key: cord-0969371-2ggmge8i authors: Scarpelli, Serena; Alfonsi, Valentina; Gorgoni, Maurizio; Camaioni, Milena; Giannini, Anna Maria; De Gennaro, Luigi title: Age-Related Effect of Sleepiness on Driving Performance: A Systematic-Review date: 2021-08-19 journal: Brain Sci DOI: 10.3390/brainsci11081090 sha: 549312b5c0e51dfd15e1bb59ce64c5a0bcd2a0a0 doc_id: 969371 cord_uid: 2ggmge8i Background: Several studies highlighted that sleepiness affects driving abilities. In particular, road traffic injuries due to excessive daytime sleepiness are about 10–20%. Considering that aging is related to substantial sleep changes and the number of older adults with driving license is increasing, the current review aims to summarize recent studies on this issue. Further, we intend to provide insights for future research. Methods: From the 717 records screened, ten articles were selected and systematically reviewed. Results: Among the selected articles, (a) five studies investigated sleepiness only by self-reported standardized measures; (b) two studies assessed sleepiness also using a behavioral task; (c) three studies obtained objective measures by electroencephalographic recordings. Conclusions: The available literature on the topic reports several limitations. Overall, many findings converge in evidencing that older drivers are less vulnerable to sleep loss and sleepiness-related driving impairments than young adults. These discrepancies in sleepiness vulnerability between age groups may be ascribed to differences in subjects’ lifestyles. Moreover, it has been hypothesized that older adults self-regulate their driving and avoid specific dangerous situations. We believe that an easy protocol to objectively evaluate the vigilance level in elderly and young adults is required, and further studies are needed. Driving is a crucial aspect of human functioning to keep a high quality of life and independence. The safety at the wheel is affected by multiple variables, such as visual abilities [1] , attention and decision-making [2] , age [3] , and drowsiness [4] . Specifically, sleepiness has been recognized as one of the main factors that affect driving skills [5] . Sleepiness has been defined as difficulty maintaining wakefulness without external stimuli [6] . Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is present in 10-20% of the general population and negatively impacts reaction time, vigilance, and judgment of performance at the wheel [5] . The sleepiness level can be evaluated by subjective or objective methods and may be influenced by poor sleep hygiene, circadian rhythm, or alcohol and drugs [7, 8] . Epidemiological observations revealed that young and adult drivers (aged 15-44 years) account for 48% of road traffic deaths worldwide [9] , and road traffic injuries due to sleepiness are about 10-20% [10] [11] [12] . Recent trends indicate that motor vehicle incidents are among the top five causes of mortality by 2030 [9] . For this reason, in the last decades, the relationship between driving performance and sleepiness received growing attention [5, 13] . The available literature showed that fatal road accidents provoked by a driver falling asleep at the wheel are more common among young individuals [10, [14] [15] [16] [17] . Nevertheless, almost 7000 older adults were involved in motor vehicle accidents in the USA, considering that it The systematic review included studies by following the PRISMA guidelines [31] . The study focused on published original quantitative studies on the relationship between sleepiness/drowsiness and driving performance in older adults. A search was conducted on PubMed, entering the following search terms in English: "Sleepiness" OR "Drowsiness" OR "Vigilance" AND "Driving" OR "Driving skills" AND "Older adults" OR "Elderly" OR "Aging". Search fields were title, abstract, and keywords. Only quantitative research and original papers published in the last twenty years were further analyzed. Two experts selected eligible articles through a multi-step procedure (title reading, abstract, and full-text assessment). The literature search was completed with a manual search, reviewing the references contained in the selected articles. Predefined inclusion criteria were: (1) papers including older adults (≥55 years) with data analysis, which explicitly considered the age factor or different age groups; (2) no subjects with relevant neurological problems and neurodegenerative diseases; (3) no studies exclusively aimed to assess the medications effect on sleepiness and/or driving; (4) protocols with an assessment of sleepiness by objective, subjective or behavioral methods; (5) protocols including driving performance evaluation through real driving, self-reported driving-related information, driving simulation or testing driving-related skills (no automated driving); (6) peer-reviewed articles (not just abstracts or conference papers); (7) no reviews, or meta-analyses; (8) articles written in English; (9) a year of publication between 2001 and 2021 (last twenty years). Duplicates were removed. Then, title and