Article
Embedded, Participatory
Research: Creating a Grounded Theory with Teenagers
Shannon Crawford Barniskis
Doctoral Student
School of Information
Studies
University of
Wisconsin-Milwaukee
Milwaukee, Wisconsin, United
States
Email: crawfo55@uwm.edu
Received: 7 Sept. 2012 Accepted:
7 Jan. 2013
2013 Crawford Barniskis.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 2.5 Canada (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/ca/), which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Abstract
Objective – This project, based on a study of the impact
of art programs in public libraries on the teenaged participants, sought to
show how library practitioners can perform embedded, participatory research by
adding participants to their research team. Embedded participatory techniques,
when paired with grounded theory methods, build testable theories from the
ground up, based on the real experiences of those involved, including the
librarian. This method offers practical solutions for other librarians while
furthering a theoretical research agenda.
Methods
– This
example of embedded, participatory techniques used grounded theory methods
based on the experiences of teens who participated in art programs at a public
library. Fourteen teens participated in interviews, and six of them assisted in
coding, analyzing, and abstracting the data, and validating the resulting
theory.
Results
– Employing
the teenagers within the research team resulted in a teen-validated theory. The
embedded techniques of the practitioner-researcher resulted in a theory that
can be applied to practice.
Conclusions
–
This research framework develops the body of
literature based on real-world contexts and supports hands-on practitioners. It
also provides evidence-based theory for funding agencies and assessment. In
addition, practitioner-based research that incorporates teens as research
partners activates teens’ voices. It gives them a venue to speak for themselves
with support from an interested and often advocacy-minded adult.
Introduction
Recently I studied how art programs in public
libraries affect teens, using a metric of civic engagement. I was curious to
learn how teenagers describe the impacts of the manga-drawing, poetry-writing, and craft-making
classes offered at many public libraries. Were these programs just fun for
individual participants? Were they for the purpose of making wonderful art?
Both “fun” and “great art” are perfectly acceptable intrinsic reasons for
individuals to participate in art programs in libraries, but do programs like
these positively impact the teens as a group? Are there extrinsic,
community-wide benefits to arts programming? This line of research has
implications that could shift how funders view programs that they sometimes consider as outside of a public
library’s mission. But the most interesting part of this study was the unique
research method my team used. My research team was not a bevy of professionals:
it was the teens whom I was studying, and me, an embedded
researcher-practitioner. Over the course of the research project, the teens and
I built a grounded theory that described their experiences of the programs, and
discovered how the programs increased social capital (Crawford Barniskis, 2012b), using embedded, participatory
techniques. These techniques, when paired with grounded theory methods, build
testable theories from the ground up, based on the real experiences of those
involved, including the librarian. This method offers practical solutions for
other librarians while furthering a theoretical research agenda.
In line with the reflexive nature of this research,
I am writing in the first person, as an “embedded author” telling a story in
which I am not merely a narrator, but a co-creator of meaning.
Background
In library and information science (LIS), as in many
fields, practitioners do little research, and those working in public libraries
do even less. Even though they comprise 28 percent of all librarians (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2012), public
librarians have contributed at most 3.37 percent of the research (Buttlar, 1991). The latest published count by Penta and McKenzie (2005) finds
that public librarians contribute a mere 3 percent to the general LIS
literature. JASIST, the largest
publisher of scholarly LIS work, published work by 1,011 authors, according to
the Penta and McKenzie study. Of those 1,011, not one
was a public librarian. In 2010, of 405 articles in 11 top LIS journals, only 2
percent of the authors identified solely as public librarians. Those public
librarians who did publish research published only in public-library-specific
journals, even though the subjects they studied were often applicable to a
wider audience (Crawford Barniskis, 2010).
Academics do the research in the LIS field, and they are generally academic
faculty, not practitioners.
