Orientation: There is some evidence that job demands and job resources such as job control and humorous coping may contribute to the
risk of upper respiratory tract infections (URTI). Research purpose: The purpose of this study was to test a model including these variables as well as job-related
affect, in order to explore their role in the explanation of the frequency of upper respiratory tract infection. Motivation of the study: This study has been conducted in order to extend our understanding of the role of traditional
variables like job demands and job control with humorous coping styles and affective variables with regard to the explanation
of the frequency of URTI. Research design, approach and method: A sample of 2094 employees filled out questionnaires assessing job demands, job
control, generic (MSHS-C), antecedent-focused and response-focused humorous coping (QOHC) and job-related affect (JAWS). Main findings: Job demands were indirectly related to the frequency of upper respiratory tract infections, mediated by
their relationships with job control and negative job-related affect. Generic and response-focused humorous coping were less
relevant for the explanation of the frequency of upper respiratory tract infections than the presumably ‘healthy’
antecedent-focused humorous coping style. The latter showed a negative association with negative job-related affect. The
frequency of upper respiratory tract infections was better predicted by job control and negative job-related affect than by
humorous coping, in the expected directions. Practical/managerial implication: These findings may have practical relevance for the improvement of stress management
interventions in organisations. Contribution/value-add: Although it was shown that healthy humorous coping does contribute to decreases in upper
respiratory tract infection, job demands, job resources and negative affective state seem the most important predictors.
Upper respiratory tract infections (URTI) like the common cold and influenza are not only associated with procedures
aimed at an increase of bodily hygiene – several studies have shown that psychological processes may also influence
the risk to catch these common types of infection. However, these studies have not looked closely at job resources
like job control and humorous coping styles and at affective states as predictors of URTI frequency, let alone that
these variables have been incorporated into one model. Furthermore, previous studies have investigated generic humorous
coping styles and have not looked at specific styles like antecedent-focused and response-focused humorous coping, which
may theoretically differ in their effect on health, including URTI frequency. The present study intends to fill these gaps, aiming to increase our understanding of the role of these occupational
variables in their association with URTI frequency. A model incorporating job demands, job control, several styles of
humorous coping and job-related affect as predictors of URTI frequency is tested, enabling investigation of the contribution
of the separate variables as well as their interactions. The focus is on a test of the predictive value of generic and specific
humorous coping styles for URTI frequency.
Psychosocial variables, affective states and upper respiratory tract infections frequency
The frequency of upper respiratory tract infections (URTI) may be predicted by general and job-related psychosocial variables.
General demands increase the frequency of the common cold (Cohen, Tyrell, & Smith, 1991, 1993). A similar relationship has
been found for job-related demands (Hao, Duan & Zhang, 2002; Mohren, Swaen, Borm, Bast, & Galama, 2001). The adverse
effects of these demands on URTI frequency may be due to their negative affective consequences. Several studies have indeed
shown that negative affect is positively related to URTI frequency (e.g. Biondi & Zannino, 1997; Cohen & Williamson,
1991). Recently, Takkouche, Regueira and Gestal-Ottero (2001) have reported support for a negative association between
positive affect and URTI frequency. However, job resources like job control and humorous coping styles may also play a
significant role in adaptation to stress (Hobfoll, 2002) and should therefore be added to our model.
Job control, humorous coping and affective states
Job control has been proposed as an important job resource which acts as a buffer for job demands in Karasek’s (1979)
job demand-control model, which was later extended to the job demands-resources model (Demerouti, Bakker, Nachreiner, &
Schaufeli, 2001). Job control is a key variable in this model, because it enables employees to decide how and when they do
certain tasks. In some job types high job demands may be buffered by high job control (Karasek calls this an active job type).
Another job resource may be humorous coping, which is an interesting coping style because – unlike many other coping
styles – it is not only aimed at preventing or decreasing negative affective consequences resulting from stress, but
also adding mirth to the coping equation (Doosje, De Goede, Van Doornen, & Goldstein, 2010). Several empirical studies
have confirmed that generic humorous coping may serve this dual-purpose function because of its relationships with decreases
in negative affect (e.g. Eggli, 1997; Olson, Hugelshofer, Kwon, & Reff, 2005) and increases in positive affect (e.g.
Anderson & Arnoult, 1989; Martin, Puhlik-Doris, Larsen, Gray, & Weir, 2003). This is consistent with relief theory,
stating that humour relieves tension in the individual (Cooper, 2008).As the present study is dedicated to the associations of humorous coping with health dimensions, it should be mentioned
that the traditional view of humorous coping as a generic concept has been challenged lately. Instead, response-focused
and antecedent-focused forms of humorous coping have been proposed, with possibly differential health effects. Combining
Gross’ (2001) work on two types of emotion regulation with the humorous coping concept, response-focused and antecedent-
focused humorous coping styles have been introduced and operationalised by Doosje et al. (2010). Antecedent-focused
humorous coping is a kind of cognitive reappraisal, aimed at changing the possibly stressful meaning of a stressor at hand
into a humorous one, thus neutralising it and preventing the development of a negative affective response. Response-focused
humorous coping is used in a later stage of the stress process, when stress-induced negative affect has already taken place.
