key: cord-1045204-4d1ei6pf authors: Caserotti, Marta; Gavaruzzi, Teresa; Girardi, Paolo; Tasso, Alessandra; Buizza, Chiara; Candini, Valentina; Zarbo, Cristina; Chiarotti, Flavia; Brescianini, Sonia; Calamandrei, Gemma; Starace, Fabrizio; de Girolamo, Giovanni; Lotto, Lorella title: Who is likely to vacillate in their COVID-19 vaccination decision? Free-riding intention and post-positive reluctance date: 2021-11-12 journal: Prev Med DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106885 sha: 9eb0d062588051ae3266f1ee178ccbdffabc1c35 doc_id: 1045204 cord_uid: 4d1ei6pf Despite the actual availability of COVID-19 vaccines to combat the pandemic, many people are still vacillating in their decision to vaccinate. In this study, we considered the effect of two relevant contextual issues on vaccination intention: the number of people infected with COVID-19 is increasing, and the pace of vaccination is gaining speed. Specifically, we hypothesized that having already contracted SARS-CoV-2 (post-positive reluctance) could lead people to underestimate the importance of vaccination. Moreover, as the number of vaccinated people increases, more hesitant people could fall into the free-riding intention category, benefitting from the immunity provided by others' vaccinations. Vaccine hesitancy becomes more critical as the vaccination campaign proceeds: at one point, it will be inevitable to deal with hesitant people. This study is part of a WHO Regional Office for Europe project and involved a representative sample of 5006 Italians interviewed in January–February 2021. In case of post-positive reluctance, both young age and female gender increase vaccine hesitancy, while a high level of education reduces free-riding intention. Considering post-positive reluctance and free riding, a protective effect on hesitancy is associated with negative affective states, adherence to protective behaviors, trust in health information sources, and resilience. In contrast, increased vaccine hesitancy is associated with a high level of conspiracy-mindedness and trust in media information sources. Recognizing and studying the post-positive reluctance and the phenomenon of free-riding people can help us to become more efficient in combatting the virus. After more than a year, the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic remains ongoing, but the situation is evolving, mainly due to the growth of scientific knowledge and the development of vaccines. To date, studies have mostly focused on vaccine hesitancy per se (Lazarus et al., 2021; Caserotti et al., 2021) , missing new salient aspects that need to be considered: many infected people have acquired some natural immunity, and vaccination proceeds at a rapid pace. If reaching out and vaccinating people who accept vaccination is certainly crucial, the real challenge is to convince reluctant persons. We know, for example, that people's beliefs and attitudes about vaccines are mainly shaped by the number of affected people and, in turn, by changes in individual risk perception (e.g., Caserotti et al., 2021; Schwarzinger et al., 2021) . We also know that even politics may play a role in modifying people's beliefs and attitudes; for example, claims made during the US presidential campaign engendered worries that vaccines were rushed and thus not safe (Limaye et al., 2021; Thorp, 2020) . Additionally, concerns are driven by the new technologies used for the development and manufacturing of some vaccines, such as worries related to the mRNA vaccines, sometimes fueled by the antivax community leveraging conspiracy beliefs (Ullah et al., 2021; Chirumbolo, 2021) . Fortunately, over time, vaccine acceptance has increased globally (Daly et al., 2021; Imperial College London, 2021) , but COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy could jeopardize high coverage, thus prolonging the need for lockdowns and other restrictive measures, with potentially negative socioeconomic consequences. According to a recent model developed by the Imperial College of London, the "mortality over a 2-year period could be up to 8 times higher in countries with high vaccine hesitancy compared to an ideal vaccination uptake," resulting in an additional 236 deaths per million population (Johns & van Elsland, 2021) . It is noteworthy that the problem of vaccine hesitancy becomes more and more critical as the vaccination campaign proceeds. As time goes by, it is inevitable to deal with hesitant persons, which can mean not only the outright vaccine refusers but also those who for different reasons avoid vaccination. Here, we hypothesize that having already contracted SARS-CoV-2, especially if symptoms are mild, could lead people to underestimate the danger of the virus and also the importance of getting vaccinated despite recommendations by guidelines (CDC, 2021; ECDC, 2021) . Additionally, as more people are getting vaccinated and the epidemiological situation improves, restrictions will decrease, and people still having lingering doubts about vaccination will be even less motivated to get vaccinated and may be tempted by free riding, benefitting from the immunity provided by others' vaccinations but J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f hindering the protection of the whole community (Milman et al., 2021) . From this perspective, it is essential to identify the predictors of vaccine