" de OT OOT CIL ra 6¢ tt cnt der. [os oe 9 & cel cil FOL LS Olt OTL COL TFL sain a set ra 6s 99 GL CLT TOL got [oo Gg -q9q ‘paiaaoouQ «= ‘ani g | ‘palasoonp, ‘“Uoatyy | potwwacrmgQ = "pert *pordsoormp, «*ALOT]aAX | ‘pataaoouy, ‘andedg aq} ut aq} uy ogy Uy ata Uy aq4 Uy G “> "@ % I SVINHdVd AO YAANON PERCEIVE DIFFERENCES IN COLOR. 225 as in the preceding set. Since, however, a certain quantity of light was transmitted through the blue, the result may indicate a want of sensitiveness to the blue rays. In the red the numbers were 1928 as against 3072. As regards the yellow, the results were very different, the numbers being, under the yellow, 3096; in the uncovered part, 1904. Here, therefure, we see a very distinct preference, all the more remarkable because the amount of light was really less than in the un- covered part. In the green the numbers were murh more equal, namely, 2406 against 2594. Here also the love fur green neutralized the preference for light. I do not, however, wish for the moment to draw any conclusion from these last figures, though I give them for what they are worth. The coloured medium was, I believe, somewhat too opaque. With a more transparent green, as will be seen subsequently, the result would have been very different. At any rate, the above observations seemed to show a marked preference for yellow. Still, [thought it might be objected that, though the Daphnias obviously pre- ferred the uncovered to the shaded half of the vessel, and the yellow to the uncovered half of the vessel, perhaps in the former the uncovered water was rather too bright, and in the latter the shaded part was rather too dark, and that after ali the yellow was chosen, not because it was yellow, but because it hit off the happy medium of intensity. The suggestion is very improb- able, because the observations were made on several successive, and very different, days, and at very different hours. I also thought that the green was 226 EVIDENCE THAT DAPHNIAS perhaps too dark; I took, therefore, a lighter tint, and rearranged my little apparatus as follows :— I placed (March 26) fifty Daphnias in a trough (1), covering over one half of it with a pale green, and another fifty in a trough (2) half of which was covered with yellow (aurine), On one side was a similar trongh (3), one end of which was shaded by a porcelain plate ; and on the other side a fourth trongh (4), one end of which had a little, though but little, extra light thrown on it by means of a mirror. As before, I counted the Daphnias from time to time, and turned the troughs round, All four were in a light room, but not actually in direct sunshine. Thus, then, in one trough I had half the water in somewhat green light; in the second trough, half the water in yellow light; in the third, one half was exposed and the other somewhat darkened ; while the fourth, on the contrary, gave me a contrast with somewhat more vivid light. If, then, the Daphnias went under the green and yellow glass, not on account of the color, but for the sake of shade, then in trough 3 a majority of them would have gone under the porcelain plate. On the other hand, if the porcelain plate darkened the water too much, and yet the open water was rather too light for the Daphnias, then in the fourth trough they would, of course, have avoided the illuminated half. The results show that the third trough was unnecessary, still, I may as well give the figures; the fourth proves that the Daphnias preferred a light somewhat brighter than the ordinary diffused light of the room. Of course, it does not follow that the effect of color is the same as with us. PERCEIVE DIFFERENCES OF COLOR. 227 Trovusn 1. TRouGH 2. Trovuen 3. Trovucu 4, G White | Yellow | White | Exposed |Darkenea] Hlumi- | Unillu- Veht. Veht. light. light. half, halt, wed ae March 27. TZ. © gay 35 15 83 17 35 15 28 22 12.25. ou u2 18 28 29 37 13 26 14 12.50 ... 27 23 33 17 36 14 25 25 1.40... 33 17 33 17 388 12 30 20 2.5 26 24 42 8 35 15 26 2b 153 97 169 81 181 69 145 105 2.25 5. 36 14 36 |. 14 26 24 35 15 SAb xs 41 9 13 32 24 26 23 27 B20. 6s 3L 19 34 16 36 14 85 15 5.15 .. 35 15 25 25 31 19 28 22 5.40 .. 30 29 35 15 32 18 27 23 1 77 148 102 149 101 148 102 March 28. TO ose | BS 17 34 16 35 15 30 20 7.50 v0 32 18 37 13 UT 23 32 18 “Bal os 34 16 33 17 29 21 30 20 8.85... 3 14 35 15 26 2+ 33 17 955°" svc] 26 24 27 23 33 17. 35 15 161 | 89 | 166 | 84 | 150 | 100 | 360 | 90 March 29, S10 sxc) 36 20 25 25 29 21 82 18 925 ...| 30 20 27 23 35 15 30 20 9.40 ... 19 31 2) 25 29 21 29 21 9.55 ... 20 30 ot 16 37 13 29 21 10.20 ...| 30 14 34 16 20 30 26 24 135 5 145 105 150 100 146 104 Total ... | 622 378 628 372 630 370 599 401 It may be said that perhaps in the previous experiments the red and blue were too dark. I therefore took a very pale solution, and counted the number twenty times for the red and ten for the blue, 228 EVIDENCE THAT DAPHNIAS placing the yellow in another trough, as before, for comparison. The preference for the yellow was as marked as ever. In the experiments with the red and yellow the numbers were respectively Trouca 1. TRroucH 2. Under the In the Under the In the yellow. uncovered half. red. uncovered half. 670 330 498 502 When, therefore, the red solution was sufficiently light, the Daphnias were indifferent to it. In the experiments with livht blue the numbers were— TROUGH 1. TroucnH 2. TrRovGH 3. eS oF a Under In the Under In the Under In the the uncovered the uncovered the porcelean uncovered yellow. hall. blue. half. plate. half. 687 313 286 Tit 336 664 One other possible objertion also suge«sted itself to me. I thought it might be said that the Daphnias went under the yellow and the green not on account of any preference for yellow or green light, but on account of the shelter afforded by the covering. To test. this, [ covered one half of a trough over with transparent glass, leaving the other uncovered; but after twenty observations I found the number of Daphnias in each half to be practically identical. The mere fact of the covering, therefore, made no difference. In this way I was able to test the preference of the Daplnias for various colours, and the result made it abundantly clear that Daphnias have the power cf distinguishing between light of different wave-leneths, and that they prefer the light which we call yellow and green. Whether it actually appears to them as it dies to us is, of course, PERCEIVE DIFFERENCES OF COLOR. 229 another and a more difficult question—one, moreover, not yet solved even for the higher animals. Nor would I necessarily claim for them any esthetic sense of beauty; it must be remembered that they feed on minute alg and other minute vegetables, the prevalent colors of which are yellow, yellowish green, and green. There is, therefore, nothing improbable, a prior, but rather the reverse, in their preference for these colors. It will be observed that though in these vessels the Daphnias made their preference unmistakable, there were always a certain number in the least popular part. This is natural, because, as the position of the light half was reversed every observation, the Daphnias had to swim across the vessel, and some naturally did not find their way to the favourite part. Then, again, in any considerable numbers of Daphnias some are changing, or have recently changed, their skin, and are, therefore, more or less inactive. Moreover, in pure water the desire for food must often overpower any preference for one colour over another. To such causes as these we must, I think, attribute the presence of so many Daphnias in the first vessel at the opaque end, and in the second in the uncovered part. Still, it was of course not impossible that the pre- sence, for instance, of a certain number under the red and blue was due to a difference of taste; that, though the majority preferred yellow, there might be some preferring blue or red. To test this I tried the follow- ing experiment. I placed, as before, fifty Dapbnias in three of the vessels, covering one half of one with the yellow, of a second with blue, and the third with red. I then from time to time, at intervals of not less than half an hour, removed these which were in the un- 230 EVIDENCE THAT DAPHNIAS covered part and replaced them with an equal number of fresh ones. If, then, some Daphnias preferred red or blue, I ought thus to eliminate the others, and gradually to get together fifty agreeing in this taste. This, how- ever, was not the case. In the first experiment, an hour after the Daphnias were placed in the vessels there were, out of 50, 41 under the yellow, 16 under the red, and 15 under the blue, the remaining 9, 34, and 35 respectively being in the uncovered portions. ‘These, then, I removed and replaced by others. After doing this five times, and thus adding 80 in the yellow division, 187 in the red, and 209 in the blue, the numbers were 37 under the yellow, 15 under the red, and 6 under the blue. In the second experiment, the nambers alter the first hour were 32 under the yellow, 10 under the red, and 11 under the blue. Aft r five observations, during which 86 were added to the yellow division, 188 to the red, and 180 to the blue, the numbers were—under the yellow, 35; red, 11; blue, 15. In the third experiment, the numbers after half an hour were 40 under the yellow, 14 under the red, and 8 under the blue. After five observations, during which 73 were added to the yellow, 186 to the red, and 206 to the blue, there were—under the yellow, 43; under the red, 15; and under the blue, 7. In the fowth experiment, the numbers after half an hour were 38 under the yellow, 15 under the red, and 14 under the blue. After six observations, during which 89 were added to the yellow, 106 to the red, and 176 to the blue, the numbers were—under the yellow, 30; under the red, 19; and under the blue, 10. In the fifth experiment, the numbers after half an hour were 40 under the yellow, 14 under the red, and PERCEIVE DIFFERENCES OF COLOR. 231 13 under the blue. After seven observations, during which 86 were added to the yellow, 263 to the red, and 272 to the blue, the numbers were—under the yellow, 88; under the red, 13; and under the blue, 15. Yellow. Red. Blue. First observation. At the beginning ... oe 41 16 15 >» end ee se 80 15 6 Second observation. At the beginning... w= 82 10 ll » end aes wae BD 11 15 Third observation. At the beginning... ve» 40 14 8 » end see wae 43 15 7 Fourth observation. At the beginning... «.. 88 15 14 » end de ve 80 19 10 Fifth observation. At the beginning... exe 40 14 13 » end mie ee 88 ik ag I conclude, then, that the presence of some of the Daphnias in the red, blue, and violet is more or less due to the causes above indicated, and not to any individual preference. for those colors. My experiments, I think, show that, while the Daph- nias prefer light to darkness, there is a certain maxi- mum of brilliancy beyond which the light becomes inconveniently bright to them, and that they can distinguish between light of different wave-lengths. I suppose it would be impossible to prove that they. actually perceive colours; but to suggest that the rays of various wave-lengths produce on their eyes a different impression from that of color, is to propose an entirely novel hypothesis. At any rate, 1 think I have shown that they do distinguish between rays of different wave-lengths, and prefer those which to our eyes appear green and yellow. 12 CHAPTER XI. ON RECOGNITION AMONG ANTS. Durtne the many years that I have had ants under observation, I have never on any occasion seen any- thing like a quarrel between any two ants belonging to the same community. This is certainly very much to their eredit. The experience of Huber, Forel, McCook, and others who have wa‘ched ants, is, moreover, the same as mine, J have also shown* that they recognize one another even after a separation of a year and nine months. On the other hand, every community of ants is hostile to every other. I am not now speaking of ants belong- ing to diff.rent kinds, but of ants belonging to the same species. Some species, indeed, are more intolerant of strangers than others; but, as regards most species of ants, it may be said that if an individnal be taken from its own nest and introduced into another, even thongh belonging to the same species, it will be at once attacked and driven, or rather drageed, out. These facts, then, show that the ants of a community all recognize one another. But when we consider the immense number of ants in a nest, amounting in some cases to over 500,000, this is indeed a wonderful fact, * See “ Ants, Bees, and Wasps.” EXPERIMENTS WITH INTOXICATED ANTS. 233 Tt may be remembered that my nests have enabled me to keep ants under observation for long periods, and that I have thus identified workers of Lasius niger and Formica fusca which were at least seven years old, but my oldest ants have been two queens of Formica fusca, which I took in a nest in December, 1874. They must then have been nine months old, and of course may have been more. One of these queens, after ailing for some days, died on July 30, 1887. She mu-t then have been more than thirteen years old. I was at first afraid that the other one might be affected by the death of her companion. She is, however, still alive (May, 1888), and, though a little stiff in the jeints, as far as I can judge, in ber usual health. Still, there are only a few queens in a nest, and no doubt the majority of the workers, at least in the summer and when the community is most active, are very young, which adds greatly to the difficulty of sup- posing that they are personally known to one another. It has been suggested that each nest has, perhaps. a special signal or pass-word. To test this I took, as I have already mentioned in my book on “ Ants, Bees, and Wasps,” a number of ants, half from one nest and half from another, and made them very drunk. so as to be thoroughly insensible. Ithen marked them with spots of different colours, so as to distinguish the two lots, and put them on a table near where some ants helonging to the nest from which one half of them had been taken, were feeding on some honey. The table was surrounded by a moat containing water to prevent the ants from wandering away. The sober ants were rather puzzled; but,after examining the intoxicated individuals, they picked up the strangers and threw them into the 234 EVIDENCE AGAINST THE ditch, while they carried their own friends into the nest, where no doubt they slept off the effects of the spirits. This experiment seemed to show that the recognition was not effected by means of any sign ; but I thought the suggestion might be tested in another way. I made, therefore, the following experiment. I took a few specimens of Formica fusca from two different nests, which I will call A and B, and placed them together. At first they were rather shy; but after a while they fraternized. Atter they had lived amicably together for three months, I put two of these ants from nest A into nest B; but they were soon attacked vigorously and driven out of the nest. I thought it desirable to repeat and extend this test. Accordingly, on June 16 I put three specimens of F. fusca from my nest No. 81 with the same number from nest No. 71. Then on September 19, one of the six having died in the interval, I put the two from nest 81 into nest 71, and the three from nest 71 into 81. They were all attacked, though not very quickly or vigorously, but eventually all five were expelled. Again, on September 25 I teok three ants from each of these nests and put the six together. hen on March 19 following (one having ated), I put the two from 71 iuto 81, and the three from 81 into 71. They were all attacked, so that they were evidently recovnized as strangers ; but it seemed to me that the attack was less vigorous, and I could not be sure that they were either killed or driven out. In the course of the weck three or four dead ants were brought out of each of the nests; but I conld not feel certain that they were those experimented with, POSSESSION OF A SIGN OR PASSWORD. 