Orientation: Workplace bullying is characterised as a counterproductive interpersonal behaviour, yielding severe consequences
for both the individual and the organisation. The occurrence of workplace bullying is often attributed to a stressful work environment. Research purpose: The purpose of the study was to test the work environment hypothesis by applying the Job Demands-Resources model
to workplace bullying. We expected job demands and job resources to relate to both perpetrators' and actors' reports of workplace bullying. Motivation for the study: We aimed to extend the outcomes examined in the Job Demands-Resources model to a specific form of
counterproductive interpersonal behaviour, namely workplace bullying. From the point of view of the literature on bullying, we aimed
to substantiate the well-known work environment hypothesis with empirical data. Research design, approach and method: We applied structural equation modelling on questionnaire data of a large heterogeneous
sample of Flemish employees (N = 749). Main findings: Job demands and job resources interacted in the prediction of perpetrators' reports of bullying: job demands
associated positively to perpetrators' reports of bullying particularly under the condition of high job resources. Job demands related
positively to targets' reports of bullying, while job resources related negatively. These associations were (partially) mediated by
emotional exhaustion. Practical/managerial implications: These results suggest that workplace bullying may indeed be reduced by good job design,
that is, by limiting the job demands and increasing job resources. Particular prevention plans may be developed for exhausted employees,
as they are vulnerable to workplace bullying, in terms of both becoming perpetrators and victims. Contribution/value-add: This study attests to the predictive validity of the JD-R model for perpetrators' and targets' reports
of workplace bullying. The findings also underline the complex and multi-causal nature of workplace bullying.
Recently, an exponential increase of publications has focussed on the Job Demands-Resources model
(JD-R model; Bakker & Demerouti, 2007). The JD-R model examines the impact of job characteristics
(i.e. job demands and job resources) on workers' impaired (i.e. burnout) and optimal (i.e. work engagement)
work-related well-being. To date, the JD-R model has been fruitful in explaining a range of outcomes such as,
workers' health and well-being, their attitudes towards the job and their behaviour, amongst others. Therefore,
the JD-R model may also be a valuable model to predict the organisations' productivity. The current study aims
to add to this line of research by examining the JD-R model's predictive value for a particular form of
counterproductive interpersonal behaviour, that is, workplace bullying. Specifically, the current study
aims to associate job demands and job resources as detailed in the JD-R model to both the perpetrators'
and targets' perspective of workplace bullying (Hoel, Zapf & Cooper, 2002; Matthiesen & Einarsen, 2007). Reasons for introducing workplace bullying as a possible outcome in the JD-R model are twofold. In view of
the JD-R model, to date, a limited number of studies have focussed on counterproductive and/or interpersonal
behaviour at work. Yet, over the last decade, organisations have gradually shifted towards jobs in which
workers are increasingly interdependent (Grant & Parker, 2009). Consequently, it may be valuable to
know whether well-established frameworks, such as the JD-R model, may be successful in understanding
(counterproductive) interpersonal behaviour (Hoel et al., 2002). Of particular resonance may
then be workplace bullying, which is an indicator of counterproductive interpersonal behaviour at work
(Zapf & Einarsen, 2003). Workplace bullying has attracted a growing body of scholarly attention
(Einarsen, Matthiesen, & Hauge, 2009) and has been linked to substantial implications for both the
employees and the organisation involved (Hoel et al., 2002). In view of workplace bullying, the literature may clearly benefit from a JD-R approach as perpetrators'
and targets' reports of bullying have been associated with a range of job characteristics such as workload
and autonomy (Hauge, Skogstad & Einarsen, 2007; Notelaers, De Witte & Einarsen, 2010) as well as
with work-related strain (Baillien, Neyens, De Witte & De Cuyper, 2009; Bowling & Beehr, 2006).
Firstly, the JD-R model may advance this line of research by modelling the fairly cluttered plethora of
job characteristics sampled in previous research. Secondly, the JD-R model may furthermore substantiate
from a theoretical point of view how these job characteristics may lead to perpetrators' and targets'
reports of bullying through strain.
The Job-Demands Resources model
The JD-R model expands earlier stress models such as the Job Demand Control Model (Karasek, 1979) and the Effort
Reward Imbalance Model (Siegrist, 1996). It assumes that a broad set of job characteristics (categorised as
job demands or job resources) may influence work related well-being, which in turn relates to individual and
organisational outcomes. Job demands (e.g. workload and role conflict) are those aspects of
the work context that tax workers' personal capacities. By wearing out workers' energy, job demands
associate with psychological and/or physiological costs, such as strain and burnout (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007).
Job demands are therefore said to elicit an energy depleting process. Job resources (e.g. social support and
task autonomy) are those aspects of the work context that (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007):1. reduce job demands and their health-impairing impact
2. are functional in achieving work goals
3. stimulate personal growth, development and learning. As frequently shown in literature on burnout (Halbesleben & Buckley, 2004), job resources may decrease
burnout (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004). They may thus further intervene in the energetic process. However,
they are also motivational in nature and therefore enhance work engagement (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007),
which is characterised as the complement of burnout (González-Romá, Schaufeli, Bakker, & Lloret, 2006). Apart from their main effects, job demands and job resources may also interact. On the one hand,
job resources may buffer the health-impairing impact of job demands (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007)
to such an extent that workers experiencing high job demands may feel less burned out if they dispose of
various job resources; this assumption builds on the Job Demand Control model (Karasek, 1979). Yet, whereas
the Job Demand Control model is mainly restricted to time demands, workload, autonomy and, later, social
support, the JD-R model advocates that a broad range of job resources may buffer the impact
of various job demands. On the other hand, the JD-R model assumes that job demands may boost the
health-enhancing effect of job resources (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007); workers may increasingly benefit from
job resources when working under demanding conditions (cf. Hobfoll, 2002). Numerous studies have supported the assumptions of the JD-R model from both a cross-sectional and longitudinal
perspective and when examining the impact of job demands and resources that are only found in specific jobs
(e.g. customer contacts, violence) as well as across various jobs (e.g. workload and role problems;
Bakker & Demerouti, 2007). These studies applied the JD-R model to a diverse set of outcomes.
Firstly, job demands, job resources, burnout and work engagement have successfully been used
to gain insight into workers' well-being and health, including psychosomatic complaints (Hakanen, Bakker & Schaufeli, 2006)
and depression (Hakanen, Schaufeli & Ahola, 2008; Martinussen, Richardsen & Burke, 2007). Secondly, the JD-R model
has been applied to work-related attitudes such as organisational commitment (Hakanen et al., 2008; Llorens, Bakker,
Schaufeli & Salanova, 2006), job satisfaction (Martinussen et al., 2007) and turnover-intentions
(Bakker, Demerouti & Schaufeli, 2003). Finally, some studies paid attention to workers' absenteeism
(Bakker, Demerouti, de Boer & Schaufeli, 2003; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004) and cooperative behaviour,
including in-role performance (Bakker, Van Emmerik & Van Riet, 2008; Salanova, Agut & Peiró, 2005;
Xanthopoulou Bakker, Demerouti & Schaufeli, 2009) as well as extra-role performance (Bakker, Demerouti & Verbeke, 2004).
These studies indicated that the JD-R model is not only instructive in explaining workers' functioning, but may also be a meaningful
framework to understand organisations' productivity. The current study aims to add to this line of work by examining the JD-R model's benefit in explaining counterproductive
interpersonal behaviour at work. Specifically, we aim to investigate the JD-R model's validity for workplace bullying.
