key: cord-0988165-na4owitn authors: Carbon, Claus-Christian title: About the Acceptance of Wearing Face Masks in Times of a Pandemic date: 2021-05-30 journal: Iperception DOI: 10.1177/20416695211021114 sha: e12c591ebfb434e026e93f212341c35a2cdf9be5 doc_id: 988165 cord_uid: na4owitn Wearing face masks in times of COVID-19 is one of the essential keystones for effectively decreasing the rate of new infections and thus for mitigating the negative consequences for individuals as well as for society. Acceptance of wearing masks is still low in many countries, making it extremely difficult to keep the pandemic at bay. In an experimental study, participants (N = 88) had to assess how strange they felt when wearing a face mask while being exposed to displays of groups of varying numbers of mask wearers. Three different types of face masks were shown: simple homemade masks, FFP2 masks, and loop scarfs. The higher the frequency of people wearing masks in the displayed social group, the less strange the participants felt about themselves, an essential precondition for accepting wearing masks. This effect of a descriptive social norm was particularly effective when people saw others wearing less intrusive masks, here, simple homemade masks. less than 5,000 deaths overall; Robert Koch Institute, 2020). Feeling "strange" (or "normal," on the other hand) is closely linked to descriptive social norms that are present in a given social environment. This implies that the mere frequency of mask wearers in society might be an essential factor in moving individuals to wear face masks. This is a paradigmatic example for the importance of understanding the psychology of pandemics (Taylor, 2019) . A psychological perspective allows the assessment of why people do or do not do certain things, which is the prerequisite for finding ways to change behaviour. In the case of mask-wearing, the question is as follows: How can we change the attitude towards and the feelings about wearing masks? One possible psychological answer is via the social norm (see Hesslinger et al., 2017a Hesslinger et al., , 2017b ). The present study tested this possibility by confronting participants with pictures that show social groups, each with varying frequencies of persons who wear different kinds of face masks. We assessed whether the different social norms that were thus implicitly communicated affected the participants' feeling about wearing a face mask themselves. The present study aims to understand how our feeling to feel strange when imaging to wear an own face mask is modulated by a group of people with persons wearing more or less face masks (from 0, 1, 2, 6 up to 12 out of 12 people in that group). As we hypothesise that a possible effect of the number of people wearing face masks is related to social norms-here, creating mainly a "descriptive norm" (Legros & Cislaghi, 2020, p.67) , we were also interested in how participant's sex played a role in this respect as sex-related processing of social norms were regularly documented (e.g., Felonneau & Becker, 2008; Trelohan, 2021) , also in healthrelated contexts (Fisher, 2009; Magallares & Morales, 2013) . Furthermore, we employed a variety of face mask types to increase the heterogeneity of stimuli. We were also interested, which face mask of the types commonly used in everyday life, does induce the lowest strangeness feeling when imagined to use. Knowledge about feelings with specific face masks might help to provide practical advice for policymakers to increase the acceptance of using face masks in publicity. Eighty-eight participants volunteered for the study (M age ¼ 28.8 years [15-87 years] , N female ¼ 62). Based on the comparison of Model #2 and Model #1 which directly tested the effect of the number of masks (see details in the Results section), we calculated the test power post hoc via R package simr (Green & MacLeod, 2016) . For both models to be compared, we set the intercept to 5.0 and the slopes of the fixed effects to þ0.1 for loop masks and -0.1 for simple face masks; for Model #2, we set the slope of the fixed effect to -0.1 for the number of mask wearers in the group of people; random intercept variance was set to 1.0 and residual standard deviation to 2.0. Based on the final number of complete data sets of N ¼ 88, let us achieve a post hoc test power of 95.0% (95% confidence interval [CI] [75.2%, 99.9%]). (Note: Initially, the required simple sample size was determined according to test planning for linear model, but this turned out to be obsolete due to the change in the analysis strategy). Based on frontal photos of 12 white European (previously called "Caucasian") faces (six female, six male) taken from the color Feret database (Phillips et al., 1998 (Phillips et al., , 2000 , we created different versions of displays of these faces. The base version showed all faces without masks at random places in the display making up a social group. For the further displays, we employed different masks which we photographed correctly positioned on an artificial head model: (a) a typical homemade (beige) community mask-in the following called "simple mask," (b) an FFP2 mask (N95; white), and (c) a black loop scarf (see Figure 1 ), We cut out the images of the masks via Photoshop to be able to apply