key: cord-1036937-slg2kq9i authors: Weber, Jan; Klein, Timo; Abeln, Vera title: Shifts in broadband power and alpha peak frequency observed during long-term isolation date: 2020-10-22 journal: Sci Rep DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-75127-0 sha: a396ad4e042bb076f1ec6f78c307579854d69fc3 doc_id: 1036937 cord_uid: slg2kq9i Prolonged periods of social isolation and spatial confinement do not only represent an issue that needs to be faced by a few astronauts during space missions, but can affect all of us as recently shown during pandemic situations. The fundamental question, how the brain adapts to periods of sensory deprivation and re-adapts to normality, has only received little attention. Here, we use eyes closed and eyes open resting-state electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings to investigate how neural activity is altered during 120 days of isolation in a spatially confined, space-analogue environment. After disentangling oscillatory patterns from 1/f activity, we show that isolation leads to a reduction in broadband power and a flattening of the 1/f spectral slope. Beyond that, we observed a reduction in alpha peak frequency during isolation, but did not find strong evidence for isolation-induced changes that are of oscillatory nature. Critically, all effects reversed upon release from isolation. These findings suggest that isolation and concomitant sensory deprivation lead to an enhanced cortical deactivation which might be explained by a reduction in the mean neuronal population firing rate. There is a strong consensus that oscillatory activity driven through cell assembly synchronization modulates a variety of behavioral and cognitive states, such as attention 1 , memory 2 and sleep 3 . A vast body of literature has demonstrated the existence of several oscillatory frequency bands, ranging from approximately 0.05 Hz to 150 Hz (up to 500 Hz in animals) 4 that have been associated with a diversity of complex cognitive operations. Furthermore, these narrowband oscillations, commonly referred to as delta (1) (2) (3) (4) , theta (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) , alpha (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) , beta (12) (13) (14) (15) (16) (17) (18) (19) (20) (21) (22) (23) (24) (25) (26) (27) (28) (29) (30) and gamma (> 30 Hz) 5 , have widely been studied during resting-state settings to better understand network dynamics. Multiple studies have also been employed in patient populations 6, 7 as resting-state narrowband oscillations have been shown to correlate with cognitive functions 8, 9 . Therefore, restingstate measurements are extensively used to derive objective biomarkers that might be used in clinical settings 5 . However, a ubiquitous property of such narrowband activity is its embedding within non-oscillatory, scalefree neural activity 10 (also termed fractal component, 1/f electrophysiological noise etc.) that has until very recently just been considered as pure noise without any deeper functional meaning. Recent studies, however, revised this view and were able to demonstrate that scale-free activity might instead constitute an important behavioral and physiological index [10] [11] [12] [13] . The 1/f component of the aperiodic signal itself is determined by the exponential decay (spectral slope) and the total offset (y-intercept) of the spectrum both of which have been shown to fluctuate with cognitive and physiological states. The spectral slope is assumed to reflect the ratio between excitation and inhibition at the synaptic level with more negative slopes reflecting enhanced inhibition 11, 14 . In contrast, the spectral offset has been shown to reliably index the mean population-averaged firing rate of single units as revealed through intracranial EEG recordings 15 as well as computational models 14 . Thus, both parameters might have the potential and robustness to serve as a proxy for neuronal population activity when only macroscopic measures are available, but information about the current cortical state wants to be inferred. It has, for example, been shown that the spectral slope reliably differentiates between different cortical states, such as sleep and wakefulness with more negative slopes during sleep indexing enhanced inhibition 16 . Another recent study revealed that medication naïve children suffering from an Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have greater offsets and steeper slopes as compared to age-matched controls. These broadband power shifts have been proposed to be a signature of asynchronously arriving input to the dendrites of pyramidal An international crew of three women and three men (aged 33.67 ± 6.41 years (mean ± SD) participated in the SIRIUS-19 (Scientific International Research in Unique Terrestrial Station 2019) isolation mission and were isolated for 120 days under space analogue conditions. We recorded EEG activity at 32 scalp sites on 6 different timepoints: The first assessment day was 13 days prior to the start of the isolation period (Pre-Isolation). The second, third, fourth and fifth measurement were taken at isolation day 15, 54, 79 & 110, respectively. The last assessment was taken 7 days after participants were released from isolation (Post-Isolation). EEG was always recorded in the same settings (stationary set up in a separate laboratory compartment, noise was kept at a minimum and participants wore noise cancelling ear plugs during recordings), but recording times randomly varied between 8am and 6pm due to the complexity of international space analogue isolation missions simulating a space flight to the moon. To ascertain that the fluctuations in daytime at which we recorded EEG did not introduce any spurious effects, we performed several control analyses (see Supplementary Fig. S14 ). By taking recordings at multiple stages during isolation, we (1) aimed to examine if the aforementioned parameters of interest (broadband power, spectral slope and alpha peak frequency) are progressively altered as a function of time spent in isolation and (2) to ascertain that any observed change during isolation is constant across multiple stages during isolation and