key: cord-0991536-owgz2g4p authors: Issa, Semona; Chaabani, Safa; Asimakopoulos, Alexandros G.; Jaspers, Veerle L. B.; Einum, Sigurd title: Maternal dopamine exposure provides offspring starvation resistance in Daphnia date: 2022-04-01 journal: Ecol Evol DOI: 10.1002/ece3.8785 sha: f0a9bfabf5ba9cd2a1d1aa6fde926f1aac6f659d doc_id: 991536 cord_uid: owgz2g4p The neurotransmitter dopamine has been shown to play an important role in modulating behavioral, morphological, and life history responses to food abundance. However, costs of expressing high dopamine levels remain poorly studied and are essential for understanding the evolution of the dopamine system. Negative maternal effects on offspring size from enhanced maternal dopamine levels have previously been documented in Daphnia. Here, we tested whether this translates into fitness costs in terms of lower starvation resistance in offspring. We exposed Daphnia magna mothers to aqueous dopamine (2.3 or 0 mg/L for the control) at two food levels (ad libitum vs. 30% ad libitum) and recorded a range of maternal life history traits. The longevity of their offspring was then quantified in the absence of food. In both control and dopamine treatments, mothers that experienced restricted food ration had lower somatic growth rates and higher age at maturation. Maternal food restriction also resulted in production of larger offspring that had a superior starvation resistance compared to ad libitum groups. However, although dopamine exposed mothers produced smaller offspring than controls at restricted food ration, these smaller offspring survived longer under starvation. Hence, maternal dopamine exposure provided an improved offspring starvation resistance. We discuss the relative importance of proximate and ultimate causes for why D. magna may not evolve toward higher endogenous dopamine levels despite the fitness benefits this appears to have. gives higher fitness across the changing environment (Lande, 2009) . Phenotypic plasticity can also be transferred from mother to offspring such that the maternal response to the environment induces changes to the offspring reaction norm (Uller, 2008) . Maternal effects can then impact offspring fitness and ultimately population dynamics (Benton et al., 2008) . Hence, expression of adaptive reaction norms is essential to maintain high fitness as well as population viability in heterogeneous environments. This is especially the case for reaction norms that are expressed in response to food abundance, as changes in these can have strong effects on different components of life history (Boggs, 2009) . In short-lived species, for example, resource allocation to somatic maintenance (including survival) increases at the cost of growth and reproduction when food is limited (Lynch, 1989; Martínez-Jerónimo et al., 1994) . Resulting changes in maternal resource allocation to offspring can ultimately influence offspring survival and reproduction (Enserink et al., 1995; Hafer et al., 2011; Saastamoinen et al., 2013) . At the physiological level, the neurotransmitter dopamine plays an important role as mediator of trait responses to food (see Barron et al., 2010 , for a review on dopamine-mediated behavioural and morphological responses to food across taxa). Issa et al. (2020) showed that in addition to influencing morphological and behavioral traits, dopamine can regulate life history responses to food abundance. Specifically, in the zooplankton species Daphnia magna, exposure to dopamine caused life history reaction norms (age at maturation and fecundity) to change in a way that resulted in higher population growth rates (calculated from maternal life history traits) when food was limited (Issa et al., 2020) . This happened without any apparent fitness costs at high food abundance. These observations raise the question of why endogenous dopamine levels do not evolve toward higher values. Issa et al. (2020) suggested this may be due to costs of high dopamine levels being paid by the offspring generation, which was not quantified in their study. Negative maternal effects on offspring size from dopamine treatments were detected. A smaller offspring size may have detrimental effects on offspring survival since body size in Daphnia is positively associated with filtering rates (Porter et al., 1983) and offspring survival under food limitation (Gliwicz & Guisande, 1992) . Because offspring survival is a crucial component of maternal fitness, investigation of costs to offspring from enhanced maternal dopamine levels could give better insight into the selective forces shaping the evolution of the dopamine system. In this study, we experimentally tested for effects of maternal dopamine exposure on offspring fitness. We exposed one generation (F 0 ) of D. magna to dopamine at high vs. restricted food ration and starved the offspring (F 1 ) to measure their starvation resistance. Based on the previous findings of Issa et al. (2020) , we predicted changes to the slopes of the life history reaction norms under dopamine exposure that result in faster somatic growth and smaller