them to the different faces of the social group. For each mask, we generated five different configurations, always consisting of the 12 faces: (a) only one female wearing a mask, (b) only one male wearing a mask, (c) one female and one male wearing masks, (d) three females and three males wearing masks (see Figure 2 ), and (e) all persons wearing masks. This yielded 1 [base] þ 3 [maskType: FFP2, loop, simple] Â 5 [nMasks: number of masks] ¼ 16 versions. The stimulus material can be retrieved from the https://osf.io/gu6xr/. The experiment, realised via the SoSciSurvey online engine, was conducted between April 20, 2020 (15:47 local time) and April 23, 2020 (16:56 local time). This was before any general legal obligations to wear masks in Germany were in action. Prior to the experimental session, written informed consent was obtained from each participant. All data were collected anonymously. The participants were exposed to all display versions, one after another, with the order of the displays being randomised across participants. Participants were asked to imagine having a face mask on in the social situation signalled by the respective display they viewed ("Imagine: You are wearing a mouth-nose mask yourself and are now facing these persons. How do you feel in such a situation?"). While observing the scene without time pressure (M response time ¼20.7 s), they were asked to answer two consecutive questions on a 7point scale (1¼ not strange at all, 7¼ very strange): (a) Feel myself: "While wearing MY mask I am feeling . . . ," (b) Feel others: "The others appear . . . " (a capitalised "my" was also used in the experimental version). There was no time limit for giving a response. This should allow a full expression of the participants' imagery for the respective social scene. The participants provided an informed written consent before the experiment started. All procedures were in accordance with the national ethical standards on human experimentation provided by the German Psychological Society (DGPs) and with the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, as revised in 2008. The study was in full accordance with the ethical guidelines of the University of Bamberg and was approved by an umbrella evaluation for psychophysical testing of the university ethics committee (Ethikrat) on August 18, 2017. Specific ethical approval beyond these means was not sought for the present study because the experimental treatment was not susceptible to trigger particularly negative emotions beyond typical displays of humans with face masks in public life, also taking into account that minors of 15þ years were involved (n ¼ 6 consisting of one 15-year-old person with special permission of his parents and five 17-year-old persons). The entire procedure lasted approximately 5-10 minutes. The data were processed using the R 4.0.4 (R Core Team, 2021). In addition to the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) to perform linear mixed effects analyses, R packages lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017) and ggplot2 (Wickham, 2012) were used during the analysis of the data. The entire anonymised data set is available at the Open Science Framework https://osf. io/gu6xr/. As we were mainly interested in the impact of mask-wearing of others on our two dependent variables, we initially defined a linear mixed model as null model (Model #0) with no fixed effects but only the participants as random factor. We subsequently added effects of interest and tested these models against the respective models without the effects in question via likelihood ratio tests. Each model's residuals were visually inspected to exclude models deviating from homoscedasticity or normality. Table 1 shows the subsequent testing of the models towards best fitting. Table 2 depicts the estimates of finally selected models for the dependent variables "strangeness regarding myself" (strangeness-myself) and "strangeness regarding others" (strangeness-others). The null model (Model #0) employed the participants (Subj) as random (intercept) factor only. Model #1 added maskType as fixed factor. For Model #2, we added nMasks as fixed ordered factor; for Model #3, we furthermore added the participants' sex (sSex) as fixed factor. Model #4 further extended the model by adding nMasks as random (slope) factor. For unordered as well as ordered factors, treatment contrasts were used. Regarding the first dependent variable on the feeling strange about one's own wearing a mask (strangeness-myself), we were able to identify Model #4 as the best fitting. This model explained 79.2% of the variance (see Table 2 ). For this final model (see Table 1 for details about the selection of the model), we revealed maskType as a significant fixed effect, with FFP2 masks and loop scarfs showing higher strangeness-myself ratings than simple face masks. The estimate for loop scarfs was doubled compared to FFP2 masks. Regarding the number of masks available in the social group of people (nMasks), we uncovered effects for all comparisons of numbers against the "none" condition, where none of the people wore a mask. A deeper look revealed that the decrease of feeling strange about one's own wearing a mask was closely related to the number of mask wearers in the social