key: cord-1056275-gaxc3i0m authors: Tomasso, Linda Powers; Yin, Jie; Cedeño Laurent, Jose Guillermo; Chen, Jarvis T.; Catalano, Paul J.; Spengler, John D. title: The Relationship between Nature Deprivation and Individual Wellbeing across Urban Gradients under COVID-19 date: 2021-02-05 journal: Int J Environ Res Public Health DOI: 10.3390/ijerph18041511 sha: 6b9791f7cf6b6250f71df2c51c4a97541a30aac2 doc_id: 1056275 cord_uid: gaxc3i0m Lockdown aiming at slowing COVID-19 transmission has altered nature accessibility patterns, creating quasi-experimental conditions to assess if retracted nature contact and perceived nature deprivation influence physical and emotional wellbeing. We measure through on-line survey methods (n = 529) how pandemic mandates limiting personal movement and outdoor nature access within the United States affect self-assessed nature exposure, perceived nature deprivation, and subsequent flourishing as measured by the Harvard Flourishing Index. Results indicate that perceived nature deprivation strongly associates with local nature contact, time in nature, and access to municipal nature during the pandemic, after controlling for lockdown mandates, job status, household composition, and sociodemographic variables. Our hypothesis is that individuals with strong perceived nature deprivation under COVID-19 leads to diminished wellbeing proved true. Interaction models of flourishing showed positive modification of nature affinity with age and qualitative modification of nature deprivation with race. Our results demonstrate the potential of local nature contact to support individual wellbeing in a background context of emotional distress and social isolation, important in guiding public health policies beyond pandemics. Evidence of nature's beneficial impact on physical [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] , cognitive [6] [7] [8] [9] , and emotional health [10] [11] [12] [13] is well substantiated in scientific literature. Nearly all studies conclude that health outcomes improve with exposure to non-threatening outdoor nature; the few studies which examine indoor nature exposure mostly yield positive associations [14] [15] [16] [17] . Variability of nature exposure differentiates one's experience in nature as well as response to nature contact, such that health outcomes are influenced by appropriate "dose" [18, 19] , frequency of contact [20, 21] , quality of nature exposure [22, 23] , biodiversity level [24] [25] [26] [27] , aesthetic preference [28, 29] , and urban greenspace proximity [30, 31] . Even with emerging awareness of these distinctions, Kuo [32] infers that cumulative exposure to green in toto-parcel size, nature type, vicinity, etc.-is responsible for imparting nature's health benefits. Studies of nature-health relationships often approach exposure additively. Intervention studies [33] [34] [35] [36] frequently contrast participant response in non-natured built environ-Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18 , 1511 2 of 20 ments versus nature-dense urban parks. Observational studies [4, [37] [38] [39] [40] [41] analyze proximity and expanse of residential greenspace in increasing increments using spatial measures such as Normalized Difference Vegetation Index (NDVI), a satellite-based capture of vegetation based on photosynthetic reflectance, or locational data such as GPS. Epidemiological methods [42] [43] [44] [45] have identified a range of health improvements, e.g., lower morbidity and annual disease prevalence, applying these tools. These studies nonetheless lack data on individual nature use and specific participant characteristics, making it difficult to learn which exposure factors most strongly impact outcome effects. A reverse scenario of nature deprivation or withdrawal from nature has rarely been empirically explored owing to scenario improbability, the ethics of withdrawing salutogenic stimuli in experiments involving humans, and from a pragmatic study design point, leakage within non-exposed groups. The extraordinary situation of a global health crisis, compulsory shelter-at-home policies, and changes in the supply of and demand for outdoor nature areas has shaped conditions for a natural experiment in which to study how alterations in established patterns of nature exposure may affect individual wellbeing under a state of generalized societal adversity. This paper associates changes to nature contact under COVID-19 with individual wellbeing. COVID-19 s emergence in late 2019 occasioned urgent public health issuances of lockdown mandates and suspended daily activity patterns including nature-seeking throughout much of the world. In the United States, lockdown or "shelter-in-place" protocols mandated closures of school, travel, and non-essential businesses in mid-March 2020 [46] . The lengthy period of home confinement imposed in many U.S. cities and states, as well as concomitant restrictions on local nature access, has focused public health concerns on changes to individual wellbeing, a construct summating positive emotion, engagement, relationship, meaning, and achievement [47] . From a wellbeing perspective, many individuals go outdoors seeking physical activity, socialization, and