As Moeller, Pettee, and Leeper (2011) point out, the lack of the “teen voice” in
library research is especially problematic; they call for teens to participate
in the research process, and for their points of view to be fully represented:
Those of us
who are granted the privilege by young adults to use their voices in our
research know that their voices give our work depth, interest, and relevance.
Those who further engage their young adult participants in the process of
member-checking more soundly validate their research and the young adult’s
experience. To produce robust, informative research, we as scholars need to
talk to young adults, not just about them (para. 6).
While I did this research before this call for more
thoroughly integrating teens in studies, this method is a response to the same
concerns of marginalization. However, the embedded, heuristic research method I
used could be effective with adult populations as well. There is no other
evidence of LIS studies done by public librarians as embedded and participatory
members of the group being studied, nor other studies
in which the participants under study are active members of the research team.
Literature Review
The concept of the embedded researcher is not new.
Anthropologists have incorporated themselves into the environments under study
using naturalistic field methods since the 1970s (Clarke, 1975). Since the 1960s, research methodologies and
their epistemic justifications have become more connected, and more aware of
contingencies and biases (Code, 1997; Robson,
2011). Researchers practicing embedded techniques are able to be more
aware and connected than aloof, “unbiased” research approaches may require.
Sociologist Valerie Jenness (2008) defines an embedded researcher as similar to an embedded
journalist, one who is “occupying multiple locations within and under the
control of a single field of play while also moving from one site to another,
one host to another, one level of analysis to the other, and one constituency
to another” (p. 6). Jenness describes the work
embedded researchers do as “systematic back-translation” (p. 4).
In this article, the term “embedded researcher”
reflects the research I did while also acting as a practitioner. In this
framework, the researcher uses heuristic techniques to derive a praxis-based
result, which is verified in partnership with the people being researched. This
definition of “embedded” is different from “embedded design” methodologies, in
which one set of data supports or explicates the other (Creswell, 2012). Instead, this is collaborative research in which
a researcher is a practitioner, or is temporarily submersed in a practitioner’s
environment. There are several related or overlapping frameworks that
researchers use instead of, or in addition to, “embedded research.”
Table 1 describes several types of methodologies that
have one or more points in common with the type of embedded, participatory
research described in this article. In each of these methods, the researcher
participates in the experience under study, utilizes a practice-informed
framework to guide the study, or is particularly sensitive to the context of
the experience under investigation. These research paths can stem from critical
analysis of power inequities by Foucault, Bourdieu, and Friere
(Leckie, Given, & Buschman,
2010), which offer useful paradigms for examining teen perspectives or the
perspectives of other marginalized groups. The pragmatic nature of these
approaches means that whichever framework a researcher chooses, the end result
will have some immediate utility to practitioners. However, the embedded,
participatory framework described in this article combines many of these
frameworks into a single method.
I will not delve deeply into the epistemic pros and
cons of the embedded researcher model I espouse here, but I will touch on a few
points. The justification for this sort of idiographic knowledge goes back to
the qualitative/quantitative break that many researchers analyze and the
tension between positivist and constructivist methodologies.
Table
1
Examples of Similar Frameworks and Methodologies
Similar frameworks and methodologies |
Potential similarities |
Potential differences |
Descriptions in the literature |
Heuristic research |
Hands-on, practice-informed research design. |
Not necessarily embedded researcher, may be interventive or modelling. |
(Poulter, 2006) |
Interdisciplinary or transdisciplinary
research |
Often praxis-based, using multiple frameworks, hermeneutics. |
May be quantitative, no embedded researcher necessary. |
(Wickson, Carew, & Russell, 2006) |
Situated research |
May mean ethnographically situated in the context being studied. |
May also mean “situated” in the sense that the research or theory is
situated in the larger corpus of research (e.g., what I am doing here with
the “embedded researcher” concept). |
(Miller & Goodnow, 1995) |
Action research |
Often involves a practitioner-researcher; results in practical
recommendations for action. |
May be only a local solution, not generalizable or abstracted. |
(Kuhne & Quigley, 1997) |
Participatory research |
Like action research, can “enable local people to seek their own
solutions according to their priorities.” Allows those being researched to
also be in the powerful position of researcher. |
May be only a local solution, not generalizable or abstracted. |
(Cornwall & Jewkes, 1995, p. 1668) |
Design-based research |
Similar to action research, a practical partnership between researcher
and practitioner. |
Focused on interventions, “doing,” and measuring rather than simply
examining. |
(Anderson & Shattuck, 2012) |
The benefits of participatory, embedded research
include:
·
This technique builds relationships between concepts,
academics and practitioners, and the researchers and the researched.
