key: cord-1038529-wt6mo481 authors: Thaker, J. title: The Persistence of Vaccine Hesitancy: COVID-19 Vaccination Intention date: 2020-12-18 journal: nan DOI: 10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 sha: 01d0303dc3f4718281522b885e3bcf6966763308 doc_id: 1038529 cord_uid: wt6mo481 Building public trust and willingness to vaccinate against COVID-19 is as important as developing an effective vaccine. However, a significant minority of the public are unwilling or hesitant to take a COVID-19 vaccine, when available. A nationally representative sample survey (N=1040) was conducted in July 2020 in New Zealand to identify factors associated with COVID-19 vaccine intention. Trust in experts and general vaccine hesitancy were significantly associated with COVID-19 vaccine intention. A communication campaign from trusted scientific experts, with information that addresses prevailing concerns about vaccines, is likely to help increase COVID-19 vaccine uptake. Vaccine hesitancy is one of the 10 most important health threats in the world today (World Health Organization, 2019) . Vaccine hesitancy can be defined as reluctance or refusal to vaccinate despite its accessibility (MacDonald, 2015) . Public health campaigns must address vaccine hesitancy long before actual refusal takes place, thereby increasing public confidence and inoculating the public against misinformation about vaccines. While most studies on vaccine hesitancy focus on subpopulations such as parents and caregivers or specific vaccines (e.g., HPV), a generalized vaccine hesitancy scale will better help understand the public unmet need for health information (Larson et al., 2015; Luyten et al., 2019) . Previous attempts to develop a generalized vaccine hesitancy scale have been limited with only one study clarifying the dimensions of vaccine hesitancy among general public (Luyten et al., 2019) . The current study examined factors associated with the intention to get a COVID-19 vaccine using data from a national sample survey of adults in New Zealand. Given the novelty of the disease, apart from demographic correlates, other factors tested in the study are vaccine hesitancy (Luyten et al., 2019; Shapiro et al., 2018) , trust in scientists (Cadeddu et al., 2020; de Figueiredo et al., 2020; Justwan et al., 2019; Larson et al., 2018; Palamenghi et al., 2020) , and knowledge about COVID-19 origin, symptoms, spread, and preventive behaviours (Reiter et al., 2020) . Trust in scientific experts is central in several science communication and health communication studies (e.g., Hmielowski et al., 2013; Malka et al., 2009; Poortinga & Pidgeon, 2003; Siegrist et al., 2005; Slovic, 1993) as information from trusted sources reduce cognitive effort and provides mental short-cuts to help individuals make decisions, particularly about highly uncertain issues, such as a new vaccine. Empirical evidence on the role of knowledge in shaping public acceptance of vaccines is mixed (Motta et al., 2018) , therefore essential to study in the context of potential new vaccines. Findings of this study can help guild theory and practice of public health campaigns for COVID-19 vaccination uptake. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101 https://doi.org/10. /2020 A nationally representative sample survey of the New Zealand adults was conducted between June 26 to July 13, 2020, after the country was briefly at the Alert Level 1, with fewer restrictions. The web-based survey was fielded by Qualtrics, an international survey agency. A stratified random sampling plan was followed to match the official Census estimates. about public opinion about current issues facing the country and the world. Following recent surveys (O'Keefe, 2020; Reiter et al., 2020) , respondents were asked, "If a coronavirus vaccine were available soon, what would you do? -I will get vaccinated against the coronavirus when one becomes available." Response options included "yes" and "no." Eleven questions measured dichotomously (True or False), were used to compute knowledge about COVID-19. These questions started with a prompt, "To the best of your knowledge, which of the following statements true or false." All scientifically accurate statements were coded 1 compared to 0. Eight statements were scientifically accurate relating to symptoms (dry cough (True, 1, 87%), fever (93%)), spread ("the coronavirus can be spread by people who do not show symptoms," Yes, 1, 93%), protection (frequent hand washing, 97%), avoiding large gatherings (92%), 6-feet distance (74%), and cure ("there is currently no cure for the coronavirus, 81%). Five statements were scientifically inaccurate, including on impact on All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10. 1101 /2020 elderly ("only elderly people get infected," False, coded as 1, 94%), and protection ("Hydroxychloroquine can prevent or kill coronavirus," False, 1, 84%; "antibiotics can prevent or kill the coronavirus," False, 1, 84%; "exposure to sun or extreme heat can prevent or kill the coronavirus," False, 1, 76%). Similarly, the respondents who correctly identified conspiracy theories as false were coded as 1 ("5G towers are spreading coronavirus," False, 1, 93%; "Bill Gates may have created the coronavirus to profit," False, 1, 90%; "coronavirus was created in a lab," False, 1, 66%). The scientifically accurate answers were summed to create an index of knowledge about COVID-19 (M = 9.58, SD = 1.56; KR-20 (Kuder-Richardson Formula 20) = .58). Following previous studies that validated vaccine hesitancy scale (Larson et al., 2018; Luyten et al., 2019; Shapiro et al., 2018) , attitudes towards vaccination was measured using 14 items using a 5-point scale, from strongly disagree to strongly agree with neither agree nor disagree serving as a mid-point. The questions followed a prompt, "Regarding vaccines, how much do you agree or disagree with following statements." These items include previously validated 9-item