key: cord-1055691-z9l8pgbj authors: Korman, Maria; Tkachev, Vadim; Reis, Cátia; Komada, Yoko; Kitamura, Shingo; Gubin, Denis; Kumar, Vinod; Roenneberg, Till title: COVID-19-mandated social restrictions unveil the impact of social time pressure on sleep and body clock date: 2020-12-17 journal: Sci Rep DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-79299-7 sha: 61612c3999a91fa19afad25cea6e96eca08536c3 doc_id: 1055691 cord_uid: z9l8pgbj In humans, sleep regulation is tightly linked to social times that assign local time to events, such as school, work, or meals. The impact of these social times, collectively—social time pressure, on sleep has been studied epidemiologically via quantification of the discrepancy between sleep times on workdays and those on work-free days. This discrepancy is known as the social jetlag (SJL). COVID-19-mandated social restrictions (SR) constituted a global intervention by affecting social times worldwide. We launched a Global Chrono Corona Survey (GCCS) that queried sleep–wake times before and during SR (preSR and inSR). 11,431 adults from 40 countries responded between April 4 and May 6, 2020. The final sample consisted of 7517 respondents (68.2% females), who had been 32.7 ± 9.1 (mean ± sd) days under SR. SR led to robust changes: mid-sleep time on workdays and free days was delayed by 50 and 22 min, respectively; sleep duration increased on workdays by 26 min but shortened by 9 min on free days; SJL decreased by ~ 30 min. On workdays inSR, sleep–wake times in most people approached those of their preSR free days. Changes in sleep duration and SJL correlated with inSR-use of alarm clocks and were larger in young adults. The data indicate a massive sleep deficit under pre-pandemic social time pressure, provide insights to the actual sleep need of different age-groups and suggest that tolerable SJL is about 20 min. Relaxed social time pressure promotes more sleep, smaller SJL and reduced use of alarm clocks. Sleep duration (SD week ), midsleep timing (MSF sc ) and social jetlag (SJL). Social restrictions lead to robust shifts in daily behavior and in exposure to daylight. The average outdoor daylight exposure showed a twofold decrease inSR: from 2 h 21 min ± 102 min to 1 h 8 min ± 75 min (Z = − 63.47, p < 0.001, r = 0.73; Wilcoxon Signed Ranks test). During SR, the individual SD week increased on average by 15 min (∆SD week , Z = − 21.297, p < 0.001, r = 0.25; Fig. 1 , panels a1-a3). This shift reflects both a robust increase in the sleep duration on workdays (∆SD W , + 26 min, + 5.84%; Z = − 28.159, p < 0.001, r = 0.36) and a small but consistent decrease in the sleep duration on work-free days (∆SD F , − 9 min, − 1.93%; Z = − 11.430, p < 0.001, r = 0.13). More participants reported no changes (42.8%) in sleep quality than those who had negative (i.e., worse sleep quality, 34 Fig. 1 , panels c1-c3), indicating a robust alignment of sleep times between work-and work-free days during the inSR period. Non-parametric LOESS regressions show the smoothed relationships between the parameters of daily behavior preSR and inSR (Fig. 1 , panels a2-c2). People who sleep shorter than ~ 8.5 h preSR tend to lengthen their sleep under the relaxed social clock (inSR), while those who slept even longer than ~ 8.5 h preSR tend to sleep shorter, and only those who slept around 8.5 h on average did not change (Fig. 1 , intersection indicated by red dot, panel a2). SJL shows a crossing point value around ~ 20 min (Fig. 3 , red dot, panel c2); those with SJL > 20 min tend to reduce it, while those who had SJL < 20 min preSR (only 18% of the sample) tend to have small or no change in SJL inSR. The fact that people suffering from ~ 20 min of SJL do not change their habits to reduce SJL suggests this amount of circadian misalignment can be tolerated. Note that some participants (17% of the sample) increased SJL (Fig. 1 , panel c2 grey dots), an evidence for an increase in social time pressure inSR for these individuals. The LOESS regression for MSF sc has a slope (k) close to 1, showing an almost constant delay across all chronotypes. Its variance reflects the known wide range of chronotypes in industrialized populations, but the distribution is delayed (e.g., shifted above the diagonal) as a whole between preSR and inSR (similar to age changes 31 ) (Fig. 1 , panel b2; 95% of the sample is to the left of the intersection point). The deviation of the SD week and SJL slopes of the LOESS curves from the diagonal suggested that the response to relaxing social constraints