·
It reduces power disparities between practitioners and
researchers (Nutley, Jung, & Walter, 2008).
·
Researchers have a framework and impetus for increased
reflexivity and examination of biases (Hoskins,
2000; Miller & Goodnow, 1995; Reis, 2011).
·
The research is more relevant for practitioners, and
may be more relevant for library boards or funding agencies.
·
This technique incorporates the practitioner’s tacit
knowledge into the research, both methodologically and as an actionable
response to the analysis (Miller & Goodnow,
1995).
·
Researchers who are aware of the subtleties of the
researched environment may more sensitively translate experiences of
marginalized populations (Li, 2008).
·
The research narrows the gap between the actual and
the empirical, and the events and the studied, recalled experiences (Tsoukas, 1989).
Some of the limitations include:
·
The contingent nature of this research requires
significant abstraction for generalizability.
·
Sometimes interactions can feel awkward or forced (Hoskins, 2000).
·
Practitioners or “teachers are usually too busy and
often ill trained to conduct rigorous research” (Anderson
& Shattuck, 2012, p. 17).
·
Challenges abound regarding the “soft” nature of any
qualitative research in its credibility and bias, but especially one in which
the researcher lacks “scientific” distance from the researched.
·
While collaborative methods can bridge the lack of
research knowledge of practitioners and the lack of tacit knowledge of
researchers, such collaboration is costly, time-intensive, or unlikely to occur
in locations where there are no local academics interested in partnerships.
Neither
the limitations nor the benefits of being an embedded researcher can be ignored,
but one benefit is paramount: adolescents, in general, perceive that they lack
a voice, power, and advocates (e.g., Levine,
2008; Marshall & Arvay, 1999;
Ross, 2006).
Practitioner-based research that incorporates teens as research partners
activates the teens’ voice. It gives them a venue to speak for themselves with
the support of an interested and often advocacy-minded adult.
Methods
The
research project used in this study generated a grounded theory through a
qualitative case study structure (Crawford
Barniskis, 2012b). I advertised for applicants in several media outlets,
through library flyers, and in the morning announcements at the local high
school, and
chose 11 teenagers out of a pool of 20 applicants. Those chosen
represented
the widest-possible range of demographics in age, gender, family income level,
church attendance, volunteerism, language spoken at home, and educational
background of parents and teens. The group included five boys and six girls,
ages 12 to 18, though one boy had to leave the study due to scheduling
conflicts. Some were regular library users; some had never been to the library.
Two lived in different towns entirely. This group of teens
had
the opportunity to participate in six art programs at the library and
earn a ten-dollar gift card for each weekly program they attended. They
attended at least five programs and the focus group interview after the program
series ended. The art was varied, including poetry readings, a modern dance
performance, manga and graffiti workshops, and artist trading card and
photography classes. Many other teens attended these programs as well.
At first the participants didn’t know which social
aspects were under investigation. At the beginning of the first program, the
teens filled out a survey asking how strongly they agreed with statements such
as, “I feel like a valued member of my community” and “I think playing sports
or exercising regularly is important.” At the end of the final program they
filled out the same survey. I never intended the pre- and post-test surveys to
generate statistically significant data because the sample size was too small.
Instead, I illustrated the survey data using comparative pie charts, which were
a jumping-off point for discussion during the focus group and individual
interviews.
