Evidence Summary
Children Display Seven Distinct Roles When Searching Online at Home
A Review of:
Foss, E., Druin, A., Brewer,
R., Lo, P., Sanchez, L., Golub, E., & Hutchinson,
H. (2012). Children’s search roles at home: Implications for
designers, researchers, educators, and parents. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology,
63(3), 558-573. doi:10.1002/asi.21700
Reviewed by:
Diana K. Wakimoto
Online Literacy Librarian
California State University, East Bay
Hayward, California, United States of America
Email: diana.wakimoto@csueastbay.edu
Received: 5 Apr. 2013 Accepted: 4 Jul. 2013
2013 Wakimoto.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 2.5 Canada (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by‐nc‐sa/2.5/ca/),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Abstract
Objective – To explore children’s Internet
searching at home in order to make recommendations to designers, researchers,
educators, and parents on how to assist children in becoming search literate
through understanding children’s search roles.
Design – Qualitative, exploratory study.
Setting – Children’s homes in the urban
areas of Maryland, Virginia, Delaware, and Pennsylvania.
Subjects – 83 children (28 children were age
7, 29 were age 9, and 26 were age 11). 41 of the children were female and 42
were male. Parents of the children were also included in the study. 77% of the
parent interviews were carried out with mothers, 15% were with fathers, and 8%
were with both parents together.
Methods – The authors conducted qualitative
interviews both with the parents and the children. Parents were interviewed
first and the interviews were audiotaped and transcribed. The interviews covered
computer rules, children’s experience in searching, searching habits, and areas
of frustration. Interviews with the children covered questions about frequency
of computer use and reasons for searching. These interviews were video recorded
and transcribed. After the interview, the children were asked to complete five
search tasks, which were video recorded, and were asked if they had
successfully completed the task and why they clicked the link results. The
researchers also took notes throughout the interviews and search tasks.
The
researchers were able to analyze 80 transcripts from the children and 75
transcripts from the parents. The interview transcripts were coded using
inductive, qualitative coding starting with open coding to identify categories
of children’s search roles. The transcripts from the children interviews were
coded three times by one researcher and the coding was verified by another
researcher. The transcripts then were coded again using the code book developed
by the first researcher. The researchers completed axial and selective coding
to refine their search role categories. The researchers also analyzed the data
in order to identify behaviours that distinguished
the categories from each other. The same coding process was used for the parent
interview transcripts. The results from the analysis of the parent interviews
were used to verify findings from the children transcripts.
Main Results – Children searching at home show
seven different searching roles: developing, domain-specific, power, nonmotivated, distracted, rule-bound, and visual, with each
search role being delineated by specific behaviors and/or abilities. Triggers
for searching change as children age, with younger children searching based on
personal interests while older children search for school-related information.
Children rely on summaries shown on the results page, as well as familiarity
with known websites, in deciding which links to click. Children are interested
in both moving and still image results, with visual searchers, power, and distracted
searchers frequently mentioning images in their interviews. Power searchers,
those with the ability to use keywords and with an understanding of search
engines, discussed less influence on their searches than others. Parents have
more influence over younger children while school has more influence over older
children. Parents helped and influenced their children’s searching in varied
ways including demonstrating and offering advice for searching and setting
rules for searching. Children often reported frustration with their searches,
which was also reported by parents. Most of the children were unable to
complete the complex search task as they were unable to separate the query into
multiple parts. Few gender differences in searching were found, although
researchers reported that games were a trigger for boys more often than girls,
and boys declined to search more than girls. Girls were more influenced in
their searching than boys and stopped searching due to boredom more often than
boys.
Conclusion – The authors suggest that the
findings can help search engine designers, researchers, educators, and parents
to assist children in becoming search literate. Designers should enable scaffolded, assisted searching in order to help searchers,
especially with separating out multiple parts of a complex question and with
encouraging fact-checking. Educators and parents can coordinate their efforts
to more effectively help children overcome searching frustrations and
challenges. Researchers could replicate the study to validate the search roles
discovered by the authors and also extend the study to focus on searching in
regards to gender and use of other devices, such as smartphones and tablets.
Commentary
This study
adds to the large body of literature on online searching and contributes to a
greater understanding of children’s searching in the home. As the ability to
search effectively for information online becomes more necessary at a younger
age due to the increased use of computers and mobile devices in grade school
and the need to search in order to complete homework assignments, understanding
children’s searching behaviours and roles becomes
crucial in order to assist them in becoming search literate. This study should
be of great interest for educators and parents who work with the children, as
well as designers who are charged with improving search engines for use by
children.
This study
is well-written and overall well-designed, providing valuable information on
the various roles children conform to while searching at home. This study’s
design and findings are valid according to an evaluation using the Critical
Appraisal Checklist by Glynn (2006). Having multiple researchers
code the transcripts increased the reliability of the results. The search roles
found by the authors do appear to be supported by the data and the authors
explicitly stated research limitations and data loss issues that allowed for
the transcription and use of approximately 90% of the transcripts. The
inclusion of the interview questions/guide would have strengthened the study;
however, the inclusion of the specific search task questions should allow
others to replicate the searching portion of the study if not to replicate exactly
the background interview questions. Also, as the interview questions and search
tasks appear to have not been previously used or validated, the use of a pilot
study would have strengthened the study.
The only
puzzling part of the study is that, although it obviously followed a grounded
theory approach, the authors never explicitly state the use of grounded theory.
However, from the references to the work of Corbin and Strauss (2008), the
construction of the literature review in relation to the results of data
collection and analysis, and the procedures of data analysis, the influence of
grounded theory can be seen clearly. Grounded theory is a reasonable approach
given the exploratory nature of the study and its use in other studies of
search behaviour and information-seeking (Ostrander,
2008).
Understanding
the various search roles of children is an important contribution to the
profession’s knowledge of children’s searching behaviours
and will assist librarians, teachers, and parents in helping children learn to
search more productively. As the researchers note, the findings may also help
designers improve search interfaces. Continued research in this area, including
the authors’ follow-up study on adolescent searchers, will strengthen the
research base, which practitioners can draw on as they create lessons,
activities, and guidelines for helping children become search literate.
References
Corbin, J.,
& Strauss, A.L. (2008). Basics
of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded
theory (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Glynn, L. (2006). A
critical appraisal tool for library and information research. Library
Hi Tech, 24(3), 387-399. doi:
10.1108/07378830610692154
Ostrander,
M. (2008). Talking,
looking, flying, searching: Information seeking behaviour in Second Life. Library Hi Tech, 26(4), 512-524. doi: 10.1108/07378830810920860