key: cord-1050264-ish3tf2k authors: Renström, Emma A.; Bäck, Hanna title: Emotions during the Covid‐19 pandemic: Fear, anxiety, and anger as mediators between threats and policy support and political actions date: 2021-06-17 journal: J Appl Soc Psychol DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12806 sha: d87774f3977db629a05151cca4fc2fee84f8718a doc_id: 1050264 cord_uid: ish3tf2k The Covid‐19 pandemic has significantly changed the lives of most people. It has been described as the most severe global health disaster of modern times by the United Nations. No doubt such a major crisis influences what citizens think of different policies, and how they become politically active, not to mention, the forceful emotional experiences that the Covid‐19 pandemic brings. This study evaluates how emotions affect support for policies related to restricting the spread of the virus and economic assistance, and how emotions affect intentions to engage politically. In an experiment (N = 1,072), we manipulated emotional reactions to threat by highlighting different aspects of the pandemic. Our findings show that different experimental treatments elicit different emotions, and that fear, anxiety, and anger are all related to policy support and political action intentions, but in different ways. Fear and anger predict support for restrictive policies to limit the spread of the virus, while anxiety predicts support for economic policies. Anger and anxiety, but not fear, increase intentions to engage politically. Hence, we find support for a mechanism where different aspects of the Covid‐19 crisis evoke different emotional reactions, which in turn affects policy support and political actions differently. The Covid-19 pandemic has shaken the world in its foundations and has had a significant impact on individual and public health (e.g., Arora & Grey, 2020) . If we are to understand how health behaviors are affected by the pandemic, we also need to understand what kind of policies citizens endorse. The governmental reactions to the pandemic-how to limit the spread of the virus while at the same time keep a society alive, differed widely between countries. In the initial phase, the most common strategy in the Western European countries was complete lockdowns with the main goal to stop the virus from spreading. That is, social confinement was in most countries nationwide and strictly enforced (Bol et al., 2020) . Countries such as Austria, France, Norway, Italy, UK, Denmark, and the Netherlands had all adopted lockdown policies by the end of March 2020. Two major problems with this approach have been noted. First, it is impossible to keep a complete lockdown for an extended period of time-both for economic and health reasons (Holmes et al., 2020; Huang & Zhao, 2020; Li et al., 2020) . Second, even though lockdowns successfully reduced the spread of the virus, a second wave was feared when restrictions were lifted. As it later turned out, this fear was warranted since lifting the restrictions led to an increase in spread and many countries have to date (March 2021), enforced several lockdown periods. The public's reactions to the different strategies also varied extensively. In times of crisis, support for the government and its actions often increase (Campbell, 2012) , which was also the case in the initial phase of Covid-19 (Bol et al., 2020) . However, as time went by and the confinement of lockdowns exerted psychological and economic stress, many became more negative. In fact, several countries have even faced civil unrest. It is clear that this pandemic, and the governmental responses to it, has brought with it a host of emotions, ranging from anger to fear and anxiety. In this article, we explore how emotions function as mediators, explaining the effect of the saliency of different aspects of the Covid-19 pandemic, on support for different policies and action intentions. In clinical and health psychology, there is a long tradition of taking emotions into account. Even though this perspective has been less present in political psychology, a growing literature explores how affect influences political actions and attitudes (Brader & Marcus, 2013; Houghton, 2009; Lambert et al., 2019) . Affect is a broad term that includes both emotions and mood. While the mood is generally not associated with a particular stimulus, emotions are described as "elicited by something, are reactions to something, and are generally about something" (Ekkekakis, 2013, p. 322) . In addition, emotions tend to be fairly quick responses that also fade fairly quickly. In the present research, we are primarily interested in emotions as reactions to specific information related to the Covid-19 pandemic. Emotions are important to human life in general, and thus also to political life. Emotions inform individuals about a situation and prepare the body for a certain course of action (Frijda, 1986) . Emotions also affect cognitive processing such as attention, information seeking, and reliance on heuristics and stereotypes (Brader & Marcus, 2013) . As such, emotions have an important place in explaining political behavior and attitudes. For instance, it is wellestablished that emotions are important predictors in collective action and political activism (Goodwin et al., 2001; Gould, 2009; Klandermans & Stekelenburg, 2013) , as well as influencing how political information is processed and how political decisions are made (Brader, 2006) , and political attitudes in a broader sense (Brader & Marcus, 2013; Lambert et al., 2019) . There are several different theories on how emotions should be conceived of, ranging from a set-up of distinct emotions, to a dimensional approach where different