depended on the initial, pre-pandemic, sleep duration and SJL: the more underslept and SJL-misaligned people were preSR, the more they increased sleep duration or decreased SJL inSR. We quantified the preSR-inSR differences (∆) using Spearman's rank correlations against the preSR-value. Indeed, the (a1-c1) Distributions of SD week , MSF sc , and SJL preSR (black line) and inSR (red line), percent from group total. (a2-c2) Scatterplots of sleep duration, SD week (hours), corrected mid-sleep time on free days, MSF sc (chronotype, local time), and social jetlag, SJL (hours), preSR (x axis) vs. inSR (y axis). Each dot represents an individual participant, overlapping dots are coded by color intensity. Diagonal line designates no restriction-induced change in parameter. Green-increase/advance, grey-decrease/delay. Black line-LOESS regression lines illustrate the relationship between the parameter values preSR (x-axis) and the smoothed parameter values inSR (y-axis), pointwise 95% confidence intervals are visualized by bands shaded in yellow. Red dot-intersection point between the diagonal and the LOESS line. Yellow cross coordinates-means of MSF sc preSR and inSR; k-slope of the tangent for the LOESS regression line at the point of mean MSF sc preSR. (a3-c3) Boxplots of individual differences (∆, hours) in SD week , MSF sc and SJL. Positive values-increase in SD week , delay in MSF sc . Negative values-decrease in SJL. Whiskers-max and min values, box borders-75th and 25th percentiles, line through the box-median, ×marker-mean. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ correlation of SD week preSR and ∆SD week was negative (ρ = − 0.302, p < 0.001). Since respondents were, on average, 3-5 weeks into the SR period, we assume that the gains in SD week presumably reflect individual sleep-need rather than a compensating of the previous sleep debt. Similarly, there was a negative correlation between the SJL level before the restriction and ∆SJL (Spearman's ρ = − 0.593, p < 0.001), i.e., ∆SJL was more robust in individuals who initially suffered from stronger circadian misalignment. As to be expected from the results described above, ∆MSF sc was independent of the preSR-MSF sc times (Spearman's ρ = − 0.006, p = 0.632). Changes in sleep duration and timing were driven by delays in sleep onset (time of falling asleep) and sleep offset (awakening) times, during both workdays and free days inSR, see Supplementary Information (SI- Fig. S2 ). Note that the delay in sleep offset times on workdays (64 min) was on average twice as large as the delay in onset times (37 min). Thus, longer sleep duration inSR is subserved primarily by postponing the time of awakening on workdays. Age related changes. There were no differences between the sexes in ∆SD week and minor but significant differences in ∆MSF sc (7.8 min) and ∆SJL (6.7 min), with negligible effect sizes (r g < 0.1), see Supplementary Information (SI- Fig. S3 ). All shifts in daily behavior (all deltas) significantly correlated with age overall (Table 1 ; note negligible ρ for ∆SD F ). To further explore these age-effects, separate post-hoc analyses were performed using six arbitrary age groups distributed, so that each had approximately the same sample size: 18- Table S2 ). The overall magnitude of the shifts was estimated via the mean values of deltas (given in minutes together with the Z-scores of the Wilcoxon Matched-Pairs two-tailed tests above each distribution panel). The magnitude of the respective changes decreased consistently with age and did not even reach significance for ∆SD week in the 65 + y age group. For MSF sc , a chronotype-independent shift (i.e., slope close to 1), observed for the total study population (Fig. 1b2) , exists only in the middle-age and older age groups. In young participants, 18-22y and 25-29y, the early-type participants (i.e., MSF sc < 2:00) delayed relatively more than late types (i.e., MSF sc > 5:00) (Fig. 2b, k values) . Except for the senior group (65 + y), the crossing points for SJL in the individual groups were similar to each other (Fig. 1c2) . The age dependency of the magnitude in shifts of SD week , MSF sc and of the SJL can be visualized in several aspects: (i) the mean values at the top of each panel demonstrate the progressive decrease in ∆SD week and ∆SJL values with older age (ii) the proportion of the group without change-height of white central bars in the ∆-distributions, is increasing with age; (iii) the kurtosis of the ∆SD week , ∆MSF sc , and ∆SJL distributions in decreasing