235 Lastly, on April 9 I again put twelve ants, six from each of these nests, together, and kept them so till October. I then took four of those from 71, put three into 81 and the fourth into 71. T also took four of those from 81, and put three into 71, and the fourth back into 81 among her old friends. The two ants thus restored respectively to their old nests were as usual recognized as friends and left quite unmolested. As regards the other six, the results were as follows. The ants were introduced into the nests at 8.15 a.m. Nest 71. Nest 81. 8.45. One was being attacked, One was being attacked. 9.15. None were ay ” ” 9.45. Two were 5 a » 10.15. One was ” 2 ” 10.45. None were of Py ” 12.30. Two were ae . 9 1.30. Two were 3 None were, 2.30. Que was rey ” ”? I do not give these results as by any means proving that ants do not recognize their friends by means of smell, They do seem, however, at any rate, to show that not even six months of close companionship under pte- cisely similar conditions will so far assimilate the odour as tolead to confusion. If the recognition is due in any degree to this cause, the odour is therefore probably an hereditary characteristic. In the interesting memoir already cited, Forel says,* “Lubbock (doe. cit.) a cru démontrer que les fourmwis enlevées de leur nid & état de nymphe et écloses hors de chez elles étaient néanmoins reconnues par leurs *. Recueil Zool. Suisse, 1887. 236 EXPERIMENTS WITH ANTS REMOVED FROM THE compagnes lorsqu’on les leur rendait. Dans mes Fourmis de la Suisse, javais cru démontrer le coutraire. Voici une expérience que j'ai faite ces jours-ci: Le 7 aotit, je donne des nymphes de Formica pratensis pres déclore 4 quelques Formica sanguinea dans une boite. Le 9 aott quelques-unes éclosent. Le 11 aott, au matin, je prends lune de jeunes pratensis dgée de deux ou trois jours seulement et je la porte & sa fourmiliére natale dont elle était sortie comme nymphe seulement 4 jours auparavant. Elle y est fort mal regue. Ses nourrices d’il y a 4 jours l’empoignent qui par la téte, qui par le thorax, qui par les pattes en recourbant leur abdomen d’un air menacant. Deux d’entre elles la tinrent longtemps en sens inverse chacune par une patte en Vécartelant. Enfn cependant on finit par la tolérer, comme on le fait aussi pour de si jeunes fourmis (encore blane jaunatre) provenant de fourmiliéres dif- féreutes. J’attends encore deux jours pour laisser durcir un peu mes nouvelles écloses, Puis j’en reporte deux sur leur nid. Elles sont violemment attaquées. L’une d’elles est inondée de venin, tiraillée et tuée. L’autre est longtemps tiraillée et mordue, mais finalement laissée tranquille (tulérée?). On m’objectera Vodeur des sanguinea qui avait vécu 4 jours avec la premiére et 6 jours avec les deux dernieres, A cela je répondrai simplement par l’expérience de la page 278 a 282 de mes Fourmis de la Suisse, ou des F. pratensis adultes séparées depuis deux mois de leurs compagnes par une alliance forvée avec des L’. sanguinea, alliance que j’avais provoyuée, reconnurent immédliatement leurs anciennes compagnes et s’allierent presque sans dispute avec elles. Je maintiens done mon opinion : les fourmis apprennent a se connaitre petit a petit & partir de leur éclosion. NEST AS PUP4 AND SUBSEQUENTLY RESTORED. 237 Je crois du reste que c’est au moyen de perceptions olfact:ves de contact.” * I have, however, repeated my previous observations, with the same results. At the beginning of August I brought in a nest of Lasius niger containing a large number of pupee. Some of these I placed by themselves, i in charge of three ants belonging to the same species, but taken from a nest I have had under observation for rather more than ten years. On August 28 I tovk twelve of the young ants, which in the mean time had emerged from the sepa- rated pup, selecting some which had almost acquired their full colour. Four of them I placed in their old nest, and four in that from which their nurses were taken. At 4.30 in their own nest none were attacked. a ay 3 nurses’ nest one was attacked. » 95.0 os own nest none were attacked. ba cag 5 nurses’ nest all four were attacked. » 8.0 35 own nest none were attacked. se 8 5 nurses’ nest three were attacked. The next day I took six more and marked them with a spot of paint as usual, and at 7.30 replaced them in their own nest. At 8.0 I found 5 quite at home; the others I could not see, but none were attacked. ” 8.30 ” 5 4” id 3” ” ” 9.0 ” 3 ” y ” ” " 10.0 9 4 ” ” ” Er ” Bt 0 ” 5 n » ” ” ” 12.0 3 3 > ” - ” ” 1.0 39 3 ” ” ” ” ” 4.0 0 4 ” ” ” ”? ” 7.0 ” 1 n ” ” ” ” 9.0 ” 2 ” ” ” ” * «Forel. Exp. et Rem. crit. sur les Sensations des Insectes,” Recueil Zool. Suisse., 1887. 238 EXPERIMENTS WITH DROWNED ANTS. The next morning I could only see two, but none were being attacked, and there were no dead ones. It is probable that the paint had been cleaned off the others, but it was not easy to find them all among so many. At any rate, vone were being attacked, nor had apy been killed. These observations, therefure, quite confirm those previously made, and seem to show that if pupe are taken from a nest, kept till they become perfect insects, and then replaced in the nest, they are recognized as friends. As regards the mode of recognition, Mr. Me Cook considers that it is by scent, and states that if ants are more or less soaked in water, they are no longer recog- nized by their friends, but are attacked. He mentions a case in which an ant fell accidentally into some water: “She remained in the liquid several moments, and crept out of it. Immediately she was seized in a hostile manner, first by one, and then another, then by a third, the two antenne and one leg were thus held. A fourth one assaulted the middle thorax and petiole. The poor little bather was thus dragged helplessly to and fro for a long time, and was evidently ordained to death. Presently I took up the struggling heap. Two of the assailants kept their hold, one finally dropped; the other I could not tear loose, and so put the pair back upon the tree, leaving the doomed immersionist to her hard fate.” His attention having been called to this, he noticed several other cases, always with t’'e same result. I have not myself been able to repeat the observation with the same species, but with two at least of our native ants the results were exactly reversed. In one RECOGNITION AFTER A YEAR AND NINE MONTHS. 239 case five specimens of Lasius niger fell into water and remained immersed for three hours. I then took them out and put them into a bottle to recover themselves. The following morning I allowed them to return. They were received as friends, and, though we watched them from 7.30 till 1.30 every hour, there was not the slightest sign of hostility. The nest was, moreover, placed in a closed box, so that if any ant were killed we could inevitably find the body, and no ant died. In this case, therefore, it is clear that the immersion did not prevent them from being recognized. Again, three specimens of Formica fusca dropped into water. After three hours I took them out, and, after keeping them by themselves for the night to recover, I put them back into the nest. They were unquestionably received as friends, without the slightest sign of hostility or even of doubt. I do not, however, by any means intend to express the opinion that smell is not the mode by which recognition is effected. It will be remembered, perhaps, that my ants (For- mica fusca) recognized one another after a separation of a year and nine months, though “after some months’ separation they were occasionally attacked, as some of the ants, perhaps the young ones, did not recognize them. Still, they were never killed or driven out of the nest, so that evidently when a mistake was made it was soon discovered.” Hence it would appear that there are differences in the memory of different species. In one case Forel had taken some ants from a large nest of Componotus, for the experiments on their sensibility to the ultra-violet rays, to which I have already referred. After his observations were 240 SUPPOSED RECOGNITION BY SCENT. concluded, he returned them to the nest, some after eight, some after forty-one days. Those which were returned after eight days were at once recognized, while as regards those which had been forty-one days away from home. “On reeulait de part et d’autre, se menagait des mandibules, s’examinait 4 fond avec les antennes, se mordait méme. Plusieurs méme allérent dans leur irritation jusqu’ 4 essayer de décapiter et méme 4 décapiter quelques-unes de leurs anciennes compagnes et scenrs avec leurs mandibules (c'est le mode de combat des Camponotus)! Les fourmis vernies prirent part 4 ces rixes aussi bien que les non vernies; je les vis méme attaquer, et elles étaient 4 peine moins adroites. Les combats ne cessérent entigrement qu’au bout d’un ou deux jours, et, 2 part les quelques victimes du premier jour, l’incident se termina par une alliance.” Forel seems to entertain no doubt that the recog- nition is effected by a form of smell, which he terms “odorat au contact.” He says, “ Beaucoup d'insectes ont en outre une sorte d’odorat au contact que nous ne possédons pas et qui permet entre autres aux fourmis de distinguer leurs compagnes de leurs ennemics.” His observations, however, do not favour the hy- pothesis that the recognition may be by smell. If the ants recognized their companions by any odour characteristic of the community, the lapse of thirty days could not have made any difference. Here the question of memory would not enter, because the per- ception of the odour would in both cases be continuaily betore them. M. Forel is so excellent an observer, and has so great a knowledge of the ways of ants, that his opinion is entitled to great weight. It RECOGNITION BY MEANS OF THE ANTENNA, 241] would be very interesting to repeat similar observations, for if it turn out to be the case that separations of comparatively few days lead, in some species, to a want of recognition, it would be a strong argument against the hypothesis that this recognition is due to - smell. It certainly seems as if the recognition was effected to a great extent by the antenne. Not only do the ants cross and recross them, almost, so to say, as two deaf mutes conversing by their fingers ; but, as M. Forel has shown, if ants of different species are brought together after the removal of their antenne they show no signs of hostility. That this latter statement is correct I am quite content to take on M. Forel’s authority; but it is not so conclusive as might seem at first sight, because in ants, as in men, ‘a feliow- feeling makes us wondrous kind,” and ants when isolated, and especially when suffering, are much less pugnacious than they are under normal conditions. CHAPTER XII. ON THE INSTINCTS OF SOLITARY WASPS AND BEES. Tur hive bee and the common wasps are so familiar and so interesting that they have to a great extent diverted attention from the so-called solitary species of the same groups. Few, for instance, are aware that about 4500 species of wild bees are known, and of wasps 1100, of which some 170 and 16 respectively live in Britain. These insects often live in association, but do not form true communities. Speaking generally, we may say that each female constructs a cell, every species having its own favourite site, sometimes underground, sometimes in a hollow stick, in an empty snail-shell, or buiit against a wall, a stone, or the branch of a tree. Having completed her cell, the female stores up in it a sufficient supply of food, which in the case of bees consists of pollen and honey; while the wasps select small animals, such as beetles, caterpillars, spiders, ete., each species generally having one kind of prey. The mother then Jays an egg, after which she closes up the cell, and commences another. Having thus pro- vided sufficiently for her offspring, she generally takes no further heed of it. This is not, however, an invari- able rule: in the genus Bembex, for instance, the INSTINCT OF RENDERING VICTIMS INSENSIBLE. 243 mother, instead of provisioning her cell once for all, brings food to the young grub from day to day. This, however, is an exceptional case, and the mode of life of the solitary wasps raises one of the most interesting questions in connection with instinct. The Ammophila, for instance, having built her cell, places in it, as food for her young, the full-grown caterpillar of a moth, Noctua segetum. Now, if the caterpillar were un- injured, it would struggle to escape and almost inevit- ably destroy the egg; nor would it permit itself to be eaten. On the other hand, if it were killed, it would decay and soon become unfit for food. The wasp, however, avoids both horns of this dilemma. Having found her prey, she pierces with her sting the membrane between the head and the first segment of the body, thus nearly disabling the caterpillar, and then proceeds to inflict eight more wounds between the following segments ; lastly crushing the head, and thus completely paralyzing her victim, but not actually killing it; so that it lies helpless and motionless, but, though living, let us hope insensible. M. Fabre, to whom we are indebted for a most interesting and entertaining series of essays on this group of insects, argues that this remarkable instinct cannot have been gradually acquired. The spots selected are, he says, exactly those occupied by the ganglia. No others among the in- numerable points which might have been chosen would have answered the purpose; not one wound is mis- placed or without effect. M. Fabre truly observes that chance offers no explanation.* Moreover, he unhesi- * In the case of other insects, such as Mutilla, Chrysis, Leucospis, Anthrax, etc., which do not possess the instinct of paralyzing their victims, the young feed on the chrysalis, which is normally without power of movement, 244 ORIGIN OF INSTINCTS. tatingly asserts that ‘‘Si de son cété lhyménoptere excelle dans son art, c’est quil est fait pour l’exercer ; c'est qu'il est doué, non seulement d'outils, mais encore de la maniére de s’en servir. Et ce don est originel, parfait des le début; le passé n’y a rien ajouté, Vavenir n’y ajoutera rien.”* But how was it acquired? M. Fabre cuts the Gordian knot. “ Et tout naivement je me dis: Puisqu’il faut des Araignées aux Pompiles, de tout temps ceux-ci ont possédé leur patiente astuce et les autres leur sotte audace. C’est pnéril, si Pon veut, peu conforme aux visées transcendantes des théories a la mode; il n’y a la ni objectif ni suljectif, ni adapta- tion ni différentiation, ni attavisme ni transformisme ; soit, mais du moins je comprends.” “Je comprends!” M. Fabre says he understands, and no doubt he thinks so; but I confess that his explanation seems to me to leave us just where we were. To my mind, I confess, it seems to me to throw no light whatever on the matter. M. Fabre asserts that the habits of these insects have been “de tout temps” exactly what they are now. I pass by the fuct that the Hymenoptera are, gevlogicaily speaking, of comparatively recent appearance. But is it the case that habits are so invariable? Quite the reverse. The cases of variation are innumerable. Romanes ft refers to a criticism of the same nature by lirby and Spence. “Why,” they ask, “if instincts are open to modification by experience and intelligence, are not -becs sometimes found to use mud or mortar instead of wax or propolis? Show us,” they say, “but one instance of their having sulstituted mud for * J. H. Fabre, “ Nouveaux Souvenirs Entomologiques,” Os : s1q “Mental Evolution in Animals.” HABITS NOT INVARIABLE. 