About the Author(s)


Sadna Balton Email symbol
Center for Alternate and Augmentative Communication, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa

Speech Therapy & Audiology, Chris Hani Baragwanath Academic Hospital, Soweto, South Africa

Kitty Uys symbol
Department of Occupational Therapy, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa

Erna Alant symbol
Center for Alternate and Augmentative Communication, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa

Department of Special Education, Indiana University, Bloomington, United States

Citation


Balton, S., Uys, K. & Alant, E., 2019, `Family-based activity settings of children in a low-income African context’, African Journal of Disability 8(0), a364. https://doi.org/10.4102/ajod.v8i0.364

Original Research

Family-based activity settings of children in a low-income African context

Sadna Balton, Kitty Uys, Erna Alant

Received: 07 Feb. 2017; Accepted: 30 Aug. 2018; Published: 23 Apr. 2019

Copyright: © 2019. The Author(s). Licensee: AOSIS.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Background: There has been an overwhelming call to improve the understanding of how children develop within an African context as Euro-American definitions of competence have been uncritically adopted as the norm for children in Africa. The activities that children engage in within the family setting are seen as important to understand how children develop within context. The use of activity settings is closely aligned with a strengths-based perspective of family-centred practice and contributes to improved sustainability of intervention.

Objectives: This study that was conducted in Soweto, South Africa, aims to describe activity settings that typically developing young children in low-income African contexts participate in.

Method: A descriptive design using structured interviews was utilised to obtain information about activity settings that children aged 3–5 years and 11 months engaged in. Structured interviews with 90 caregivers were conducted.

Results: Findings show that children participate in a variety of activities with varied participation levels. The types of activities are dependent on the context and perceptions of caregivers.

Conclusion: These findings draw attention to understanding activities that children engage in within the family context.

Keywords: activity settings; culture; family; indigenous knowledge; intervention; contribute; low income; utilised; Euro-American; poor sustainability.

Introduction

Children within the African context have historically been judged by Euro-American definitions of competence, which have uncritically been adopted as the norm for all children (Nsamenang 2008a, 2008b; Pence, Evans & Garcia 2008; Pence & Schafer 2006). Ecocultural theory proposes that indigenous conceptions of competence should be uncovered by looking at how children are reared according to what parents know about what would be useful for their children within their specific communities (Berry 2003; Norton 1990). Various studies (Beckert, Strom & Strom 2004; Bornstein & Cote 2004; Evans, Matola & Nyeko 2008; Gaskins 1999; Geiger & Alant 2008; Rao, McHale & Pearson 2003) have shown that each culture focuses on what is valued and valid within its specific context (Serpell & Marfo 2011; Super et al. 2011). According to Ecocultural theory, development occurs along pathways determined by culture and society, and actively chosen and engaged in by parents and children, within a particular cultural ecology (Weisner 2002; Weisner et al. 2005). These pathways consist of activities and practices that are viewed as being the most important influences in the child’s and family’s life (Bernheimer & Weisner 2007). Children experience different kinds of learning opportunities, depending on where they live, what their parents enjoy doing and their values and desires for their children and families (Dunst & Bruder 1999). It is within this context that the family context provides developmental pathways for children, which are made up of the everyday routines that children engage in (Weisner 2002). The family therefore is the primary milieu in which children begin to learn the competencies expected of them within their culture and community (Britto & Ulkeur 2012; Turnbull, Turbiville & Turnbull 2000).

Activity settings which consist of the everyday experiences and events that involve the child’s interactions with various people and the environment have been recommended as the method of looking at the child within context (Farver 1999; Trivette, Dunst & Hamby 2004). Through participation in activity settings, children learn what is expected of them and learn how to determine which activities are considered appropriate or inappropriate (Tudge et al. 2013). According to Weisner (2002), parents want children to have the dispositions that would help them gain relevant skills to maintain a respectful life in their community and family. Children learn these skills through activity settings, which provide an understanding of how families structure their time, based on tradition, the socio-economic system within which they live and the orientations provided by culture (Goldenberg, Gallimore & Reese 2001; Tudge et al. 2013). Activities are therefore useful units for cultural analysis, because they are meaningful for both parents and children (Weisner 2002).

Dunst (2007) defined early childhood intervention as the experiences and opportunities afforded to children with disabilities by their parents and other caregivers that are intended to promote their competencies to shape and influence their interactions with people and objects. It therefore becomes critical for early childhood interventionists to gain insight into the activities that young children are exposed to within family settings as this influences their participation, engagement and learning.

This article describes the of activities that children living in Soweto, South Africa, participate in within the family context. The types of activities, frequency of participation and the importance of activities as rated by caregivers are discussed.

Method

Design

A descriptive design using structured interviews was utilised to obtain information about the activity settings that children aged 3–5 years and 11 months engaged in. Face-to-face interviews with 90 caregivers were conducted. A structured interview schedule consisting of a written list of closed-ended and open-ended questions was used. This approach was chosen as it holds no bias against participants who have varied literacy levels. Ethical clearance was obtained from the ethics committee in the Faculty of Humanities at the university.