emotional states are more fluid. Regardless, a broad distinction is made by focusing on valence, that is, the positive or negative nature of emotions. In the past, negative emotions have often been lumped together, regardless of their specific nature, including anger, fear, anxiety, and sadness (see, e.g., Brader & Marcus, 2013 for an overview). The widely influential PANAS is an example of this. PANAS stands for Positive Affect (PA) and Negative Affect (NA) and suggests that affect should be conceptualized in terms of these two dimensions (Watson et al., 1988) . However, research suggests that different negative emotions have a different impact on cognition and behavior. In contemporary political psychology, one of the most common distinctions is that between fear/anxiety and anger. There is much evidence that fear and anxiety, compared to anger, differently impact cognition and behavior (Banks & Valentino, 2012; Bodenhausen et al., 1994; Marcus et al., 2000; Merolla & Zechmeister, 2009 ). Anger is one of the most well-researched emotions in psychology in general, including clinical psychology. Hence, there is much work on its causes and consequences both on cognition and behavior. Anger has been described as a goal-pursuing and approach-linked emotion, motivating action, and decreasing cognitive processing. Anger is related to physical activity and increased blood flow ("fight" response). Several studies have shown that anger leads to confrontation and approach (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009; Harmon-Jones et al., 2009 , 2014 Huddy et al., 2007) . The fact that anger has been in the empirical spotlight for such a long time makes anger a good candidate to apply to a political context, and not surprisingly, anger is one of the most explored emotions in political psychology. Anger has been shown to be a strong motivation for participation in collective actions, such as demonstrations (Klandermans & van Stekelenburg, 2013) . Anger has a mobilizing function, which was for example shown by Valentino and colleagues (2011) in their influential paper on the mobilizing effect of anger on voting behavior. One reason why anger has this mobilizing function is that anger is elicited by perceived unfairness. Anger is the signal that something must be done to make things just (Huddy et al., 2007; Lambert et al., 2019) . Hence, angry people are more likely to engage. Anger has also been related to political attitudes and policy support, even though the literature here is not as extensive. For instance, anger, but not fear or anxiety, has been related to support for "hawkish," high-risk governmental policies (Lerner et al., 2003; Sadler et al., 2005; Skitka et al., 2006) . Less attention has been paid to the difference between fear and anxiety, even though there are some indications that these two emotions could have distinctive effects as well (Sylvers et al., 2011) . We here draw on Brader and Marcus (2013) , who suggest that a possible distinction between fear and anxiety could be important for future research in the field of political psychology. In the present study, we explore these two emotions as separate and argue that fear and anxiety should have distinctive effects on political attitudes and behavior. The labels fear and anxiety are often used interchangeably in the political psychology literature (Brader & Marcus, 2013; Wagner & Morisi, 2019) . Fear and anxiety share many more features as compared to for instance anger, even though all three are considered negative emotions (Watson et al., 1988) . Both fear and anxiety are elicited by threats and serve the evolutionary purpose of survival (Lang et al., 2000) . Even though fear and anxiety in the social and political psychology literature most often are treated as reflecting the same underlying emotion, but perhaps of varying intensity, clinical researchers distinguish between anxiety (such as Generalized Anxiety Disorder) and fear (present in specific phobias) (Öhman & Mineka, 2001) . In DSM-5 (The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, APA, 2013), anxiety is described as a worry about future events, whereas fear is a reaction to current events. Moreover, fear and anxiety seem to operate through different neural pathways and have different defining characteristics (Sylvers Lilienfeld & LaPrairie, 2011) . Fear is a fast emotion in that it is elicited when a threat is imminent or clear and the response also quickly fades. Fear is elicited when a threat is perceived to target the self (Davis & Stephan, 2011) . Fear is associated with a sense of danger and motivates fleeing behavior, which also suggests that fear is a high-arousal emotion, just like anger. Anxiety, however, is elicited when the threat is ambiguous, uncertain, or less specified, and the reaction is a prolonged emotional state. Since the threat is not defined, anxiety does not motivate fleeing in the same manner as fear and has been shown to lead to both approach and avoidance reactions (Gray & McNaughton, 2000; Perkins et al., 2012) . Previous research on fear has shown that fear increases risk estimates and precautionary planning (Lerner et al., 2003) . Fear also evokes uncertainty and feelings of lack of individual control, which are central in determining risks (Lerner & Keltner, 2001; Slovic, 1987) . There is an extensive body of research in political psychology about how threat affects political attitudes and political behavior. Although, it should be noted that emotions are usually not measured in this line of research (Lambert et al., 2019) , which means that even if threat has been found to influence attitudes and behavior, the specific emotion elicited by the threat is not