Herein, we approach workplace bullying from perpetrators' as well as from targets' perspective, which aligns with recent
developments in the workplace bullying research domain (Baillien, De Cuyper & De Witte, in press; De Cuyper,
Baillien & De Witte, 2009).
Workplace bullying
Workplace bullying, also labelled as mobbing referring to a group of employees acting negatively towards an employee
(Leymann, 1996), as emotional abuse (Keashly, 1998), as victimisation (Aquino & Thau, 2009) or as harassment
(Brodsky, 1976). It refers to a situation in which one or more workers are subjected to negative acts at work.
According to the European perspective, workplace bullying is typified as a long-term process (i.e. a minimum of six months)
in which minor negative acts accumulate to a pattern of systematic maltreatment (Einarsen, 2000; Salin, 2008).
These negative acts may concern personal issues (e.g. insults and/or humiliation) as well as work-related issues
(e.g. withholding documentation) and social isolation (Cowie, Naylor, Rivers, Smith, & Pereira, 2002).
Bullying typically results in a power imbalance between the parties involved; targets often experience problems
in defending themselves against the negative acts of perpetrators (Einarsen, Raknes, & Matthiesen, 1994;
Einarsen, 2000; Leymann, 1996) and are gradually stigmatised into an inferior position (Einarsen &
Skogstad, 1996; Zapf & Gross, 2001). To date, most knowledge of workplace bullying has been developed from targets' experiences and perceptions
(Zapf & Einarsen, 2003). However, much can also be learned from the perpetrators' perspective (Rayner, 1999).
Indeed, when wanting to prevent workplace bullying in an adequate way, it takes more than only impacting on factors
that may make employees less prone to becoming a target. Interventions that prevent or reduce bullying enactment may
be highly necessary. Therefore, the current study aims to examine both perpetrators' and targets' reports of bullying,
which aligns with a recent development in the workplace bullying research domain (Baillien et al., in press). Personal characteristics such as, amongst others, low social competencies or a neurotic personality, may make
employees more prone to bullying (Parkins, Fishbein & Ritchey, 2006; Zapf, 1999). Most studies detailing
bullying antecedents were, however, inspired by the work environment hypothesis, which attributes bullying to a
stressful work environment (i.e. Einarsen et al., 2009; Notelaers et al., 2010). Such studies
examined in particular whether various job characteristics may elicit (mainly targets' reports of) workplace bullying.
This body of research resulted in an extensive list of job characteristics that may be involved in this process. Specifically, targets' reports of bullying have been related to job characteristics such as workload
(Agervold & Mikkelsen, 2004; Baillien, Rodriguez-Muñoz, De Witte, Notelaers & Moreno-Jiménez, in press),
cognitive and emotional demands (Notelaers et al. 2010; Zapf, Knorz & Kulla, 1996), role problems
(Hauge et al., 2007; Notelaers et al., in press) and job insecurity (Neyens, Baillien, Notelaers &
De Witte, 2007). Conversely, job aspects such as autonomy (Baillien, De Cuyper et al., in press; Hubert, Furda &
Steensma, 2001), skill utilisation (Einarsen et al., 1994; Notelaers et al., 2010) and social support
(Neyens et al., 2007; Zapf et al., 1996) have been negatively related to targets' reports of workplace bullying. In addition, recent studies convincingly showed that job characteristics such as job insecurity, workload and
autonomy may explain perpetrators' reports of bullying too (Baillien, De Cuyper et al., in press; De Cuyper
et al., 2009). Overall, building upon the work environment hypothesis, these studies defined a broad range
of job-related antecedents of workplace bullying. However, they, remained rather explorative in nature. Besides these studies, some authors have started to construct models to explain the emergence of workplace bullying.
Baillien and colleagues (2009), for example, developed a Three Way Model. This model describes the processes through
which workplace bullying may be triggered. It is grounded in incident based qualitative research and details
two processes through which job characteristics may link to perpetrators' as well as targets' reports of
bullying1. Firstly, poor job characteristics are assumed to pave the way for bullying through
interpersonal conflicts (see also Leymann, 1996). Secondly, and perhaps most important in view of the current
study, poor job characteristics are expected to elicit work-related strain. Strain may, in turn, lead to perpetrators'
and targets' reports of workplace bullying. Specifically, from the perpetrator's perspective, poor job characteristics
may elicit negative affect and strain, which may be relieved by directing negative acts towards a co-worker. A similar
process has been described in the Revised Frustration Aggression Theory (Berkowitz, 1989) and General Strain Theory (Hinduja, 2007).
From the target's perspective, exposure to poor job characteristics may trigger feelings of helplessness, which may
lead the employee to violate work-related habits and expectations. Co-workers may then respond to this situation by
acting negatively to their colleague, which in the end may escalate into bullying. The employee may therefore become
a target of bullying (see also Felson & Tedeschi, 1993). Similarly, poor job characteristics may wear out the
employee's resources. The particular employee then becomes an 'easy target' who offers little resistance against
workplace bullying (De Cuyper et al., 2009; Hoel & Salin, 2003). The Three Way Model has been valuable in providing possible processes from which perpetrators' and targets' reports
of bullying may develop. To date, however, this model has not been empirically tested; probably because of its heuristic nature.
JD-R model and workplace bullying
The present study aims to introduce workplace bullying, both from the perspective of perpetrators
and targets, as new outcomes in the JD-R model. Research detailing bullying antecedents has on the
one hand produced a long list of job characteristics that may be linked to workplace bullying.
On the other hand, a model was developed suggesting that job characteristics may lead to (perpetrators'
and targets' reports of) workplace bullying via a process of work-related strain (Baillien et al., 2009).
In our view, the JD-R model may bridge the two traditions in the bullying literature in two ways. Firstly, the JD-R provides a sound theoretical model that may structure the long and broad list of job
characteristics that (according to the bullying literature) are involved in
the emergence of workplace bullying. Specifically, the JD-R model allows specifying the content
of poor job characteristics in terms of both the presence of job demands and the absence of job resources.
As elaborated in the Three Way Model, we expect these job characteristics to trigger both perpetrators'
and targets' reports of bullying. Following the principles of the JD-R model, we then expect job demands
to associate positively and job resources to associate negatively with perpetrators' and targets' reports
of bullying. Moreover, based on the JD-R model, we expect job demands and job resources to interact.
According to the JD-R model, job resources may reduce the burden that job demands place on employees.
Therefore, we also expect them to reduce the potential negative relationship between job demands and
both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying. In sum, we hypothesise as follows: • Hypothesis 1: Job demands relate positively to perpetrators' reports of bullying
(Hypothesis 1a) as well as to targets' reports of bullying (Hypothesis 1b).
• Hypothesis 2: Job resources relate negatively to perpetrators' reports of bullying
(Hypothesis 2a) as well as targets' reports of bullying (Hypothesis 2b).
• Hypothesis 3: Job demands and job resources interact, so that the relationship between
job demands and perpetrators' reports (Hypothesis 3a) and targets' reports (Hypothesis 3b) of bullying
are weaker under the condition of high compared to low job resources (i.e. buffer hypothesis).