Setting

Participants reside in Soweto, South Africa. Soweto is a large residential urban area where a diverse group of African cultural groups reside. This city has seen rapid development and transformation over the past few years (Phadi & Ceruti 2011). However, a large proportion of Soweto’s residents remain unemployed (Patel 2012) and accommodation is mostly limited to small houses with limited space (Seekings 2000). The extended family system, which consists of multi-generational female-headed households, is still the most prevalent structural family form in Soweto (Moeno 2006).

Sampling

Permission to conduct the study at African Self-Help Association’s (ASHA) crèches in Soweto, South Africa, was obtained in writing from the director of ASHA. Four crèches were randomly selected from a group of 40 crèches run by a non-governmental organisation in Soweto. Consent letters were sent to the caregivers of children who met the selection criteria of being between 3 and 5 years and 11 months with no known disability. Typically developing children were identified by the ASHA’s inclusion coordinator who is responsible for developmental screening at the crèches. Participants were selected through a stratified sampling procedure that accounted for age, gender and development. Ninety participants were assigned to groups of children from three-, four- and five-year age groups with equal gender representation among the children.

Participants

The majority (56%, n = 50) of participants were mothers, followed by grandmothers (20%, n = 20), fathers (10%, n = 9) and others which included cousins, siblings and neighbours (10%, n = 9) and aunts (4%, n = 4). The age of participants ranged from 16 to 72 years, with 52% of participants being under 32 years, 22% between 35 and 45 years, 17% between 46 and 59 years and 9% over 60 years. Of the participants, only 20% completed higher education; 38% completed matric, which is the final year of high school in South Africa; 31% completed secondary school; 10% completed senior primary; and 1% completed junior primary. The monthly income of the majority (75%) of families was below the minimum individual taxable income of R4500.00 (USD $67.79) per month (South African Revenue Services [SARS] 2008). An average of 5.6 family members share a four-room house.

Description of interview schedule

The interview schedule was based on the Parent Survey of Home and Family Experiences (Dunst & Bruder 1999). Permission to use the survey was obtained in a face-to-face meeting with one of the authors. The survey was adapted for the South African context through an expert panel and focus group discussions (Balton 2009). The expert panel consisted of three speech therapy assistants employed at a hospital in Soweto. The speech therapy assistants have over 20 years of experience each in working with families from the Soweto community and also live within the same community as the target population. The interview schedule (Appendix 1) included a list of closed-ended and open-ended questions. Part 1 included biographical information and part 2 contained 50 closed-ended questions relating to activities that children participated in. The following eight main categories of activities were included: (1) child routine activities (family mealtimes, bathing, dressing and undressing, toileting, washing hands, brushing teeth, haircut or styles, visiting the local clinic and carried on the back); (2) play activities (running, jumping and chasing, playing with toys, pretend games, lap games, playing with water, playing with sand, hand or finger games, ‘mokuku’ hide-and-seek, building blocks, arcade games and cell phone games); (3) early literacy activities (having a conversation, telling stories, listening to stories, reading or looking at books, colouring, drawing, painting, cutting and pasting); (4) entertainment activities (watching television, singing, listening to music and dancing); (5) chores (cleaning the yard, washing socks and underwear, setting the table, assisting with preparing meals and gardening); (6) spiritual activities (attending church, praying, attending an ancestral ceremony and attending funerals); (7) family activities (family gatherings, visiting family or friends in the neighbourhood and visiting the family or traditional home); (8) community activities (visiting shopping malls, eating out, going to the ‘spaza’ informal shop, attending parties, attending weddings, taxi rides and visiting a park).

In part 2 of the interview, participants were asked to comment on the frequency of participation and reasons for non-participation and to identify the partners involved in the activity with the child and state the main purpose of the activity. The participants were also required to rate each activity in terms of its importance for learning on a scale of 1–3, with 1 = not important, 2 = important and 3 = very important. Part 3 of the interview was composed of four open-ended questions to obtain insight into caregiver beliefs and perceptions about activities.

General procedures
Data collection procedures

The scripted, structured face-to-face interviews were conducted by the researcher at one of three crèches. The interviews were audio-taped and took approximately 35 minutes to complete. The researcher commenced the interview by stating its purpose and allowing participants time for questions. Instructions were read out exactly as they appeared on the interview schedule, following a sequential order of questions and using the same material for all interviews (Mathers, Fox & Hunn 2002). The responses were recorded directly on the interview schedule. The interview concluded with the interviewer thanking the participant and allowing time for any further questions about the study.

Reliability of data

Thirty per cent of the interviews were randomly selected by two speech-language therapists who checked the reliability of the recording and the coding of information (McMillan & Schumacher 2001). Inter-rater agreement of 100% was achieved.

Data analysis

The data were analysed using both qualitative and quantitative methods. Descriptive statistical procedures, in particular frequency tables, were used to organise the data collected. The results were quantified in terms of means, standard deviation, frequencies and relationships between variables. A content analysis procedure was conducted on responses to open-ended questions in order to identify common categories that were then utilised to establish codes.