abstract screening was carried out. All potentially relevant papers were independently assessed for their eligibility. Studies that included older adults but not focusing on the difference between age groups were also excluded. A consensus session solved any disagreement between reviewers with a third reviewer. Figure 1 shows the flow chart of the article selection. Ten articles fulfilled the inclusion criteria [32] [33] [34] [35] [36] [37] [38] [39] [40] [41] . Among the selected papers, (a) five studies investigated sleepiness only by self-reported standardized measures [34] [35] [36] [37] 40] ; (b) two studies assessed sleepiness also using a behavioral task [39, 41] ; and (c) three studies obtained objective measures by electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings [32, 33, 38] . Table 1 summarizes the main characteristics and results of the ten papers included in the final study synthesis. The project by Vaz Fragoso and colleagues produced three studies on longitudinal data collecting from a sample of 430 older adults (≥70 years) [34] [35] [36] . They aimed to assess the relationship between drowsiness/sleep complaints and driving capacity. Both sleepiness and driving measures were self-reported in all studies [34] [35] [36] . Moreover, other sleep questionnaires to evaluate insomnia and sleep apnea risk were administered. Older adults had chronic disturbances, e.g., diabetes and hypertension, while a small part of the sample reported sleep apnea. As assessed by the Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS) [42] , subjective sleepiness was related both to sleep apnea risk and chronic disease. At the baseline study [34] , the authors revealed that subjects reporting low nighttime driving ability had significant insomnia symptoms with difficulties falling asleep or maintaining sleep, distress with sleep, and sleep-related impairment in functioning. Lower levels of driver self-ratings capacity and driving mileage were also associated with daytime sleepiness. Surprisingly, high scores in sleep apnea were not linked to driving measures, and sleep disturbances were not related to prior adverse driving episodes [34] . (1) Driving mileage (2) Adverse driving events evaluated every 6 months for 2 years. Longitudinal design 215 older drivers had at least one adverse driving events. Subjects with adverse driving events showed at the baseline higher ESS scores and higher driving frequency than older adults not experiencing adverse events. Older good sleepers and older insomniacs did not differ in driving performance and driving-related skills, as well as in PVT performance. Good sleepers showed faster mean reaction times at the PVT in the morning than evening. Younger drivers reported higher tiredness than older adults during the 50-min monotonous driving. AMT significantly advantaged the younger group who made more driving errors than older drivers during the first and second monotonous segments. Older adults did not show increased errors with fatigue. AMT did not negatively impact on their driving performance, but they showed increased speed variability when driving with AMT. Bartolacci In a second study, the authors assessed the longitudinal association between sleep disturbances and adverse driving events in the same cohort of older adults [35] . Longitudinal evaluations of these episodes (i.e., crash or traffic infraction and near-crash or getting lost) were planned every six months for two years. Overall, 418 older drivers participated in the follow-up. Among the participants, 215 older drivers had at least one crash, traffic infraction, near-crash, or getting lost. Crashes appeared to be correlated with a traffic infraction and near-crash with getting lost. Subjects with these adverse events showed at the baseline higher ESS scores and greater driving frequency than older adults not experiencing these events at the wheel. However, it should be noted that the median ESS score was not clinically relevant [35] . Moreover, sleep disturbances did not significantly affect the odds of having adverse driving events [35] . In a third article, Vaz Fragoso and colleagues evaluated the relationship between sleep disturbances at the baseline and the subsequent driving cessation over a two-years period [36] . Results revealed that insomnia, daytime sleepiness, and sleep apnea risk were not longitudinally associated with driving cessation. Nevertheless, insomnia was the only sleep-related variable predicting the reduction of driving mileage. Moreover, multiple pharmacological treatments and age were associated with decreased mileage, while being male predicted increasing in daily driving mileage. Another group aimed to cross-sectionally assess the relationship between near-crash/misses or accidents and sleepiness [37] . As in the previous studies, the ESS was used to evaluate self-perceived daytime sleepiness. Moreover, information about other disorders and driving behavior were collected from a sample of 4774 French drivers. Subjects were subdivided into four age groups: 18-30, 31-50, 51-65, and >65. Among