Secondly, the Three Way model describes work-related strain as one of the main processes linking job
characteristics to both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying. The JD-R model may
further add to this literature as it substantiates the energetic process through which job demands
and job resources may lead to outcomes such as workplace bullying. Indeed, the JD-R model details that job
demands relate positively and job resources relate negatively to work related strain, mostly operationalised
as burnout. Through their influence on psychological strain, job demands and job resources may then associate
with other aspects of workers' functioning, herein: the degree to which employees consider themselves as
perpetrators and/or targets of workplace bullying. In a similar vein, the JD-R model outlines that job
resources may also buffer the negative impact of job demands on strain, which then explains why interaction
job demands and job resources may interact in the prediction of workplace bullying. Therefore, we argue that
burnout and more specifically emotional exhaustion as its main component (Maslach, Schaufeli & Leiter, 2001),
explains (i.e. mediates) the association between job demands, job resources and perpetrators' and targets' reports of bullying. Specifically, we hypothesise: • Hypothesis 4: The association between job demands and perpetrators' reports (Hypothesis 4a)
and targets' reports (Hypothesis 4b) of bullying is mediated by emotional exhaustion.
• Hypothesis 5: The association between job resources and perpetrators' reports (Hypothesis 5a)
and targets' reports (Hypothesis 5b) of bullying is mediated by emotional exhaustion.
• Hypothesis 6: The association of the interaction between job demands and job resources
with perpetrators' reports (Hypothesis 6a) and targets' reports (Hypothesis 6b) of bullying is mediated
by emotional exhaustion. We selected four job demands following both the JD-R model's and workplace bullying research traditions,
that is, workload, cognitive demands, emotional demands and role ambiguity. With respect to the JD-R model,
these demands have frequently been used to test the energetic process (e.g. Bakker, Demerouti & Schaufeli,
2005; Hallberg & Schaufeli, 2006). With respect to the bullying literature, these demands have been related to
targets' reports of workplace bullying (e.g. Hauge et al., 2007; Neyens et al., 2007; Notelaers et al.,
2010); and some have also been related to perpetrators' reports of bullying (Baillien, De Cuyper et al., in press;
De Cuyper et al., 2009). Similarly, four job resources (task autonomy, skill utilisation, social support from colleagues,
and social support from the supervisor) were selected based on the JD-R model's and workplace bullying literature. Firstly,
they have frequently been indicated as resourceful job aspects in the realm of the JD-R model (e.g. Bakker et al., 2004;
Bakker, Demerouti, Taris, Schaufeli & Schreurs, 2003). Secondly, they have been associated with a decrease in workplace
bullying (e.g. Hauge et al., 2007; Neyens et al., 2007; Notelaers et al., 2010).
Procedure and participants
To be able to realistically capture the variability in the variables under study (Rousseau & Fried, 2001), we collected
a heterogeneous sample in 17 Flemish organisations during Spring and Summer of 2008. In all organisations, the Human
Resource department supported data collection. In three organisations, participants completed an electronic questionnaire sent
to their professional e-mail address. In the other organisations, participants filled out a paper and pencil questionnaire.
After completion, questionnaires could be posted in a box at the organisation's Human Resource department,
from which they were picked up by the researchers2. In total, 965 questionnaires were returned (response rate 43%). Complete information were provided by
749 respondents, and included in the sample. As participation was voluntary and anonymous, we did not
acquire any information about the non-respondents. The majority of the respondents were female (67%).
Educational level was rather high: 41% of the participants obtained a Bachelors' degree and 37% acquired
a Masters' degree. About 11% of the participants were blue-collar workers, 14% were white-collar workers,
50% were professionals. Most participants occupied a full time position (77%) and had a permanent contract (73%).
Mean age of the respondents was 37 years (SD = 11.01).
Measures
Job demands: Workload was tapped with four items such as 'I have a lot of work to do' (Chronbach's a = 0.90;
Van Veldhoven & Meijman, 1994). Two items assessed cognitive demands (r = 0.77; Van Veldhoven & Meijman,
1994); an example being 'My job requires a lot of concentration'. Three items such as 'I get conflicting orders' measured
role ambiguity (Chronbach's a = 0.78; Van Veldhoven & Meijman, 1994). Finally, two items such as 'My job requires me
to hide my feelings' measured emotional demands (r = 0.63; Kristensen, Hannerz, Hogh & Borg, 2005). For all
items, responses varied on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree). Job Resources: Task autonomy was measured using five items such as 'I can plan my own work'
(Chronbach's a = 0.75; Rosenthal, Guest & Peccei, 1996). Social support from colleagues (Chronbach's a = 0.83)
and social support from the supervisor (Chronbach's a = 0.89) were assessed with five items each. Examples
were 'My colleagues/supervisor help(s) me to get the work done' and 'My colleagues/ supervisor appreciate(s)
me' (Rigotti, Mohr, De Cuyper, De Witte, Bernhard et al., 2003). Skill utilisation was tapped using
four items such as 'My job requires me to learn new things' (Chronbach's a = 0.73; Van Veldhoven & Meijman, 1994).
For all items, respondents indicated whether or not they endorsed the statement on a 5-point scale ranging
from 'totally disagree' (1) to 'totally agree' (5). Emotional exhaustion was measured with the five items of the Dutch version of the Maslach Burnout
Inventory-General Survey (a = 0.88; Schaufeli & Van Dierendonck, 2000). Items such as 'I feel totally
exhausted in my job' were answered on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 'never' (0) to 'always' (6). Workplace bullying was measured with the 9-item Short Negative Acts Questionnaire for targets
(Chronbach's a = 0.85; Notelaers & Einarsen, 2008) as well as perpetrators (Chronbach's a = 0.82;
De Cuyper et al., 2009). This questionnaire describes personal (e.g. gossiping) and as well as
work-related (e.g. being withheld information) negative acts that may be considered as bullying when
occurring on a regular basis. Respondents indicated how often they were exposed to (to measure to what e
xtend one is a target) and performed (to measure to what extent one is a perpetrator)
each act during the last six months on a 5-point frequency scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (daily).
In line with literature on bullying, all items for bullying exposure and enactment were included in
their respective scales (Nielsen, Skogstad, Matthiesen, Glaso, Aasland et al., 2009).
Plan of analysis
The hypotheses were examined through Structural Equitation Modeling (SEM) using the maximum-likelihood method in LISREL
8.72 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004). To be able to also test the job demands-job resources interaction, we followed
the procedure of Mathieu, Tannenbaum and Salas (1992) as outlined by Cortina, Chen and Dunlap (2001; see also Bakker,
Hakanen, Demerouti, & Xanthopoulou, 2007, for a similar approach in the realm of the JD-R model). Specifically, the
independent variables (i.e. job demands, job resources and the job demands-job resources interaction) were represented by
one indicator: their standard scale score. For example, the indicator of the latent job demands-job resources variable was
the product of the standardised manifest variables for job demands and job resources. The mediating variable (i.e. emotional
exhaustion) as well as the dependent variables (i.e. perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying) were represented by
their respective items. In line with earlier observations that perpetrators' reports may associate with targets' reports of bullying
and vice versa, both factors were allowed to correlate (De Cuyper et al., 2009). Significant job demands-job resources
interactions were further analysed using simple slope analyses (Meier, 2008). As data screening using Prelis 2.72 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004) revealed data non-normality at univariate and
multivariate level, in addition to the covariance matrix, the asymptotic covariance matrix was used and the Satorra-Bentler
Scaled Chi-square (SBS-x2; Satorra & Bentler, 1994) was inspected. Model fit was evaluated using three goodness of fit
indices (Hu and Bentler, 1999): • the Root Means Square Error of Approximation (RSMEA)
• the Comparative Fit Index (CFI)
• the Standardised Root Means Square Residuals (SRMR). CFI values larger than 0.95 indicate excellent fit; values larger than 0.90 indicate good fit (Holye, 1995; Bentler, 1990).
