key: cord-1002427-cit3l3fv authors: WAMSLER, STEFFEN; FREITAG, MARKUS; ERHARDT, JULIAN; FILSINGER, MAXIMILIAN title: The pandemic and the question of national belonging: Exposure to covid‐19 threat and conceptions of nationhood date: 2022-02-17 journal: Eur J Polit Res DOI: 10.1111/1475-6765.12515 sha: 189edbf9a2296bb9bc11ec8e96167e28ec1aa20a doc_id: 1002427 cord_uid: cit3l3fv Drawing on the behavioural immune system hypothesis, we argue that the prevalence of the Covid‐19 pandemic threat in an individual's respective environment relates to exclusive, ethnic conceptions of nationhood. Referring to the affective intelligence theory, we maintain that specific negative emotions are prompted by the perception of being exposed to a pandemic threat, and these emotional states in turn structure political preferences regarding national belonging. Using an original survey in six European countries during the first peak of the pandemic in late April and early May 2020, we analyze both the impact of individual Covid‐19 experiences and the contextual exposure to a pandemic threat through hierarchical analyses of 105 European regions. Our empirical analysis shows that exposure to the pandemic is linked to stronger ethnic national identities for both levels of analysis. We also find that anger substantially mediates this relationship and has primacy over feelings of fear. Taken together, our results indicate that the behavioural immune system appears as a pervasive obstacle to inclusive orientations. By mid-February 2022, around 405 million people have been infected with the novel coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 (Johns Hopkins University, 2022). 1 With the rapid spread of the virus and the decisive shutdown of social and economic activities around the globe, citizens have experienced large-scale exposure to the consequences of such a pandemic not only in terms of risks to their own health and that of their loved ones but also with regard to economic recession and a compartmentalization of everyday life enforced by social distancing measures. Previously open borders regularly crossed by people were closed within days or even hours. Across the globe, political leaders and epidemiologists relied on restricting access to countries for outsiders, sometimes banning citizens from foreign nations. At the same time, out-group negativity rose significantly as demonstrated by anti-Asian hate speech and crime (Dhanani & Franz, 2020; Reny & Barreto, 2020) or the use of racial slurs to denote the origins or particular variants of the virus targeted against for example, Brazil, China, India, South Africa or the United Kingdom (van Bavel et al., 2020) . In that sense, the exact nature of setting criteria for belonging to national in-and out-groups grew increasingly important. This is the starting point of our investigation. We want to find out whether exposure to the Covid-19 pandemic threat is reflected in our understanding of national belonging. To evaluate the impact of exposure to the threat posed by the Covid-19 pandemic on conceptions of nationhood, we refer to the behavioural immune system (BIS) hypothesis, also known as the parasite stress theory (Murray & Schaller, 2016; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . The behavioural immune system hypothesis links the prevalence of disease-causing parasites to an increased avoidance of unfamiliar out-group targets and to a strengthened cohesion with close in-group targets in order to inhibit contact with pathogens (Ackerman et al., 2018, p. 3 ). 2 By focusing on how boundaries between the in-group and out-group(s) are drawn, we study conceptions of nationhood as a major embodiment of group membership across the globe (Ariely, 2018; Greenfeld & Eastwood, 2007; Schatz et al., 1999) . Given that nation states were the main institutions for combating especially at the onset of the pandemic, nations should also provide the most relevant in-group in this particular context. 3 Further, we are interested in the mechanisms linking the threat of the pandemic to conceptions of nationhood. Accordingly, we tie together insights of the behavioural immune system hypothesis with the affective intelligence theory (AIT), a common model of emotional processing in political science and sociology (Marcus et al., 2000) . We argue that emotions triggered by exposure to the Covid-19 pandemic threat shape how citizens form their conceptions of nationhood. Using an original survey in six European countries (France, Germany, Italy, Spain, Switzerland and the United Kingdom) during the first peak of the pandemic in late April and early May 2020, we analyze the impact of both individual threat experience and exposure to the pandemic at the regional level through hierarchical analyses of 105 European regions. Our empirical analysis, which combines both comparative and within-country models, shows that exposure to the pandemic is indeed linked to stronger ethnic conceptions of nationhood. This effect proves to be conclusive for both levels of analysis (individual and contextual). We also find that anger is a substantial mediator of this relationship and has primacy over feelings of fear, which reflects other recent findings in political psychology referring to emotional responses to threat . Overall, this study contributes to existing research in several ways. First, in the absence of realworld data from global pandemics, direct measurements of health threat exposure by infections are missing. In order to map pathogen exposure, previous studies have focused on the experimental design of (artificially) constructed health threats, exposure to disgust experiences, the creation of macrolevel indices of pandemic exposure based on parasite presence in a society or fatalities caused by infectious diseases (Albertson & Gadarian, 2015; Faulkner et al., 2004; Murray & Schaller, 2016; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014 Tybur et al., 2014) . Consequently, our approach of directly operationalizing Covid-19 exposure puts pandemic threat as such into focus and, for the first time, makes it possible to evaluate how direct perceptions of threat from an infectious disease on a global scale affect definitions of group membership, particularly conceptions of nationhood. Second, while existing theories mostly aim to account for the impact of fear as one single threat-oriented emotion on political attitudes, we build on the insights of functional neuroscience perspectives and the AIT and scrutinize the role of fear and anger as two pivotal emotions activated simultaneously, yet distinctly, in threatening situations. Third, while most studies referring to emotional responses to threatening stimuli rely on single country studies, we provide a fitting analysis with a rich, comparative dataset to study the effects of the Coronavirus pandemic. Finally, we offer a theoretical argument for linking pandemic exposure with views on belonging to inand out-groups that has only tentatively been touched upon by previous political science research. Consequently, the present study adds a likely crucial explanatory variable to the research field of national identity, which mainly focuses on the impact of issues like globalization, populism or social status (Ariely, 2018; Inglehart & Norris, 2017; Kriesi et al., 