the near misses, 46% were sleep-related. Interestingly, the regression model showed that being male, being young (18-30 years), being sensitive to caffeine, and having at least an episode of sleepiness at the wheel in the last year were the best predictors of near misses [37] . More recently, to compare the driving performance of younger and older drivers, Song et al. [40] recruited 68 healthy and cognitively intact subjects (29 younger vs. 39 older adults). They were tested through a driving simulator in two different conditions: (a) during monotonous driving simulation; and (b) during two 10-min driving sessions accompanied by an alertness-maintaining task (AMT). AMT aimed to promote vigilance in drivers and contained trivia questions on different topics. Finally, a visual analogue of fatigue scale was used to assess subjective levels of tiredness and stress. The authors found that AMT benefitted the younger group during the monotonous driving as compared with AMT condition. Younger drivers reported higher tiredness than older adults during the 50 min monotonous driving. Moreover, AMT significantly advantaged the younger group who made more driving errors than older drivers during the first and second monotonous segments. Older adults did not show increased mistakes with fatigue. Surprisingly, AMT did not negatively impact their driving performance. However, they showed increased speed variability when driving with AMT [40] . Leufkens et al. [39] and Bartolacci et al. [41] used the psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) [43] as an instrument to assess psychomotor vigilance. The PVT is based on a simple visual reaction time test and is generally accompanied by a self-reported sleepiness rating (e.g., Karolinska sleepiness scale-KSS [44] ). The study by Leufkens et al. [38] involved 63 subjects with 50-75 years who were subdivided into three groups: insomnia group with medications, insomnia group without medications, and healthy subjects. All participants performed the highway driving test [45] , measuring tracking road performance. The standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP) was used as an index of individual driving performance. Moreover, other tasks were administered to assess driving-related skills (e.g., selective attention, decision making, stimulus interpretation, speed, and adaptive motor response to driving events; divided attention). A preliminary sleep assessment with polysomnography was performed, not finding differences between groups. Subjective sleepiness and sleep complaints were also assessed. Older good sleepers and older insomniacs did not differ in driving performance and driving-related skills, as well as in PVT performance. Reaction times at the PVT differed only between morning and evening performance [39] . Indeed, mean reaction time in the PVT was faster in the morning than the evening in controls, but not in subjects with insomnia. More recently, Bartolacci et al. [41] compared 40 healthy older adults with 40 young subjects. Along with the subjective assessment of sleep quality and self-reported sleepiness, the PVT was administered to collect a behavioral sleepiness evaluation. Moreover, drivingrelated skills were tested: selective attention, tachistoscopic perception (e.g., the ability to obtain an overview, the skills about visual orientation, and the perceptual speed), and the risk assumption. Older adults reported lower sleep efficiency and worse performance in PVT (tendency to make more mistakes and slowing reaction times in the 10% of fastest responses) than the younger group. However, self-reported sleepiness was more significant in young subjects. In addition, older adults had lower performance in attention and tachistoscopic perception tests but appeared to be more cautious in traffic situations. The regression model showed that age was the only best predictor of cognitive driving-related skills, while sleepiness did not show any relation with these abilities. Excluding the age factor, the regression revealed that subjective sleepiness, PVT scores, and sleep quality tend to predict driving-related performances [41] . Campagne et al. [32] , Lowden et al. [33] , and Filtness et al. [38] assessed the relationship between sleepiness and driving by recordings EEG during a driving simulation. Specific features of the EEG recordings are described in Table 2 . EEG recordings allowed the authors to consider alpha and/or theta power as indices of sleepiness. Campagne et al. [32] compared older adults (60-70 years) with two other age groups (20-30 years and 40-50 years). One group was required to drive on a lighted motorway, while the other group drove on a nonlighted motorway condition. EEG was acquired at baseline periods before the driving test and during the monotonous and pro-longed night-driving simulation. No difference between the two lighting conditions and among the three age groups was found concerning the EEG patterns. Driving errors did not vary between the two lighting conditions. However, driving errors such as "running-off the road