does not reflect a spurious fluctuation. However, as we did not observe a parametric modulation of any parameter throughout isolation (e.g. a progressive reduction of broadband power from isolation day 15 to isolation day 110), we sought to average across all isolation timepoints. This substantially reduced our degrees of freedom for follow-up post-hoc comparisons. With 6 timepoints representing 6 levels of our main factor time, we would need to perform 15 follow-up comparisons. When averaging across all isolation timepoints, the factor levels reduce from 6 to 3 and the post-hoc comparisons from 15 to 3. It is important to note, however, that the inference that can be drawn from the study does not differ if we average across isolation timepoints or treat them all separately. This can be appreciated in the Supplementary Material in Tables S1 to S5 where all possible post-hoc results for each analysis are listed. Sensory deprivation reduces aperiodic spectral power offset. In a first step we sought to test whether sensory deprivation as induced by exposure to a space-analogue environment alters the broadband power spectrum as mainly determined by the aperiodic spectral offset. We mainly predicted that broadband power would be reduced as a consequence of reduced sensory input to the brain. The grand average aperiodic power spectral density (PSD) estimates for both conditions (eyes open and eyes closed) are shown in Fig. 1 . The average number of trials was 74.47 ± 3.07 and 76.42 ± 5.18 (mean ± SD) for eyes closed and eyes open, respectively. Note that for all subsequent post-hoc analyses (spectral offset, spectral slope, alpha peak frequency, periodic PSD), we always used the average over all data obtained during isolation and refer to this as Iso. (Isolation), if not stated otherwise. In order to test whether the aperiodic spectral offset was modulated across all testing sessions, we applied a within-subject repeated measures ANOVA (implemented in Fieldtrip with ft_statfun_depsamplesFunivariate), separately for eyes closed and eyes open conditions. Both conditions revealed a significant change in spectral offset over time (cluster permutation test eyes closed: sum(F) = 37.27, p < 0.01; eyes open: sum(F) = 24.74, p < 0.05). Follow-up pairwise comparisons (ft_statfun_depsamplesT) revealed a significant decrease in spectral offset upon exposure to isolation. This effect was again manifested in both resting-state conditions and was spatially extended over a broad range of electrodes (cluster permutation eyes closed: sum(t) = − 41.74, p < 0.001, d isolation − pre-isolation = − 2.12, 11 significant channels; eyes open: sum(t) = − 29.39, p < 0.001, d isolation − pre-isolation = − 1.88, 7 significant channels, see Fig. 2A,B) . Due to the limited number of subjects that are allowed to participate in these particular studies, we sought to further investigate the effects on a single-subject level using a cluster-based trial-shuffling procedure. Remarkably, we found robust spectral offset reductions upon exposure to isolation that were highly significant for all Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:17987 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75127-0 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ 6 subjects in both resting state conditions. The spatial distribution of this effect was again global (see Fig. 3 for eyes closed & Supplementary Fig. S8 for eyes open). Importantly, we observed the reversal effect in terms of spectral offset increases in both conditions once subjects were released from the space-analogue environment (cluster permutation eyes closed: sum(t) = 6.48, p < 0.05, d post-isolation − isolation = 1.9, 2 significant channels; eyes open: sum(t) = 10.56, p < 0.05, d post-isolation − isolation = 0.95, 3 significant channels, see Fig. 2C ,D). We again examined this effect on the single-subject level and found significantly increased offsets after release from isolation in 5 out of 6 subjects in both resting-state conditions (see Supplementary Figs . S5, S9 for eyes closed and eyes open, respectively). In a final contrast, we compared the spectral offset between pre-& post-isolation, but did not find significant differences. These results indicate that sensory deprivation during isolation leads to a reduction in broadband power (here between 2 and 45 Hz) indicating an enhanced cortical deactivation. Crucially, the brain re-adapted upon release from isolation in terms of an increase in broadband power. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Isolation-induced changes in aperiodic spectral slope. Next, we investigated whether sensory deprivation and its subsequent release modulated the aperiodic spectral slope as this has been shown to be an index for the synaptic excitatory/inhibitory balance 11, 12 with more negative slopes indexing enhanced inhibition. We essentially predicted a more negative slope during isolation as a proxy for increased cortical inhibition as a consequence of sensory deprivation. We again employed a within-subject repeated measures ANOVA, separately for both resting-state conditions, to test for a main effect of time over all sessions. However, we did not observe a significant effect over sessions (no resultant cluster). Although one should be cautious in interpreting post-hoc effects when the main effect was not significant, we performed follow up post-hoc pairwise comparisons. A group-level, non-parametric permutation approach revealed a significant increase in spectral slope from pre-to within-isolation. This effect, however, could only be observed during eyes closed resting state (sum(t) = 8.88, p < 0.05, d isolation − pre-isolation = 1.56, 3 significant channels clustered at occipital electrodes; eyes open: no resultant cluster, see Fig. 4A ,B). Although analyses on the group level are substantial for making conclusive inference, single-subject analyses are of equal importance in regards to the search of potential neurophysiological screening parameters for astronauts in space. Therefore, we again tested this effect on the single-subject domain. We found evidence for an increase in spectral slope in 4/6 subjects whereas in 2/6 we were