key: cord-1031729-4lwwtjqx authors: Schunk, Fabian; Zeh, Franziska; Trommsdorff, Gisela title: Cybervictimization and well-being among adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: The mediating roles of emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulation date: 2021-09-30 journal: Comput Human Behav DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107035 sha: 4c40986f1bc37893c7f1dec109630c906386d647 doc_id: 1031729 cord_uid: 4lwwtjqx Cybervictimization has been linked to adverse psychological consequences but little is known about the mechanisms linking cybervictimization to lower well-being. We conducted two studies to examine emotional self-efficacy and distinct emotion regulation strategies as potential mediators in the relationship between cybervictimization and lower well-being among German adolescents during the school closures due to the COVID-19 pandemic in early 2020. In Study 1, 107 adolescents (M(age) = 15.76) reported their cybervictimization frequency, emotional self-efficacy beliefs, and aspects of well-being (i.e., self-esteem, perceived social support, and subjective well-being during the COVID-19 related school closures). Emotional self-efficacy mediated the link between cybervictimization and all well-being measures. Specifically, cybervictimization was related to lower well-being through lower self-efficacy for managing negative emotions. For further examination, in Study 2, 205 adolescents (M(age) = 15.45) were asked to report their cybervictimization experiences, use of specific emotion regulation strategies (rumination, reappraisal, and suppression), and well-being (i.e., self-esteem and life satisfaction). Cybervictimization was related to lower well-being through more rumination, but not through reappraisal or suppression. Taken together, our findings suggest that cybervictims may have lower emotional self-efficacy beliefs and engage in more rumination, a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy. These deficits in adolescents’ beliefs and capabilities for effectively managing negative emotions may be accountable for the adverse psychological consequences of cybervictimization. Notably, exploratory analyses suggest that cybervictimization frequency did not increase among adolescents during the lockdown (e.g., homeschooling, social distancing) due to the COVID-19 pandemic. The Mediating Roles of Emotional Self-Efficacy and Emotion Regulation Most adolescents regularly use information and communication technologies (ICT) to communicate with others and to access information online (e.g., for education or entertainment; see Kowalski et al., 2019 , for a review). Despite many advantages of ICT, digital technologies provide new platforms and opportunities for aggressive behavior in the form of cyberbullying (i.e., cyberaggression, cybervictimization) . Extending the definition of traditional bullying, cyberbullying can be defined as "willful and repeated harm inflicted through the use of computers, cell phones, and other electronic devices" (Hinduja & Patchin, 2014, p. 11) . Cyberaggression describes the act of perpetrating cyberbullying, while cybervictimization refers to the exposure to cyberbullying (Turliuc et al., 2020) . Reports on prevalence rates of cybervictimization are mixed due to different conceptualizations and measurements used. Approximately 7% to 18% of adolescents report cybervictimization within the last months, with some studies reporting rates of up to 50% (Kowalski et al., 2019) . In 2020, a survey among 4 418 German students found a cybervictimization rate of 17.3%, marking a remarkable increase compared to a rate of 12.7% in a similar survey three years earlier (Bündnis gegen Cybermobbing, 2020). The COVID-19 pandemic might be responsible for a recent increase in cybervictimization as many people had to move their communication and workplace to the digital world. With school lessons being taught online, adolescents in particular might spend comparatively more time on the internet, facing a higher risk of cybervictimization. Cybervictimization has frequently been shown to be related to negative psychological consequences. Yet, little is known about the coping mechanisms used by cybervictims to respond to the experience of being bullied in cyberspace (Raskauskas & Huynh, 2015) . Specifically, cybervictimization was found to be associated with lower well-being, such as lower life satisfaction, less social support, lower self-esteem, more depressive and somatic symptoms, loneliness, and suicide ideation (Bai et al., 2021; Pabian & Vandebosch, 2021; Urano et al., 2020 ; see also Kowalski et al., 2014 , for a meta-analysis). Victimization from cyberbullying might induce even worse health consequences compared to traditional bullying due to unique characteristics like anonymity and publicity (Wang et al., 2011) . For instance, individuals may feel comparatively more hopeless when targeted by a perpetrator in front of a possibly large online community. Research does indeed show that victims perceive cyberbullying as worse than traditional bullying, especially if the attacks occur publicly and anonymously (Sticca & Perren, 2013) . Furthermore, in contrast to traditional bullying, a single cyberbullying event may develop a repetitive effect as insulting posts stay online and can torment victims for a long time. Considering the high prevalence of cybervictimization among adolescents and little research examining how cybervictims select strategies for coping with such a stressful event, it seems crucial to gain a better understanding of the coping mechanisms linking cybervictimization to lower well-being. Recent studies emphasize the role of emotional selfefficacy (e.g., Heiman et al., 2015) and emotion regulation (e.g., Erreygers et al., 2018; Turliuc et al., 2020) for cybervictimization. In the present research, we conducted two studies to examine emotional self-efficacy and specific emotion regulation strategies as potential mediators in the association between cybervictimization and well-being. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Self-efficacy beliefs refer to people's perceived capabilities to control their lives and accomplish their goals (Bandura, 1997) . Self-efficacy beliefs influence people's motivation to face difficult challenges, their adjustment to situational demands, and their engagement in successful self-regulation (Bandura, 1997; Caprara et al., 2013; Gunzenhauser et al., 2013) . Emotional self-efficacy, in particular, can be described as people's perceived capability to successfully manage their emotions . Among adolescents and young adults, self-efficacy in managing negative emotions was shown to be related to higher self-esteem, less depressive symptoms, higher life satisfaction, more optimism, and less delinquent conduct (Bandura et al., 2003; Caprara & Steca, 2005; Gunzenhauser et al., 2013) . Emotional self-efficacy was found to be associated with less traditional bullying victimization and aggression among adolescents (Özer et al., 2011; Samper-García et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2018) . Similarly, even though research on self-efficacy in cybervictimization is still scarce, cybervictims were shown to have lower general (Wong et al., 2014) and emotional selfefficacy (Heiman et al., 2015) in adolescent samples. Individuals who experience frequent victimization may lose confidence in their ability to control events in their lives and tend to develop lower self-efficacy beliefs. As a result, they might be less likely to use coping strategies if they do not believe in their capability to do so effectively. Consequently, individuals may suffer more negative psychological consequences after experiencing victimization. In line with this assumption, low self-efficacy for using coping strategies has been shown to partially mediate the relationship between traditional bullying victimization and psychological maladjustment in a study among adolescents (Singh & Bussey, 2011) . Emotional self-efficacy might be crucial for coping with victimization and may constitute a valuable protective factor against negative psychological consequences after experiencing victimization. However, to the best of our J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f knowledge, no study has yet examined whether emotional self-efficacy mediates the relationship between cybervictimization and well-being (see also Raskauskas & Huynh, 2015 , for a review). Emotion regulation describes "processes by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express these emotions" (Gross, 1998b, p. 275 ). Gross's process model of emotion regulation distinguishes among distinct emotion regulation strategies which he categorized according to their temporal position during emotion generation. We will focus on three frequently examined emotion regulation strategies, each affecting one of the last three stages (attentional deployment, cognitive change, and response modulation) indicated in the process model (Gross, 1998b) : Rumination (i.e., recurrently directing attention to negative emotional stimuli), reappraisal (i.e., cognitively reevaluating an emotional situation), and suppression (i.e., masking the outward expression of emotions; McRae & Gross, 2020). Emotion regulation is crucial for the development of social competence (Eisenberg et al., 2010; Trommsdorff & Cole, 2011) . Previous research demonstrated different associations with well-being and social functioning for specific emotion regulation strategies (see Joormann & Stanton, 2016 , for a review). Rumination and suppression are frequently described as maladaptive emotion regulation strategies as their use is related to lower well-being, more and more perceived social support (Gross & John, 2003; Schunk, Trommsdorff, & König-Teshnizi, 2021; Schunk, Trommsdorff, Wong, et al., 2021) . Limited research on emotion regulation in cybervictimization among adolescents and young adults demonstrated that cybervictims are more likely to use maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as suppression, self-blame, and rumination (Ak et al., 2015; Arató et al., 2020; Erreygers et al., 2018; Vranjes et al., 2018) . On the other hand, adolescents who experienced no cybervictimization reported more reappraisal . Research on traditional bullying suggests that victimization during adolescence may affect future emotion regulation as the use of suppression among university students has been linked to greater retrospective reports of victimization during high school (Chervonsky & Hunt, 2018) . Children who experience victimization may develop emotion regulation difficulties that negatively affect their social functioning and well-being. In line with this assumption, in a study among children, victimization was shown to be related to social maladjustment (e.g., peer rejection) through the mediation of maladaptive emotion regulation (Schwartz & Proctor, 2000) . Similarly, Gardner et al. (2017) found that children who experienced traditional bullying victimization used less reappraisal. Lower reappraisal, in turn, predicted more maladaptive coping which in turn predicted more school loneliness. The few studies that examined emotion regulation strategies as mediators for the relationship between cybervictimization and well-being measures focused primarily on rumination. Specifically, rumination was found to mediate the positive link between cybervictimization and depressive symptoms among US-American female college students (Feinstein et al., 2014) and Chinese adolescents (Chu et al., 2019) . Rumination was also shown to partially mediate the positive relationship between cybervictimization and somatic symptoms J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f four months later in a study among adolescents (Rey et al., 2020) . These findings support the assumption that rumination functions as a mediator between cybervictimization and aspects of well-being. The function of other emotion regulation strategies (e.g., reappraisal, suppression) remains to be examined. In sum, previous research demonstrated associations of emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulation with victimization. However, only a few studies investigated self-efficacy and emotion regulation in cybervictimization. Deficits in emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulation may explain the negative effects of cybervictimization on well-being. We conducted two complementing studies among adolescent samples aiming to clarify the mediating functions of emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulation in associations between cybervictimization and well-being. We assessed specific aspects of well-being, namely life satisfaction, self-esteem, and perceived social support (Diener et al., 2010) . We also examined subjective well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic (SWB-COV). In Study 1 we tested whether emotional self-efficacy mediates the relationship between cybervictimization and lower well-being (i.e., less self-esteem, less perceived social support, lower SWB-COV). Further, we examined in Study 2 whether specific emotion regulation strategies (i.e., rumination, reappraisal, suppression) mediate the link between cybervictimization and lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction). The following hypotheses were formulated (see Fig. 1 .): H1: Cybervictimization predicts lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, less perceived social support, lower SWB-COV) via lower emotional self-efficacy. H2: Cybervictimization predicts lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction) via more rumination. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f H3: Cybervictimization predicts lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction) via less reappraisal. H4: Cybervictimization predicts lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction) via more suppression. We conducted exploratory analyses to address additional questions of interest. Specifically, our data collection coincided with the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, providing us with the unexpected opportunity to assess how cybervictimization frequency may have changed during this situation (e.g., because of more ICT use due to homeschooling and social distancing). The purpose of Study 1 was to examine whether emotional self-efficacy functions as a mediator for associations between cybervictimization and various aspects of well-being (i.e., self-esteem, perceived social support, SWB-COV). We also conducted additional analyses to examine the unique effects of emotional self-efficacy for the management of specific emotions (e.g., anger, fear). Furthermore, we explored the possible influence of the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., homeschooling, social distancing) on cybervictimization frequency. The initial sample included 120 participants. We excluded thirteen participants as they were outside our previously defined age range of 13 to 18 years (n = 9), did answer less than 50% of the items (n = 1), or did not provide demographic information (n = 3). The final sample consisted of 107 adolescents (Mage = 15.76, SD = 1.38, age range between 13 to 18 years; 64.5% female). Participants were recruited between May and June 2020 from two secondary schools (Gymnasien) in Germany and invited to complete an online survey through the platform SoSci Survey (https://www.soscisurvey.de/). Students attended online classes from home during the survey period due to the COVID-19 pandemic and received the survey invitation via email from their school. Participation was voluntary and parents and students gave informed consent. Cyberbullying. We applied the Mobbing Questionnaire for Students (Jäger et al., 2007) to measure cybervictimization and cyberaggression with four items each. The items cover central areas of bullying (harassment, denigration, outing, and exclusion) according to Willard (2007) . To capture cyberbullying during COVID-19 (i.e., after the German government-enforced school J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f closures), students were asked to rate their experiences with cybervictimization and their own involvement in cyberaggression "during school closure" (1 = never, 2 = once or twice, 3 = two to three times in a month, 4 = once a week, 5 = few times a week). At the time of data collection, schools had been closed for approximately two months. We slightly adjusted the original items by Jäger et al. (2007) to include the intention to harm which is significant in the definition of cyberbullying (e.g., "How many times did someone [did you] spread rumors or defamations about you [others] via the internet/cell phone to hurt you [them]"). Moreover, we asked participants after each item whether they had experienced a certain cyberbullying event less frequently, equally frequently, or more frequently compared to the time before COVID-19 ("before school closure"). This follow-up question was not asked if participants answered an item with "never". We abstained from calculating a score for cyberaggression, which was originally intended to serve exploratory purposes, due to a low Cronbach's alpha of only .05. Emotional self-efficacy. The Multidimensional Negative Emotions Self-Regulatory Efficacy Scale ) was used to assess participant's self-efficacy beliefs in their ability to manage negative emotions (15 items; 1 = not well at all, 7 = very well). The scale consists of five subscales measuring self-efficacy for the management of anger/irritation, despondency/sadness, fear, shame/embarrassment, and guilt. Two fluent English speakers translated the scale into German via backtranslation. Well-being measures. To assess participants' well-being we applied the following measures: We used the revised German version of the Rosenberg Scale (Rosenberg, 1965; German version by von Collani & Herzberg, 2003) to assess participant's self-esteem (10 items; to measure perceived social support by family and friends (8 items; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Furthermore, we applied the World Health Organization Five-Item Well-Being Index (WHO-5; World Health Organization, 1998 ; available at http://www.who-5.org/) to explicitly measure subjective well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic (SWB-COV) by adding "during school closure" as a reference to the instruction (5 items; 1 = at no time, 6 = all of the time). IBM SPSS Statistics was used for data analyses. We estimated missing values with expectation maximization if at least 80% of the items of a scale had been answered. All mediation analyses were conducted using PROCESS version 3.5 (Hayes, 2017 ; model 4) with 5 000 bootstraps. We tested for common method bias which may exist in our study since we only used selfreported data (Podsakoff et al., 2003) . Harman's single-factor test was applied by running an unrotated principal component analysis including all items of major constructs assessed in Study 1. We obtained 12 components with eigenvalues above 1. The largest component explained only 29.68% of the total variance which is far below the traditional threshold of 50%. We can therefore