acceptance and to establish who is likely to vacillate in their COVID-19 vaccination decision. In this study, conducted in the early stages of the vaccine roll-out in Italy, we first estimated the predictors of COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy and then modeled the additional effect of factors predicting a person's reluctance to vaccinate after testing positive for and factors predicting unwillingness to vaccinate while counting on others getting vaccinated (i.e., free riding). Investigating these factors will shed a unique light on vaccine acceptance and reluctance. The online questionnaire was administered in two waves (January-February 2021) by a survey company (BDA-Doxa), which selected a national Italian representative sample (N=5,006), weighted by gender, age (18-70 years), area of residence, size of living center, education, and employment status (unemployed and employed). Employment status was further classified as unemployed, employed, and employed as a health worker. Details about the sampling strategy, response rates, and weights can be found in the supplementary materials (Method S1). This cross-sectional study is part of a larger project promoted by the WHO Regional Office for Europe called "Monitoring knowledge, risk perceptions, preventive behavior and trust to inform pandemic outbreak response" and conducted in more than 30 countries (see WHO 2021 for the full protocol). In Italy, the study has been approved by the Ethical Committee of the Italian coordinating institution (protocol 286/2020, registration ISRCTN 39724). In the present study, we considered a subset of variables available in the entire dataset, including participants' Vaccine Hesitancy (VH) against SARS-CoV-2, post-positive reluctance, and free-riding intention, all rated on a 7-point response scale. In addition to the demographic variables used to collect the representative sample, participants were asked during the survey whether they suffered from chronic diseases and whether they had contracted COVID-19 (both "Yes," "No," "Do not know"), whether they knew people who had contracted the disease ("No," "Yes and still alive," "Yes, deceased"), and their employment status ("Unemployed," "Employed," "Employed as health worker"). Participants J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f were asked about their concerns for the direct economic consequences of the pandemic (rated on a 7-point scale). The questionnaire also included the 5-item Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire (Bruder et al., 2013) and three items taken from the Brief Resilience Scale (Smith et al., 2008) . Finally, participants were asked whether they thought it would be appropriate for the entire population to follow national vaccination plans (vaccination propensity; "Yes," "No," "Do not know"). Questions related to other investigated domains are described in detail in the following section. The dataset has been uploaded on a public repository (http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5040719). The study variables were summarized in frequency tables and figures (frequency for categorical variables, median and InterQuartile Range (IQR) for continuous variables). Nonparametric tests were computed to compare the distribution of variables on ordinal Likert scale across the two waves. Categorical variables were compared using chi-squared or Fisher's exact test where expected frequencies in any combination were less than 10. Statistical significance was assumed at the 5% level. A total of 11 different Explorative Factorial Analyses (EFAs) were performed on groups of variables related to specific domains: 1) COVID-19 perceived risk, 2) Negative affective states, 3) Control, 4) Protective behaviors, 5) Trust in media information sources (e.g., traditional and social media), 6) Trust in health information sources (e.g., Ministry of Health, WHO), 7) Frequency of use of media information sources, 8) Frequency of use of health information sources, 9) Trust in health institutions, 10) Conspiracy-mindedness, and 11) Resilience. The metric invariance between the two waves was confirmed for each domain by a likelihood ratio test, allowing us to consider the two waves together in each EFA (Method S2). Since the scales of all variables reported 7-point discrete ordinal values, each factorial analysis was performed on the polychoric correlation matrix, hypothesizing normally distributed continuous latent variables. We extracted from each EFA only the first factor, which explained the highest percentage of variance: the amount of variance explained by the J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f one-factor solution was satisfactory, ranging from 44% to 68%, with the exception of COVID-19 perceived risk and Control, which explained a limited amount of variability (30% and 28%, respectively). The estimated loadings were then used to calculate the regression factor scores for both waves. Regression scores were categorized in tertiles (1 st tertile = low risk; 2 nd tertile = medium risk; 3 rd tertile = high risk) for inclusion in the following regression models. For each EFA, the number and the name of items included, their internal consistency (Cronbach's α), the estimated loadings, and the proportion of deviance explained are reported in Table S1 . To evaluate which factors influenced the respondent's