RSMEA below 0.05 in combination with SRMR values below 0.09 indicate excellent fit, whereas values below 0.08 and 0.10,
respectively, indicate good fit (Byrne, 2001). The mediating role of emotional exhaustion was examined following Holmbeck
(1997) (see also Van den Broeck, Vansteenkiste, De Witte, & Lens, 2008). Firstly, we tested in the direct model whether
job demands, job resources and the interaction-term related to perpetrators' as well as targets' reports of workplace bullying.
Second, we examined the full mediation model in which job demands, job resources and the job demands-job resources interaction-term
associated with emotional exhaustion, which, in turn, was related to both aspects of workplace bullying. Then, we computed multiple
partial mediation models in which we additionally allowed direct relationships between the job characteristics and perpetrators' and
targets' reports of workplace bullying. We examined whether these models showed improved fit compared to the full mediation model
using chi-square
Preliminary analysis
Measurement model
Firstly, we tested the divergent validity of the latent factors by computing three different models.
As prescribed by Harman (1979), the first model was a null model, in which all items loaded on one factor.
The second model was the expected measurement model including all study variables: job demands, job resources,
job demands-job resources, emotional exhaustion and perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying. Finally,
in line with Podsakoff, Mackenzie, Lee and Podsakoff (2003), we calculated a third model in which the respective items
loaded on both their expected factors as well as on a latent common method factor. The expected measurement model, including
26 observed variables and 6 latent factors, yielded a good fit to the data:• SBS-x2 (424) = 1967.43
• p < 0.001
• RSMEA = 0.05
• SRMR = 0.07
• CFI = 0.95. It fitted better to the data than the one factor model, DSBS-x² (10) = 5678.69; p < 0.001. Moreover,
the common method factor in the one factor model explained only 13, 51% of the total variance, which is below the 25%
threshold for common method variance observed by Williams, Cote and Buckley (1989). The third model provided yet a better
fit to the data than the expected measurement model, ∆SBS-x² (31) = 695.97; p < 0.001. However, in this model,
the common method factor only explained 8.94% of the total variance. Therefore, it could be concluded that common method
variance did not significantly bias the participants' responses (Podsakoff et al., 2003).
Correlation analysis
Table 1 provides an overview of the correlations between the variables under study. The overall category
of job demands did not relate to perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying. In terms of the different
job demands, role ambiguity and emotional demands related positively with perpetrators' reports of bullying.
Workload was unrelated and cognitive demands were negatively related to bullying enactment. Each of the job demands
related positively to targets' reports of bullying. Job resources generally did not relate to perpetrators' reports
of workplace bullying. Autonomy forms an exception, as it related positively to perpetrators' reports of bullying.
Job resources related negatively to targets' reports of workplace bullying.
TABLE 1: Correlation amongst the study variables.
|
In line with the JD-R model, job demands related negatively with job resources and positively with
emotional exhaustion; job resources related negatively with emotional exhaustion. Emotional exhaustion
related positively with both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying. Both perspectives
on workplace bullying were positively related.
Primary analysis
Direct model: Job demands and job resources predicting workplace bullying
We started with the computation of the direct model in which job demands, job resources and job demands-job
resources were related to both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying. This model yielded a good fit to the data: • SBS-x2 (182) = 1045.64
• p < 0.001
• RSMEA = 0.05
• SRMR = 0.06
• CFI = 0.97. Contrary to Hypothesis 1a, job demands did not relate to perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying; y = 0.11,
not significant. They related positively to targets' reports of workplace bullying; y = 0.24, p < 0.001,
thereby confirming Hypothesis 1b. Job resources showed no significant relationship with perpetrators' reports of workplace
bullying; y = -0.04, not significant. Hypothesis 2a was therefore not corroborated. In line with Hypothesis 2b,
however, job resources related negatively with targets' reports of workplace bullying; y = -0.25, p < 0.001.
The job demands-job resources interaction was significant for perpetrators' reports of bullying; y = 0.15, p <
0.05.3 Hypothesis 3a suggests that the impact of job demands on workplace bullying would be buffered by job resources. This hypothesis was not supported.
Simple slope analyses for low (i.e. 1 SD below the mean) and high (i.e. 1 SD above the mean) job demands revealed
that job demands associated positively with being an perpetrator of workplace bullying particularly under conditions of
high (i.e. 1 SD above the mean; b = 0.07, SE = 0.02, t = 3.54, p < 0.001) job
resources,. Moreover, job demands were unrelated to being a bully when job resources were
low (i.e. 1 SD below the mean; b = -0.01, SE = 0.01, t = -0.56,
not significant). This interaction is depicted in Figure 1. Contrary to Hypothesis 3b,
job resources did not moderate the association between job demands and targets' reports of
workplace bullying; y = 0.02, not significant.
|
FIGURE 1: Interaction effects of job demands and job resources on perpetrators’ reports of workplace bullying.
|
|
Full and partial mediation model
To test Hypotheses 4, 5 and 6, we subsequently conducted a full mediation model in which emotional exhaustion was expected to
fully mediate the relationships from job demands, job resources and job demands-job resources to perpetrators' and target's
reports of bullying. This model showed good fit to the data; SBS-x2 (293) = 1288.58, p < 0.001; RSMEA = 0.05; SRMR = 0.06,
and CFI = 0.97. In line with the JD-R model, job demands related positively to emotional exhaustion; y = 0.37, p < 0.001,
whereas job resources related negatively; y = -0.12, p < 0.01. Contrary to expectations, the interaction between job demands
and job resources was not significantly related to emotional exhaustion; y = 0.00, not significant. Emotional exhaustion
related positively to both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying; y = 0.28 and y= 0.34,
respectively, p-values < 0.001. We then continued by computing a series of partial mediation models in which we completed the full mediation model with direct
relationships between the independent and dependent variables that were significant in the direct model. The following direct
relationships resulted in improved model fit: • from job demands to targets' reports of workplace bullying; ∆SBS-x2 (1) = 13.72; p < 0.001
• from job resources to targets' reports; ∆SBS-x2 (1) = 39.33; p < 0.001
• from the job demands-job resources interaction to perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying; ∆SBS-x2 (1) = 21.42;
p < 0.001. We therefore calculated a final model that included these direct relationships. This model fitted the data well: • SBS-x2 (291) = 1226.93
• p < 0.001
• RSMEA = 0.05
• SRMR = 0.04
• CFI = 0.97. As depicted in Figure 2, in this final model, job demands related positively to emotional exhaustion. Job demands also
yielded a direct positive association with targets' reports of workplace bullying. Job resources related negatively to
emotional exhaustion, but were also negatively related to targets' reports of workplace bullying. The job demands-job
resources interaction was only related to perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying. Emotional exhaustion was related
to both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying. Perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying
were positively related.
|
FIGURE 2: Emotional exhaustion mediating the associations from job demands and job resources to perpetrators’ and targets’ reports of workplace bullying.
|
|
Finally, we examined more formally the mediational role of emotional exhaustion in the associations of job demands and
job resources with workplace bullying through a series of Sobel tests. Sobel tests indicated that emotional exhaustion
partially mediated the positive relation of job demands with targets' reports of bullying; z = 0.08, p < 0.01,
thereby confirming Hypothesis 4b. The negative relationship between job resources and targets' reports was also partially mediated
by
exhaustion; z = -0.02, p < 0.05. This supports Hypothesis 5b. In sum, no direct associations were found of job demands and job resources with perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying;
rejecting Hypotheses 1a and 2a. Hypotheses 1b and 2b that the presence of job demands and the lack of job resources associated
with targets' reports of workplace bullying were confirmed. The job demands-job resources interaction was significant
when predicting perpetrators' reports of bullying. However, whilst we expected that high job resources would buffer
the positive association between job demands and bullying perpetration in Hypothesis 3a, results showed that particularly
the combination of high job demands and high job resources was indicative of perpetrators' reports of bullying. Regarding
the explanatory process linking job demands, job resources and workplace bullying (i.e. mediation by emotional exhaustion),
results revealed that only the relationships between job demands and targets' reports of bullying and between job resources
and targets' reports of bullying were partially mediated by emotional exhaustion. Hypotheses 4b and 5b were thereby confirmed.