accidents" were more frequent in young subjects. Differently, speed variability was higher in older drivers, consistently with the observations of Song et al. [40] . Only young adults showed in both conditions a positive correlation between alpha power (high alpha power = low vigilance) and driving errors (i.e., running-off the road episodes on the left-hand side). Separate analyses on the two lighting conditions revealed a positive correlation between sleepiness and driving errors in the younger group during the lighted condition. For the older drivers, no correlation was found between any type of running-off the road errors and vigilance level assessed through the alpha power, whatever the lighted conditions. Differently, a positive correlation between the theta power and driving errors was found for older adults. Moreover, the theta power-representing a high level of sleepiness-was correlated to the speed variability in this group during a lighted condition. In the older group, excessive, low, and overall speed values were associated with the total number of running-off the road accidents. Moreover, the analysis of the time course of the EEG power in the alpha and theta bands showed a significant increase in both indices during the prolonged driving task at night. Driving errors increased progressively as the number of laps increased. A physiological assessment through EEG during the driving simulation was carried out also by Lowden et al. [33] . Ten young drivers (18-24 years) were compared with ten older drivers (aged 55-64 years). In this study, each subject participated in two 45-min driving simulations: (a) morning driving and (b) evening driving. The driving period was divided into 5-min bins (9 intervals), and the factor "time" was included in the analysis. Results showed that EEG power activity increased across the nine 5-min bins. In particular, alpha and sigma power (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) showed an increase. A main effect of age was observed for higher frequency band (12-32 Hz) that being increased during both conditions in older drivers. Age differences became bigger at the end of the night. Indeed, older drivers showed increased power in the frequency 10-16 Hz. Moreover, the sigma 1 band (12-14 Hz) increased across the time and was higher in older adults. Further, after driving, increased salivary cortisol in older adults was found compared with the younger group. Subjective sleepiness evaluated by KSS was higher during the night driving in both groups. However, sleepiness appeared to be more pronounced in young subjects than older drivers during the night and at the end of the driving simulation. The last EEG study by Filtness et al. [38] evaluated sleepiness at the wheel only in the early morning, considering two different situations: (a) after a regular night; and (b) after a sleep restriction. Twenty young subjects were compared with 19 older drivers. EEG was recorded during the driving task, and subjective sleepiness was assessed at regular intervals. After normal sleep, no difference between groups was found. Following sleep restriction, both groups had more driving accidents. A time effect was also observed: both age groups increased the number of incidents during the task. Moreover, in this condition, the younger group showed significantly more sleepiness-related incidents during driving simulation. Partly according to Campagne et al. [32] , alpha and theta (4-11 Hz) EEG power was higher in younger drivers than older drivers. Consistently, subjective sleepiness (KSS) positively correlated with EEG measures after sleep restriction in both groups. A critical discussion of the selected articles may encourage the reflection on potential instruments and protocols to systematically assess the effect of sleepiness on driving performance in older adults. Firstly, the current literature highlighted that the relationship between sleepiness and driving performance during aging is not linear. Self-reported studies by Vaz Fragoso et al. [34] revealed that daytime sleepiness and insomnia were related to lower self-perceived driving abilities and lower mileage, especially concerning nighttime driving. However, higher sleepiness at the baseline and a higher driving frequency were longitudinally associated with the probability of having a crash over the subsequent period, while clinically relevant sleep disorders were not associated with accidents [35] . These results suggest that both insomnia symptoms and sleepiness deserve attention in evaluating driving performance. This evidence is strengthened by the fact that higher insomnia scores and apnea risk at the baseline predicted a reduction in driving mileage at the follow-up [36] . To some extent, this is consistent with the hypothesis that older adults self-regulate their driving behaviors [2, 46, 47] . In other words, older drivers having sleep disturbances such as insomnia and/or EDS symptoms may change their driving practices avoiding high-risk settings or driving for shorter distances [36] . Moreover, it should be hypothesized