offspring when food is limited. Moreover, we hypothesized that the smaller offspring originating from dopamine-exposed mothers would experience reduced survival under starvation. As previously outlined in Issa et al. (2020) , ephippia (i.e., the capsule containing resting eggs) resulting from sexual reproduction of D. magna were collected in November 2014, in a pond at Vaerøy Island (1.0 ha, 67.687°N 12.672°E), northern Norway. Ephippial eggs were hatched in the laboratory and propagated clonally. For the current experiment, juveniles of a single clone (clone 49) of D. magna were asexually propagated for 10 successive generations prior to use. A maximum of 30 individuals of D. magna were cultured in 2.5-L aquaria at 20°C in a modified "Aachener Daphnien Medium" (ADaM) (Klüttgen et al., 1994 , SeO2 concentration reduced by 50%), under long photoperiods (16 h L: 8 h D) using white fluorescent lamps. The medium was exchanged weekly and the animals were fed three times a week with Shellfish Diet 1800 ® (Reed mariculture Inc.) at a final concentration of 3.2 × 10 5 cells/ml. For the F 0 generation, a similar experimental design as in Issa et al. (2020) was used, corresponding to a full factorial design with control, dopamine, and two food rations (high vs. restricted), with six 500-ml replicate beakers (non-aerated borosilicate beakers, Fisherbrand) for each of the four combinations ( Figure 1 ). Ten neonates (<24 h old) were introduced into each beaker and kept at 20°C under long photoperiods (16 h L: 8 h D) until maturation. The exposure concentration of dopamine (2.3 mg dopamine hydrochloride/L) was chosen for successfully inducing changes in D. magna growth (Issa et al., 2020; Weiss et al., 2015) . An exposure protocol as previously outlined in Issa et al. (2020) was followed. Specifically, dopamine hydrochloride (Sigma-Aldrich) was first dissolved in 100 ml ultrapure water before dilution in ADaM to the desired exposure concentration. Controls containing only ADaM medium were performed parallel to the exposure replicates. The medium was renewed in all replicates (N = 24) three times a week, and the animals were fed at each renewal event with Shellfish Diet 1800 ® at a final concentration of 2.88 × 10 5 cells/ ml (ad lib at 20°C) for the high food ration and 8.6 × 10 4 cells/ml (30% ad lib at 20°C) for the restricted food ration. All replicates were checked daily for mature individuals. In Daphnia, maturity can be determined by visual inspection of the brood chamber (the dorsal part of the animal) through the transparent carapace, into which fully developed eggs are oviposited. Mature individuals were removed from the beakers, photographed for size measurements (see Section 2.3 below), and kept individually in 2-ml non-aerated wells containing ADaM at 10°C until they had released their neonates. Seventeen of the 240 individuals in the beakers were found to be males and were thus not transferred (Figure 1 ). To prevent released neonates from obtaining food, no food was added to the wells. The wells were checked daily and once neonates had been released (i.e., <24 h old, F 1 generation), two individuals were randomly sampled from each well, photographed for size measurements (see Section 2.3 below), and transferred individually to new 2-ml non-aerated wells containing ADaM (no food) at 10°C. The medium was not exchanged, and individuals were checked daily to record mortality. The low exposure temperature was chosen to slow down metabolism and increase longevity under starvation, thus making it more likely to accurately quantify any variation in offspring longevity under the chosen frequency of observations. Immediately prior to dopamine exposure on day 0 of the experiment, a random sample of 30 F 0 neonates were photographed for body length measurements (mm, measured from the upper margin of the eye to the junction of the carapace and spine, using ImageJ v1.52a, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD). Individual measurements of body length at first reproduction for the F 0 generation (after releasing their first clutch of eggs) and at birth for the F 1 generation were measured in the same way. All body length measurements (mm) were converted into dry mass (mg) using the equation The somatic growth rate in the F 0 generation was calculated for each individual as: Somatic growth rate = ln(dry mass end ) − ln(dry mass start ) Duration , where dry mass start is the average dry mass (in mg) at the neonatal stage (day 0), dry mass end is the individual dry mass (in mg) at maturation, and duration is the number of days between the two stages. As in Issa et al. (2020) , conductivity (WTW LF 330 conductivity meter), pH (WTW pH 340i), and dissolved oxygen (WTW Multi 3410 multiprobe meter) were measured. This was done on two separate occasions throughout the experiment (Table 1) (2) liquid-liquid extraction. Subsequent analysis was performed by ultra-performance liquid chromatography coupled to a triple F I G U R E 1 Schematic diagram of the experimental design. The F 0 generation was kept at restricted (R) and high (H) food