Vaccine Hesitancy Scale (Luyten et al., 2019) : one-item, "I do not need vaccines for diseases that are not common anymore," was not used as two recent studies found the item to be unreliable (Luyten et al., 2019; Shapiro et al., 2018) . In addition, six new items sourced from systematic reviews about vaccine hesitancy were tested: "Vaccines cause diseases" (Gidengil et al., 2019) , "Government over hypes the need for vaccines," (Gidengil et al., 2019) , "Corporations manufacturing vaccines only care for profit," (Gidengil et al., 2019) , "I am uncomfortable getting a vaccine that was rushed into production" (Hoffman, 2020; Seale et al., 2010) , and "I feel uncomfortable getting vaccinated" (Maisonneuve et al., 2018) . All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 doi: medRxiv preprint Trust in scientific exerts was measured using four items starting with a prompt, "How much do you trust or distrust the following organizations or people as a source of accurate information?" Using a 5-point response scale, with strongly distrust to strongly trust, respondents were asked how much they trust in (1) The survey assessed a range of demographic variables (see Table 1 ). The data was weighted, post-survey, on gender, age, education, and ethnicity to match the New Zealand census estimates. Weights ranged from 0.47 to 3.30, with a mean of 1.21, median of 0.98, and a standard deviation of 0.63. 95% of the weights fall between 0.58 and 2.15. To assess the dimensions of the 14-item vaccine hesitancy scale, the dataset was randomly and evenly split to test and validate the scale. Relative risk regression models with robust standard errors (Zou, 2004) were used to identify correlates of COVID-19 vaccine intension, as they are more intuitive than odds ratios and is a common method in public health research. Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS version 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Two-thirds (74%) of the respondents said they intend to get vaccinated against COVID-19 when one becomes available and a quarter (26%) said they do not intend to do so. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 doi: medRxiv preprint The sample was randomly split into half to test the dimensions of vaccine hesitancy scale. Exploratory factor analysis for the 14-items with the first half of the sample indicated a two factor solution, similar to prior studies (Luyten et al., 2019; Shapiro et al., 2018 ) (see Table 2 ). Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was .93, above the commonly recommended value of .6, and Bartlett's test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (91) = 5224.38, p < .001). These two factors were labelled as "lack of confidence" factor, accounting for 49.65% of the variance, and "risks" factor, accounting for 17.49% variance, based on previous studies (Luyten et al., 2019; Shapiro et al., 2018) . These two factors accounted for 67.15% of the common variance of the scale. Confirmatory factor analysis assessed the vaccine hesitancy scale using items from the other half of the sample. The best goodness-of-fit for the 14-item version was with two factors (RMSEA = 0.078, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.94). Thus, a two-factor structure, consisting of a "lack of confidence" (r's = 0.62 to 0.82, α = .95) part with 7 items, and a "risks perception" (r's = 0.28 to 0.63, α = .82) part with 7 items, showed the best psychometric characteristics of the VHS. On a scale from 1 to 5, with a higher score indicating higher vaccine hesitancy, the average respondent scored 1.97 (SD = 0.98) for the lack of confidence factor and 2.89 (SD = 0.93) for risks, highlighting that the hesitancy was driven more by risk perceptions than by lack of confidence in vaccines. Several demographic variables were correlated with willingness to get vaccinated against COVID-19 in bivariate analyses (See Table 3 ). Men compared to women, older respondents compared to younger, more educated compared to less educated, higher annual income compared to lower-income, Asian and other ethnicities compared to Māori, respondents without children compared to those with children, and those who did not smoke or vape compared to All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 doi: medRxiv preprint those who do, were more likely to say they intend to get vaccinated against COVID-19 when one becomes available. In multivariable analyses (See Table 4 ), men compared to women (RR = 1.11, 95% CI: 1.05-1.18) and those without children compared to those with children (RR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.02-1.16) were more willing to get a COVID-19 vaccine. Participants were also more willing if they reported higher levels of trust in scientific experts (RR = 1.10, 95% CI: 1.04-1.16). Moreover, with respect to general vaccine hesitancy attitudes, respondents with higher confidence about vaccines (RR = 0.77, 95% CI: 0.73-0.83) and those who perceived fewer risks (RR = 0.91, 95% CI: 0.87-0.96) were more willing to get a COVID-19 vaccine. There was no significant association between COVID-19 knowledge and intention to get vaccinated against COVID-19. A national sample survey in New Zealand shows that 74% of adults intend to get a COVID-19 vaccine when one becomes available. This is similar to findings from global surveys The most novel finding of this study is that a generalized measure of vaccine hesitancy is significantly associated with COVID-19 vaccine intention. The vaccine hesitancy scale with 14items was developed following the 9-item VHS scale for the general population (Luyten et al., 2019) , with additional items sourced from systemic reviews of vaccine hesitancy (Gidengil et al., 2019; Larson et al., 2015 Larson et al., , 2018 Maisonneuve et al., 2018; Verger & Dubé, 2020) . In probably the first study of its kind, vaccine hesitancy scale was tested in New Zealand, adding to few studies of vaccine hesitancy outside Europe and