245 propolis, .. . and there conld be no doubt of their having been guided by reason.” Such cases have. how- ever, been observed. Andrew Knight found that his bees collected some wax and turpentine with which he had covered some decorticated trees, and used it instead of propolis, the manufacture of which they discontinued. Nay, M. Fabre has himself placed on record some cases of the same kind, and shown that the instincts of these animals are not absolutely unalterable. Thus one solitary wasp, Sphex flavipennis, which provisions its nest with small grasshoppers, when it returns to the cell, leaves the victim outside, and goes down for a moment to see that all is right. During her absence M. Fabre moved the grasshopper a little. Out came the Sphex, soon found her victim, dragged it to the mouth of the cell, and left it as before. Again and again M. Fabre moved the grasshopper, but every time the Sphex did exartly the same thing, until M. Fabre was tired ont. All the insects of this colony bad the same curious halit; but on trying the same experiment with a Sphex of the following year, after two or three dis- appointments she learned wisdom by experience, and carried the grasshopper directly down into the cell. Eumenes pomiformis builds, as already mentioned, a cell in the open air. If attached to a broad base, “C’est un déme avec goulot central, évasé en embon- chure d’urne. Mais quand l’appui se réduit 4 un point, sur un rameau d’arbuste par exemple, le nid dcvient une capsule sphérique, surmontée toujours d’un goulot, bien entendu.” * Again, he has shown good reason for believing that, although the Tachytes nigra generally makes its * Loe cit., p. 66. 246 CHANGE OF INSTINCTS—BEMBEX. own burrow and stores it with paralyzed prey for its own larve to feed on, yet that, when this insect finds a burrow already made and stored by another Sphex, it takes advantage of the prize, and becomes for the occasion parasitic. On which Mr. Darwin has justly observed that he could see no difficulty in natural selection making an occasional habit permanent, if of advantage to the species, and if the insect whose nest and stored food are thus feloniously appropriated be not thus exterminated. The problem is certainly one of great difficulty, and it is with diffidence that I would suggest to M. Fabre certain considerations which may perhaps throw some light on it. Let us examine some of the other solitary wasps, and see whether their habits afford us any clue. That an animal of prey knows where its victim is most vulnerable, has not in itself anything unusual or unaccountable. The genus Bembex kills the insects on which its young are fed, and supplies the cell with a fresh victim from time to time. Eumenes, like Ammophila and Sphex, stores up the victims once for all. They are grievously wounded, but not altogether paralyzed. Here, then, we have the very condition which M. Fabre considers would be fatal to the tender egg of the wasp. But not necessarily so. The wretched caterpillars lie in a wriggling mass at the buttom of the cell; a clear space is left above them, and from the summit of the cell the delicate egg is suspended by a fine thread, so that, even if touched by a caterpillar in one of its con- vulsive struggles, it would simply swing away in safety, When the young grub is hatched, it suspends itself to this thread by a silken sheath, in which it hangs head ODYNERUS—AMMOPHILA. 247 downwards over its victims. Does one of them struggle ? quick as lightning it retreats up the sheath out of harm’s way. In Odynerus the arrangement is very similar, but the grub simply attaches itself to the support, and does not construct a tube. Moreover, while in the solitary bees and wasps the laying of the egg is generally the final operation before the closing of the cell, in Odynerus, on the contrary, or at least in Odynerus reniformis, the egg is laid before the food is provided. This, perhaps, may have reference to the different con- dition of the victims. According to Marchal,* Cerceris ornata practically kills her victim ; moreover, she stings it not in, but between, the ganglia, and though the first sting is planted between the head and thorax, the following ones do not always follow the same order. At present the Ammophila supplies each cell with one large caterpillar; but was this always so? One species of Odynerus deposits in each cell no less than twenty-four victims, another only eight. Humenes Amedei regulates the number according to the sex: ten for the female grub, five only for the smaller male. Moreover, while phytopbagous larve will not gene- rally eat any plants but those to which they are accustomed, it has been proved that, asa matter of fact, these larvee will feed and thrive on other insects almost, if not quite, as well as on their natural food. Is it, then, impossible that in far bygone ages the larve may have grown more rapidly, so that the victims had not time to decay; or that the ancestors * Marchal, “Sur l’Instinct du Cerceris ornata,” Arch. d. Zool. Exper., 1887. 248 MODIFIABILITY OF INSTINCTS. of our present Ammophilas may have fed their young from day to day with fresh food, as Bembex does even now; that they may then have gradually brought the provisions at longer intervals, choosing small and weak victims, and laying the ege in a special part of the cell, as Eumenes does? that during these long ages they may have gradually learnt the spots where their sting would be most effective, and, thus saving themselves the trouble of capturing a number ot victims, have found that it involved less labour to select a fine fat common caterpillar, such as that of Noctua segetum, and so have gradually acquired their present habits? Wonderful doubtless they are; but, though I hint the suggestion with all deference, such a sequence does not seem to me to present any in- superable difficulty. This suggestion was made in the Contemporary Review for 1885, and I was much interested to find in Mr. Darwin’s life that he had made a similar suggestion in a letter to M. Fabre. He refers to the great skill of the Gauchos in killing cattle, and suggests that each young Gaucho sees how the others do it, and with a very little practice learns the art. “I suppose that the sand-wasps originally merely killed their prey by stinging them in many places (see p. 129 of Fabre’s ‘Souvenirs, and p. 241), and that to sting a certain segment was found by far the most successful method, and was inherited like the tendency of a bulldog to pin the nose of a bull, or of a ferret to bite the cerebellum. It would not be a very great step in advance to prick the ganglion of its prey only slightly, and thus to give its larve fresh meat instead of only dried meat.” * * “Life and Letters of Charles Darwin.” DIFFERENCES UNDER DIFFERENT CIRCUMSTANCES. 249 Perhaps, however, it may be asked, Why should the insect change its habits? Several reasons might be sug- gested. The prey first selected might be exterminated, or at any rate diminish in numbers, and, though each Species as a general rule confines itself to one special victim, some exceptions have already been noticed. For instance, Sphea flavipennis habitually preys on a species of grasshopper, but on the banks of the Rhone M. Fabre found it, on the contrary, attacking a field cricket, whether from the absence of the grasshopper or not he was unable to determine. Take another case. M. Fabre denies* that the different species of Sphex can ever have been derived from one source. Every species now, he observes, has some one victim, some one insect on which it preys, to which it restricts itself, and which the other species do not attack. But “Que chassait, je vous prie, ce proto- type des Sphégiens? Avait il régime varié ou régime uniforme ? Ne pouvant décider, examinons les deux cas.” He begins by supposing that with the ancestor of the Sphex, “ Le régime était varié. J’en félicite hautement ce premier né des Sphex. I] était dans les meilleures conditions pour laisser descendance prospére.” Is it likely then, he says, that they would have limited themselves to one prey, and thus have foolishly diminished their chances in life? “Mais non,” he adds, in his lively style, “mes beaux Sphex, vous n’avez pas été aussi idiots que cela. Sivous étesde nos jours can- tonnés chacun dans un mets de famille, c’est que votre ancétre ne vous a pas enseigné Ja variélé.” He then discusses the alternative whether the * « Sony. Entcm., troisitme série,” 250 ORIGIN OF THE HABITS OF SPHEX. ancestral Sphex restricted itself to one victim, and that its descendants “subdivisés en groupes et con- stitués enfin en autant d’especes distinctes par le lent travail des siécles, se sont avisés qu’en dehors du comestible des ancétres il y avait une foule d’autres aliments.” This, he says, supposes that they experimented on various victims, found several of them to their liking, and then, after a period of varied and plentiful diet, voluntarily abandoned so great an advantage. “ Avoir découvert, par vos essais d’age en Aze, la variété de l’alimentation ; avoir pratiquée, au grand avantage de votre race, et finir par l’uniformité, cause de décadence ; avoir connu l’excellent et le répudier pour le médiocre, ‘Oh! mes Sphex, ce serait stupide si le transformisme avait raison. ’” “Jestime,” then he concludes, “que votre ancétre commun, votre précurseur, & gotits simples ou bien a gotits multiples, est une pure chimére.” No doubt the habits of Hymenoptera present many difficulties, and have undoubtedly many surprises in store for us, and I cannot think the matter is so clear as M. Fabre imagines, or that he has exhausted the possible cases. It is possible, though it is, I admit, only a supposition, that the ancestral Sphex hunted some species which does not now exist—at least not in the south of France—and which might have disappeared gradually. As it became rarer, they might be driven to attack other prey, and M. Fabre has himself shown by a variety of most ingenious experiments that the larvee are by no means fastidious as to their food. The Hymenoptera vary considerably in size, and the larger individuals might be able to overmaster some large RACE DIFFERENCES. 251 insect, while the feebler specimens were compelled to content themselves with humbler fare. This is no purely imaginary case. M. Fabre himself distinguishes three races—or are they species ?—of Leu- cospis which live on the three species of Chalicodomas. “Venu du Chalicodome des galets ou des murailles, dont Vopulente larve le sature de nourriture, il mérite par sa grosseur le nom le Leucospis gigas, que lui donne Fabricius; venu du Chalicodome des hangars, il ne mérite plus que le nom de Leucospis grandis, que lui octroie Klug. Avec une ration moindre, le géant baisse d’un degré et n’est plus que le grand. Venu du Chalicodome des arbustes, il baisse encore, et si quelque nomenclatenr s’avisait de le qualifier, il n’aurait plus droit qu’au titre de médiocre. The Anthrax, again, differs considerably a-cording to the species on which it has fed, those coming from the cocoons of Osmia tricornis being much larger from those from 0. cyanea. Or it might well happen that while the victim was from some cause or other, say for instance the absence of food elsewhere, limited to a particular district, the region beyond was suited to the ancestress Sphex. In that case, would she not naturally try whether she could not find some other suitable food? This again, is not a purely imaginary case. M. Fabre himself tells us that while “ la Scolie interrompue avait pour gibier aux environs d’Avignon, la larve de l’Anoxie velue (Anowia villosa). Aux environs de Sérignan, dans un sol sablon- neux semblable, sans autre végétation que quelques maigres gramens, je lui trouve pour vivres l’Anoxie matutinale (Anoxia matutinalis), qui rewplace ici la yelue.” 252 POWER OF DETERMINING SEX. That bees soon take to newly introduced flowers is a familiar case which every one must have noticed, and which it is surely not logical to dismiss by the conve- nient process of referring it to “instinct.” It is indeed difficult for any one who watches these insects to deny to bees the possession of a higher and conscious faculty. In considering the question whether these remarkable instincts were originally, so to say, engrafted in the insect, or whether they were the result of innumerable repetitions of similar actions carried on by a long series of ancestors, we may perhaps be aided by the consideration that, though the results would in either case be in many respects the same, there are some in which they would altogether differ. In the former, for instance, we might expect that the insect would be so gifted that uo slight obstacle should interfere with the great end in view: in the latter, on the contrary, the very repetition which gave such remarkable results would tend to incapacitate the insect from dealing with any unusual conditions. LimITATION OF INSTINCT. We should, in fact, find side by side with these won- derful instincts almost equally surprising evidence of stupidity. Now, one species of Sphex preys on a large grasshopper (Ephippigera). Having disabled her vic- tim, she drags it along by one of the antenna, and M. Fabre found that if the antenne be cut off close to the head, the Sphex, after tryiny in vain to get a grip, eives the matter up as a bad job, and leaves her victim in despair, without ever think ng of dragging it by one of its legs. Again, when a Sphex had provisioned her cell, laid her egg, and was about to close it up, M. LIMITATION OF INSTINCT. 253 Fabre drove her away, and took ont both the Ephippi- gera and the egg. He then allowed the Sphex to return. She went down into the empty cell, and though she must have known that the grasshopper and the egg were no longer there, yet she proceeded calmly to stop up the orifice just as if nothing had happened. The genus Sphex paralyzes its victims and provisions its cell once for all. Bembex, on the contrary, as already mentioned, kills the insects on which its young are to feed, and, perhaps on this account, brings its young fresh food (mainly flies) from time to time. But while the Bembex thus preys on some flies, there are others which avenge their order. The genus Miltogramma lays its eggs in the cell of the Bembex ; and, though there seems no reason why the Bembex, which is by far the stronger insect, should tolerate this intrusion, which, moreover, sbe shows unmistakably to be most unpalatable, she never makes any attack on her enemy. Nay, when the young of the Miltogramma are hatched, so far from being killed or removed, these entomological cuckoos are actually fed until they reach maturity. Nevertheless, it seems contrary to etiquette for the fly to enter the cell of the Bembex ; she watches the opportunity when the latter is in the cell and is dragging down the victim. Then is the Miltogramma’s opportunity; she pounces on the victim, and almost instantaneously lays on it two or three eggs, which are then transferred, with the insect on which they are to feed, to the cell. It is remarkable how the Bembex remembers (if one may use such a word) the eutrance to her cell, covered as it is with sand, exactly to our eyes like that all ronnd. On the other hand, M. Fabre found that if he 254 TOLERATION OF PARASITES. removed the surface of the earth and the passage, exposing the cell and the larva, the Bembex was quite at a loss, and did not even recognize her own offspring. Tt seems as if she knew the door, the nursery, and the passage, but not her child. Another ingenious experiment of M. Fabre’s was made with a mason bee (Chalicodoma). This genus constructs an earthen cell, through which at maturity the young insect eats its way. M. Fabre found that if he pasted a piece of paper round the cell, the insect had no difficulty in eating through it; but if he enclosed the cell in a paper case, so that there was a space even of only a few lines between the cell and the paper, in that case the paper formed an effectual prison. The instinct of the insect taught it to bite through one enclosure, but it had not wit enough to do so a second time. One of the most striking instances of stupidity (may I say) is mentioned by M. Fabre, in the case of one of his favourite bees, the Chalicodoma pyrenaica, This species builds cells of masonry, which she tills with honey as she goes on, raising the rim a little, then making a few journeys for honey, then raising the rim again, and so on until the cell is completed. She then prepares a last load of mortar, brings it in her mandibles, lays her egg, and immediately closes up the cell; having doubtless provided the mortar beforehand, lest during her absence an enemy should destroy the egg or any parasitic insect should gain admittance. his being so, M. Fabre chose a cell which was all but finished, and during the absence of the bee he broke aay part of the cell-covering. Again, in some half- finished cells he broke away a little of the wall. In all these cases the bee, as might be expected, repaired CASES OF APPARENT STUPIDITY. 