key: cord-1026192-03ct019o authors: Wnuk, Anna; Oleksy, Tomasz; Maison, Dominika title: The acceptance of Covid-19 tracking technologies: The role of perceived threat, lack of control, and ideological beliefs date: 2020-09-11 journal: PLoS One DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238973 sha: ac240c73613f6a567190c173454a85fcdc6aa830 doc_id: 1026192 cord_uid: 03ct019o New technological solutions play an important role in preventing the spread of Covid-19. Many countries have implemented tracking applications or other surveillance systems, which may raise concerns about privacy and civil rights violations but may be also perceived by citizens as a way to reduce threat and uncertainty. Our research examined whether feelings evoked by the pandemic (perceived threat and lack of control) as well as more stable ideological views predict the acceptance of such technologies. In two studies conducted in Poland, we found that perceived personal threat and lack of personal control were significantly positively related to the acceptance of surveillance technologies, but their predictive value was smaller than that of individual differences in authoritarianism and endorsement of liberty. Moreover, we found that the relationship between the acceptance of surveillance technologies and both perceived threat and lack of control was particularly strong among people high in authoritarianism. Our research shows that the negative feelings evoked by the unprecedented global crisis may inspire positive attitudes towards helpful but controversial surveillance technologies but that they do so to a lesser extent than ideological beliefs. The Covid-19 pandemic (declared by WHO on 11 March 2020) poses an enormous threat to global health, the economy and human well-being. The disease, which first broke out in Wuhan, China in mid-December 2019, has spanned the globe and now exceeds 15,500,000 cases in more than 200 countries, with a death toll reaching 630,000 (as of 24.07.2020). Currently, the USA has become the epicentre of the Covid-19 pandemic, with almost one-third of all cases now being identified in that country. In line with World Health Organization statements that mitigating the impact of Covid-19 should be a top priority for governments, most countries have taken large-scale action to slow down the spread of the disease. Most governments are restricting movement in various ways, from flight cancellations and reinstated border controls to recommending self-isolation for healthy people and enforcing quarantine for all those found to be infected with the virus. In some cases (as in Spain, Italy and France), absolute quarantine was introduced during which leaving home was allowed only for buying medicine or essential products. Large gatherings were forbidden, public events cancelled and school One of the central human motivations is the pursuit and maintenance of personal control [14] . Individuals experiencing a sense of personal control feel that they are effectively able to influence important aspects of their environment and to steer events in their present and future [14, 15] . Seeing the connection between one's efforts and what happens in one's life can provide meaning in an often chaotic world and prevent the unpleasant feeling of randomness [16, 17] As a result, the feeling of control is often associated with numerous benefits, including physical and mental well-being, productivity and happiness (e.g., [18, 19] . On the other hand, a subjective lack of control increases anxiety and depression (e.g., [20, 21] ). The Covid-19 pandemic can certainly be regarded as an extreme threat to the sense of personal control, one even more serious than previous global events linked to a decline in control, such as financial crises (e.g., [22, 23] ). First, Covid-19 poses a threat to one's very existence (or that of relatives), a kind of threat that, according to much research, is deeply interrelated with a lack of control [17, 24] . The epidemic seems to be difficult to counteract, either by individual actions (for example, people cannot be sure that those they meet are not infected), governments (as the public is witnessing ongoing disputes over the most effective ways to counter the epidemic and its consequences) or science (the epidemiology of the virus is still not fully understood, and cautious forecasts indicate that at least a year's work is needed to develop a vaccine) [25] . Together with financial concerns, incomplete access to information and the indefinite necessity of isolation and social distancing, the above factors may lead to a generalised sense of uncontrollability. Numerous studies have shown that control deprivation inspires attempts to restore it, either by increased personal effort (i.e., primary control) [26] or by seeking sources of external control (e.g., by believing in external powers) [17, 26, 27] . In a time of crisis of the magnitude of the Covid-19 pandemic, people may be even more desperate to reduce the unpleasant feeling of uncontrol via indirect measures, for example, by relying on social norms [28] or endorsing in-group membership [29, 30] . The potential external source of control is often the government, an institution that represents one's own group and has the capacity to restore the order and predictability of the world. Kay et al. [17] show that people exhibit an increased tendency to rely on social systems when their levels of personal control are low. This compensatory control model assumes that formal social systems, such as governments, can provide 