In this type of coping response, humour is used to avoid or deny feelings of stress already present. In short, antecedent-
focused humorous coping is used for stress prevention, whereas response-focused humorous coping is used for ‘damage
control’ caused by stress. These two types of humorous coping styles may therefore differ in their associations with
affective states as well as health status.
Job control, humorous coping and upper respiratory tract infections frequency
How are job resources related to URTI frequency? Job control was negatively associated with URTI frequency in studies
by Hao et al. (2002) and Mohren et al. (2001). An interaction between job demands and job control has also
been shown: high job demands and low job control groups show higher common cold frequency than groups high in job control
(Van de Schoot, Hoytink, & Doosje, 2009). The potential role of humorous coping in the association between demands and
URTI frequency has been supported by studies that observed negative associations between generic humorous coping and common
cold frequency for specific populations, namely mothers of newborns (Dillon & Totten, 1989) and young cancer patients
(Dowling, 2000). These humorous coping studies did not examine the impact of affective variables. Similarly, studies on
the JD-R model do not typically include affective variables. It may be argued that affective states should play a
pivotal role in the explanation of URTI frequency, because they relate to both job control and humorous coping and
to URTI frequency. Although there is some evidence for differential health effects of response-focused and antecedent-
focused emotion regulation (Austenfeld & Stanton, 2004; John & Gross, 2004), no studies to date have explored
their effect on URTI frequency. Also, associations between antecedent-focused and response-focused humorous coping
styles and physical health have not been investigated before.
Common method variance
Before presenting our research aims and questions, a possible bias in survey studies, common method bias, needs to be
addressed. This is a methodological problem described by several authors (e.g. Lindell & Whitney, 2001; Podsakoff,
McKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2001). It can be ascribed to ‘common method variance which is attributable to
the measurement method rather than to the constructs the measures represent’ (Podsakoff et al., 2001).
However, the magnitude and even existence of this problem have been subject to debate (see Spector, 2006).
Research aims, questions and hypotheses
The main aim of the current study is to extend the job demand-resources model with affective states and humorous coping
styles, assessing their separate and joint contribution to URTI frequency. Job demands are expected to show a positive
relationship with URTI frequency and job control is expected to show a negative relationship with it (Hao et al.,
2002; Mohren et al., 2001). Turning to the affective part of our model, positive affect is expected to relate
negatively with URTI frequency (Takkouche et al. 2001), whereas negative affect is positively related with it
(e.g. Biondi & Zannino, 1997). No empirical evidence for a relationship of job demands and job control with affective
parameters has been reported. These associations will therefore be explored. With regard to humorous coping, some studies
showed weak associations between generic humorous coping and common cold frequency (Dillon & Totten, 1989; Dowling, 2000).
We expect to find these associations in our study, too. However, since generic humorous coping has shown relationships with
positive and negative affect (Eggli, 1997; Olson et al., 2005), it is also possible that the association between
humorous coping styles and URTI frequency is mediated by positive or negative affect. These relationships will therefore
be explored in the present study. An interesting outcome of this study may be the differences between generic, antecedent-
focused and response-focused humorous coping with regard to their associations with affective states and URTI frequency.
Based on previous studies (Austenfeld & Stanton, 2004; John & Gross, 2004), antecedent-focused humorous coping
may prove the healthiest humorous coping style of the three. This leads us to the following hypotheses:• Hypothesis 1: Job demands are positively associated with URTI frequency (1a), whereas job control is negatively
associated with it (1b).
• Hypothesis 2: Negative affect is positively related to URTI frequency (2a), whereas the reverse association
is expected for positive affect (2b).
• Hypothesis 3: Generic humorous coping is weakly and negatively associated with URTI frequency.
• Hypothesis 4: Humorous coping styles are negatively related to negative affect (4a) and positively related
to positive affect (4b).
• Hypothesis 5: Antecedent-focused humorous coping shows stronger negative associations with URTI frequency than
generic (5a) and response-focused humorous coping (5b), possibly through their differential relationships with affect (5c). Furthermore, the current study will explore the relationship between job demands, job control and affect. In addition, the
relationships between different humorous coping styles, affect and URTI frequency and the relative contribution of all variables to the explanation of URTI frequency will be explored. We will also explore possible mediation patterns in the humorous coping-affect-URTI frequency relationship. Finally, common method bias will be addressed by testing the assumption that there is a basic underlying factor that will fit the model better than a model based on separate variables.