motivation to avoid this measure against COVID-19, we employed Cumulative Logistic Models (CLM): one model was estimated on VH, and two other models were used to predict vacillation (Method S2). J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f The main characteristics of the sample are reported in Table 1 Due to the sampling strategy, the distribution of gender (50% females), age, employment, and geographical area of residence were the same in both waves (Table 1) . Educational level was low in 41% of the sample, with a slight reduction in the number of respondents who were placed in the highest category in the second wave (p=0.043). The VH was summarily low (median 2, Figure 1 ), as well as the post-positive reluctance or the free-riding intention (high intention with values 6 or 7 for only 10.1% and 8.2%, respectively, of the sample), with no statistically significant difference between the two waves (Table 1, Figure 1 ). In general, concerns about the economic consequences and the support of the public health policies were medium to high (median 5), while a relevant proportion (21%) of respondents reported the presence of a chronic disease. The lifetime prevalence of COVID-19 disease was 6.7%, with an increase in the second wave (from 5.7% in wave 1 to 7.7% in wave 2, p=0.015); at least 40% of respondents knew someone who had died from COVID-19. The pairwise marginal distribution and Spearman's correlations between 11 scores resulting from the EFAs performed on 11 dimensions are shown in Figure 2 . We found good agreement between trust and frequency of use of information obtained from health institutions (Spearman's ρ=0.57) and from media (ρ=0.61). A strong correlation was seen between trust in health institutions and in the information they provide (ρ=0.82). The COVID-19 perceived risk score was positively correlated with the negative affect score (ρ=0.51) and negatively correlated with the score related to the feeling of control (ρ=-0.28). J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f § Pairwise correlation test:*p<0.05, **p<0.01,***p<0.001. In Table 3 reports the results of the two models estimated for COVID-19 hesitancy due to a post-positive reluctance or a free-riding intention. In the first model, there was a slightly increased hesitancy in the second wave (+20% with respect to the first wave, 95%CI: 1.08−1.33), and the intention not to get the COVID-19 vaccine due to a previous contagion (Table S2 ). J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f 4. Discussion In line with the growing literature on the topic, our data confirmed several predictors of COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy. A decreased VH was associated with older age (Daly et al., 2021; Lazarus et al., 2020; Malik et al., 2020; KFF COVID-19 Vaccine Monitor -April 2021; Murphy et al., 2021; Robertson et al., 2021; Schwarzinger et al., 2021; Seale et al., 2021; Soares et al., 2021) , higher education level (Daly et al., 2021; Lazarus et al., 2020; Malik et al., 2020; Robertson et al., 2021; Schwarzinger et al., 2021) , being a health worker (Gagneux-Brunon et al., 2021; Maltezou et al., 2021) , having a chronic health condition (Schwarzinger et al., 2021; Seale et al., 2021; Soares et al., 2021) , being in favor of vaccination in general (Attwell et al., 2021; Caserotti et al., 2021; Palamenghi et al., 2020; Schwarzinger et al., 2021) , supporting public health policies (Soares et al., 2021) , adopting recommended public health measures (Soares et al., 2021) , higher risk perception (Attwell et al., 2021; Caserotti et al., 2021; Viswanath et al., 2021) , trusting health sources of information (Murphy et al., 2021; Palamenghi et al., 2020) , and trusting health institutions (Murphy et al., 2021; Viswanath et al., 2021) . Our data also confirmed that knowing someone who has been infected with COVID-19 decreased VH (e.g., Schwarzinger et al., 2021) , as knowing someone who died of COVID-19. Furthermore, VH decreased for participants feeling strong negative affective states, similar to previous findings showing that COVID-19-related anxiety was positively associated with vaccine acceptance (Bendau et al., 2021) . Our results also showed that worry about the future economic consequences of the pandemic decreased VH. While this is in line with the results of a Portuguese study showing that loss of income during the pandemic was a positive predictor of vaccine intention (Soaes et al., 2021) , other studies conducted in Germany found an inverse relationship between economic fears and vaccine acceptance (Bendau et al., 2021) or no relationship with non-pharmaceutical intervention acceptance (Rosman et al., 2021) . Reduced vaccination intentions were associated with female gender (Daly, 2021; Lazarus et al., 2020; Murphy et al., 2021; Robertson et al., 2021; Schwarzinger et al., 2021; Soares et al., 2021; Ishimaru et al., 2021 ; but see also Caserotti et al., 2021; Seale et al., 2021) , high trust in media information sources (Murphy et al., 2021) , and high conspiracy-mindedness (Murphy et al., 2021) . J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f We also aimed at investigating which factors hinder vaccination hesitancy among people who tested positive for COVID-19 and among those who counted on others getting vaccinated. Those who were more likely to vacillate in their COVID-19 