'rules, guidelines, norms and structures' as religious systems do, which can help people in their quest for control. According to the authors, however, this way of regaining control could be related to increased acceptance of governmental control, which could be particularly visible during crises [14, 28] . The above-mentioned studies demonstrate that, when personal control is threatened, people tend to restore their sense of certainty by adhering to those in power and those with agency (e.g., government and an interventionist God). It is plausible that the acceptance of new types of surveillance technology constitutes an example of an indirect tool for regaining a sense of control. Many such technologies enable the establishment of strict rules that are hard to evade, and, therefore, they may fulfil the need for guidelines and structures. Their advanced technology may also be perceived as ensuring success in the fight against the pandemic. However, so far no research has examined whether lack of personal control would be related to acceptance of measures of control that also pose a threat to privacy. Perceptions of a lack of personal control can be strongly interconnected with the justified concerns experienced by individual during a crisis, e.g., worrying about one's own life and livelihood or the safety of loved ones [30, 31] . Such concerns are common experiences during the Covid-19 pandemic [32, 33] and can be classified as realistic threats, defined as general concerns for one's well-being [34] . While realistic threats are often related to a personal lack of control, they reflect diverse needs. The first need relates to the immediate risk of losing important resources (including one's life) and the second to the threat of being unable to cope with the situation (see, e.g., [31] ). Research has shown that perceived threats predict attitudes (e.g., [32, 35, 36] ), so we examined whether and how the threat of Covid-19 inspires greater support for potential surveillance technologies. Previous studies on the consequences of perceived threats on attitudes towards civil rights, conducted mainly in the context of terrorist attacks, have shown that people under threat are more likely to accept restrictions on democratic procedures and to exchange their rights for security (see, e.g., [37, 38] ). For example, the perception of a threat that a person or a loved one could die in a terrorist attack was shown to increase support not only for stricter methods of dealing with terrorists [38, 39] but also for the harsher punishment of social norm breakers [40] , increased surveillance [41] and limiting the freedom of speech [42] . On the basis of previous findings, we hypothesised that the level of personal threat related to the Covid-19 pandemic would inspire greater support for the implementation of anti-epidemic technologies that involve a trade-off between civil rights and security. People's reactions to surveillance have also been associated with their ideological views, specifically with adherence to authoritarianism. Right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) is considered to be an ideological response intended to reduce high levels of perceived threat and anxiety [43, 44] , and it has been shown that a perceived terrorist threat predicts stronger support for surveillance among authoritarian people [45] . RWA is also one of the key psychological variables that explain support for increased governmental control and surveillance. It is defined as a submission to authority, adherence to traditional views and hostile behaviour towards groups that violate legitimised rules. Previous research has shown that RWA is positively related to acceptance of human rights restrictions and counterterrorism surveillance measures [45] [46] [47] [48] [49] [50] ; for a review see [51] . Authoritarian people may thus be more inclined to support surveillance technologies because they are usually introduced as government-led solutions and, as such, promote compliance with norms and obedience to authority. Previous outcomes suggest that RWA beliefs should strengthen the relationship between perceived personal threat and lack of control and a person's attitude towards surveillance technology. Thus, we tested whether this relationship is particularly strong amongst people high in authoritarianism. When considering the individual factors that may predict attitudes towards such technological solutions, individual endorsement of liberty and freedom must also be taken into account (see e.g., [45, 52] ). Research suggests that, under threat, people are generally more willing to accept restrictions on their freedom (e.g., personal emails being scanned and phone calls being recorded) to enjoy a sense of security [50, 52, 53] and that, if they value security, they are not concerned about privacy (see also [54] ). On the other hand, people who are characterised by a strong endorsement of individual liberty often reject other moral considerations, meaning they will resolve the conflict between security and civil rights unequivocally in favour of the latter (see, e.g., [55] ). Cohrs, Kielmann, et al. [45] also show that a strong desire for personal freedom and autonomy negatively predict endorsement of restraints on civil liberties. Thus, we hypothesised that endorsement of liberty would be strongly negatively associated with support for potential surveillance technologies during the Covid-19 pandemic. We also considered the role of political views (liberal or conservative), which have been studied as predictors of attitudes