emotional resilience derived from immersion in natural environments. Green exercise, i.e., physical activity performed outdoors, offers known remediation pathways for wellbeing improvements [48] [49] [50] [51] . Because immersion in nature has been shown to mitigate feelings of anxiety [52] and attention deficit [53] , and promote self-efficacy [54, 55] and meaningfulness [56] , habitual nature contact may provide an essential coping mechanism for many people during times of distress. Current lockdown policies may be causing nature-dependent individuals to experience feelings of nature deprivation in situations where habitual nature-seeking behaviors might otherwise have assuaged a heightened pathogenesis brought on by health, financial, or emotional concerns stemming from the COVID-19 pandemic [57] . While we do not presume that most individuals experienced total separation from nature under COVID-19, the overlay of restricted personal mobility and closure of public nature sites like state and national parks-what nature-seekers refer to as "the nature I desire" [58] -irrefutably contracted the supply of nature and individual access to it. Furthermore, lockdown restrictions in many states and urban cities have created immediate and captive audiences for nature where it is circumstantially found. In densely built environments, publicly managed greenspace governs opportunities for nature exposure, unlike less densely developed areas with private greenspace or open viewsheds. Restrictions on public greenspace-urban vegetation for recreational use [59] -and blue space-outdoor water environments [60] -therefore may directly relate to wellbeing, particularly within urban settings. Multiple studies indicate that exposure to nature amplifies beneficial physical, psychological, and emotional outcomes. Nature comprehends "the range of scale and degrees of human management, together with [ . . . ] sunsets and mountain views", as suggested by Frumkin et al. [1] . Nature connectivity is core to the environmental psychology literature regarding pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors [61] [62] [63] [64] . Measures which operationalize the concept of nature affinity or connectivity share a common construct [65, 66] rooted in positive affect, though their predictive power does not extend to the effects of nature withdrawal. Nature connectivity has been positively linked with psychological resilience and maintenance of positive mental health under challenge [67] and individual reliance on favorite places in nature for restoration [29] . However, as far as we know, only one study [68] formally analyzes nature connectivity as a potential modifier of nature contact to assess wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviors. Our study considers both nature connectivity and the lesser-explored feelings of nature deprivation under conditions where altered nature contact may be perceived to insufficiently support challenges to individual wellbeing. The selected outcome of flourishing amplifies the notion of human wellbeing to include physical and emotional health, virtue, and adequate material sustenance [69] . Because nature exposure has been linked to sentiments of positive psychological functioning [26, 64] , flourishing is a suitable outcome to measure the impact of nature deprivation under conditions of psychological challenge and adversity. Although prior studies [70, 71] have empirically associated wellbeing with nature forms, our study is also the first to apply the Harvard Flourishing Index as a formal psychometric construct to relate perceived nature deprivation to flourishing. Harvard's Human Flourishing Program provides an empirical framework for integrating quantitative social sciences methods across disciplines to better understand the health implications of human flourishing. Many studies [72] [73] [74] [75] have considered the association of green space in one's immediate residential vicinity to health outcomes. As urbanization displaces traditional venues for nature contact, nearby greenspace increasingly becomes a key exposure criterion for health outcomes. Some researchers theorize [76, 77] that individuals with lower mobility-the elderly and children-and those of lower socioeconomic status (SES) concentrate their outdoor activities closer to home. Mental illness and emotional disorders strongly relate to proximate green space [18, 78, 79] . Improved emotional wellbeing and mental health outcomes that derive from increased nature exposure have shown further to preferentially benefit residents with less neighborhood greenspace, as often typical of low SES areas [80] [81] [82] . Nature found in the immediate neighborhood vicinity thus takes on a larger predictive role to accommodate intentional outdoor nature-seeking in the absence of routinely accessible natural sites. Loss of nature contact has been widely reported [32, 83, 84] and attributed to urban lifestyles, with ensuing disengagement from and disaffection for nature repercussive for human and planetary health. Researchers have measured baseline indicators of time in nature to conclude that absence of nature contact is the population norm, with the consequence of "nature