with age. Changes in sleep onset and sleep offset times inSR were larger in young adults and decreased with age (SM- Fig. S4 , separate panels for workdays and free days). Use of alarm clock and working from home. In modern societies, compliance with social times is ubiquitously achieved by using an alarm clock. Among working/studying participants (N = 6012, 80% of the total sample), preSR, 84% usually used alarm clocks during workdays and 12% worked from home, while inSR only 56% of working participants woke up with alarm clock on workdays and 82% worked from home. Before the pandemic, alarm clock users (N = 5060) were-on average-later chronotypes, had shorter sleep on workdays and longer sleep on work-free days than participants who did not use alarm clocks (N = 952) and consequently suffered from more SJL (Mann-Whitney tests: MSF sc : + 35 min, Z = − 15.44, p < 0.001, r g = − 0.31; SJL: + 46 min, Z = − 26.33, p < 0.001, r g = − 0.54; SD W : − 19 min, Z = − 8.48, p < 0.001, r g = 0.17; SD F : + 28 min; Z = − 11.18, p < 0.001, r g = − 0.22). Alarm clock use preSR was more prevalent among adults < 50 years, compared to older adults (18-22y-89%, 23-29y-86%, 30-39y-80%, 40-49y-81%, 50-64y-66%, 65 + y-23%). The low prevalence of alarm clock use in 65 + y group presumably related to retirement since only 48% were employed preSR. To evaluate this assumption, we calculated the alarm-use/employment ratio (see SI- Table S2 , Ratio (Alarm clock usage/Employed)). This ratio declines with age preSR, suggesting that employment status is not a sole factor in alarm use on workdays. To assess the impact of the relaxed social time pressure (STP) and alarm-clock use on changes in daily behavior, we selected a group of participants who worked/studied both preSR and inSR, used an alarm clock on workdays preSR, and worked/studied from home inSR. This group (N = 4135) was then subdivided into those who stopped using an alarm clock inSR (Alarm/NoAlarm; N = 1539 [37%]) and those who continued to use alarm clock inSR (Alarm/Alarm; N = 2596 [63%]). The two groups were similar in age and sex composition (SI -Table S3 ). While both groups showed robust changes in sleep duration, mid-sleep time, and SJL ( Fig. 3) , magnitudes of changes were significantly larger in the Alarm/NoAlarm group than in the Alarm/Alarm group (Mann-Whitney tests; ∆SD week : Z = − 5.957, p < 0.001, r g = 0.11; ∆MSF sc : Z = − 7.101, p < 0.001, r g = 0.13; ∆SJL: Z = − 15.227, p < 0.001, r g = 0.28; complementary LOESS regressions and box plots of individual differences in SI- Fig. S4 ). www.nature.com/scientificreports/ The groups did not differ in sleep onset and sleep offset timings on workdays preSR (Table 2) . InSR, both groups fell asleep and woke up later, but these changes were significantly larger in the Alarm/NoAlarm group. Thus, the differences in SD week , MSF sc and SJL between the groups are primarily related to alarm-independent awakening on workdays in the Alarm/NoAlarm group. Mann-Whitney tests were also used to compare the people who worked from home (4977) to those that worked not from home and presumably had stricter schedules (1126), inSR. All deltas (∆SD week , ∆MSF sc and ∆SJL) were larger in the "worked from home" subgroup (SI -Table S4 ). Social restriction-induced changes (preSR → inSR) in sleep-wake behavior by age-group (18-22y, 23-29y, 30-39y, 40-49y, 50-64y, and 65 + y). Upper panels-distributions of (a) delta of sleep duration, ∆SD week , (b) delta of corrected mid-sleep time on free days, ∆MSF sc (chronotype), (c) delta of social jetlag, ∆SJL. The magnitude of shifts is represented in 0.5 h color-coded bins, with white bars representing no change, green bar-increase in SD week , advance in MSF sc and decrease in SJL, grey bars-decrease in SD week , delay in MSF sc and increase in SJL. Mean ∆ in minutes and Z statistic of the post-hoc Wilcoxon Signed Ranks two-tailed tests for each parameter are shown above the bars. *p < 0.001, r effect size, ns non-significant. Lower panels-Scatterplots of individual shifts in (a) SD week (h), (b) MSF sc (chronotype, local time), and (c) SJL (h), preSR (x axis) vs. inSR (y axis). Notations-as in Fig. 1a2 The SR issued as a response to the Covid-19 pandemic constituted a huge circadian experiment. Weaker STP due to home office, no commute, and possibly less stringent workhours most frequently accompanies the implemented SR. Before SR, many people suffered from a mismatch between circadian times (e.g., when we best sleep) and social times (e.g., when we go to work, school). This