As neither job demands nor job resources yielded a direct relation with perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying,
no mediation could take place in these relationships. Hypotheses 4a and 5a were therefore rejected. The job demands-job
resources interaction was unrelated to emotional exhaustion, thereby excluding emotional exhaustion as a potential mediator
in its relationship with workplace bullying. Hypothesis 6 was therefore not corroborated.
The current study aimed to expand the JD-R model to perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying.
Such an approach may firstly add to the JD-R model literature. A plethora of studies attested to the JD-R
model's predictive validity regarding workers' health and functioning (for an overview; see Bakker &
Demerouti, 2007). However, research on the JD-R model's validity to understand social stress is fairly lacking.
As interdependence amongst employees is growing, smoothing the social relationships in the work context may be
amongst the most important challenges for organisations to deal with in order to remain effective (Grant & Parker, 2009).
Therefore, the current research aimed to examine whether the JD-R model may be a fruitful approach to understand these phenomena. Secondly, the current study may advance insights in workplace bullying by introducing the JD-R model as a sound and
well-established theoretical model. This study follows the work environment hypothesis (Einarsen et al., 2009;
Notelaers et al., 2010), further detailed in the Three Way Model (Baillien et al., 2009). Herein both
perpetrators' and targets' reports of bullying are ascribed to job characteristics and psychological strain. These
frameworks have generally been tested by exploring the impact of various job characteristics and aspects of work
related strain on workplace bullying. The JD-R model may provide a comprehensive framework to group the job
characteristics studied this far and allows to define clear hypotheses on how these job characteristics may
lead to various aspects of workers' functioning through strain, that is, emotional exhaustion or burnout. Firstly, the current results largely confirmed the assumptions of the JD-R model and the energetic process in particular:
job demands were positively and job resources were negatively related to emotional exhaustion. However, no evidence was found
for the association between the job demands-job resources interaction term and emotional exhaustion. Previous research attesting
to this interaction, however, largely focused upon the interplay of particular job demands and job resources rather than the general
categories (e.g. Hakanen, Bakker, & Demerouti, 2005). These studies established that specific job resources were a stronger buffer
for particular job demands than others were. However, to date, it remains unclear which combinations of job demands and job resources may
particularly interact. Therefore, we would like to encourage future research to unravel the conditions under which such interactions are
most likely to be found to predict emotional exhaustion. Most important for the current study were, however, the associations of job demands, job resources, and their interaction
with workplace bullying (partially through emotional exhaustion). Regarding the perpetrators' perspective, results revealed
no relationships between the general categories of job demands, job resources and perpetrators' reports of bullying.
Hypotheses 1a and 2a were thus rejected, thereby also rejecting Hypotheses 4a and 5a, in terms of the mediational
role of emotional exhaustion in these relationships. The correlation analysis may perhaps shed further light on the
lack of direct relationships of job demands and job resources and perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying. In terms of job resources, the correlation analysis indicated that only autonomy yielded a significant relation with perpetrators'
reports of workplace bullying. Contrary to expectations, this relationship was positive. Regarding job demands, the correlation
analysis seems to indicate that particular job demands may yield opposite relationships with perpetrators' reports of bullying:
whereas role demands and emotional demands displayed the expected positive relationship, cognitive demands were negatively related
to perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying. These results seem to align with the recent differentiation of job demands into
'job hindrances' and 'job challenges'. Whereas job hindrances unambiguously lead to negative consequences, job challenges may
have additional beneficial effects (Podsakoff, Lepine, & Lepine, 2007; Van den Broeck, Vansteenkiste, De Cuyper, &
De Witte, 2010). Previous research indicated that encountering job hindrances elicit counterproductive behaviour such as
arriving late and neglecting instructions on a daily basis. Job challenges relate negatively to such behaviour
(Rodell & Judge, 2009). The current results may suggest that similar associations can be found in view of
perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying. Future research is, however, necessary to test further this assumption,
for example, by using a larger set of job demands. Perhaps most importantly in view of the current focus on the JD-R model, the results indicate that job demands and job resources
particularly associate with perpetrators' reports of bullying when 'joining forces'. Contrary to the predictions based on the
JD-R model (Hypothesis 3a); however, job resources did not buffer the associations of job demands with perpetrators'
reports of bullying. To the contrary, job resources appeared to boost the relation of (high) job demands with bullying:
whilst job demands were unrelated to workplace bullying when job resources were absent, they related positively to perpetrators'
reports of bullying, when job resources were high. One possible explanation for this result could be that job demands may provide
the necessity for bullying perpetrators to act out. The presence of job resources may then provide perpetrators' with the
possibility to deal with job demands by involving in bullying enactment. This may hint at yet other processes through which employees may become a perpetrator of bullying. One process suggested in the
literature on bullying may then be the emergence and the procession of interpersonal conflicts (Leymann, 1996). Specifically, as
elaborated in the second process of the Three Way model (Baillien et al., 2009), poor job characteristics may pave the way
for conflicts. Escalated conflicts may, in turn, encourage workplace bullying. The more (formally or informally) powerful
employee may then become perpetrator; the powerless employee may become the target. In terms of job demands and job resources,
it may be reasonable to assume that job demands may agitate employees, which then serves as a breeding ground for conflicts amongst
workers. The availability of high job resources may then grant particular workers with high power, which makes them susceptible to
enactment of workplace bullying. The positive association between task autonomy and perpetrators' reports of bullying seems to align
with this view. Indeed, employees who can act autonomously in their job might have more freedom to involve in bullying perpetration.
De Cuyper and colleagues (2009) also prompted the reasoning that resources may provide the opportunity for bullying in demanding
circumstances. These researchers established that employability, as a personal resource, may foster workplace bullying behaviour
amongst job insecure employees. In terms of targets' reports of bullying, job demands displayed the expected positive association, confirming Hypothesis 1b.
Job resources demonstrated the expected positive relation, in line with Hypothesis 2b. Thus, demanding work contexts as well
as the lack of job resources may make workers vulnerable to becoming a target of workplace bullying. These findings expand
previous research in the realm of the JD-R model by indicating that job demands and job resources may not only successfully
explain workers' well-being, attitudes and behaviour, but may also explain systematic interpersonal maltreatment. From a bullying perspective, these results replicate earlier research exploring job characteristics as antecedents of targets'
reports of bullying, but also advance the literature on bullying as they meaningfully group the cluttered list of job characteristics
into job demands and job resources. In line with the expectations of both the JD-R model and the Three Way Model, emotional exhaustion
may partially account for the associations of both categories of job characteristics and targets' reports of workplace bullying
(Hypotheses 4b and 5b). These findings further sustain the much cited work environment hypothesis, which links bullying to a
stressful work environment (e.g. Einarsen et al., 1999; Notelaers et al., 2010). They also confirm the Three
Way model (Baillien et al., 2009) arguing that poor job characteristics lead to targets' reports of bullying through
the development of strain. Contrary to our expectations (Hypothesis 3b), job demands and job resources did not interact when predicting targets'
reports of bullying. Job resources did thus not buffer the negative impact of job demands with regard to targets of bullying.
