key: cord-0940067-g3l78npb authors: David, Jean-Charles; Nadarajah, Kévin; Niang, Anta; Delouvée, Sylvain; Goyette, Martin; Bordel, Stéphanie; Somat, Alain title: Step In, Step Out from the First Lockdown: An Exploration of COVID-19 Perceptions in France and Quebec date: 2021-11-22 journal: Infect Dis Rep DOI: 10.3390/idr13040089 sha: 1f5e9f8dc1bb88448a46d9914f70274f8a3ecf1f doc_id: 940067 cord_uid: g3l78npb Objective. The objective of this research was to describe and analyze the role of psychological and behavioral factors on perceptions of COVID-19 in France and Quebec at three different times during the pandemic. Design. We conducted three qualitative and quantitative studies (Study 1 N = 255, Study 2 N = 230, Study 3 N = 143). Participants were asked to evaluate psychological and behavioral measures: at the beginning of lockdown (Study 1), during lockdown (Study 2), and during lockdown exit (Study 3). Results. Results of Study 1 show that perceptions of COVID-19 are organized around fear and a sense of threat. During the lockdown, participants mentioned for the first time the health practices to prevent the spread of COVID-19 (Study 2). Psychological and social impacts constitute a central theme in participants’ discourse (Study 2 and 3). Conclusions. The results show that perceptions of risk during a pandemic are socially constructed. Perceptions seem to be influenced by the political and health management of a territory and by the evolution of behavioral and psychological responses. In December 2019, coronavirus was first identified in Wuhan, China. Since then, many governments have implemented health measures that have proven to be effective in reducing the spread of the virus [1] . Despite the deployment of these measures, populations are facing health [2] , economic [3] , social [4] , cultural [5] , environmental [6] , and political [7] consequences. In this context, many studies have been conducted in a wide range of fields to better understand the impacts of the pandemic [8] . They have also helped to provide government authorities with appropriate social and macroeconomic policy responses [9] . In the social sciences particularly, Van Bavel et al. [10] have even called on the scientific community to pursue several research topics related to COVID-19 (e.g., social and cultural context). Thus, several studies have focused on the psychological impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic [11] , lockdown [12] , and social distancing [13] . These studies have already provided knowledge to help populations adopt the recommendations of epidemiologists and public health experts [14] . First, epidemics have marked human history and have caused panic and anxiety among populations [15] . Some studies have shown that the adoption of health practices to control infection was predicted by individuals' perception of epidemic risk and their level of anxiety [16] . More generally, epidemics provoke strong emotional reactions such as fear. As this was the case for the severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) epidemic. The results of a study conducted by Chang [17] strongly suggest that fear of SARS significantly influenced people's behavior in seeking medical care and that this fear seriously affected access to quality care. Negative emotions and cognitive evaluations are in fact the result of the defensive mechanisms developed by individuals to cope with the threat they perceive [18] . These defense mechanisms can thus be studied and understood using perceptions [19] . They would also be a key to understanding how to properly manage future epidemics [20] . Second, as Slovic argues, "Danger is real, but risk is socially constructed" (p. 689, [21] ). Indeed, for many authors, and in particular for the human and social sciences, risk is socially constructed and depends on the perceptions of actors, i.e., the meanings and values they bring into play when they perceive situations [22, 23] . Research on collective risks (related to ecosystem, health, natural, technological and food risks) and the crises they provoke shows that there is a common and socially shared knowledge on the subject, which, according to Gilbert [24] , is linked to the fact that these risks can threaten the integrity and interests of the populations concerned. Furthermore, Rudnick [25] argues that the international dynamics associated with the pandemic are important because societies are measured by how they learn from these collective crises. Thus, in order to understand these collective risks and the impact of prevention measures on them, more and more researchers are looking at their social perceptions. Finally, because 'risk is intrinsically embedded in time' (p. 530, [26] ), it seems essential to put these different research topics into perspective with the pandemic's temporal dimension. This temporal dynamic can be shaped by governmental decisions or by evolution in media discourse. These can shape the relationship of individuals to a virus, as was the case with SARS in 2003 [27] and more recently with COVID-19 [28] . Political actions can also generate excessive behavior, or blind compliance, as well as variations in emotional levels and sensitivity to social risks [29] . Time