255 the mischief, the operation being in the natural order of her work. But now comes the curious fact. In another series of cells M. Fabre pierced a hole in the cell below the part where the bee was working, and through which the honey at once began to exude. The poor stupid little bee, however, never thought of repairing the breach. She worked on as if nothing had happened. Tn her alternate journeys she brought first mortar and then honey, which, however, ran out again as fast as it was poured in. This experiment he repeated over and over again with various modifications in detail, but always with the same result. It may be suggested that possibly the bee was unable to stop up a hole once formed. But that could not have been the case. M. Fabre took one of the pellets of mortar brought by the bee, and successfully stopped the hole himself. The omission, therefore, was due, not to a want of power, but of intellect. But M. Fabre carried his experiment still further. Perhaps the bee had not noticed the injury. He chose, therefore, a cell which was only just begun and contained very little honey. In this he made a comparatively large hole. The bee returned with a supply of honey, and, seeming much surprised to find the hole in the bottom of the cell, examined it carefully, felt it with her antennz, and even pushed them through it. Did she then, as might naturally have been expected, stop it up? Nota bit. The unexpected catastrophe transcended the range of her intellect, and she calmly proceeded to pour into this vessel of the Danaides load after load of honey, which of conrse ran out of the bottom as fast as she poured it in at the top. AI the afternoon she laboured at this fruitless task, and began again undiscouraged the next morning, At length, when she 18 256 M. FABRE’S EXPERIMENTS, had brought the usual complement of honey, she laid her egg, and gravely sealed up the empty cell. In another case, he made a large hole in the cell just above the level of the honey—a hole so large that through it he was able to see the bee lay her ege. Having done so, she carefully closed the top of the cell, but though she closely examined the hole in the side, it did not enter into the range of her ideas that such an accident could take place, and it never occurred to her to cover it up. Another curious point raised by these ingenious experiments has reference to the quantity of honey. The cell is by no means filled; a space is always left between the honey and the roof of the cell. The usual depth of the honey in a completed cell is ten milli- metres, But the bee is not guided by this measure- ment, for in the preceding cases she sometimes closed the cell when the honey had a depth of only five milli- metres, of three, or even when the cell was almost empty. No; in some mysterious manner the bee feels when she his provided as much honey as her ancestress had done before her, and regarls her work as accomplished. What a wonderful, but what a narrow, nature! She has built the cell and provided the honey, but there her instinct stops: if the cell is pierced, if the honey is removed, it does not occur to her to repair the one or fill up the other, M. Fabre not unnaturally asks, “ Avec la moindre lueur rationnelle, Pinsecte déposerait- il son ceuf sur le tiers, sur le dixiéme des vivres réces- saires ; le déposerait-il dans une cellule vide; laisserait- il le nourrisson sans nourriture, incroyable aberration de la maternité? J’ai raconté, que le lecteur décide.” The family of bees is generally reckoned to be one of great intelligence, but these and many other similar LIMITATION OF INSTINCT. 257 instances which might be recorded seem to show great limitation of intelligence. Let me give one other, which any person may easily test for himself. I took a glass shade or jar eighteen inches long, and with a mouth six and a half inches wide, turning the closed end to the window, and put in a common hive bee. She buzzed about for an hour, when, as there seemed no chance of her getting out, T put her back into the hive. Two flies, on the contrary, which I put in with her, got out at once. Again I put another bee and a fly into the same glass; the latter flew out at once. For half an hour the bee tried to get out at the closed end; I then turned the glass with its open end to the light when she flew out at once. To make sure, I repeated the experiment once more, with the same result. And yet there is, no doubt, ample foundation for the ordinary view which attributes considerable intelligence to the bze, within the sphere of her own operations. Several other points of resemblance between instincts and habits could be pointed out. As in repeating a well-known song, so in instincts, one action follows another by a sort of rhythm. If a person be interrupted in a song, or in repeating anything by rote, he is often forced to go back to recover the habitual train of thought; so P. Huber found it was with a caterpillar, which makes a very complicated hammock; for if he took a caterpillar which had completed its hammock up to, say, the sixth stage of construction, and put it into a hammock completed up only to the third stage, the caterpillar simply re-performed the fourth, fifth, and sixth stages of construction. “If, how- ever, a caterpillar were taken out of a hammock made 298 INSTINCTS AND HABITS: up, for instance, to the third stage, and were put into one finished up to the sixth stage, so that much of its work was already done for it, far from feeling the benefit of this, it was much embarrassed, and, in order to complete its hammock, seemed forced to start from the third stage, where it had left off, and thus tried to complete the already fivished work.’’* Another very interesting series of observations which we owe to M. Fabre has reference to the question of sex, and it would really seem that the mother can regulate the sex of the egg at will. In many of our wild bees, the females are much larger than the males. The male lives a life of pleasure, idle but short. “Quinze jours de bombance dans un magasin a miel, un an de sommeil sous terre, une minute d’amour au soleil, puis la mort.” But the female “C’est la mére, la mére seule qui, péniblement, creuse sous terre des galeries et des cellules, pétrit le stue pour enduire les loges, magonne la demeure de ciment et de graviers, taraude le bois et subdivise le canal en étages, dé‘oupe des rondelles de feuilles qui seront assemblées en pots a miel, malaxe la résine cueillie en Jarmes sur les blessures des pins pour édifier des voites dans la rampe vide d’un es- cargot, chasse la proie, la paralyse et la traine au logis, cueille la poussiére pollinique, élabore le miel dans son jabot, emmagasine et mixtionne la patée. Ce rude labeur, si impérieux, si actif, dans lequel se dépense toute la vie de Vinsecte, exige, c’est évident, une puissance corporelle bien inutile au male, l’amou- reux désceuvré.” In the hive bee the drone cells differ materially in shape from those of the queens and workers. * Darwin, “ Origin of Species.” INFLEXIBILITY OF INSTINCT. 259 In the solitary wasps, where the females are much larger than the males, the mother builds a larger cell and provides more food for the former than for the latter. The Chalicodoma (one of the mason bees) often lays her eggs in old cells of the previous year. These are of two sizes—large ones, originally built for the females, and small ones for the males. Now, in utilizing old cells, the bee always places male eggs in male cells and female eggs in female cells. If, how- ever, a female cell be cut down so as to reduce the size, then indeed the bee deposits in it a male egg. The bees belonging to the genus Osmia* arrange their cells in a row in a hollow stick, or some other similar situation, and it has long been krfown that in these and similar cases the cells first provisioned, and which are therefore furthest from the entrance, always contain females, while the outer cells always contain males. There is an obvious advantage in this, because the males come out a fortnight or more before the females, and it is, of course, convenient that those which have to come out first should be in the cells nearest the door. The bee does not, however, lay all the female eggs first, and then all the male eggs. By no means. She produces altogether from fifteen to thirty eggs, but seldom arranges them in one row; generally they are in several series, and in every one the same sequence occurs—females further from, and males nearest to, the door. For instance, one of M. Fabre’s marked bees—one, moreover, of exceptional fertility—occupied some glass * Osmia tridentata constitutes an exception to the general rule in this respect, as in some otliers. 260 DIFFERENT HABITS OF MALES AND FEMALES. tubes, which he arranged conveniently for her. From the 1st to the 10th of May she constructed, in one tube, eight cells—first seven female, and then one male. From the 10th to the 17th, in a second tube, she built first three female and then three male cells; from the 17th to the 25th, in a third, three female and then two male; on the 26th, in a fourth, one female; and, finally, from the 26th to the 30th, in a fifth, two female and three male: altogether twenty-five, seventeen female and eight male cells. The advantage of this is clear, but the manner in which it is secured is not so obvious. It might be suggested that the quantity of food was not regulated by the sex of the young one, but that the sex depended on the quantity of food. This would be very improb- able, and M. Fabre attempted to disprove it by some very ingenious experiments. He found that if he took some of the food from a female cell, the bee or wasp produced was still a female, though a starveling; while if he added food to a male cell, the larva still pro- duced a male, though a very large and fine one. M. Fabre then made some of his most ingenious experiments. He brought into his room a large number of cocoons of Osmia. When the perfect insects were about to emerge, he arranged for them a number of glass tubes, of which the Osmias gladly availed them- selves, and in which they proceeded to construct their cells. The usu:nl arrangement, as already mentioned, is that the males are placed nearest to, and the female furthest from, the door. But M. Fabre so arranged the tubes that each was in two parts, an outer wider portion having a diameter of eight to twelve milli- metres, which is sufficient for a female cell; and an ARRANGEMENT OF MALE AND FEMALE CELLS. 261 inner narrower portion with a diameter of five to five and a half millimetres, which is too small for a female, but just large enough for a male. This arrangement placed the Osmias in a difficulty. They could not follow their natural instinct and construct at the end of the tube cells large enough for females. What happened? Some of the Osmias shut off the narrow ends, and used only the outer wider portion. Others, reluctant, as it were, to throw away a chance, built also in the narrow part of the tube, and under these circumstances, contrary to the otherwise invari- able rule, the inner and first constructed cells contained males. M. Fabre concludes then, and it seems to me has given very strong reasons for thinking so, that these privileged insects not only know the sex of the insect which will emerge from the egg they are about to lay, but that at their own will they can actually control it! Certainly a most curious and interesting result! He concludes his charming work as follows :—“ Mes chérs insectes, dont l'étude m’a soutenu et continue a me soutenir au milieu de mes plus rudes épreuves, il faut ici, pour aujourd’hui, se dire adieu. Autour de moi les rangs s’eclaircissent et les longs espoirs ont fui. Pourrai-je encore parler de vous ?” and every lover of nature will, I am sure, echo the wish. CHAPTER XIII. ON THE SUPPOSED SENSE OF DIRECTION. OxE of the most interesting questions connected with the instincts and powers of animals has reference to the manner in which they find their way back, after having been carried to a distance from, home. This has by some been attributed to the possession of a special “sense of direction.” Mr. Darwin sugeested that it would be interesting to try the effect of putting animals “in a circular box with an axle, which could be made to revolve very rapidly, first in one direction and then in another, so as to destroy for a time all sense of direction in the insects. I have sometimes,” he said, “imagined that animals may feel in which direction they were at the first start carried.” In fact, in parts of France it is considered that if a cat is carried from one house to another in a bag, and the bag is whirled round and round, the cat loses her direction and cannot return to her old home. On this subject M. Fabre has made some interesting and amusing experiments. He took ten bees belonging to the genus Chalicodoma, marked them on the back with a spot of white, and put them in a bag. He then carried them half a kilometre in one direction, stopping at a point where an old cross stands by the EXPERIMENTS WITH BEES. 263 wayside, and whirled the bag rapidly round his head. While he was doing so a good woman came by, who was not a little surprised to find the professor stand- ing in front of the old cross, solemnly whirling a bag round his head, and, M. Fabre fears, strongly suspected him of some satanic practice. However this may be, M. Fabre, having sufficiently whirled his bees, started off back in the opposite direction, and carried his prisoners to a distance from their home of three kilometres. Here he again whirled them round, and then let them go one by one. They made one or two turns round him, and then flew off in the direction of home. In the meanwhile his daughter Antonia was on the watch. The first bee did the mile and three-quarters in a quarter of an hour. Some hours after two more re- turned; the other seven did not reappear. The next day he repeated this experiment with ten other bees. The first returned in five minutes, and two more in about an hour. In this case, again, seven out of ten failed to find their way home. In another experiment he took forty-nine bees, When let out, a few started wrong, but he says that “lorsque la rapidité du vol me laisse reconnaitre la direction suivie;” the great majority flew homewards. The first arrived in fifteen minutes. In an hour and a half eleven had returned, in five hours six more, making seventeen out of forty-nine. Again he experi- mented with twenty, of which seven found their way home. In the next experiment he took the bees rather further—to a distance of about two and a quarter miles. In an hour and a half two had returned, in three hours and a half seven more ; total, nine out of forty. Lastly, he took thirty bees: fifteen marked rose he took by 264 WHIRLING BEES. a roundabout route of over five miles; the other fifteen marked blue he sent straight to the rendezvous, about one and a half miles from home. All the thirty were let out at noon; by five in the evening seven “rose” bees and six “blue” bees had returned, so that the long détour had made no appreciable difference. These experiments seem to M. Fabre conclusive. “La démonstration,” he says, “est suffisante. Ni les mouve- ments enchevétrés d’une rotation comme je l’ai décrite; ni Vobstacle de collines a franchir et de bois & traverser; ni les embichesd’unevoie qui s'avance, rétrograde et revient par un ample circuit, ne peuvent troubler les Chali- codomes dépaysés et les empécher de revenir au nid.” * I am not ashamed to confess that, charmed by M. Fabre’s enthusiasm, dazzled by his eloquence and ingenuity, I was at first disposed to adopt this view. Calmer consideration, however, led me to doubt, and though M. Fabre’s observations are most ingenious, and are very amusingly described, they do not carry conviction to my mind. There are two points specially to be considered— 1. The direction taken by the bees when released. 