towards restrictions on rights and civil liberties [49, 52] . Political conservatism, which is closely associated with RWA, has already been shown to be related to a willingness to restrict civil liberties and to attitudes against freedom of movement [56] [57] [58] . Because gender is correlated with risk taking and protective behaviour [59] [60] [61] and age is related to attitudes towards new technologies [62, 63] , in both studies, we examined our models with gender and age as control variables. In this research, we examined the feelings related to the Covid-19 pandemic and ideological beliefs as predictors of support for the surveillance technologies that are currently being discussed as posing a potential threat to individual rights. To our knowledge, this is the first research that examines whether factors related to the pandemic situation (perceived personal threat and lack of control) are stronger predictors of surveillance technologies than individual factors related to personal values and ideological beliefs. We conducted two studies among Polish citizens. In Study 1 we tested a hypothesis that lower sense of personal control would be associated with an increased acceptance of such technologies because they have the potential to restore order and normality and we also expected that a high personal threat would be positively related to support for such technologies because they might sufficiently reduce the risk of being infected. In Study 2, we intended to replicate the results of Study 1 and furthermore we tested the role of ideological beliefs, RWA and individual endorsement of liberty, in predicting the acceptance of surveillance technologies, hypothesising that RWA would be positively and endorsement of liberty negatively related to the acceptance of such measures. We also tested whether feelings related to the pandemic would be still valid predictors of acceptance of surveillance technologies when RWA and endorsement of liberty were included in the model. In both studies, we also controlled for political views. In these two studies, we used a different operationalization of the dependent variable. In Study 1, due to a lack of space, we needed to use a single item measure while, in Study 2, we developed a longer scale to measure attitudes towards the tracking technologies counteracting the pandemic. The studies were approved by the Scientific Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw. In the first study, using a correlational design, we tested the extent to which perceived level of personal control regarding the coronavirus pandemic was related to support for radical measures for counteracting the pandemic. The study was conducted in Poland, where the first case of Covid-19 was diagnosed on March 4. On March 12, the Polish government announced the closure of schools, shopping malls and restaurants and recommended social distancing. The study was done from March 13 through March 15, during the period in which the first restrictions were introduced. 7.1.1 Participants and procedure. Study 1 was a part of broader research conducted on a nationwide sample (N = 1,046) via an online research panel. The panel's participants were volunteers who participate in surveys for small material rewards). We used quota sampling with quotas based on the gender, age and size of place of residence of the general Polish adult population (aged . Women accounted for 52% of the sample, and the mean age was 44.35 years (SD = 14.63). All the participants provided informed consent to take part in the research by clicking the button "I agree" after reading information about the study. 7.1.2 Measures. Support for radical measures to counteract the pandemic. We asked the participants to indicate their agreement with a single face-valid item: 'Authorities should have the right to control with sensors and applications the movement of citizens' (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). Lack of personal control was measured by three items referring to the coronavirus pandemic, e.g., 'The coronavirus outbreak has made me feel less in control in my life' (see also [64] ). The participants were asked to answer on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree) (α = .77). We used the mean value of three items in the analyses. Personal threat was measured by two items related to the personal risk of being infected by coronavirus, e.g., 'I'm afraid I could get infected and get sick'. Participants answered 'yes' or 'no' to these questions. We used the mean value of two items in the analyses. We controlled for moral conservatism, which was measured by seven items on attitudes towards abortion, euthanasia, same-sex marriages, in-vitro procedures and contraception usage. The participants responded on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree) (α = .90). The measure was based on the scale previously used in Maison, [65, 66] . The full scales are provided in the S1 Appendix. The correlations between the main variables of both studies are presented in Table 1 . Both lack of control and moral conservatism were positively correlated with support for radical measures to counteract the pandemic. The results of the multiple regression analysis showed that both personal threat and lack of personal control predicted support for radical measures to counteract the pandemic The results did not change significantly when controlled for moral conservatism, gender and age. Moral conservatism and age were also significant predictors of support for tracking technologies (see Table 2 ). Study 1 confirmed our hypothesis that personal threat and lack of personal control are positively related to higher support for technologies that track citizens to fight the spread of coronavirus. In the second study, we examined whether the two predictors included in Study 1 as well as ideological views, such as RWA and endorsement of liberty, would significantly predict attitudes towards surveillance technologies to counteract the pandemic. The acceptance of Covid-19 tracking technologies Study 2 was a part of broader research conducted in Poland from 19 through 24 March during a period in which further cases of Covid-19 were diagnosed. On March 20, the state of epidemic was introduced in Poland, giving the state authorities new entitlements. Free movement was gradually restricted, and private gatherings were banned. The government also introduced the possibility of using an application to monitor compliance with the home quarantine. The application, which uses geo-location and facial recognition algorithms, became mandatory on April 1. We intended to recruit at least 1,000 people before March 24 because, the next day, the Polish government planned to introduce further restrictions. In the end, 1,680 persons (74% women) participated in an online study conducted on Facebook. The information about our research was distributed on several Facebook pages and groups. The full text of the Facebook post is included in the S1 Appendix. Participants were offered a possibility of remuneration (participation in a drawing of five vouchers worth c.a. $13). The sample consisted of people aged 18-69 (M = 26.20, SD = 6.95). All the measures were presented in randomised order and some participants withdrew before the end of questionnaire which is why the number of participants differs for various measures. All the participants provided informed consent to take part in the research by clicking the button "I agree" after reading information about the study. Because no existing measure could be found that assessed support for surveillance technologies related to the current pandemic, we designed items based on solutions that have been introduced in several countries. The measure consisted of five items related to surveillance technologies, e.g., 'Surveillance cameras with an automatic facial recognition system to quickly identify persons who do not comply with the authorities' recommendations' (α = .78). We asked the participants to indicate their attitude towards these technological solutions on a scale from 1 (It definitely shouldn't be introduced in Poland) to 7 (It definitely should be introduced in Poland). We used the mean value of five items in the analyses. Lack of personal control was measured using the same scale as in Study 1 (α = .68). Personal threat was measured with three items related to the personal risk of being infected and becoming ill, e.g., 'I consider the risk of personally getting Covid-19 to be high' (α = .66). We used the mean value of three items in the analyses. The acceptance of Covid-19 tracking technologies Endorsement of liberty was measured with one item: 'The freedom to do what we want is more important than following the recommendations of the authorities' designed for the purpose of this study. The participants responded on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Right-wing authoritarianism was measured with four items related to authoritarian aggression and submission based on Funke's scale [67] e.g., 'What our country really needs is a strong, determined leader who will crush evil and take us back to our true path' (see also [68] ). We decided to include questions related to submission and authorities because conservatism was measured separately (as political views). Participants responded on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) (α = 0.66). We used the mean value of four items in the analyses. Political views were measured with two items: 'What are your moral views?' and 'What are your economic views?' (see e.g., [69, 70] .Participants responded on a scale from 1 (conservative) to 7 (liberal) for moral views and on a scale from 1 (liberal) to 7 (social) for economic views. The full scales are provided in the S1 Appendix. A confirmatory factor analysis in MPLUS (version 7, [71] ) showed that attitudes towards surveillance technologies, RWA, perceived threat, and lack of control represent empirically distinct constructs (we excluded endorsement of liberty from this analysis since it was measured by one item). A model with four latent factors fitted the data well, χ 2 (84) = 317.57, p < .001, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.93, SRMR = 0.04. The results of principal axis EFA also revealed four factors accounting for 17.96%, 13.23%, 12.85% and 11.99% of variability. The correlations between the main variables of both studies are presented in Table 3 . Perceived lack of control, personal threat and RWA were positively correlated and endorsement of liberty negatively correlated with support for surveillance technologies. Political views were not related to attitudes towards these technologies. The results of the multiple regression analysis demonstrated that both personal threat and lack of control were significant predictors of attitudes towards surveillance technologies, but personal threat seemed to be a stronger predictor. When RWA and endorsement of liberty were added, RWA appeared to be the strongest predictor of acceptance of such technologies (change in R 2 was significant at p < .001). The main pattern of results remained