misalignment (social jetlag, SJL) is associated with health and performance deficits and becomes apparent in the usage of alarm clocks that shorten our biological sleep program due to social time pressure (STP). More than 80% of the working population uses alarm clocks on workdays 32 , which was replicated here for the preSR 'normality' (84% use an alarm clock). The two conditions (preSR, inSR) allowed computing within-subject changes in variables, such as mid-sleep times, sleep duration or SJL. The most important outcome of the GCCS study for circadian/sleep and public health research is the obtained robust gain in sleep duration inSR compared to preSR, accompanied by a substantial reduction of SJL. These benefits were achieved without major negative changes in sleep quality, in line with the parallel findings by Leone et al. 33 . Our findings are also in line with the results of several smaller scale studies of the impact of COVID-19 mandated SR during spring 2020 on sleep and circadian behavior 34, 35 . The GCCS sample is large-scale and represents adults from all age groups. In conjunction with a within-individual analysis these sample characteristics provided a chance to quantify in great detail sleep-related variables (duration, timing and SJL) and determine actual sleep-need more accurately via modelling. The preSR-inSR slopes of regression line in sleep duration and SJL deviate substantially from 1 (Fig. 1 , row 2 and second row of panels in Fig. 2a,c) . One interpretation of this "rubber-band" effect is that the further apart the social and the biological time-zone (the greater the circadian strain) the further individuals "travel" (relax) when STP weakens. The difference between these allegorical time-zones strongly correlates with SJL, and the larger its extent the greater the change when STP weakens. Greater SJL is associated with shorter average sleep duration 11 , consequently, the shorter people sleep preSR, the more they extend their sleep inSR. Traditionally, differences in sleep duration between workdays and free days were used to describe longer sleep duration on free days as compensatory for the sleep debt accumulated over the workweek, especially in adolescents and young adults 36 . However, our current results show that sleep timing and duration on free days primarily reflect the actual individual sleep need and not a compensation for sleep debt accumulated on workdays: after full adaptation to the relaxed STP, sleep duration on workdays is lengthened by about 6%, while on free days sleep duration is shortened by only 2%. The large number of participants in our GCCS database allowed us to look at different age groups, and results show age-dependencies in all variables (Table 1) . Adolescents are the latest chronotypes among in any population 31, 37, 38 (SI-Table S2 , mean MSFsc by age group) and therefore are exposed to the most rigorous STP leading to the largest sleep deprivation and SJL 17 . As to be expected, this age group shows the strongest responses to weaker STP inSR 39 . The 65 + y age group is exposed to the weakest STP under preSR conditions (only 48% are employed compared to 89-99% in other age groups, see SI- Table S2 ) and consequently shows only small changes in response to social restrictions. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ While the 18-22y age group increases average sleep duration across the week (SD week ) by on average 36 min, the 65 + y age group gains 3 min; while the youngest age group in our population reduces SJL by almost one hour, the 65 + y participants get a relief of 7 min. Not mutually exclusive is the possibility that reduced sensitivity to environmental zeitgebers (primarily, light) could contribute to reduced circadian response to social restrictions induced changes in later life 40 . Our data suggests that age-dependent disparities in sleep duration gains are also likely to be mediated by differences in both the employment-related STP and alarm-use in different age groups (SI -Table S2 ). Regression lines for both sleep duration and SJL produce intersects with the 1:1 diagonal (Fig. 1a2,c2 , red dots): the intersect for sleep duration in the entire cohort is ~ 8.5 h and that for SJL 20 min. We propose that these intersects might be predictors for sleep need and for the tolerance for SJL, respectively. When segregated into age groups, the intersects indicate a higher sleep need for younger people that steadily decreases with age (Fig. 2a) . This is in line with previously published data 17 and