dynamics related to measures of physical and social distancing can also highlight socially supportive behaviors as a lever of resilience, which is one of the possible long-term effects of the epidemic [30] , with respect to perceived loneliness [31] . The perception of health risks has therefore been widely studied in recent decades, notably during the COVID-19 pandemic [32] . However, to our knowledge, few studies have focused on how psychosociological, cultural, and temporal factors structure the way populations make sense of emerging health threats. The objective of this research was to describe and analyze the role of psychological and behavioral factors on perceptions of COVID-19 in France and Quebec at three distinct times during the pandemic-i.e., at the beginning of lockdown (Study 1), at 1 month of lockdown (Study 2), and at the lockdown exit (≥1 month) (Study 3)-in France and Quebec, and the individuals' responses towards COVID-19 ( Figure 1 show the survey timeline). In an exploratory perspective, we chose to compare two territories with different political systems, a Western European country, France, and a North American country, Canada. France is a unitary republic whereas Canada is a monarchy with a federated parliamentary system. This implies that the health measures for COVID-19 in France are the same for the whole country, whereas the responses to the epidemic are made by each province in Canada. In order to control the political and health decisions made across Canada on the perception of the virus, we chose to focus on one province, Quebec, where the implementation of the lockdown was relatively similar to France (e.g., the start of the lockdown). The choice of these two territories also allows us to bring a comparative dimension, as they share a common history [33] and have the same official language, French. Despite these similarities, cross-cultural differences remain and can have an impact on the perception of the epidemic [34] . Indeed, Guan et al. [35] highlight the influence of national culture (i.e., the shared meanings and practices within a nation) on collective actions and norms (macroeconomic, political, respect for barrier gestures) during the COVID-19 pandemic. We were thus able to compare two territories with common characteristics, and having put in place sanitary measures (e.g., barrier gestures; Lockdown) at relatively similar periods but culturally and politically distant, which may influence the perception of the pandemic. Participants aged 18 or older were recruited to participate in the study at the start of the lockdown (from 18 to 31 March). The lockdown and therefore mobility restrictions did not allow us to use traditional means to recruit participants. We have therefore carried out a survey on Limesurvey ® . We chose to disseminate the survey link on open access group pages of the 3 largest social media platforms (Linkedin ® ; Facebook ® and Twitter ® ). This allowed us to reach people where they were, as these social media sites saw an increase in usage during the lockdown [36] . To prevent individuals from completing the questionnaire multiple times, only one questionnaire could be submitted from a particular IP address. In an exploratory perspective, we chose to compare two territories with different political systems, a Western European country, France, and a North American country, Canada. France is a unitary republic whereas Canada is a monarchy with a federated parliamentary system. This implies that the health measures for COVID-19 in France are the same for the whole country, whereas the responses to the epidemic are made by each province in Canada. In order to control the political and health decisions made across Canada on the perception of the virus, we chose to focus on one province, Quebec, where the implementation of the lockdown was relatively similar to France (e.g., the start of the lockdown). The choice of these two territories also allows us to bring a comparative dimension, as they share a common history [33] and have the same official language, French. Despite these similarities, cross-cultural differences remain and can have an impact on the perception of the epidemic [34] . Indeed, Guan et al. [35] highlight the influence of national culture (i.e., the shared meanings and practices within a nation) on collective actions and norms (macroeconomic, political, respect for barrier gestures) during the COVID-19 pandemic. We were thus able to compare two territories with common characteristics, and having put in place sanitary measures (e.g., barrier gestures; Lockdown) at relatively similar periods but culturally and politically distant, which may influence the perception of the pandemic. Participants aged 18 or older were recruited to participate in the study at the start of the lockdown (from 18 to 31 March). The lockdown and therefore mobility restrictions did not allow us to use traditional means to recruit participants. We have therefore carried out a survey on Limesurvey ® . We chose to disseminate the survey link on open access group pages of the 3 largest social media platforms (Linkedin ® ; Facebook ® and Twitter ® ). This allowed us to reach people where they were, as these social media sites saw