Ethical considerations

Ethical clearance was obtained from the ethical committee at the University of Pretoria, reference number: 21277177.

Results

Table 1 shows the percentage participation rate in activities as well as the frequency (daily, weekly, monthly and annually or none) of participation. Participation levels were subjectively divided into high (80% – 100%), moderate (50% – 79%) and low (< 50%). High daily participation was recorded for family meals (100%), bathing (100%), dressing and undressing (100%), toileting (100%), washing hands (100%), brushing teeth (99%), having a conversation (95%), watching television (92%) and listening to music (87%). Moderate daily participation rates were indicated for playing with toys and praying (78%), singing (72%), dancing (57%), colouring and pretend games (52%), playing with water and reading and looking at books. High weekly participation rates were recorded for attending church (72%) and visiting shopping malls (56%). Moderate monthly participation rates were indicated for attending parties (67%), having a haircut (61%), eating out (59%), family gatherings and going to the clinic (52%).

TABLE 1: Frequency of participation (n = 90).

Low participation rates (< 50%) were reported for attending weddings (49%), cleaning the yard (48%), washing socks and underwear and cell phone games (44%), setting the table (38%), assisting with preparation of meals (37%), attending an ancestral ceremony and gardening (35%) and attending funerals (19%).

Caregiver perceptions of activities that are important for learning

Caregivers were asked to rate the importance of activities for learning by stating if an activity was very important, important or not important. The results displayed in Table 2 indicate the mean score obtained, with the maximum being 3 and the minimum 1. Activities rated as very important have a mean of 2.5–3.0, activities rated as important have a mean of 2.00–2.49 and not important has a mean score of < 2 (see Table 2).

TABLE 2: Caregiver ratings on the importance of activities for learning.

The categories rated as very important for learning include most child routine activities and all early literacy activities, with colouring, drawing and painting (mean = 2.74, SD = 0.43) rated the most important of all activities. Other activities in this category include having a conversation (mean = 2.68, SD = 0.46), reading or looking at books (mean = 2.65, SD = 0.47), listening to stories (mean = 2.62, SD = 0.51), cutting and pasting (mean = 2.61, SD = 0.51) and telling stories (mean = 2.53, SD = 0.50). Spiritual activities rated as very important consisted of attending church (mean = 2.74, SD = 0.45) and praying (mean = 2.66, SD = 0.47). All activities in the entertainment, chores and family category were rated as important for learning. Most play activities except for playing with water and playing with sand were rated as important for learning. Two activities from the play category, namely playing with water and playing with sand, were rated as not important for learning.

Caregivers were also requested to state what they viewed as the main purpose of an activity according to the following categories: fun, work, socialisation, care, educational, exercise, spiritual and other. The percentage was calculated for each category (see Table 3).

TABLE 3: Caregivers perceptions about the purpose of activities (N = 90).

Results show that most activities from the play and entertainment categories were perceived as fun, while family activities were considered to serve the purpose of socialisation. Only four activities were perceived care, as these were from the child routine and community categories. Activities identified as educational were mainly from the early literacy and child routine categories. No play activities were highly rated as educational. All activities in the spiritual activity category were considered as having a spiritual purpose. An insignificant percentage of activities were seen as work or being done for the purpose of exercise.

Open-ended questions

The first open-ended question aimed at determining if there were any activities that the children participated in which were not included in the questionnaire. Of the participants, 54% did not add any further activities and 24% added play activities which included soccer, wrestling and basketball. The second open-ended question explored what participants considered as important lessons that children should learn from home. Fifty per cent of participants identified morals and values as important lessons, 48% identified self-care and hygiene while only 22% stated that educational and literacy activities were important lessons from home. The third open-ended question surveyed participants’ perceptions on what activities the child enjoyed most at home. Of the participants, 80% stated that children enjoyed playing, 58% listed entertainment and social activities and 56% stated that children enjoyed singing and dancing. The forth open-ended question required the respondents to share their perceptions on how children learnt, from which four themes were identified. Fifty-three per cent of participants stated that children learnt best by participating in activities, 27% stated through spending time with family, 19% stated by being at a crèche and 12% stated through observation.

Discussion

Families provide a rich cultural context in which children learn and develop (Carpenter 2000). Gaining insight into children’s activity settings within the family context is important as families prefer intervention approaches that can easily be incorporated into their daily lives (Sheldon & Rush 2001) and are congruent with their beliefs and practices. The discussion will focus on the frequency of participation in activities within the eight categories identified earlier as well as how these activities provide an opportunity for learning within the family context.