that the relationship between driving, aging, and sleepiness is mediated by other factors, such as the quality of health and medical conditions [48] . Indeed, older drivers in the investigations by Vaz Fragoso et al. [34] [35] [36] were affected by mixed chronic pathologies such as diabetes, hypertension, and arthritis, despite the median scores of both tests assessing insomnia and EDS were in the normal range. It is worth noting that driving, especially during the night, required different intact functions (i.e., vision, attention, cognition) that in older adults may be impaired, and sleepiness could worsen the chronic conditions or be a consequence of these [49] . In this vein, pharmacotherapy may express a high medical burden that would influence both sleepiness and driving abilities [36, [50] [51] [52] . On the one hand, it has been shown that the use of multiple medications longitudinally impacted the driving mileage [36] . On the other hand, it seems that older chronic hypnotics users did not show impaired driving behavior than healthy older drivers [39] . Actually, Leufkens et al. [39] , comparing insomniacs and good sleepers, found differences only in the reaction time task (PVT). The fact that controls reported faster reaction time in the morning than evening was in line with the evidence that older adults tend to have a morning chronotype [53, 54] . As previously mentioned, aging is associated with a phase advance of sleep-wake rhythm that would provoke more drowsiness during the evening than morning [26] . Consistently, some studies highlighted older adults avoided driving in the evening or at night [2] . In this respect, it should be noted that Bartolacci et al. [41] placed the experimental session in the afternoon between 4.00 and 7.00 p.m., so the time of the day may have influenced the outcomes in older drivers [55, 56] . Indeed, the older adults' performances were worse than the young group for the tachistoscopic perception test and selective attention test [41] . Nevertheless, Bartolacci and colleagues [41] did not reveal a significant correlation between driving-related skills and vigilance. Differently, in line with previous literature [57, 58] , younger drivers showed a high propensity to risk-taking (i.e., greater impulsiveness) than older adults [41] , and being young represented a better predictor for traffic crashes [37] . This evidence seems to be-once again-partly in line with the idea that during aging, people tend to assume protective behaviors and self-regulate their driving performance because of the reduced cognitive driving-related abilities, such as selective attention and tachistoscopic perception [41] . Regarding the comparisons between sleepiness levels in different age groups, several studies found that younger adults reported higher subjective sleepiness during driving performances [33, 40, 41] . Consistently, younger subjects showed significant advantages at driving using a task aimed to promote vigilance (AMT) throughout monotonous drive [40] . Interestingly, older adults benefitted from AMT, not showing impaired sustained attention during the driving. Younger drivers could be more sensitive to sleepiness, as highlighted by previous studies [59] . Actually, several investigations reported that low vigilance in young drivers was related to road accidents [16, 60] . The greater sensitivity to sleepiness at the wheel of young drivers appears to be supported by EEG investigations. For instance, Lowden et al. [33] observed that the older group had higher sigma power and greater cortisol levels than the young group. Both these indices would protect older adults from driving sleepiness-related errors. In other words, young drivers could more rapidly develop severe sleepiness during pro-longed driving, also affecting the odd of having crashes. Accordingly, Campagne and colleagues [32] found that younger subjects had more running-off-the-road incidents. Further, alpha power, as an expression of objective sleepiness [61] , was higher in the younger group during the lighted motorway condition. However, this investigation showed increased theta activity during driving in older adults, that may reflect a local sleep phenomenon [62, 63] . Indeed, in previous studies on young samples, both alpha and theta power show specific patterns during the wake-sleep transition [61] . The theta activity exhibits a fronto-central maximum peak before and after sleep onset. At the same time, the alpha power shows occipital maximum peak before sleep onset and fronto-central maximum expression during post-sleep onset (e.g., [64, 65] ). The increase of theta power along with driving errors among elderly people would indicate that they made mistakes at the wheel only in severe drowsiness conditions able to provoke sleep attacks. Alternatively, since young adults showed driving errors already with lower sleepiness levels (i.e., related to alpha activity), the result could support-once again-the idea of a greater impact of low vigilance in younger as compared to older adults [32] . In this vein, Filtness et