the same when we added political views as control variables. Gender was The acceptance of Covid-19 tracking technologies significantly related to attitudes towards surveillance technologies with women being more in favor of them than men. See Table 4 for the exact coefficients. To test our hypotheses about moderating role of RWA we used macro PROCESS, model 1. In line with predictions, the relation between lack of control and attitudes towards surveillance was strengthen by RWA, B = 0.04, SE = 0.02, p = 0.02 (assuming RWA at + 1 SD: B = 0.19; SE = 0.03; p < .001; assuming RWA at −1 SD: B = 0.08; SE = 0.03; p = .01; see Fig 1) . We further used the Johnson-Neyman technique to probe for interaction and to identify ranges of values of the moderator for which the interaction effect is significant. It showed that lack of control significantly predicted acceptance of surveillance technology for level of RWA higher than 1.84. Similarly, RWA and personal threat interacted negatively, B = 0.04, SE = 0.02, p = 0.04. Personal threat was positively related to support for surveillance technologies among those The acceptance of Covid-19 tracking technologies participants who declared high level of RWA (assuming RWA at + 1 SD: B = 0.27; SE = 0.04; p < .001). The relation between perceived threat and support for surveillance technologies for people low on RWA was weaker (assuming RWA at −1 SD: B = 0.17; SE = 0.04; p < .001; see Fig 2) . The results of Study 2 confirm our main hypotheses that both feelings related to the pandemic and ideological views are significantly correlated with support for surveillance technologies; however, ideological views appear to be stronger predictors than variables related to the pandemic. The sudden outbreak of the coronavirus pandemic has led to high levels of uncertainty in society. Decisions on how to proceed in handling the situation are made on an ongoing basis, often without access to sufficient data. Almost every week, important decisions are made that affect the everyday lives of citizens who suddenly find themselves in a new reality with remote work and schooling and restrictions on movement. This causes feelings of uncertainty and lack of control and thus a natural desire to overcome that state (see e.g., [3] ). Additionally, perceptions of one's own lack of control can be accompanied by justified concerns about one's life and health. Our research examined the extent to which situational perception of lack of control and the individual threat of being infected and ideological beliefs (authoritarianism and endorsement of liberty) were associated with greater acceptance of technological solutions that both aim to mitigate the coronavirus pandemic and simultaneously pose a potential threat to privacy and other civil rights. We argued that, in line with the compensatory control model [17] , such technologies by their very nature can help to restore the impression that there are strict standards and rules and a need to comply with them. Thus, these restrictive measures can help in dealing with the uncertain and dynamically changing situation of a pandemic, particularly if someone perceives a lack of personal control. Moreover, based on previous research on terrorist threats [45, 50] , we expected that a higher personal threat would be related to greater support for technological surveillance. Our results show that, although both lack of control and perceived threat significantly predict the acceptance of surveillance technologies, perceived threat had a greater explanatory value, while ideological beliefs were included. It seems, therefore, that a perceived realistic threat to life or health during a pandemic is a more important predictor of acceptance of potentially helpful yet controversial technologies than a sense of uncertainty and unpredictability in times of crisis. These results extend the findings of Kay et al. [17] to two new contexts: (1) modern technology as a potential new method for indirect restoration of control but posing a threat to privacy and (2) the extraordinary global crisis. The second main objective of our study was to investigate how the acceptance of surveillance technologies is related to ideological beliefs: RWA and the endorsement of liberty. RWA and the endorsement of liberty were both associated (whether positively or negatively) with acceptance of these technologies, and both were stronger predictors than the characteristics related to the Covid-19 pandemic. In line with previous studies, which have shown that those who appreciate freedom are particularly guided by that value in assessing moral issues [55] , high endorsement of liberty predicted less positive attitudes towards the implementation of surveillance technologies. In turn, RWA was a positive predictor of acceptance of these technologies as in previous studies on the possible trade-off between civil rights and security under terrorist threats [51] . Moreover, as previous research has shown that RWA is particularly related to the need for safety and risk minimisation [43, 44] , we examined whether RWA moderated the relationship between both perceived threat and lack of control and acceptance of surveillance technologies. These two relationships were indeed particularly apparent among people high in authoritarianism, who, when feeling a lack of control or a coronavirus-related threat, have more positive attitudes towards such technologies. In Study 2, we also found that women are more likely than men to accept surveillance technologies. This is in line with evidence in the literature