2006) . Indeed, including pathogen avoidance in previous explanations of political values and behaviours has far-reaching implications for our understanding of societal processes since predisposed, evolutionary traits are very stable and deeply enshrined in human psychology even if we are not aware of them most of the time (Aarøe et al., 2017, p. 281 ). Preventing contagion with infectious diseases like Covid-19 and coping with the consequences in case of an infection have been major drivers of human attitudes and behaviour since ancestral times (Brown et al., 2016; Faulkner et al., 2004; Gilles et al., 2013; Murray & Schaller, 2016; Navarrete & Fessler, 2006; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . Biological sciences as well as evolutionary psychology demonstrate that the prevalence of infectious diseases and related parasites has led to the development of not only the classical immune system but also of a behavioural one as 'a motivational system […] inhibiting contact with disease-causing parasites' (Murray & Schaller, 2016, p. 76 ; see also Ackerman et al., 2018) . From the viewpoint of natural selection, the 'behavioral prophylaxis' (Schaller, 2016, p. 299) provided by this BIS is more efficient than the costly (i.e., resource intensive) immunological reactions of the body (Ackerman et al., 2018) . Overall, if individuals view themselves as comparatively vulnerable to becoming infected, for example by living in an area of high parasite stress or due to having certain medical conditions, the BIS is activated, which in turn triggers certain attitudes and ways of behaviour by these individuals. This 'functional flexibility' (Ackerman et al., 2018; Murray & Schaller, 2016) or context-dependent adoption (Thornhill & Fincher, 2020) incentivizes individuals to hold more negative views of out-groups posing potential infection risks (Aarøe et al., 2017; Ackerman et al., 2018; Brown et al., 2016; Faulkner et al., 2004) . Such negativity stems from the notion that members of out-groups often hold different values or patterns of behaviour with little compatibility to combat threatening parasites (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006, p. 271) . Correspondingly, individuals portray greater loyalty towards one's in-group, which is unlikely to carry pathogens to which one has not yet become immune, but is likely helpful in mitigating the consequences of an infection (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006; Sugiyama, 2004; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . Consequently, the extent of one's interaction with members of out-groups results from a tradeoff between the advantages of interacting with out-groups and the (perceived) risks of contracting potentially dangerous diseases from doing so (Brown et al., 2016; Faulkner et al., 2004; Navarrete & Fessler, 2006; Oaten et al., 2009; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014 . Importantly, individuals do not deal with this trade-off by ways of a cost-benefit calculation under perfect information. Instead, people rely on cues and heuristics to assess infection risks (Brown et al., 2016, p . 100) and do not even need to be conscious of doing so (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006, p. 280) . In case the (perceived) risks of infection are high enough to push this trade-off in this direction, the BIS may activate a set of attitudes and behaviour referred to as 'assortative sociality' to prevent contagion (Thornhill & Fincher, 2020, p. 169) . These attitudes and behaviours include stronger in-group orientations, an aversion to new ideas and hostility towards outsiders. The idea of the BIS and its underlying psychological mechanisms have received substantial empirical support, especially regarding more negative attitudes towards out-groups if the prevalence of infectious pathogens is high (Aarøe et al., 2017; Ackerman et al., 2018; Brown et al., 2016; Duncan et al., 2009; Faulkner et al., 2004; Fincher & Thornhill, 2012; Gilles et al., 2013; Krings et al., 2012; Navarrete & Fessler, 2006; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014 . Whereas results from previous studies lend clear support to an increased out-group negativity in the face of pathogen stress, research lacks a thorough understanding of whom people actually conceive of as belonging to their respective in-and out-group(s). This is even more astonishing given the established view that definitions of group membership are of paramount importance in this context (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . To address this research gap, this paper focuses on the relationship between pandemic threat and conceptions of national belonging, which today constitute the major form of group attachment around the world (Davidov, 2009; Greenfeld & Eastwood, 2007; Lenard & Miller, 2018; Schatz et al., 1999) . Since adaptive immunity is highly localized, out-group members may be (perceived as) hosts to novel parasites to which the immunological defences of one's in-group are not yet adapted (Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . Moreover, out-group members are likely to be unaware of -and thus violating -local rituals, norms and customs implicitly relevant in preventing infection with local parasites (Kusche & Barker, 2019; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . In this sense, it is imperative to study precisely how delineations between the in-group and out-group(s) are constructed to gain a thorough understanding of the applicability of the behavioural immune system hypothesis. Although many ways of distinguishing between in-and out-group(s) are conceivable in the context of a pandemic, national membership should be pivotal as it creates borders to combat an infectious disease geographically (limiting the territorial spread), politically (access to health care and large-scale containment measures) and socially (grouping of similar people with shared rituals, values and norms) as stipulated by the BIS-hypothesis. In the context of the latter, national identity is particularly important for constructing perceptions of sameness (Anderson, 2006; Greenfeld & Eastwood, 2007) . National membership based on ethnic criteria as a 'thick' set of criteria (Berg & Hjerm, 2010 ) provides many of the cues regarding norms, values and other disease-inhibiting behaviours aiming to protect oneself and the in-group that have developed over the course of evolution. Conceptualizing national identity, this source of group membership constitutes one of the most extensively researched topics in the social sciences. Current research addresses the question of its emergence as a product of nationalist movements (Anderson, 2006; Gellner, 1983; Hobsbawm, 1992; Hobsbawm & Ranger, 1983) , its lasting impact on modern societies (Calhoun, 2007; Newman, 2000) or the differences between nationalist and patriotic identities (Ariely, 2020; Davidov, 2009; Schatz et al., 1999; Schatz & Staub, 1997) . Besides these issues, the content dimension of national identity is decisive for setting the criteria according to which national membership is constructed (Citrin et al., 2001; Helbling et al., 2016) . 