al. [38] demonstrated that, albeit both age groups had more driving accidents after partial sleep deprivation, younger participants were more impaired by this condition characterized by severe sleepiness. Indeed, after sleep restriction, the young group exhibited more sleepiness-related accidents during early morning driving, associated with the presence of alpha and theta power [38] . Overall, most of these findings indicate that older drivers are less vulnerable to sleep loss and sleepiness-related driving impairments than young adults [32, 33, 38, 40, 66] . These discrepancies in sleepiness vulnerability between age groups may be due to differences in subjects' lifestyles. For instance, young participants could have poor sleep habits and a sort of "chronic sleep deprivation", as hypothesized by previous literature [67, 68] , and observed in studies on the consequences of COVID-19 on sleep-wake rhythm [69] [70] [71] . It is worth noting that this conclusion should be taken with caution since some studies revealed that older adults might underestimate their level of sleepiness in the assessments by self-report instruments [72, 73] , and the time of the day in which the studies have been performed could have affected the results [38, 41] . The literature specifically aimed to better understand the relationship between sleepiness, aging, and driving behavior is sparse and presents several limitations. Firstly, investigations on a large sample are scarce [34] [35] [36] [37] , and none of these have studied sleepiness and driving abilities providing a comparison between young and older adults. The existing longitudinal results highlighted that sleep complaints (i.e., insomnia, EDS) might influence the possibility to keep driving. Nevertheless, the main limitation of these studies was the adoption of protocols using exclusively self-reported instruments to evaluate both driving performance and sleepiness [34] [35] [36] [37] . In this respect, we believe that prospective protocols may help shed light on the longitudinal effects of sleep quality and sleepiness on driving during aging by integrating objective methods to study drive abilities (i.e., driving simulator). Moreover, the longitudinal repeated measures would allow researchers to understand whether the observed changes of driving behavior in the geriatric population are ascribed to sleep loss/sleep complaints (i.e., state-like factors) or due to stable individual differences (i.e., trait-like factors). Another issue to consider in the current literature is the composition of the samples. For example, some studies involved mainly males [32, [34] [35] [36] 38] , making the results not representative of the driving population. In fact, many studies point to the fact that males are more willing to take risks than females in terms of risk behavior in road traffic than females [74, 75] . Moreover, gender differences during aging should be assessed since females tend to have more sleep disturbances such as insomnia in the post-menopausal period [76, 77] . In addition, the age ranges considered in the reviewed studies were not homogeneous. Some studies included in the same age group both participants with 55 and ≥65 years considering all "older adults" [38] [39] [40] [41] . Future studies should take into account that the elderly population is defined a people aged 65 and over [78] . Moreover, distinct age ranges should be assessed to detect slight differences during the geriatric age: (a) youngest-old ranging 65-74 years; (b) middle-old ranging 75-84 years and (c) oldest-old ≥85 years [79] . It is worth noting that the older participants selected in the reviewed studies were mainly considered "healthy" [32, 33, 37, 38, 40, 41] to guarantee a certain homogeneity between subjects. However, comorbidities [34] [35] [36] 39, 48] and drugs usage [48] are frequent during aging. Indeed, medical disorders can affect sleepiness level and, consequently driving abilities [48] . The Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration (FMCSA) has reported a comprehensive review of the risk of car incidents for a variety of medical disorders [80] [81] [82] [83] [84] . The time course, severity state and treatment of the disease modulate the impact on daily performances [48] . Diabetes, chronic insomnia, restless leg syndrome, obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), and dementia with related medications can all contribute to sleep deprivation and/or EDS [85] . Several pharmacotherapies could alter vigilance, e.g., anticonvulsants, antihistamines, antidepressants, antipsychotics and narcotics, dopaminergic therapy, and benzodiazepines [48] . In particular, OSA has been recognized as a significant risk factor for motor vehicle crashes and affects approximately 10-20% of adults [48, 83] . Nevertheless, it is frequently undiagnosed in the general population [86] and very few studies were directly aimed to investigate the relationship between OSA, aging, and driving abilities (e.g., [87] ). Therefore, future investigations should consider this issue providing further evidence