showing that women are usually risk averse and take more precautions than men [59, 60] . A recent study has shown, for example, that women more than men intended to wear a face mask during the Covid-19 pandemic and that men believe it is shameful to wear a face covering [61] . However, while this effect was seen in Study 2, it did not appear in Study 1, which featured a more balanced group of participants in terms of gender and age. It seems, though, that young men (who were the majority of male participants in Study 2) may feel that needing any measure to protect them from coronavirus is a sign of weakness (see [61] for a similar effect). To sum up, our studies show that variables related to the situational context (such as situational lack of control and perceived threat to individual health and life) significantly predict the acceptance of technologies used to combat the pandemic; nevertheless, the strongest predictors were variables related to more stable individual features, i.e., RWA and the endorsement of liberty. To our knowledge, this study represents the first research on the predictors of acceptance of surveillance technology used to mitigate the Covid-19 pandemic and fills the gap in the existing research on crises, showing that individual ideological beliefs are stronger predictors of attitudes towards surveillance than variables related to threat to life and uncontrollability of the situation. In contrast to previous studies that focused mostly on terroristic threats, our study focused on the danger of a much larger scale and less controllable event, namely, a global pandemic. The research was ecologically valid as we asked about technologies that have already been or have begun to be implemented in other countries. Moreover, by conducting the research at two time points (shortly after the first major restriction and 10 days later), we showed that the expected relationships between our variables are stable (however, it is important to note that this was at a relatively early stage of the pandemic in Poland). Future studies are needed to further develop the results. First, it would be important to determine how far citizens are willing to accept such technological solutions even after the pandemic and, in particular, whether getting used to their presence during a 'state of exception' has a mitigating effect on possible concerns in the future. Additionally, our results can provide the basis for important experimental studies. In particular, it is worth investigating whether the experimental manipulation of the sense of control has an impact on increased acceptance of new technologies that potentially threaten privacy. Another interesting study would involve verifying the extent to which acceptance of these surveillance technologies depends on presenting them as temporary or permanent. We should stress some limitations of this research. First, both our studies were correlational, so they do not establish causal relationships between the variables; however, it seems more plausible that lack of control, personal threat, RWA and endorsement of liberty predicted support for radical measures counteracting the pandemic than the reverse (see e.g., Carriere, 2019) . Second, we did not measure the perceived efficacy of these surveillance technologies, and it would be worth examining directly whether the introduction of such solutions would, in fact, restore the sense of personal control and reduce the sense of threat. Our study differed in terms of sample characteristics, Study 1 was based on a representative sample of Polish and in Study 2, women and young people were overrepresented, so replication in a more gender-and age-balanced sample is required, however, we have shown that the main relationships between IVs and DVs hold true over and above the effects of gender and age. Another limitation of our studies is that some variables were measured by relatively short scales. It is possible that these results are more representative for countries that have not yet introduced such technological measures to fight the coronavirus pandemic. Future research could include citizens from countries that have already used such technologies and could also compare states with stricter (e.g., China, Taiwan) and more relaxed (e.g., Sweden) policies regarding the coronavirus pandemic. Epidemiologists warn against the next wave of the Covid-19 pandemic and future similar epidemic crisis [72, 73] . We may expect that many countries will be willing to introduce tracking technologies to counteract the epidemics, hence, understanding people's attitudes towards these technologies seems extremely important. Our studies suggest that negative feelings evoked by the Covid-19 pandemic may not be as important predictors of these attitudes as people's general viewpoints on submission to authority and civil freedoms. COVID-19: Commission sets out European coordinated response to counter the economic impact of the Coronavirus COVID-19 tax relief: a snapshot of what's out there. The Conversation 3 ways the coronavirus pandemic is changing who we are. The Conversation 10 Covid-busting designs: spraying drones, fever helmets and anti-virus snoods. The Guardian As coronavirus surveillance escalates, personal privacy plummets. The New York Times Response to COVID-19 in Taiwan: big data analytics, new technology, and proactive testing Coronavirus contact-tracing apps: can they slow the spread of COVID-19? Nature How Much Privacy Would You Give Up to Stay Healthy?