roughly corresponds to the average free-day sleep duration in the respective age groups. The intersect for the SJL scatter plots is close to the 20-min tolerance in all age groups except for the elderly. If one considers this limit of tolerance to distinguish between affected and non-affected people, then the prevalence of SJL on population level would be 88% 11 . In the GCCS cohort, the preSR prevalence of SJL > 20 min is 82%, which is reduced inSR to 65%. Unlike for the preSR-inSR associations of both SJL and sleep duration, the slope of regression line for midpoint of sleep on free days (MSF sc -chronotype) is close to 1 (at least within the 95-percentile range; Fig. 1b2) . The fact that chronotype shows so much less "rubber band" effect underlines the stability of this phenotype. Nevertheless, on average, all chronotypes delay by about half an hour from preSR to inSR. This delay is more or less independent of preSR chronotype, which indicates that participants being exposed to less outside daylight inSR cannot simply explain the changes that we found. Responses to this zeitgeber change should be chronotypespecific 17, 41, 42 . This delay also cannot simply be explained by less morning-light exposure due to getting up later: preSR people (using alarm clocks on workdays) are exposed at different internal times to morning light-early chronotypes are exposed at a later internal phase than late chronotypes. Thus, late types will advance more than early types under strong STP and should respond stronger, i.e., chronotype-specific, when STP weakens inSR. When segregating the data into age-groups, the results do indicate a chronotype-specific response: the preSR-inSR slope in the younger participants, who are also the latest chronotypes within a population, deviates from 1 (see k-values in Fig. 2b) . However, in this case, early types delay more than the late types. Since everybody delays and less STP is exerted in the mornings, it is likely that the early types comply with the increasing social pressure in the evenings (presumably via socializing in digital environments 43 or with the family) and stay up later than they would under preSR conditions. This hypothesis would predict that early types get less sleep inSR than preSR, which we didn't find. Another explanation would be that early chronotypes differ in sleep pressure buildup 44 and therefore go to bed later inSR since they get more sleep. SJL is predominantly caused by using an alarm clock to wake up on workdays 11, 45 , and our findings indicate that alarm clocks are circadian disruptors rather than synchronizers (supporting entrainment): alarm-clock use reduces the benefits for sleep duration and SJL associated with the lower STP inSR. In general, people approach their preSR free-day behavior even on inSR workdays, indicating that their free-day sleep without an alarm clock is a good surrogate for their actual circadian sleep window. This finding has important consequences. For example, decreasing sleep timing variability should be accomplished by changing work-day sleep timing rather than using alarm clocks also on free days; in other words, sleep on workdays and free days should be aligned by time-in-bed (onset) and not wake-up time (forced offset) as often suggested in the Internet (e.g., (https ://www. youtu be.com/watch ?v=VzM45 9aKAJ Q.)), but also in scientific publications 46 . The longer sleep on weekends or other work-free days fosters health and longevity 47 . Chronotyping, assessment of sleep and circadian misalignment under conditions of quarantine provided insights to the actual sleep need of different age-groups and to the tolerable SJL. Our results indicate that long-term relaxation in STP should benefit health via increase in sleep duration, decrease in SJL and decrease in alarmclock use. We conclude that alarm clocks are circadian disruptors, not synchronizers, and that the ubiquitous use of alarm clocks should be discouraged. Several large companies have already indicated to partly continue with work-from-home due to the positive experience under the recent social restrictions, and our data suggests that these reforms should benefit health. Altogether, the data suggest that current structuring of social schedules and changing our attitudes towards alarm clock use deserve serious consideration. To the best of our knowledge, the GCCS is the largest published epidemiological study on changes in temporal behavior, including sleep, in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, providing important