about risk and protective factors affecting driving performances in the context of medical disorders among elderly people. The further methodological question concerns the identification of instruments to evaluate sleepiness. Although the EEG studies provide a reliable measure of sleepiness assessing specific cortical oscillations (e.g., alpha and theta power) [32, 33, 38] , EEG does not represent an easy and quick tool to use. It should be mentioned that in Europe, the sleepiness assessment recently became mandatory for high-risk subjects who apply to renew their driving license. The European Union (EU) directive on driving licenses was made subject to mandatory implementation by all member state from 31 December 2015 [88] . Specifically, this directive was based on the recommendations from a working group established by the Transport and Mobility Directorate of the European Commission in 2012 [89] . Accordingly, the Italian Government has issued a decree stating the obligation to assess sleepiness in people suffering from narcolepsy, sleep apnea, and diseases EDS-related (i.e., Decreto Legge del Ministero delle Infrastrutture e dei Trasporti del 22 dicembre 2015). This situation required an easy protocol to objectively evaluate the vigilance level both in elderly and young adults. For instance, an attempt in this direction was provided by the European Obstructive Sleep Apnea Screening (EUROSAS) questionnaire for drivers, developed by the European Union Driver License Committee as a screening tool for OSA [89, 90] . In this vein, the assessment of sleepiness through a short and easy-to-administer behavioral task, such as PVT lasting few minutes [43, 91] , should be considered. Further, the behavioral task, administered at different times of the day, should be associated with a driving simulation test. However, very little data are available on the relationship between PVT, driving, and aging [39, 41] since the issue deserves further investigation. The authors declare no conflict of interest. The relationship between the extent of visual field and driving performance-A review Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers The effect of age on driving skills Sleepiness and the risk of road traffic accidents: A systematic review and meta-analysis of previous studies Driving simulators in the clinical assessment of fitness to drive in sleepy individuals: A systematic review Temporal placement of a nap for alertness: Contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness Sleepiness: Evaluating and quantifying methods Sleep apnea, sleep debt and daytime sleepiness are independently associated with road accidents. A cross-sectional study on truck drivers Summary (No. WHO/NMH/NVI/18.20); World Health Organization Sleep related vehicle accidents Sleepiness at the wheel across Europe: A survey of 19 countries Global Status Report on Road Safety 2013: Supporting a Decade of Action: Summary (No. WHO. NMH. VIP 13.01). 2013. Available online Sleep restriction, sleep hygiene, and driving safety: The importance of situational sleepiness Temporal patterns of reported single-vehicle car and truck accidents in Texas, USA during 1980-1983 Sleep-related automobile accidents-When and who? Characteristics of crashes attributed to the driver having fallen asleep Age, gender and early morning highway accidents Older Population Fact Sheet (Report No. DOT HS 812 500); National Highway Traffic Safety Adminstration Older driver involvements in police reported crashes and fatal crashes: Trends and projections Aggiornamento del Comunicato Diffuso in Data 25 Luglio Are older drivers actually at higher risk of involvement in collisions resulting in deaths or non-fatal injuries among their passengers and other road users? Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crashes among older adults Cognitive test performance and crash risk in an older driver population Development of a test battery to identify older drivers at risk for self-reported adverse driving events Sleep and human aging Analysis of Quantitative Sleep Parameters From Childhood to Old Age in Healthy Individuals: Developing Normative Sleep Values Across the Human Lifespan. Sleep An Investigation of the Problems and Opinions of Aged Drivers Vision Testing of Renewal Applicants: Crashes Predicted When Compensation for Impairment is Inadequate Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement Correlation between driving errors and vigilance level: Influence of the driver's age Wakefulness in young and elderly subjects driving at night in a car simulator Prevalence of sleep disturbances in a cohort of older drivers Sleep disturbances and adverse driving events in a predominantly male cohort of active older drivers Sleep disturbances and driving practices of older drivers Sleepiness, near-misses and driving accidents among a representative population of French drivers Driver sleepiness-Comparisons between young and older men during a monotonous afternoon simulated drive On-the-road driving performance and drivingrelated skills in older untreated insomnia patients