4 Following the seminal work of Hans Kohn (1939) , research on national identity tends to distinguish between ethnic and civic conceptions of nationhood, which define what people consider necessary for being a 'true' member of any nation. On the one hand, ethnic conceptions of nationhood give priority to objectivist criteria for national belonging, which refer to national ancestry, being born in a country or adhering to a particular religious belief (Brubaker, 1992; Lenard & Miller, 2018; Reeskens & Hooghe, 2010) . As such criteria are considered largely fixed, ethnic views on nationhood treat national boundaries as generally impermeable and fixed for most individuals (Sarrasin et al., 2020; Wimmer, 2008) . In that sense, people are unlikely to change their national belonging over the course of their lives even if they relocate to another nation. Civic conceptions of nationhood, on the other hand, revolve around adherence to an explicitly political culture, its norms and values and knowledge of the national language (Helbling et al., 2016; Ignatieff, 1993) . Such views on nationhood gained prominence in the context of the French Revolution and invite any person interested to join another nation as long as they conform with certain values and take part in the respective nation's political life irrespective of where they were born or where their ancestors originated from (Habermas, 1994; Luong, 2016) . Moreover, a civically informed notion of the nation conceptually relates to a higher acceptance of immigration and an endorsement of multiculturalism, which is substantially different from ethnic conceptions of nationhood (Ariely, 2020; Simonsen, 2016) . Importantly, ethnic and civic conceptions of nationhood are mostly ideal types since most people combine elements of both in defining membership to their own nation (Lenard & Miller, 2018; Wright et al., 2012) and this combination may be re-assessed regularly to adapt to developments both within an individual's mindset and outside their immediate locus of control. To sum up, these differences provide a valid theoretical framework for studying patterns of national boundary making to define membership of the in-group (Ariely, 2020) . With respect to the relationship between pandemic threat and different conceptions of nationhood, we argue that wariness towards out-group members resulting from the activation of the BIS resonates well with more exclusive conceptions of nationhood. As stipulated by the concept of assortative sociality, members of out-groups are denied the possibility of becoming 'true' members of the respective nation due to their lack of national ancestry and their birth in another country. Further, the behavioural immune system may trigger more clear-cut distinctions between in-and out-groups (Murray & Schaller, 2016; Reid et al., 2012) , which also supports ethnic conceptions. Consequently, citizens being more exposed to a pathogen-rich environment, such as one hit by the Coronavirus pandemic, should be more inclined to hold ethnic conceptions. As opposed to ethnic views on nationhood, civic views are generally more welcoming towards outsiders aspiring to become part of another nation and consider national boundaries as permeable and flexible. Given that the requirements for joining any nation refer only to political norms and values as well as to the national language, this notion runs somewhat contrary to the desire for cue-based similarity of cultural norms and practices stemming from the heuristics used by the BIS-hypothesis. Consequently, we expect ethnic conceptions of nationhood to be more prevalent in environments with higher exposure to infectious diseases, whereas civic views should be less widespread in such areas. H1: Individuals exposed to the Covid-19 pandemic threat are more likely to hold ethnic conceptions of nationhood and are less likely to embrace civic ones. Beyond establishing these direct links between pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood, we are interested in potential mediating mechanisms. Here, emotions move into the centre of analytical interest. When the BIS perceives an infection risk, it triggers adaptive psychological responses -including the activation of aversive emotional states and cognitive knowledge structures in working memory that expedite behavioural avoidance or the demand for controlling the infectious disease (Schaller & Park, 2011, p. 99; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014, p. 12) . Research from political psychology follows neural process theories arguing for a physical location of different emotions in the brain (Gray, 1987) . Drawing on these insights, affective intelligence theory posits that three brain systems operate constantly and routinely to sort information we confront (Marcus et al., 2000 . When the first system responds with enthusiasm, it signals that all is well. The second system relies on anxiety or fear to signify the extent to which circumstances are novel or uncertain. A third system focuses on anger signalling that a threat to familiar norms and practices of thought and action exists. These brain systems operate constantly and routinely to sort information we confront. In particular, the latter two emotional states emerge as a response to threat . AIT holds that all relevant appraisals are executed simultaneously and largely independently. Thus, rather than feeling angry or fearful, individuals feel angry and fearful when being confronted with a threat . Whichever system and thus emotion is more robust, at any given moment, will determine the course of action taken (Brader et al., 2010; Marcus et al., 2000; Vasilopoulos et al., 2019) . Following AIT, we assume that the novelty of the Covid-19 pandemic may cause fear, since what is unknown may also be dangerous, disrupt security and induce uncertainty. Moreover, fear is linked to a consideration of alternative options to the status quo in the face of uncertainty (Marcus et al., 2019, p. 120) . A tightening of membership criteria and a stronger emphasis on exclusive, ethnic views on nationhood poses an important alternative as compared to previously widespread definitions of national membership emphasizing other factors beyond objectivist criteria (Abascal, 2020; Kenworthy & Jones, 2009 ). Second, we can also anticipate that the detrimental circumstances surrounding the crisis cause anger. This emotional state of aversion arises particularly if we face challenges to central norms that we consider fundamental to the social or political order. The permeability of national borders may be viewed as one major example for such challenges in contemporary societies altering previously existing structures of society (Hainmueller & Hopkins, 2014) . Therefore, we hypothesize that both fear and anger mediate the relationship between exposure to the pandemic threat and the political orientations toward nationhood. H2: Feelings of fear and anger mediate the relationship between exposure to the Covid-19 pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood. To test the theoretical argument outlined in the previous chapter empirically, we rely on original survey data collected during the first peak of the coronavirus pandemic in spring 2020 with approximately 6,000 respondents in six European countries. Taking into account the situation at the onset of the crisis in early spring 2020, the survey includes respondents from France, Germany, Italy, Spain, Switzerland and the