the US (Larson et al., 2018) . For example, (Seale et al., 2010) found that the perception of rushed vaccine production was one of the most cited fears for the H1N1 vaccine intention. This measure is particularly important given that All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 doi: medRxiv preprint public perceptions of a rushed vaccine development or inadequate testing may lead them to question the safety of the vaccine and increase distrust with vaccination in general (Hoffman, 2020) . Findings of this study can help standardize the VHS scale to advance research on vaccine hesitancy and its enduring impact on public attitudes towards new vaccines. Knowledge about COVID-19 was not associated with COVID-19 vaccine intention. This finding is worrying as another study in the US also shows that knowledge about COVID-19 infection was only moderately associated with COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in bivariate analysis and was not significantly associated in the presence of other covariates (Reiter et al., 2020) . Knowledge about a disease origin and prevalence alone may be an insufficient motivator for individual action, indicating a need for future studies to replicate these findings. Trust in scientific experts is one of the strongest correlates of COVID-19 vaccine intention, similar to previous studies on general vaccination behaviors (e.g., de Figueiredo et al., 2020) . Individuals rely on trusted sources of information to make judgments in uncertain information context, such as the rapid, multinational effort to develop a COVID-19 vaccine. Building public trust in scientific experts, particularly among historically disenfranchised communities who may already harbor high distrust with government and scientific elite, by building community relationships is particularly important to decrease the vaccination gap among the most vulnerable groups. The strengths of this study include a large representative sample, multiple items to measure and test vaccine hesitancy, trust in scientific experts, and knowledge about COVID-19. A primary limitation is the cross-sectional design of the study. Attitudes towards vaccine are likely to shift, as seen in other countries (e.g., Rhodes et al., 2020) , therefore a need to further monitor shifting public attitudes and correlates affecting this shift. Future research should also explore other dimensions of vaccine hesitancy, such as the desire for autonomy and morality concerns (Gidengil et al., 2019) . All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 doi: medRxiv preprint The World Health Organization identifies vaccine hesitancy as one of the 10 most important health threats to the world (World Health Organization, 2019). Increasing public enthusiasm for vaccination should co-occur with the development of a COVID-19 vaccine. Findings of this study suggest that a health communication campaign from trusted sources, with information that addresses prevailing concerns about vaccines, is likely to help increase COVID-19 vaccine uptake. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.16.20248139 doi: medRxiv preprint Tables Table 1 Demographic preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101 https://doi.org/10. /2020 Note: Items were coded on a 5-point scale, from strongly agree to strongly disagree-higher scores indicate higher vaccine hesitancy. EFC refers to exploratory factor analysis; CFA refers to confirmatory factor analysis; (R) refers to recoded items; VHS refers to Vaccine Hesitancy Scale (e.g., Luyten et al., 2019) . For EFA, factors were extracted using Principal Component Analysis with varimax rotation (with Kaiser Normalization). All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. perpetuity. preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10. 1101 /2020 preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10. 1101 /2020 preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted medRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for this this version posted December 18, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10. 1101 /2020 The soul of a polarized democracy: Testing theoretical linkages between talk and attitude extremity during the 2004 presidential election Beliefs towards vaccination and trust in the scientific community in Italy Mapping global trends in vaccine confidence and investigating barriers to vaccine uptake: A largescale retrospective temporal modelling study Intention to participate in a COVID-19 vaccine clinical trial and to get vaccinated against COVID-19 in France during the pandemic When a COVID-19 vaccine is ready, will we all be ready for it? Beliefs around childhood vaccines in the United States: A systematic review An attack on science? Media use, trust in scientists, and perceptions of global warming Mistrust of a coronavirus vaccine could imperil widespread immunity. The New York Times The effect of trust and proximity on vaccine propensity Measuring trust in vaccination: A systematic review Measuring vaccine hesitancy: The development of a survey tool Assessing vaccine hesitancy in the UK population using a generalized vaccine hesitancy survey instrument Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants Educating children and adolescents about vaccines: A review of current literature The association of knowledge with concern about global warming: Trusted information sources shape public thinking. Risk Analysis, the pandemic (H1N1) 2009 vaccine The vaccine hesitancy scale: Psychometric properties and validation Perception of risk: The influence of general trust, and general confidence Perceived risk, trust, and democracy Restoring confidence in vaccines in the COVID-19 era Ten health issues WHO will tackle this year New Zealand takes early and hard action to tackle COVID-19 Follow the heart or the mind? Examining cognitive and affective attitude on HPV vaccination intention Attitudes to vaccination: A critical review