empirically investigated. Much of the previous research that has examined the relationship between threat and political attitudes or behavior have focused on intergroup threat (such as terrorist threat, or the threat of immigration). The observed following attitude shift has also mainly been related to political attitudes relating to the threat, such as more restrictive immigration policies, increased military spending following the 9/11 terrorist attacks, but not on other issues related to ideology such as feminism or socialized medicine (Nail & McGregor, 2009 ). Research from the terror management literature has shown that the existential threat elicited by the thought of an unavoidable and imminent death causes existential anxiety (Greenberg et al., 1990; Pyszczynski et al., 2003) . Mortality salience has been linked to political conservatism, increased nationalism, and Republican voting. This effect has been labeled the "conservative shift" hypothesis (Arndt et al., 2002; Jost et al., 2003; Landau et al., 2004) , and is based on research that has linked existential anxiety with a low tolerance for ambiguity. Relatedly, a recent study found that existential anxiety was related to voting for the more secure, status quo, option in the Brexit referenda (Bäck et al., 2020) . Research that focuses on the role of existential threat in the mortality salience literature, has found mixed effects on political attitudes (Burke et al., 2010) . A possible explanation for such mixed results is the lack of emotion measures that could function as mediators. The research mentioned above has mainly used experimentally induced anxiety, but there are also individual variations in trait anxiety. One study investigated the relation between trait anxiety and political attitudes and found weak relations that anxiety disorders were related to concerns about economic inequality and environmental issues (Helminen, 2018) . These specific concerns seem fitting, given that anxiety is related to an ambiguous (future) threat. In relation to anxiety disorders, anxiety could potentially be a source of action. Obsessive-compulsive disorder, which is specified as an anxiety disorder by ICD-10 (WHO), is clearly action-oriented. People with this disorder tend to have specific behavioral rituals that aim to relief feelings of discomfort (Soomro, 2012) . This literature review indicates that anxiety influences attitudes, such that it increases a preference for what is known and safe, decreases support for risky policies (Huddy et al., 2007) and that it influences political behavior, such as voting for the safe option (Bäck et al., 2020) , leads to increased deliberation (MacKuen et al., 2010) , and increases the likelihood of other low-cost political actions (Denny, 2016) . Hence, it seems that anxiety may have some effects both on political attitudes and political behavior, but more research on this link is clearly needed. All in all, there is very little research on how fear, and especially in contrast to anxiety, affects political attitudes and behavior. One reason for this may be that the development of good measures for discrete emotions to be used in self-report surveys has fallen behind (Harmon-Jones et al., 2016) . The present study utilizes the Covid-19 pandemic to explore how different negative emotions, specifically anger, fear, and anxiety, influence political attitudes and action intentions. Specifically, we expect that highlighting certain aspects of the pandemic will evoke specific emotions that in turn will influence policy attitudes and action intentions. Before specifying our hypotheses, we briefly describe the Covid-19 context and focus particularly on Sweden where the present research was conducted. In an overview of policies implemented in reaction to Covid-19, Cheng et al. (2020) present data of over 10 000 policy announcements in over 190 countries. The most commonly implemented policy was to close national borders. The second most common policy was to close schools and nonessential businesses. In most cases, the implementations were also compulsory. Such measures undoubtedly affect citizens both individually and as a nation (e.g., Arora & Grey, 2020). The Swedish approach initially deviated from most other countries since Sweden did not go for a lockdown strategy. The Swedish goal was still to "flatten the curve," that is, to allow the health care system to keep up and avoid a collapse. Hence, while the means to reach the goal differed, the goal itself was the same across all countries. Whereas most other countries opted for restrictions regulated by law enforcement, the Public Health Agency of Sweden provided recommendations for individual behavior. The term recommendation is heavily culturally dependent and also varies with sender within Sweden. As Prime Minister Stefan Löfven stated in a rare speech to the public on March 22, when the Public Health Agency gives a recommendation, it is expected that the public complies-it is not a choice to do it or not, you should do it. Still, there was no punishment for citizens that did not follow the recommendations, which were, among others, to keep distance, avoid nonessential travels, increase hand hygiene, and to stay at The most urgent policies that were been discussed in relation to the Covid-19 pandemic were (a) policies that aimed to restrict the spread of the virus and (b) policies to support the economy. We expect that information focusing on the related threats, spread of the virus and economic collapse, will influence what kind of policies people support, but that the mediating mechanism is via what emotions the threats trigger. The stimuli material focused on either transgressors of the Public