group. All estimates were much larger in absolute numbers than for the factor maskType, illustrating a relative large (negative) influence of nMasks on strangeness-myself ratings. Furthermore, male participants expressed a lower feeling strange about one's own wearing a mask. Regarding the second dependent variable on the feeling strange about others wearing a mask (strangeness-others), we were able to identify (again) Model #4 as the best fitting. This model explained 62.7% of the variance (see Table 2 ). For this final model (see Table 1 for details about the selection of the model), we revealed maskType as a significant fixed effect, with FFP2 masks and loop scarfs showing higher strangeness-others ratings than simple face masks. The estimate for loop scarfs was just a bit larger compared to FFP2 masks. Regarding the number of masks available in the social group of people (nMasks), we uncovered effects for all comparisons of numbers (1, 2, 6, 12 ¼ all) against the "none" condition, where none of the people wore a mask. All estimates were much larger than for the factor maskType, illustrating a relative large (positive) influence of nMasks on strangeness-others ratings. As shown in Figure 3 , we uncovered a clear decrease of feeling strange about one's own wearing a mask (strangeness-myself) with increasing numbers of masks (nMasks) worn by the people depicted in the group. Meanwhile, the participants evaluated the appearance of the persons shown in the social scene as being increasingly strange with increasing numbers of mask wearers in the group (strangeness-others). Both effects were found to be significant when tested with linear mixed models against a null model without taking the number of mask wearers into account (p's<.0001). Based on the final models (#4), the estimates in absolute numbers for the factor nMasks were much larger for strangeness-myself than for strangeness-others. This means that the clear decrease in feeling strange about oneself when more persons wore face masks in the group of people was not only accompanied by a reversed effect of feeling strangeness (oddness) of the group of viewed people but was also more pronounced. Wearing face masks in times of COVID-19 is one of the essential keystones for effectively decreasing the rate of new infections and for mitigating the negative consequences for individuals as well as for society. Wearing masks does not belong to natural human's habits and is still not easily acceptable for many people (Wong, 2020) and has been emerged as a political issue (Rabinovitch-Fox, 2020)-many people just feel strange when wearing masks (Robb, 2021 ) and therefore will not follow recommendations to put on masks in public. Here, we tested how the mere exposure to people in the social environment who do or do not wear masks can dramatically change the feeling of strangeness when wearing a mask oneself. It is of particular interest that the number of mask wearers had dissociative effects on both dependent variables employed in the present study: The participants experienced the idea of wearing masks themselves as less and less strange when more people in the shown social group wore face masks as well. At the same time, however, they kept perceiving the other mask wearers in the displayed social group as strange, especially when they wore loop scarfs, in this case, black, loop scarfs. We suggest that this dissociation of effects is the outcome of two different mechanisms that are at work here: A more perceptual one and a more cognitive (normative) one. To illustrate this, we would like to give an example: Imagine you are invited by a good friend who grew up in Venice to visit his/her beautiful hometown to which you have never been. You travel to Venice, and upon arriving there in a small taxi boat, you realise that the world-famous Carnival of Venice is well underway. People all around you, including your friend who is welcoming you at the landing stage, are wearing the typical, highly elaborate masks. You were not prepared for the festival, so you do not have a mask. You will, most probably, experience the following: The people around you will appear somewhat strange to you-this mainly perceptual effect is based on an insufficient familiarity with the specific disguise. Furthermore, with such masks on, we cannot rely anymore on typical processes which we effortlessly use in normal, everyday life without any masks, for example, reading the emotional state (Carbon, 2020) and further mental states (Schmidtmann et al., 2020) of others by merely processing the holistic facial information. Yet, you will probably feel less strange about yourself as soon as you put on a mask as wellthis effect traces back to the descriptive social norm that is established by the outward appearance (the shared dress code) of the majority of people around you in this specific situation. This effect of taking social norms into account is a cognitively based effect. It is important to understand this perceptuo-cognitive dissociation because it is not limited to wearing masks: We often adopt descriptive social norms that are signalled by the empirical conditions of present situations, and we try to behave like the others around us, but this does not necessarily mean that we like or would