United Kingdom. Although all countries selected were strongly affected by the first wave of the pandemic, they vary greatly in their governmental responses to it as well as their levels of infection. Whereas Italy, Spain and France were hardhit early and imposed strict lockdown measures, the United Kingdom issued such orders much later and experienced a continuing surge of infections. Germany and Switzerland also employed early lockdown measures, but mostly managed to contain the outbreak at comparatively low levels. Thus, the sample contains a substantial degree of variation in pandemic exposure. Further, national identity and the boundaries of national membership are a highly salient issue in each of the countries, while each has a very different history in this regard. Thus, results based on this dataset should not be limited to this specific set of countries in the European context. A detailed description of the survey is presented in Table OA1 in the Supporting Information Appendix. For our dependent variable, conceptions of nationhood, we employ five widely used indicators referring to the importance that respondents place on being born in the respective country, having national ancestry and being a Christian (the main religion in all countries surveyed) for ethnic conceptions and respecting national political laws and institutions, and being able to speak the respective national language(s) for civic conceptions (Kunovich, 2009; Reeskens & Hooghe, 2010) . In light of previous research on the relatively continuous nature of conceptions of nationhood between two extremes, we combined these five items into one variable. First, based on principal component analysis we distinguished between two factors that reflect earlier research for civic and ethnic conceptions, respectively (see Table OA2 ). 5 Second, we reversed the civic factor and combined it with the ethnic factor. Consequently, we use a continuous scale that runs from civic conceptions (low values) to ethnic conceptions (high values) with a mean value that locates each respondent between the two ideal points. This allows for obtaining a more fine-grained picture that pays attention to individuals located in between since most people likely combine elements of both ideal types within themselves (Ariely, 2020; Reeskens & Hooghe, 2010; Smith, 1991) . We measure exposure to the Covid-19 pandemic threat both at the individual and contextual level, thus focusing directly on perceptions of pandemic threat (Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . 6 In line with previous research, we propose that pandemic threat not only originates from subjective threat experience, but also from pathogen stress within the larger region of an individual (Thornhill & Fincher, 2020) . As the pandemic disturbs societal life extensively in the personal environments and communities, it may well be that all individuals living in strongly affected areas, regardless of whether or not they report subjective threat from the virus, are facing pandemic threat. People may not be affected personally but still be exposed to pandemic threat and restrictions to contain the virus in their everyday lives. Thus, we expect that individuals living in areas strongly hit by the pandemic feel more threatened than individuals living in environments with low pandemic threat, irrespective of their personal pandemic-related experiences. For individual-level exposure to pandemic threat, we asked respondents 'To what extent do you feel the coronavirus pandemic is a threat to you personally?' 7 For regional-level prevalence of the pandemic in the 105 European regions across the six countries in our survey, we introduce two measures for the severity of the pandemic, that is, the number of cases and the number of Covid-19-related deaths, both per 100,000 inhabitants. 8 We gathered these data from the responsible statistical offices of the respective countries. As these regional-level variables are vastly skewed towards lower values and given the potential for measurement error (Wooldridge, 2009, p. 191) , we took the logarithm. This approach should ensure that we can assess pandemic exposure thoroughly and that we are able to draw valid conclusions based on several modelling strategies. To measure the role of emotions, we rely on the well-known Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) scale in its short version (Crawford & Henry, 2004; Watson et al., 1988) . The question reads as follows: 'Now we would like to know how you feel. The following words describe different kinds of feelings and emotions. Read every word and mark the intensity on the scale. You have the choice between five gradations. Please indicate how you feel at the moment.' For anger, we use (a) upset and (b) hostile. For fear we use (c) afraid and (d) nervous. 9 Although restrictions on the questionnaire size allow for only a limited number of items per latent construct, confirmatory factor analysis with maximum likelihood estimation supports the notion that fear and anger are correlated, yet distinct, emotional states (see Table OA3 ). The model fit for the confirmatory factor analysis implies a very good model fit with RMSEA < 0.08 and CFI > 0.9. Given that the strength of factors may vary between countries, we conducted a test for measurement invariance (see Table OA4 ), which supports full metric invariance that is crucial for a substantive interpretation of our results (cf. Davidov, 2009 ). In accordance with previous research, we control for a range of socio-demographic variables that likely affect conceptions of nationhood as well as exposure to the pandemic, such as age, gender, education, income situation, health, migration background and left-right self-placement (see Table OA5 for summary statistics and OA6 for exact item wording of the main variables). All variables were z-standardized (mean = 0 and variance = 1) to make the respective coefficients comparable. In addition, we collected macro data for all regions to control for variation at the regional level that might affect the relationship between pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood. We use the share of elderly among the population, population density, unemployment rates and gross domestic product per capita as this set of macrolevel control variables contains a wide range of potentially influential predictors linked to the respective region. To test the first hypothesis, we employ two linear regression models with country-fixed effects and region-clustered standard errors as our respondents are nested within the 105 regions across the six countries surveyed. Adjusting our estimates for country-fixed effects allows controlling for unobserved heterogeneity between the countries. Besides this comparative approach, we conduct the microlevel analysis separately for each country surveyed to get a better understanding of the relationship that we are interested in. In addition to this individual-level analysis, we conduct a macrolevel analysis using random-intercept multilevel models for numbers of both cases and deaths at the level of the respective region. 