key: cord-0948114-80ilwj60 authors: Solak, Çağlar; Peker-Dural, Hilal; Karlidağ, Sercan; Peker, Mehmet title: Linking the behavioral immune system to COVID-19 vaccination intention: The mediating role of the need for cognitive closure and vaccine hesitancy date: 2021-09-08 journal: Pers Individ Dif DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111245 sha: 696db9d187bec32c32440760cef76d526509b8c0 doc_id: 948114 cord_uid: 80ilwj60 Vaccination has become one of the most effective ways of controlling the spread of COVID-19. Consequently, revealing the evolutionary and cognitive antecedents of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and vaccination intention has become crucial. Drawing on the theory of behavioral immune system (BIS), we investigate whether perceived vulnerability to disease (PVD) is associated with vaccination intentions through the need for cognitive closure (NCC) and vaccine hesitancy. The data was collected from 525 adults from Turkey. The structural equation modeling results indicate that of the two dimensions of PVD, germ aversion predicts COVID-19 vaccination intention through sequential mediation of NCC and vaccine hesitancy. Perceived infectability, on the other hand, is directly and positively related to vaccination intention. By showing the mediating role of NCC, our results offer an insight as to why germ aversion translates into vaccine hesitancy, and low vaccination intention. We discuss the potential benefits of considering the roles of BIS and NCC in campaigns and policies targeted at increasing COVID-19 vaccine uptake and suggest implications for such practices. Since December 2019, the world has been challenged by a global health crisis, a new coronavirus pandemic , which brought many problems that people and governments had to manage. One of them pertains to the uncertainty and hesitancy that surround the vaccination decisions. Shortly after the initial ideas of vaccination to fight COVID-19 had been voiced, public debates have been sparked worldwide on whether to get vaccinated or not. Consequently, vaccine hesitancy has become a prominent issue that poses a threat to public health (Freeman et al., 2020; Salali & Uysal, 2020 ). Behavioral immune system (BIS) has been theorized as an antecedent of positive health-related behaviors (Shook et al., 2020) , including vaccine attitudes and vaccination intention (Karlsson et al., 2021b) . However, a number of studies have documented a negative relationship between BIS and vaccine-related outcomes (Clay, 2017; Luz et al., 2019; Reuben et al., 2020) . This study aimed to address this finding by examining need for cognitive closure (NCC) as an intermediary variable in the relationship between BIS and COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. In confirming the proposed relationships, this study extends past research on the BIS and vaccination by uncovering a process through which a heightened BIS would translate into hesitancy about vaccination, and consequently reluctance to get vaccinated. Throughout the evolution of humans, parasites and infectious diseases have been serious threats to survival and reproduction, which caused the physiological defense mechanisms to evolve. However, fighting against pathogens with such mechanisms may be metabolically costly. Thus, germ/contamination aversion) and differences in perception of oneself's vulnerability to diseases are commonly known facets of BIS. The protective functions of BIS have received support in past research investigating the relationship of BIS with health-related behaviors. Individuals who feel vulnerable to infections are more likely to overestimate the pathogen risks (Miller & Maner, 2012) , and engage more in preventative health behaviors (Shook et al., 2020) . Related to the COVID-19, germ aversion increases by reading information about the COVID-19 pandemic (Bacon & Corr, 2020) , and pathogen avoidance predicts anxiety and protective practices such as social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic (Makhanova & Shepherd, 2020) . However, even though the recent evidence indicates a positive association between BIS and vaccination intention (Karlsson et al., 2021b) , a considerable number of studies found a somewhat surprising negative relationship between BIS and favorable attitudes towards vaccination. For example, people with high disgust sensitivity were more skeptical about the efficacy and safety of vaccines (Clifford & Wendell, 2016) . Similarly, contamination aversion was positively correlated with negative attitudes toward vaccines (Clay, 2017; Luz et al., 2019) , and vaccine-hesitant individuals had higher disgust sensitivity (Reuben et al., 2020) . This surprising negative relationship between BIS and vaccination attitudes has been explained by the possibility that BIS treats vaccines as contamination