conclude that common method bias is unlikely as no single component accounted for most of the variance. Indicators for the reliability and validity of constructs are presented in Table 1 together with descriptive statistics, including skewness and kurtosis, and partial correlations among study J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f variables. Partial correlations were computed to account for age and gender effects. Specifically, girls reported lower emotional self-efficacy (M = 2.85, SE = 0.07 versus M = 3.18, SE = 0.09; t(102) = -2.69, p = .008) and perceived more social support (M = 6.00, SE = 0.14 versus M = 5.52, SE = 0.19; t(105) = 2.05, p = .043) compared to boys. Age was negatively correlated with SWB-COV (r = -.19, p = .046). Descriptive statistics showed that cybervictimization was nonnormally distributed with a skewness of 3.70 and a kurtosis of 14.73. Skewed data is common in research on aggressive behavior because the highest categories on the scale ("once a week", "few times a week") are less often selected (Einarsen et al., 2009 ; see also Antoniadou et al., 2016; Del Rey et al., 2015; Forssell, 2016; Wang et al., 2019) . Our planned mediation analyses apply a bootstrapping method that is robust against violations of normality and thus suitable for analyzing the data (Hayes, 2017; Preacher & Hayes, 2008) . We calculated Cronbach's alpha and composite reliability to examine internal consistency. Composite reliability was at least .79 for all constructs, suggesting adequate internal consistency (Hair et al., 2017) . Cronbach's alpha was slightly below the common threshold of .70 for cybervictimization (α = .66), self-efficacy in managing anger (α = .64), and self-efficacy in managing guilt (α = .69). The respective items consist of only three or four items which may partly explain the low alpha values. For each of these scales, deleting one item would have only slightly improved alpha values, thus we decided against this step to avoid losing valuable information since alphas were marginally below .70 and composite reliabilities were satisfactory. Discriminant validity was examined by computing the square rooted averaged variance extracted (AVE) and by its comparison with the respective correlations of each construct (Fornell & Larcker, 1981) . Please note that high correlations among overall self-efficacy and its subscales have to be expected since the overall score was calculated through the sub-scores. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Table 1 illustrates that the square roots of the AVE are larger than the inter-construct correlations for each major construct, except for a high correlation between the overall self-efficacy score and self-esteem (rpartial = .60). We calculated the variance inflation factor (VIF) to follow up on some of the high correlations by testing for multicollinearity. All VIF values were lower than 2.00 and thus below the threshold of 5.00 (Hair et al., 2017) , indicating that multicollinearity was no concern. Mediation analyses were conducted to examine whether emotional self-efficacy mediates the link between cybervictimization and well-being (i.e., self-esteem, social support, SWB-COV). We ran a mediation analysis for each of the three outcomes and included age and gender as covariates (see Fig. 2 ). Results revealed a significant indirect effect of cybervictimization on self-esteem through emotional self-efficacy, b = -0.34, 95% CI [-0.49, -0.18], indicating that lower self-efficacy partially mediated the relationship between cybervictimization and lower self-esteem. Specifically, more cybervictimization was related to lower emotional self-efficacy, and lower self-efficacy was related to lower self-esteem. Similarly, emotional self-efficacy partially mediated the negative relationship between cybervictimization and perceived social support, b = -0.33, 95% CI [-0.59, -0.10]. Emotional self-efficacy also mediated the negative link between cybervictimization and lower SWB-COV, b = -0.57, 95% CI [-0.88, -0.29 ]. The direct effect of cybervictimization on SWB-COV became nonsignificant (p = .164), indicating that emotional self-efficacy completely mediated the relationship between cybervictimization and lower SWB-COV. These findings demonstrate the mediating role of emotional self-efficacy beliefs in the relationship between cybervictimization and various aspects of well-being. Hypothesis 1 was thus supported. We followed up on these findings by conducting post-hoc analyses to examine the possibility of gender or age moderating the mediating effect of self-efficacy on the three wellbeing measures. The moderated mediation models were calculated using PROCESS model 7 and included the same variables as the respective mediation models (e.g., age was included as a covariate in the analysis testing gender as a moderator and vice versa). Neither age nor gender moderated the link between cybervictimization and emotional self-efficacy nor did we observe any moderated mediation. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Subscales of emotional self-efficacy. We conducted additional analyses to explore the unique effects of emotional self-efficacy beliefs regarding the management of specific emotions (i.e., subscales of the self-efficacy scale). As can be seen in Table 1 , cybervictimization was negatively related to almost every subscale of emotional self-efficacy (anger/irritation, despondency/sadness, fear, and shame/embarrassment), but unrelated to self-efficacy in managing guilt (p = .804). Fischer's z-tests showed no significant differences in the strengths of the correlations of cybervictimization with self-efficacy in managing anger/irritation, despondency/sadness, fear, or shame/embarrassment, respectively. We Cybervictimization frequency might have changed during the COVID-19 pandemic in light of increased ICT use due to homeschooling and social distancing regulations. We asked participants who reported having experienced cybervictimization whether they had experienced the respective cyberbullying event less frequently, equally frequently, or more frequently compared to the time "before school closure". We obtained 35 answers indicating cybervictimization (counting each of the four items of cybervictimization separately). Of those replies 25.7% indicated experiencing cybervictimization less frequently, 54.3% indicated equally frequent, and 20.0% indicated more