Research approach
This is a cross-sectional survey study studying several models incorporating job demands, job resources, job-related affect and the
frequency of upper respiratory infections incidence (URTI).
Research method
Participants
Five hundred and twenty four first-year bachelor students of psychology at a Dutch university each selected four employees in their
own social network, resulting in a quota sample of 2094 employees in a broad range of work settings in The Netherlands. Students
gathered these data as part of an assignment for a methodology and statistics course on survey methods. They received grade points
for the assignment. Inclusion criteria were employment for 50% of working time or more (actual mean working time was 62.6%, SD = 12.9),
no present illnesses, an even division of men and women (1055 male participants and 1039 female participants) and a normal distribution
of age (actual mean age was 40.1 years, SD = 6.1). Although we aimed at a balance between participants in terms of high and low education,
there was an overrepresentation of highly educated participants (47.6% in this study), compared to 28.9% in the Dutch working population
(Statistics Netherlands, 2002).
Measuring instruments
Job demands and job control: Job demands were measured using four items, based on Karasek’s (1985) job content
instrument (adapted for Dutch populations by Furda, 1995). Its items refer to having to work too much and too fast, for example,
‘How often do you have to put in extra effort to finish the job?’ Answers were given on a 5-point scale ranging from
1 (never) to 5 (always). A similar scale was used for job control (e.g. ‘Can you decide yourself how to execute your work?’),
referring to the amount of job control experienced. This scale was originally developed by Karasek (1985, Dutch translation by Furda,
1995). A measurement model including both of these job characteristics as separate factors showed satisfactory fit to the data,
χ2(404) = 85.5, NFI = 0.98, CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.05. Job-related affect: Job-related affect has been measured by the 30-item Job Affective Well-Being Scale (JAWS), developed
by Van Katwyk, Fox, Spector and Kelloway (2000), asking participants how they felt about their work in the previous month. Both arousal
(high-low) and valence (positive-negative) dimensions have typically been included in the original scale, resulting in four subscales.
Since this study is focused on the valence dimension, two subscales have been used, a 15-item positive job-related affect subscale
(e.g. ‘My job made me feel at ease’) and a 15-item negative job-related affect subscale (e.g. ‘My job made me feel
intimidated’). Although positive and negative affect scales are strongly related in this study (r = -0.85, p ≤ 0.001),
there are good reasons to consider positive and negative affect as separate affective dimensions (see Tellegen, Watson, & Clark, 1999,
for a discussion of the dimensionality of affect). To test the 2-factor solution for the JAWS, a measurement model including the two scales
was conducted, х2(404) = 5785.5, NFI = 0.81, CFI = 0.82, RMSEA = 0.08. Although the Root-Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA) parameter shows adequate model fit, Normed Fit Index (NFI) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) parameters are below
0.90, indicating a suboptimal fit. However, internal consistency of the positive and negative affect scales is very good (see Table 1
in the Results section for Cronbach’s alphas), contributing to the conclusion that these two scales may be consistent ones. We
also conducted an analysis based on the original 4-factor solution as proposed by Van Katwyk et al. (2000). This analysis resulted
in an unidentified path model.
TABLE 1: Descriptive correclation and reliability analyses of job characteristics, humorous coping styles, job-related affect and upper respiratory tract infection frequency.
|
Humorous coping scales: Three humorous coping scales were used measuring generic, antecedent-focused and response-focused
humorous coping styles. Also, an overall humorous coping style was computed, combining scores of these three humorous coping scales. The coping scale of the Multidimensional Sense of Humor Scale (MSHS-C) measured generic humorous coping. This is an 8-item Likert-scale
generic humorous coping scale (e.g. ‘Uses of wit or humor help me master difficult situations’). It is part of a questionnaire
also measuring other dimensions of a sense of humour (Thorson & Powell, 1993). Antecedent-focused humorous coping is one of the scales of the Questionnaire of Occupational Humorous Coping (QOHC), a 4-scale, 23-
item questionnaire originally developed and tested by Doosje et al. (2010) with answers ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often).
The 9-item antecedent-focused humorous coping scale (e.g. ‘When I have to work more to finish something I am able to see the humor
in the situation’) refers to attempts to use humorous behaviour to cognitively reappraise a situation. Response-focused humorous coping (e.g. ‘When my work makes me feel tense, I make jokes to avoid that feeling’) is also a
part of the QOHC. This scale has five items, measuring the tendency to use humorous behaviour to ease, avoid or decrease stress or
negative feelings that are already present. Upper respiratory tract infection (URTI) frequency: Two types of URTI frequency were measured: • common cold (e.g. ‘How often have you experienced a common cold in the previous two months’) frequency
• influenza (‘flu’, e.g. ‘How often have you experienced the flu in the previous two months?’) frequency. Participants are asked how often (1 = never, 5 = very often) they have experienced these infections of the upper respiratory tract.
