Evidence Summary
Age and Context
Sensitivity Associated with Reduced Success in Finding Health Information
Online
A Review of:
Agree, E. M., King, A. C., Castro, C. M., Wiley, A., & Borzekowski,
D. L. G. (2015). “It’s got to be on this page”: Age and cognitive style in a
study of online health information seeking. Journal
of Medical Internet Research, 17(3), e79. http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3352
Reviewed by:
Cari
Merkley
Associate
Professor
Mount
Royal University Library
Calgary, Alberta, Canada
Email: cmerkley@mtroyal.ca
Received: 01 Sep. 2015 Accepted: 01 Sep. 2015
2015 Merkley.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 4.0
International (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Abstract
Objective – To determine
the extent to which age and cognitive style influence an individual’s ability
to successfully locate health information online.
Design –
Quantitative study employing scales and regression analysis.
Setting – A
school of public health and a school of medicine at two universities in the
United States of America.
Subjects – 346
men and women 35 years or older.
Methods – Participants
for the Online Health Study (OHS) were recruited from the community at both
study sites using stratified sampling and screened with a web-based tool to
ensure they had the necessary level of digital literacy to complete the study
tasks. Once enrolled, participants completed the Rapid Estimate of Adult
Literacy in Medicine (REALM) to measure their health literacy and the Witkin
Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT) to determine their cognitive style (labelled
by researchers as context sensitive or context independent). Participants were
asked to search online for answers to six specific questions on heart-healthy
diets, flu vaccinations, alternative medicine and memory, genetic testing,
assistive medical technology, and skin cancer, with 15 minutes of search time
allowed for each question. Participants reported their answers after each
search, which were later assigned scores for accuracy and for specificity. When
combined, these two scores were used as a measure of success. Researchers used
STATA 11 statistical software to run logistic regression, ordinal logistic
regression, and generalized linear models on the data in order to predict which
variables were associated with success on the search tasks.
Main Results – Only 323 of
the 346 participants completed all study tasks, and their data formed the basis
of the analysis. On average, participants correctly answered 4.1 out of 6
questions. Participants provided the most accurate and successful answers for
the question on heart-healthy foods, and the least accurate answers on the
question about seasonal flu shots. They were the least successful in answering
the question about herbal supplements for memory. Across all models, older
participants were less likely to be successful in locating the answers to the
questions than younger participants, even controlling for the other variables
measured in the study. In particular, older participants had the most
difficulty with the question on medical technology, which required the use of
mapping. Overall, the models suggest that higher levels of education, greater
daily Internet use, and higher health literacy were associated with greater
success on the search tasks, the extent to which varied from question to
question. The exception in the case of education was the question relating to
herbal supplements and memory, as participants with higher levels of education
were more likely to score poorly in their responses. Participants whose
cognitive style was found to be context sensitive were less likely to find the
information needed in their online searches than those who were context
independent, particularly on the questions relating to a heart-healthy diet,
skin cancer, and medical technology.
Conclusion – The study suggests that age, cognitive
style, level of health literacy, daily Internet use, and prior education are
all important variables in determining whether an individual can successfully
take advantage of the increasing amount of health information available on the
Internet. Specific approaches to web design could be used to improve the
success rate of those who are context sensitive, and greater support and
direction to reputable online health sources from medical and information
professionals could assist those who are less health literate.
Commentary
For
those working to address disparities in health literacy, the results of the
Online Health Study suggests that there is a tough road ahead. Even among a
non-representative sample characterized by familiarity with the Internet, high
levels of health literacy, and in many cases a college education, only 9% of
the study participants were able to correctly answer all 6 health related
questions (p. 10). Older adults struggled more than their younger counterparts,
even when their computer skills were not in question. While the study
acknowledges the potential role of librarians in supporting health literacy,
they may face the same struggles with having adequate time, resources, and
training to do so as the authors note for medical professionals (Luo &
Park, 2013). Studies such as this demonstrate that this work, while
challenging, is necessary for public health.
While
cognitive styles and their relationship to search behaviour have been studied
extensively, their specific impact on electronic health literacy appears to be
relatively unexplored. The decision to use the terms “context sensitive” and
“context independent” to describe participants’ cognitive styles throughout the
article is curious. The GEFT test itself specifically measures the concepts of
field independence and field dependence (Witkin, 1971), and those are the terms
used in the studies cited by the researchers to support their discussion of
cognitive styles. Employing this more common terminology would be helpful to
readers wishing to explore the concepts further in the literature, and not
contribute to the already confusing array of potentially synonymous terms used
to describe cognitive (or learning or intellectual) styles. There is ongoing
debate among scholars about how field independence-dependence should be best
addressed, or whether it needs to be addressed at all, in the design of online
resources and learning environments (Evans, Richardson,
& Waring
2013), which may not be clear from the limited literature review provided.
The
literature review also references only a fraction of the extensive research
that has taken place on older adults’ use of the Internet. Variables such as
income, health status, race/ethnicity, and access to broadband Internet have
been shown to impact Internet use among this population, but their influence
was not investigated in this study (e.g., Choi, 2011; Flynn, Smith, &
Freese, 2006; Smith, 2014). Further detail on the regression analysis conducted
on the included variables should have been reported, such as the goodness of
fit of the models or the p-values
(Lang & Altman, 2013). It would also have been helpful to know if the
computer labs where participants completed their exercises at each institution
were comparable in terms of available equipment and systems.
The
findings of this study may be of interest to information professionals who work
with the public on questions of consumer health, as well as those involved in
the development or selection of online tools addressing this need. Those
interested in cognitive styles and the design of online resources will wish to
explore the literature further to find the evidence needed to support their
decision making.
References
Choi, N. (2011). Relationship between health service use and health
information technology use among older adults: Analysis of the US National
Health Interview Survey. Journal of
Medical Internet Research, 13(2),
e33. http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/jmir.1753
Evans, C., Richardson, J. T. E., & Waring, M. (2013). Field
independence: Reviewing the evidence. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 83(2), 210-224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12015
Flynn, K. E., Smith, M. A., & Freese, J. (2006). When do older
adults turn to the internet for health information? Findings from the Wisconsin
Longitudinal Study. Journal of General
Internal Medicine, 21(12),
1295-1301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00622.x
Lang, T. A, & Altman, D. G. (2015). Basic statistical reporting for
articles published in biomedical journals: The “Statistical Analyses and
Methods in the Published Literature” or the SAMPL Guidelines International Journal of Nursing Studies, 52(1), 5-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2014.09.006
Luo, L., & Park, V. T. (2013). Preparing public librarians for
consumer health information service: A nationwide study. Library &
Information Science Research, 35(4), 310-317. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lisr.2013.06.002
Smith, A. (2014). Older adults and technology use. Retrieved from the
Pew Research Centre website: http://www.pewinternet.org/2014/04/03/older-adults-and-technology-use/
Witkin, H. A. (1971). Group
Embedded Figures Test [Database record]. Retrieved from PsycTESTS, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t06471-000