facing the opposite pattern (in both resting-state conditions, see Supplementary Figs . S6, S10). Subsequently, we tested whether this effect reverses from within-to post-isolation. We only found evidence for a significant decrease in spectral slope after isolation for the eyes open condition (sum(t) = − 20.31, p < 0.001, d post-isolation − isolation = − 2.13, 6 significant channels clustered at right-frontal electrodes), but not for the eyes closed condition (Fig. 4C,D) . On the singlesubject level, we observed spectral slope decreases post-isolation in 4/6 subjects during eyes closed and in 6/6 subjects during eyes open condition (see Supplementary Material Figs. S7, S11). In order to test if the two main aperiodic features, spectral slope and offset are related to one another, we performed an exploratory correlation analysis and found a negative correlation between both metrics (eyes been proposed to reflect the activation level of neuronal populations with higher APF mirroring a more active and lower APF mirroring a more inactive cortical state 19 . Therefore, we predicted that APF would slow down during isolation as a consequence of reduced sensory input and increase after release from isolation as a result of re-adjustment to augmented sensory input. To test this hypothesis, we performed a detection of APF using the local maxima between 8 and 12 Hz. We performed a channel-wise peak detection (see "Methods" on how peak detection was accomplished) for the eyes closed condition as alpha peaks could be reliably estimated on a channel basis. On average only 1.06 ± 2.08 (mean ± SD) channels did not show a reliable peak. If this was the case, we interpolated the channel's APF based on its neighbors (see "Methods" on how neighbors were defined). In contrast, a channel-averaged peak detection was performed for the eyes open condition as many channels did not show a reliable peak between 8 and 12 Hz. We used a non-parametric, cluster-based permutation approach to test for APF modulations during the eyes closed condition, whereas we employed a repeatedmeasures ANOVA for the eyes open condition as no inference on the spatial extent of the effect could have been made (due to the channel-averaged approach that was used Fig. 5C ]). We again examined this effect on the single-subject level for the eyes closed condition as peak detection was reliable on a single trial level as compared to the eyes open condition (see Supplementary Fig. S3A for examples of single-trial APF estimates). Using an adaptive power threshold approach (see Methods), we discarded on average 13.99 ± 15.22% (mean ± SD) of all trials. This led to a remaining number of 60.54 ± 9.47 trials (mean ± SD) that was used for the statistical within-subject APF analysis. 4/6 subjects displayed increases in APF from within-to post-isolation whereas 1 subject displayed a www.nature.com/scientificreports/ decrease and 1 subject did not show any alteration (Fig. 6) . We did not find strong evidence for a modulation of APF upon exposure to isolation (cluster permutation eyes closed: , although it should be noted that there was a trend towards a decreased APF. On a single-subject level for the eyes closed condition, we observed a significant decrease in APF for 3/6 subjects. 1 subject displayed the reversed pattern and 2 subjects did not show any alteration in APF. Interestingly, we observed a significant increase in APF from pre-to postisolation for the eyes open condition (t(5) = − 2.36, p < 0.05, d post-isolation − pre-isolation = 1.97, see Fig. 5D ). No such effect was found for the eyes closed condition (no resultant cluster). The oscillatory PSDs between 7 and 14 Hz for both resting state conditions are visualized in Fig. 5E ,F. Since both APF and spectral offset have been proposed as a proxy of the activation level of neuronal populations, we performed an exploratory analysis to test our prediction that both parameters should be positively correlated. We thus correlated the channel-averaged APF and spectral offset. A one-sided spearman correlation indeed revealed a significant positive correlation between both parameters for the eyes closed condition (r(28) = 0.44, p < 0.01, CI = [0.09 0.69], see Fig. 7A ). It should be noted that for this correlation analysis, 6 data samples were excluded as they were clearly clustered away from the main data cloud and led to a non-normal distribution of the APF data. However, the inference of the correlation analysis does not change when including these data samples (Spearman correlation: r(34) = 0.29, p < 0.05, CI = [− 0.05 0.56], see Supplementary Fig . S13 for further explanation on why we excluded these data samples). A non-significant, but considerable trend towards No isolation-induced changes in rhythmic oscillatory activity. So far, we have demonstrated that both spectral offset and slope are significantly modulated during isolation and that APF is mainly modulated after release from isolation. In a final step, we sought to investigate overall changes in oscillatory frequency bands. We therefore subtracted the aperiodic spectral estimates that were obtained using IRASA from the spectral estimates that were obtained by regular spectral analysis, finally leading to a PSD matrix that is mostly driven by oscillatory activity and less confounded by the 1/f power law (see Supplementary Material Fig. S2 for visualization of the periodic PSD). A within-subject repeated measures ANOVA did not reveal a significant effect for the eyes closed condition (no resultant cluster). Further, upon exploratory post-hoc tests comparing pre-isolation, isolation and post-isolation separately, we did not find any change in oscillatory brain activity. However, we observed a significant modulation of oscillatory brain activity over time for the eyes open condition (sum(F) = 389.04, p < 0.01). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a significant increase in the alpha frequency range from pre-to post-isolation (sum(t) = 32.95, p < 0.001, 13 significant channels, see Fig. 7B ). A similar, yet not significant pattern, could be observed when contrasting the periodic PSD during isolation and post-isolation, such that alpha power was increased post-isolation (see Fig. 7C ). It should, however, be noted that by careful inspection of the periodic PSD, it becomes apparent that the change in higher alpha frequency power is mainly due to the shift in the APF, although the amplitude itself is also increased as depicted in the Supplementary Material Fig. S2B right panel. The large majority of studies has particularly emphasized the role of narrowband oscillations in neuronal communication, cognition and behavior [21] [22] [23] [24] . However, in addition to narrowband oscillations, the 1/f component of the electrophysiological power spectrum has been shown to equally index important physiological features 13, 15, 25 . Here, we provide first unique insights into how long-term isolation alters the electrophysiological power spectrum. We show that long-term isolation (120 days), where sensory input is at a minimum state, does not introduce strong changes in terms of narrowband (de)synchronization, but instead leads to broadband power shifts, spectral tilts as well as shifts in alpha peak frequency (APF). Broadband power and APF are reduced during long-term isolation. Broadband power was estimated after subtraction of band-limited oscillations using irregular resampling 20 (IRASA) and refers to the intercept of the corrected, aperiodic-like power spectrum. We observed a reduction in broadband power (2-45 Hz) upon exposure to isolation. A rebound effect, in terms of increased broadband power, was observed upon release from isolation. Single-subject analyses using a cluster-based trial-shuffling procedure further substantiated the robustness of this observation as it was evident and consistent across all subjects in both resting-state conditions. Previous research has tried to examine the association between broadband spectral changes and activation levels of neuronal populations. One example is a study by Manning, et al. 15 where they simultaneously recorded LFP and single-unit activity in neurosurgical patients and demonstrated that broadband LFP power across all frequencies reliably tracked single neuron activity in widespread subcortical and cortical areas. This observation has been further substantiated by Ray and Maunsell 26 who showed a robust relationship between broadband gamma and spiking activity in macaque visual cortex. Furthermore, broadband power shifts in fusiform and parahippocampal gyri reliably classified the visual presentation of either faces or houses with high temporal precision indicating that broadband power closely tracks the activation level at local cortical circuits 17 . Even though EEG and LFP recordings both reflect the superposition of multiple transmembrane currents in the brain (e.g. synaptic activity, action potentials etc.), they do represent underlying activity at fairly different spatiotemporal scales where activity recorded at a single EEG electrode reflects a spatiotemporally smoothed version of the LFP 27,28 Therefore, we cannot unambiguously extrapolate our findings to the aforementioned relation of broadband power to single unit activity. However, it is plausible to argue that periods of sensory deprivation and spatial confinement during long-term isolation reduce the level of vigilance and arousal which in turn might Figure 6 . Single-subject change in Alpha Peak Frequency (APF) after removal of the aperiodic signal using IRASA. Bar plots show the mean APF during isolation (red) and post-isolation (green). Error bars represent the standard deviation over trials. The topographical t-value distribution (given by random trial shuffling between the two conditions, see methods for details on single-subject cluster permutation analysis) obtained via cluster permutation is plotted for each condition difference. Black dots represent significant sensors that are part of a negative cluster (here: APF increased during isolation as compared to post-isolation). Gray dots represent significant sensors that are part of a positive cluster (here: APF reduced during isolation as compared to postisolation). Individual subjects are denoted as Subject A, B, C, D, E, F. Note that APF increased in 4/6 subjects after isolation whereas 1 subject showed the opposite pattern and in 1 subject APF was not altered. Please find the figure for single-subject comparison between pre-isolation and isolation for APF in the Supplementary Fig. S11 . *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ns not significant. Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:17987 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75127-0 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ lead to an enhanced cortical deactivation. This notion can be further accentuated when taking the global, yet not significant (p = 0.09), reduction in APF during and its subsequent (significant) increase after isolation into consideration. The interpretation of the reduction in APF during isolation as a corollary of a reduced vigilance state can be further supported by a recent study 29 which revealed a systematic decrease of APF over the course of an experimental session (1 h). In addition, Samaha and Postle 30 measured the temporal resolution of visual perception by estimating the temporal threshold at which two successive light flashes could no longer be discriminated and correlated this behavioral measure with eyes-closed APF. Intriguingly, they were able to demonstrate that APF reliably tracked the temporal resolution of visual perception. That is, subjects with a higher APF had a finer temporal resolution of visual perception. Importantly, however, APF has been shown to be robust against bottom-up sensory fluctuations which suggests that APF is rather regulated in a top-down manner 31 . Our present results converge with this idea and suggest that the brain adaptively top-down regulates APF in dependence to sensory stimulation. Hence, as we show here, APF is down-regulated during isolation where sensory input is at a minimum level, but is again up-regulated post-isolation when sensory stimulation is back to normal. This interpretation is coherent with the assumption that the frequency at which active neural ensembles oscillate is self-regulated and can be