cues. It was argued that a mechanism evolved to detect the probable sources of pathogens and contamination which would otherwise tax the immune system is likely to operate against vaccines because they contain substances that elicit an immune response (Clay, 2017) . present our reasoning by which we formed expectations about the mediating role of NCC in the relationship between BIS and vaccination intention as follows. NCC refers to "the desire for a firm answer to a question and an aversion towards ambiguity" (Kruglanski & Webster, 1996, p. 264) . It is involved in decision-making such that it alludes to the motivational tendency towards getting a definite answer to reduce uncertainty (Raglan et al., 2014) . High NCC has been associated with ambiguity intolerance, rapid decision making (Kruglanski, 2004) , limited information searching (Choi et al., 2008) , and reduced preference for new options (Mannetti et al., 2007) . People with high NCC do not favor uncertain situations and situations that require a decision as opposed to those in which people have freedom to abstain from deciding (Kruglanski, 2004) . Consequently, people with high NCC may make hasty decisions based on insufficient evidence to alleviate the unease incited by uncertainties and decision-making (Webster & Kruglanski, 1994) . Such a motivational disposition can work hand in hand with an active BIS and underlie the link between the BIS and vaccine hesitancy. Heightened BIS makes individuals sensitive to the disease and contamination cues (e.g., COVID-19 vaccines) and directs cognitive and behavioral tendencies to eliminate sources of such cues. NCC would complement this relationship with a decision-making process based on simplistic information processing (for a review see Kruglanski, 2004) , at the expense of thorough and careful consideration about COVID-19 vaccines. Supporting the association between NCC and vaccine hesitancy, Cole et al. (2015) revealed that individuals with high NCC are more resistant to getting vaccinated than their low NCC counterparts. This line of argument is further solicited by how people with high dispositional NCC react to risky situations. Schumpe et al. (2017) showed that when confronted with risky choices, high NCC people tend to make less risky decisions. Thus, the heightened BIS would be associated with a decisional tendency (i.e., NCC) towards reducing the uncertainty and avoiding a salient risky circumstance, which in turn translates into hesitancy about COVID-19 vaccines, and consequently leads to lower intentions about getting vaccinated. Journal Pre-proof Our arguments thus far outline a process from BIS to vaccination intention through NCC and vaccine hesitancy. BIS generates various affective and cognitive mechanisms that guide behaviors when contextual cues point out potential pathogens (Schaller & Park, 2011) . Infectious diseases create contexts characterized by risk perceptions and uncertainty that drive people to seek a definite answer and safer behavioral patterns to manage their behaviors (Kruglanski, 2004) . Thus, we expect that (1) BIS is positively associated with NCC, (2) that NCC is positively associated with vaccine hesitancy, (3) and that vaccine hesitancy is negatively associated with intention to get vaccinated. Taken together, we hypothesize that BIS is associated with COVID-19 vaccination intention through sequential mediation of NCC and vaccine hesitancy. The sample consisted of 525 people from Turkey who volunteered to participate in an online survey. Their age ranged between 18 and 74 (M = 32.18, SD = 10.22). Other demographic information is presented in Table 1 . Participants were informed about the purpose and method of the research via an informed consent form. The data was collected between January 5 and 24, 2021. In this J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Journal Pre-proof period, the first COVID-19 vaccine was administered in Turkey (January 14, 2021) and vaccination was not available for the public except for health care workers and pharmacists. In addition to the measurements of the demographic information, we used four scales. Unless otherwise stated, all scale items were rated on a 5-point scale (1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree). . All the subscales were combined into a single score (α = .86). Turkish version (Sevi, 2017) of the 14-item PVD scale (Duncan et al., 2009) , which consisted of a 7item Perceived Infectability (PI) subscale (α = .81), and a 7-item Germ Aversion (GA) subscale (α = .72). One item of GA ("I avoid using public telephones because of the risk that I may catch something from the previous user.") was discarded because of the low frequency of usage of public telephones in Turkey (Sevi, 2017) . Participant Demographics vaccine intention in our data, and the analyses with and without them were