key: cord-1031799-875mx5r4 authors: Raneri, Phillip C.; Montag, Christian; Rozgonjuk, Dmitri; Satel, Jason; Pontes, Halley M. title: The role of microtransactions in Internet Gaming Disorder and Gambling Disorder: A preregistered systematic review date: 2022-02-22 journal: Addict Behav Rep DOI: 10.1016/j.abrep.2022.100415 sha: 4b79e42f0b20f4946a6a756bbbdff59c5dda9c3b doc_id: 1031799 cord_uid: 875mx5r4 Recently there has been increased interest in understanding the relationship between microtransactions, gaming, and gambling. This review aimed to synthesise the evidence on the relationship between microtransactions, ‘Internet Gaming Disorder’ (IGD), and Gambling Disorder in order to report on the: psychometric assessments used, sampling and demographic information, study design and sampling methods, relationships between microtransactions and both IGD and gambling disorder. Inclusion criteria included: refereed studies quantifying microtransactions and/or loot boxes examining their relationship with IGD and/or gambling disorder that were published between 2013 and 2021. Electronic databases were searched and the results were synthesised qualitatively. 14 studies were included. The quality of the evidence was ‘Good’ and clear positive relationships between microtransactions and both IGD and gambling disorder were identified. These relationships apply more to loot boxes than other microtransactions, and risky loot box use was identified as a possible mediator of these relationships. Additionally, microtransaction expenditure increased with the risk of gambling disorder. There is some evidence that adolescents who purchase loot boxes may be more at risk of developing gambling disorder. External validity is limited due to the cross-sectional nature of the evidence, the use of convenience sampling, and the predominantly Western samples resulting in non-representative samples. Prevalence rates of IGD and gambling disorder varied significantly across studies and were different to general prevalence rates. We conclude that there is a need to develop consistent methods for assessing IGD and microtransaction engagement in future research. Implications for policy-makers and future research are discussed. Electronic gaming has become extremely prevalent in the modern world. According to recent large-scale studies, around 65% of American adults and 66% of Australians play video games, with 90% of Australian households having at least one device dedicated to playing video games (Brand, Jervis, Huggins, & Wilson, 2019; Entertainment Software Association, 2019) . In reference to key demographics using convenience samples, in Australia alone 69% of people aged 1-17 years old, 62% of people aged 18-64 years old, and 42% of people aged 65-94 played video games. The average gamer was aged 33-34 years old and 47% of gamers were female (Brand et al., 2019) . According to Newzoo (2020), the global gaming market had $USD159.3 billion in revenue in 2020, and it is estimated that in 2023 there will be 3.07 billion gamers in the world. These statistics illustrate that gaming is a pervasive activity that is enjoyed throughout the lifespan and not only by men, further moving away from the stereotypical image of gamers being lonely teenage boys (Pontes, 2018) . As electronic gaming is a relatively new phenomenon with such prevalent participation, it is imperative to investigate the impacts it has on the lives of all individuals worldwide. Accompanying the popularity of electronic gaming is a wide range of associated positive and negative psychosocial outcomes (Pontes, 2018; Saunders et al., 2017) . At the qualitative level, gamers in Australia report that playing any sort of video game helps them reduce stress, keeps their mind active, and helps with not only creativity, but also emotional, and social wellbeing (Brand et al., 2019) . Additionally, playing shooter games at moderate levels has been found to result in a variety of cognitive benefits such as faster and more accurate allocation of attention, better spatial resolution in visual processing, and improved spatial skills (Granic, Lobel, & Engels, 2014; Howard, Wilding, & Guest, 2017) . Nevertheless, null effects in relation to these cognitive benefits have been reported in recent research (Sala, Tatlidil, & Gobet, 2018) . For a more complete review on the benefits of playing video games see Griffiths (2019) . Furthermore, a recent systematic review and meta-analysis on the relationship between the Big Five personality factors and gaming (Akbari et al., 2021) revealed that most personality factors (i.e., agreeableness, openness to new experiences, and extraversion) may be somewhat protective of engaging in problematic gaming behaviours. Neuroticism was found to have either a positive relationship with problem gaming or no relationship at all. Conscientiousness was found to be the most protective against problem gaming. This was attributed to conscientious individuals being more likely to be organised and prioritise their 'real life' goals, and therefore have a healthier relationship with gaming. Conversely, excessive gaming has been shown to be associated with psychiatric disorders (e.g., depression and anxiety) alongside addictive and aggressive behaviours (Greitemeyer, 2018; Pontes, 2018; Wang, Cho, & Kim, 2018) , with a recent large-scale study (Pontes, Schivinski, Kannen, & Montag, 2022) including 123,262 gamers reporting that disordered gaming translates to an average of 34.53 to 40.13 hours of weekly time spent gaming. Additionally, Akbari et al. (2021) found that neuroticism, in some cases, is a personality vulnerability factor that may result in an individual developing problematic gaming behaviours. It was thought that this may be due to the fact that neurotic individuals are less confident, and may use gaming as a way to cope with, suppress, or escape negative emotions. A recent large scale study by supports the already mentioned findings that high neuroticism and low conscientiousness seem to be the driving personality factors of gaming disorder tendencies. Evidence of disordered gaming first came to light in the 1980s (Klein, 1984; Nilles, 1982; Ross, Finestone, & Lavin, 1982; Soper & Miller, 1983) and its status as a mental health disorder has been heavily debated by scholars (Billieux, Schimmenti, Khazaal, Maurage, & Heeren, 2015; Griffiths, Kuss, & Pontes, 2016; Kuss, 2013; Maraz, Kiraly, & Demetrovics, 2015) . This debate intensified after 'Internet Gaming Disorder' (IGD) was included in Section III of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth edition (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) as a condition for further study. According to the DSM-5, IGD is defined as persistent and recurring internet use to play games, usually with other people, which results in clinically significant impairment or distress as indicated by at least five of the following nine criteria within a 12-month period: (1) preoccupation with video games, (2) withdrawal symptoms in the absence of gaming, (3) tolerance as indicated by increasing amounts of time spent engaging in gaming, (4) unsuccessful attempts to control participation in gaming, (5) loss of interest in previous hobbies/entertainment due to (and with the exception of) gaming, (6) continued excessive use of video games despite knowledge of psychosocial problems, (7) deceiving family members, therapists, or others about the amount of gaming undertaken, (8) use of video games in order to escape or relieve negative moods, and (9) jeopardising or losing a significant relationship, job, or educational or career opportunity because of participation in video games. Following the inclusion of IGD in Section III of the DSM-5, the World Health Organization (WHO) recognised 'Gaming Disorder' (GD, 6C51) as an official diagnosis in the International Classification of Disease, 11th edition (ICD-11; World Health Organization, 2019) . According to the ICD-11, GD is classified by a pattern of recurrent and persistent gaming behaviour (online and/or offline), as indicated by the following three criteria: (1) impaired control over gaming, (2) increasing priority assigned to gaming to the point that it takes priority over daily activities and life interests, and (3) continued/increased gaming despite negative consequences. Such dysregulated gaming behaviours must result in significant impairment in personal, family, social, educational, occupational, or other important areas of functioning in order to be classified as GD. A recent review study (Stevens, Dorstyn, Delfabbro, & King, 2021) reported that the worldwide prevalence of IGD ranged from 1.96% to 3.05%, although significant variance in prevalence rates was found among published studies. In terms of gender