and chronic users of hypnotics Fatigue in Younger and Older Drivers: Effectiveness of an Alertness-Maintaining Task The influence of sleep quality, vigilance, and sleepiness on driving-related cognitive abilities: A comparison between young and older adults A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual Driving performance under the influence of drugs: Rationale for, and application of, a new test Changes in self-regulatory driving among older drivers over time Driving life expectancy of persons aged 70 years and older in the United States Impaired driving from medical conditions: A 70-year-old man trying to decide if he should continue driving Co-morbidity, mortality, quality of life and the healthcare/welfare/social costs of disordered sleep: A rapid review The association between the number of prescription medications and incident falls in a multi-ethnic population of adult type-2 diabetes patients: The diabetes and aging study Clinical practice guidelines and quality of care for older patients with multiple comorbid diseases Use of prescription and over-the-counter medications and dietary supplements among older adults in the United States Happy as a lark: Morning-type younger and older adults are higher in positive affect Longitudinal change of sleep timing: Association between chronotype and longevity in older adults Circadian rythms and memory in Aged Humans and Animals Age and time-of-day effects on learning and memory in a non-matching-to-sample test Which young people accept a lift from a drunk or drugged driver? Alcohol-impaired driving behavior and sensationseeking disposition in a college population receiving routine care at campus health services centers Alertness pattern in healthy individuals of various ages Age related in drivers' crash risk and crash type The electroencephalographic features of the sleep onset process and their experimental manipulation with sleep deprivation and transcranial electrical stimulation protocols Going local: Insights from EEG and stereo-EEG studies of the human sleep-wake cycle Sleepiness as a local phenomenon Topographical characteristics of slow wave activities during the transition from wakefulness to sleep How we fall asleep: Regional and temporal differences in electroencephalographic synchronization at sleep onset Suppression of sleepiness in drivers: Combination of caffeine with a short nap Sleepiness, driving, and motor vehicle crashes. Council on Scientific Affairs Causes and consequences of sleepiness among college students Changes in sleep pattern, sense of time and digital media use during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy Poor sleep quality and its consequences on mental health during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy COVID-19 lockdown and poor sleep quality: Not the whole story Arnulf, I. Underperception of naps in older adults referred for a sleep assessment: An insomnia trait and a cognitive problem? Limits of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale in older adults What causes the differences in driving between young men and women? The effects of gender roles and sex on young drivers' driving behaviour and self-assessment of skills Gender effects in young road users on road safety attitudes, behaviors and risk perception Sleep complaints among elderly persons: An epidemiologic study of three communities The problems of sleep for older women: Changes in health outcomes OECD. Elderly Population (Indicator). 2021. Available online Invecchiamento Attivo e Condizione di Vita Degli Anziani in Italia Executive Summary: Diabetes and Commercial Motor Vehicle Driver Safety. Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration Executive Summary: Cardiovascular Disease and Commercial Motor Vehicle Driver Safety. Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration Executive Summary: Seizure Disorders and Commercial Motor Vehicle Driver Safety. Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration Executive Summary: Obstructive Sleep Apnea and Commercial Motor Vehicle Driver Safety. Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration. 12 Executive Summary: Traumatic Brain Injury and Commercial Motor Vehicle Driver Safety. Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration Medical factors of falling asleep behind the wheel Management of obstructive sleep apnea in commercial motor vehicle operators: Recommendations of the AASM sleep and transportation safety awareness task force Moderate sleep restriction in treated older male OSA participants: Greater impairment during monotonous driving compared with controls Amending Directive 2006/126/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council on Driving Licences New Standards and Guidelines for Drivers with Obstructive Sleep Apnoea Syndrome: Report of the Obstructive Sleep Apnoea Working Group Reliability of the Turkish version of the European Obstructive Sleep Apnea Screening (EUROSAS) questionnaire for drivers 0: An updated platform for psychomotor vigilance task testing, analysis, prediction, and visualization