deprivation" [85] as permanent removal of nature contact being vulnerability to a range of negative health outcomes. Shared concern for a phenomenon described as the "rarity of direct experience in nature" [86] and characterized in the literature as nature deficiency [87] or nature impoverishment [23] has led to research on prescriptive nature re-engagement [88, 89] and nature-based health treatment [90] , especially among children. Still, conceptualization of nature deficit disorder [91] remains a descriptive and not diagnostic condition, with formal study elusive given research bounds, though a few exceptions exist [92] . The term nature deprivation here denotes perceived nature deficiency, i.e., unmet personal need to access the nature one desires, rather than comparative resource inadequacy described in relative deprivation theory, where individuals or groups cognitively appraise their situation as unfairly disadvantaged [93, 94] . We draw attention to our use of this term in the emotional sense of withdrawal from habituated forms of nature exposure rather than as the relative area-level absence of green or blue space. To our knowledge, ours is the first study to examine the effects on wellbeing from diminished or withdrawn nature exposure, particularly where society-wide restrictions on personal mobility and the accessibility of some types of nature-rich areas account for nature separation. In this study, we hypothesize that changes to nature exposure at the indoor, neighborhood, and municipal levels will induce feelings of individual nature deprivation during the period of COVID-19 restrictions, adjusting for age, gender, race, urbanicity, area-level poverty, and US geographic region, as these variables may confound the nature exposurenature deprivation relationship [95] . Second, we posit that individuals who feel deprived of nature will experience a loss of baseline flourishing after accounting for job status and household composition under the pandemic. While our exposure of interest consists of nature contact potentially modified by pandemic restrictions, we hypothesize that subjective nature affinity may underlie pre-established patterns of nature pursuit that will continue under sheltering. We additionally consider secondary objectives specific to restrictive policies existing at the time of survey-taking, given information available from our results. First, will municipal restrictions on publicly managed nature areas influence feelings of nature deprivation under lockdown? Second, will pre-existing patterns of nature exposure impact subsequent wellbeing when habitual nature contact is altered? Third, do sociodemographic variables modify relationships with self-defined nature affinity and self-expressed nature deprivation that predict individual flourishing during COVID-19-like conditions? Our study population consisted of individuals originally recruited for focus group interviews to explore formative experiences and origins of attitudes shaping nature-seeking behaviors as adults. Study participants were recruited and directed to an on-line enrollment portal through Facebook advertisements placed October 2019 in four regionally distinct metropolitan areas representative of differing climate, topography, and urban building density-Boston, Atlanta, San Francisco, and Phoenix. Additional focus group recruitment occurred with the assistance of university faculty in the targeted metropolitan areas in the attempt to diversify the age, race, and gender of the study base. The original study base (n = 625) had already voluntarily enrolled and consented through electronic recruitment. We had no pre-existing data on study participants. A new recruitment email for this study addressing exposure to nature under COVID-19 restrictions was sent to all enrolled study participants via email addresses on file explaining survey study objectives and linking to a Qualtrics-distributed on-line survey instrument. This survey is found in Supplementary Materials S1. Study participants were sent a reminder email at seven and 14 days to request survey completion. No contact was made with study participants after two attempts. A survey link specific to the enrollment ID of each originally enrolled participant allowed us to monitor response rates of the initial cohort at 37.6%; a second, non-specific survey link created for survey forwarding allowed us to track the snowball effect of the study design that provided 62.4% of our study population. Survey forwarding extended the initial four metropolitan areas to 36 U.S. states, Puerto Rico, and the District of Colombia. Six-hundred participants returned the survey during the month the link was active. Survey-takers who omitted items were dropped from the study, resulting in a final sample size of 529 participants. This final study population resided in areas of population density (large urban areas and suburbs each represented 36% of respondents), were majority female (75%), white non-Hispanic (82%), and of slightly younger age (age 25-34 = 29%) although no age category was below 10.6%. Individuals who identified as black or Hispanic lived within zip codes of on-average lower NDVI levels and higher poverty rates. Nature affinity