ration for both control and dopamine treatments (2 x 2 design) from birth until maturity, with six replicate beakers per treatment combination and with each replicate initially containing 10 individuals. Due to the occurrence of males in the F 0 generation, total sample sizes, given as N, varied among treatments. Individuals in the F 1 generation were kept without food in separate wells and were not exposed to dopamine All statistical analyses and graphic illustrations were performed in R v. 3.6.3 (R Core Team, 2020). We tested whether the slopes of the reaction norms of the measured life history traits in response to food abundance differed among dopamine treatments (control vs. dopamine exposure). To assess this for dry mass at maturation and somatic growth rate, we used linear mixed effects models (LME, implemented using the lme functions in the package nlme, Pinheiro et al., 2020) , with fixed effects of treatment and food ration and the interaction between these, and with beaker ID as a random effect. For offspring dry mass, LME models were fitted with treatment, food ration, and their interaction as fixed predictor variables, and maternal ID nested in beaker as a random predictor variable. Finally, we tested the effects of treatment, food ration and their interaction on age at maturation and offspring longevity (both being probability distributions), using Poisson generalized linear mixed effects (GLMM, implemented using the glmer function in the package lme4, Bates et al., 2015) . For these latter analyses, beaker ID and maternal ID nested in beaker ID were included as random effects, respectively. For LME models, the VarIdent command from the nlme package was used to allow residual variance to differ among treatments, food ration, and the two-way interaction between these (Pinheiro & Bates, 2000) . Using a chi-square test of the residual deviance and degrees of freedom, GLMM models were found not to be overdispersed, and their Pearson and deviance residuals were found to satisfy model assumptions. For all analyses, the appropriate random structure was first established by comparing the Akaike's information criterion with correction for small sample size (AIC c ) of models that included all fixed factors but differed in their random effects structure. Once the appropriate random structure was determined, fits of alternative models with different fixed effects structures were compared, again using AIC c . All life history traits were influenced by both food ration and dopamine treatments ( Table 2) . Most of them responded in the same direction to a change in food ration for both control and dopamine treatments. Specifically, somatic growth rate increased with higher food ration, whereas age at maturation, offspring dry mass, and offspring longevity decreased (Figure 2 , Table 3 ). For dry mass at maturation, there was a strong interaction between food ration and dopamine treatment (Table 2) . While dry mass at maturation decreased with increasing food ration in the control treatment, the opposite pattern was observed in the dopamine treatment (Figure 2c , Table 3 ). The steepness of the food ration reaction norms depended on dopamine treatment for somatic growth rate and offspring dry mass (Table 2) . Specifically, dopamine exposure increased somatic growth rate, but this was more pronounced at high food ration (Table 3) , causing the reaction norm to be steeper for the dopamine exposed group compared to the control (Figure 2a) . For offspring dry mass, treatment had no effect at high food ration, whereas exposure to dopamine reduced offspring dry mass at restricted food ration (Table 3) . Thus, for this trait, the reaction norm was steepest for the control group ( Figure 2d ). In contrast, exposure to dopamine lowered age at maturation to an equal extent across food rations ( Figure 2b , Table 2 ). Similarly, the effect of dopamine treatment on offspring longevity did not depend on the maternal food ration (Table 2) . Rather, exposure to dopamine increased offspring longevity across food rations (Figure 2e ). In a previous study, Issa et al. (2020) investigated the role of dopamine in shaping life history responses to food abundance in D. magna. This was done both through aqueous exposure to dopamine and to the antidepressant bupropion, a dopamine reuptake inhibitor. Both treatments led to higher population growth rates (calculated from maternal life history traits) when food was restricted, without any apparent costs to fitness. The higher dopamine exposure resulted, however, in smaller offspring, which could potentially perform worse particularly when facing restrictions in food availability (Gliwicz, 1990) . Since offspring survival is a crucial component of maternal fitness, this may represent a fitness cost of higher maternal dopamine levels. In the current study, we tested this by exposing F 0 D. magna to aqueous dopamine and quantifying life history responses of mothers (F 0 ) to food abundance as well as the starvation resistance of their offspring (F 1 ). Similar to the findings of Issa et al. (2020) , offspring