2. The success of the bees in making good their return home. As regards the first point, it will be observed that the successful bees were in the following proportion, viz. :— 3 out of 10 4° 4 10 IT « «= T « 20 yY . 40 © a 35 Oraltogether 47 ,, 144 * J. H. Fabre, “ Nouveaux Souvenirs Entomologiques.” BEHAVIOUR OF BEES IF TAKEN FROM HOME. 265 This is not a very large proportion. Out of the whole number no less than ninety-seven appear to have lost their way. May not the forty-seven have found theirs by sight or by accident? Instinct, how- ever inferior to reason, has the advantage of being generally unerring. When two out of three bees went wrong, we may, I think, safely dismiss the idea of instinct. Moreover, the distance from home was only one and a half to two miles. Now, bees certainly know the country for some distance round their home ; how far they generally forage 1 believe we have no certain information, but it seems not unreasonable to suppose that if they once came within a mile of their nest they would find themselves within ken of some familiar landmark. Now, if we suppose that 150 bees are let out two miles from home, and that they flew away at random, distributing themselves equally in all directions, a little consideration will show that some twenty-five of them would find themselves within a mile of home, and consequently would know where they were. I have never myself experimented with Chalicodomas, but 1 have observed that if a hive bee is taken to a distance, she behaves as a pigeon does under similar ” circumstances ; that is to say, she flies round and round, gradually rising higher and higher and enlarging her circle, until I suppose her strength fails or she comes within sight of some known object. Again, if the bees had returned by a sense of direction, they would have been back in a few minutes. To fly one and a half or two miles would not take five minutes. One bee out of the 147 did it in that time; but the others teok one, two, three, or even five hours. Surely, then, it is reasonable to suppose that these lost some time before 266 MODE OF FINDING THEIR WAY. they came in sight of any object known to them. The second result of M. Fabre’s observations is not open to these remarks. He observes that the great majority of his Chalicodomas at once took the direction home. He confesses, however, in the sentence I have already quoted, that it is not always easy to follow bees with the eye. Admitting the fact, however, it seems to me far from impossible that the bees knew where they were ; and, at any rate, this does not seem so improbable that we should be driven to admit the existence of a new sense, which we ought only to assume as a last resource. Moreover, M. Fabre himself says, “ Lorsque la rapidité du vol me laisse reconnaitre la direction suivie,” which seems to imply a doubt. Indeed, some years previously he had made a similar experiment with the same species, but taking them direct to a point rather over two miles (four kilometres) from the nest, and not whirling them round his head. I looked back, there, fore, to his previous work to see how these behaved, and I found that he seys— “ Aussitét libres, les Chalicodomes fuient, comme effarés, qui dans une direction, qui dans la direction tout opposée. Autant que le permet leur vol fougueux, je crois néanmoins reconnaitre un prompt retour des abeilles lancées 4 l’opposé de leur demeure, et la majorité me semble se diriger du cété de Vhorizon ot se trouve le nid. Je laisse ce point avec des doutes, que rendent inévitables des insectes perdus de vue a une vingtaine de métres de distance.” In this case, then, some went in one direction, some in another. It certainly would be remarkable if bees which were taken direct missed their way, while those EXPERIMENTS WITH ANTS. 267 which were whirled round and round went straight home. Moreover, it appears that after all, as a matter of fact, they did not fly straight home. Ifthey had done so they would have been back in three or four minutes, whereas they took far longer. Even then, if they started in the right direction, it is clear that they did not adhere to it. I have myself tried experiments of the same kind with hive bees and ants. For instance, I put down some honey on a piece of glass close to a nest of Lasius niger, and when the ants were feeding I placed it quietly on the middle of a board one foot square, and eighteen inches from the nest. I did this with thirteen ants, and marked the points at which they left the board. Five of them did so on the half of the board nearest the nest, and eight on that turned away from it. I then timed three of them. They all found the nest eventually, but it took them ten, twelve, and twenty minutes respectively. Again, I took forty ants which were feeding on some honey, and put them down on a gravel-path about fifty yards from the nest, and in the middle of a square eighteen inches in diameter, which I marked out on the path by straws. I prepared a corresponding square on paper, and, having indicated by the arrow the direction of the nest, I marked down the spot where each ant passed the boundary. They crossed it in all directions; and dividing the square into two halves, one towards the nest and one away from it, the number in each were almost exactly the same. After leaving the square, they wandered about with every appearance of having lost themselves, and crossed 268 MR. ROMANES’ EXPERIMENTS. the boundary backwards and forwards in all directions, Two of them, however, we watched for an hour each. They meandered about, and at the end of the time one was about two feet from where she started, but scarcely any nearer home; the other about six feet away, and nearly as much further from home. I then tvok them up and replaced them near the nest, which they at once joyfully entered. I mentioned some of the foregoing facts in a paper which I read at the meeting of the British Association at Aberdeen, and they have since been confirmed by Mr. Romanes.* “Tn connection,” he says, “with Sir John Lubbock’s paper at the British Association, in which this subject is treated, it is perhaps worth while to describe some experiments which I made last year. The question to be answered is whether bees find their way home merely by their knowledge of landmarks, or by means of some mysterious faculty usually termed a sense of direction. The ordinary impression appears to have been that they do so in virtue of some such sense, and are therefore independent of any special knowledge of the district in which they may be suddenly liberated ; and, as Sir John Lubbock observes, this impression was corroborated by the experiments of M. Fabre. The conclusions drawn from these experiments, however, appeared to me, as they appeared to Sir John, un- warranted by the facts; and therefore, like him, I re- peated them with certain variations. In the result I satisfied myself that the bees depend entirely upon their special knowledge of district or landmarks, and it is because my experiments thus fully corroborate those * Nature, October 29, 1886, MR. ROMANES’ EXPERIMENTS. 269 which were made by Sir John that it now occurs to me to publish them. “The house where I conducted the observations is situated several hundred yards from the coast, with flower-gardens on each side, and lawns between the house and the sea. Therefore bees starting from the house would find their honey on either side of it, while the lawns in front would be rarely or never visited— being themselves barren of honey, and leading only to the sea. Such being the geographical conditions, I placed a hive of bees in one of the front rooms on the basement of the house. When the bees became thoroughly well acquainted with their new quarters by flying in and out of the open window for a fortnight, I began the experiments. The modus operandi consisted in closing the window after dark when all the bees were in their hive, and also slipping a glass shutter in front of the hive door, so that all the bees were doubly im- prisoned. Next morning I slightly raised the glass shutter, thus enabling any desired number of bees to escape. When the desired number had escaped, the glass shutter was again closed, and all the liberated bees were caught as they buzzed about the inside of the shut window. ‘These bees were then counted into a box, the window of the room opened, and a card well smeared over with birdlime placed upon the threshold of the beehive, or just in front of the closed glass shutter, The object of all these arrangements was to obviate the necessity of marking the bees, and so to enable me not merely to experiment with ease upon any number of individuals that I might desire, but also to feel confident that no one individual could return to the hive un- noticed. For whenever a bee returned it was certain 270 MR. ROMANES’ EXPERIMENTS. to become entangled in the bird-lime, and whenever I found a bee so entangled, I was certain that it was one which [had taken from the hive, as there were no other hives in the neighbourhood. “ Such being the method, I began by taking a score ot bees in the box out to sea, where there could be no Jand- marks to guide the insects home. Had any of these insects returned, I should next have taken another score out to sea (after an interval of several days, so as to be sure that the first lot had become permanently lost), and then, before liberating them, have rotated the box in asling for a considerable time, in order to see whether this would have confused their sense of direction. But, as none of the bees returned after the first experiment, it was clearly needless to proceed to the second. Ac- cordingly, I liberated the next Jot of bees on the sea- shore, and, as none of these returned, I liberated another lot on the lawn between the shore and the house. I was somewhat surprised to find that neither did any of these return, although the distance from the lawn to the hive was not above two hundred yards. Lastly, I liberated bees in different parts of the flower-garden, and these I always found stuck upon the bird-lime within a few minutes of their liberation. Indeed, they often arrived before I had had time to run from the place where I had liberated them to the hive. Now, as the garden was a large one, many of these bees had to fly a greater distance, in order to reach the hive, than was the case with their lost sisters upon the lawn, and therefore I could have no doubt that their uniform success in finding their way home so immediately was due to their special knowledge of the flower-garden, and not to any general sense of direction. NO EVIDENCE OF SEPARATE SENSE OF DIRECTION. 271 “T may add that, while in Germany a few wecks ago, I tried on several species of ant the same experiments as Sir John Lubbock describes in his paper as having been tried by him upon English species, and here also I obtained identical results; in all cases the ants were hopelessly lost if liberated more than a moderate dis- tance from their nest. M. Romanes’ experiments, therefore, as he himself says, entirely confirm the opinion I have ventured to express—that there is no sufficient evidence among insects of anything which can justly be called a “ sense of direction.” CHAPTER XIV. ON THE INTELLIGENCE OF THE DOG. CoNSIDERING the long ages during which man and the other animals have shared this beautiful world, it is surely remarkable how little we know about them. We have recently had various interesting works on the intelligence and senses of animals, and yet I think the principal impression which they leave on the mind is that we know very little indeed on the subject. Toe Doe. As to the intelligence of the dog, a great many people, indeed, seem to me to entertain two entirely oppos:te and contradictory opiniuns. I often hear it said that the dog, for instance, is very wise and clever. But when I ask whether a dog can realize that two and two make four, which is a very simple arithmetical calculation, I generally find much doubt expressed. That the dog is a loyal, true, and affectionate friend must be gratefully admitted, but when we come to con- sider the psychical nature of the animal, the limits of our knowledge are almost immediately reached. I have else- where suggested that this arises in great measure from the fact that hitherto we have tried to teach animals, rather than to learn from them—to convey our ideas to EDUCATION OF THE DEAF AND DUMB. 273 them, rather than to devise any language or code of signals by means of which they might communicate theirs to us. The former may be more important from a utilitarian point of view, though even this is questionable, but psychologically it is far less interest- ing. Under these circumstances, it occurred to me whether some such system as that followed with deaf mutes, and especially by Dr. Howe with Laura Bridg- man, might not prove very instructive if adapted to the case of dogs. A very interesting account of Laura Bridgman has been published by Wright, compiled almost entirely from reports of the Perkins Institution, and the Massachusetts Asylum for the Blind, in which Dr. Howe, the director of the establishment, details the history of Laura Bridg- man, who was deaf, dumb, and blind, almost without the power of smell and taste, but who, nearly alone among those thus grievously afflicted, possessed an average, if not more than an average, amount of intelligence, although, until brought under Dr. Howe’s skilful treat- ment and care, her physical defects excluded her from all social intercourse. Laura Bridgman was born of intelligent and respect- able parents, in Hanover, New Hampshire, U.S., in December, 1829. She is said to have been a sprightly, pretty infant, but subject to fits, and altogether very fragile. At two years old she was fairly forward, had mastered the difference between A and B, and, indeed, is said to have displayed a considerable degree of intelligence. She then became suddenly ill, and had to be kept in a darkened room for five months. When she recovered she was blind, deaf, and had nearly lost the power both of smell and taste. 274 LAURA BRIDGMAN. “What a situation was hers! The darkness and silence of the tomb were around her; no mother’s smile gladdened her heart, or ‘called forth an answering smile;’ no father’s voice taught her to imitate his sounds, To her, brothers and sisters were but forms of matter, which resisted her touch, but which differed not from the furniture of the house, save in warmth and in the power of locomotion, and in these respects not even from the dog or cat.” Her mind, however, was unaffected, and the sense of touch remained. “As soon as she was able to walk, Laura began to explore the room, and then the house ; she became familiar with the form, density, weight, and heat of every article she could lay her hands on. “She followed her mother, felt her hands and arms, as she was occupied about the house, and her disposi- tion to imitate led her to repeat everything herself. She even learnt to sew a little, and to knit. Her affections, too, began to expand, and seemed to be lavished upon the members of her family with p-culiar force. “The means of communication with her, however, were very limited. She could only be told to go to a place by being pushed, or to come to one by a sign of drawing her. Patting her gently on the head signified approbation ; on the back, the contrary.” The power of communication was thus most limited, and her character began to suffer, when fortunately Dr. Howe heard of her, and in October, 1837, received her into the institution. “Fora while she was much bewildered, till she became acquainted with her new locality, and somewhat familiar with the inmates; the attempt was made to give her LAURA BRIDGMAN. 