principally endorse this behaviour as well. In the present experiment, the perception of others as being strange was particularly strong for loop scarfs and FFP2 masks. The loop scarfs resemble so-called bandanas-may be because of negative connotations triggered by the resemblance with the clich e masking of bank robbers in movies or cartoons. The FFP2 masks, at least at the early phase of the pandemic when this study was conducted, were obviously also seen as being strange-but probably due to another phenomenon: Most people were unfamiliar with this kind of mask which should have fundamentally changed meanwhile due to the everyday usage of such masks. So, which masks seem to be optimal for everyday usage? From a physical (Verma et al., 2020) , mathematical (Mittal et al., 2020) as well as a medical (Chu et al., 2020) perspective, there are clear answers to this question: The mask should be capable of filtering a maximum of airborne particles, so the certified face masks with FFP2 (N95; filtering at least 95% of airborne particles, if they show a diameter of at least 100 nm; O'Dowd et al., 2020) and FFP3 (N99; 99%) filtering levels seem to be the best (O'Dowd et al., 2020) . From a psychological perspective, the answer might differ. In the present study, we observed least perceived strangeness when observing other people wearing less intrusive masks, concretely simple self-made masks, while loop scarfs and FFP2 masks showed higher levels of perceived strangeness in this respect. Meanwhile, participants did not feel particularly strange themselves, actually even a bit less strange than the others shown as a social group. Such simple face masks offer a series of other advantages: First, they are relatively easy and comfortable to use (Yao et al., 2019) , they can be easily and privately produced by simple means, and they are cheap enough to equip many people around the globe in high quantity and fresh quality. Second, as the suggestions for wearing masks for private persons refer to the protection of others and because there is no clear evidence of a difference in protecting others between simple masks and FFP2/N95 masks (Jefferson et al., 2020) , simple masks prevent a shortage of professional medical masks that should be primarily reserved for medical workers. Third, in our study, the simple masks showed the highest acceptance rate in terms of feeling least odd when imaging wearing such a mask. This is an important precondition to face masks actually being worn in different situations and over a longer period (see MacIntyre et al., 2009; MacIntyre & Chughtai, 2015) , especially by non-medical workers (Matusiak et al., 2020) . Furthermore, they do not emit large amounts of microplastic fibres as one-way masks might do (Fadare & Okoffo, 2020) . Of course, such general ideas should be adjusted for specific contexts and fields of applications, for example, it was shown that wearing masks has race-specific effects on other perceivers (Christiani et al., in press ). This last point also calls for extensions of such studies as we only tested a relatively narrow sample employing White European faces only wearing three different types of face masks that were popular and available in April 2020. For instance, there are reports and societally meaningful discussions on interactive effects between wearing specific masks and ethnic background and the morphological group of the wearer, for example, black bandanas worn by people of colour which triggered racial stereotypes (Ray, 2020) . This should be systematically analysed to understand and to counteract against such mechanisms. In general, our results will also assist policymakers in predicting the future acceptance of wearing masks in which generally more people comply with these new hygienic practices, following role models wearing masks and propagating them instead of denying and problematising them (Hornsey et al., 2020) . COVID-19 pandemic repercussions on plastic and antiviral polymeric textile causing pollution on beaches and coasts of South America Fitting linear mixed effects models using lme4 [sparse matrix methods; linear mixed models Wearing face masks strongly confuses counterparts in reading emotions Surgical face masks impair human face matching performance for familiar and unfamiliar faces Masks and racial stereotypes in a pandemic: The case for surgical masks Physical distancing, face masks, and eye protection to prevent person-to-person transmission of SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis Covid-19 face masks: A potential source of microplastic fibers in the environment Pro-environmental attitudes and behavior: Revealing perceived social desirability. Revue Internationale De Psychologie Sociale The impact of socially conveyed norms on the reporting of sexual behavior and attitudes by men and women Face masks are in. The Atlantic simr: An R package for power analysis of generalised linear mixed models by simulation The sense of being watched is modulated by arousal and duration of the perceptual episode. i-Perception Social factors in aesthetics: Social conformity pressure and a sense of being watched affect aesthetic judgments. i-Perception Donald Trump and vaccination: The effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy Gender, face mask