Health Agency's recommendations or the spread and danger of the virus. First, we expect that focusing on transgressors of the recommendations will evoke anger, and that that anger will increase support for policies directed at limiting the spread of the virus, that is, more restrictive policies. This could be seen as a way to "get back" at the transgressors. Anger has been shown to be related to more aggressive policies (Sadler et al., 2005; Skitka et al., 2006) , and the policies directed to limiting spread severely limits individual freedom. Hence, we expect that: focusing on transgressors of the Public Health Agency's recommendations should lead to anger, which in turn is likely to increase support for restrictive policies (H1a). We do not expect that anger will affect support for economic policies. Second, we expect that focusing on the spread and danger of the virus will evoke fear, which in turn will increase policies that target spread control (restrictive policies). Fear in this case should mainly be related to a fear for the own health. Hence, both anger and rear are predicted to increase restrictive policy support, but for different reasons. We hypothesize that: focusing on the virus and the strained health care situation should lead to fear, which in turn is likely to increase support for restrictive policies (H1b). We do not expect that fear will affect support for economic policies. Third, we expect that a focus on the spread and danger of the virus will also evoke anxiety, which in turn will increase economic policy support. Given that anxiety should be related to a prolonged concern, compared to fear, we expect that anxiety will have a stronger impact on support for policies related to the economy. The economic threat of the Covid-19 pandemic is less clear and it is uncertain how life will pan out in the post-crisis period. Here, we also draw on Fetzer et al. (2020) , who found that focusing on the mortality rate of Covid-19 increased economic concerns in a US population. Hence, we hypothesize that: focusing on the virus and strained health care situation should lead to anxiety, which in turn is likely to increase support for economic policies (H1c). Anger is an action-oriented emotion and has long been considered a predictor of political activity, hence we here expect that anger will increase intentions to engage in political actions. We thus hypothesize that: focusing on transgressors of the Public Health Agency's recommendations should lead to anger, which in turn is likely to increase political action intentions (H2a). Regarding fear, we expect that fear is not linked to political engagement or possibly even negatively related to engagement, since fear is generally related to avoidance. Because of the exploratory focus or this study, we do not specify a hypothesis for fear and political action intentions. Since there is very little prior research on anxiety and political engagement, it is not straightforward to hypothesize about its effects. Nonetheless, since anxiety is related to an ambiguous threat and a prolonged emotional state, we believe that anxiety could also be related to increased political action tendencies. However, we make this claim with the precondition that the political activities that we measure here are of a fairly specific nature. Since participating in demonstrations is not an option in relation to the Covid-19 pandemic, the actions are more in the form of online participation. This would be in line with the study by Denny (2016) , who found that financial anxiety was related to political participation when participation was measured as signing an online petition. We tentatively expect that: focusing on the virus and strained health care situation should lead to anxiety, which in turn is likely to increase political action intentions (H2b). This experimental study designed to evaluate the hypotheses presented above employs two treatments designed to elicit different emotional responses-either anger, or fear and anxiety. The two experimental conditions consisted of short fictive news articles (about 300 words each). The first article focused on the virus, the limits of the health care system when dealing with too many intensive care patients, the dangers of the virus and estimates of deaths and severe cases. To make it more relevant to the individual, potential consequences of worst case scenarios such that it would entail that almost everybody would be severely affected by the disease-either themselves or someone close to them, were highlighted. Other highlights included that the need for intensive care was not restricted to the elderly population. The aim of this text was to present both concrete and specific stimulus that would elicit fear, where specific measures could help to avoid it, and a more ambiguous stimulus that could elicit anxiety. Even though the virus itself constitutes a specific source of fear, the future consequences of behavioral restrictions are not clear and hence could function to elicit anxiety. We here draw on Fetzer and colleagues (2020), who found that economic anxiety increased following the entrance of Covid-19 in the US as measured by a substantial increase in google searches for economic recession, as well as survey data. Further, they also found that focusing on the high mortality rate of Covid-19 led to increased economic worries. The second article described people who defied authority recommendations to stay at home, and because of this, contributed to unnecessary spread of the virus. The data was collected just before Easter, when the basic school system in Sweden has a one-week break, which is among certain groups of people often spent going skiing or on