275 knowledge of arbitrary signs, by which she could interchange thoughts with others. “The first experiments were made by taking the articles in common use, such as knives, forks, spoons, keys, etc., and pasting upon them labels, with their names embossed in raised letters. These she felt carefully, and soon, of course, distinguished that the crooked lines s-p-o-o-n differed as much from the crooked lines k-e-y, as the spoon differed from the key in form, Then small detached labels with the same words printed upon them were put into her hands; she soon observed that they were the same as those pasted upon the articles. She showed her perception of this similarity by laying the label k-e-y upon the key, and the label s-p-o-o-n upon the spoon. “Hitherto, the process had been mechanical, and the success about as great as that of teaching a very know- ing dog a variety of tricks. “The poor child sat in mute amazement, and patiently imitated everything her teacher did. But now her intellect began to work, the truth flashed upon her, and she perceived that there was a way by which she could herself make a sign of anything that was in her own mind, and show it to another mind. At once her countenance lighted up with a human expression. It was no longer as a mere instinctive animal; it was an immortal spirit, eagerly seizing upon a new link of union with other spirits. I could almost fix upon the moment when this truth dawned upon her mind, and spread its beams upon her countenance; I saw that the great obstacle was overcome, and that henceforth nothing but patient and persevering, but plain and straightforward, efforts were necessary. 276 APPLICATION OF THE METHOD FOLLOWED “The result, thus far, is quickly related and easily conceived; but not so was the process, for many weeks of apparently unprofitable labour were spent before it was effected. “The next step was to procure a set of metal types, with the different letters of the alphabet cast separately on their ends; also a board, in which were square holes, into which she could set the types, so that the letters could alone be felt above the surface. “Thus, on any article being handed to her, as a pencil or watch, she would select the component letters and arrange them on the board, and read them with apparent pleasure, assuring her teacher that she understood by taking all the letters of the word and putting them to her ear, or on the pencil.” It is unnecessary, from my present point of view, to carry the narrative further, interesting as it is. I will only observe that even in the case of Laura Bridgman the process was one of much difficulty and requiring great patience. For a long while it was found im- possible to make her realize the use of adjectives; she could not “understand any general expression of quality.” Again, we are told that “Some idea of tie difficulty of teaching her common expressions may be derived from the fact that a lesson of two hours upon the words ‘right’ and ‘le{t’ was deemed very profitable if she had in that time really mastered the idea.” Now, it seemed to me that the ingenious method devised by Dr. Howe, and so successfully carried ont in the case of Laura. Bridgman, might be adapted to the case of dogs, and I have tried this in a small way with a black poodle named Van. WITH THE DEAF AND DUMB TO ANIMALS. 277 VAN AND HIS CARDS. I took two pieces of cardboard about ten inches by three, and on one of them printed in large letters the word FOOD leaving the other blank. I then placed the two cards over two saucers, and in the one under the “food” card put a little bread and milk, which Van, after having his attention called to the card, was allowed to eat. This was repeated over and over again till he had had enough. In about ten days he began to distinguish between the two cards. I then put them on the floor and made him bring them to me, which he did readily enough. When he brought the plain card I simply threw it back, while when he brought the “food” card I gave him a piece of bread, and in about a month he had pretty well learned to realize the difference. I then had some other cards printed with the words “out,” “tea,” “bone,” “ water,” and a certain number also with words to which I did not intend him to attach any significance, such as “nought,” “plain,” “ball,” etc. Van soon learned that bringing a card was a request, and soon learned to distinguish between the plain and printed cards; it took him longer to realize the difference between words, but he gradually got to recognize several, such as “food,” “out,” * bone,” “tea,” etc. If he was asked whetber he would like to go out for a walk, he would joyfully fish up the “out” card, choosing it from several others, and bring it to me, or run with it in evident triumph to the door. 278 MY DOG VAN. I need hardly say that the cards were not always put in the same places. They were varied quite indiscrimi- nately and in a great variety of positions. Nor could the dog recognize them by scent. They were all alike, and all continually handled by us. Still, I did not trust to that alone, but bad a number printed for each word. When, for instance, he brought a card with “food” on it, we did not put down the same identical card, but another bearing the same word; when he had brought that, a third, then a fourth, and so on. For a single meal, therefore, eighteen or twenty cards would be used, so that he evidently is not guided by scent. No one who has seen him look down a row of cards and pick up the one he wanted could, I think, doubt that in bringing a card he felt that he is making a request, and that he could not only distinguish one card from another but also associate the word and object. I used to leave a card marked “ water” in my dress- ing-room, the door of which we used to pass in going to or from my sitting-room. Van was my constant companion, and passed the dor when I was at home several times in the day. Generally he tovk no heed of the card. Hundreds, or I may say thousands, of times be passed it unnoticed. Sometimes, however, he would run in, pick it up, and bring it to me, when of course J gave him some water, and on such occasions I invariably found that he wanted to drink. I might also mention, in corroboration, that one morning he seemed unwell. A friend, being at break- fast with us, was anxious to see him bring his cards, and I therefore pressed him to do so. To my surprise he brought three dummy cards successively, one marked COMMUNICATION BY MEANS OF CARDS. 279 “ham,” one “bag,” and one “brush.” I said re- proachfully, “Oh, Van! bring “food,” or “tea;” on which he looked at me, went very slowly, and brought the “tea” card. But when I put some tea down as usual, he would not touch it. Generally he greatly enjoyed acup of tea, and, indeed, this was the only atime I ever knew him refuse it. A definite numerical statement always seems to me clearer and more satisfactory than a mere general assertion. I will, therefore, give the actual particulars of certain days. Twelve cards were put on the floor, one marked “food” and one “tea.” The others had more or less similar words. I may again add that every time a card was brought, another similarly marked was put in its place. Van was not pressed to bring cards, but simply left to do as he pleased. 1 Van brought “food” 4 times. “Tea” 2 times, 2 ” ” 6 y 3 ” ” 8 yy ” 2 4 » bed 7 ” ” 3 » 5 ” ” 6 ” ” 4 ” 6 ” ” 6 ” ” 3 ” - Nought 7? once. 7 ” ” 8 ord ” 2 ” 8 i] ” 5 ” ” 3 ” 9 ” ” 4 4, ” 2 4 10 ” » 10 , » * 4 “Door” once. il ” ” 10 ” ” 3 ” 12 » ” 6 ” ” 3 ” 80 31 Thus out of 113 times he brought food 80 times, tea 31 times, and the other 10 cards only twice. Moreover, the last time he was wrong he brought a card—namely, “door”—in which three letters out of four were the same as in “ food.” 14 280 ATTEMPTS TO CONVEY IDEAS. This is, of course, only a beginning, but it is, I venture to think, suggestive, and might be carried further, though the limited wants and aspirations of the animal constitute a great difficulty. My wife has a beautiful and charming collie, Patience, to whom we are much attached. This dog was often in the room when Van brought the “ food” card and was rewarded with a piece of bread. She must have seen this thou- sands of times, and she begged in the usual manner, but never once did it occur to her to bring a card. She did not touch, or, indeed, even take the slightest notice of them. I then tried the following experiment :—I prepared six cards about ten inches by three, and coloured in pairs—two yellow, two blue, and two orange. I put one card of each colour on the floor, and then, holding up one of the others, endeavoured to teach Van to bring me the duplicate. That is to say, that if the blue was held up, he should fetch the corresponding colour from the floor ; if yellow, he should fetch the yellow, and soon. When he brought the wrong card he was made to drop it and return for another, until-he brought the right one, when he was rewarded with a little food. We continued the lessons for nearly three months, but asa few days were missed, we may say for ten weeks, and yet at the end of the time I cannot say that Van appeared to have the least idea what was expected of him. It seemed a matter of pure accident which card he brought. There is, I believe, no reason to doubt that dogs can distinguish colours; but as it was just possible that Van might be colour-blind, we then repeated the same experiment, only substituting for the coloured cards others marked respectively with one, ARIPTHMETICAL POWERS OF ANIMALS. 281 two, and three dark bands. This we continued for another three months, or, say, allowing for intermissions, ten weeks ; but, to my surprise, entirely without success, for we altogether failed to make Van understand what we wanted. I was rather disappointed at this, as, if it had succeeded, the plan would have opened out many interesting lines of inquiry. Still, in such a case one ought not to wish for one result more than another as, of course, the object of all such experiments is merely to elicit the truth, and our result in the present case, though negative, is very interesting. I do not, however, regard it as by any means conclusive, and should be glad to see it repeated. If the result proved to be the same, it would certainly imply very little power of combining even extremely simple ideas. Can ANIMALS COUNT ? I then endeavoured to get some insight into the arithmetical condition of the dog’s mind. On this subject I have been able to find but little in any of the standard works on the intelligence of animals, Considering, however, the very limited powers of savage men in this respect—that no Australian language, for instance, contains numerals even up to four, no Australian being able to count his own fingers even on one hand—we cannot be surprised if other animals have made but little progress. Still, it is curious that so little attention should have been directed to this subject. Leroy, who, though he ex- presses the opinion that “the nature of the soul of animals is unimportant,” was an excellent observer, mentions a case in which a man was anxious to shoot 282 PREVIOUS OBSERVATIONS. acrow. “To deceive this suspicious bird, the plan was hit upon of sending two men to the watch-house, one of whom passed on, while the other remained; but the crow counted, and kept her distance. The next day three went, and again she perceived that only two retired. In fine, it was found necessary to send five or six men to the watch-house to put her out in her calculation. The crow, thinking that this number of men had passed by, lost no time in returning.” From this he inferred that crows could count up to four. Lichtenberg mentions a nightingale which was said to count up to three. Every day he gave it three meal- worms, one at a time; when it had finished one it returned for another, but after the third it knew that the feast was over. I do not find that any of the recent works on the intelligence of animals, either Buchner, or Peitz, or Romanes in either of his books, give any additional evidence on this part of the subject. There are, however, various scattered notices. According to my bird-nesting recollections, which I have refreshed by more recent experience, if a nest contains four eggs, one may safely be taken; but if two are removed, the bird generally deserts. Here, then, it would seem as if we had some reason for supposing that there is sufficient intelligence to distinguish three from four. sn interesting consideration rises also with refer- ence to the number of the victims allotted to each cell by the solitary wasps. Ammophila considers one large caterpillar of Noctua segetum enough; one species of Kumenes supplies its young with five victims; one ten, another fifteen, and one even as many as twenty-four. The number is said to be constant in SUPPOSED POWERS OF COUNTING. 283 each species. How, then, does the insect know when her task is fulfilled? Not by the cell being filled, for if some be removed she does not replace them. When she has brought her complement she considers her task accomplished, whether the victims are still there or not. How, then, does she know when she has made up the number twenty-four? Perhaps it will be said that each species feels some mysterious and innate tendency to provide a certain number of victims. This would not under any circumstances be an ex- planation, nor is it in accordance with the facts. In the genus Eumenes the males are much smaller than the females. Now, in the hive bees, humble bees, wasps, and other insects where such a differ- ence occurs, but where the young are directly fed, it is, of course, obvious that the quantity can be pro- portioned to the appetite of the grub. But in insects with the habits of Eumenes and Ammophila the case is different, because the food is stored up once for all. Now, it is evident that if a female grub was supplied with only food enough for a male, she would starve to death ; while if a male grub were given enough for a female it would have too much. No such waste, how- ever, occurs. In some mysterious manner the mother knows whether the egg will produce a male or female grub, and apportions the quantity of food accordingly. She does not change the species or size of her prey; but if the egg is male she supplies five, if female ten, victims. Does she count? Certainly this seems very like acommencement of arithmetic. At the same time, it would be very desirable to have additional evidence before we can arrive at any certain conclusion. Considering how much has been written on instinct, 284 MR. HUGGINS’S EXPERIMENT. it seems surprising that so little attention has been directed to this part of the subject. One would fancy that there ought to be no great difficulty in determining how far an animal can count; and whether, for in- stance, it could realize some very simple sum, such as that two and two make four. But when we come to consider how this is to be done, the problem ceases to appear so simple. We tried our dogs by putting a piece of bread before them, and preventing them from touching it until we had counted seven. To prevent ourselves from unintentionally giving any indication, we used a metronome (the instrument used for marking time when practising the pianoforte), and to make the beats more evident we attached a slender rod to the pendulum. It certainly seemed as if our dogs knew when the moment of permission had arrived ; but their movement of taking the bread was scarcely so definite as to place the matter beyond a doubt. Moreover, dogs are so very quick in seizing any indication given them, even unintentionally, that, on the whole, the attempt was not satisfactory to my mind. I was the more discouraged from continuing the experiment in this manner by an account Mr. Huggins gave me ofa very intelligent dog belonging to him. A number of cards were placed on the ground, numbered respectively 