differences, the prevalence rates for males ranged from .23% to 22.7%, while for females it ranged from .04% to 9.32%. Although IGD prevalence rates vary significantly, it is apparent that IGD is a prominent phenomenon worldwide, and is thus paramount to be further investigated as it may be affecting a relatively large number of individuals, both directly and indirectly. The inclusion of GD as a bona fide disorder in the ICD-11 resulted in a new era of research (Pontes & Griffiths, 2020) , enabling future research to consistently investigate the aetiology, epidemiology, clinical features, comorbidities, and negative impacts of this disorder. Throughout this review both IGD and GD will be referred to as IGD as most of the studies conducted in the field still adopts the APA's framework and nomenclature to describe disordered gaming. One crucial emerging area of research focuses on the relationship between IGD and Gambling Disorder. The similarity between gaming and gambling is not a novel realisation, with the structural and potential addictive similarities being brought to light in the 1990s (Griffiths, 1991) . However, with the advancement of technology, the gambling and gaming environments have changed and research on arcade games and slot machines may no longer be relevant in a modern-day context (Macey & Hamari, 2018) . Not only have gaming and gambling changed drastically over the years, but these two activities became intrinsically intertwined (Rozgonjuk, Schivinski, Pontes, & Montag, 2021) . Gambling-like mechanisms are being introduced into a broad range of video games (Macey & Hamari, 2018) . For example, at The Diamond Casino and Resort in the video game 'Grand Theft Auto V' gamers can use real money to gamble (Rockstar Games, 2013) , and internet gambling is becoming a more prominent and attractive feature in social media and video games (King, Delfabbro, Kaptsis, & Zwaans, 2014) . The nature of gaming also has been changed with the advent of smartphones, allowing mobile access to video games from anywhere, while also giving rise to new games such as 'Freemium' games (i.e., Free and Premium; see also Montag, Lachmann, Herrlich, & Zweig, 2019) . Although, Freemium games are 'free to play', they offer in-game purchases for real money (e.g., currency, virtual goods, or 'skins'), which are referred to as 'microtransactions' (King & Delfabbro, 2018) . Therefore, the 'Premium' in these Freemium games need to be paid for. Furthermore, microtransactions can take many different forms, such as one-off specific purchases, timed or permanent (e.g., item is for sale for 24 hours or item is always for sale), or can alternatively be in the form of random purchases known as 'loot boxes'. Some microtransactions are available in video games that do not require real money, however, microtransactions utilising real currency have become a dominant business model within the video game industry (Tomić, 2017) . Loot boxes, when opened, provide in-game rewards on a chance basis, and are purchased with the hope of collecting a rare or valuable reward that is worth more than the cost of the loot box (Yokomitsu, Irie, Shinkawa, & Tanaka, 2021) . In some games, loot boxes require 'keys' to open them, and, in some cases, gamers are able to earn both keys and loot boxes without using real money (Yokomitsu et al., 2021) . However, they are often purchased using real-world currency. Both loot boxes and gambling can rely on an individual risking money on the outcome of a chance event with the goal of receiving a reward of high value (Zendle & Cairns, 2018) . Opening loot boxes is often accompanied by exciting effects such as sounds and lights (McCaffrey, 2019) , which is similar to what occurs in a live 'brick and mortar' gambling context (i.e., sound effects, music, and lights being triggered when you win on a slot machine). Gambling rewards are also similar to rewards obtained by opening loot boxes, as intermittent reinforcement plays an important role. Not all loot boxes will provide a valuable item (i.e., win) every time, but only from time to time, similarly to gambling rewards. The similarity between loot boxes and gambling has led to concern that loot box purchases may lead to or exacerbate gambling disorder symptoms (Zendle & Cairns, 2018) . Gambling disorder can be defined as a pattern of excessive gambling behaviours which causes problems for an individual in their personal, vocational, and/or family life (Raylu & Oei, 2004; Zendle & Cairns, 2018) . This is thought to occur when an individual becomes conditioned to the arousing features of gambling so much so that the need to be excited through gambling causes significant harm to the self and others (Blaszczynski & Nower, 2002) . Interestingly, the WHO's criteria for Gambling Disorder (6C50) are nearly the same as for GD, but clearly the terms "gaming" and "gambling" need to be exchanged. It is possible that conditioning can occur to a similar effect through the use of loot boxes, which could lead to an increase in gambling disorder amongst those with IGD who engage in loot box purchasing, especially risky loot box purchasing (Zendle & Cairns, 2018) . Risky loot box purchasing refers to purchasing behaviours that have typical addictive motivations, such as opening loot boxes for the 'thrill' of it (but see overlap with impulse control disorders where thrill in parts plays also a role), playing video games for longer than intended in order to earn loot boxes, and putting off other activities in order to obtain a loot box (Brooks & Clark, 2019) . Research into the relationship between IGD and gambling disorder within the context of microtransactions is still in its infancy. However, understanding these interactions is imperative for policy-makers to inform and guide policy surrounding young people, gaming, and gambling, as well as for clinicians to guide treatment, intervention, and develop effective harm minimisation strategies. At the time this project was initiated, no systematic review study had been conducted to investigate the relationship between IGD and gambling disorder in the context of microtransactions. However, at the time of writing of this study there were at least three review studies that have been published as of September 2021 (see Garea, Drummond, Sauer, Hall, & Williams, 2021; Spicer, Nicklin, Uther, Lloyd, Lloyd, & Close, 2021; Yokomitsu et al., 2021) . Despite this, the present review is still warranted as it builds and expands upon the previously published reviews in the field since these previous reviews have focused on a specific type of microtransaction, such as loot boxes (Garea et al., 2021; Spicer et al., 2021; Yokomitsu et al., 2021) . Moreover, the present review included a slightly different set of studies compared to the previously published reviews, thus, generating new insights into the problem under investigation. These previously published reviews all found a significant relationship between loot boxes and IGD and gambling disorder. However, it is important to examine the relationship between both loot boxes and other non-random microtransactions with IGD and gambling disorder in order to further understand whether the gamblinglike structure of loot boxes plays a unique role in these relationships. The current review aims to synthesise and evaluate the literature on microtransactions and their relationships with both IGD and gambling disorder. To achieve this goal, this study will synthesise and evaluate the existing literature in order to report on the following methodological and psychological features: Methodological features: (1) measurement and psychometric assessments typically adopted, (2) sample characteristics and demographics information, and (3) study design and sampling characteristics. Psychological features: (4) relationship between microtransactions and gambling disorder, (5) relationships between microtransactions and IGD, and the (6) associations between microtransactions, IGD, and gambling disorder. Doing so will provide insight into the potential harm microtransactions may have on individuals and inform policy-makers on how to better protect vulnerable people from harms associated with microtransaction engagement. The current review methodology, including the research question, search strategy, inclusion and exclusion criteria, and risk of bias assessments were developed a priori and were described in the preregistration protocol (PROSPERO CRD42020216371, https://www.crd.york. ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42020216371; Raneri & Pontes, 2020) . The present review was conducted in accordance with the latest revised guidelines for the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) checklist . Additionally, the Risk of Bias in Systematic Reviews tool (ROBIS; Whiting et al., 2016) was used to assess the risk of bias within the present review, which was completed by the first author in consultation with the last author. ROBIS consists of four primary domains where bias can be introduced in a systematic review: (1) the eligibility criteria, (2) the identification and selection of studies, (3) data collection and study appraisal, and (4) the review's synthesis and findings. For each domain the rater was required to answer five to six questions regarding the review process and answer 'Yes', 'Probably Yes', 'Probably No', 'No', or 'No Information'. 'Yes' and ' Probably Yes' answers indicate low levels of bias in the review, 'No' and 'Probably No' indicate higher levels of bias in the review, and 'No Information' responses indicate unclear levels of bias. Each domain was then rated as 'Low Risk', 'High Risk' or 'Unclear', and the rater was required to record any rationale for concerns. The rater was then asked whether any concerns in domains 1-4 were addressed in the study, if the included studies were appropriate to the research question, and if the researchers avoided emphasising results based on their statistical significance. An overall rating of 'low' 'high' or 'unclear' risk of bias is given to the review based on these final questions. A set of strict inclusion criteria was adopted when deciding if a study should be included in the review or not. Each study had to be a peerreviewed empirical investigation that operationalised microtransactions and/or loot boxes and examined their relationship with either IGD, gambling disorder, or both IGD and gambling disorder. Only studies published in English, Spanish, or Portuguese in refereed journals were included as these were the languages spoken between the reviewing authors. Studies required a publication date between 2013-2021. The cut-off date of 2013 was used as it was the year that IGD was first formally and tentatively introduced as a potential behavioural addiction in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), which encouraged and enabled further research to be carried out into the aetiology and epidemiology of the disorder. Studies were excluded from the review if they were theoretical or qualitative studies, single case studies (i.e., studies in which N = 1), or did not meet the above inclusion criteria. PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science were searched on 15/09/2020 using the following search strategy: (microtransac* OR loot box* OR purchas*) AND ((disorder* OR patholog* OR problem* OR addict* OR compulsive OR dependen*) AND (video OR computer OR internet OR gaming OR game OR gamb*) ). The development of the search strategy was based on previous literature and was assisted by an independent academic librarian from the University of Tasmania. Different search strategies were piloted and the strategy that yielded the highest number of relevant studies was utilised in the final search. The search strategy was adapted for use with each individual database and only titles and abstracts were searched. The search results were imported into a systematic review management website (i.e., Covidence; Veritas Health Innovation, n.d.) to remove duplicates. Next, titles and abstracts were screened by the first author and potentially eligible studies were collated using Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Corporation, n.d.) . The last author then reviewed the potentially eligible studies and discrepancies were discussed between the research team. The first author then reviewed the full texts based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria outlined. The last author then reviewed the full texts independently, with emerging discrepancies being discussed and resolved within the team via an online discussion aimed at resolving these discrepancies by reaching a joint decision. Inter-rater agreement coefficients were calculated using Fleiss' Kappa analysis to assess the level of agreement between raters (coded as '1 = study to be included' and '0 = study not to be included') in relation to the final inclusion of eligible studies. The results of this analysis provided coefficients between .61 and .80, which indicate substantial agreement among raters (Landis & Koch, 1977) . Publications were then separated into three groups based on studies that investigated (i) microtransactions and gambling disorder, (ii) microtransactions and IGD, or (iii) microtransactions and both IGD and gambling disorder. An Excel spreadsheet was used to extract the following data from eligible studies: design of the study, participant demographics, prevalence rates of IGD and gambling disorder, the types of microtransactions measured, microtransaction expenditure, correlation coefficients, regression coefficients, mean difference estimates, effect sizes, and psychometric tests utilised. If the data set of a study was made available online, it was examined to extract data that was measured but not reported in the published report. When necessary, corresponding authors of the eligible studies were directly contacted via email to request additional data that was not publicly available. Seven authors were sent emails regarding nine studies and additional information was provided from five authors regarding their respective five studies. One author was unable to provide additional information while another author did not respond despite a follow up email. For transparency purposes, the final data set for the present study is publicly available at the Open Science Framework (see Raneri & Pontes, 2020 , https://osf.io/9gepb/). 1 The Appraisal tool for Cross-Sectional Studies (AXIS; Downes, Brennan, Williams, & Dean, 2016) was used to assess the quality of the evidence and risk of bias in the studies included in the present review. This deviated from the preregistered plan. Originally, the Grading of Recommendations Assessments, Development and Evaluation (GRADE; Schünemann, Brożek, Guyatt, & Oxman, 2013) tool was going to be used to assess evidence quality. However, it was later deemed that the GRADE tool was more suited for assessing evidence quality for intervention studies. As such, the AXIS tool was identified after preregistration and was considered to be more appropriate as none of the included studies were intervention studies. Additionally, almost all of the journal articles identified in the search performed were cross-sectional, for which the AXIS tool is specifically designed to assess. One study by Zendle (2019) was a prospective cohort study; however, the questions were still appropriate for assessing the quality of the evidence in that study. The AXIS tool has 20 different questions for each journal article that are answered with 'Yes', 'No', or "Maybe'. The wording for question 13 was shifted slightly for ease of reporting and consistency with the other questions so that all 'Yes' answers indicated good quality of evidence or low risk of bias and 'No' answers indicated poor quality of evidence or high risk of bias. Questions 13 and 14 were often marked non applicable as response rate and information about non responders were not measurable due to the nature of the study. The studies included in this review were evaluated based on these 20 questions following the AXIS guidelines. A percentage score was calculated based on the number of questions answered as 'Yes' divided by the total number of applicable questions for each journal article. The methodological quality of each study was categorised into one of four categories: poor (0-24%), fair (25-49%), good (50-74%) or excellent (75-100%) based on previous studies (Maass, Roorda, Berendsen, Verhaak, & de Bock, 2015; Yokomitsu et al., 2021) . The quality appraisal was conducted by the first author in consultation with the last author who helped resolve outstanding concerns about the rating of the studies. Brooks and Clark's (2019) article reported the findings of two separate studies (in terms of samples). For the purpose of this review, the article was included as two separate studies which were then reviewed and reported as such. The initial searches performed for the literature review yielded 752 articles. Duplicate articles were removed (n = 187), which left 565 entries to be screened. After screening titles and abstracts, 549 articles were removed, leaving 16 articles for full-text review. One additional study (Hall, Drummond, Sauer, & Ferguson, 2021) was published after the search was conducted and identified during the data extraction phase as being a potential study for inclusion. This study was also included resulting in a total of 17 articles for full-text review. After reviewing the full-texts, three articles were excluded due to not meeting the inclusion criteria: One study (Columb, Griffiths, & O'Gara, 2020) was further excluded as it did not include inferential statistics, another study (Macey & Hamari, 2018) was excluded as it did not clearly operationalise microtransaction and loot box expenditure, and one final study was excluded as it only investigated the variables within the context of the COVID-19 pandemic and did not have baseline data that could be compared to the other studies. As . The results of the Fleiss' Kappa analysis yielded: κ = .63, z = 2.44, p = .015, which indicates substantial agreement between authors when reviewing full-texts for inclusion in the final review (Landis & Koch, 1977) . The reference lists of these studies were searched for additional studies to include; however, none were identified. See Fig. 1 for PRISMA flow diagram. The ROBIS assessment identified a low risk of bias within this review when considering study eligibility criteria, study identification and selection, data collection and appraisal, and synthesis of findings. See Table 1 (Drummond et al., 2020) had more females than males (i.e., >60% females), and four studies (Brooks & Clark, 2019; A. Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Zendle, 2020) had equal distributions of males and females. The proportion of male participants ranged from 35.46% to 91.92% while the proportion of female participants ranged from 5.50% to 63.39%, and the proportion of non-binary, other, or non-specified genders ranged from .56% to 4.46%. Age was reported as an ordinal variable across seven of the 14 studies (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Macey & Hamari, 2019; Zendle, 2019 Zendle, , 2020 Zendle & Cairns, 2018 Zendle et al., 2020) and the categorisation of age was not consistent across these studies, thus, restricting the extraction and comparison of age-related demographic information for these studies. For example, Macey and Hamari (2019) categorised age into groups with a range of 3 (e.g., 15-17 years, 18-21 years, and so on) whereas Zendle (2019) categorised age into groups with a range of seven (e.g., 18-24 years) or five (e.g., 25-29 years, 30-34 years, and so on). Brooks and Clark (2019) reported the median age for both their studies, and the mean was used for all other studies. The mean and median age of participants ranged from 17.21 to 38.08 years, with standard deviations ranging from .83 to 14.58. Moreover, a total of 10 studies only included adults in their sample (Brooks & Clark, 2019; A. Li et al. 2019; Zendle, 2019 , 2020 , Zendle & Cairns, 2018 Zendle et al., 2020) and the age of what constituted an adult varied between studies. One study (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020) investigated adolescents between the ages of 12-16 years, and another study investigated older adolescents between the ages of 16 and 18 years. Two studies did not exclude participants based on age (Drummond et al., 2020; Macey & Hamari, 2019) . Ethnicity and nationality were reported in nine of the eligible studies (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020; Macey & Hamari, 2019; Zendle, 2020; Zendle & Cairns, 2018 , however, there was no consistency in how it was measured, rendering comparisons inviable. Overall, most studies had 'Caucasian' dominant samples, except for one study (Brooks & Clark, 2019) which had a majority of participants who described themselves as 'Asian' (62.1%). Two studies included samples that were considered representative of the general population (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Zendle, 2020). All studies included in this review were cross-sectional in design, except for one study (Zendle, 2019) which was a prospective cohort study. (Drummond et al., 2020; Zendle, 2020) used quota sampling, and one study (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020) employed random sampling and used a national database to select participants. Six studies had their first author from the United Kingdom (Zendle, 2019 (Zendle, , 2020 Zendle & Cairns, 2018 Zendle et al., 2020; , two studies from Canada (Brooks & Clark, 2019), two from the United States (A. Li et al., 2019) , and one from each Aotearoa New Zealand (Drummond et al., 2020) , Australia (D. , Denmark (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020), and Finland (Macey & Hamari, 2019) . Please see Table 2 for more information regarding study design and characteristics. All studies investigated loot boxes, with five studies also investigating non-random microtransactions in addition to loot boxes (A. Zendle & Cairns, 2018 , and one study investigated all types of microtransactions (Zendle, 2019) . Seven studies ( The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale -9 item scale (IGDS-9; Lemmens, Valkenburg, & Gentile, 2015) was used to assess for IGD in three studies (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Zendle, 2020 ). An adapted version of the Internet Gaming Disorder Checklist (Przybylski, Weinstein, & Murayama, 2016 ) was used in one study (Drummond et al., 2020) , the (King, Russell, Delfabbro, & Polisena, 2020) , and restating the DSM-5 criteria as questions was used in one study (Li et al., 2019) . IGD was found to have a varied range of prevalence rates, ranging from 7.90% . Prevalence rates of gambling disorder reported were variable, ranging from 1.40% to 48.55%. gambling disorder severity (ρ = .16). Loot box expenditure was more strongly and positively associated with gambling disorder severity (ρ = .24) than other microtransaction expenditure in this study (see Table 3 ). Again, loot box expenditure refers to payment allowing to open loot boxes, whereas microtransaction expenditures may include loot box expenditure and other in-game purchases providing customisation of ingame experiences. Five studies conducted mean difference analyses ( Tables 4 and 5 ). Of these, three studies had loot box expenditure as the dependent variable and problem gambling severity as the independent variable (Zendle & Cairns, 2018 Zendle et al., 2020) . All three of these studies found a significant main effect (p < .001) of problem gambling severity on loot box expenditure, with effect sizes ranging from η 2 = .05 to .60. Two of these same studies (Zendle & Cairns, 2018 conducted additional analyses with microtransaction expenditure as the dependent variable and problem gambling severity as the independent variable. Both found a significant main effect (p < .001) of problem gambling severity on loot box expenditure with effect sizes (η 2 ) ranging from < .01 to .03. Furthermore, Zendle (2019) used overall in-game expenditure as the dependent variable and problem gambling severity as the independent variable in relation to one specific video game (i.e., Heroes of the Storm). The results of this study reported a significant main effect (p < .001) of problem gambling severity on in-game expenditure with an effect size of η 2 p = .19 and found that problem gamblers spent significantly less money (U.S. dollars; USD) in-game after the loot boxes were removed from the game. Zendle et al. (2020) had problem gambling severity as the dependent variable and whether a person pays for loot boxes (i.e., dichotomous: yes or no) as the independent variable. The results revealed a significant main effect of paying for loot boxes on problem gambling severity (p < .001), with an effect size of η 2 = .60 (see Table 5 ). Table 5 reports all pairwise analyses of the three studies (Zendle & Cairns, 2018 that used loot box expenditure as the dependent variable and problem gambling severity as the independent variable. Results revealed that individuals in the non-problem gambling group spent significantly less money (USD) on loot boxes than individuals at risk of problem gambling (i.e., low-risk and moderate-risk gamblers) and individuals who meet criteria for problem gambling (p < .001). Two studies (Zendle & Cairns, 2018; also found that individuals at risk of problem gambling spent significantly (USD) less money on loot boxes than individuals who met criteria for problem gambling (p < .001-.002), however these results were not replicated in the Zendle and Cairns (2019) study. Two studies (Zendle & Cairns, 2018 included pairwise analyses with other microtransaction expenditure as the dependent variable and problem gambling severity as the independent variable. Both studies found that non-problem gamblers spent significantly less money than those at risk of problem gambling and those who meet criteria for problem gambling (p < .001). Zendle and Cairns (2018) also found that individuals with low-risk of problem gambling spent significantly less than those at moderate-risk of problem gambling. Only one study (King, Russell, Delfabbro, & Polisena, 