holidays abroad. On the 15 March-three weeks before the Easter break-the Ministry for Foreign Affairs issued a recommendation to avoid all nonessential international travels. This led to a fear that people would instead travel within Sweden and hence spread the virus. Especially, focus was on the Stockholm region which was the epicenter of the disease in Sweden, while most other parts of Sweden remained fairly unaffected at this point. Health care workers from around Sweden expressed their concerns that travelers would bring the virus there, and with a limited capacity of health care institutions in smaller regions, they were dreading a disaster. Hence, the text focused on the possibility that some people might disregard the recommendations to stay at home and go traveling to the Swedish skiing resorts, the south of Sweden or the archipelagos regardless of the recommendations. This ensured that the text had a clear focus on specific transgressors where the idea was that their behavior would be perceived as unjust and egoistic and thus elicit anger. Research shows that when violations of justice are severe and transgressors are clearly identified, anger is the prime emotion (Aquino et al., 2001; Bennett & Earwaker, 1994; Bradfield & Aquino, 1999; Darby & Schlenker, 1982; Lickel et al., 2006) . Importantly, everything in the texts were taken from real life news and debates so the texts did not present anything untrue or something that the participants would not encounter just reading a newspaper or watching the news on television. The articles simply summarized different aspects of the pandemic and presented them to the participants. Translations of the texts are found in the Appendix. We first pre-tested the material to make sure the conditions did in fact elicit the expected emotions. The pre-test led to minor changes and also some changes in the emotion scale that we used so that we would be better equipped to capture the emotions of interest. The changes that we made to the stimulus material were small and mainly done to increase emotional reactions. For instance, we made it more explicit that the virus was very dangerous and that most people would probably be affected by it. Data was collected online by the survey company Enkätfabriken. Participants (N = 1,072) were randomly assigned one of the two conditions (see more information on the participants below). Data was collected between April 2 and 7, 2020. The study began by information the participants about the study and its purpose, data handling and right to withdraw at any time. Before starting the survey, the participant was required to provide informed consent. After this, participants responded to demographic questions re- The participants then read one of the short news pieces that constituted our main independent variable, and was designed to elicit either anger or fear and anxiety. Following the news piece, we assessed the participant's emotional state. The measure that we used is based on Harmon-Jones and colleagues' Discrete Emotions Questionnaire (2016). We first pre-tested the translation of the entire Discrete Emotions Questionnaire in relation to our stimulus material. This pre-test showed that some items did not translate very well, so we made some changes. For example, the item "Grossed out" was extremely skewed (2.14). Since the disgust scale was not our main interest, we removed this item. When running reliability analyses on the Sadness subscale, the item "Lonely" decreased Cronbach's alpha. Since sadness was not the main interest of our study, we dropped Lonely in the main study. We also removed some other items from the emotions scale that were of less interest to the present study, to shorten the questionnaire. For instance, we removed the happiness subscale entirely, since it made little sense to rate happiness in relation to Covid-19. We also removed the relaxation and desire subscales. Finally, we added some items to better capture the emotions we were primarily Out of the 1,072 participants, 547 (51%) were in the fear/anxiety condition and 525 (49%) in the anger condition. There were 496 men and 505 women in total (71 did not answer the gender question). In conditions. There were no significant differences, ts < 1.34, ps > .18. Because our theoretical argument is that the experimental conditions lead to different emotional reactions, which in turn affect policy support and political action intentions, we ran mediation models. To test all mediators (all emotions) simultaneously, we ran parallel mediation analysis. This means that both of the experimental conditions were allowed to influence all emotions, and all emotions were allowed to influence the outcome variables. All p-values < .05 were considered significant. We used Hayes' PROCESS macro for SPSS, model 4 to run the analyses (Hayes, 2013 (Hayes, , 2018 . Before presenting the results from the mediation analyses, we present descriptive results. Table 1 shows means and standard deviations on the mediation variables split on condition along with results from separate t-tests. There were significant differences between the conditions on all three of the focal emotion indices. As expected, fear was higher in the fear/anxiety condition focusing on contamination and spread of the virus compared to the anger condition, and anger was higher in the anger condition compared to the fear/anxiety condition. Anxiety was also higher in the fear/anxiety condition compared to the anger condition. Hence, the information in the stimulus texts successfully affected the participants' emotional state. Even though we did not have any expectations about sadness, sadness was significantly higher in the fear/anxiety condition