insights. Current results may help in understanding of the consequences of social restrictions in the general population. The GCCS. The internet-based Global Chrono Corona Survey (GCCS) study was approved by the Ariel University Human Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences (AU-HEA-MK-20200629). All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. All respondents were informed of the study purposes and of the names of the principal and coordinating investigators and provided electronic consent prior to commencement. Participation in the survey was anonymous; no personal identification data was requested. We collected the data via SoGoSurvey platform (Herndon, Virginia, US), a cloud-based platform that enables creation and distribution of multilingual surveys. To translate the GCCS and advertise it world-wide, we formed an international network of colleagues and universities (see Acknowledgements). Recruitment methodologies included digital advertisements at universities, academic and non-academic social networks, and email-based approaches. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ The GCCS contained 40-54 items, implemented in a smart-logic design where the presentation of the downstream questions depended on the answer to the key question (e.g., participants who declared unemployment, did not receive questions about habits on workdays). Participants could choose a preferred language (English, German, Hebrew, Arabic, Hindi, Japanese, Italian, Portuguese, Russian, and Spanish). Respondents were asked to provide basic demographic information (country, postal code, age, sex, height, weight, having/not having COVID-19 or history of and then to answer questions about daily behavior and lifestyle in separate sections, each pertaining to before social restrictions (preSocialRestriction, preSR) and during the restrictions (inSocialRestriction, inSR). Each section included questions about current work/study status, whether the work is a work-from-home or/and shift/night work. The core GCCS questions were about the sleep onset and wakeup times on workdays and on work-free days, typical meal times (not analyzed for this report) and exposure to outdoor daylight. Participants were asked about habitual use of alarm clock on workdays, and the time they spent outdoors in daylight on work-and work-free days. To avoid misunderstandings, participants were explicitly and repeatedly reminded to use the 24-h opposed to an AM-PM time-format. In the inSR section, additional questions queried for changes in subjective sleep quality, napping habits, quality of life, physical activity, time in front of screens and productivity, compared to the preSR period, using Likert scales (very negative (− 2), negative (− 1), no change (0), positive (+ 1), or very positive (+ 2) changes) for each wellbeing parameter. Study participants. 11 ,431 respondents (≥ 18 years) from 40 countries completed the GCCS between April 4 and May 16, 2020; top response rates (> 200 respondents) came from Portugal, Italy, US, UK, Germany, Israel, India, Russia, Japan and Brazil. Exclusions for this analysis were responders diagnosed with COVID-19 (1.1%), shift/night workers (preSR or inSR, 16.3%), responders with extreme sleep durations (< 3 h and > 14 h; 3.1%), and those with missing or invalid data (8.3%). Occasional input errors due to confusion between time formats were systematically traced and manually checked. Obvious time format confusion cases were manually corrected, ambiguous cases were classified as invalid. The same researcher (M.K.) corrected/disqualified the cases in question, applying consistent criterions for decision-making. To correct for the overrepresentation of young (18-22y.o) participants from Russia relative to two other leading countries in this age group (India and Japan), we excluded 656 participants from Russia (5.7%) using random procedures. The final sample included 7517 respondents (68.2% females). Length of stay inSR was calculated for each participant based on the response date and the onset date of the country-specific official social restrictions. Participants were on average 32.7 ± 9.1 days (range 10-59 days) under social restrictions or stay-at-home orders. We assumed this time duration allowed full adaptation to the new conditions and a steady-state for circadian entrainment phase. 