described as a function of the input statistics (e.g. sensory input, task demands etc.) 19 . In order to further justify the claim that both broadband power and APF track cortical deactivation during long-term sensory deprivation, we correlated APF and broadband power, hypothesizing that if both reflect a proxy of neural de(activation), they should be positively correlated. We were indeed able to confirm our prediction by demonstrating a positive correlation between both parameters (significant for eyes closed, highly trending for eyes open). This reduction in cortical activation observed here during long-term isolation further raises the important question to what extent this effect might reflect a pathological condition. We argue that the broadband power decrease observed during isolation reflects a self-correcting mechanism where neuronal population activity is reduced as a consequence of sensory deprivation. However, in contrast to the broadband power increase observed in medication naïve young ADHD patients 13 which might indeed reflect a disturbed feedback mechanism, we do not think that the decrease within-and subsequent increase post-isolation are signs of pathological conditions, but rather reveal a homeostatic adaption to the brain in response to reduced needs of sensory integration. We recently observed a comparable homeostatic self-regulating process for cortisol secretion during short-term isolation with highly increased cortisol values during isolation that, however, returned back to baseline values post-isolation 32 . Flatter 1/f spectral slope during isolation. An additional parameter that characterizes the aperiodic power spectrum is its slope. Previous work has shown that the spectral slope reflects an index of the local balance between excitation and inhibition 11, 16, 33 . Thereby, steeper slopes index enhanced inhibition and vice versa. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Therefore, we essentially predicted that isolation would lead towards a steeper slope due to reduced excitation and relatively more inhibition as induced by a lack of sensory perturbation. However, our results point towards the opposite direction. We show that the slope increases upon exposure to isolation, however, only during the eyes closed condition. In contrast, we observed a steeper slope upon release from isolation that was significant for the eyes open condition, but the mean difference pointed towards the same direction for the eyes closed condition. This observation was also highly consistent across participants when single-subject analyses were performed. Although we expected the change in slope to point towards the opposite direction, the following scenario according to the neural noise hypothesis 12 can be derived: The neural noise hypothesis states that increases in asynchronous and periodically decoupled population spiking leads to a flattening of the power spectrum. This feature has been shown to increase as a function of age 12 and to mediate age-related working memory deteriorations. Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that increases in desynchronized neuronal spiking activity contributed to the flattening of the power spectrum during isolation. Potential mechanisms that drive spectral changes during long-term isolation. What are the potential mechanisms that drive such cortical deactivation (reduced broadband power and APF) and increases in neural noise (flatter spectral slope) during long-term isolation? Vigilance and arousal levels are known to be partially modulated by the reticular activation system (RAS) through diffuse neuromodulation 34 . Furthermore, stimulation of the RAS led to a disruption in narrowband oscillations and induced stronger desynchronized neuronal activity, prompting its potential role in regulating scale-free cortical activity 35 . This leads to the hypothesis that the ascending sensory input from the RAS to subcortical and cortical regions is reduced during periods of sensory deprivation and spatial confinement. However, further research is needed to directly assess this hypothesis. Whereas the general cortical activation state might be regulated by the ascending RAS, the change in neural noise, as measured by the slope of the aperiodic-like power spectrum, is likely to have different origins. Noradrenaline (NA) is thought to modulate sensory gain control mechanisms that regulate the balance between excitation and inhibition and determine the sensitivity of a behavioral response towards relevant sensory input 36, 37 . Thus, it would be reasonable to speculate that NA and the 1/f neural noise are interrelated. A recent study 38 could indeed provide evidence that NA as measured reliably by the pupil diameter 39 and the 1/f neural noise are correlated. Counterintuitively, however, they showed a positive correlation suggesting that higher levels of NA indicate a flatter power spectrum and stronger neural noise. To what extent NA is altered during long-term isolation should be focus of future investigations, but initial evidence suggests that levels of NA are highly reduced during social isolation as investigated in rats 40 . Over the past years, a considerable number of studies has been employed to aid our understanding on how prolonged periods of isolation impact physiological mechanisms in both human and animal models 18, 32, [41] [42] [43] . We here address how our present results converge with previous investigations. Early studies in animal models have shown that isolation modulates the behavioral response towards novel stimuli and induces locomotor hyperactivity 44 and leads depletions of NA in diverse brain regions including the hippocampus 45 . Furthermore, sensory deprivation as induced through isolation adversely affects cognition in animals 46 . Although the direct mechanisms underlying deterioration in cognitive performance has not been fully elucidated, it is tempting to speculate that NA might be a driving mechanism due to its important role in the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) to facilitate memory consolidation 47 . To what extent a reduction in NA release through decreased activation of the Locus Coeruleus (LC) can