compared to the anger condition. Disgust and empathy did not vary with condition. Hence, we include sadness, but not disgust or empathy, in our main analyses (see the Appendix for analyses including all emotion indices). Table 2 shows means and standard deviations for the dependent variables, split on condition. There were no significant differences between the conditions on any of the outcome variables, ts absolute values <1.5, ps > .12. Table 3 shows correlations between the emotions and the outcome variables. As can be seen, there were fairly strong correlations between most emotions and opinions and action intentions. All emotions were positively related to policy support for both economic and spread control policies. All emotions were also positively related to political action intentions. To test our hypotheses that highlighting different aspects of the Covid-19 outbreak will lead to different emotional reactions, which in turn affect policy support, and political action intentions, we performed a series of parallel mediation models, one for each dependent variable. In all models, we controlled for age, gender, education, ideological position (both left-right and liberal-conservative), and political interest. We also included sadness in all models since sadness was elevated in the fear/anxiety condition. See the Appendix for models with all emotions. The results can be seen in Table 4 , which shows the regression results, and Table 5 As can be seen there is an effect of condition on all three focal emotions. Anger is higher in the anger condition, and fear and anxiety are higher in the fear/anxiety condition. In these models, the effect of condition on sadness did not quite reach conventional significance levels (p = .052). Gender had a significant effect on all emotions such that women reported stronger emotional experiences overall. There was a weak negative effect of age on anger and anxiety indicating that both anger and anxiety tended to decrease with higher age. Higher education was related to less anger but not to any other emotion. Ideological position on a left-right scale had no effect on emotions. However, people who were more conservative, reported stronger emotional experiences overall, except for sadness. Higher political interest was weakly related to anxiety. Hypothesis H1a stated that when information focused on people who disobeyed the Public Health Agency's recommendation, people would experience anger and, in turn, anger would predict support for more restrictive policies. As can be seen in Table 4 , the coefficient for anger on support for restrictive policies was significant, in support of H1a. The results indicate that the condition influences the outcome variable via anger. Hypothesis H1b stated that when information focused on the virus and the strained health care situation, this would elicit fear, which in turn would predict support for restrictive policies. Again, the coefficient between fear and restrictive policy support was significant, indicating support for H1b. Hence, both anger and fear were related to increased support for more restrictive policies, so called "flatten the curve"-policies. Age, gender and left-right position was unrelated to support for restrictive policies, while higher education and higher political interest was related to lower support, more conservative self-positioning was related to more support. Hypothesis H1c stated that information focusing on the virus and health care situation should also elicit anxiety, and this in turn should be related to support for economic policies since anxiety is rather related to an ambiguous stimulus. As can be seen, the coefficient for anxiety on economy policy support was significant, such that higher anxiety was related to more support for economy policies. Again, the condition influenced policy support via anxiety, supporting hypothesis H1c. There were no effects of the other emotions on economy policy support. Again, education was negatively related to policy support Anger ( Gender is dummy coded with men = 0, women = 1. ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05. such that higher education was related to lower support for economy policies. None of the other control variables exerted a significant effect. Finally, we expected the focal emotions to have different effects on political action intentions. H2a stated that information focused on transgressors would lead to anger, and anger would increase political action intentions. As can be seen in the last column of Table 4, there was a positive effect of anger on political action intentions indicating that angry people were more likely to be willing to engage politically. H2b stated that information about the virus leading to anxiety should also increase participation intentions. In line with this, we found that the coefficient for anxiety on political action intentions was positive, indicating that anxious people were more willing to engage politically. Neither fear nor sadness was significantly related to political action intentions. Subsequent modeling showed that both fear and anger led to support for more restrictive policies, but for arguably different reasons. In the anger condition, the information was focused on justice violations by specific individuals which led people to become angry. Restrictive policies for these people should be related to restriction of transgressors' possibility to violate recommendations-to restore justice and fairness. For fearful individuals, however, restrictive policies should be related to a desire to limit the spread of the disease to decrease the risk to the own health. Participants who reported higher levels of fear were informed