scores rose with age, consistent with findings of higher nature connectedness at progressive age [96] [97] [98] . The Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health Institutional Review Board approved both the original focus group and survey studies under IRB 19-1419 on 21 April 2020. Our main outcomes of interest were individual feelings of nature deprivation, operationalized across five levels of agreement with the statement, "I feel nature deprived since coronavirus restrictions were imposed", and subsequent flourishing self-assessed through survey items comprising the Harvard Flourishing Index. The exploratory nature of this measure meant we approached the development of nature deprivation in terms of content validity, operationalizing the concept under the domains of behavior (time in nature), affect (sense of withdrawal, isolation), cognition (restlessness, loss of temporal awareness), somatic symptoms (vitality, lethargy), and motivation (goal-abandonment). The Harvard Flourishing Index (HFI), a validated measurement approach to human flourishing [99] , highlights five central domains nearly universally apprised as vital elements of human wellbeing: happiness and life satisfaction, mental and physical health, meaning and purpose, character and virtue, and close social relationships; a sample item from Domain 4 is "I always act to promote good in all circumstances, even in difficult and challenging situations." Supplementary Materials S2, contains the complete HFI. This Index was selected over alternative subjective wellbeing indices for its optional sixth domain, financial and material stability, deemed highly relevant to COVID-19 s implications of wage or job loss on individual wellbeing within the United States. Each of the flourishing questions was assessed on a 10-point scale, with flourishing outcomes calculated on a continuous 0-90 scale. We substituted resilience measures for the Flourishing Index's two happiness items due to potential temporal confounding as pandemic malaise. Happiness has previously been positively associated with nature exposure [70, 96, 100] , though the COVID-19 Response Tracking Study conducted 21-29 May 2020 (n = 2279 adults nationwide) and the General Social Survey reported the highest percentage of individuals since record-keeping began in 1972 (23%) responding at the lowest level of happiness [101] . A third scale item, "My relationships are as satisfying as I would want them to be", similarly was measured in terms of resiliency as "I feel close to others in my community." Cronbach's alpha test of internal consistency was performed on the nine remaining items comprising the Flourishing Index, resulting in an alpha of 0.82, a high indicator of composite scale reliability. We tested for missingness and dropped survey responses containing omitted values. We did not adjust for length of lockdown due to response homogeneity: 85.97% of respondents were under restriction for 4 weeks, 6.85% for 3-4 weeks, and 5.87% considered essential workers temporally unaffected by restrictions. A second covariate, mode of transportation, was dropped from analysis due to similar response homogeneity, with under 1% using public transit, a marker initially presumed to associate with accessing nature under COVID-19. Our exposure of interest was cumulative nature contact experienced by individuals during lockdown restrictions at the time of survey-taking. We analyzed three levels of nature exposure-indoor, neighborhood, and municipal-under conditions of pandemic restrictions. We distinguished in situ, i.e., incidental, nature contact from intentional nature desired for outdoor activities [102] . Survey-takers cited intentional nature-based activities, e.g., hiking, kayaking, and community gardening, as most commonly missed due to pandemic restricted non-accessibility. The term "outdoors" was explicitly defined for survey-takers as "time intentionally spent in or near nature: backyard, outdoor gardening, urban park, other urban/suburban greenspace, greenway for walking or biking, open woodlands, state or national park/forest/ seashore, all forms of outdoor sport or recreation". Indoor nature exposure was represented by four measures: nature seen through window views, stage of springtime nature reemergence, having indoor plants, and having a pet. Neighborhood-level exposure consisted of summative nature contact dictated by local shelter-in-place policies (four categories: complete lockdown; can go outside but do not come in contact with nature; can go outside but preferred nature is inaccessible; and no pandemic restrictions: normal outdoor access) and by a second item comparing amount of time spent outdoors in local nature under COVID-19 vis-à-vis pre-pandemic conditions (three categories: less, same, and more). Municipal-level nature exposure concerned access to municipally-managed nature areas such as parks, conservation lands, and beaches (four categories: full access with social distancing required, reduced parking to limit park occupancy, non-vehicular foot or bicycle egress only into parks, and complete closure of nature areas to the public). Frequency of time in nature prior to COVID-19 restrictions (four categories: never or