10 The final step of our analysis consists of mediation analyses by means of path models to test hypothesis 2 that focuses on the mediating role of fear and anger (Preacher et al., 2010; Rabe-Hesketh et al., 2004) . This set of models tests the mediation effect of fear and anger simultaneously as stipulated by previous research . These structural equation models are based on maximum likelihood estimations with regionally clustered standard errors and allow us to test whether fear and anger mediate the relationship and whether there are indirect effects of the exposure to pandemic threat on conceptions of nationhood. As with the first set of models, we conduct our mediation analyses using both individual-and regional-level indicators for measuring pandemic threat exposure and split the individual-level analysis for each of the six countries. When using regional-level indicators, we rely on generalized structural equation models (Preacher et al., 2010; Rabe-Hesketh et al., 2004) . Figure 1 shows the results obtained from our first model looking at pandemic threat at the individual level (see Table OA7 ). The positive coefficient linking higher pandemic threat exposure to ethnic conceptions of nationhood supports our first hypothesis. Individuals perceiving Covid-19 as a higher subjective threat are more likely to view national belonging as based on criteria such as having national ancestry and birth as well as adhering to the Christian religion, indicating an activation of the BIS and its disease-avoidant norms and behaviours. Accordingly, respondents experiencing less subjective pandemic threat are more likely to hold civic conceptions of nationhood that draw on language and political values to define in-group membership and allow for a higher permeability of national borders, which lends support to hypothesis 1. The relationships for our control variables equally point in the expected directions and are in line with previous studies. Right-leaning, less educated individuals with a more adverse income situation or without a migration background are more likely to hold ethnic conceptions of nationhood. 11 Table OA7 . Linear regression coefficients are displayed with confidence intervals at 90 per cent (black bars) and 95 per cent (light grey bars) levels. When investigating this relationship across countries (Figure 2 ), we find that it is significant in Germany, France and Italy. In Switzerland, Spain and the United Kingdom, the coefficients point in the expected direction but are not significant. Overall, looking at the countries individually supports the main finding from the comparative analysis, but it is also evident that it does not apply equally to the exclusion of other factors, which emphasizes the importance of investigating the nature of this relationship in more detail by means of mediation analysis to uncover underlying patterns. 12 Figure 3 details the respective results for the multilevel analyses (see Table OA12 ). At the regional level, we observe that both higher numbers of Covid-19 cases and Covid-19-related fatality rates are significant predictors of more ethnic-based conceptions of nationhood. Individual threat levels remain robust and significant in both models. Including further macrolevel control variables (share of elderly among the population, population density, unemployment rates and gross domestic product per capita) does not change our main results (see Table OA13 ). To test for a potential sensitivity of our estimates to extreme cases or clusters, we estimated the same models using a jack-knifing procedure, that is, we re-estimated each model several times, removing all respondents from each region once to control for highly influential clusters. The results reflect those found in the base model (see Table OA14 ). In a next step, we use mediation analysis to uncover whether the emotional states of fear and anger mediate the relationship between exposure to pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood. We test both micro-and macrolevel exposure to the virus as focusing only on the Table OA11 . Displayed are linear regression coefficients with confidence intervals at 90 per cent (black bars) and 95 per cent (light grey bars) levels. direct relationship between independent and dependent variables has been shown to hamper theory development and overlook potentially important indirect effects (Preacher et al., 2010) . Again, we start with exposure to pandemic threat at the individual level as the explanatory variable and test whether negative emotional responses mediate the relationship with conceptions of nationhood. Taking into account that both anger and fear likely occur simultaneously (Marcus et al., 2000) , we analyze both emotions in a single, comprehensive model. Table 1 shows the results obtained from our path models to uncover the hypothesized mediation effects. This mediation analysis reveals that the Covid-19 pandemic threat is significantly related to both fear and anger at the individual and contextual levels. In other words, people who feel more threatened by the pandemic are likely to be both more angry and more fearful than those experiencing less pandemic threat. Yet, the results clearly show that these two emotions play decisively different roles. Whereas we do not find any mediation effect for fear, anger is a significant mediator linking pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood. These two findings hold regardless of how we measure pandemic threat. 13 If we estimate these path models in the six countries separately, the results are largely substantiated. Anger significantly mediates the relationship between immediate pandemic threat and ethnic conceptions of nationhood in Germany, France, Switzerland, Spain and the United Kingdom (see Figure 4) , whereas it is insignificant only in Italy. For fear, there are mostly insignificant and inconclusive results. We find a negative indirect effect only in Italy. Taken together, we find substantive evidence that emotional responses in the form of anger affect the Table OA12 . Displayed are linear regression coefficients with confidence intervals at 90 per cent (black bars) and 95 per cent (light grey bars) levels. relationship between exposure to pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood as argued in hypothesis 2. Fear, however, plays a decisively subordinate role in the context of pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood. This finding reflects recent advances in the role of emotions and the primacy of anger over fear in shaping individuals' political attitudes and behaviours . These mediation analyses for the individual countries lend additional support to the hypothesized relationship between pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood, as this main relationship is significant in all but one country once emotional responses to threat are properly accounted for. Taken together, our empirical analyses reveal a clear relationship between the Covid-19 pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood. Individuals threatened by this infectious disease are more likely to hold ethnic views on national belonging that should be better suited for avoiding the contraction with novel pathogens than civic ones implying a greater permeability of national boundaries. These results support earlier findings that 'intergroup bias is […] [affected] by features of the mind designed to enact approach avoidance mechanisms for negotiating adaptive intergroup relations in a way