an increase in usage during the lockdown [36] . To prevent individuals from completing the questionnaire multiple times, only one questionnaire could be submitted from a particular IP address. In total, 486 people opened the survey links, and 255 completed it (a response rate of 52.4%). Our sample includes 203 women, aged from 18 to 77 years (M = 37, SD = 12.8). A total of 146 resided in France and 109 in Quebec (Table 1) . Perceptions of the virus. These perceptions were measured by a free association task [37] . This method is based on free association and consists of a prime word (here "coronavirus") to which participants have to associate spontaneously four words, phrases or feelings. Health practices. Each participant evaluated "barrier gestures" that were advocated by WHO such as regular hand washing or monitoring for symptoms of infection, on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (absolutely). An average compliance score was produced for all participants (α= 0.59). Personal involvement. We used the three components proposed by Ernst-Vintila et al. [38] . Participants completed a scale adapted from previous work [39] to assess personal involvement in the Coronavirus. Exploratory factor analysis with Oblimin rotation revealed a three-factor structure yielding a satisfactory fit. The first factor referred to "personal identification" (e.g., "I feel particularly concerned by Coronavirus"). The second factor referred to the valuation of the object (e.g., "I think that coronavirus issues are important in our society"). The third factor referred to the perceived capacity for action (e.g., "I feel that through my thoughts and knowledge of coronavirus issues, I am in a position to really act."). Maximum involvement occurs when a person feels concerned about the virus (ID+), when issues related to the virus are considered important (VAL+), and when something can address it (PCA+). McDonald's omega coefficient was 0.80 indicating good test reliability. We conducted a correspondence factor analysis on the participants' evocations with a frequency greater than or equal to 6 (see [40] ). We studied the relationship of these evocations with several variables: the location of residence (Quebec, France), the individual's identification with the object, valuation of the object, perceived capacity to act and health practices. For each measure, "low" and "high" classes were defined with a median split. We also used univariate ANOVAs to examine whether there was a difference between the French and Quebecers in their identification, valuation, perceived capacity for action towards COVID-19, and their health practices. The CORR. F. A. highlights two factors that explain 75.68% of the table's inertia (Factor 1 = 44.96%; Factor 2 = 30.72%). Factor 1 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.26 + CF (Quebec) = 0.36, and "health practices": CF (low health practices) = 0.11, i.e., a contribution of 73%, to the formation of the factor. Factor 2 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.10 + CF (Quebec) = 0.14, and "personal involvement": CF (high identification) = 0.14, + CF (low identification) = 0.20, + CF (high valuation) = 0.13, + CF (low valuation) = 0.16, i.e., a contribution of 87%, to the formation of the factor. Figure 2 shows this configuration. ocations with several variables: the location of residence (Quebec, France), the individual's identification with the object, valuation of the object, perceived capacity to act and health practices. For each measure, "low" and "high" classes were defined with a median split. We also used univariate ANOVAs to examine whether there was a difference between the French and Quebecers in their identification, valuation, perceived capacity for action towards COVID-19, and their health practices. The CORR. F. A. highlights two factors that explain 75.68% of the table's inertia (Factor 1 = 44.96%; Factor 2 = 30.72%). Factor 1 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.26 + CF (Quebec) = 0.36, and "health practices": CF (low health practices) = 0.11, i.e., a contribution of 73%, to the formation of the factor. Factor 2 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.10 + CF (Quebec) = 0.14, and "personal involvement": CF (high identification) = 0.14, + CF (low identification) = 0.20, + CF (high valuation) = 0.13, + CF (low valuation) = 0.16, i.e., a contribution of 87%, to the formation of the factor. Figure 2 shows this configuration. Factor 1 shows that the perceptions of French participants differ from those of Quebecers. The French associated coronavirus with danger, anguish, or death. The health measures deployed in France during the first study are also described by the French as lockdown. For Quebecers participants, the coronavirus is more associated with mental health by the evocation of terms such anxiety. Factor 1 also shows that health practices such as hand washing or greeting without shaking hands determined participant's evocations. Participants who least adopted these practices used negative terms such as panic, isolation, and quarantine to describe the coronavirus. Finally, the term mutual aid is evoked Factor 1 shows that the perceptions of French participants differ from those of