In other words, in contrast to perpetrators' reports of bullying in which job demands and job resources joined forces, both
categories of job characteristics are independently associated with targets' reports of bullying in an either-or manner: either
the presence of high job demands or the presence of low job resources suffices in view of being a target.
Limitations and practical recommendations
Some limitations need to be taken into account when interpreting the current results. Firstly, this study relies on self-reports,
which might increase the risk of common method variance. As indicated, however, our analyses indicated that such method effects
did not significantly influence our results (Podsakoff et al., 2003). This suggests that the precautions to prevent common
method variance were effective. Indeed, during data-collection, we underlined that there were no right or wrong answers and
guaranteed confidentiality. All these measures are assumed to reduce the risk of common method variance (Podsakoff et al., 2003).
A related methodological problem could be social desirability, particularly in view of perpetrators' reports of workplace bullying.
Our findings may therefore bear on a sub-group of respondents who are willing to admit their negative acts. Social desirability,
however, generally results in an underestimation of effects due to a lack of variance. This implies that relationships may become
even stronger when accounting for social desirability. Using measures of different sources could have been an effective way to
reduce further common method variance and social desirability. Although this might be feasible for job demands and job resources
(see for example, Demerouti, Bakker, Nachreiner, & Schaufeli, 2001), the reliance on employees' perceptions allowed us to
capture fully nuances in the degree one experiences workplace bullying, which may be considered as a strength of this study. Secondly, as the current study relied on cross-sectional data, no conclusions regarding causality can be drawn. However,
we relied on theoretical as well as empirical reasons to present job demands and job resources as antecedents of emotional
exhaustion and workplace bullying. From a theoretical point of view, the JD-R model conceptualises job demands
and job resources as antecedents of work related well-being through which other outcomes emerge (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007).
This causal order has received large empirical support (Hakanen et al., 2008), although some evidence is also found for
the reversed causation (e.g. Xanthopoulou et al., 2009). Similarly, the literature on bullying in general and the
Three Way model in particular assume that the work environment leads to workplace bullying via strain, rather than the other
way around. Some scholars, however, suggest that workplace bullying may also result in psychological strain (Mikkelsen &
Einarsen, 2002; Rodriguez-Munoz, Baillien, De Witte, Moreno-Jiménez & Pastor, 2009), whilst still others advocate reciprocal
relationships (Hoel et al., 2002). Empirically, unreported analyses from the current study, however, indicated that targets'
reports of bullying did not mediate the association from job demands and job resources to emotional exhaustion; these results are
available upon request. Yet, workplace bullying and psychological strain may nonetheless be closely intertwined. Therefore,
we encourage future studies to apply longitudinal or cross-lagged designs to gain further understanding in targets' reports
of bullying and to explore the possibility of reversed causation or reciprocal relationships. Another promising route for future research could be to examine further the processes through which workplace bullying
may develop from job characteristics. Previous literature suggested that strain might be amongst the most important
processes linking job demands and job resources with both perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying.
Our findings, however, underline that workplace bullying may develop from a complex and multi-causal process (Salin, 2003).
Indeed exhaustion did not fully account for the association between job demands, job resources, their interaction and both
perspectives on workplace bullying. Future research might therefore examine whether, for example, work engagement may also
play a role, as outlined in the motivational process of the JD-R model. In such cases, the lack of job resources might harm
workers' enthusiasm. Less enthusiastic workers may then show less commitment to their work (Hakanen et al., 2008)
and invest less in extra role behaviour (Bakker, Demerouti, & Verbeke, 2004). Such behaviour might, in turn, isolate
the particular workers from their more enthusiastic colleagues, resulting in neglect and, eventually, in becoming a target
of bullying. Despite these limitations, from a practical point of view, the current results may assist practitioners in facilitating
the good social relationships amongst workers. Specifically, the results suggest that, in general, workplace bullying may
be reduced by limiting job demands and increasing job resources. Particular attention may be paid to employees who feel
exhausted, as they either run increased risk to become perpetrators' or targets of workplace bullying.
Conclusion
The current study demonstrates that the JD-R model may be a fruitful approach to understand the growing interdependence amongst
workers in general (Grant & Parker, 2009) and perpetrators' and targets' reports of workplace bullying in particular.
The JD-R model furthermore closes the gap between the theoretical assumptions and empirical findings in the bullying domain
suggesting that job characteristics may elicit workplace bullying trough job strain. The results largely support this assumption.
Yet, at the same time, the current study's findings underline that workplace bullying is a multi-causal phenomenon, which deserves
further research attention.
Agervold, M., & Mikkelsen, E.G. (2004). Relationships between bullying, psychosocial work environment and individual
stress reactions. Work and Stress, 18, 336–351.
doi:10.1080/02678370412331319794
Aquino, K., & Thau, S. (2009). Workplace victimization: Aggression from the targets' perspective.
Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 717–741.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163703;
PMid:19035831
Baillien, E., De Cuyper, N., & De Witte, H. (in press). Job autonomy and workload as antecedents of workplace bullying:
A two-wave test of Karasek's Job Demand Control Model for targets and Perpetrators. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology. Baillien, E., Neyens, I., De Witte, H., & De Cuyper, N. (2009). Towards a three way model of workplace bullying:
a qualitative study. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 1–16.
doi:10.1002/casp.977
Baillien, E., Rodriguez-Muñoz, A., De Witte, H., Notelaers, G., & Moreno-Jiménez, B. (in press).
The Demand-Control Model and targets' reports of bullying at work: A test within Spanish and Belgian blue-collar workers.
European Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology. Bakker, A.B., & Demerouti, E., (2007). The Job Demands-Resources model: state of the art. Journal of
Managerial Psychology, 22, 309–328.
doi:10.1108/02683940710733115
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., De Boer, E., & Schaufeli, W. (2003). Job demands and job resources as predictors
of absence duration and frequency. Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 62, 341–356.
doi:10.1016/S0001-8791(02)00030-1
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2003). Dual processes at work in a call centre: An application of the
Job Demands - Resources model. European Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology, 12, 393–417.
doi:10.1080/13594320344000165
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2005). The crossover of burnout and work engagement among working
couples. Human Relations, 58, 661–689.
doi:10.1177/0018726705055967 Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., Taris, T., Schaufeli, W., & Schreurs, P. (2003). A multigroup analysis of
the Job Demands-Resources Model in four home care organizations. International Journal of Stress Management,
10, 16–38.
doi:10.1037/1072-5245.10.1.16
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., & Verbeke, W. (2004). Using the job demands-resources model to predict burnout
and performance. Human Resource Management, 43, 83–104.
doi:10.1002/hrm.20004
Bakker, A.B., Hakanen, J.J., Demerouti, E., & Xanthopoulou, D. (2007). Job resources boost work engagement,
particularly when job demands are high. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 274–284.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.274
Bakker, A.B., Van Emmerik, H., Van Riet, P., (2008). How job demands, resources, and burnout predict objective performance:
A constructive replication. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21, 1–16.
doi:10.1080/10615800801958637
Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,
238–246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238;
PMid:2320703
Berkowitz, L. (1989). The frustration-aggression hypothesis: An examination and reformulation.
Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.1.59;
PMid:2667009
Bowling, N.A., & Beehr, T.A. (2006). Workplace harassment from the victim's perspective:
A theoretical model and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 998–1012.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.5.998 ;
PMid:16953764
Brodsky, C.M. (1976). The harassed worker. Toronto: Lexington Books, D.C. Heath and Co. Byrne, B.M. (2001). Structural Equation Modelling with Amos. Basic Concepts Application and Programming,
Erlbaum, Mahwah, London (2001). Cortina, J.M., Chen, G., & Dunlap, W.P. (2001). Testing interaction effects in LISREL: Examination and
illustration of available procedures.Organisational Research Methods, 4, 324–360.
doi:10.1177/109442810144002
Cowie, H., Naylor, P., Smith, P.K., Rivers, I. & Pereira, B. (2002). Measuring workplace bullying.
Aggression and Violent Behaviour, 7, 35–51.
doi:10.1016/S1359-1789(00)00034-3
De Cuyper, N., Baillien, E., & De Witte, H. (2009). Job insecurity and workplace bullying among targets
and perpetrators: moderation by employability. Work and Stress, 23, 206–224.
doi:10.1080/02678370903257578
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W. (2001). The job demands-resources model of burnout.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 499–512.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499
Einarsen, S. (2000). Harassment and bullying at work: a review of the Scandinavian approach. Agression and Violent
Behaviour, 5, 379–401.
doi:10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00043-3
Einarsen, S., Matthiesen, S.B., & Hauge, L.J. (2009). Bullying and harassment at work. In S. Cartwright &
C.L. Cooper (Eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Personnel Psychology. (pp. 464–495). London: Sage publications. Einarsen, S., Raknes, B.I., & Matthiesen, S.B. (1994). Bullying and harassment at work and their relationships
to work environment quality: an exploratory study. European Work and Organisational Psychologist, 4,
381–401.
doi:10.1080/13594329408410497
Einarsen, S., & Skogstad, A. (1996). Bullying at work: Epidemiological findings in public and private
organisations. European Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology, 5, 185–201.
doi:10.1080/13594329608414854
Felson, R.B, & Tedeschi, J.T. (1993). Aggression and violence: Social interactionists' perspectives.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
doi:10.1037/10123–000
González-Romá, V., Schaufeli, W.B., Bakker, A.B., & Lloret, S. (2006). Burnout and work engagement:
Independent factors or opposite poles? Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 68, 165–174.
doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2005.01.003
Grant, A.M., & Parker, S.K. (2009). Redesigning work design theories: The rise of relational
and proactive perspectives. Academy of Management Annals, 3. Hakanen, J., Bakker, A.B., & Demerouti, E. (2005). How dentists cope with their job demands and stay
engaged: the moderating role of job resources. European journal of Oral Sciences, 113, 479–487.
doi:10.1111/j.1600-0722.2005.00250.x;
PMid:16324137
Hakanen, J., Bakker, A.B., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2006). Burnout and work engagement among teachers. Journal
of School Psychology, 43, 495–513.
doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2005.11.001
Hakanen, J.J., Schaufeli, W.B., Ahola, K. (2008). The Job Demands-Resources model:
A three-year cross-lagged study of burnout, depression, commitment, and work engagement. Work & Stress,
22, 224–241.
doi:10.1080/02678370802379432
Halbesleben, J.R.B., & Buckley, M. R. (2004). Burnout in organisational life. Journal of Management,
30, 859–879.
doi:10.1016/j.jm.2004.06.004
Hauge, L.J., Skogstad, A., & Einarsen, S. (2007). Relationships between stressful work environments and bullying:
Results of a large representative study. Work & Stress, 21, 220–242.
doi:10.1080/02678370701705810
Hinduja, S. (2007). Workplace violence and negative affective responses: A test of Agnew's general strain theory.
Journal of Criminal Justice, 35, 657–666.
doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2007.09.002
Hobfoll, S.E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Review of General Psychology, 6, 307–324.
doi:10.1037/1089–2680.6.4.307
Hoel, H., & Salin, D. (2003). Organisational antecedents of bullying at work. In S. Einarsen, H. Hoel, D. Zapf &
C.L. Cooper (Eds.). Bullying and Emotional Abuse in the Workplace: International Perspectives in Research
and Practice (pp. 145–162). London: Taylor & Francis. Hoel, H., Zapf, D., & Cooper, C.L. (2002). Bullying at work and stress. In P.L. Perrewe & D.C. Ganster (Eds.).
Historical and Current Perspectives on Stress and Health, 2 (pp. 293–333). Amsterdam: JAI.
doi:10.1016/S1479–3555(02)02008–5
Holmbeck, G.N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and
moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 65, 599–610.
doi:10.1037/0022–006X.65.4.599; PMid:9256561.
Hoyle, R.H. (1995). The structural equation modeling approach: Basic concepts and fundamental issues.
In R.H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 1-15). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Hubert, A., Furda, J. & Steensma, H. (2001). Mobbing, systematisch pestgedrag in organisaties:
Twee studies naar antecedenten en gevolgen voor de gezondheid [Mobbing, systematic bullying in organisations:
Two studies about antecedents and health related consequences]. Gedrag en Organisatie, 14, 378–369. Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (2004). LISREL 8.71 [Computer software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc. Karasek, R. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: Implications for job redesign.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 285–308.
doi:10.2307/2392498
Keashly, L. (1998). Emotional abuse in the workplace: conceptual and empirical issues. Journal of Emotional
Abuse, 1, 85–117.
doi:10.1300/J135v01n01_05
Kristensen, T. S., Hannerz, H., Hogh, A., & Borg, V. (2005). The Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire: A tool
for the assessment and improvement of the psychosocial work environment Scandinavian Journal of
Work Environment & Health, 31, 438–449.
PMid:16425585
Leymann, H. (1996). The content and development of mobbing at work. European Journal of Work and Organisational
Psychology, 5, 165–184.
doi:10.1080/13594329608414853
Llorens, S., Schaufeli, W.B., Bakker, A.B, Schaufeli, W.B. & Salanova, M. (2006). Testing the robustness of the
Job Demands-Resources Model International Journal of Stress Management, 13,378–391.
doi:10.1037/1072-5245.13.3.378
Martinussen, M., Richardsen, A.M. & Burke, R.J. (2007). Job demands, job resources, and burnout among police
officers, Journal of Criminal Justice, 35, 239–249.
doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2007.03.001
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W., & Leiter, M. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 397–422.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397;
PMid:11148311
Mathieu, J.E., Tannenbaum, S.I., & Salas, E. (1992). Influences of individual and situational
characteristics on measures of training effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 828–847.
doi:10.2307/256317
Matthiesen, S.B., & Einarsen, S. (2007). Perpetrators and targets of bullying at work: role stress and
individual differences. Violence and Victim, 22, 735–753.
doi:10.1891/088667007782793174;
PMid:18225386
Meier, L.L. (2008). IRSE. Interactions in Multiple Linear Regression with SPSS and Excel (Version 1.6)
[Computer software and manual]. Retrieved November 22, 2009, from http://www.urenz.ch/irse
Mikkelsen, E.G. & Einarsen, S. (2002). Bullying in Danish work-life: Prevalence and health correlates.
European Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology, 10(4), 393–413.
doi:10.1080/13594320143000816
Neyens, I., Baillien, E., Notelaers, G., & De Witte, H. (2007). Pesten op het werk: Risicofactoren in werk en
organisatie [Workplace bullying: Risk factors in the job and the organisation]. Tijdschrift voor Arbeidsvraagstukken, 23,
306–320. Nielsen, M.B., Skogstad, A., Matthiesen, S.B., Glaso, L., Aasland, M.S., Notelaers, G., & Einarsen, S. (2009).
Prevalence of workplace bullying in Norway: Comparisons across time and estimation methods. European Journal of Work
and Organisational Psychology, 18, 81–101.
doi:10.1080/13594320801969707
>Notelaers, G., & Einarsen, S. (2008). The construction and validation of the Short - Negative Acts Questionnaire.
Paper presented at 6th International Conference on Workplace Bullying. Montreal, Canada. Notelaers, G., De Witte, H. & Einarsen, S. (2010). A job characteristics approach to explain workplace bullying.
European Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology, 19, 487–504.
doi:10.1080/13594320903007620
Parkins, I.S., Fishbein, H.D., & Ritchey, P.N. (2006). The influence of personality on workplace bullying and
discrimination. Journal of Applied Psychology, 36, 2554–2577.
doi:10.1111/j.0021–9029.2006.00117.x
Podsakoff, N.P., LePine, J.A., LePine, M.A. (2007). Differential challenge stressor-hindrance stressor relationships
with job attitudes, turnover intentions, turnover and withdrawal behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 92, 438–454. Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.-Y, & Podsakoff, N.P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioural
research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88,
879–903.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.438;
PMid:17371090
Rayner, C. (1999). From research to implementation: finding leverage for prevention. International Journal of Manpower, 20,
28–38.
doi:10.1108/01437729910268614
Rigotti, T., Mohr, G., Cuyper, N. De, Witte, H. De, Bernhard, C., Isaksson, K., Jong, J. de, Schalk, R., Caballer, A.,
Gracia, F., Peiró, J.M., Ramos, J., Clinton, M., Guest, D., Krausz, M. & Staynvarts, N. (2003). Instruction work
and blue print for methodology. Unpublished research report. Leipzig: Germany. Rodell, J.B., & Judge, T.A., (2009). Can "Good" Stressors Spark "Bad" Behaviors? The Mediating Role of Emotions in
Links of Challenge and Hindrance Stressors With Citizenship and Counterproductive Behaviors. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 94, 1438–1451.
doi:10.1037/a0016752;
PMid:19916654
Rodriguez-Muños, A., Baillien, E., De Witte, H., Moreno-Jiménez, B., & Pastor, J.C. (2009). Cross-lagged
relationships between workplace bullying, job satisfaction and engagement: Two longitudinal studies.
Work and Stress, 23, 225–243.
doi:10.1080/02678370903227357
Rosenthal, P., Guest, D., & Peccei, R. (1996). Gender difference in managers' explanation for their work
performance: A study in two organisations. Journal of Occupational and Organisational Psychology, 69, 145–151. Rousseau, D.M., & Fried, Y. (2001). Location, location, location: contextualizing organisational research.
Journal of Organisational Behaviour, 22, 1–13.
doi:10.1002/job.78
Salanova, M., Agut, S., & Peiró, J. (2005). Linking organisational resources and work engagement to
employee performance and customer loyalty: The mediation of service climate. Journal of Applied Psychology,
90, 1217–1227.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1217;
PMid:16316275
Salin, D. (2003). Ways of explaining workplace bullying: A review of enabling, motivating and precipitating
structures and processes in the work environment. Human Relations, 56, 1213–1232.
doi:10.1177/00187267035610003 Salin, D. (2008). The prevention of workplace bullying as a question of human resource management: Measures
adopted and underlying organisational factors. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 24, 221–231.
doi:10.1016/j.scaman.2008.04.004
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P.M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance
structure analysis. In A. von Eye & C.C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables analysis: Applications for
developmental research (pp. 399–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Schaufeli, W.B., & Bakker, A.B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and their relationship with burnout
and engagement: a multi-sample study. Journal of Organisational Behaviour, 25, 293–315.
doi:10.1002/job.248
Schaufeli, W.B., & Van Dierendonck, D. (2000) Utrechtse Burnout Schaal (UBOS): Testhandleiding.
Amsterdam: Harcourt Test Services. Siegrist, J. (1996). Adverse health effects of high effort-low reward conditions. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 1, 27-41.
DOI:10.1037/1076–8998.1.1.27
PMid:9547031
Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Witte H., & Lens, W. (2008). Explaining the relationships between
job characteristics, burnout and engagement: The role of basic psychological need satisfaction. Work &
Stress, 22, 277–294.
doi:10.1080/02678370802393672 Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Cuyper, N., & De Witte, H. (2010). Not all Job Demands are Created Equal:
Differentiating Job Hindrances and Job Challenges in the Job Demands-Resources Model. European Journal of Work
and Organizational Psychology,19, 735–759.
doi:10.1080/13594320903223839
Van Veldhoven, M., & Meyman, T.F. (1994). Het meten van psychosociale arbeidsbelasting met een vragenlijst:
de vragenlijst beleving en beoordeling van de arbeid (VBBA) [Measuring psychosocial demands with a questionnaire:
the VBBA]. Amsterdam: NIA. Williams, L.J., Cote, J.A., & Buckley, M.R. (1989). Lack of method variance in self-reported affect and perceptions
of work: Reality or artifact? Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 462–468.
doi:10.1037/0021–9010.74.3.462
Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2009). Work engagement and financial returns:
A diary study on the role of job and personal resources. Journal of Organisational and Occupational
Psychology, 82, 183–200.
doi:10.1348/096317908X285633
Zapf, D. (1999). Organisational, work group related and personal causes of mobbing/bullying at work. International
Journal of Manpower, 20, 70–85.
doi:10.1108/01437729910268669
Zapf, D., & Einarsen, S. (2003). Individual antecedents of bullying: victims and perpetrators. In S. Einarsen,
H. Hoel, D. Zapf, & C.L. Cooper (Eds.). Bullying and Emotional Abuse in the Workplace. International Perspectives
in Research and Practice (pp.165–184). London: Taylor & Francis. Zapf, D., & Gross, C. (2001). Conflict escalation and coping with workplace bullying: A replication and extension. European
Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology, 10, 497–522.
doi:10.1080/13594320143000834
Zapf, D., Knorz, C., & Kulla, M. (1996). On the relationship between mobbing factors, and job content, social work
environment, and health outcomes. European Journal of Work and Organisational Psychology, 5,
215–237.
doi:10.1080/13594329608414856
1.Besides two process through which job characteristics may give rise to workplace bullying, the
Three Way Model also describes a third route through which bullying may emerge; a route that is
not related to job characteristics. Specifically, this third process suggests that specific team
and organisational aspects, such as a culture of gossip or a highly competitive climate, may
encourage employees to engage in workplace bullying.
2.Controlling for the sampling method did not significantly alter the results.
3.Follow up exploratory regression analysis revealed that particularly social support interacted with the job demands.
Supervisory support interacted with each of the job demands, whilst social support from colleagues interacted with workload and
cognitive demands. In addition, task autonomy interacted with both role ambiguity and cognitive demands. These results are available
from the corresponding authors upon request.
|