that would attenuate disease threat' (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006, p. 279) . Further, the mediation analyses show that certain emotional states are crucial to understand the relationship between the Covid-19 pandemic and definitions on group membership thoroughly. Here, anger in particular is highly relevant in shaping the relationships between our main variables of interest. The threat of pandemic exposure activates anger, which in turn fosters ethnic conceptions of nationhood. Table 1 . Combined path models for fear and anger with pandemic threat Individual-level threat Regional-level threat (cases) Regional-level threat ( Clustered standard errors (region) in parentheses. + p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. How does the Covid-19 pandemic threat affect views on belonging to in-or out-groups? In this paper, we evaluate whether pandemic threat experience is related to conceptions of nationhood among citizens in six Western European countries. Looking at national membership as today's most important form of group membership (Davidov, 2009; Greenfeld & Eastwood, 2007) , we suggest that ethnic conceptions of nationhood stressing the role of 'thicker' (Berg & Hjerm, 2010, p. 393 ) criteria, like national ancestry, beyond the political sphere resonate well with the premise of the BIS-hypothesis. Conversely, civic views on nationhood that deem respecting national political institutions and laws alongside being able to speak the national language as sufficient for being a full member of a nation should be more in conflict with attitudes and behaviours aiming to reduce the risk of contracting infectious diseases. The empirical analyses based on original survey data collected during the early phase of the pandemic support this argument. Individual experiences of pandemic threat as well as macrolevel data in the form of both case numbers and fatality rates predict conceptions of nationhood. In addition, we follow AIT and add key insights regarding emotional responses in threatening situations to the behavioural immune system hypothesis. Focusing on fear and anger as crucial and distinct drivers of people's response to threat and uncertainty (Marcus et al., 2000 Figure 4 . Coefficient plot for individual-level mediation analysis by country. Note: Full regression results available upon request. Displayed are linear regression coefficients with confidence intervals at 90 per cent (black bars) and 95 per cent (light grey bars) levels. Vasilopoulos et al., 2019) , we find that anger proves to be a crucial mediator of the relationship between pandemic threat exposure and conceptions of nationhood. Fear, in contrast, appears to play no mediating role if anger is properly accounted for. This crucial finding substantiates recent claims that both scholarly research and political commentators must be careful not to conflate these two emotions reflecting distinct cognitive systems operating largely independently of each other (cf. Marcus et al., 2019) . Afraid individuals react very differently to pandemic threat perceptions than angry ones do. Anger drives individuals to confront an adversary and to protect the in-group, reinforcing ethnic conceptions of nationhood as stipulated by the behavioural immune system hypothesis. Fear, however, more likely induces individuals to seek new perspectives and to go through a learning process (Albertson & Gadarian, 2015; Marcus et al., 2019; Vasilopoulos et al., 2019) . Consequently, people facing pandemic threat should assess the complexity of the pandemic and the transmission of the virus from a more differentiated point of view that does not lead to a withdrawal into more close-knit social groups and a scapegoating of national out-groups. Yet, our approach also has limitations requiring further attention. Although our observational data gives important, first-hand evidence on pandemic threat regarding both the individual and contextual levels, we cannot make causal claims as compared to experimental settings in laboratory studies or time-series analyses. While there exists substantial causal evidence that the activation of the BIS has an effect on xenophobia (Aarøe et al., 2017; Faulkner et al., 2004) or ingroup favouritism (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006) , similar studies have not yet been conducted for conceptions of nationhood. We argue that it appears unlikely that people are more likely to contract Covid-19 because they hold more ethnic conceptions of nationhood. Individuals convinced that only those born and raised in their respective countries are true members of their in-group will have comparably closed social environments that are less open towards newcomers. Consequently, such individuals should be less likely to become infected with contagious diseases from outgroups. Evidence that many infections happened among in-group members (family, friends, coworkers, religious communities, associations or sports clubs) indeed supports the main tenets of the BIS hypothesis (Tybur et al., 2020) . If individuals are particularly careful in their dealings with out-group members, they may also systematically underestimate the threat of infections emanating from members of their own in-group since evolutionary developed heuristics lead to misperceptions of infectiousness. While this pattern might call into question the success of the BIS as a means of ensuring protection from a contagious virus like SARS-CoV-2 in modern, largescale societies, we provide substantive evidence that it plays a decisive role in shaping people's attitudes and behaviours, nonetheless. In sum, we argue that providing first-hand, real-world data on pandemic threat is pivotal to complement previous studies that have provided substantive evidence on the issue of causality (see also Murray & Schaller, 2016; Tybur et al., 2014) . Finally, the countries selected for our original survey do not cover all possible contextual factors that may drive the relationship under study. This is particularly relevant for certain parts of the world where ethnic, religious or regional identities are much more important than national ones. However, our dataset provides insight into cases with substantial variation, which certainly allows for drawing conclusions more broadly applicable than the six countries studied in this paper. In conclusion, our results have far-reaching implications for a better understanding of group membership in modern societies. We show how exposure to pandemic threat relates to belonging to the national in-group, thereby providing the first evidence on the validity of the BIS in the context of a novel, yet highly salient, pandemic among citizens, for whom parasite stress and infectious diseases arguably played a minor role for many decades. Our findings corroborate the claim that liberal, open societies -which all surveyed countries claim to be -must combat infectious diseases not only as a matter of public health, but also to ensure their own survival (cf. Thornhill & Fincher, 2020, p. 175) . This is even more crucial if the boundaries of social groups become less permeable in the face of pandemic threat and should be equally applicable to other societal groups, where (non-)membership contributes to individuals' social identities beyond national belonging. If