-The Atlantic Quantifying SARS-CoV-2 transmission suggests epidemic control with digital contact tracing Technology threatens human rights in the coronavirus fight. The Conversation Israeli malls, required to monitor shoppers in the battle against coronavirus, fear losing traffic-Business-Haaretz State of Exception Law in a Time of Emergency: States of Exception and the Temptations of 9/11. U Compensatory control: Achieving order through the mind, our institutions, and the heavens Extending control perceptions to the social self Perceived control qualifies the effects of threat on prejudice God and the government: testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems Your place or mine? Organizational identification and comfort as mediators of relationships between the managerial control of workspace and employees' satisfaction and well-being Long-term effects of a control-relevant intervention with the institutionalized aged Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation A guide to constructs of control The relationship between control, job seeking, and well-being in unemployed people The enemy between us: The psychological and social costs of inequality The role of control motivation in mortality salience effects on ingroup support and defense Covid-19 is forcing us to dwell in uncertainty. History can help.-The Washington Post Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control Compensatory control and the appeal of a structured world Evidence for the specificity of control motivations in worldview defense: Distinguishing compensatory control from uncertainty management and terror management processes The power of we: Evidence for group-based control The great recession and groupbased control: Converting personal helplessness into social class in-group trust and collective action Collective reactions to threat: Implications for intergroup conflict and for solving societal crises Measuring two distinct psychological threats of COVID-19 and their unique impacts on wellbeing and adherence to public health behaviors Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response Anxiety in intergroup relations: A comparison of anxiety/ uncertainty management theory and integrated threat theory Gendering human rights: Threat and gender perceptions as predictors of attitudes towards violating human rights in asymmetric conflict. Peace and Conflict Studies Consequences of the 2008 financial crisis for intergroup relations: The role of perceived threat and causal attributions Terrorism as a form of violence. Vazsonyi AT, Flannery D, & DeLisi M: The Cambridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression The political consequences of perceived threat and felt insecurity Authoritarianism, threat, and Americans' support for the war on terror Terror salience and punishment: Does terror salience induce threat to social order? Perceptions of threat, national representation, and support for policies and procedures to protect the national group. Collateral damage: The psychological consequences of America's war on terrorism Perceptions and attitudes following a terrorist shock: Evidence from the UK Flight into security: A new approach and measure of the authoritarian personality The politics of fear: Is there an ideological asymmetry in existential motivation? Effects of right-wing authoritarianism and threat from terrorism on restriction of civil liberties Perceived Threat, and Human Rights Attitudes in US Law Students: A Brief Look Right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation: As mediators of worldview beliefs on attitudes related to the war on terror Belief, motivational, and ideological correlates of human rights attitudes Do Americans care about human rights? Relationships between right-wing authoritarianism, terrorism threat, and attitudes towards restrictions of civil rights: A comparison among four European countries Threats to human rights: A general review Personal values and attitudes toward war Determinants of human rights attitudes and behavior: A comparison and integration of psychological perspectives Assessing Dimensions of the Security-Liberty Trade-off in the United States Understanding libertarian morality: The psychological dispositions of self-identified libertarians Globalization and a conservative dilemma: Economic openness and retributive policies Race and the response of state legislatures to unauthorized immigrants The ideological effects of framing threat on immigration and civil liberties Internet social network communities: Risk taking, trust, and privacy concerns. Computers in human behavior Strong evidence for gender differences in risk taking The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission Understanding age differences in PDA acceptance and performance Age differences in technology adoption decisions: Implications for a changing work force. Personnel psychology Who's to blame? Causal attributions of the economic crisis and personal control The psychological perspective in financial behavior Values and Lifestyle Segmentation of Poles 55+ and its Implications for Consumers' Attitudes and Behaviour. Management and Business Administration The dimensionality of right-wing authoritarianism: Lessons from the dilemma between theory and measurement Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism Attitudes Towards Places Associated With Communism Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology MPlus: statistical analysis with latent variables-User's guide World Economic Forum. Outbreak Readiness and Business Impact Planning for the next global pandemic