Research procedure
Survey data were collected by students, who imputed the data in SPSS data files. After checking for errors and extreme
values, scales were computed and descriptive, correlational and model analyses were performed.
Statistical analysis
For the descriptive and correlational analyses, SPSS 16.0.2 software was used, whereas structural equation modeling analyses
were conducted using AMOS 16.1.0 software. Because of the high correlation between the two job-related affect scales, a collinearity
test was performed ahead of the main multivariate analyses. In structural equation modeling (SEM) the χ2
parameter is usually employed as a goodness of fit parameter. However, since it is sensitive to sample size (Kline, 2005), additional
fit indices have been used: • Bentler-Bonett NFI (Bentler & Bonett, 1980)
• CFI (Bentler, 1990)
• RMSEA (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Steiger, 1990). To allow model comparison the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978) was included, which takes both model fit and
model complexity into account. When mediation or partial mediation was thought to be present, these indirect effects were tested
using the Sobel test (Sobel, 1986). Common method bias was investigated by adding a method factor to the structural equation models.
Descriptive and correlational analyses
Table 1 shows that internal consistency of all variables is adequate. The average values of job demands and job control are quite high,
which is typical of an active job type (high job demands, high job control) population. In addition, positive job-related affect is high
and negative job-related affect is low. Job demands are negatively related to positive job-related affect and positively related to negative
job-related affect, whereas reverse and stronger associations have been established for job control.Although we expected to find a positive relationship between job demands and URTI frequency (hypothesis 1a), no such relationship
can be demonstrated in the data. Hypothesis 1b has been confirmed: job control and URTI frequency are negatively correlated. Also,
negative job-related affect correlates positively with URTI frequency and positive job-related affect correlates negatively with it,
confirming hypotheses 2a and 2b. Generic humorous coping is indeed very weakly associated with URTI frequency, but the sign of the
coefficient is positive, not negative, disconfirming hypothesis 3. Antecedent-focused humorous coping is negatively associated with
negative job-related affect, whereas generic humorous coping is unrelated to it and response-focused humorous coping is positively
related to it. This largely disconfirms hypothesis 4a. Hypothesis 4b, about a positive relationship between humorous coping styles
and positive job-related affect, is confirmed for generic and antecedent-focused humorous coping styles, but not for response-focused
humorous coping. Humorous coping styles do seem to be differentially related to job-related affect, which is preliminary evidence for
hypothesis 5, stating that humorous coping styles may relate to URTI frequency through their association with job-related affect.
Demographic variables and collinearity
Before proceeding to the path analyses, we will address two additional issues, namely: • the role of demographic variables in the model
• the possible collinearity of the job-related affect scales. With regard to the first issue, demographic variables like gender, age and educational level have been analysed in
terms of their contribution to URTI frequency. There are no gender differences in URTI frequency, t (2084) = 0.29,
not significant. Age is negatively and weakly associated with URTI frequency (r = -0.16, p ≤ 0.001).
People with high educational levels (advanced professional and university level, M = 1.81, SD = 0.72)
have significantly lower URTI frequency than people with low education (M = 1.94, SD = 0.72), t
(2088) = 3.9, p ≤ 0.001). With regard to the second issue, collinearity, positive and negative job-related
affect are highly correlated. This means that this relationship may inflate variance in our model. To test this assumption,
collinearity diagnostics have been computed in a multiple regression model including key independent variables like job
demands, job control, humorous coping styles, the two job-related affect scales and the URTI frequency variable, the
dependent variable. The results are satisfactory, since the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) is 4.0 for positive job-related
affect and 3.7 for negative job-related affect. This is way below the value of 10 suggested as a maximum for this test.
Basic characteristics of the path analyses
Structural equation modeling enables the exploration of the contribution of several humorous coping styles to an explanatory
theoretical model for URTI frequency. Table 2 shows model fit parameters for the three different humorous coping styles and
all humorous coping styles (‘overall’) combined. Although chi-square values are not significant for generic humorous
coping (a formal requirement for interpretation), the large N enables comparison with fit indexes for the other two
humorous coping styles. NFI, CFI and RMSEA parameters are all within the required boundaries, meaning that all models show
excellent fit to the data. Comparing the models, the BIC parameter (which computes model fit correcting for model complexity)
is lowest for generic humorous coping, so at first sight this would seem the most successful humorous coping predictor for
URTI frequency. We will return to generic and overall humorous coping later. In the next paragraph, we will present the
findings with regard to antecedent-focused and response-focused humorous coping, which are expected to relate differentially
to URTI frequency, possibly through differences in their relationships with positive and negative job-related affect (hypothesis 5).