The focus group interview, held after the series of
programs ended, lasted two hours and involved junk food, 10 teens, and me –
armed with a list of questions, charts, and an audio recorder. The interview
covered questions such as, “How did the art programs you attended here change –
or not change – how you feel about the people in the program? The library? The community?” These
were big questions, and required big answers. I expected that some of the teens
would avoid the hard questions with a joke or a brief answer, e.g. “The
programs didn’t change anything for me.” But I was intrigued to receive complex
and reflective answers from all of the participants, regardless of age or
personality. Early in the planning process for this project, I decided to take
the unusual step of inviting the teens into the researcher’s role by asking any
interested participants to work on the grounded theory coding with me, after
they had finished the interview process and their answers could not be biased
by seeing preliminary results. When I saw that the teens were being careful and
conscientious scientists, I knew I was on the right track. One teen untangled correlation from causation when she
said:
Somebody who makes art is pretty much probably
a smarter person. As someone who writes a letter to the editor is also a
thinking person. And they are likely to do both of those things, but one does
not lead to the other.
Even the youngest participant,
aged 12, stepped back from her own experiences and biases to clarify her
conjectures about others’ motivations: “It depends on the person.” Every teen interviewed
tested the falsifiability of the theory they were generating, establishing the
parameters of “trueness” by exploring who could not or would not be likely to
be affected by library art programs. They did this with little prompting from
me. They were good scientists.
Researchers deduce top-down theories, such as those
described in a meta-synthesis of the literature on this topic (Crawford Barniskis, 2012a), based on previous research (all of which
had been done exclusively by adults, and never in this intersection of art,
libraries, and teens). We inductively developed a “bottom-up” theory based on
the teens’ perceptions, building a testable theory using Charmaz’s
(2006) active coding method. This approach allowed the teens to speak their
language with as little mediation as possible.
Grounded theory is a useful method when a phenomenon
is established but little work has been done to examine it. In this instance,
public libraries have offered art programs to teens for years. No one had
published research on this phenomenon, but practitioners wrote about it in the
form of how-to manuals and descriptive articles. A phenomenological method
would have been appropriate to simply describe the experience of the teens in
these programs, but I was seeking a deeper, more correlative examination of the
impact of the programs – the “why” and “how” as opposed to the “what.”
The basic steps in constructivist grounded theory
construction are iterative (Charmaz, 2006):
·
Coding line by line, or capturing the essence of the
statements using slightly simplified terms. These codes are “open”; the
researcher is not attempting to make them similar or standard at this point
·
Adding more data in the form of memos.
·
Coding sentence by sentence, often at a slightly more
theoretical distance, or other larger granularities.
·
Sorting these codes into groups that make sense.
·
Using constant comparative methods. When describing
this to teens, I used the visual explanation at http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=nxIErzX3aQQ (researchjimminy,
2009), which was more instructive than anything I could write.
·
Discovering the themes in the sorted “piles” of codes.
·
Acquiring more data when the theory needs fleshing
out, using theoretical sampling.
·
Describing the themes to create the theory.
·
Connecting the themes as far as is possible, to make
the theory.
In this project, I recorded and transcribed the focus
group interview, then followed these steps. Using Atlas.ti
software, I lightly coded each line of the interview transcript. I often
listened to the recording to get a sense of the context of the comment, and to
refresh my memory of the non-verbal communication. I coded each line with
simple gerund-based codes that captured the activity occurring in each line.
For example, one teen said:
Yeah, I definitely do feel closer to the libraries that I go to after
going through a program. So it’s like the more programs that you go to the more
it feels kind of like home in a way. Not as really home, but like a really
comforting awesome place.
For this statement, the codes included “feeling closer
to library” and “feeling comforted.”