Child routine activities, which are mainly essential for daily care, have the highest weekly participation rates and were rated by participants as very important for learning (see Table 2). This is consistent with the literature, as Dunst, Meter and Hamby (2001) suggest that the repetitiveness and frequency of occurrence of routine activities provide children with an opportunity to learn and practise new skills within context. The importance of self-care and hygiene was stated as an important lesson from home and seen by most participants as having an educational purpose. These activities also allow children to gain insight into family culture. Larson and colleagues explain that mealtime is seen as a vehicle of culture because ‘through mealtime activities and conversation, family members often enact and reaffirm cultural meanings and create new meaning’ (Larson, Branscomb & Wiley 2006:3). Jarret, Bahar and Kersh (2014) showed that low-income African American caregivers showed that they valued family mealtimes and acknowledged the benefits for family life. Various studies have shown the benefits of family mealtimes; this activity provides an opportunity for children to learn new words in context (Beals 1997), for parents to listen to children talk about their daily lives (Fulkerson et al. 2010) and has also been associated with enhancing family cohesion and contributing to positive developmental outcomes. A study conducted by De Grace et al. (2016) showed the benefits of improved social and family outcomes for children with special therapeutic and behavioural needs. The positive benefits of family mealtimes highlight the need for early childhood interventionists to look at strategies to increase the participation of children with disabilities in this key family activity.

The role of play in child development has historically been applied by looking at it through the lens of western cultures. According to Roopnarine and Davidson (2015:231), play is ‘culturally situated, and mothers and fathers support play in multiple ways across cultures and time’ This depends on how the community is structured, how play is defined and the kind of significance attributed to play by the community (Göncü et al. 1999). Children in this study most frequently engage in running, jumping and chasing, playing with toys and pretend games. Participants viewed most play activities as having a fun purpose for children. These results correlate with a study of 127 families across 28 developing countries which found that taking children outdoors and play were the most predominant activities that children were involved in (Bornstein & Putnick 2012). Pretend play themes are often linked to culture (Nielsen, Cucchiaro & Mohamedally 2012) as different cultural groups may engage in pretend play for different purposes and play themes may vary according to children’s settings (Göncü et al. 1999). Participants in the focus groups that were part of the preparatory phase of this study stated that children liked pretending to be a mother by tying a doll on their backs, or being a taxi driver or a teacher. These are the roles that children are regularly exposed to in their daily settings. Furth (1996) showed how cultural practices link to pretend play in a township in Durban, South Africa, where children were pretending to slaughter a cow which is a ‘real-world’ activity that is transferred into children’s pretend world (Göncü et al. 1999). Results indicate that children frequently participate in water play which is seen as having a fun purpose, and was rated as not important for learning. Caregiver views on water play may be related to the fact that water is considered an expensive commodity in South Africa. Water is free up to 6000 L per household and usage is monitored by pre-pay water metres (Ruiters 2007). This resource is also shared by large families and sometimes by more than one family and is therefore unlikely to be used in play activities. The lower frequency of play with sand could be that most families do not keep gardens because it is expensive to maintain because of the cost of water and that access to sand may be limited because of lack of space (Balton 2009). Early interventionists need to explore alternate activities within the family context that can provide children with alternate sensory experiences that sand and water play would expose them to. This could include activities such as assisting with washing vegetables for cooking, helping to measure and mix ingredients during baking and making a fruit salad to explore different textures.

Colouring, drawing and pasting was rated as the most important activity for learning; this is an interesting ranking as it was seen as more important than reading or looking at books and telling stories. This perception may be based on the nature of activities that children take home from school or that colouring and drawing may not require adult supervision. Children engage in conversation on a daily basis, and the topics of these conversations include what they did at school, details of their play with friends and discussions of what they watched on television (Balton 2009). Telling stories occurs less frequently than listening to stories, which has been identified as a means for family history to be shared with young children, thus providing an avenue for values to be imparted (Sameroff & Fiese 2000). Participants in the focus group phase of this study stated that grandmothers often told children stories about their past to teach children lessons and for them to learn about their family’s history (Balton 2009). According to Ouduaran (2013), in African culture, grandmothers often teach younger generations about African wisdom and culture through storytelling.

Children’s participation in entertainment activities is important for literacy development as it increases children’s ability to shape and understand the available meanings in any number of expressive systems including the media, the arts and popular culture (Dills 2007; Eisner 1998). Children’s high participation rate for watching television suggests that this activity plays a significant role in their daily experiences. Results also showed that 70% of the participants perceive watching television as having an educational purpose. Children are allowed to watch television because it is believed to improve their English as well as their concentration, and that it is much safer than playing outdoors (Balton 2009). This sentiment on safety was echoed by Jordan (2005) who interviewed 42 families who live in high-crime areas, where watching television was seen as a safe and relatively inexpensive way of keeping young children occupied. Burdette and Whitaker (2005) also found in a sample of 20 large cities in in the United States (US) that mothers’ perceptions of neighbourhood safety impacted on children’s viewing time. Their findings showed that children who lived in neighbourhoods that were perceived as unsafe watched more television.

Children’s high participation in singing, dancing and listening to music could be ascribed to the fact that music and music making is an inherent part of South African culture which assists in the transmission of its cultural heritage (Woodward 2007). These activities, especially music, are highly accessible in daily life in varied settings (Getz et al. 2011). Participants viewed singing and listening to music as fun and educational; one of the participants in the focus group stated that he got his child to sing the national anthem to learn about his country (Balton 2009). This is important as researchers in the field of early literacy have realised that promoting literacy at home no longer means recreating academic settings within the home but rather taking advantage of opportunities that arise in daily life to help children’s transition towards literacy (Cutspec 2006). Interventionists need to take cognisance of these activities as potential avenues for facilitating early literacy, because a high number of children frequently engage in them and because participants identified the educational worth of these activities.