TABLE 2: Model fit and model comparison of several humorous copying styles in relation to the frequency of upper respiratory tract infection.
|
Comparing antecedent-focused and response-focused humorous coping
The path models depicted in Figures 1 and 2 show an interesting pattern. Response-focused humorous coping (depicted in figure 1)
is positively related to negative job-related affect and unrelated to positive job-related affect. Also, there is a small positive
association between response-focused humorous coping and URTI frequency, which improves model fit. Two possible mediation patterns
are of interest here: • one of response-focused humorous coping in the job demands-negative job-related affect relationship (1)
• one of negative job-related affect in the response-focused humorous coping-URTI frequency relationship (2).
|
FIGURE 1: Path model for response-focused humorous coping (RFHC) and
upper respiratory tract infection (URTI) frequency, standardised coefficients.
|
|
The Sobel test for the first pattern shows that response-focused humorous coping partially mediates the job demands-negative job-related
affect relationship (z = 2.84, p ≤ 0.01, two-tailed). The second pattern shows that negative job-related affect acts as
a partial mediator in the relationship between response-focused humorous coping and URTI frequency (z = 3.36, p ≤ 0.001,
two-tailed). Antecedent-focused humorous coping (see Figure 2) shows a different associational pattern than response-focused humorous coping. It is
unrelated to job demands and it is positively related to positive job-related affect. Also, this humorous coping style is negatively
related to negative job-related affect (although regression coefficients are small). It is apparently related to URTI frequency by
its association with negative job-related affect, but not by its association with positive job-related affect. Of all humorous coping
styles, antecedent-focused humorous coping seems to bear the strongest relationship to URTI frequency, through its association with
negative job-related affect, confirming this part of hypothesis 5.
|
FIGURE 2: Path model for antecedent-focused humorous coping (AFHC) and
upper respiratory tract infection (URTI) frequency, standardised coefficients.
|
|
Comparing generic and overall humorous coping
Although generic humorous coping has shown the best model fit of all humorous coping styles, the associational patterns
are quite different from those for antecedent-focused and response-focused humorous coping. Generic humorous coping shows
a weak association with job demands (β = 0.04, p ≤ 0.05), no association with negative job-related affect
(β = 0.02, n.s.) and a weak association with positive job-related affect (β = 0.04, p ≤ 0.05). Because of
this associational pattern, generic humorous coping seems to have no predictive value for URTI frequency. A similar
pattern exists for overall humorous coping, a combination of all three humorous coping styles: again, no association
with negative job-related affect has been found. Also, weak associations with job demands (β = 0.04, p ≤ 0.05)
and positive job-related affect (β = 0.08, p ≤ 0.05) have been established.
Comparison of the relative contribution of model variables
Comparing the variables in the models, the right hand side of the models depicted in figures 1 and 2, seem much less successful
predictors of URTI frequency than variables in the left-hand side of the model. Job control and negative job-related affect are
related to each other and both are also related to URTI frequency. It is also clear that negative job-related affect is a better
predictor than positive job-related affect and that job demands do show associations with URTI frequency, but mainly by its
relationships with job control and negative job-related affect.
Path analyses including demographic variables and common method bias
Age and gender do not seem to contribute to the model (with low and non-significant β’s). However, educational level
does: there is a small but significant relationship to URTI frequency in the AFHC model (β = 0.04, p ≤ 0.001).
However, it is left out of the analyses, because model parameters are much less favorable for this model, χ2
(8) = 954.2, NFI = 0.25, CFI = 0.24, RMSEA = 0.23. No BIC parameter can be computed for this analysis. Finally, common method bias was counteracted by linking a
new common variable to all other variables in the models presented here. This operation results in an inability to produce
identifiable models. Therefore, we were unable to gather evidence for the assumption that there was common method bias in our data.
Summary of findings
The main objective of this study was the test of a model explaining URTI frequency, including not only job demands and job control,
but also humorous coping styles and job-related affect. It succeeded in building models fitting the data, combining relevant variables
and their associations to URTI frequency, which have not been combined before. We were unable to replicate a direct effect of job
demands on URTI frequency. We did find associations of job demands with negative and positive job-related affect and job control,
suggesting that there may have been an indirect effect. This study showed that the favourable effect of job control on URTI frequency
existed independently as well as by its association with negative job-related affect. Humorous coping styles were differentially
related to URTI frequency. Although all humorous coping styles fitted well in models containing job characteristics and job-related
affect, antecedent-focused humorous coping was more strongly related to parameters predicting URTI frequency than generic and
response-focused humorous coping. Antecedent-focused humorous coping, the style considered ‘healthiest’ beforehand,
was related to job-related affect in the expected directions, associating with URTI frequency by a relationship with negative
job-related affect. Generic humorous coping was related to positive job-related affect only, whereas response-focused humorous
coping was unrelated to positive job-related affect and positively related to negative job-related affect, which itself was
positively related to URTI frequency. In this model, we also found a direct positive association between this humorous coping
style and URTI frequency. Overall, humorous coping styles seemed to be poorer predictors of URTI frequency than job control and
negative job-related affect. Antecedent-focused humorous coping and job control were positively associated. No common variance
factor could be detected.