2020) examined the relationship between various microtransactions and IGD alone. This study conducted a logistical regression analysis and reported unstandardised coefficients. As these cannot be compared to the other studies conducting similar regression analyses that reported standardised coefficients, they were not extracted. Further, this study assessed loot box expenditure within a single game (i.e., Fortnite) and its association with IGD symptoms and did not find a statistically significant association. Six studies investigated the relationship between microtransactions and both IGD and gambling disorder (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020; Li et al., 2019; Zendle et al., 2020) . Four studies examined the association between IGD and gambling disorder (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020; and reported positive correlations ranging from .19 to .60. The same four studies also found positive correlations between the RLI and IGD, ranging from .32 to .60, and the RLI and gambling disorder, ranging from .32 to .49. Three studies investigated the association between the RLI and loot box expenditure (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020) and reported positive correlations ranging from .25 to .49. Three studies investigated the association between loot box expenditure and IGD (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Zendle, 2020) . Two of these found a positive correlation of .18 and .41 (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Zendle, 2020) , however Brooks and Clark's (2019) second study did not find a significant association between loot box expenditure and IGD. Four studies examined the relationship between loot box expenditure and gambling disorder (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020; Zendle, 2020) . Three of these (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020; Zendle, 2020) found positive correlations ranging from .21 to .34, however Brooks and Clark's (2019) second study did not find a significant relationship between loot box expenditure and gambling disorder. Moreover, one study (A. found positive relationships between all microtransaction expenditure and both IGD and gambling disorder levels (.46 and .47, respectively), suggesting that higher expenditure is associated with greater symptoms of IGD and gambling disorder (see Table 6 ). One study (Drummond et al., 2020) examined loot box expenditure and conducted pairwise comparisons investigating the difference in loot box expenditure between problem gambling groups. The overall mean expenditure was 12.92 USD (SD = 23.29). The non-problem gambling group had the lowest average expenditure (M = .88 USD, SD = 5.24), followed by low-risk gamblers (M = 1.88 USD, SD = 7.82), which was followed by the moderate-risk gamblers (M = 4.64 USD, SD = 15.60), with problem gamblers having the highest expenditure (M = 12.91 USD, SD = 23.29). Simple effects analysis revealed significant differences in expenditure between all groups of gamblers, except for non-problem gamblers and low-risk gamblers (see Table 7 ). Four studies conducted regression analyses and each study used different outcome variables (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Drummond et al., 2020; Li et al., 2019; Zendle, 2020) . The results of these studies revealed that IGD was found to significantly predict RLI scores, loot box expenditure, and loot box purchasing behaviours. Additionally, gambling disorder was found to significantly predict loot box expenditure and loot box expenditure was found to predict both IGD and gambling disorder symptoms (see Table 8 ). The mean percentage on the AXIS tool was 71.22% (range 55.56%-83.33%) falling in the 'Good' category. Out of the 14 studies assessed, no studies fell in the 'Poor' or 'Fair' categories, nine studies fell in the 'Good' category, and five studies fell in the 'Excellent' category. In reference to the individual AXIS questions, there were specific questions that were generally not answered as 'Yes' across the body of the evidence. Two studies (Drummond et al., 2020; Zendle et al., 2020) received 'Yes' scores for question 3 ('Was the sample size justified?'), question 6 ('Was the selection process likely to select subjects/participants that were representative of the target/reference population under investigation?') was only assessed as 'Yes' on three studies (Drummond et al., 2020; Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Zendle, 2020) , question 7 ('Were measures undertaken to address and categorise non-responders?') was scored as 'Yes' for three studies (King, Russell, Delfabbro, & Polisena, 2020; Macey & Hamari, 2019; Zendle, 2020) , and question 10 ('Is it clear what was used to determined statistical significance and/or precision estimates? (e.g. p-values, confidence intervals)') was only assessed as 'Yes' on six studies (Brooks & Clark, 2019; A. Macey & Hamari, 2019; Zendle, 2020) . Additionally, question 13 ('Does the response rate raise concerns about non-response bias?') could only be assessed at all for three studies with two 'Yes' scores (King, Russell, Delfabbro, & Polisena, 2020; Kristiansen & Severin, 2020) , and question 14 ('If appropriate, was information about non-responders described?') could only be assessed on two studies with one 'Yes' score (King, Russell, Delfabbro, & Polisena, 2020 ) (see Table 9 ). The present study encompassed a preregistered systematic review on the existing literature to clarify the relationship between microtransactions, IGD, and gambling disorder within the context of different types of in-game microtransactions. Across all studies that investigated only gambling disorder, a significant and positive relationship was found between greater severity of gambling disorder symptoms and increased in-game microtransaction expenditure. Additionally, the studies reviewed found that the amount of money individuals spend on microtransactions increased as the risk of gambling disorder also increased. Moreover, the positive relationship found between loot boxes and gambling disorder was stronger than for other types of have been written as 'et al. ' . Values have been rounded to 2 decimal places. y: Both entries pertain to the same publication, however they report to two separate studies. P.C. Raneri et al. microtransactions (e.g., ρ = .24 vs ρ = .16) in one study (Zendle & Cairns, 2019) , while another study (Zendle, 2019) reported that in-game expenditure (in Heroes of the Storm) decreased after loot boxes were removed from the game. These results support the positive link between gambling disorder and loot boxes specifically. The nature of loot boxes, which is akin to gambling (McCaffrey, 2019), may be underlying the relationship between microtransaction expenditure and gambling disorder symptoms. Furthermore, only one study (King, Russell, Delfabbro, & Polisena, 2020) investigated microtransactions and IGD in isolation. This study only assessed these variables in a single game (i.e., Fortnite) and did not find a significant relationship between microtransaction expenditure on Fortnite and IGD. It is important to note that given its narrow focus, the findings reported in this study may not be generalisable to the broader microtransaction context in other video games. The studies where microtransactions and both IGD and gambling disorder were investigated simultaneously found significant positive relationships between IGD and gambling disorder. Additionally, significant relationships were found between loot box expenditure with both IGD and gambling disorder. Significant positive relationships were also identified between microtransaction expenditure and both IGD and gambling disorder. However, Brooks and Clark's (2019) second study was the only study that did not identify a significant relationship between loot box expenditure with IGD and gambling disorder. There was also a significant positive relationship between the RLI and IGD, gambling disorder, and loot box expenditure. Drummond et al. (2020) found that the amount individuals spent on loot boxes increased as the risk of gambling disorder also increased. Furthermore, IGD was also found to significantly predict greater RLI scores and loot box expenditure, and loot box expenditure significantly predicted higher levels of IGD and problem gambling severity. The positive correlation between microtransaction expenditure and gambling disorder severity may be due to the fact that certain types of microtransaction engagement present with gambling-like features, particularly when it comes to loot boxes (Drummond & Sauer, 2018) . Moreover, modern video games feature other gambling-like practices that are associated with both gaming and gambling disorder , while