Religion and spirituality play an important role in children’s lives and are vital to family relationships (Bartowski, Xu & Levin 2008) and in African traditional practice, religion is integral to people’s cultural background (Van Rensburg et al. 2013). Very high daily participation rates for praying and attending church weekly are shown in Table 2. Participants in the focus groups reported that children attend church to learn about their religion to become good Christians, to learn how to pray and to be thankful to God for what they have (Balton 2009). Religious activity is also reported to increase children’s resilience and provide a sense of coherence within the family (Bartowski et al. 2008; Mercer 2006; Werner 2000). Most participants identified morals and values as important lessons from home, which ties in with the high participation rate for spiritual activities.

Community life also provides children with a range of experiences in the contexts of family outings, community celebrations and other community activities (Dunst 2001). In recent years, visiting shopping malls has rapidly become an important and valuable ‘cultural’ form which is popularly seen as a mixture of convenience and leisure (Murray 1997). The accessibility of shopping malls to residents of Soweto has increased over the past 5 years, with two major malls built in 2005 and another three in 2007 (Mazibuko 2007). Visits to shopping malls are linked to participation in other activities like playing arcade games and eating out. Participants in the focus groups stated that they prefer to take children to the shopping malls because they were safer than other spaces like community parks. The high participation rate for visiting shopping malls highlights the lack of safe spaces for children to play in communities.

The activity that most children participate in at least once a month is going to the ‘spaza shop’, which is a home-based enterprise often within walking distance of children’s homes (Ligthelm 2005). This errand is reported to provide children with opportunities to learn about the environment, the dynamics of interacting with others and offers the opportunity to practise being helpful and responsible, which are important lessons in African culture (Nsamenang 1992).

The lower participation levels for being carried on the back were attributed to the age of children in this study. The reasons provided by participants for children’s lower participation levels for riding a bike or scooter and playing with blocks are ascribed to the lack of money to purchase these toys (Balton 2009). Children’s participation in playing arcade games, which are relatively expensive, highlights caregiver’s concerns related to safety as these games are played in a contained area under adult supervision. The findings of studies conducted in Australia by Carver, Timperio and Crawford (2008) and Veitch et al. (2006) and in South Africa by Kruger and Chawla (2005) concur with this statement. Their research concluded that parents’ issues about the safety of their children playing in places other than their own yard were influenced by concerns surrounding strangers, gangs and road traffic. The physical settings have also influenced children’s participation in ‘mokuku’ (hide-and-seek) as caregivers stated concerns about safety and lack of space.

Many shopping malls have been built in Soweto over the past 5 years; this has increased the availability of fast food outlets with most children eating out at least once a month. The mean score for taxi rides also indicates that most children travel by taxi once a month, which relates to the results which show that activities away from home and which require more money occur less frequently. Other activities which children participate in at least once a month include attending parties and family gatherings. While children’s participation in chores in parts of Africa have historically been an expected activity (Nsamenang 1992), results showed that the only chore activity that most children participate in is washing socks and underwear. Children’s participation in chores and work-related activities can be attributed to a number of factors and identifying a causal relationship is not possible for this complex matter. Poverty, social and economic factors, children’s rights, family size, female-headed households and whether it is an urban or rural community are some of the factors identified, which have impact on children’s participation in work-related activities (Cummings 2016). The reason for children’s low participation in chores could be attributed to the young age group investigated in this study, the larger family structure where other family members take on the responsibilities or that children spend a large proportion of their time at crèche.

Children hardly ever participated in chores like cleaning the yard and family activities such as visiting the family or traditional home. While participation levels were high for religious activity, it was much lower for traditional practices such as ancestral ceremonies and attendance at funerals. This could be because key traditional practices have been replaced by modern ones (Evans, Matola & Nyeko 2008). Less frequent visits are also conducted to the family or traditional home with participants stating that children visit at least once a year. This is understandable within the context of urbanisation, often implying that the traditional home is far from where families live and visiting would therefore incur expenses that the family may not be able to afford. Children’s visits to the park were reported as less frequent, which could be because of the fear of exposure to drugs, violence, vandalism and parents having less time available because of various stressors (Milteer & Ginberg 2012). Participants in the focus group stage of the study stated that they did not consider parks as safe for children as they were not clean and often had broken glass on the field and that there were possible criminal and drug-related activities taking place at these places (Balton 2009).

Conclusion

Participants in this study believe that children learn most by participating in activities and by observing others. The results show that children are exposed to different types of activities and experiences depending on the beliefs, values, practices and resources of families. This was highlighted by children’s high participation in care, play and spiritual activities as well as lower participation in certain chores and educational activities like cutting and pasting. Participation in activities is also determined by access to resources (water and sand play and eating out), the lack of safety and security, which has possibly affected activities like increased visits to shopping malls and decreased visits to parks. Interventionists need to develop an understanding of family activities and integrate developmental goals within these.