belonging to in-and out-groups in general becomes more static, the very foundations of liberal and pluralistic societies are shaken substantially and even democratic governance itself may come under pressure (Erhardt et al., 2021) . Given that globalization fosters the presence of diverse ethnic groups across the globe, a continued salience of infectious diseases likely becomes a serious obstacle to societal acceptance of people with different backgrounds (Aarøe et al., 2017) . Concern about pathogen prevalence may thus inhibit inclusive societies and exchange across different ethnic groups severely if the mere presence of physically and culturally distinct out-group members may be seen as threatening due to the prevalence of infectious diseases like Covid-19. The importance of emotions in general and of anger in particular, as shown by the uncovered mediation effects, further stresses that distinct affective responses to threat and uncertainty like fear and anger must not be used interchangeably as they vary decisively in their potential to shape attitudes and behaviours with regard to a large variety of social sciences concepts . Correspondingly, political communication and policy design should always consider the emotional reactions invoked among citizens to avoid backfiring if citizens react with fear or anger to them (cf. Albertson & Gadarian, 2015) . This is all the more important given the increasing consensus among leading epidemiologists that globalization may continue to accelerate both the frequency and severity of pandemics over time. Future research might now look for data to gain an even deeper understanding of the role of (other) emotions and to assess the role of the Coronavirus pandemic in shaping other relevant sets of attitudes, such as norm-conformity, authoritarianism or social conservatism (Brown et al., 2016; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . Another promising avenue for future research will be to examine the role of the Coronavirus pandemic in shaping within-country outgroup-hostility (e.g., rich vs. poor, ethnic majority vs. ethnic minority) or to delve deeper into aspects of national identity not covered by conceptions of nationhood, such as nationalist attitudes or ethnocentrism. Finally, researchers might theorize and study, which contextual factors might play a prominent role in the context of pandemic threat and conceptions of nationhood to explain differences across countries in more detail. Overall, our study gives vital insight into the extent to which the Covid-19 pandemic threat structures feelings of national belonging and initiates avenues for future research on the social and political consequences of the crisis. the contributions by Filsinger and Freitag (2022) and Freitag and Hofstetter (2022) , who are elaborating theoretically and empirically similar designs within the same research program. 2. We understand parasites as 'infectious disease agents of all types: viruses, bacteria, fungi, protozoa, helminths […] , and arthropods' (Thornhill & Fincher, 2020, p. 167 ). 3. Although there are other important forms of national identity, such as nationalism or patriotism (Ariely, 2020; Citrin et al., 2001; Davidov, 2009 ), we focus explicitly on conceptions of nationhood since definitions of group membership are considered vital for a comprehensive understanding of the behavioural immune system hypothesis, and which are as yet understudied (Navarrete & Fessler, 2006; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) . 4. Although it would without a doubt prove fruitful to study how pandemic exposure may alter these other aspects of national identity or even lead to profound changes, for example in our understanding of the nation and its sovereignty or nationalist attitudes, the present paper focuses precisely on this content dimension to address one specific research gap and to maintain conceptual concision. Thus, the following argument is restricted to this particular part of national identity, whereas other, related reasoning is beyond the scope of this study. 5. Cronbach's alpha for ethnic conceptions is 0.81. The Spearman-Brown Prophecy Reliability Estimate (as there are only two items) for civic conceptions is 0.66. 6. Contributions referring to disgust sensitivity do not focus directly on perceptions of infection risk but instead on individual differences in the tendency to experience the emotion -disgust -associated with a possible infection risk (Murray & Schaller, 2016) . Disgust, however, is subject to conceptual disarray in broader political psychology literature. While in AIT, aversion includes 'anger, disgust, contempt, and hatred … [which signal] the need to confront an adversary' (MacKuen et al., 2010, p. 441) , other relevant literature conceptualizes disgust explicitly as an avoidance-orientation and not an emotion that motivates individuals to confront any adversaries as specified in AIT (Kam & Estes, 2016, p. 482) . 7. Aside from this subjective measure of pandemic threat, we also asked respondents how many people they know personally that have been infected with the virus as an alternative measurement signalling pandemic threat. However, a direct measure of subjective threat at the individual level should be more compelling as an indicator of pandemic threat and less prone to measurement error. 8. The six countries studied consist of 112 regions in total. Due to data availability, our sample contains only 105 of them. The number of respondents per region ranges from two (Obwalden, CH) to 250 (North Rhine-Westphalia, GER). With regard to pandemic threat, we have considerable variation between the regions ranging from 9 to over 1,000 cases per inhabitants and 0 to over 112 deaths per 100,000 inhabitants. This provides us with leverage for the multilevel analyses. 9. The short PANAS scale does not include disgust as an emotional state. The adjectives used here are terms describing emotional reactions that fall under the more general dimensions of fear/anxiety and anger/aversion, albeit to different degrees of intensity (other adjectives that have been used in this regard include 'uneasy', 'scared' 'worried' for anxiety and 'angry', 'disgusted' 'resentful' for aversion (Marcus et al., 2000, pp. 152-174) . As argued by , anger and fear are highly correlated. This is true for the survey we use in this study (r = 0.61). The Spearman-Brown Prophecy Reliability Estimate for anger is 0.74 and 0.75 for fear. 10. Splitting the macrolevel study into individual countries is less promising since the number of regions within some countries is too small for a meaningful multilevel analysis. 11. Running the estimations with infection numbers in respondents' respective personal environments does not alter the results (see Table OA8 ). 12. We conducted the same analysis for the two factors on ethnic and civic conceptions of nationhood separately and for each country (Tables OA9 and OA10 ). Whereas ethnic conceptions are related to pandemic threat in three countries, there is a significant relationship for civic ones only for Germany. Measuring civic conceptions on their own, however, is conceptually difficult since approval to either of the two items does not preclude approval to the 'ethnic' ones (Reeskens & Hooghe, 2010) . Thus, civic conceptions on their own are less valid for distinguishing between civic and ethnic conceptions and we refrain from interpreting too much into this particular finding. 