perceptions, and face mask wearing: Are men being dangerous during the COVID-19 pandemic? Physical interventions to interrupt or reduce the spread of respiratory viruses: Systematic review Physical interventions to interrupt or reduce the spread of respiratory viruses. Part 1 -Face masks, eye protection and person distancing: Systematic review and meta-analysis COVID-19 Creating another problem? Sustainable solution for PPE disposal through LCA approach. Environment Development and Sustainability {lmerTest} Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models Why are Asian countries outperforming the Western world in controlling COVID-19 pandemic? Pathogens and Global Health Mapping the social-norms literature: An overview of reviews Face mask use and control of respiratory virus transmission in households Facemasks for the prevention of infection in healthcare and community settings Gender differences in antifat attitudes Behavioral immune system linked to responses to the threat of COVID-19 I'm smiling back at you": Exploring the impact of mask wearing on communication in healthcare Face masks use during the COVID-19 pandemic: Differences in attitudes and practices between medical and non-medical students. A survey of 2256 students in Poland Compliance with containment measures to the COVID-19 pandemic over time: Do antisocial traits matter? A mathematical framework for estimating risk of airborne transmission of COVID-19 with application to face mask use and social distancing Understanding the impact of face mask usage through epidemic simulation of large social networks Theories and simulations of complex social systems Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau Face masks and respirators in the fight against the COVID-19 pandemic: A review of current materials, advances and future perspectives Keep your (social) distance: Pathogen concerns and social perception in the time of COVID-19 The FERET evaluation methodology for face recognition algorithms The FERET database and evaluation procedure for face-recognition algorithms Impact of face masks on voice radiation R: A language and environment for statistical computing This is why some black men fear wearing face masks during a pandemic, Opinion. The Washington Post Why some people like wearing masks In the blink of an eye: Reading mental states from briefly presented eye regions. i-Perception Disease avoidance in the time of COVID-19: The behavioral immune system is associated with concern and preventative health behaviors The use of facemasks may not lead to an increase in hand-face contact The psychology of pandemics: Preparing for the next global outbreak of infectious disease Do women engage in pro-environmental behaviours in the public sphere due to social expectations? The Effects of social norm-based persuasive messages Professional and home-made face masks reduce exposure to respiratory infections among the general population Visualizing the effectiveness of face masks in obstructing respiratory jets Wearing face masks in public during the influenza season may reflect other positive hygiene practices in Japan ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis The psychology behind why some people refuse to wear face masks. The Huffington Post Advice on the use of masks in the community, during home care, and in health care settings in the context of COVID-19: Interim guidance Characteristics of and important lessons from the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak in China: Summary of a report of 72 314 cases from the Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention Impact of structural features on dynamic breathing resistance of healthcare face mask. The Science of the Total Environment How to cite this article About the acceptance of wearing face masks in times of a pandemic. i-Perception where he currently holds a full professorship leading the Department of General Psychology and Methodology and the "Forschungsgruppe EPAEG"-a research group devoted to enhancing the knowledge, methodology, and enthusiasm in the fields of cognitive ergonomics, psychological aesthetics, and Gestalt (see www.experimental-psychology.com and www. epaeg.de for more details). In 2013, he founded the Bamberg Graduate School of Affective and Cognitive Sciences (BaGrACS), Germany, devoted to a research focus on the link between affective and cognitive processing. CCC is editor-in-chief of the scientific journal Art & Perception, section editor of Perception and i-Perception, associate editor of Frontiers in Psychology I would like to thank the Hesslinger family for equipping me with different types of face masks. Furthermore, I am indebted to the anonymous reviewers of a previous version of the article who inspired me to expand the view on face masks and to further develop the psychological theory behind mask-wearing. The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Claus-Christian Carbon https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3446-9347 Note 1. The term mask will be used within this article to refer to typical face masks used by nonprofessionals, including improvised masks, for example, loop scarfs, dust, and surgical maskssuch protective items are now frequently called community masks.Author Biography