size was larger in mothers that had experienced restricted food abundance, a pattern commonly observed in Daphnia (Garbutt & Little, 2014; Glazier, 1992) , as well as in other organisms (Reznick et al., 1996; Vijendravarma et al., 2010) . This response was in the same direction for both dopamine and control treatments but was considerably steeper for the control treatment. The latter arose as a consequence of dopamine exposure causing a reduction in offspring size when mothers experienced restricted food ration, but not when mothers received ad lib food. In general, a larger investment in offspring size when food is limited is expected to boost offspring survival and fitness (Gorbi et al., 2011; Tessier & Consolatti, 1989) . In support of this, we found that the larger offspring of mothers having experienced restricted food ration survived better under starvation across treatments. Mothers that experience low food abundance tend to produce offspring with a larger maternal lipid reserve, which can increase offspring starvation resistance (Tessier et al., 1983) . In contrast to our hypothesis, TA B L E 2 Model comparison of candidate models for testing effects of treatment (control vs. dopamine) and food ration (high vs. restricted) on somatic growth rate, age at maturation, dry mass at maturation, offspring dry mass, and offspring longevity in D. magna. The least complex model within two ΔAIC c is given in bold. vI refers to the varIdent function Unlike the effects of dopamine exposure on offspring longevity, its effects on maternal life history reaction norms were overall as predicted based on the findings of Issa et al. (2020) . Specifically, at restricted food ration, somatic growth rate decreased, and maturation was delayed. However, dopamine exposure resulted in faster growth and earlier age at maturation across food rations compared to the control. Higher dopamine levels have been shown to promote cell proliferation and/or increase cell volume, explaining the positive effect of dopamine exposure on somatic growth rate (Huet & Franquinet, 1981; Weiss et al., 2015) . As in Issa et al. (2020) , a smaller adult size was observed at restricted food ration in the dopamine treatment, with potential costs to adult survival and reproduction (Cleuvers et al., 1997; Lampert, 2001) , as body size in Daphnia positively correlates with the ability to satisfy metabolic requirements at low food levels. Contrastingly, at high food ration, dopamine exposure increased adult size, and hence Daphnia competitiveness (Brooks & Dodson, 1965) , compared to the control. The significant effects of dopamine treatment on adult size and somatic growth rate (and consequently age at maturation) at high food ration were surprising, given that treatment effects were expected to occur at restricted food ration only (Issa et al., 2020) . There were, however, some differences in the setup between the current study and the study by Issa et al. (2020) (Rochet et al., 2000; Trippel, 1995) . Higher dopamine levels may stress individuals through dopamine oxidation. Elevated intracellular dopamine levels can promote the intracellular oxidation of dopamine, by increasing the production of reactive quinones and free radicals for a given rate of oxidation (Miyazaki & Asanuma, 2008; Sun et al., 2018) . In addition, increased extracellular dopamine can overload the antioxidant capacity in the extracellular space, causing dopamine oxidation (Blesa et al., 2015; LaVoie & Hastings, 1999; Miyazaki & Asanuma, 2008) . In the case of limited investment in antioxidant defense, dopamine oxidation can then lead to oxidative stress, which can potentially reduce longevity (Ishii et al., 1998; Moskovitz et al., 2001) . On the other hand, increased investment in antioxidant defense may lower investment in immune defense (Takahashi et al., 2017) , ultimately increasing the susceptibility of individuals to diseases and parasites. A higher investment in antioxidant defense may also lower investment in reproduction (Speakman & Garratt, 2013) . There may also be ecological costs of rapid growth associated with enhanced dopamine levels due to biotic interactions. One such cost may be higher predation risk from faster growth (Urban, 2007) , as individuals increase growth rates by increasing feeding rates, which in turn can lead to increased predator exposure (Lankford et al., 2001; Stoks et al., 2005) . Thus, whether through oxidative stress, or increased investment in antioxidant defense, dopamine oxidation may pose a proximate restriction for why Daphnia do not produce more endogenous dopamine. This in addition to more ultimate restrictions in the form of reduced survival, from oxidative stress, reduced immunity or increased predation risk, as well as lowered reproduction may be important to explore in order to understand the evolution of the dopamine system. proximate cause may be that higher dopamine levels promote dopamine oxidation. More ultimate causes involve the potential costs of dopamine oxidation to reproduction and survival, as well as ecological costs of rapid growth due to