intention, both for post positive reluctance and free riding intention, had medium or high frequency of use of media information and medium or high levels of conspiracy-mindedness. A high frequency of use of health information sources only increased the free riding intention. This might be moderated by the protective role played by trust in health information sources, which is highly correlated Additionally, it should be pointed out that the COVID-19 pandemic has fueled conspiracy beliefs with some people questioning the safety of vaccines and viewing the incentives as "bribes." Moreover, incentives are aimed not only at the "maybes" (Attwell et al., 2021) but also at those who would have been vaccinated anyway, resulting in a large waste of resources that might be used to promote more targeted interventions to increase, for example, confidence in vaccines among vulnerable and underserved groups (Curtis et al., 2021) . Further, given that convenience (e.g., free administration, ease of access, time, and place) affects uptake of the vaccine (Betsch et al., 2018; MacDonald, 2015) , these resources could be used, for example, to ensure compensation for lost wages among those who take time off work to be vaccinated, or to ensure convenient vaccination times and spaces, by securing widespread access to vaccines. While incentives are mainly directed at individuals, evidence is growing about the benefits of vaccines for the community too, although the issue is complex and dynamic (Milman et al., 2021) . Protecting vulnerable people in the community is a common benefit of most vaccines, and there is some evidence that efforts to improve understanding of community immunity (e.g., Hakim et al., 2021) may increase vaccination intention (Arnesen et al., 2018; Betsch et al., 2017; Logan et al., 2018) . Some studies suggest that stressing the pro-social benefits of vaccination might not be as decisive as focusing on aspects related to one's own emotions (Chou et al., 2020; Gavaruzzi et al., 2021; Tomljenovic et al., 2020) . In the COVID-19 context, prosocial messages seem to be effective in promoting protective behavior (e.g., Jordan et al, 2020) , but this may be driven by the protection of closer circles rather than by the community at large (Banker & Park, 2020) . It remains to be determined whether the pandemic has changed the way people understand community immunity and whether it can foster vaccine acceptance. Finally, we have all testified that the pandemic has been accompanied by an infodemic (Zarocostas, 2020) , with social media often considered as one of the factors contributing to vaccine hesitancy and anti-vaccine sentiment (e.g., Basch et al., 2021) , even if they can also be leveraged to promote critical thinking using pre-inoculation against fake news (Banas et al., 2010; van der Linden et al., 2021) . Pre-inoculation could be effective also for a segment of hesitant people who adhere to conspiracy beliefs, as it leverages people's fear of J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f manipulation, to alert them against misinformation (Basol et al., 2021; Lewandowsky et al., 2021; van der Linden et al., 2020) . We view these results as useful pieces of a puzzle in which psychological aspects must be considered to better understand vaccination intention. This study's main limitation is that we assessed intention, and, while intention is considered the best predictor of behavior, there might be mediating factors between the two (Brewer et al., 2017) . Another limitation is that we asked participants whether they had SARS-CoV-2 without inquiring about the timing of infection. Indeed, having already had COVID-19 reduced the baseline VH (Table 2 ), but it did not affect conditional hesitancy (Table 3 ). This might be because people who got infected were included without being asked how long ago they had had it. As clear information about the duration of antibodies following infection is still lacking, it is possible that those who had it more recently are more reluctant to get vaccinated than those who had it earlier on in the pandemic. Finally, the two conditional hesitations are not mutually exclusive events and further analysis could clarify how the estimates would change taking into account their competitive effect. Despite the constant monitoring of the efficacy of vaccines against new variants of the virus, adequate vaccination coverage undoubtedly remains one of the best weapons we have to prevent SARS-CoV-2 infection. It is therefore evident how crucial it is to know people's response to vaccination campaigns, as the context changes. The unprecedented analyses considered in this paper confirmed the importance of investigating how the predictors of post-positive reluctance and free-riding intention affect COVID-19 VH. Could information about herd immunity help us achieve herd immunity? Evidence from a population representative survey experiment Converting the maybes: Crucial for a successful COVID-19 vaccination strategy A meta-analysis of research on inoculation theory Evaluating prosocial COVID-19 messaging frames: Evidence from a field study on Facebook YouTube videos and informed decision-making about COVID-19 vaccination: Successive sampling study Towards psychological herd immunity: Cross-cultural evidence for two prebunking interventions against COVID-19 misinformation COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and related fears and anxiety On the benefits of explaining herd immunity in vaccine advocacy Beyond confidence: Development of a measure assessing the 5C psychological antecedents of vaccination Increasing vaccination: putting psychological science into action Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: conspiracy mentality questionnaire Associations of COVID-19 risk perception with vaccine hesitancy over time for Italian residents Vaccination hesitancy and the "myth" on mRNA-based vaccines in Italy in the COVID-19 era: Does urgency meet major safety criteria? Considering Emotion in COVID-19 vaccine communication: addressing vaccine hesitancy and fostering vaccine confidence Trends and clinical characteristics of COVID-19 vaccine recipients: a federated analysis of 57.9 million patients' primary care records in situ using OpenSAFELY. medRxiv Public Trust and Willingness to Vaccinate Against COVID-19 in the US From Intention to get vaccinations against COVID-19 in French healthcare workers during the first pandemic wave: a cross-sectional survey The Role of Emotional Competences in Parents' Vaccine Hesitancy. Vaccines A Web Application About Herd Immunity Using Personalized Avatars: Development Study Gender differences in the determinants of willingness to get the COVID-19 the working-age population in Japan. medRxiv Don't get it or don't spread it? Comparing selfinterested versus prosocially framed COVID-19 prevention messaging A global survey of potential acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine Hesitant or not? The association of age, gender, and education with potential acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine: A country-level analysis The COVID-19 Vaccine Communication Handbook. A practical guide for improving vaccine communication and fighting misinformation Politicizing public health: the powder keg of rushing COVID-19 vaccines What have you HEARD about the HERD?'Does education about local influenza vaccination coverage and herd immunity affect willingness to vaccinate? Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants Determinants of COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in the US Determinants of intention to get vaccinated against COVID-19 among healthcare personnel in hospitals in Greece Community-level evidence for SARS-CoV-2 vaccine protection of unvaccinated individuals Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom Mistrust in biomedical research and vaccine hesitancy: the forefront challenge in the battle against COVID-19 in Predictors of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK household longitudinal study Will COVID-19-related economic worries superimpose health worries, reducing nonpharmaceutical intervention acceptance in Germany? A prospective pre-registered study COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in a representative working-age population in France: a survey experiment based on vaccine characteristics Examining Australian public perceptions and behaviors towards a future COVID-19 vaccine The brief resilience scale: assessing the ability to bounce back Factors associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy A dangerous rush for vaccines It just doesn't feel right-the relevance of emotions and intuition for parental vaccine conspiracy beliefs and vaccination uptake Myths and conspiracy theories on vaccines and COVID-19: Potential effect on global vaccine refusals Psychological inoculation against fake news Inoculating Against Fake News About COVID-19 Individual and social determinants of COVID-19 vaccine uptake How to fight an infodemic ECDC. Questions and Answers on COVID-19 Vaccines for COVID-19 Global attitudes towards a COVID-19 vaccine as of YouGov-Covid-19-beavior-Tracker_20210301.pdf, last accessed on COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy could lead to thousands of extra deaths COVID-19 Vaccine Monitor Governor Murphy Announces New Incentives to Encourage Including Free Entrance to State Parks and Free Wine at Participating Wineries Joints for Jabs" Promotions to Support Vaccinations COVID-19 UPDATE: Gov. Justice provides additional details on vaccine lottery giveaway WHO. Survey tool and guidance: behavioural insights on COVID-19 Marta Caserotti: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Roles/Writing -original draft and Writing -review & editing Conceptualization, Roles/Writing -original draft and Writing -review & editing Paolo Girardi: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Roles/Writing -original draft and Writing -review & editing Conceptualization, Roles/Writing -original draft and Writing -review & editing Chiara Buizza: Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing -review & editing Valentina Candini: Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing -review & editing Cristina Zarbo: Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing -review & editing Flavia Chiarotti: Writing -review & editing Sonia Brescianini: Writing -review & editing Gemma Calamandrei: Supervision, Writing -review & editing Fabrizio Starace: Supervision, Writing -review & editing Giovanni de Girolamo: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing -review & editing Lorella Lotto: Conceptualization , Supervision, Roles/Writing -original draft and Writingreview & editing