This study has assisted in building on the ‘indigenous’ knowledge base of children and families in an African context, thus heeding the call to increase the knowledge base ‘about Africa for Africa’ (Pence et al. 2008). The use of activity settings is closely aligned to the strengths-based perspective of family-centred practice. The findings have increased the knowledge base regarding children within their natural environments, with these environments being rich in opportunities for learning; furthermore, the findings also contribute to (Pence & Marfo 2008):

… development of a science of child development that is not narrowly constructed on the lives of a small minority of the world’s children, but rather a science that opens up to other populations and other possibilities. (p. 85)

This study was conducted in a South African urban setting and cannot necessarily be generalised to other African contexts.

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank Prof. Shakila Dada from the University of Pretoria Centre for Alternate and Augmentative Communication for encouraging her to write this article and supporting her through the process.

This article is based on the first author’s unpublished thesis titled ‘Family-based activity settings of typically developing three-to-five-year old children in a low-income African context’ currently uploaded onto an academic repository available at https://repository.up.ac.za/handle/2263/28788.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article.

Authors’ contributions

S.B. was the main author of this article. K.U. and E.A. were the research supervisors of S.B.

Funding information

Funding for this study was provided by the National Research Foundation.

References

Balton, S., 2009, Family-based activity settings of typically developing three-to-five year old children in a low income African context, Electronic Thesis and Dissertations, University of Pretoria, UP Space. URN no: etd-10172009-122119.

Bartowski, J.P., Xu, X. & Levin, M.L., 2008, ‘Religion and child development: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study’, Social Science Research 37, 18–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.02.001

Beals, D.E., 1997, ‘Sources of support for learning words in conversation from mealtimes’, Journal of Child Language 24, 673–694. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000997003267

Beckert, T.E., Strom, P.S. & Strom, R.D., 2004, ‘Parent expectations of young children in Taiwan’, Early Childhood Research and Practice 6, 2.

Bernheimer, L. & Weisner, T., 2007, ‘“Let me just tell you what I do all day”. The family story at the center of research and practice’, Infants and Young Children 20(3), 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.IYC.0000277751.62819.9b

Berry, J.W., 2003, ‘Ecocultural perspective on human psychological development’, in T.S. Saraswathi (ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives in human developmental theory, research and applications, pp. 51–97, Sage, New Delhi.

Bornstein, M.H. & Cote, L.R., 2004, ‘Mothers’ parenting cognitions in cultures of origin, acculturating cultures and cultures of destination’, Child Development 75(1), 221–235. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00665.x

Bornstein, M.H. & Putnick, D.L., 2012, ‘Cognitive and socioemotional caregiving in developing countries’, Child Development 83(1), 46–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01673.x

Britto, P.B. & Ulkuer, N., 2012, ‘Child development in developing countries: Child rights and policy implications’, Child Development 83(1), 92–103. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01672.x

Burdette, H.L. & Whitaker, R.C., 2005, ‘A national study of neighborhood safety, outdoor play, television viewing, and obesity in preschool children’, Pediatrics 116, 657–662. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2004-2443

Carpenter, B., 2000, ‘Sustaining the family: Meeting the needs of families of children with disabilities’, British Journal of Special Education 27(3), 135–144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8527.00176

Carver, A., Timperio, A. & Crawford, D., 2008, ‘Playing it safe: The influence of neighborhood safety on children’s physical activity – A review’, Health and Place 14, 217–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.06.004

Cummings, P.M., 2016, ‘Child labour and household composition: Determinants of child labor in Mexico’, Asian Journal of Latin American Studies 29(3), 29–54.

Cutspec, P.A., 2006, ‘Oral storytelling within the context of the parent-child relationship’, Talaris Research Institute 1(2), 1–8.

De Grace, B.W., Foust, R.E., Sisson, S.B. & Lora, K.R., 2016, ‘Benefits of family meals for children with special therapeutic and behavioral needs’, The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 70, 1–6.

Dills, A., 2007, ‘Why dance literacy?’, Journal of the Canadian Association for Curriculum Studies 5(2), 95–113.

Dunst, C., 2001, ‘Participation of young children with disabilities in community learning activities’, In M. Guralnick (Ed.), Early childhood inclusion: Focus on change, Baltimore, Paul. H Brookes Publishing.

Dunst, C., 2007, ‘Early intervention for infants and toddlers with developmental disabilities’, in S.L. Odom, R.H. Horner, M. Snell & J. Blacher (eds.), Handbook of developmental disabilities, pp. 161–180, Guilford Press, New York.

Dunst, C. & Bruder, M., 1999, ‘Increasing children’s learning opportunities in the context of family and community life’, Children’s Learning Opportunities Report 1(1). Available at http://puckett.org/Childrens%20Learning%20Opps%20reports/lov1-1.pdf

Dunst, C.J., Meter, D. & Hamby, D.W., 2011, ‘Relationship between young children’s nursery rhyme experiences and knowledge of phonological and print-related activities’, Cell Reviews 4(1), 1–12.

Eisner, E., 1998, ‘Does experience in the arts boost academic achievement?’, NSEAD1 17, 51–60.