At this point I returned to six of the teens who had
volunteered to be part of the research team, to make sure I was coding using
language with which they felt comfortable, and that I was capturing what they
were intending to say. This process went beyond what is known as
member-checking, in which researchers show their analysis to participants for
basic validation (Lincoln & Guba, 1985, p. 314).
I met with two teams of teens. The first session included three teens, and
occurred just after I had finished my initial open coding of the focus group
interview. I showed the group a brief video on the constant comparative
process, and spoke briefly about how coding and categorizing worked. The teens
mostly learned by doing. They used my codes, tweaking some of them to better
reflect the intended meaning, and added codes of their own. The teen coders, for
instance, changed the original code “feeling comforted.” They believed that the
library was the critical reason for the comforting feeling this teen
experienced, and had to be included in the code as the active force: “library
comforting.” The teen coders reasoned aloud as they changed and added codes. I
recorded, transcribed, coded, and analyzed the teens’ discussion during the coding sessions, because, as one of the originators of the
grounded theory method notes, “all is data” (Glaser,
2001, p. 145). The second meeting with the teen researchers included one
returning teen and three teens who had not coded before. In this session, I
repeated the video and basic instruction. We worked at a more abstract distance
from the focus group and some individual interview transcripts. This team of
coders was also encouraged to change or add codes, at either open, descriptive
levels, deeper more theoretical levels, or to sort the codes into categories.
They reasoned aloud as they worked with the data.
I treated the transcriptions of these two coding
sessions as data for coding, and as memos of the researchers’ thought
processes. Writing down one’s own feelings, biases, intuitions, and reasoning
is central to the grounded theory method. My memos, as these diary-like notes
are called, were treated as data, as were the thought processes of the teen
researchers. By coding and comparing these memos, I was able to notice and
challenge some of my own biases and assumptions, clarify the teens’ thought
processes, and build a theory more grounded in the teens’ experiences. In
addition, the teen coders validated the nascent theory, keeping me on track.
As a theoretical sample to flesh out the theory, I
interviewed four teens not part of the original focus group, who had attended
some of the art programs. I interviewed these teens one at a time, until new
concepts appeared exhausted, and the theory was fully fleshed out. These teens
were just as sensitive to the contextual experiences of art program
participants as were the focus group teens, and were equally careful
scientists. In addition, the one-on-one interviews offered much more data than
the original focus group interview. While the point of the focus group
interview was that teens could feel comfortable and bounce ideas off each other
– and that did happen – some teens spoke up more than others and depth was
sometimes sacrificed in the interest of everyone having a chance to speak.
Results
In the end, the teens and I had compared over 2,000
lines of open coding, and discovered 60 categories, which we grouped into 7
themes. The central theme was “The library can make a difference for us,” with
the library as locus of change. The remaining themes described what change
occurs through art programming in libraries and how it occurs:
These themes describe the contexts, processes, and activities
of teens who have experienced library art programs and
how these experiences affected their civic engagement. I wrote a paper
describing the theory and how we generated it. Five of the teen researchers
read my written interpretation of the data and verified the theory and
implications. The original intent of the research project came full circle with
the teen validation. The teens themselves decided how legitimate the theory
was, whether statements meant what I assumed they meant, and ensured that the
theory remained firmly grounded in their perspective. Validating the findings
of a research process with the people involved in the study can address issues
of bias. It can also signal to the participants whether their concerns were
heard and understood, which can be helpful when dealing with marginalized
groups.
Discussion
Our research described how art programs in public
libraries positively affect the social capital of teens, including their sense
of power and capability. The programs revealed new role models, friends and
advocates, as well as new ways to civically engage. Teens experienced
heightened empathy, and shifted their concept of the library from book-place to
creation-space in a way that empowered both creativity and social connectedness.