identical. Thus, we omitted them from further analyses. Past flu vaccine experience was related to COVID-19 vaccine intention; thus, it was retained. Journal Pre-proof Table 2 shows means, standard deviations, correlations among variables, and reliability estimates of the scales. We used structural equation modeling to test our hypotheses and created three parcels for PVD dimensions and four parcels for NCC scale by considering their item-toconstruct relationships (Little et al., 2002) . We used the items as indicators of COVID-19 vaccine hesitation since it was measured with three items. COVID-19 vaccine intention was measured with a single item. We set its reliability to .85 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1982) and followed the procedure for the single-item constructs recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988) by constraining the factor loading to the square root of reliability score and error variance to one minus reliability multiplied by the observed item variance. Note. N = 525. Reliability estimates were reported in parentheses on the diagonal. a 0 = male, 1 = female; b 0 = No past vaccination experience, 1 = past vaccination experience. *p < .05, ** p < .01. We first tested the measurement model with Mplus 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 which provided a good fit to the data. The details are provided in the online supplementary material. Next, we tested the hypothesized structural model. The results showed that GA (β = .48, p < .001) significantly predicted NCC while PI did not (β = .04, p = .49). NCC significantly predicted vaccine hesitancy (β = .22, p < .001) and vaccine hesitancy significantly predicted vaccine intention, (β = -.63, p = .001). The model explained 42% variance in vaccine intention, 5% variance in vaccine hesitancy, and 24% variance in NCC. We investigated the mediated relationships for GA using the bootstrap analysis. Specifically, we estimated bias-corrected confidence intervals of three indirect effects using 5,000 bootstrapped resamples. The indirect relationship between GA and vaccine hesitancy through NCC was significant, confirmed our predictions for GA. PI, on the other hand, was directly related to COVID-19 vaccination intentions. The results showed that GA had a strong relationship with NCC. The risk of disease resulting from pathogens has always created uncertainty in the ancestral environment as it does today because people cannot know for sure who is sick in a group and how diseases could be transmitted. For our minds, which evolved in this social niche, seeking for certainty and dislike of ambiguity seems to be adaptive (for a review, see Rosen et al., 2014) . The finding that people with high GA were also inclined to be high in NCC revealed that the mechanism that drives cognitive and behavioral tendencies towards detecting and avoiding pathogens may operate in conjunction with a cognitive structure characterized by a desire for closure and avoidance of risks. This result is consistent with the past research showing both BIS and NCC have a significant relation to structures such as risk tolerance (Prokosch et al., 2019; Schumpe et al., 2017) , and being more open to new experiences (Onraet et al., 2011; Kupfer & Tybur, 2017) . Our model indicated that GA was related to the vaccine hesitancy through the mediation of NCC. These findings demonstrate that cognitive processes involved in decision-making may have a role in why aversion of germs drives feelings of hesitancy towards vaccines. It seems that once heightened, GA is inclined to take action against any cue that it deems dangerous, including COVID-19 vaccines. This initial inclination drives the hesitancy about vaccines through a simplistic decisionmaking process, motivated towards a prompt closure (Kruglanski, 2004) , and avoiding risks (e.g., Schumpe et al., 2017) . PI, on the other hand, was not significantly associated with NCC and vaccine hesitancy. Despite this unexpected result, previous research (e.g., Bacon & Corr, 2020; Clay, 2017; Luz et al., 2019) reported the distinct relationships of the two facets of BIS with the tested constructs, as was the case in our model. However, PI was directly and positively associated with participants' intention of vaccination. Those who perceive themselves at more risk of contamination and believe that the effects of the disease will be more severe for them may see the vaccine as a more reliable method of J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Journal Pre-proof protection. In line with this finding, it was found that perceived vulnerability to COVID-19 is one of the predictors of intention to get vaccinated (Ruiz & Bell, 2021) , higher PI is related to accepting the influenza vaccine (Luz et al., 2019) , and negatively correlated with anti-vaccination attitudes (Kempthorne & Terrizzi, 