Theoretical significance of the findings
There is evidence from our study that antecedent-focused humorous coping is a ‘healthier’ humorous coping style
than response-focused and generic humorous coping, which lends some support to the findings presented by Austenfeld and
Stanton (2004) and John and Gross (2004), predicting different health effects of antecedent-focused and response-focused
emotion regulation. The (weak) association between antecedent-focused humorous coping and URTI frequency is no direct one.
However, we were unable to demonstrate mediation effects here. Compared to the other two humorous coping styles, antecedent-
focused humorous coping seems to comply best with the twofold aim of humorous coping: • reduction in negative affect
• promotion of positive affect. The associations between all humorous coping styles and job-related affect were generally small and weaker than reported
in previous studies (e.g. Anderson & Arnoult, 1989; Ward & Kennedy, 2001). Our findings confirmed the weak associations
between coping styles and health reported before in the review by Penley, Tomaka and Wiebe (2002). We could not replicate earlier
studies reporting a direct relationship between job demands and common cold frequency (Hao et al., 2002; Mohren et al., 2001).
The associations of job demands and URTI frequency were indirect and seemed to follow the path of job control and negative job-related affect.
Alternative explanation of the findings
The generally weak associations that we found may be due to certain characteristics of the research population.
Although Mohren et al.’s (2001) study is comparable with regard to the number of participants
(5896 individuals in theirs, 2094 in ours), mean age (40.9 in theirs, 40.1 in ours), they used a four-month
period for common cold incidence, whereas this study used a two-month period. In their study, 46.6% of the
population reported no common cold in that period; in our study, this was 62.8%. Psychological health in our
study was also excellent, as indicated by the high positive job-related affect and low negative job-related
affect scores. Despite this affective bias, it is remarkable that the expected relationships with URTI frequency
could still be found. Our population was high in job demands but also in job control, this may best be described
as an active job type population, which Karasek (1979) already predicted should be in good health, a prediction
which was confirmed in our study. The effect sizes in our study may be an underestimation. Increased variations
in URTI frequency, affective states and educational level may increase them markedly. In fact, this was a healthy
population from scratch, excluding less healthy employees with a higher risk of getting a common cold or influenza.
Because low educational levels apparently increase health risks (e.g. Kunst et al., 2005), addressing the
educational level bias could result in a research population with higher average URTI frequencies than in the population we studied.
Methodological issues
With regard to other methods and statistical procedures used in this study, the sampling method used in this study was not
random. The advantage of quota sampling is that it may increase participants’ willingness to cooperate in the study.
However, the focus in this type of sampling is on the inclusion criteria (gender and age) and not on nonresponse bias.
As we have seen, this has led to an overrepresentation of participants with high education. The possible consequences for
other variables may be small, because many of the hypotheses we derived from previous research could be confirmed, indicating
that sampling may have been valid. With regard to causality issues, no causal inferences can be made from our cross-sectional
data – longitudinal studies will have to address this problem, although even they sometimes seem to show a causality
bias, as concluded by Bakker and Demerouti (2007). In terms of common method variance, AMOS software was unable to detect
it, confirming Spector’s (2006) point of view on the issue. Of course this does not mean that it was not there.
Future studies
Although the incorporation of humorous coping styles into a job demands-resources (JDR) model has ended somewhat disappointing,
it seems worthwhile to keep studying coping mechanisms as well as affective parameters in relation to occupational stress models
(see Shimazu, Schaufeli, & Taris, in press, for a successful example). Although weakly correlated, job control and antecedent-
focused humorous coping showed a positive association. A promising avenue for future research would be the interaction of this
type of humorous coping under varying job control conditions. Therefore, the role of a sense of humor in occupational health
should not be ruled out in the future and may enrich more traditional work psychological models, like the JDR model. Especially
Hobfoll’s (2002) suggestion that proactive forms of coping (antecedent-focused humorous coping being one of them) seem
much more prevalent nowadays, is a plea to keep following this path. We think it is also useful to continue combining JDR models
with psychoneuroimmunological models in the future. Since this study did assess several successful predictors of URTI frequency,
this line of research could be extended, for example, with the experimental approach to upper respiratory tract infection
(see Cohen, 2005, for an overview). Also, because there is reason to believe that immunoglobulins in saliva (e.g. Ig-A) play
a role in the chance to catch URTI (see Phillips et al., 2006), future studies may include this variable, preferably
in a longitudinal design. An additional advantage of this type of design is that seasonal variations in URTI frequency may be
included, which was not the case in our study. As a specific method, diary studies may be helpful, which may also learn us
more about the frequency of humorous coping behaviors and their immediate and long-term effects. Since common infections like
upper respiratory tract infections are so common and so hard to prevent, it seems worthwhile to continue examining the influence
affective, job and humorous coping factors have on their occurrence. Another candidate for future research is cardiovascular
functioning, which was one of the first physical health categories linked to the JDC model (see Rozanski et al., 2005,
for a review). Therefore, it seems interesting to extend the model in this study to that research area.