80% of respondents worked or studied both preSR and inSR (66% from home). Full sociodemographic details are presented in Supplementary Material (SI -Table S1 ). Outcome measures. The following daily behaviors were calculated for each individual: (i) mean sleep duration across the week (SD week ) was calculated as weighted average of the sleep duration on workdays (SD W , assuming 5 workdays) and work-free days (SD F ), [SD week = (5 × SD W + 2 × SD F )/7]. (ii) Chronotypes (MSF sc ), quantifying individual sleep timing as an indicator for phase of entrainment 11 . MSF sc is based on the mid-point of sleep on free days (MSF) and corrected for sleep deficit accumulated on workdays. The mid-point of sleep was also calculated for workdays (MSW). (iii) As an assessment for circadian misalignment, social jet-lag (SJL) was calculated as the difference between MSF and MSW. To quantify the magnitude of changes in daily behavior we calculated individual restriction-induced Deltas (∆) as [inSR-preSR] for each parameter. The means and standard deviations of the main outcome parameters by country are reported in the SI- Table S5 . Statistical analysis. We used non-parametric data analyses since none of the daily behavior variables showed normal distribution and/or was homoscedastic. Wilcoxon Matched-Pairs tests were used to assess the within-subject changes in daily behavior, effect sizes were calculated according to r = Z/√N 48 . Spearman's rank correlation analysis was performed to assess associations between daily behavior measures, and light data. Mann-Whitney U-tests, with Glass rank-biserial correlation as the measure of effect size (r g = 2(M1 − M2)/ N1 + N2, where M1, M2-mean ranks, N1, N2-group sizes), were used to compare between the ad-hoc groups (e.g., age groups). LOESS regressions (locally weighted scatterplot smoothed curves) 49 were applied on each parameter to smoothen it and to predict the Y (inSR) locally as a function of X (preSR) using the stats package (version 4.00) and plotting function ggplot2 (version 3.3.2) in R. The size of the neighborhood was controlled using the span argument of 1. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 26 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and R; the level of significance was set at p < 0.05. Where appropriate, the p-value thresholds were adjusted for multiple comparisons. We did not obtain many relevant variables that may impact daily behaviors and also affect the response to social restrictions, such as general health, marital status, small children, socio-economic status, living in apartment vs. private house and many other. Other relevant variables, such as geographical factors (latitude and position in the time zone), dietary habits, exposure to daylight and wellbeing parameters were obtained but were not included in this report since it would have gone beyond the already complex scope of this paper. We note that the assessment of preSR parameters is based on retrospective reports and that only 32% percent of the sample were males. A possible validation to the retrospective sampling may come from comparison between current findings and a similar study by Write et al. 34 . In this study 139 university students from the University of Colorado Boulder (US), enrolled in the study at two time-points: at baseline, preSR, data collection was conducted as part of a class project between January 29 to February 4, 2020, while the inSR data was collected remotely between April 22 to April 29, 2020. The main findings of the two studies are in total accord-Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:22225 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-79299-7 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ ance with each other, supporting the validity of the GCCS preSR data. We also acknowledge possible selection bias-we estimate that GCCS sample was drawn predominantly from people close to the academic circles, e.g., undergraduate and graduate students, university staff, biotech and high-tech workers and their family and social circles. However, the large sample size, geographical and cultural diversity, combined with the uniformity in the time spent under strict social restrictions (first wave COVID-19 responses), reduce any potential systematic bias. We included all the data needed for the evaluation of the conclusions in the Results section or in the Supplementary Information file. Additional data related to this article may be requested from the authors. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ NobelPrize.org. Nobel Media AB 2020 The art of entrainment Social influences on mammalian circadian rhythms: Animal and human studies The Circadian System of Man: Results of Experiments Under Temporal Isolation Rhythms of REM and slow-wave sleep in subjects living on abnormal time schedules A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes The µMCTQ: An ultra-short version of the Munich ChronoType questionnaire Chronotype and social jetlag: A (self-)critical review Social jetlag: Misalignment of biological and social time Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in social jetlag and mental illness Greater social jetlag associates with higher HbA1c in adults with type 2 diabetes: A cross sectional study Depression scores associate with chronotype and social jetlag in a rural population Social jetlag, obesity and metabolic disorder: Investigation in a cohort study Social jetlag and obesity Is social jetlag associated with an adverse endocrine, behavioral, and cardiovascular risk profile Social jetlag, chronotype, and cardiometabolic risk Social jetlag and menstrual symptoms among female university students Decreased psychological well-being in late@ chronotype" is mediated by smoking and alcohol consumption Habitual sleep, social jetlag, and reaction time in youths with delayed sleep-wake phase disorder. A case-control study The effect of chronotype and social jetlag on sleep, mental health, quality of life, and academic performance of medical students Subclinical scores in self-report based screening tools for attention deficits correlate with cognitive traits in typical evening-type adults tested in the morning Conflicted Clocks: Social Jetlag, Entrainment and the Role of Chronotype: From Physiology to Academic Performance; from Students to Working Adults An Investigation of Attention, Sleep, Impulsivity, and Employment in College Students The alerting effect of the wake maintenance zone during 40 hours of sleep deprivation Mismatch of sleep and work timing and risk of type 2 diabetes Chronotype modulates sleep duration, sleep quality, and social jet lag in shift-workers Entrainment of the human circadian clock to the natural light-dark cycle A marker for the end of adolescence Light and the human circadian clock Effects of lockdown on human sleep and chronotype during the COVID-19 pandemic Sleep in university students prior to and during COVID-19 stay-at-home orders Effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on human sleep and rest-activity rhythms A survey on social jetlag in Japan: A nationwide, cross-sectional internet survey Chronotypes in the US-Influence of age and sex Sleep in adolescents: The perfect storm Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms The aging clock: Circadian rhythms and later life Epidemiology of the human circadian clock Light and the human circadian clock The young and the restless: Socializing trumps sleep, fear of missing out, and technological distractions in firstyear college students The effects of chronotype, sleep schedule and light/dark pattern exposures on circadian phase The effects of self-selected light-dark cycles and social constraints on human sleep and circadian timing: A modeling approach The effects of regularizing sleep-wake schedules on daytime sleepiness Sleep duration and mortality-Does weekend sleep matter The Handbook of Research Synthesis Locally weighted regression: An approach to regression analysis by local fitting This work is supported by Ariel University (M.K.). We thank Elizabeth B. Klerman for her valuable comments on the manuscript. We thank Nebal Egbariah, Mahmud Sindiani, Ashraf Abdo, Rinatia Maaravi-Hesseg, Luisa Piltz, Roumen Kirov, Maria Robles, Vera Vladimirova, Alexander Krivosheev, Ivan Petrov, Tatyana Vasilkova, Svetlana Solovieva and Elena Dergousova for their contributions in translation of the GCCS to different languages and/or for the efforts to advertise the study in different communities. We thank the Portuguese Sleep Society (APS), Japanese Society for Chronobiology (JSC) and Japanese Society of Sleep Research (JSSR) for their help in advertisement. M.K. and T.R. designed research; M.K., V.T. and T.R. performed research; M.K., T.R., C.R., Y.K., S.K., D.G. and V.K. contributed translations of the GCCS to different languages and advertised the study in their countries; M.K., V.T. and T.R. analyzed data; M.K., V.T., C.R., Y.K., S.K., D.G., V.K. and T.R. wrote the paper. The authors declare no competing interests. The online version contains supplementary material available at https ://doi. org/10.1038/s4159 8-020-79299 -7.Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.K.Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/.