13. Again, we re-estimated the path models for regional Covid-19 cases and deaths using a jack-knifing procedure, that is, we re-estimated each model several times, removing all respondents from each region once to control for highly influential clusters. Equally, the results yield no significant or substantial differences to those shown here (results available upon request). The behavioral immune system shapes political intuitions: Why and how individual differences in disgust sensitivity underlie opposition to immigration Contraction as a response to group threat: Demographic decline and whites' classification of people who are ambiguously white The behavioral immune system: Current concerns and future directions Anxious politics: Democratic citizenship in a threatening world Imagined communities: Reflections of the origin and spread of nationalism Globalization and global identification: A comparative multilevel analysis Measuring dimensions of national identity across countries: Theoretical and methodological reflections National identity and political trust Fight of flight? When political threats arouse public anger and fear Personality, parasites, political attitudes, and cooperation: A model of how infection prevalence influences openness and social group formation Nations matter: Culture, history, and the cosmopolitan dream The meaning of American national identity The positive and negative affect schedule (panas): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample Measurement equivalence of nationalism and constructive patriotism in the ISSP: 34 countries in a comparative perspective Unexpected public health consequences of the covid-19 pandemic: A national survey examining anti-Asian attitudes in the USA Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument National identity between democracy and autocracy: A comparative analysis of 24 countries Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes Pandemic threat and authoritarian attitudes in Europe: An empirical analysis of the exposure to Covid-19 The parasite-stress theory May be a general theory of culture and sociality. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences Pandemic Threat and Intergroup Relations: How negative emotions associated with the threat of Covid-19 shape attitudes towards immigrants Nations and nationalism Collective symbolic coping with disease threat and othering: A case study of avian influenza National identity Struggles for recognition in the democratic constitutional state Public attitudes toward immigration The mobilisation of identities: A study on the relationship between elite rhetoric and public opinion on national identity in developed democracies Nations and nationalism since 1780 Blood and belonging: Journeys into the new nationalism Trump and the populist authoritarian parties: The silent revolution in reverse Coronavirus resource center Disgust sensitivity and public demand for protection The roles of group importance and anxiety in predicting depersonalized ingroup trust The nature of nationalism Globalization and the transformation of the national political space: Six European countries compared Preventing Contagion With Avian Influenza: Disease Salience, Attitudes Toward Foreigners, and Avoidance Beliefs1 The sources and consequences of national identification Pathogens and immigrants: A critical appraisal of the behavioral immune system as an explanation of prejudice against ethnic outgroups The Oxford handbook of social and political trust The crisis of western democracies and national identity: Citizenship, immigration, and constitutional patriotism Civic engagements: Resolute partisanship or reflective deliberation Affective Intelligence and Political Judgment Applying the theory of affective intelligence to support for authoritarian policies and parties The behavioral immune system: Implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes Nationalism in postindustrial societies: Why states still matter Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism A general multilevel sem framework for assessing multilevel mediation Generalized multilevel structural equation modeling Beyond the civic-ethnic dichotomy: Investigating the structure of citizenship concepts across thirty-three countries Parasite primes make foreignaccented English sound more distant to people who are disgusted by pathogens (but not by sex or morality) Xenophobia in the time of pandemic: Othering, anti-Asian attitudes, and covid-19. Politics, Groups, and Identities Consensual versus heterogeneous conceptions of nationhood: The role of citizenship regimes and integration policies across 21 European countries The handbook of evolutionary psychology The behavioral immune system (and why it matters) Manifestations of blind and constructive patriotism: Personality correlates and individual group relations On the varieties of national attachment: Blind versus constructive patriotism How the host nation's boundary drawing affects immigrants' belonging National identity Illness, injury, and disability among shiwiar forager-horticulturalists: Implications of healthrisk buffering for the evolution of human life history The parasite-stress theory of values and sociality: Infectious disease, history and human values worldwide The Cambridge handbook of evolutionary perspectives on human behavior Behavioral immune system methods: Surveying the present to shape the future Behavioral immune tradeoffs: Interpersonal value relaxes social pathogen avoidance Using social and behavioural science to support covid-19 pandemic response Fear, anger, and voting for the far right: Evidence from the November 13 Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The panas scales Introductory econometrics. A modern approach The making and unmaking of ethnic boundaries: A multilevel process theory Alternative measures of American national identity: Implications for the civic-ethnic distinction The authors wish to thank several people who contributed to this study. We are grateful to Michael Wicki, Sara Gomm, Oriane Sarrasin, Thomas Kessler as well as all participants of the respective workshops at the Annual Conference of the Swiss Political Science Association 2021 and the Scientific Online Meeting of the German Political Psychology Network 2021, whose invaluable feedback provided us with excellent opportunities to improve our paper.Open access funding provided by Universitat Bern. This article was written as part of the research project "The Politics of Public Health Threat" that is financially supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF) (grant no. 100017_204507) and the Berne University Research Foundation (grant no. 25/2020). The authors declare no conflict of interest. Additional supporting information may be found in the Online Appendix section at the end of the article:Online Appendix Notes 1. This article was written as part of the research project 'The Politics of Public Health Threat' that is financially supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF) (grant no. 100017_204507) and the Berne University Research Foundation (grant no. 25/2020). In this context, reference should also be made to