Quebecers. The French associated coronavirus with danger, anguish, or death. The health measures deployed in France during the first study are also described by the French as lockdown. For Quebecers participants, the coronavirus is more associated with mental health by the evocation of terms such anxiety. Factor 1 also shows that health practices such as hand washing or greeting without shaking hands determined participant's evocations. Participants who least adopted these practices used negative terms such as panic, isolation, and quarantine to describe the coronavirus. Finally, the term mutual aid is evoked by these same participants for the importance of social ties during the period of lockdown. Factor 2 shows another difference between territories. The French participants associated the coronavirus with fever, i.e., the infectious nature of the coronavirus. They also referred to the geographical origin of the coronavirus with the evocation of China. The economic and social consequences of lockdown were more important in the discourse of Quebecers. Factor 2 also shows a link between the participant's personal involvement and their evocations. For example, the participants least involved with the virus produced a particularly descriptive discourse with evocations such as disease and also associated coronavirus with flu. Results (Table 1) show that Quebecers identified more with the virus (F (1, 253) = 5.71, p = 0.018, η 2 = 0.022), placed more importance on COVID-19 issues (F (1, 253) = 6.64, p = 0.011, η 2 = 0.026), and perceived more capacity to act on the pandemic (F (1, 253) = 41, p < 0.001; η 2 = 0.139). No significant differences were observed between the two populations regarding their compliance to health practices (p = 0.992). We used the same procedure as in Study 1, except that the participants were recruited 1 month after the start of the first lockdown (from 20 April to 4 May). Participants were also exposed to the Study 1 measures, but they also had to answer an additional question regarding their attitudes towards lockdown measures. In total, 1131 people opened the survey links, and 230 completed it (a response rate of 20.3%). Our includes 172 women, aged from 18 to 83 years (M = 36.5, SD = 13.6). A total of 119 resided in France and 111 in the province of Quebec (Table 2 ). The items to measure perceptions of virus, health practices (α = 0.59), and personal involvement (ω = 0.78) were the same as in Study 1. Attitudes towards lockdown measures. Attitude is the "evaluation" of a subject or group with respect to an object, an action, another individual or group [41] . To measure participants' attitudes towards lockdown measures, they assessed their global attitude ("Overall, in relation to lockdown, I am . . . ") on a single-item scale from 1 (totally against) to 7 (totally in favor) adapted from the favorability item proposed by Fabrigar, Krosnick, and MacDougall [42] . As in Study 1, a correspondence factor analysis was used on the participants' evocations with a frequency greater than or equal to 5. The CORR. F. A. highlights two factors that explain 65.91% of the table's inertia (Factor 1 = 44.17%; Factor 2 = 24.74%). Factor 1 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.40 + CF (Quebec) = 0.40, i.e., a contribution of 80%, to the formation of the factor. Factor 2 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "health practices": CF (low health practices) = 0.10, "personal involvement": CF (high identification) = 0.10, + CF (low identification) = 13, + CF (high valuation) = 0.15, + CF (low valuation = 0.12, and "attitude toward lockdown measures": CF (low attitude toward lockdown measures) = 0.11, i.e., a contribution of 71%, to the formation of the factor. Figure 3 shows this configuration. with respect to an object, an action, another individual or group [41] . To measure participants' attitudes towards lockdown measures, they assessed their global attitude ("Overall, in relation to lockdown, I am …") on a single-item scale from 1 (totally against) to 7 (totally in favor) adapted from the favorability item proposed by Fabrigar, Krosnick, and MacDougall [42] . As in Study 1, a correspondence factor analysis was used on the participants' evocations with a frequency greater than or equal to 5. The CORR. F. A. highlights two factors that explain 65.91% of the table's inertia (Factor 1 = 44.17%; Factor 2 = 24.74%). Factor 1 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.40 + CF (Quebec) = 0.40, i.e., a contribution of 80%, to the formation of the factor. Factor 2 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "health practices": CF (low health practices) = 0.10, "personal involvement": CF (high identification) = 0.10, + CF (low identification) = 13, + CF (high valuation) = 0.15, + CF (low valuation = 0.12, and "attitude toward lockdown measures": CF (low attitude toward lockdown measures) = 0.11, i.e., a contribution of 71%, to the formation of the factor. Figure 3 shows this configuration. Factor 1 shows that French participants still had differences in their perceptions compared to Quebecers. French participants associated coronavirus with virus and lockdown. On the contrary, Quebecer participants were more specific in their