Evans, J., Matola, C. & Nyeko, J., 2008, ‘Parenting challenges for the changing African family’, in M. Garcia, A. Pence & J. Evans (eds.), Africa’s future, Africa’s challenge: Early childhood development in sub-Saharan Africa, pp. 1–9, The World Bank, Washington, DC.

Farver, J.A., 1999, ‘Activity setting analysis: A model for examining the role of culture in development’, in A. Göncu (ed.), Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural Perspectives, pp. 99–127, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Fulkerson, J.A., Pasch, K.E., Stigler, M.H., Farbaksh, K., Perry, C.L. & Komro, K.A., 2010, ‘Longitudinal associations between family dinner and adolescent perceptions of parent-child communication among racially diverse urban youth’, Journal of Family Psychology 24(3), 261–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019311

Furth, H.G., 1996, Desire for society, children’s knowledge as social imagination, Springer, New York.

Gaskins, S., 1999. Children’s daily lives in a Mayan village: A case study of culturally constructed roles and activities, In A. Göncü (Ed.), Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural Perspectives, Chicago: Cambridge University Press.

Geiger, M. & Alant, E., 2005, ‘Child-rearing practices and children’s communicative interactions in a village in Botswana’, Early Years 25(2), 183–191. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575140500128079

Getz, L.M., Chamarro-Premuzic, T., Roy, M. & Devroop, K., 2011, ‘The relationship between affect, uses of music, and music preferences in a sample of South African adolescents’, Psychology of Music 40(2), 1–15. http://doi.org.10.1177/0305735610381818

Goldenberg, C., Gallimore, R. & Reese, L., 2001, ‘Using mixed methods to explore Latino children’s literacy development’, in T. Weisner (ed.), Discovering successful pathways in children’s development and family life, pp. 21–46, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.

Göncü, A., Tuermer, U., Jain, J. & Johnson, D., 1999, ‘Children’s play as cultural activity’, in A. Göncü’ (ed.), Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives, pp. 155–178, Cambridge University Press, Chicago, IL.

Jarret, R.L., Bahar, O.S. & Kersh, R., 2014, ‘When we do sit down together’, Journal of Family Issues 37(11), 1483–1513.

Jordan, A.B., 2005, ‘Learning to use books and television: An exploratory study in the ecological perspective’, American Behavioural Scientist 48(5), 523–538. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764204271513

Kruger, J. & Chawla, L., 2005, ‘We know something someone doesn’t know… Children speak out on local conditions in Johannesburg’, Children, Youth and Environments 15(2), 89–104.

Larson, R.W., Branscomb, K.R. & Wiley, A.R., 2006, ‘Forms and functions of family mealtimes: Multidisciplinary perspectives’, New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 111, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.152

Ligthelm, A., 2005, ‘Informal retailing through home-based micro-enterprises: The role of Spaza shops, Development Southern Africa 22(2), 199–214. https://doi.org/10.1080/03768350500163030

Mathers, N., Fox, N. & Hunn, A., 2002, Using interviews in a research project, Institute of General Practice, Sheffield.

Mazibuko, Z., 2007, ‘Soweto’s battle of the malls’, City Press, 01 December.

McMillan, J.H. & Schumacher, S., 2001, Research in education, Addison Wesley Longman, Inc., USA.

Mercer, J.A., 2006, ‘Children as mystics, activists, sages, and holy fools: Understanding the spirituality of children and its significance for clinical work’, Pastoral Psychology 54(5), 497–515. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11089-005-0013-y

Milteer, R.M. & Ginberg, K.R., 2012, ‘The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bond: Focus on children in poverty’, American Academy of Pediatrics 129(1), e204–e213.

Moeno, S.N., 2006, ‘Family life in Soweto, Gauteng’, in Y. Ohebeneba-Sakyi & B.K. Takyi (eds.), African families at the turn of the 21st century, Praeger Publishers, Westport, CT.

Murray, S., 1997, ‘An academic milling around ‘the mall’: (De)constructing cultural knowledge’, Critical Art 11(1–2), 153–176. https://doi.org/10.1080/02560049785310121

Nielsen, M., Cucchiaro, J., Mohamedally, J., 2012, ‘When transmission of culture is child’s play’, PLoS One 7(3), e34066. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034066

Nsamenang, B., 1992, Human development in a cultural context: A third world perspective, Sage Publications, Newbury Park, CA.

Nsamenang, B., 2008a, ‘A critical peek at early childhood care and education in Africa’, Child Health and Education 1(1), 1–12.

Nsamenang, B., 2008b, ‘(Mis)understanding ECD in Africa: The force of local and global motives’, in M. Garcia, A. Pence & J. Evans (eds.), Africa’s future, Africa’s challenge: Early childhood care and development in Sub-Saharan Africa, pp. 135–149, The World Bank, Washington, DC.

Norton, D.G., 1990, ‘Understanding the early experience of black children in high risk environments: Culturally and ecologically relevant research as a guide to support for families’, Zero to Three 10(4), 1–7.