explain our present observations in regards to reductions in broadband power and a flattening of the aperiodic slope remains to be seen. However, it has been indeed demonstrated that LC stimulation leads to a stronger rhythmicity in spike timing and a more reliable response towards external stimuli 47 which would well align with the hypothesis that lower levels of NA decrease arousal and cortical activation. So far, the effect of isolation on human physiology has only been studied on a macroscopic level. However, our results align well with previous studies showing a reduction in global brain activity in the alpha and beta frequency during isolation 18, 42, 43 . Yet, it should be noted once more that in comparison to previous studies that only focused on changes in oscillatory activity over the course of isolation, we here applied an approach to disentangle oscillatory and non-oscillatory (aperiodic) features. Thereby, we showed that isolation induces a reduction in broadband power rather than changes in narrowband oscillations. Therefore, we suggest that future studies investigating changes in neural activity during isolation follow this approach to prevent potential conflation of changes in rhythmic and arrhythmic activity. In conclusion, we have demonstrated that both broadband power and APF are reduced during and subsequently increased after isolation. This suggests that long-term isolation reduces cortical activation levels. As previous studies have found a strong correlation between broadband power across all frequencies and single-unit activity, our findings let us to speculate that the mean population firing rate is reduced during long-term isolation. Furthermore, our data reveals a trend towards a flatter spectral slopes during isolation indicating more desynchronized spiking activity. In conjunction, our results are the first ones to show that isolation induces changes in (1) broadband EEG power, (2) alpha peak frequency and (3) 1/f neural noise. It is, however, important to note that, although isolation studies provide unique insights into how long-term isolation shapes cortical activity, we cannot simply extrapolate our findings onto effects of pandemic situations and concomitant isolation. This is in particular because the circumstances, such as financial security, general health (not being exposed to a risk situation through a virus) and the concern regarding the health status of other family members, remain stationary during isolation studies whereas they are uncertain and highly unpredictable during pandemic situations. We address three major limitations of the present study. (1) We did not include a control group with which potential interaction effects could have been studied to highlight that our observations were indeed caused by isolation. However, we were able to show in previous studies that a non-isolated control group did not show any alterations over time 32 . Thus, we believe that our results here are caused by sensory deprivation during long-term isolation. (2) Due to the high logistic expense and cost effort due to the nature of these particular studies that try to mimic a space-analogue conditions, the sample size is extremely low. However, we tried to statistically analyze the data in a way (non-parametric clusterbased permutations and single-subject analyses) that allows us inferring the conclusions that we finally made. (3) Some confounding factors, that might explain some variance in our results, such as mood, boredom or shifts in sleep cycles, cannot be entirely excluded. We do, however, believe that these factors are not orthogonal to the phenomenon of sensory deprivation, but rather describe psychological consequences of prolonged periods of monotony and sensory deprivation. Participants. Data acquisition timepoints. We recorded EEG activity at 32 scalp sites on 6 different timepoints: The first assessment day was 13 days prior to the start of the isolation period (Pre-Isolation). The second, third, fourth and fifth measurement were taken at isolation day 15, 54, 79 and 110, respectively. The last assessment was taken 7 days after participants were released from isolation (Post-Isolation). EEG was always recorded in the same settings (stationary set up in a separate laboratory compartment, noise was kept at a minimum and participants wore noise cancelling ear plugs during recordings), but recording times randomly varied between 8am and 6 pm due to the complexity of internal space analogue isolation missions simulating a space flight to the moon. To ascertain that the fluctuations in daytime at which we recorded EEG did not introduce any spurious effects, we performed several control analyses (see Supplementary Fig. S14 ). EEG data acquisition. EEG data was obtained during eyes closed and eyes open resting state conditions while subjects were seated in a relaxed position. Both conditions were continuously recorded for 2.5 min with a transition period of 30 s. EEG data was recorded at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz using an electrode cap Ag/AgCl active electrodes (Acti-Cap EEG Active Electrode System, Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) located at 32 scalp sites based on an international 10-20 system 48 . Preprocessing and statistical analysis of the data was carried out in MATLAB 2019a (MathWorks Inc.) using custom written code along with functions from the Fieldtrip toolbox 49 . EEG data analysis was performed separately for the eyes open and eyes closed condition. All analyses steps, however, were equal for both conditions. In order to prevent edge artifacts, we applied a bandpass-filter between 1 and 45 Hz to the continuous data. Data was then demeaned, detrended and re-referenced the data to an average reference before we separated the data into non-overlapping epochs of 2 s. Subsequently, data was visually inspected and trials containing visually prominent artifacts were rejected. Subsequently, we used an independent-component analysis (ICA) to remove eye-blinks, saccades or cardiac artefacts. We then extracted the variance of the signal channel-wise for each trial by averaging over channels. Trials were rejected if the variance