The teens felt more valued by their community after the programs and were able
to pinpoint why and how this shift occurred. The art programs supported the
skills, values, and motivation for civic engagement through these social
connections and the activities in the programs. However, we revealed little
evidence that library art programs bridge the “activation gap” (Rheingold, 2008) between the desire to engage
and engagement behaviour such as volunteering or
political activism. The gap between the building blocks of civic engagement and
the actual engagement behaviour is significant. The
teens in this study described their intent to engage inchoately and
fragmentarily, and the factors of active engagement need to be explored more
thoroughly in further research. Nevertheless, the data reveals an intent for
the teens to bridge the activation gap and become more civically engaged with
the understanding that they would be supported by public library programming in
specific ways that address their concerns of adult hegemony and limited time to
engage. Since a grounded theory should reveal why and how something occurs,
researchers can use quantitative methods to determine the extent to which the
themes play out in a larger sample population. Practitioners can examine their
programs to see if or how they would want to address this gap.
Implications
This project discovered implications for further
research. The embedded participatory research process reveals an ethnographic description
of a problem or experience, but also allows for the discovery of conceptual
associations for further examination. In this project, we were able to uncover
several themes, and qualitative studies can now test these themes. Many library
services could benefit from similar studies. Libraries are often under attack
during the funding process and must justify their existence to those who do not
value the intrinsic benefits of funding such programs.
Research is needed to addresses teen participation as
research partners and the effects of this participation. Participation as
research partners may improve skills in research or critical thinking, teen
self-image, and teen perception of the institution that offers them an
opportunity to participate. One may wonder if research participation
opportunities, such as the one described in this paper, increase the social
capital of teens. If this is true in this research project, how much of the
teens’ shift in civic engagement and social capital derived from the art
program participation, and how much derived from participating as researchers?
It is difficult to say. Still, the teens described most of the increases in
social capital before they acted as part of the research team, and even before
that they knew they would have the opportunity to do so. In addition, those
teens who did not participate as researchers described similar shifts. Further
research should consider whether acting as a researcher impacts the teens’
social capital, educational outcomes, or otherwise positively affects teens. In
this way, the body of theoretical knowledge for LIS scholars can be improved
with more embedded, heuristic research.
The project also made visible several implications for
practice. Outcome-based research can help to justify a library’s need for
funds. How do library services affect civic engagement, or student satisfaction
with their school, or bullying, or … ? Pick an outcome
that a funding agency cares about, and determine if or how your program helps.
Such research may ignore or marginalize the individual, intrinsic benefits of
library programs, such as having a good time and making friends, but few
funding agencies appear to care about such intrinsic benefits. They are more
interested in how a library service furthers their social agenda. Social
agendas such as that of educational attainment and civic engagement are good touchpoints for funders interested in attaining educational
and democratic goals. The pedagogical benefits for students who learn research
skills could impact their educational attainment.
Librarians need more evidence-based practice,
especially research grounded in the users’ experience. Librarians already
collect and share assessments of various programs, often through surveys or
informal interviews. Expanding local assessments into generalizable research
may be a more or less seamless process, leveraging these surveys and interviews
into user-validated results. Dissemination of research results can help improve
the practice of other librarians, because the research is often based on
pragmatic situations which practitioners can emulate.
The act of research itself also may improve practice.
In my case, I became more aware of the smallness of the teen world, their lack
of opportunities to create and display art, and the way they feel
systematically silenced by the institutions that rule their lives. This
improved my work as a librarian. During this research project, I was able to
extrapolate from the findings to offer many ways for public librarians to
support civic engagement for teens. The teens offered many of these
suggestions, and validated the ones I generated. Such recommendations are
responses to the librarian’s need for more evidence based practices. As
practitioner-researchers delve deeply into their work and the impact it has on
those they serve, the feedback can reinforce the best of their work and help
better align outcomes with the needs of users. In fact, this research method
blurs the line between research and practice. This research project bled into
my practice, and emerged from it, being at the same time a recursive “cause”
and “effect.” While the research project was a formal, albeit embedded,
grounded theory study, it evolved into the
participatory heuristic described in this paper. The method is the message, to
paraphrase McLuhan.