additional literature has reported that paying for loot boxes is also linked to problem gambling . Thus, there is overall convincing evidence for a positive relationship between microtransaction expenditure with both IGD and gambling disorder. Specifically, this relationship appears to be stronger with loot boxes compared to other non-random microtransactions. However, the way an individual engages with loot boxes may contribute to this relationship. Risky loot box use was found in multiple studies to be Table 7 Mean difference analysis for Drummond et al. (2020) . Appraisal tool for Cross-Sectional Studies (AXIS) Quality Appraisal Assessment. P.C. Raneri et al. positively associated with both gambling disorder, IGD, and loot box expenditure. It is plausible that risky loot box usage may mediate the relationship between microtransactions and IGD and/or gambling disorder. While it is evident that there is a positive relationship between microtransaction expenditure with both IGD and gambling disorder, it is important to also understand the impact that this relationship may have on the lives of individuals. Carey, Delfabbro, and King (2021) identified that IGD was associated with higher levels of general harm to an individual and were six times more likely to experience risk of both physical harm (e.g., poor sleep, diet, hygiene, and drug use), psychological harm (e.g., anxiety, depressed, or general psychological distress), and higher levels of financial harm. Interestingly, a recent review on the characteristics of gamers who purchase loot boxes (Yokomitsu et al., 2021) identified that loot box expenditure was associated with both positive mood, psychological distress, and negative mood. The association with positive mood, negative mood, and psychological distress may appear counter intuitive but plausible nevertheless if considered within an engagement process. Firstly, positive mood could emerge as an anticipatory response to the purchasing and potential reward associated with the loot box (i.e., high value reward). Following this, negative mood and psychological distress could ensue, particularly in cases where the reward did not present high psychological and in-game value (i.e., low value reward). Moreover, Drummond et al. (2020) suggested the relationship with positive mood could be due to the benefits of purchasing items that are congruent with a person's personality, and that gamers with disposable income to be spent on loot boxes may be experiencing a benefit to their mood. Additionally, they argued that the act of opening loot boxes can be 'fun', much like traditional gambling, and that a relationship with positive mood does not take away from the associated risk of financial harm. Two studies (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Zendle et al., 2019) investigated only adolescent samples. conducted correlation analyses and identified a significant positive correlation between loot box spending and gambling disorder. Interestingly, this relationship was stronger than in other studies that included adult samples (Brooks & Clark, 2019; Zendle, 2020; Zendle & Cairns, 2019; Zendle et al., 2020) . One study (Drummond et al., 2020) that investigated the general population also found a positive relationship of similar strength to between loot box spending and gambling disorder, whereas another study (Macey & Hamari, 2019 ) that included both adolescents and adults found a weaker positive relationship between loot box spending and gambling disorder. However, neither of these studies included a comparison between adolescent and adults. These findings may suggest that adolescents who purchase loot boxes are at a higher risk of developing gambling disorder symptoms. This is unsurprising considering adolescents have poorer impulse control and decision making due to their developing prefrontal cortex and subcortical limbic regions (Casey, Getz, & Galvan, 2008) . Consequently, this can result in increased risk taking and negative outcomes (Casey et al., 2008) . Gambling disorder is heavily associated with deficits in impulse control (Slutske, Caspi, Moffitt, & Poulton, 2005) and therefore engaging with loot boxes in adolescence, when there is a natural lack of impulse control, may leave adolescents at a higher risk of developing gambling disorder symptoms than adults. Therefore, the links between microtransactions such as loot boxes with IGD and gambling disorder, as well as their associated risk of harm to an individual, have significant implications for policy-making decisions. Loot boxes remain largely unregulated, with the exception of some European nations (McCaffrey, 2019) . While causality cannot be established in this relationship due to the cross-sectional nature of the existing evidence, future research should further explore the relationships identified in the present study in order to ascertain the need for regulatory actions within the industry. This is a key aspect to be explored, particularly amongst minors since a recent study (González-Cabrera et al., 2022) found that individuals under the age of 18 engaged in loot box purchasing behaviours to the same extent as adults. Consequently, this highlights the importance of protecting children and adolescents who engage in both video gaming and loot box activities regularly from potential harms. For instance, the Parliament of Australia has identified that loot boxes fall into a legal grey area as to whether they are constituted as gambling, however has pushed for further regulations and inquiry (Australian Parliament House, 2018) . It was identified that video games with loot boxes psychologically resembling gambling should be age-restricted as either MA15 + or R18 + and labelled with a content descriptor of 'Simulated Gambling'. Additionally, they call for clear disclosure of the odds of loot box outcomes and the development of an ethical framework to guide video game production. However, such recommendations are yet to be implemented. The introduction of policies such as these, in cooperation with video game production companies, would help protect vulnerable individuals from loot box related harms and are further supported by the findings of this study. In relation to the ethical design of video games, Montag et al. (2019) called for social responsibility (as recently also mentioned in the context of social media; see Montag, Hegelich, Sindermann, Rozgonjuk, & Marengo, 2021) as it is of utmost importance. Overall, the evidence quality of the studies reviewed was deemed as being in the 'Good' category. A closer analysis of the questions that were adopted to assess the studies reviewed showed some common methodological issues that were consistent throughout many of the studies. Most studies used convenience sampling, which resulted in very few studies having samples that represented the target population, thus limiting the external validity of the body of evidence reviewed. Additionally, as is the nature of online survey studies, many studies were unable to address or categorise non-responders in their survey. This made it impossible for the most part to rule out non-response bias as contributing to the results found in most studies. Furthermore, only six studies met the AXIS criterion for having clarity in terms of the significance and/or precision estimates reported. This required studies to explicitly state the statistical methods, software packages, and significance levels that were used. Most commonly studies did not state what α level was used to ascertain statistical significance or the statistical package utilised. While it is likely that the α levels were implied to be .05, this was not made explicit in the reviewed studies. Additionally, only two studies justified their sample sizes, making it difficult to interpret the results meaningfully and limits the quality of the evidence. Interestingly, the quality assessment conducted in this review varied to some extent from other reviews (Spicer et al., 2021; Yokomitsu et al., 2021) , with the current review ratings being higher. Spicer et al.'s (2021) review included many older studies published prior to 2013, and stated that the quality of the evidence on loot box papers has increased in more recent years. This may explain some of the difference in quality appraisal between their assessment and the current review as only newer studies published after 2013 were included. However, it is unclear why there is such a difference between Yokomitsu et al.'s (2021) appraisal and the current one considering the approach to assessment was very AXIS = Appraisal Tool for Cross-sectional Studies. Green squares represent a rating classed as 'Yes' in relation to the criterion assessed. Red squares represent a rating classed as 'No' in relation to the criterion assessed. Yellow squares represent an rating that was 'Maybe' in relation to the criterion assessed. Grey squares represent the criterion in question was not assessable. Category classification thresholds: Poor = 0-24%, Fair = 25-49%, Good = 50-74%, Excellent = 75-100% *: The wording of question 13 was slightly shifted for ease of reporting so that 'Yes' responses all indicated positive ratings. y: Both entries pertain to the same publication, however they report to two separate studies. All studies with more than two authors have been written as 'et al.'