2021) . In this regard, the extent of people's confidence in their immunity seems to be related to their risk perception for COVID-19, and consequently considerations about vaccination. Even though the tested relationships were significant, the explained variances in vaccine hesitancy (5%) and vaccination intention (24%) were low to moderate. (Brizi & Biraglia, 2021) . Thus, messages that reduce risk and uncertainty perceptions about the vaccines and policies enacted to fight misinformation about vaccines are particularly important and potentially beneficial to overcome COVID-19 vaccine hesitation and increase vaccination rates. Lastly, PI was positively and directly related to the vaccination intention, which indicates that the preventive health campaigns would benefit from highlighting the effectiveness of vaccines and the severity of COVID-19 disease. The findings and contributions should be interpreted by taking the limitations of the study into account. The cross-sectional design and single-source data of the study bring about two issues that should be noted. First, causal interpretations cannot be made based on current data. Even though the causal ordering of the study variables was supported by theoretical reasoning, we cannot rule out the alternative bidirectional or reverse relationships. Second, single-source and singlemethod data is prone to common method bias. Although the measurement model tests indicated that the study variables are distinct constructs, care should be taken in interpreting the results. Future research with longitudinal designs will be helpful to confirm our results and extend them by establishing causality of the relationships. The online data collection method is another limitation of the current study. Such a method may limit the generalizability of the findings as the recruited participants may not represent the population well and the data is susceptible to skewed participant demographics. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and attitudes toward lockdown How need for cognitive closure and gender impact stockpiling and food waste during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-national study in India and the United States of America Need for cognitive closure and information search strategy The behavioral immune system and attitudes about vaccines: Contamination aversion predicts more negative vaccine attitudes How disgust influences health purity attitudes Understanding African American college students' H1N1 vaccination decisions Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance: Correlates in a nationally representative longitudinal survey of the Australian population Beliefs in conspiracy theories and the need for cognitive closure To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits Assessing vaccine hesitancy in the UK population using a generalized vaccine hesitancy survey instrument Disgust as an emotional driver of vaccine attitudes and uptake? A mediation analysis Behavioral immune system linked to responses to the threat of COVID-19 Determinants of COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in the US. EClinicalMedicine, 26, 100495 Who regrets more after choosing a non-status-quo option? Post decisional regret under need for cognitive closure Overperceiving disease cues: The basic cognition of the behavioral immune system Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Journal Pre-proof and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom The behavioral immune system: Implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence Mplus user's guide. Eighth Edition The closed mind: 'Experience' and 'cognition' aspects of openness to experience and need for closure as psychological bases for right-wing attitudes Caution in the time of cholera: Pathogen threats decrease risk tolerance Need to know: The need for cognitive closure impacts the clinical practice of obstetrician/gynecologists Mistrust of the medical profession and higher disgust sensitivity predict parental vaccine hesitancy Item selection and validation of a brief, 15-item version of the Need for Closure Scale Differentiating intolerance of uncertainty from three related but distinct constructs Predictors of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Results of a nationwide survey COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy is associated with beliefs on the origin of the novel coronavirus in the UK and Turkey The behavioral immune system (and why it matters) Need for cognitive closure decreases risk taking and motivates discounting of delayed rewards Effects of behavioral immune system on risk taking: The case of framing effect The vaccine hesitancy scale: Psychometric properties and validation Disease avoidance in the time of COVID-19: The behavioral immune system is associated with concern and preventative