Finally, doing research with teens is fun. Though it
can be difficult to strip away assumptions based on one’s own teen years, or
adult “wisdom,” hearing the teens describe their experiences is enlightening
and enjoyable. Reflexively, I know that enjoying being in a position to ensure
teens are heard is ego-based. Still, it is satisfying to identify a problem
(teens feel voiceless), offer a small, local, and temporary solution (a few are
given a venue to speak), and perhaps a more lasting and pervasive one (if
others take on such research, many teens will be offered venues to speak). This
research process can act as an amplification of the participants’ voices.
Conclusion
The method described in this paper is recursive not
only in the methodology – in the way it coded the coding process and utilized
the voices of the participants to drive the research – but also in that the
phenomenon under study, social capital, may have been furthered by the research
method itself. The framework straddles the intersection of research and
practice. Yet the method is emergent, and needs further trial. It has both
theoretical and practical implications for librarians, researchers,
communities, and users of library services. Practitioners who do research in
partnership with those they are serving can make new connections between their
practice and the big picture of librarianship. They can support
transformational experiences for those they serve, aside from and in concert with
the services themselves. Librarians further expand, improve, legitimize, and
advocate for their field of work when they publish research. Embedded,
participatory methods build testable worldviews from the ground up, based on
the real experiences of those involved, offering practical solutions for other
librarians while furthering a useful research agenda. Users and communities
benefit from these embedded, participatory research processes when their
stories are authentically conveyed and conceptualized and their voices are
amplified, and they sample the skills and habits of research. Libraries can
benefit from the establishment of user-verified outcomes of programs and
services. This is especially valuable when libraries are trying to demonstrate
how their services benefit the communities that fund them.
References
Anderson, T., & Shattuck, J. (2012). Design-based research:
A decade of progress in education research? Educational Researcher, 41(1),
16-25. doi:10.3102/0013189X11428813
Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor
(2012). Librarians. In Occupational outlook handbook (2012-13 ed.) Retrieved 15 Aug. 2012, from http://www.bls.gov/ooh/education-training-and-library/librarians.htm
Buttlar, L. (1991). Analyzing the library periodical literature: Content and
authorship. College & Research Libraries, 52(1), 38-53.
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative
analysis. London: SAGE Publications.
Clarke, M. (1975). Survival in the field:
Implications of personal experience in fieldwork. Theory and Society, 2(1),
95-123.
Code, L. (1997). Voice and voicelessness: A modest proposal? In J. Kourany (Ed.), Philosophy in a Different Voice (pp.
204-230). Princeton, CT: Princeton University Press.
Cornwall, A., & Jewkes,
R. (1995). What
is participatory research? Social Science & Medicine, 41(12), 1667-1676.
Crawford
Barniskis, S. (2010). Public
practitioner-researchers: How LIS research is informed by praxis. Paper
presented at the Student Research Symposium 2010, Milwaukee, WI.
Crawford Barniskis, S. (2012). Graffiti, poetry, dance: How public
library art programs affect teens. Part 1: Introduction & literature
review. Journal of Research on Libraries and Young Adults,
2(3). Retrieved 3 Mar. 2013 from http://www.yalsa.ala.org/jrlya/2012/09/graffiti-poetry-dance-how-public-library-art-programs-affect-teens-part-1-introduction-literature-review/
Crawford Barniskis, S. (2012). Graffiti, poetry, dance: How public
library art programs affect teens. Part 2: The research study and its practical
implications. Journal of Research on Libraries and Young
Adults, 2(3). Retrieved 3 Mar. 2013 from http://www.yalsa.ala.org/jrlya/2012/09/graffiti-poetry-dance-how-public-library-art-programs-affect-teens-part-2-the-research-study-and-its-practical-implications/
Creswell,
J. W. (2012). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five
approaches (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Finlay,
L. (2002). Negotiating the swamp: The opportunity and
challenge of reflexivity in research practice. Qualitative Research,
2(2), 209-230.