frequent cybervictimization during the pandemic/school closure as compared to before. It should be noted that we only asked current cybervictims (during COVID-19/school closure) about their perceived changes in cybervictimization frequency; we do not have data from participants who were victimized before COVID-19 but not during COVID-19. Thus, the percentage indicating fewer cybervictimization might be an underrepresentation of the true decrease of cybervictimization. Despite this limitation and our rather small sample, these exploratory findings suggest that cybervictimization did not occur more frequently during the first few months of the COVID-19 pandemic. Study 1 examined the role of subjective emotional self-efficacy beliefs in cybervictimization. We aimed to add to these findings in Study 2 by examining whether the relationship between cybervictimization and well-being is mediated by specific emotion J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f regulation strategies (i.e., rumination, reappraisal, suppression). Furthermore, we explored whether similar findings will be obtained for traditional bullying victimization as for cybervictimization. The initial sample included 208 participants. We excluded three participants who were older than 18 years. The final sample included 205 high school students (Mage = 15.45, SD = 1.10, age range between 14 to 18 years; 63.9% female, 1.5% diverse) from Germany (n = 182) and Switzerland (n = 23). The small amount of participants with Swiss nationality is explained by the fact that we distributed our survey primarily near the German city Konstanz which is located at the border to Switzerland. We collected data from March to April 2020 by sending out email invitations that directed participants to an online survey on the platform SoSci Survey (https://www.soscisurvey.de/). Students gave informed consent and voluntarily completed the survey. Cyberbullying. We applied the scales by Wong and McBride (2018) to measure the frequency of cybervictimization and cyberaggression during the last three months with 15 items each (1 = never, 2 = once or twice, 3 = two to three times in a month, 4 = about once a week, 5 = few times a week). The items measure whether a person has experienced or carried out cyberaggressive behavior based on several distinguishable cyberbullying types (i.e., relational, pictorial, verbal, and extortion). Items measured relational, pictorial, verbal, and extortion bullying behavior. Cyberbullying and traditional bullying scales were translated to German from the English version and doublechecked by a bilingual speaker. In addition, the author of the original Chinese cyberbullying scales was involved in the translation process and all items were carefully discussed to preserve their meaning (e.g., sometimes the Chinese version used a single word to describe a concept, whereas in German several words were needed). Questionnaire (Ehring et al., 2011) to measure rumination (0 = never, 4 = almost always). Reappraisal and suppression were measured with the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ten items; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree; Gross & John, 2003; German version by Abler & Kessler, 2009 ). We assessed self-esteem with the same measure used in Study 1. Moreover, the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985; German version by Glaesmer et al., 2011) was used to assess life satisfaction (five items; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The analytic strategy was the same as in Study 1. Analogous to Study 1, Harman's single-factor test was used to check for common method bias. The principal component analysis resulted in 17 components with eigenvalues above 1, with the largest component explaining only 28.02% of the total variance. Since no single component accounted for most of the variance, common method bias is unlikely. Descriptive statistics and partial correlations (controlled for age and gender) for Study 2 as well as indicators for the reliability and validity of constructs are given in Table 2 Internal consistencies were satisfactory as Cronbach's alphas and composite reliability scores were above .70 (Hair et al., 2017) , except for suppression with a Cronbach's alpha of .69 which we judged as marginally acceptable. Discriminant validity, as examined by the square rooted AVE, was adequate for most constructs (see Table 2 ). However, we obtained high intercorrelations of cybervictimization with cyberaggression and traditional bullying victimization, respectively. This is in line with previous research which suggests that victims of cyberbullying are often also perpetrators of cyberbullying and victims of traditional bullying (Kowalski et al., 2014 ). An examination of VIF values indicated no multicollinearity issues (VIFs < 3.89). We ran mediation analyses to examine whether emotion regulation strategies mediate the negative relationship between cybervictimization and well-being (i.e., self-esteem, life satisfaction). We simultaneously tested rumination, reappraisal, and suppression as mediators (see Fig. 3 ). Age, gender, and cyberaggression were included as covariates. Due to the high correlation between cybervictimization and cyberaggression (rpartial = .84), we included cyberaggression as a covariate since individuals who are only cybervictims may differ from individuals who are both victims and perpetrators. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f indicating that rumination completely mediated the negative link between cybervictimization and life satisfaction. Hypothesis 2 was thus supported regarding the mediating role of rumination. Hypotheses 3 and 4, however, were rejected as reappraisal and suppression did not function as mediators in any of the relationships. Rumination may play a comparatively more crucial role as rumination affects attentional processes early in the emotion regulation process (Gross, 1998b) . Post-hoc analyses were conducted to examine whether age or gender moderate the We tested whether our findings also apply to traditional bullying by repeating the mediation analyses for traditional bullying victimization (see Fig. 4 ). Analogous to the previous J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f The objectives of this research were to examine the potential mediating functions of emotional self-efficacy and specific emotion regulation strategies (i.e., rumination, reappraisal, suppression) in the relationship between cybervictimization and well-being among adolescents. In support of hypothesis 1, the findings of Study 1 showed that lower emotional self-efficacy in managing negative emotions mediated the link between cybervictimization and lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, less perceived social support, and lower SWB-COV, respectively). Furthermore, results of Study 2 revealed that more rumination (hypothesis 2), but not reappraisal or suppression (hypotheses 3-4), mediated the relationship between cybervictimization and lower well-being (i.e., lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction). Previous studies emphasized the role of emotional processes in cybervictimization by examining whether emotional self-efficacy or adaptive emotion regulation moderate (buffer) the negative association between cybervictimization and impaired well-being (e.g., Turliuc et al., 2020) . Recently, researchers suggested that social-cognitive factors (e.g., self-efficacy) and coping strategies (e.g., emotion regulation) may not only moderate but mediate the link between cybervictimization and psychological consequences (Raskauskas & Huynh, 2015, p. 123) . Moderator and mediator variables are conceptually distinct as "moderator variables specify when certain effects will hold, [whereas] mediators speak to how or why such effects occur" (Baron & Kenny, 1986 , p. 1176 . Testing mediation thus allows examining whether a specific variable accounts for the relationship between a predictor and an outcome (Baron & Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon et al., 2007) . In our case, the mediators emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulation were examined to better understand the processes or mechanisms linking cybervictimization to lower well-being. In support of our assumptions and consistent with past findings on traditional bullying victimization (Singh & Bussey, 2011) , we found that self-efficacy in managing negative emotions mediated the link between cybervictimization and lower self-esteem, less perceived social support, and lower SWB-COV, respectively. Specifically, cybervictimization was related to lower emotional self-efficacy which in turn was related to lower well-being. Notably, cybervictimization was related to lower self-efficacy beliefs in managing several specific emotions, such as anger, sadness, fear, and shame. Only the subscale for self-efficacy in managing guilt was unrelated to cybervictimization. Self-efficacy in managing sadness and shame, in particular, were unique mediators in the link between cybervictimization and lower self-esteem. These findings are in line with focus group analyses suggesting that the negative emotions of adolescent cybervictims play a crucial role in cybervictimization (Dennehy et al., 2020) . Specifically, cybervictims were found to experience extensive shame that discouraged them from reporting cybervictimization or seeking social support. Self-efficacy in managing such negative emotions might be crucial for coping with cybervictimization as self-efficacy beliefs affect the vulnerability to psychological distress (Bandura, 1997; Singh & Bussey, 2011 ). Individuals who have high self-efficacy beliefs are more likely to take adequate actions to cope with negative life events (Bandura, 1997) . People may be unlikely to even initiate emotion regulation if they do not believe in their ability to manage their emotions in a stressful situation. Thus, people's subjective belief in successfully managing negative emotions should be a crucial first step for coping with cybervictimization by initiating effective emotion regulation . The role of emotion regulation in cybervictimization is underlined by our second study which demonstrated that cybervictimization is related to lower well-being through more J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f rumination. This finding is consistent with previous research showing rumination to mediate the relationship between cybervictimization and more depressive symptoms (Chu et al., 2019; Feinstein et al., 2014) . We found, however, no indirect effects of cybervictimization through reappraisal or suppression on well-being. In contrast to reappraisal and suppression, rumination is used early in the emotion regulation process, at the stage of attentional deployment (Gross, 1998a) . Therefore, rumination might be more crucial for the link between cybervictimization and lower well-being than reappraisal or suppression. Previous research found that rumination both mediated and moderated the relationship between cybervictimization and depressive symptoms among Chinese adolescents (Chu et al., 2019) . It should be mentioned that we attempted to replicate this finding by additionally testing rumination as a moderator. However, we obtained no significant moderation, even though the tendency was the same, suggesting that rumination increased the negative effect of cybervictimization on well-being (see supplementary material for details). This absence of the previously reported moderation effect is intriguing. Since Chu et al. (2019) conducted their study in China, the contrasting findings might be explained by the different cultural contexts in which the research took place. For instance, a recent study suggests that cyberbullying is less prevalent among Germans as compared to Hong Kong Chinese and functions differently in these cultures . The moderating role of rumination might be more important in cultures where cyberbullying is more frequent by increasing the negative effect of cybervictimization on well-being. Going back to the distinction by Baron and Kenny (1986) , the moderation analysis by Chu et al. (2019) illustrated for whom cybervictimization is linked to lower well-being, namely for Chinese adolescents with high levels of rumination. While we obtained no moderation, rumination functioned as a mediator in our study, hinting at the why and the J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f mechanism linking cybervictimization to lower well-being. This mediation suggests that rumination might be critical for a decrease in well-being among cybervictims. By definition, rumination is a response to distressing situations (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008) . The pervasive nature and omnipresence of cyberbullying