biotic interactions (predation). Hence, further understanding of the evolution of the dopamine signaling system may require a combined investigation of ultimate and proximate causes. We want to thank the staff at our lab facility for technical assistance and senior engineer Dr. Susana V. Gonzalez for performing the sam- The authors have no conflict of interest to declare. The roles of dopamine and related compounds in reward-seeking behavior across animal phyla Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 Maternal effects mediated by maternal age: From life histories to population dynamics Oxidative stress and Parkinson's disease Understanding insect life histories and senescence through a resource allocation lens Predation, body size, and composition of plankton Enhanced offspring production in Daphnia magna clones exposed to serotonin reuptake inhibitors and 4-nonylphenol. Stageand food-dependent effects Life-strategy shift by intraspecific interaction in Daphnia magna: Change in reproduction from quantity to quality Influence of food quantity and lead exposure on maturation in Daphnia magna; evidence for a trade-off mechanism Maternal food quantity affects offspring feeding rate in Daphnia magna Effects of food, genotype, and maternal size and age on offspring investment in Daphnia magna Food thresholds and body size in cladocerans Family planning in Daphnia: Resistance to starvation in offspring born to mothers grown at different food levels Anticipatory maternal effects in two different clones of Daphnia magna in response to food shortage Transgenerational effects of food availability on age at maturity and reproductive output in an asexual collembolan species Histofluorescence study and biochemical assay of catecholamines (dopamine and noradrenaline) during the course of arm-tip regeneration in the starfish A mutation in succinate dehydrogenase cytochrome b causes oxidative stress and ageing in nematodes Dopamine mediates lifehistory responses to food abundance in Daphnia ADaM, an artificial freshwater for the culture of zooplankton Survival in a varying environment phenotypic and genotypic responses in Daphnia populations Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation Evolution of intrinsic growth and energy acquisition rates. II. Trade-offs with vulnerability to predation in Menidia menidia Dopamine quinone formation and protein modification associated with the striatal neurotoxicity of methamphetamine: Evidence against a role for extracellular dopamine Environmental heterogeneity leads to higher plasticity in dry-edge populations of a semi-arid Chilean shrub: Insights into climate change responses The life history consequences of resource depression in Daphnia pulex Effect of food type and concentration on the survival, longevity, and reproduction of Daphnia magna Dopaminergic neuron-specific oxidative stress caused by dopamine itself Methionine sulfoxide reductase (MsrA) is a regulator of antioxidant defense and lifespan in mammals Test No. 211: Daphnia magna reproduction test Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models (Version R package 3.1-144) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing Maternal effects on offspring quality in poeciliid fishes Comparative analysis of phylogenetic and fishing effects in life history patterns of teleost fishes Direct and transgenerational responses to food deprivation during development in the Glanville fritillary butterfly Oxidative stress as a cost of reproduction: Beyond the simplistic trade-off model Predation cost of rapid growth: Behavioural coupling and physiological decoupling Kinetic modeling of pH-dependent oxidation of dopamine by iron and its relevance to Parkinson's disease Long-term organic selenium supplementation overcomes the trade-off between immune and antioxidant systems in pacu (Piaractus mesopotamicus) Variation in offspring size in Daphnia and consequences for individual fitness Starvation in Daphnia: Energy reserves and reproductive allocation Age at maturity as a stress indicator in fisheries Developmental plasticity and the evolution of parental effects The growth-predation risk trade-off under a growing gape-limited predation threat Effects of parental larval diet on egg size and offspring traits in Drosophila Dopamine is a key regulator in the signalling pathway underlying predatorinduced defences in Daphnia Developmental plasticity and evolution Negative relationships between population density and metabolic rates are not general Semona Issa: Conceptualization (equal); investigation (lead); writing -original draft (lead); writing -review and editing (lead). Safa Chaabani: Investigation (supporting); writing -review and editing (supporting). Alexandros G. Asimakopoulos: Investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); writing -review and editing (supporting). Veerle L. B. Jaspers: Methodology (equal); writing -review and editing (supporting). Sigurd Einum: Conceptualization (equal); methodology (equal); resources (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing -review & editing (supporting).