exceeded the 1.5 times the interquartile range of the median variance. In a final step, we again visually inspected the signal for residual artifacts and rejected artifact-contaminated trials. We computed the spectral estimates by means of a Fast-Fourier Transformation after applying a single Hanning window to all artefact-free segments. Spectral estimates were obtained between 2 and 45 Hz in steps of 0.5 Hz on single trials. For later group-level analyses, we averaged the power spectrum along the trial dimension. We limited the analysis to 45 Hz to prevent confounds in later analyses on spectral slope and broadband power estimates due to line noise at 50 Hz. Besides, no further additional information was expected in the higher frequency ranges. To reliably estimate offset, slope and alpha peak frequency (APF) of Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:17987 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75127-0 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ the electrophysiological power spectrum, we used irregular resampling 20 (IRASA) to separate oscillatory components from broadband 1/f contribution. To extract purely oscillatory features from the signal, we subtracted the spectral estimates that were obtained using IRASA from the spectral estimates that were obtained by regular spectral analysis 50 . This procedure allowed us to estimate the APF by means of the purely oscillatory driven signal as well as spectral slope and broadband power (offset of the spectral signal) by means of the aperiodic 1/f signal that itself has been shown to encompass information about the physiological state 25 . Spectral slope and offset estimation. We estimated spectral slope and spectral offset on the aperiodic signal using IRASA. The key aperiodic features (slope and offset) were calculated by fitting a linear regression to the aperiodic signal in semi-log power space (polyfit.m, MATLAB and Curve Fitting Toolbox Release R2015a, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States) 13 . Fitting was performed channel-wise on the trialaveraged data for group-level analysis and on a trial-basis for single-subject analysis. Analysis of Alpha Peak Frequency. We calculated the Alpha Peak Frequency (APF) by means of the periodic signal that entails the residual oscillatory components of the signal. This approach facilitates the identification of a spectral peak and prevents misleading detection of a peak that is purely explained by the 1/f contribution to the signal 51, 52 . As a reminder, the purely oscillatory driven signal was obtained by subtracting the aperiodic 1/f contribution from the original power spectrum. We used a semi-automatic detection for APF detection. We predefined a range for APF between 8 and 12 Hz. The algorithm then extracted the APF that was defined as the local power maxima between 8 and 12 Hz. To finally determine whether the local maximum indeed reflected a peak, we used an adaptive power threshold procedure. Therefore, we first computed the power spectrum for each channel and derived its mean and standard deviation over the whole frequency range. We then multiplied the standard deviation by a factor of 3 and added the resulting product to the spectral mean. This computation finally resulted in a single number that predefined the threshold for a peak to be considered as robust and reliable for further statistical analysis. We visually inspected the power spectrum and confirmed the decision of the algorithm on each trial. There was, however, no need for manual adjustment. APF detection was performed channel-wise on the trial-averaged data for group-level analysis and on a trial-basis for single-subject analysis. Statistical analysis. We used cluster-based permutation tests to test for changes in spectral slope, offset and APF over time 53 . Since, to the best of our knowledge, no prior study has investigated the effects of isolation and sensory deprivation on spectral slope, offset or APF, no prior assumptions on the spatial extent of the effects were made. Therefore, we decided against testing the effects on a certain subset of electrodes, but to take advantage of the non-parametric cluster-based permutation approach that provides insights into the spatial extent of the effect while still correcting for the multiple comparison problem. Statistical effects were employed both on the group as well as on the single-subject level. We used exactly the same statistical approach for examining changes in spectral slope, offset and APF. Group-level analysis. In a first step, we employed a non-parametric, cluster-based within-subject repeated measures ANOVA (implemented in Fieldtrip with ft_statfun_depsamplesFunivariate) to assess the main effect of time using all timepoints (6 factor levels: pre-isolation, isolation day 15, 54, 79, 110, post-isolation). To ascertain that the main effect of time was still present when we averaged across all isolation timepoints (isolation day 15, 54, 79 and 110), we also employed the ANOVA on the isolation-averaged data (3 factor levels: pre-isolation, average isolation, post-isolation). This extra ANOVA was conducted as post-hoc tests were employed on the isolation-averaged data (see beginning of results section for a more detailed explanation). Follow-up pairwise comparisons were performed using cluster-based permutation tests. Permutation distributions for higher group level analysis were obtained by randomly shuffling the condition labels (timepoints of interest, e.g. pre-isolation vs. isolation). Due to the small sample size, we used all possible random partitions (here: 6 2 = 64) to derive the permutation distribution. For every random partition, we contrasted the two conditions of interest using dependent-sample t-tests on the channel-level. Spatial clusters were formed by thresholding at the t-statistic at a critical alpha of 0.05. Clusters were formed by two or more neighboring channels in space. The selection of neighbors was based on the triangulation method. The maximum t-statistic within a spatial cluster was calculated by summation of all t-values. Finally, the largest t-sum among all clusters was extracted. Repeating this step for the maximal number of possible permutations ensured a distribution of the maximum t-statistics. The p-value was then computed by calculating the proportion of random partitions that exceeded the observed (true) test statistic. The p-value was directly adjusted for two-tailed testing and thereby multiplied by a factor of two. Single-subject analysis. We followed the same rationale to test for effects on the single-subject level. However, instead of randomly shuffling the condition labels on the subject-level, we randomly permuted the labels on a trial-level. That is, the mean value of a single channel was derived by averaging across trials instead of averaging across subjects. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Cortical state and attention Oscillations and episodic memory: addressing the synchronization/desynchronization conundrum Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks EEG frequency bands in psychiatric disorders: a review of resting state studies Disturbed fluctuations of resting state EEG synchronization in Alzheimer's disease Resting state EEG abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders Relation between resting EEG to cognitive performance and clinical symptoms in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder Resting EEG theta power correlates with cognitive performance in healthy older adults The temporal structures and functional significance of scale-free brain activity Inferring synaptic excitation/inhibition balance from field potentials Age-related changes in 1/f neural electrophysiological noise EEG power spectral slope differs by ADHD status and stimulant medication exposure in early childhood & den Nijs, M. Power-law scaling in the brain surface electric potential Broadband shifts in local field potential power spectra are correlated with single-neuron spiking in humans An electrophysiological marker of arousal level in humans Broadband changes in the cortical surface potential track activation of functionally diverse neuronal populations Exercise in isolation: a countermeasure for electrocortical, mental and cognitive impairments State-dependent alpha peak frequency shifts: experimental evidence, potential mechanisms and functional implications Separating fractal and oscillatory components in the power spectrum of neurophysiological signal Oscillatory power decreases and long-term memory: the information via desynchronization hypothesis Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory Gamma, alpha, delta, and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes Directional coupling of slow and fast hippocampal gamma with neocortical alpha/beta oscillations in human episodic memory Decomposing alpha and 1/f brain activities reveals their differential associations with cognitive processing speed Different origins of gamma rhythm and high-gamma activity in macaque visual cortex The origin of extracellular fields and currents-EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future Frequency and power of human alpha oscillations drift systematically with time-on-task The speed of alpha-band oscillations predicts the temporal resolution of visual perception Individual alpha frequency increases during a task but is unchanged by alpha-band flicker Neurophysiological, neuropsychological, and cognitive effects of 30 days of isolation Simulated power spectral density (PSD) of background electrocorticogram (ECoG) An ascending reticular activating system in the brain stem Broadband electrophysiological dynamics contribute to global resting-state fMRI signal Adaptive gain and the role of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in optimal performance Noradrenaline modulates visual perception and late visually evoked activity On the interrelation of 1/f neural noise and norepinephrine system activity during motor response inhibition The norepinephrine system shows information-content specific properties during cognitive control: evidence from EEG and pupillary responses The co-occurrence of zinc deficiency and social isolation has the opposite effects on mood compared with either condition alone due to changes in the central norepinephrine system Influence of postweaning social isolation in the rat on brain development, conditioned behavior, and neurotransmission The influence of exercise on prefrontal cortex activity and cognitive performance during a simulated space flight to Mars (MARS500) The impact of long-term confinement and exercise on central and peripheral stress markers isolation syndrome" in mice The effect of isolation on catecholamine concentration and turnover in discrete areas of the rat brain The effects of drives conflicting with exploration on the problem-solving behavior of rats reared in free and restricted environments The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data Electrocorticographic dissociation of alpha and beta rhythmic activity in the human sensorimotor system Inter-and intra-individual variability in alpha peak frequency Peak alpha frequency is a neural marker of cognitive function across the autism spectrum Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data Our deepest gratefulness is dedicated to the six professional and ambitious volunteers as well as to the team managing and operating this complex mission on Russian, American and German site. Special thank goes to Elena Fomina and Julia Popova and their team for their collaboration and hospitality and to Christian Rogon from DLR for administrative support. The study was supported and funded by the German Space Agency (DLR) and the Ministry of Economy and Energy (DLR grant 50WB1813). V.A., J.W. and T.K. conceived the research and designed the experiment. J.W. analyzed the data. J.W. wrote the manuscript with input from V.A. All authors provided ongoing critical review of the results and commented on the manuscript. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. The authors declare no competing interests. Supplementary information is available for this paper at https ://doi.org/10.1038/s4159 8-020-75127 -0.Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to J.W. or V.A.Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/.