1, 2, 3, and soon up to1U. A question was then asked : the square root of 9 or 16, or such a sum as 6-++55—3. Mr. Huggins pointed consecutively to the cards, and the dog always barked when he came to the right one. Now, Mr. Huggins did not consciously give the dog any sign, yet so quick was the dog in seizing the slightest indication, that he was able to give tbe correct answer. “The mode of precedure is this. His master tells CONCLUSION, 285 him to sit down, and shows him a piece of cake. He is then questioned, and barks his answers. Say he is asked what is the square root of 16, or of 9; he will bark four or three times, as the case may be. Or such asum as **13-* he will always answer correctly. The piece of cake is, of course, the meed of such cleverness. It must not be supposed that in these performances any sign is consciously made by his questioner. None whatever. We explain the per- formance by supposing that he reads ia his master’s expression when he has barked rightly; certainly he never takes his eyes from his master’s face.” * This observation seems to me of great interest in connection with the so-called “thought-reading.” No one, I suppose, will imagine that there was in this case any “thought-reading” in the sense in which this word is generally used. Evidently “Kepler” seized upon some slight indication unintentionally given by Mr. Huggins. The observation, however, shows the great.difficulty of the subject. The experiments I have made are, I feel, very incomplete, but I have ventured to place them on record, partly in hope of receiving some suggestions, and partly in hope of inducing others with more leisure and opportunity to carry on similar observa- tions, which I cannot but think must lead to interesting results, * M. L. Huggins, “Kepler: a Biography.” INDEX. A Acalles, 66 Acanthopleura, 15, 145 Acheta, 61, 63, 97, 117 Acridiide, 100, 106 Actinia, 13 Ageronia, 73 Aglaura, 188 Alciopide, 14, 22, 137 Ammophila, 243, 282 Amphibia, 32, 129 Amphicora, 87 Amphioxus, 129 Angler, 186 Anguis, 126 Annelides, touch, 135 taste, 22; smell, 34; hearing, 87; sight, 134; problematical organs, 189 Anobium, 67 Anoxia, 251 Anthidium, 71 Anthrax, 251 Ants, 24, 31, 43, 56, 69, 107, 115, 178, 202 Apion, 94 Apis, 26, 29, 58, 69, 70, 115, 150, 172, 194, 258, 283 Arca, 141 Arenicola, 87 Arithmetic of animals, 281 Arthropods, touch, 16; taste, 23; smell, 35; hearing, 88; sight, 146 ; problematical organs, 188 Articulata. Sze Annelides, Insects Ascidians, 129 Asellus, 48 Astacus, 23, 51, 88 Asteracanthion, 133 Asterope, 22 Astropecten, 132 Ateuches, 66 Auditory hairs, 16, 79, 85, 28, 118 —— organs, 77 — rods, 18, 104, 111 5 Balanus, 220 Bee, hive. See Apis Bee, solitary, 242 Beetles. See Coleoptera Bembex, 242, 246 Birds, 129, 282 Blatta, 46, 152 Blethisa, 68 Blind spot in eye, 125 Bohemilla, 13, 134 Bombardier beetle, 65 Bombus, 28, 70, 73, 178, 283 Bostrychida, 67 Brachinus, 64, 68 Brachyura, 90 Butterfly. Sce Lepidoptera c Calanella, 159 Calotis, 127 Callianassa, 50 288 Camponotus, 208 Capitellide, 34 Capricorn beetle, 96 Carcinus, 92 Cards, Van and his, 277 Carinaria, 87 Caterpillars, 23, 243, 259 Cats, 262 Centipedes, 49, 74 Cephalopoda, 34, 141 Cerambys, 67, 95, 96 Ceratius, 186 Ceratophyus, 68 Chalcidide, 27 Chalicodoma, 251, 262 Chiasognathus, 68 Chitons, 15, 144 Cicadas, 61, 64, 151 Cicadide, 151 Clepsine, 134 Cockchafer. See Melolontha Cockroach, 46, 152 Coelenterata, touch, 11; taste, 22; smell, 33; hearing, 82; sight, 131 Coleoptera, 58, 67, 111, 151 Collie, 280 Color of deep-sea fish, 185 of flowers, 199 » sense of, 190, 194, 202, 280 Componotus, 239 Compound eyes, 163 Copepoda, 48 Copilia, 158 Copris, 68, 95 Corephium, 145 Corethra, 18, 113, 117, 151 Corixa, 75 Corti, the organ of, 80, 105 Cory cus, 157 Cossus, 148 Count ? can animals, 281 Crabs, 90, 92 Crayfish. ce Astacus Cricket. See Acheta Crioceris, 68 Crow, 282 Crustacea, touch, 16; taste, 23; smell, 46 ; hearing, 88; sight, 156; sense of color, 211; problematical organs, 188 Crystalline cone, 166 INDEX Culex, 68, 115 Curculionidae, 68 Cychrus, 68 Cyclostoma, 140 Cymbulia, 88 D Daphnia, 48, 206, 212 Dead-nettle, 200 Death-watch, 66 Dias, 220 Dinetus, 39 Diptera, 52, 69, 110, 149, 151 Direction, sense of, 262 Dog, intelligence of the, 272 Dragon-fly. See Libellula Dytiscus, 5, 6, 112, 131, 146, 167 E See Auditory organs in tail of Mysis, 92 , structure of the human, 78, 101 Earthworms, 206 Elaphrus, 68 Elaterida, 67 Empusa, 176 Endosmosis, 25 Englena, 130 Epeira, 146 Ephippigera, 103 Epithelial cells, 14, 20 Epithelium, 11, 19 Eristalis, 69, 174, 178 Encopide, 85 Eucorybar, 74 Eumenes, 245, 282 Euphausia, 161 Eurycopa, 189 Eutima, 83 Evaneada, 27 Eye, compound, 163 of man, 121 ——,, pineal, 126 , Simple, 170 Har. P Fish, 182 Flowers, 200 INDEX. Fly. See Musca Forficula, 151, 167 Formica. See Ants G Gammarus, 49, 188 Gasteropods, 86 Geotrupes, 68 Geryonia, 86 Glomeris, 50 Glossopharyngeal nerves, 19 Gnat, 68, 115 Gryllotalpa, 102 Gryllus, 63, 98, 106, 108 HH Hairs, auditory, 16, 79, 85, 88, 116 , depressed, 17 ——,, flattened, 56 ——, glandular, 29 , hollow, 17 —— in insects, 16 —— of touch. See Tactile ——,, olfactory, 16, 25 ——, ordinary surface, 16, 56 ——., plumose natatory, 16, 94 ——, simple, 18 » solid, 17, 82 — , tactile, 16, 18, 28, 29, 56 , taste, 16, 28 Haliotis, 5, 139 Hattaria, 127 Hearing, organs of, in Vertebrata, 77 ; Coelenterata, 82; Mollusca, 86; Annelida, 87; Arthropods, 88 , sense of, 60, 97 Helix, 14, 139 Hemiptera, 112, 151 Hesione, 135 Humble-bee. Hydaticus, 40 Hydrachna, 28 Hydromedusa, 86 Hydrophilus, 168 Hydrozoa, 13 Hyleus, 58 Hymenoptera, 23, 25, 56, 57, 58, 69, 70, 96, 151, 181, 250 See Bombuas 289 Hyperia, 171 Hypoderm, 5, 16 I Ichneumon, 54, 58 Ichthyosaurus, 129 Infusoria, 11 Insects, touch, 16; taste, 235; smell, 35, 52; hearing, 61, 943; sight, 146; problematical organs, 188 Instinct— Ant, 202, 232, 267 Bee, hive, 194, 253 , solitary, 255, 260, 262 Birds, 282 Bombardier beetle, 64 Change in, 244 Crustacea, 90 Daphnia, 229 Dog, 272 Fish, 186 Fly, 174, 177 Limitation of, 253 Of direction, 262 Onchidium, 144 Paussus, 65 Wasp, solitary, 243, 282 Isopteryx, 109 J Jelly-fish. See Medusa Julus, 49 L Labyrinthodons, 129 Lacerta, 126, 128 Lamellibranchiata, 14, 141 Lamellicornia, 37, 52 Lamium, 200 Lampyris, 167 -Lancelet, 129 Lasius. See Ants Laura Bridgman, 273 Leech, 189 Lema, 68 Lepidoptera, 37, 71, 94, 111, 148, 151, 168, 181 Leptodora, 156 290 Leucospis, 251 Libellula, 69, 70, 149, 152, 171 Light-organs, 161, 185 Ligia, 167 Limitation of instinct, 253 Limpet, 4, 138 Limulus, 159 Lithobius, 155 Lizzia, 132 Lobster, 90, 91 Locusts, 62, 99, 106, 111, 149, 176 Longicorn beetles, 66, 95 Lucanus, 43, 52 Lucilia, 177 Lycosa, 179 Lyda, 58 M Mammals, 129 Maxille, 25 Meconema, 102, 105 Medusa, 6, 22, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86, 117 Meissner’s corpuscles, 7 Melolontha, 52, 58, 67, 68, 148, 152, 168 Mesonotum, 67 Metronome, 284 Miltogramma, 254 Mollusca, 14, 22, 34, 61, 86, 120, 137, 140 Mordella, 148 Mosaic vision, 163 Mosquito. See Culex Moths. See Lepidoptera Murex, 139 Musca, 17, 29, 30, 45, 53, 58, 68, 71, 110, 113, 148, 153, 165, 172, 174, 177, 254 Mutilla, 69, 70 Myriapods, 155, 205 Myrmica. See Ants Mysis, 92, 98, 157, 161 N Nautilus, 140 Necrophorus, 66, 68 Needle cells, 21 Nematocera, 151 INDEX. Nematocysts, 12 Nereis, 12, 135 Nesticus, 180 Neuroptera, 111, 151 Newts, 207 Noctua, 73, 243, 282 0 Oceanida, 86 Ocypoda, 61 Odynerus, 247 Cstrus, 148 Olfactory organs. smell Omaloplia, 68 Onchidium, 14, 131, 143 Oniscoide, 170 Ontorchis, 6, 84 Organs of hearing, 17, 19, 77, 81, 93, 109, 114 of sight, 19, 130, 146 of smell, 17, 88 of taste, 17, 19, 21 —— of temperature, 6, 10 —— of touch, 11, 14, 17, 19, 181 » problematical, 182 Origin of organs of sense, 3 Orthoptera, 37, 99, 107, 112, 181, 176 Oryctes, 68 Osmia, 251 Otolithes, 52, 82, 84, 85, 89, 90, 91, 92 See Organs of , possible origin of, 3 P Pacinian corpuscle, 8 Pagurus, 51 Palemon, 51 Palinurus, 61 Palpi, 30, 37, 38, 39, 41, 73 Paludina, 140 Pamphila, 184 Paniscus, 58 Patella, 138, 140 Paussus, 65 Pectens, 61, 141 Pectunculus, 141 Pelagia, 86 Pelobius, 68 Periplaneta, 152 Perophthalmus, 144 Pheidole, 108 Phialidium, 85 Photichthys, 185 Pineal eye, 127 Pinnotheres, 51 Piscicola, 134 Platyarthrus, 207 Plesiosaurus, 129 Pleuromona, 189 Podophthalmata, 50, 156 Polydesmus, 189 Polyophthalmus, 33, 98, 134 Pompilus, 58 Ponera, 69 Pontella, 47, 48 Pontinia, 51 Poodle dog, 276 Pressure-point, 10 Prionus, 67 Proctotrupide, 27 Pronotum, 67 Prosobranchiata, 138 Protoplasm, 21 Protozoa, 32, 61 Pteropods, 87 Ptychoptera, 113 B Recognition among ants, 234 Reptilia, 127, 130 Respiration in insects, 35 Retina, 123 Rhopalonema, 85 Rods, auditory, 18, 104, 111, 187 , olfactory, 55 ——,, retinal, 124 s Salivary gland, 30 Sarcophaga, 111 Schizochiton, 145 Scolopendra, 155 Scopelus, 186 Scorpions, 179 Sea-anemone, 12, 187 INDEX. 291 Sense-hairs. Sce Hairs Sense of direction, 262 Sense-organs, origin of, 3, 86, 111 Senses, unknown, 192 Serolis, 189 Sete. See Hairs Sex, power of regulating, 262 Sight, organs of, in Vertebrata, 121; Celenterata, 131; Annelida, 133; Mollusca, 137; Arthropods, 146 , sense of, 118 ——, three possible modes of, 118 Silpha, 38, 41 Sirex, 58 Skin, termination of nerves in, 18 Smell, organs of, in Vertebrata, 32; Protozoa, 33; Coelenterata, 33; Annelida, 33; Mollusca, 34; Arthro- pods, 35 Smerinthus, 73 Solaster, 133 Sound, organs of, not known in Pro- tozoa or Coelenterata, 61; Mollusca, 61; Crustacea, 61; Insects, 62 Sphex, 245 Sphinx, 73, 148 Spherotherium, 74 Spiders, 74, 146, 155, 170, 178 Spondylis, 67, 141 Squilla, 51 Stag-beetle, 43, 52 Staphylinus, 50 Stenobothrus, 62, 63 Stratiomys, 167 Syrphus, 69, 170 T Tachytes, 246 Taste, organs of, in Vertebrata, 19; Annelida, 22; Mollusca, 22; Ar- thropods, 23 Telephorus, 112 Temperature, organs of, 10 Tenebrionida, 68 Tenthredo, 27, 58 Theridium, 75 Touch, organs of, in Vertebrata, 7 ; Protozoa, 11; Celenterata, 11; Meduse, 12; Annelida, 13; Mol- lusca, 14; Arthropods, 16 292 INDEX. Touch, sense of, 7 Varanus, 127 Trachex, 29, 30, 101 Vaterian corpuscles, 7 Trachymeduse, 85 Vertebrata, 7, 19, 32, 77 Trachynemade, 187 Vespa, 28, 55, 58, 175, 178, 283 Tritonia, 87 Trochus, 138 Trox, 68 wv Tunicata, 129 Wagner’s corpuscles, 7 Turbellaria, 133 Warmth organs, 6, 10 Wasp. See Vespa Vv , solitary, 242, 282 Weevils, 67, 94 Van, 276 Wolffian glands, 27 Vanessa, 73, 174 Worms. See Annelidea THE END, D, APPLETON & @0,’8 PUBLICATIONS, SIR JOHN LUBBOCK’S (Bart.) WORKS. THE ORIGIN OF CIVILIZATION AND THE PRIMI TIVE CONDITION OF MAN, MENTAL AND SOCIAL ‘CONDITION OF SAVAGES. Fourth edition, with numerous Ad. ditions, With Illustrations. 8vo. Cloth, $5.00. “The first edition of this work was published in the year 187). The work has been twice revised for the press in the interval, and nuw appears in its ee enlarged to the extent of nearly two hundred vages, including a full index. “This interesting work—for it is intensely so in its aim, scope, and the abil- ity of its author—treats of what the scientists denominate anthropology, or tha natural history of the human species ; the complete science of man, body and soul, including sex, temperament, race, civilization, etc.”—Providence Press. PREHISTORIC TIMES, AS ILLUSTRATED BY ANCIENT REMAINS AND THE MANNERS AND CUSTOMS OF MODERN SAVAGES. Illustrated. Entirely new revised edition. 8vo. Cloth, $5.00. The book ranks among the noblest works of the interesting and important class to which it belongs. As a résumé of our present knowledge of prehistoric man, it leaves nothing to be desired. It is not only a good book of reference but the best on the subject. “This is, perhaps, the best summary of evidence now in our possession con- comnts general character of prehistoric times. The Bronze Age, The Stone Age, The Tumuli, The Lake Inhabitants of Switzerland, The Shell Mounds, The Cave Man, und The tay of Man, are the titles of the most important chap- ters."—Dr. C. K. Adams's Manual of Historical Literature. ANTS, BEES, AND WASPS. A Record of Observations on the Habits of the Social Hymenoptera. With Colored Plates. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. “This volume contains the record of various experiments made with ante, bees, and wasps during the last ten years, with a view to test their mental con- dition and powers of sense. The principal point ip which Sir John’s mode of experiment differs from those of Huber, Forel, McCook, and others, is that he has carefully watched and marked particular insects, and has had their nests under observation for long periods—one of his ants’ nests having been under constant inspection ever since 1874, His observations are made principally upon ants, because they show more power and flexibility of mind; and the value of his studies 1s that they belong to the department of original research.’ “We have no hesitation in saying that the author has presented us with the most valuable series of observations on a special subject that has ever been pro- duced, charmingly written, full of logical deductions, and, when we consider his multitudmous engagements, a remarkable illustration of economy oftime. Ara contribution to insect psychology, it will be long before this book finds a par: allel.’"—London Atheneum. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1,3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO0,’8 PUBLICATIONS. Professor JOSEPH LE CONTE’S WORKS. EVOLUTION AND ITS RELATION TO RELIGIOUS THOUGHT. By Joseru Le Cons, LL. D., Professor of Geology and Natural History in the University of California. With numer- ous Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. “Much, very much has been written, especially on the natnre and the evi- dences of evolution, but the literature is 80 voluminous, much of it so fragment- ary, and most of it so technical, that even very intelligent persons have still very vague ideas on the subject. I have attempted to give (1) a very concise account of what we mean by evolution, (2) an outline of the evidences of its truth drawn from many different sources, and (3) its relation to fundamental religious beliefs.”* —Extract from Preface. ELEMENTS OF GEOLOGY. A Text-book for Colleges and for the General Reader. By Josern Le Conte, LL.D. With upward of 900 Illustrations. New and enlarged edition. 8vo. Cloth, $4.00. “ Besides preparing a comprehensive text-book, suited to present demands, Professor Le Conte has given us a volume of great value as an exposition of the subject, thorougbly up todate. The examples and applications of the work are almost entirely derived from this country, so that it may, be properly considered an American geology. We can commend this work without qualification to all who desire an intelligent acquaintance with geological science, as fresh, lucid, full, authentic, the result of devoted study and of long experience in teaching.” —Popular Science Monthly. RELIGION AND SCIENCE. A Series of Sunday Lectures on the Relation of Natural and Revealed Religion, or the Truths revealed in Nature and Scripture. By JoserpH Le Conrz, LL.D. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. “We commend the book cordially to the regard of all who are interested in whatever pertains to the discussion of these grave questions, and especially to those who desire to examine closely the strong foundations on which the Chris- tian faith is reared.”—Boston Journal. SIGHT: An Exposition of the Principles of Monocular and Binocular Vision. By Josep Le Conts, LL.D. With Illustrations. i2mo. Cloth, $1.50. ‘* Professor Le Conte has long been known as an original investigator in this department: all that he gives us is treated with a master-hand. It is pleasant to find an American book that can rauk with the very best of foreign books on this subject.’"— Zhe Nation. COMPEND OF GEOLOGY. By Joseru Le Conte, LL. D, 12mo, Cloth, $1.40. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D, APPLETON & C0,’8 PUBLICATIONS. ALEXANDER BAIN’S WORKS. THE SENSES AND THE INTELLECT. By ALexanpDER Barn. LL. D., Professor of Logic in the University of Aberdeen. 