. similar . It is possible that the difference in quality assessment may be caused by using different assessment measures or due to difference in rater opinions. Neither review included an inter-rater assessment and therefore it is unclear how much variance in opinion attributed to the quality ratings. When examining the characteristics of participants, the majority of studies were dominated by Caucasian individuals mostly from North America, the United Kingdom, Australia, and other countries in Europe. Additionally, most samples had a majority of male participants. This is unsurprising considering that only two studies had representative samples. No studies included in this review targeted an exclusive Asian population, and only one study (Brooks & Clark, 2019) had a high number of participants who described themselves as Asian. This is concerning considering the fact that the prevalence of both IGD and gambling disorder is higher in Asia than Europe and the USA (Abbott, 2017; Montag & Becker, 2020; Stevens et al., 2021) . In reference to the general design and study characteristics, all studies but one (Zendle, 2019) were cross-sectional. Prevalence rates of IGD varied drastically between studies and were higher than the prevalence rates recently reported by Stevens et al. (2021) , which ranged from 1.96% to 3.05%. The IGDS-9 was the most used tool to assess for IGD. It is possible that the discrepancy in prevalence rates may be due to non-representative samples and selection bias stemming from the recruitment of convenience and self-selected samples. However, five different methods of assessment were used across seven studies which may also have contributed to the observed heterogeneity in prevalence rates reported among the reviewed studies as this is a known issue likely to bias prevalence estimates (Stevens et al., 2021) . The way microtransaction engagement was measured across studies was also variable, and there was little consistency between studies in how this was assessed. Often these were researcher developed ad hoc questions about whether an individual had obtained a loot box, used a loot box, bought a loot box or loot box key, or sold virtual items they obtained from a loot box. Loot box and microtransaction expenditure and the RLI were also used to measure microtransaction engagement. One study (A. adapted the RLI and two studies (Macey & Hamari, 2019; Zendle, 2019) converted their questions on engagement into constructs of engagement and were not piloted previously. It is evident that there is little consistency among researchers about what tools should be used in the assessment of IGD and microtransaction engagement. A recent study has developed a promising tool to measure microtransaction engagement (the Problematic Use of Loot Boxes Questionnaire; PU-LB; González-Cabrera et al., 2022) in a Spanish sample to assess factors that underly loot box purchasing behaviours. This work demonstrated sound psychometric properties and may prove useful in further research to better understand what motivates loot box usage. Gambling disorder prevalence rates also varied significantly between studies and were different to general prevalence rates, ranging from .1% to 6.0% (Abbott, 2017) . However, gambling disorder was more consistently assessed across studies when compared to IGD and microtransaction engagement. The PGSI was used in most studies reviewed, with the SOGS-RA and CAGI being appropriately chosen for adolescent populations. Nevertheless, it is likely that the variance in prevalence rates is also attributed to the non-representative samples and selection bias. The current review did not take into consideration other variables that may relate to microtransactions, IGD, and/or gambling disorder such as psychological distress or impulsivity. Therefore, the role of these variables has not been accounted for in the present review. Additionally, the database search was conducted in September of 2020. While it would have been optimal to re-run these searches and integrate newly published studies into the review, this was not possible due to time constraints. Therefore, it is possible that new data is available on this topic which has not been included in this review. Thus, interpretation of the findings reported must take the search date into consideration. The body of evidence reviewed in this paper is largely correlational and therefore causality has not been robustly established regarding the relationships between microtransactions, risky loot box use, IGD, and gambling disorder. Additionally, the sampling methods employed have resulted in non-representative samples across most studies and impacts the external validity of the evidence. Methods of assessing IGD and microtransaction engagement were inconsistent across studies and may account for some of the variance in the evidence. It is important to acknowledge that the validity of the methods used to examine such rapidly evolving technology may also need to be updated to enable greater levels of validity and reliability. Furthermore, while the studies included assessed IGD using diagnostic or screening measures, no studies conducted clinical interviews. Therefore, the current research on this topic measures clinical symptomology and not clinically diagnosed samples. To support the push for policy development, implementation, and better regulation of loot boxes, it is paramount that future studies investigate causality within the relationships between microtransactions and both IGD and gambling disorder as this will encourage and guide policy-makers on what type of regulations may prove beneficial. Additionally, it is important that future research investigate the relationship between microtransactions with IGD and gambling disorder in other cultures to ensure that appropriate recommendations can be made regarding policy and intervention that are culturally and clinically sensitive rather than based solely on Western individualistic populations. Furthermore, researchers should adopt a more consistent and standardised method of assessment for both microtransaction engagement and IGD to ensure accurate measurement of these variables and improve the internal validity of the body of evidence. To this end, new standardised tools assessing disordered gaming according to the latest WHO framework (e.g., the Gaming Disorder Test; see Pontes, 2021) may prove beneficial. To increase the quality of evidence of future studies on this topic, there should be a focus on using sampling methods which are more likely to yield a representative sample, using recruitment techniques that enable insight into non-responders and comparison between nonresponders and responders, reporting all α levels and relevant statistical cut-offs as well as the software packages used, and justifying their sample size to ensure this growing body of research is robust and generalisable. Furthermore, using clinically diagnosed samples may provide a clearer picture on the relationships identified in the present review and improve the overall quality of the evidence. Additionally, it may be beneficial for future reviews to include an inter-rater assessment regarding the quality of the evidence to improve transparency and review quality. Future reviews on this topic should synthesise the evidence on how variables such as psychological distress or impulsivity relate to microtransactions, IGD, and/or gambling disorder not only in a general and broader context but also within subgroups (e.g., male, female). This may provide invaluable information on factors that contribute to this relationship and may also help highlight potential targets for therapeutic intervention. As it is plausible that risky loot box usage may mediate the relationship between microtransactions and IGD and/or gambling disorder, future research should investigate the relationship between risky loot box use and its underlying cognitive distortions and behaviours. These may prove to be important targets for cognitive behavioural interventions (Dong & Potenza, 2014; Yokomitsu et al., 2021) . Future measuring of actual recorded behaviours and applying digital phenotyping principles may