about the danger of the virus and the possibility of overwhelming the health care system. For these individuals, restrictions should mainly restore a sense of control and security. Relatedly, a recent paper (Harper et al., 2020) showed that fear was related to increased public health behaviors such as social distancing and increased hand hygiene. Even though previous research has shown that fear may lead to support for less aggressive policies (Lerner et al., 2003) , which could be considered contradictive to our results where fear was related to support for restriction of individual freedom, we argue that these results are not incompatible. First, the aggressive policies studied in earlier research are related to intergroup threat (i.e., 9/11 terrorist attacks), while the current situation has no social group as enemy, but a virus. Thus, restrictions of individual freedom should not be conceived of as an aggressive policy, but rather a precautionary measure, which also has been shown to increase when individuals experience fear (Brader & Marcus, 2013; Lerner et al., 2003) . Moreover, fear was not assessed in the earlier study, Even though fear and anger are fundamentally different emotions, they are both high-arousal emotions (Marcus et al., 2000) . This could explain why they both are related to restrictive policies, which focus on the situation here and now, but not economic policies. To support economic policies, individuals need to extend their focus beyond the immediate situation and evaluate what might come after the pandemic has passed. Such an extended focus may be difficult under states of high-arousal, which tends to narrow the attentional span (Brader & Marcus, 2013) . Anxiety, however, is related to increased attention to information and less reliance on prior convictions (Brader & Marcus, 2013) . Similarly, while anger strengthens reliance on prior convictions, anxiety in fact had the opposite effect, undermining such use of heuristics (Banks & Valentino, 2012) . In line with previous research on emotions and political activity (see, e.g., Brader & Marcus, 2013) , we found that anger increased intentions to engage in political actions. For instance, Brader et al. (2010) found that when faced with a potentially deadly viral outbreak, angry citizens were more likely to engage politically by for instance contacting officials, and demanding investigations and prosecution of those responsible for the outbreak. Fearful individuals, however, were more likely to engage in protective behaviors such as wearing a face mask, increasing hand hygiene and getting informed. Interestingly, we also found that intentions to engage politically was mediated by anxiety. The political psychology research to date has not focused particularly on anxiety (Brader & Marcus, 2013) , although some exceptions exist. For instance, Denny (2016) found that financial anxiety led to increased participation in the form of signing an online petition. Similarly, in the present study, the participation items mainly concerned actions that could be performed online, such as sharing material, signing petitions and contacting authorities. Another reason to assume that anxiety may increase participation tendencies is that anxious people may tend to see beyond the immediate situation, and research show that anxiety heightens attention to information. Even though previous research also show that fear is related to precautionary planning (see, e.g., Brader & Marcus, 2013; Lerner et al., 2003) , it may be confounded with anxiety since these emotions rarely have been separated in previous research. Compared to fear, which is focused on seeking out immediate security from the present situation, anxiety should relate more to future prospects. For instance, in the APA diagnostic manual, DSM-5, anxiety is described as a concern about future events. This idea is further corroborated in our study, where anxiety was related to economic policy support rather than restrictive policy support. Together, this indicates that anxious people have a different focus than fearful people, or angry for that matter. Relating these results to trait anxiety, or anxiety disorders, they make intuitive sense. Highly anxious people tend to ruminate over certain aspects and hence seek out information and plan ahead. Such behavior should be facilitated in a state of anxiousness compared to fear, where the response is to seek out immediate protection from The effect of condition on support for economic and restrictive policies (panel a and b), and political action intentions (panel c), via fear, anxiety, and anger the fear-arousing stimulus. Anxiety can also be relieved by perform- (Brader & Marcus, 2013; Lambert et al., 2019) . Fear and anxiety, even though they may elicited by the same information, lead to very different outcomes-both support for different policies and political action intentions. This means that much previous research in the field of political psychology that has used threat to manipulate attitudes and behavior may be confounded with differential effects of emotions that have not been measured. Finally, it should be noted that there are some inherent problems associated with measuring emotions in the format we did here, that is, when the participant is asked to rate a host of emotions. Because they read all emotions and rate them, this may remind them that they also feel other emotions that they had not initially thought of. Therefore, it would be good to develop a measure of emotions that is less intrusive. As the measure looks now, there needs to be very strong effects between the eliciting stimuli for the scale to fall out on the expected dimensions, which is difficult to