8vo. Cloth, $5.00. The object of this treatise ia to give a full and systematic account of two principal divisions of the science of mind—the senses and the intellect. The value of the third edition of the work is greatly enhanced by an account of the psychology of Aristotle, which has been contributed by Mr. Grote. THE EMOTIONS AND THE WILL. By ALExanpeR Bain, LL.D. 8vo. Cloth, $5.00. The present publication is a sequel to the former one on ‘‘ The Senses and the Intellect,” and completes a systematic exposition of the human mind. MENTAL SCIENCE. A Compendium of Psychology and the His- tory of Philosophy. Designed as a Text-book for High-Schools and Colleges. By ALexanpeR Bain, LL.D. 12mo, Cloth, leather back, $1.50. The present volume is an abstract of two voluminous works, ‘‘The Senses and the Intellect” and ‘The Emotions and the Will,” and presents in a com- pressed and lucid form the views which are there more extensively elaborated. MORAL SCIENCE. A Compendium of Ethics. By ALEXANDER Bary, LL.D. 12mo. Cloth, leather back, $1.60. The present dissertation falls under two divisions. The first division, en- titled The Theory of Ethics, gives an account of the questions or points brought into discussion, and handles at length the two of greatest prominence, the Ethical Standard and the Moral Faculty. The second division—on the Ethical Systems —is a full detail of all the systems, ancient and modern, MIND AND BODY. Theories of their Relations. By ALEXANDER Bain, LL.D. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. “A forcible statement of the connection between mind and body, studying their subtile interworkings by the light of the most recent physiological investi- gations.” — Christian Register. LOGIC, DEDUCTIVE AND INDUCTIVE. By ALEXANDER Barn, LL.D. Revised edition. 12mo. Cloth, leather back, $2.00. EDUCATION AS A SCIENCE. By Auexanver Bain, LL.D. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. ENGLISH COMPOSITION AND RHETORIC. Enlarged edition. Part I. Intellectual Elements of Style. By ALExaNDER Barn, LL.D., Emeritus Professor of Logic in the University of Aberdeen. 12mo. Cloth, leather back, $1.50. ON TEACHING ENGLISH. With Detailed Examples and an Inquiry into the Definition of Poetry. By ALexanper Bain, LL. D. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25. PRACTICAL ESSAYS. By Arexanper Bain, LL.D. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & C0,’8 PUBLICATIONS. DR. HENRY MAUDSLEY’S WORKS. BODY AND WILL: Being an Essay concerning Will in its Metaphysical, Physiological, and Pathological Aspects. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. BODY AND MIND: An Inquiry into their Connection and Mutual Influence, specially in reference to Mental Disorders. 1 vol., 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. PHYSIOLOGY AND PATIOLOGY OF MIND: PHYSIOLOGY OF THE MIND. New edition. 1 vol., 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. Coytenrs: Chapter I. On the Method of the Study of the Mind.—II. The Mind and the Nervous System.—III. The Spinal Cord, or Tertiary Nervous Centres; or, Nervous Centres of Reflex Action.—IV. Secondary Nervous Centres; or, Sensory Ganglia; Sensorium Commune.—V. Hemispherical Ganglia; Cortical Cells of the Cerebral Hemispheres; Ideational Nervous Centres; Primary Nervous Centres; Intellectorium Commune.—VI. The Emotions.—VII. Volition.—VIII. Motor Nervous Centres, or Mo- torium Cummune and Actuation or Effection—IX. Memory and Imagination. PATHOLOGY OF THE MIND. Being the Third Edition of the Second Part of the “Physiology and Pathology of Mind,” recast, enlarged, and rewritten. 1 vol.,12mo. Cloth, $2.00. Con- tents: Chapter I. Sleep and Dreaming.—II. Hypnotism, Somnam- bulism, and Allied States—IHI. The Causation and Prevention of Insanity: (A) Etiological—IV. The same continued—V. The Causation and Prevention of Insanity: (B) Pathological.—VI. The Insanity of Early Life—VII. The Symptomatology of Insanity.— VIII. The same continued.—IX. Clinical Groups of Mental Disease. —xX. The Morbid Anatomy of Mental Derangement.—XI. The Treatment of Mental Disorders. RESPONSIBILITY IN MENTAL DISEASE. (International Scientific Series.) 1 vol.,12mo. Cloth, $1.50. _ ‘The author is at home in his subject, and presents his views in an almost singularly clear and satisfactory manner. ... The volume isa valuable contri- bution to one of the most difficult and at the same time one of the most impor- tant subjects of investigation at the present day."—New York Observer. ‘Handles the important topic with masterly power, and its suggestions are practical and of great value.”"— Providence Press. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & CO0,’8 PUBLICATIONS, GEORGE J. ROMANES’S WORKS. JELLY-FISH, STAR-FISH, AND SEA-URCHINS. Being a Research on Primitive Nervous Systems. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. “Although I have throughout kept in view the requirements of a general reader, I have alsu sought to render the book of service to the working physi- ologist, by bringing together in one consecutive account all the more important observations and results which have been yielded by this research.’—Aatract From Preface. ‘““A profound research into the laws of primitive nervous systems conducted by one of the ablest English investigators, Mr. Romanes set up a tent on the beach and examined his beantiful pets tor six summere in succession. Such patient and loving work has borne its fruits in a monograph which leaves nothing to be said about jelly-fish, star-fish, and sea-urchins. Every one who has studied the lowest forms of life on the sea-shore admires these objects. But few have any idea of the exquisite delicacy of their structure and their nice adaptation to their place in nature. Mr. Romanes brings out the subtile beauties of the rudimentary organisms, and shows the resemblances they bear to the higher types of creation. His explanations are made more clear by a large uumber of illustrations. While the book is well adapted for popular reading it is of special value to working physiologists.’—New York Journal of Commerce. ‘‘ A most admirable treatise on primitive nervous systems. The eubject-matter is fall of pee investigations and experiments upon the animals mentioned as types of the lowest nervous developments.” — Boston Commercial Bulletin. ‘““Mr, George J. Romanes has already established a reputation as an exact and comprehensive naturalist, which his later work, ‘Jelly-Fish, Star-Fish, and Sea- Urchins,’ fully confirms. These marine animals are well known upon onr coasts. and always interest the on-lookers, In this volume (one of the ‘International Scientific Series ’) we have the whole story of their formation, existence, nervous system, etc., made most interesting by the simple and non-professional manner of treating the subject. LZlustrations aid the text, and the professional student. the naturalist, all lovers of the rocks, woods, and shore, as well as the general reader, ae find instruction as well as delight in the narrative.”"—Boston Com- monwealth. ANIMAL INTELLIGENCE. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. “A collection of facts which, though it may merely amuse the unscientific reader, will be a real boon to the student of comparative psychology, for this is the first attempt to present systematically the well-assured results of observation ou the mental life of animals."—Saturday Review. MENTAL EVOLUTION IN ANIMALS. With a Posthumous Essay on Instinct, by CHartes Darwin. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. ‘Mr. Romanes has followed up his careful enumeration of the facts of * Ani- mal Intelligence,’ contributed to the ‘International Scientific Series,’ with a work dealing with the successive stages at which the various mental phenomena appear in the scale of life. The present installment displays the same evidence of industry in collecting facts and caution in co-ordinating them by theory as the former.”""— The Atheneum. New York:, D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D, APPLETON & C0.’8 PUBLICATIONS, ERNST HAECKEL’S WORKS. THE HISTORY OF CREATION; OR, THE DEVELOP- MENT OF THE EARTH AND ITS INHABITANTS BY THE ACTION OF NATURAL CAUSES. A Popular Exposition of the Doctrine of Evolution in general, and of that of Darwin, Goethe, and Lamarck in particular. From the German of Ernst HarckEL, Professor in the University of Jena. The translation revised by Professor E. Ray Lankester, M.A., F.R.S., Fellow of Exeter Col- lege, Oxford. Illustrated with Lithographic Plates. In two vols., 12mo. Cloth, $5.00. THE EVOLUTION OF MAN. A Popular Exposition of the Principal Points of Human Ontogeny and Phylogeny. From the German of Ernst Harcxet, Professor in the University of Jena, author of ‘The History of Creation,” ete. With numerous Illus- trations. In two vols.,12mo. Cloth. Price, $5.00. “In this excellent translation of Professor Haeckel’s work, the Eng- lish reader has access to the latest doctrines of the Continental school of evolution, in its application to the history of man. It is in Germany, be- yond any other European country, that the impulse given by Darwin twenty years ago to the theory of evolution has influenced the whole tenor of philosophical opinion. There may be, and are, differences in the degree to which the doctrine may be held capable of extension into the domain of mind and morals; but there is no denying, in scientific circles at least, that as regards the physical history of organic nature much has been done toward making good a continuous scheme of being.” —London Saturday Review, FREEDOM IN SCIENCE AND TEACHING. From the German of Ernst Haucxen, With a Prefatory Note by T. H. Hoxiey, F.R.S. 12mo. $1.00. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street, D, APPLETON & €0,’8 PUBLICATIONS, CHARLES DARWIN’S WORKS. ORIGIN OF SPECIES BY MEANS OF NATURAL SELECTION, OR THE PRESERVATION OF FA- VORED RACES IN THE STRUGGLE FOR LIFE. Revised edition, with Additions, 12mo,. Cloth, $2.00. DESCENT OF MAN, AND SELECTION IN RELATION TO SEX. With many Illustrations, A new edition. 12mo. Cloth, $3.00. JOURNAL OF RESEARCHES INTO THE NATURAL HISTORY AND GEOLOGY OF COUNTRIES VIS- ITED DURING THE VOYAGE OF H. M.S. BEAGLE ROUND THE WORLD. New edition. 12mo, Cloth, $2.00. EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN AND THE LOWER ANIMALS. 12mo. Cloth, $3.50. THE VARIATIONS OF ANIMALS AND PLANTS UNDER DOMESTICATION. With a Preface, by Professor Asa Gray. 2vols. Illustrated. Cloth, $5.00. INSECTIVOROUS PLANTS. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. MOVEMENTS AND HABITS OF CLIMBING PLANTS. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25. THE VARIOUS CONTRIVANCES BY WHICH ORCHIDS ARE FERTILIZED BY INSECTS. Revised edition, with Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. THE EFFECTS OF CROSS AND SELF FERTILIZA- TION IN THE VEGETABLE KINGDOM. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. DIFFERENT FORMS OF FLOWERS ON PLANTS OF THE SAME SPECIES. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. THE POWER OF MOVEMENT IN PLANTS. By Cuartes Darwin, LL. D., F.R.S., assisted by Francis Darwin. With Illus- trations. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. THE FORMATION OF VEGETABLE MOULD THROUGH THE ACTION OF WORMS. With Observations on their Habits. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & C0,’8 PUBLICATIONS. THOMAS H. HUXLEY’S WORKS. SCIENCE AND CULTURE, AND OTHER ESSAYS. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. THE CRAYFISH: AN INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY OF ZOOLOGY. With 82 Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. SCIENCE PRIMERS: INTRODUCTORY. 18mo. Flexible cloth, 45 cents. MAN°’S PLACE IN NATURE. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25. ON THE ORIGIN OF SPECIES. 12mo. Cloth, $1.00. MORE CRITICISMS ON DARWIN, AND ADMINISTRATIVE NIHILISM. 12mo. Limp cloth, 50 cents. MANUAL OF THE ANATOMY OF VERTEBRATED ANIMALS. Illustrated. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. MANUAL OF THE ANATOMY OF INVERTEBRATED ANIMALS. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. LAY SERMONS, ADDRESSES, AND REVIEWS. 12mo. Cloth, $1.75. CRITIQUES AND ADDRESSES. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. AMERICAN ADDRESSES; WITH A LECTURE ON THE STUDY OF BIOLOGY. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25, PHYSIOGRAPHY: AN INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY OF NATURE, With Illustrations and Colored Plates. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. HUXLEY AND YOUMANS’S ELEMENTS OF PHYSIOL- OGY AND HYGIENE. Bs T. H. Hoxtey and W. J. Youmans, 12mo, Cloth, $1.50. New York: D, APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street. D. APPLETON & C0,’S PUBLICATIONS. JOHN TYNDALL’S WORKS. ESSAYS ON THE FLOATING MATTER OF THE AIR, in Relation to Putrefaction and Infection. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. ON FORMS OF WATER, in Clouds, Rivers, Ice, and Glaciers. With 35 Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $1.50. HEAT AS A MODE OF MOTION. New edition. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. ON SOUND: A Course of Eight Lectures delivered at the Royal Institution of Great Britain. JIllustrated. 12mo. New edition. Cloth, $2.00. FRAGMENTS OF SCIENCE FOR UNSCIENTIFIC PEO- PLE. 12mo. New revised and enlarged edition. Cloth, $2.50. LIGHT AND ELECTRICITY. 12mo. Cloth, $1.25. LESSONS IN ELECTRICITY, 1875-76. 12mo. Cloth, $1.00 HOURS OF EXERCISE IN THE ALPS. With Illustrations. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. FARADAY AS A DISCOVERER. A Memoir. 12mo. Cloth, $1.00. CONTRIBUTIONS TO MOLECULAR PHYSICS in the Do main of Radiant Heat. $5.00. SIX LECTURES ON LIGHT. Delivered in America in 1872- 73. With an Appendix and numerous Illustrations. Cloth, $1.50. FAREWELL BANQUET, given to Professor Tyndall, at Del- monico’s, New York, February 4, 1873. Paper, 50 cents. ADDRESS delivered before the British Association, assembled at Bel. fast. Revised with Additions, by the author, since the Delivery 12mo. Paper, 50 cents. sesh New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1,3, & 5 Bond Street. D, APPLETON & CO0,’8 PUBLICATIONS, Dr. H. ALLEYNE NICHOLSON’S WORKS. TEXT-BOOK OF ZOOLOGY, for Schools and Colleges. 12mo. Half roan, $1.60. MANUAL OF ZOOLOGY, for the Use of Students, with a Gen- eral Introduction to the Principles of Zodlogy. Second edition. Revised and enlarged, with 248 Woodcuts. 12mo. Cloth, $2.50. TEXT-BOOK OF GEOLOGY, for Schools and Colleges. 12mo. Half roan, $1.25. INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY OF BIOLOGY. Illustrated. 12mo. Cloth, 60 cents. THE ANCIENT LIFE-HISTORY OF THE EARTH. A Comprehensive Outline of the Principles and Leading Facts of Paleontological Science. 12mo. Cloth, $2.00. “ A work by a master in the science who understands the significance of every phenomenon which he records, and knows how to make it reveal its lessons. As regards its value there can scarcely exist two opinions. As a text-book of the historical phase of paleontology iv will be indispensable to students, whether specially pursuing geology or biology; and without it no man who aspires even to an outline now ede of natural science can deem his library complete.’—The Quarterly Journal of Science. “The Professor of Natural History in the University of St. Andrews has, by his previous works on zodlogy and paleontology, so fully estab- lished his claim to be an exact thinker and a close reasoner, that scarcely any recommendation of ours can add to the interest with which all students in natural history will receive the present volume. It is, as its second title expresses it, a comprehensive outline of the principles and leading facts of paleontological science. Numerous woodcut illustrations very delicately executed, a copious glossary, and an admirable index, add much to the value of this volume.?’— Atheneum. New York: D. APPLETON & CO., 1, 3, & 5 Bond Street.