Flowers,
S. (2010). Young adults deserve the best: YALSA's competencies in action.
Chicago, IL: ALA Editions.
Glaser,
B. G. (2001). The grounded theory perspective: Conceptualization contrasted
with description. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press.
Hoskins,
M. L. (2000). Living research: The experience of researching self, other, and
discourse. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 13(1), 47-66.
Jenness, V.
(2008).
Pluto, prisons, and plaintiffs: Notes on systematic back-translation from an
embedded researcher. Social Problems, 55(1), 1-22.
Kuhne, G.
W., & Quigley, B. A. (1997). Understanding
and using action research in practice settings. In B. A. Quigley &
G. W. Kuhne (Eds.), Creating practical knowledge
through action research (pp. 23-40). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Leckie, G. J., Given, L. M.,
& Buschman, J. E. (Eds.). (2010). Critical theory
for library and information science: Exploring the social across the
disciplines. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.
Levine, P. (2008). A
public voice for youth: The audience problem in digital media and civic
education. In W. L. Bennett (Ed.), Civic life online: Learning how digital
media can engage youth (pp. 119-138). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Li,
J. (2008). Ethical challenges in participant observation: A reflection on
ethnographic fieldwork. The Qualitative Report, 13(1), 100-115.
Lincon, Y. S., & Guba,
E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Newbury Park, CA,
Sage Publications.
Marshall, A. M., & Arvay,
M. (1999). Perspectives on voice and sense of self among young adolescents.
Professional School Counseling, 3(1), 43-51.
Miller, P. J., & Goodnow,
J. J. (1995).
Cultural practices: Toward an integration of culture and development. New
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1995(67), 5-16.
Moeller, R., Pattee, A.,
& Leeper, A. (2011). The
young adult voice in research about young adults. Journal
of Research on Libraries and Young Adults. Retrieved 30 Nov. 2011 from
http://www.yalsa.ala.org/jrlya/2011/11/the-young-adult-voice-in-research-about-young-adults/
Nutley, S., Jung, T., & Walter, I. (2008). The many forms of
research-informed practice: A framework for mapping diversity. Cambridge
Journal of Education, 38(1), 53-71.
Penta, M.,
& McKenzie, P. J. (2005). The big gap remains:
Public librarians as authors in US journals, 199-2003. Public Library
Quarterly, 24(1), 33-46.
Poulter, J.
(2006). The
two embedded research functions of heuristic case practice. Australian Social
Work, 59(3), 328-341.
Reis, A. C. (2011). Bringing
my creative self to the fore. Creative Approaches to Research, 4(1),
2-18.
researchjimminy. (2009, June 21).
Constant comparison stop motion demo [Video file]. Retrieved 20 Jan. 2013 from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=nxIErzX3aQQ
Rheingold,
H. (2008). Using participatory media and public voice to
encourage civic engagement. In W. L. Bennett (Ed.), Civic life online:
Learning how digital media can engage youth (pp. 97-118). Cambridge, MA: The
MIT Press.
Robson,
C. (2011). Real world research: A resource for users of social-research mthods in applied settings (3rd ed.).
Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Ross,
L. (2006). Where do we belong? Urban adolescents' struggle
for place and voice. American Journal of Community Psychology, 37(3/4),
293-301. doi:10.1007/s10464-006-9041-8
Tsoukas, H. (1989). The validity of idiographic research explanations. The
Academy of Management Review, 14(4), 551-561.
Vittek, R. E. (2010). The
people in your neighborhood: Using local collaboration to advocate for teen
patrons. Young Adult Library Services, 9(1), 13-14.
Wickson, F.,
Carew, A. L., & Russell, A. W. (2006). Transdisciplinary
research: Characteristics, quandaries and quality. Futures, 38(9), 1046-1059.
doi:10.1016/j.futures.2006.02.011