seem almost predetermined to promote rumination (Dennehy et al., 2020) . Specifically, this may take the form of victims repeatedly reading the same tormenting messages or posts. Focusing on the causes and consequences of a cyberbullying event may then induce adverse psychological consequences for the victim. Notably, traditional bullying victimization was also related to lower well-being through more rumination, suggesting that the mediating function of rumination might be similar for traditional and cybervictimization. It should be noted that the causation of the calculated mediation models is questionable as we conducted mediation analyses with cross-sectional data and other untested variables might be responsible for the obtained associations (Fiedler et al., 2018) . Future studies may test our findings using a longitudinal or experimental design. For instance, Rey et al. (2020) showed that rumination mediated the link between cybervictimization and somatic symptoms four months later which supports a causal interpretation of the mediating effect of rumination. An experimental design, such as a double randomization design, is to be preferred. Researchers could conduct two experiments by randomly assigning participants to a cybervictimization manipulation in the first experiment (measuring mediator and outcome) and assigning participants to a mediator manipulation in the second experiment. This method allows differentiating the effect of the predictor on the mediator and the effect of the mediator on the outcome (see Pirlott & MacKinnon, 2016 , for further details). Ethical considerations forbid any "real" cybervictimization from being inflected, but other options are available (e.g., vignettes or asking participants to remember past cybervictimization experiences). Using cyberbullying vignettes (e.g., Machmutow et al., 2012) for assessing self-efficacy and emotion regulation J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f strategies after cybervictimization may also avoid potential biases and misunderstandings related to self-report scales (Brewer & Kerslake, 2015) . For instance, participants might fail to report cybervictimization because they do not remember its occurrence or do not appraise an event as cyberbullying. Since we examined emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulations in two separate studies, we were unable to discuss their associations with each other and their relative importance. An obvious next step for future studies is the examination of both factors in a single process model. A serial mediation model might be tested, with cybervictimization being related to lower emotional self-efficacy, which might be related to more maladaptive emotion regulation, which might then be related to lower well-being. The Transactional Model of Stress and Coping (TMSC; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984 , 1987 ) may serve as a theoretical framework for this purpose as the model describes how individuals' cognitive appraisals (e.g., self-efficacy beliefs) are linked to the selection of coping strategies (e.g., emotion regulation) after experiencing stressful events (e.g., cybervictimization; as suggested by Raskauskas & Huynh, 2015) . However, no study seems to have tested the TMSC in the context of cybervictimization yet, even though the model has been used as a theoretical guideline for coping with cybervictimization by some researchers (e.g., Dennehy et al., 2020; Völlink et al., 2013) . Our research has practical implications for cyberbullying interventions as cybervictims may benefit from programs focusing on building self-efficacy beliefs and emotion regulation skills. A review across 19 studies (including randomized controlled trials) shows that mindfulness-based and cognitive-behavioral interventions are effective for reducing rumination (Querstret & Cropley, 2013) . Notably, the review also revealed that both internet-based and faceto-face delivered interventions are equally effective. Adolescents might be particularly J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f vulnerable for dysfunctional coping with cybervictimization as past research found that adolescents possess a comparatively small repertoire of adaptive emotion regulation strategies (Zimmermann & Iwanski, 2014) . Since adolescents face a high risk of experiencing cybervictimization (Kowalski et al., 2019) , they should be empowered with the confidence and knowledge to effectively regulate their emotions. Emotion regulation might be especially crucial for cybervictimization as cybervictims might have more difficulties in escaping cyberbullying compared to traditional bullying (Vranjes et al., 2017) . Furthermore, Völlink et al. (2013) suggest that cybervictims are more likely to use emotion-focused (versus problem-focused) coping due to their belief that cyberbullying is unchangeable. The possibly higher engagement in emotion-focused coping highlights the need to teach adolescents effective emotion regulation strategies to cope with cybervictimization by counteracting dysfunctional emotion regulation (e.g., rumination). Our research provides support for the mediating functions of emotional self-efficacy and emotion regulation (i.e., rumination) in the relationship between cybervictimization and lower well-being among adolescents. Future studies may test self-efficacy and emotion regulation in a single model and apply an experimental or longitudinal design to examine the effects of cybervictimization on well-being while considering the mediating roles of emotional selfefficacy and emotion regulation. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Emotion regulation questionnaire -Eine deutschsprachige Fassung des ERQ von Gross und John Cybervictimization and cyberbullying: The mediating role of anger, don't anger me! Computers in Human Behavior Development, construct validation and measurement invariance of the Greek cyber-bullying/victimization experiences questionnaire (CBVEQ-G) Cybervictimization and cyberbullying: The role of socio-emotional skills Cyberbullying victimization and suicide ideation: A crumbled belief in a just world Self-efficacy: The exercise of control Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse spheres of psychosocial functioning The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations Cyberbullying, self-esteem, empathy and loneliness Cyberlife III Spannungsfeld zwischen Faszination und Gefahr: Cybermobbing bei Schülerinnen und Schülern [Cyberlife III Tension between fascination and danger: Cyberbullying among students Mastery of negative affect: A hierarchical model of emotional self-efficacy beliefs Affective and social self-regulatory efficacy beliefs as determinants of positive thinking and happiness How adolescents use social media to cope with feelings of loneliness and anxiety during COVID-19 lockdown Emotion suppression and reappraisal associated with bullying involvement and other social outcomes in young adults Rumination mediates and moderates the relationship between bullying victimization and depressive symptoms in Chinese early adolescents Structural validation and cross-cultural robustness of the European Cyberbullying Intervention Project Questionnaire The psychosocial impacts of cybervictimisation and barriers to seeking social support: Young people's perspectives The Satisfaction With Life Scale New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking Measuring exposure to bullying and harassment at work: Validity, factor structure and psychometric properties of the Negative Acts Questionnaire-Revised Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment The interplay of negative experiences, emotions and affective styles in adolescents' cybervictimization: A moderated mediation analysis Rumination mediates the association between cyber-victimization and depressive symptoms Unwarranted inferences from statistical mediation tests -An analysis of articles published in 2015 Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error Exploring cyberbullying and face-to-face bullying in working life -Prevalence, targets and expressions The role of emotion regulation for coping with school-based peer-victimisation in late childhood The German version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS): Psychometric properties, validity, and populationbased norms Internal and external resources as determinants of health and quality of life Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being Self-efficacy in regulating positive and negative emotions: A validation study in Germany A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach Cyberbullying involvement among students with ADHD: Relation to loneliness, self-efficacy and social support Bullying beyond the schoolyard: Preventing and responding to cyberbullying Mobbing bei Schülerinnen und Schülern in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland: Eine empirische Untersuchung auf der Grundlage einer Online-Befragung Examining emotion regulation in depression: A review and future directions Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth A developmental approach to cyberbullying: Prevalence and protective factors Stress, appraisal, and coping Transactional theory and research on emotions and coping Peer victimisation and depressive symptoms: Can specific coping strategies buffer the negative impact of cybervictimisation? Mediation analysis Emotion regulation Rethinking rumination Individual correlates of bullying behaviour in Turkish middle schools Perceived long-term outcomes of early traditional and cyberbullying victimization among emerging adults Design approaches to experimental mediation Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models Assessing treatments used to reduce rumination and/or worry: A systematic review The process of coping with cyberbullying: A systematic review Cyberbullying victimization and somatic complaints: A prospective examination of cognitive emotion regulation strategies as mediators Society and the adolescent self-image Victimization and peer and parents attachment: The mediating effect of regulatory emotional self-efficacy Regulation of positive and negative emotions across cultures: Does culture moderate associations between emotion regulation and mental health? Associations between emotion regulation and life satisfaction among university students from Germany, Hong Kong, and Japan: The mediating role of social support Community violence exposure and children's social adjustment in the school peer group: The mediating roles of emotion regulation and social cognition Peer victimization and psychological maladjustment: The mediating role of coping self-efficacy The ZPID lockdown measures dataset for Germany Is cyberbullying worse than traditional bullying? Examining the differential roles of medium, publicity, and anonymity for the perceived severity of bullying Emotion, self-regulation, and social behavior in cultural contexts The relation between cyberbullying and depressive symptoms in adolescence. The moderating role of emotion regulation strategies Cyber bullying victimization and adolescent mental health: The differential moderating effects of intrapersonal and interpersonal emotional competence Coping with cyberbullying: Differences between victims, bully-victims and children not involved in bullying Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie The dark side of working online: Towards a definition and an Emotion Reaction model of workplace cyberbullying Patterns of cybervictimization and emotion regulation in adolescents and adults Cyber and traditional bullying: Differential association with depression Childhood maltreatment, moral disengagement, and adolescents' cyberbullying perpetration: Fathers' and mothers' moral disengagement as moderators The mediating effect of regulatory emotional self-efficacy on the association between self-esteem and school bullying in middle school students: A cross-sectional study Cyberbullying and cyberthreats: Responding to the challenge of online social aggression, threats, and distress Cyberbullying perpetration and victimization among adolescents in Hong Kong Program effectiveness of a restorative whole-school approach for tackling school bullying in Hong Kong Fun over conscience: Fun-seeking tendencies in cyberbullying perpetration Fun-seeking tendencies and moral disengagement in cyberbullying: An exploratory study among emerging adults from Germany and Hong Kong Wellbeing measures in primary health care: The DepCare Project. WHO, Regional Office for Europe The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support We thank Isabel Brünecke and Peer Homann for their input on the research design and their assistance with data collection.J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f