yield promising findings, but this clearly needs support from the industry (Montag & Rumpf, 2021) . In conclusion, this review identified a clear positive relationship between microtransactions with IGD and gambling disorder with evidence to suggest that the gambling-like nature of loot boxes may underlie this relationship. Additionally, there is evidence to suggest that the way an individual engages with loot boxes, such as risky loot box use, could mediate or moderate the relationship between microtransactions with IGD and gambling disorder. Furthermore, there is evidence to suggest that adolescents who purchase loot boxes may be at greater risk of developing gambling disorder. It is important to note that these findings are largely correlational in nature and that causality has not been established. The current systematic review also identified a lack of generalisability and cross-cultural validity within the currently body of evidence due to sampling practices and the nature of online surveys resulting in very few representative samples. There is also a clear need for more consistency between researchers in how to assess both IGD and microtransaction engagement. Based on these fundings, the current review calls for policies and industrial changes to be made regarding loot boxes in video games to protect vulnerable individuals from harm. These include MA15+ or R18+ age and 'Simulated Gambling' classifications for video games with loot boxes, transparency of the odds associated with loot box outcomes, and the development of an ethical framework to guide video game production (Australian Parliament House, 2018) . The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper. The epidemiology and impact of gambling disorder and other gamblingrelated harm [Discussion Paper]. World Health Organization Forum on alcohol The Big Five personality traits and online gaming: A systematic review and meta-analysis Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders Gaming micro-transactions for chance-based items Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling Associations between loot box use, problematic gaming and gambling, and gambling-related cognitions An Evaluation of Gaming-Related Harms in Relation to Gaming Disorder and Loot Box Involvement The adolescent brain A descriptive survey of online gaming characteristics and gaming disorder in Ireland A cognitive-behavioral model of Internet gaming disorder: Theoretical underpinnings and clinical implications Development of a critical appraisal tool to assess the quality of cross-sectional studies (AXIS) Video game loot boxes are psychologically akin to gambling The relationship between problem gambling, excessive gaming, psychological distress and spending on loot boxes in Aotearoa New Zealand, Australia, and the United States-A crossnational survey Essential facts about the computer and video game industry The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final Report Metaanalysis of the relationship between problem gambling, excessive gaming and loot box spending Loot boxes in Spanish adolescents and young adults: Relationship with internet gaming disorder and online gambling disorder The benefits of playing video games The spreading impact of playing violent video games on aggression Amusement machine playing in childhood and adolescence: A comparative analysis of video games and fruit machines The therapeutic and health benefits of playing video games A brief overview of Internet Gaming Disorder and its treatment Effects of self-isolation and quarantine on loot box spending and excessive gaming-results of a natural experiment Light Video Game Play is Associated with Enhanced Visual Processing of Rapid Serial Visual Presentation Targets Risk Factors of Problem Gaming and Gambling in US Emerging Adult Non-Students: The Role of Loot Boxes, Microtransactions, and Risk-Taking Predatory monetization schemes in video games (e.g. 'loot boxes') and internet gaming disorder Fortnite microtransaction spending was associated with peers' purchasing behaviors but not gaming disorder symptoms Adolescent simulated gambling via digital and social media: An emerging problem Loot box engagement and problem gambling among adolescent gamers: Findings from a national survey Internet gaming addiction: Current perspectives The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale The relationship of loot box purchases to problem video gaming and problem gambling The prevalence of long-term symptoms of depression and anxiety after breast cancer treatment: A systematic review Investigating relationships between video gaming, spectating esports, and gambling eSports, skins and loot boxes: Participants, practices and problematic behaviour associated with emergent forms of gambling Commentary on: Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. The diagnostic pitfalls of surveys: If you score positive on a test of addiction, you still have a good chance not to be addicted The macro problem of microtransactions: The self-regulatory challenges of video game loot boxes Microsoft Corporation (n.d.). Microsoft Excel in Microsoft 365 Internet and smartphone use disorder in Asia Article 106380 On Corporate Responsibility When Studying Social Media Use and Well-Being Addictive Features of Social Media/Messenger Platforms and Freemium Games against the Background of Psychological and Economic Theories The Potential of Digital Phenotyping and Mobile Sensing for Psycho-Diagnostics of Internet Use Disorders Empirical evidence for robust personality-gaming disorder associations from a large-scale international investigation applying the APA and WHO frameworks Exploring the world of the personal computer The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach Making the Case for Video Game Addiction: Does It Exist or Not? Measurement and Conceptualization of Gaming Disorder According to the World Health Organization Framework: the Development of the Gaming Disorder Test A new era for gaming disorder research: Time to shift from consensus to consistency The interplay between time spent gaming and disordered gaming: A large-scale world-wide study Internet Gaming Disorder: Investigating the Clinical Relevance of a New Phenomenon The role of microtransactions in gaming disorder and gambling disorder: A systematic review. PROSPERO, 2020, Article CRD42020216371 Role of culture in gambling and problem gambling Grand Theft Auto V Space Invaders obsession Problematic Online Behaviors Among Gamers: the Links Between Problematic Gaming, Gambling, Shopping, Pornography Use, and Social Networking Video game training does not enhance cognitive ability: A comprehensive meta-analytic investigation Gaming disorder: Its delineation as an important condition for diagnosis, management, and prevention GRADE handbook for grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendaitons. . The GRADE Working Group Personality and problem gambling: A prospective study of a birth cohort of young adults Junk-time junkies: An emerging addiction among students Loot boxes, problem gambling and problem video gaming: A systematic review and metasynthesis Global prevalence of gaming disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis Effects of micro transactions on video games industry Clinical validation of the C-VAT 2.0 assessment tool for gaming disorder: A sensitivity analysis of the proposed DSM-5 criteria and the clinical characteristics of young patients with 'video game addiction Veritas Health Innovation. (n.d.). Covidence systematic review software Prevalence and correlates of comorbid depression in a nonclinical online sample with DSM-5 internet gaming disorder ROBIS: A new tool to assess risk of bias in systematic reviews was developed Toward the development of an adolescent gambling problem severity scale International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems Characteristics of Gamers who Purchase Loot Box: A Systematic Literature Review Problem gamblers spend less money when loot boxes are removed from a game: A before and after study of Heroes of the Storm Beyond loot boxes: A variety of gambling-like practices in video games are linked to both problem gambling and disordered gaming. PeerJ, 8, Article e9466 Video game loot boxes are linked to problem gambling: Results of a large-scale survey Loot boxes are again linked to problem gambling: Results of a replication study Paying for loot boxes is linked to problem gambling, regardless of specific features like cash-out and pay-to-win Adolescents and loot boxes: Links with problem gambling and motivations for purchase Thank you to Michaela Venn, academic librarian from the University of Tasmania, for her support in developing the search strategy used in this study.