achieve when the emotions are close to each other. For instance, it may be difficult for a participant to separate if they feel worried or alarmed, and probably they do experience both to some extent, and as the format is now, participants may overestimate their experience on some, closely related, emotions. The present research has both theoretical and societal relevance. We contribute to the research on emotions and how they are related to political attitudes and behavior. Specifically, fear and anxiety have different effects on both policy preferences and political action intentions. Societally, the results are important for understanding how citizens respond to different reports of the pandemic, and how the responses may elicit support for different policies and political actions, which has consequences for health related behaviors. The fast-track emotions fear and anger seem both to increase support for restrictive policies that aim to reduce the spread of the virus and "flatten the curve." However, since these are high-arousal emotions, their effect is unlikely to be prolonged. Anxiety, in contrast, probably does have a prolonged effect, but this emotional response was unrelated to restrictive policies. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. Coeff. Coeff. Coeff. Health behaviour changes during COVID-19 and the potential consequences: A mini-review Threat, emotions, and voting for the status quo in the Brexit referendum Emotional substrates of white racial attitudes Victims' responses to apologies: The effects of offender responsibility and offense severity Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness The effect of Covid-19 lockdowns on political support: Some good news for democracy? Campaigning for hearts and minds: How emotional appeals in political ads work Fight or flight: When political threats arouse public anger and fear Emotion and political psychology The effects of blame attributions and offender likableness on forgiveness and revenge in the workplace Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research Policy makes mass politics Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications CoronaNet: A dyadic dataset of government responses to the Covid-19 Children's reactions to apologies Electromyographic analyses of responses to intergroup threat The good intention gap: Poverty, anxiety, and implications for political action The measurement of affect, mood, and emotion: A guide for health-behavioral research Coronavirus perceptions and economic anxiety The emotions Partisan endorsement experiments do not affect mass opinion on COVID-19 (SSRN Working Paper Conceptualizing and measuring participation in the age of the internet: Is online political engagement really different to offline? Passionate politics: Emotions and social movements Moving politics: Emotion and ACT UP's fight against AIDS The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview Detecting transient emotional responses with improved self-report measures and instructions PANAS positive activation is associated with anger Approach motivation and its relationship to positive and negative emotions Functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach Fear and anxiety as predictors of political attitudes: A prospective cohort study (master thesis in Psychology) Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: A call for action for mental health science Political psychology: Situations, individuals, and cases Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep quality during COVID-19 outbreak in Chine: A web-based cross-sectional survey On the distinct political effects of anxiety and anger The affect effect: Dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior Political conservatism as motivated social cognition Social movements and the dynamics of collective action Anger and its consequences for judgment and behavior: Recent developments in social and political psychology Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality salience and reminders of 9/11 on support for Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment Fear, anger, and risk Progression of mental health services during the COVID-19 outbreak in China Vicarious retribution: The role of collective blame in intergroup aggression Civic engagements: Resolute partisanship or reflective deliberation Affective intelligence and political judgment Immigration policy and right-wing populism in Western Europe Democracy at risk: How terrorist threats affect the public Conservative shift among liberals and conservatives following 9/11/01 Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Towards an evolved module of fear and fear learning Is online participation distinct from offline participation? A latent class analysis of participation types and their stratification A facial expression for anxiety Ideology, prejudice, and attitudes toward sexual minority social policies and organizations In the wake of 9/11: Rising above the terror Emotions, attributions, and policy endorsement in response to the September 11th terrorist attacks Confrontational and preventative policy responses to terrorism: Anger wants a fight and fear wants "them" to go away Perception of risk Obsessive compulsive disorder Differences between trait fear and trait anxiety: Implications for psychopathology Election night's alright for fighting: The role of emotions in political participation Anxiety, fear and political decision-making, oxford research encyclopedia of politics Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales Emotions during the Covid-19 pandemic: Fear, anxiety, and anger as mediators between threats and policy support and political actions