Ouduaran, A., 2013, ‘Living legacies: An ethnographic study of grandmother’s stories in Niger Delta’, Journal of Communication 4(1), 25–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/0976691X.2013.11884804

Patel, L., 2012, ‘Poverty, gender and social protection: Child support grants in Soweto, South Africa’, Journal of Policy Update 11(1–2), 106–120.

Pence, A., Evans, J.L. & Garcia, M., 2008, ‘Introduction’, in M. Garcia, A. Pence & P. Evans (eds.), Africa’s future, Africa’s challenge: Early childhood care and development in Sub-Saharan Africa, pp. 1–6, World Bank, Washington, DC.

Pence, A. & Marfo, K., 2008, ‘Early childhood development in Africa: Interrogating constraints of prevailing knowledge bases’, International Journal of Psychology 43(2), 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590701859143

Pence, A. & Schafer, J., 2006, ‘Indigenous knowledge and early childhood development in Africa: The early childhood development virtual university’, Journal for Education in International Development 2(3), 1–16.

Phadi, M. & Ceruti, C., 2011, ‘Multiple meanings of the middle class in Soweto, South Africa’, African Sociological Review 15(1), 89–107.

Rao, N., McHale, J.P. & Pearson, E., 2003, ‘Links between socialisation goals and child rearing practices in Chinese and Indian Mothers’, Infant and Child Development 12, 475–492. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.341

Roopnarine, J.L. & Davidson, K.L., 2015, ‘Parent-child play across cultures’, American Journal of Play 7(2), 228–252.

Ruiters, G., 2007, ‘Contradictions in municipal services in contemporary South Africa: Disciplinary commodification and self-disconnections’, Critical Social Policy 27, 487–508. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261018307081809

Sameroff, A. & Fiese, B., 2000, ‘Transactional regulation: The developmental ecology of early intervention’, in J.P. Shonkoff & S.J. Meisels (eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention, pp. 135–159, Cambridge University Press, United Kingdom.

Seekings, J., 2000, ‘Introduction: Urban studies in South Africa after Apartheid’, International Journal of Urban and Regional Research 24(4), 833–837. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-2427.00281

Serpell, R. & Marfo, K., 2014, ‘Some growth in African child development research’, in R. Serpell & K. Marfo (eds.), Child development in Africa: Views from inside, new directions for child and adolescent development, vol. 146, pp. 97–112, Wiley Periodicals, Inc, Hoboken, NJ.

Sheldon, M.L. & Rush, D.D., 2001, ‘The ten myths about providing early intervention services in natural environments’, Infants and Young Children 14(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200114010-00004

South African Revenue Services (SARS), 2008, SARS Tax Tables 2008/2009, viewed August 24 2008, from http://www.psiberworks.com

Super, C.M., Harkness, S., Barry, O. & Zeitlin, M., 2011, ‘Think locally, act globally: Contributions of African research to child development’, Child Development Perspectives 5(2), 119–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00166.x

Trivette, C., Dunst, C. & Hamby, D., 2004, ‘Sources of variation in consequences of everyday activity settings on child and parent functioning’, Perspectives in Education 22(2), 17–33.

Tudge, J., Piccinini, C.A., Sperb, T.M., Odero-Wanga, D., Lopes, R.S., Freitas, L., 2013, ‘Vulnerable children? The heterogeneity of young children’s experiences in Kenya & Brazil’, in D.J. Johnson, L. Agbenyiga & O. Hitchcock (eds.), Vulnerable children: Global challenges in education, health, well-being and child rights, pp. 121–138, Springer Science, New York.

Turnbull, A.P., Turbiville, V. & Turnbull, H.R., 2000, ‘Evolution of family-professional partnerships’, in S.J. Meisels & J.P. Shonkoff (eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention, pp. 630–650, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Van Rensburg, A.B., Myburgh, C.P.,Szabo, C.P., & Poggenpoel, M., 2013, ‘The role of spirituality in specialist psychiatry: A review of the medical literature’, African Journal of Psychiatry 16(4), 247–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/ajpsy.v16i4.33

Veitch, J., Bagley, S., Ball, K. & Salmon, J., 2006, ‘Where do children usually play? A qualitative study of parents’ perceptions of influences on children’s active free-play’, Health and Place 12, 383–393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2005.02.009

Weisner, T., 2002, ‘Ecocultural pathways, family values and parenting’, Parenting: Science & Practice 2(3), 325–334. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327922PAR0203_06

Weisner, T.S., Matheson, C., Coots, J. & Bernheimer, L.P., 2005, ‘Sustainability of daily routines as a family outcome’, in A. Maynard & M. Martini (eds.), The psychology of learning in cultural context: Family, peers and school, Kluwer/Plenum, New York.

Werner, E.E., 2000, ‘Protective factors and individual resilience’, in J.P. Shonkoff & S.J. Meisels (eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention, pp. 115–134, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Woodward, S.C., 2007, ‘Nation building – One child at a time: Early childhood music education in South Africa’, Arts Education Policy Review 109(2), 33–42. https://doi.org/10.3200/AEPR.109.2.33-42

Appendix 1: Interview schedule.