Practical implications of the study
Although (especially antecedent-focused) humorous coping does seem to play a modest role in the frequency of upper
respiratory tract infections, affecting both positive affect and negative affect variables, job control and negative
affect seem more important predictors for URTI frequency. Practical implications of these outcomes may be relevant
for the development of stress intervention programs, which in fact do seem to have some effect on infections of the
upper respiratory tract (see Reid, Mackinnon, & Drummond, 2001, for an effect study). The focus of these interventions
should preferably be on the management of negative affect and on increases in job control. The development or enhancement
of humorous coping capabilities may also be part of these programs, with a focus on cognitive reappraisal of demanding
situations before they get stressful, which is the aim of antecedent-focused humorous coping
Anderson, C.A., & Arnoult, L. H. (1989). An examination of perceived control, humor, irrational beliefs, and positive stress
as moderators of the relation between negative stress and health. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10(2), 101−117.
doi:10.1207/s15324834basp1002_1
Austenfeld, J.L., & Stanton, A.L. (2004). Coping through emotional approach: A new look at emotion, coping, and health-related
outcomes. Journal of Personality, 72, 1335−1363.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00299.x,
PMid:15509285
Bakker, A.B., & Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands-resources model: State of the art. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 22(3), 309−328.
doi:10.1108/02683940710733115
Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238−246.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238,
PMid:2320703
Bentler, P.M., & Bonett, D.G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures.
Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 588−606.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588
Biondi, M., & Zannino, L.G. (1997). Psychological stress, neuroimmunomodulation, and susceptibility to infectious diseases
in animals and man: A review. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66(1), 3−26.
doi:10.1159/000289101,
PMid:8996711
Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K.A. Bollen J.S. Long (Eds.), Testing
structural equation models (pp.136−162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Cohen, S. (2005). The Pittsburgh common cold studies: Psychosocial predictors of susceptibility to respiratory infectious
illness. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12(3), 123−131.
doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm1203_1,
PMid:16083315
Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D.A.J., & Smith, A.P. (1991). Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold. New
England Journal of Medicine, 325, 606−612.
doi:10.1056/NEJM199108293250903,
PMid:1713648
Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D.A.J., & Smith, A.P. (1993). Negative life events, perceived stress, negative affect, and susceptibility
to the common cold. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(1), 131−140.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.131,
PMid:8421249
Cohen, S., & Williamson, G.M. (1991). Stress and infectious disease in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 109(1), 5−24.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.5,
PMid:2006229
Cooper, C. (2008). Elucidating the bonds of workplace humor: A relational process model. Human Relations, 61(8), 1087−1115.
doi:doi:10.1177/0018726708094861
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2001). The job demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 86(3), 499−512.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499,
PMid:11419809
Dillon, K.M., & Totten, M.C. (1989). Psychological factors, immunocompetence, and health of breast-feeding mothers and their
infants. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150(2), 155−162.
doi:10.1080/00221325.1989.9914587,
PMid:2769241
Doosje, S., De Goede, M.P.M., Van Doornen, L.P.J., & Goldstein, J.H. (2010). Measurement of occupational humorous coping.
Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 23(3), 273−305. Dowling, J.S. (2000). Sense of humor, childhood cancer stressors, and outcomes of psychosocial adjustment, immune function,
and infection. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amsherst. Eggli, P. (1997). Humor und Gesundheit: Eine Langschnittuntersuchung an Studienanfängerinnen über ‘Sinn für Humor’
als personale Ressource. Unpublished Abhandlung zur Erlangung der Doktorwürde, Universität Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland. Furda, J. (1995). Werk, persoon en welzijn: Een toets van het JD-C model. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Utrecht
University, the Netherlands. Gross, J.J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 10, 214−219.
doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00152
Hao, Q., Duan, Z., & Zhang, A. (2002). Relations between the occupational stress of nurses and their salivary
immunoglobulin A level. Chinese Nursing Research, 16(4), 207−208. Hobfoll, S.E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Review of General Psychology, 6(4), 307−324.
doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.4.307
John, O.P., & Gross, J.J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences,
and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301−1333.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x,
PMid:15509284
Karasek, R.A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude and mental strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 24, 285−308.
doi:10.2307/2392498
Karasek, R.A. (1985). Job content instrument: Questionnaire and user’s guide, revision 1.1. University of Southern
California, Los Angeles. Kline, R.B. (2005). Structural Equation Modelling. (2nd edn.). New York: The Guilford Press. Kunst, A., Bos, V., Lahelma, E., Bartley, M., Lissau, I., Regidor, E., et al. (2005). Trends in socioeconomic inequalities
in self-assessed health in 10 European countries. International Journal of Epidemiology, 34, 295−305.
doi:10.1093/ije/dyh342,
PMid:15563586
Lindell, M.K., & Whitney, D.J. (2001). Accounting for common method variance in cross-sectional research designs. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 86, 114−121.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.114,
PMid:11302223
Martin, R.A., Puhlik-Doris, P., Larsen, G., Gray, J., & Weir, K. (2003). Individual differences in uses of humor and their
relation to psychological well-being: Development of the Humor Styles Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 37
(1), 48−75.
doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00534-2
Mohren, D.C., Swaen, G.M., Borm, P.J., Bast, A., & Galama, J.M. (2001). Psychological job demands as a risk factor for common
cold in a Dutch working population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 50(1), 21−27.
doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00212-9
Olson, M.L., Hugelshofer, D.S., Kwon, P., & Reff, R.C. (2005). Rumination and dysphoria: The buffering role of adaptive forms
of humor. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1419−1428.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.05.006
Penley, J.A., Tomaka, J., & Wiebe, J.S. (2002). The association of coping to physical and psychological health outcomes:
A meta-analytic review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 551−603.
doi:10.1023/A:1020641400589,
PMid:12462958
Phillips, A.C., Carroll, D., Evand, P., Bosch, J.A., Clow, A., Hucklebridge, F., Der, G. (2006). Stressful life events are
associated with low secretion rates of immunoglobulin A in saliva in the middle aged and elderly. Brain, Behavior, and
Immunity, 20, 191−197.
doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2005.06.006,
PMid:16055305
Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.-Y, & Podsakoff, N.P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioural research:
A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879−903.
doi:10.1037/00219010.88.5.879,
PMid:14516251
Reid, M.R., Mackinnon, L.T., & Drummond, P.D. (2001). The effects of stress management on symptoms of upper respiratory
tract infection, secretory immunoglobulin A, and mood in young adults. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 51, 721−728.
doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00234-3
Rozanski A., Blumenthal J.A., Davidson K.W., Saab P.G., Kubzansky L. (2005). The epidemiology,
pathophysiology, and management of psychosocial risk factors in cardiac practice: the emerging field of behavioral cardiology.
Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 45, 637–51.
doi:10.1016/j.jacc.2004.12.005,
PMid:15734605
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461−465.
doi:10.1214/aos/1176344136
Shimazu, A., Schaufeli, W.B., & Taris, T.W. (in press). How does workaholism affect worker health and performance? The mediating
role of coping. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. Epub ahead of publication. Sobel, M.E. (1986). Some new results on indirect effects and their standard errors in covariance structure models. In N.B. Tuma
(Ed.), Sociological Methodology (pp. 159−186). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Spector, P.E. (2006). Method variance in organizational research: Truth or urban legend? Organizational Research Methods,
9(2), 221−232.
doi:10.1177/1094428105284955
Statistics Netherlands (2001). Working population by educational level. Voorburg/Heerlen: Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Steiger, J.H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 25, 173−180.
doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4
Takkouche, B., Regueira, C., & Gestal-Otero, J. J. (2001). A cohort study of stress and the common cold. Epidemiology,
12(3), 35−349. Tellegen, A., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1999). On the dimensional and hierarchical structure of affect. Psychological
Science, 10(4), 297−303.
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00157
Thorson, J. A., & Powell, F. C. (1993). Development and validation of a multidimensional sense of humor scale.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(1), 13−23.
doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199301)49:113::AID-JCLP2270490103>3.0.CO;2-S
Van de Schoot, R., Hoytink, H., & Doosje, S. (2009). Van onderzoeksvraag naar resultaten: Nulhypothesetoetsing
versus Bayesiaanse modelselectie. De Psycholoog, april, 196−203. Van Katwyk, P. T., Fox, S., Spector, P. E., & Kelloway, E.K. (2000). Using the Job-Related Affective Well-Being
Scale (JAWS) to investigate affective responses to work stressors. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(2), 219−230.
doi:10.1037/1076-8998.5.2.219,
PMid:10784286
Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (2001). Coping with cross-cultural transition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
32(5), 636−642.
doi:10.1177/0022022101032005007
|