evocations, us- Factor 1 shows that French participants still had differences in their perceptions compared to Quebecers. French participants associated coronavirus with virus and lockdown. On the contrary, Quebecer participants were more specific in their evocations, using terms such as social distancing and washing your hands. Moreover, they used words such as fear, concern, and anxiety. Factor 2 indicates that personal involvement, health practices, and attitudes towards lockdown measures emerged from the participants' discourse. For example, people who had low scores on these dimensions evoked ideas of constraint such as long term or stress and social separation such as seniors or family. By contrast, participants who had high scores in terms of personal involvement and health practice used terms such as epidemic or protecting yourself. Results show that Quebecers (compared to French) identified more with the virus (F (1, 228) We used the same procedure as in Study 1, but this time the participants were recruited at the lockdown exit (from 8 June to 20 July). Participants were also exposed to the same measures as in Study 1, except that they had to answer an additional question related to their attitudes towards lockdown exit measures. In total, 1416 people opened the survey links, and 143 completed it (a response rate of 10.1%). Our sample includes 108 women, aged from 19 to 76 years (M = 35.9, SD = 12.8). 71 resided in France and 72 in Quebec (Table 3) . The items used to measure perceptions of virus, health practices (α = 0.74), and personal involvement (ω = 0.86) were the same than in Study 1 and 2. Attitudes towards lockdown exit measures. The same method as in Study 2 was used to develop the attitude item. The only difference is the object of attitude assessed by the participants. Indeed, they had to assess their global attitude towards lockdown exit measures ("Overall, in relation to lockdown exit, I am . . . ") on a single-item scale from 1 (totally against) to 7 (totally in favor). We conducted a third correspondence factor analysis on the participants' evocations with a frequency greater than or equal to 5. The CORR. F. A. highlights two factors that explain 63.15% of the table's inertia (Factor 1 = 33.09%; Factor 2 = 30.06%). Factor 1 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.37 + CF (Quebec) = 0.49, i.e., a contribution of 86%, to the formation of the factor. Factor 2 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "personal involvement": CF (high identification) = 0.18, + CF (low identification) = 0.19, + CF (high valuation) = 0.18, + CF (low valuation) = 0.22, i.e., a contribution of 77%, to the formation of the factor. Figure 4 shows this configuration. We conducted a third correspondence factor analysis on the participants' evocations with a frequency greater than or equal to 5. The CORR. F. A. highlights two factors that explain 63.15% of the table's inertia (Factor 1 = 33.09%; Factor 2 = 30.06%). Factor 1 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "Territories": CF (France) = 0.37 + CF (Quebec) = 0.49, i.e., a contribution of 86%, to the formation of the factor. Factor 2 receives a contribution from the terms of the variable "personal involvement": CF (high identification) = 0.18, + CF (low identification) = 0.19, + CF (high valuation) = 0.18, + CF (low valuation) = 0.22, i.e., a contribution of 77%, to the formation of the factor. Figure 4 shows this configuration. Factor 1 shows the same pattern as in the previous study, in terms of differences between French and Quebecers evocations. As in Study 2, the French participants associated very descriptive terms with coronavirus, such as virus or epidemic. However, during this phase, these participants mentioned terms related to health practices such as barrier gesture and mask. While the discourse of Quebecers is still characterized by fear, Factor 1 shows the same pattern as in the previous study, in terms of differences between French and Quebecers evocations. As in Study 2, the French participants associated very descriptive terms with coronavirus, such as virus or epidemic. However, during this phase, these participants mentioned terms related to health practices such as barrier gesture and mask. While the discourse of Quebecers is still characterized by fear, loneliness, and isolation, these participants also mentioned ideas about resilience by referring to terms such as adaptation and change. Factor 2, on the other hand, reveals a contradiction between participants with high and low levels of involvement. Highly involved participants reported the importance of barrier gestures with evocations such as danger and prevention. They also discussed the long-term consequences of the virus with the term crisis. Finally, participants with low levels of involvement used the term anxiety to refer to the psychological state of individuals during the pandemic. The objective of this research was to describe and analyze the role of psychological and behavioral factors on perceptions of COVID-19 in France and Quebec at three different times during the pandemic. In line with our objectives, the results of these studies show how people structured their perceptions of COVID-19 during the first lockdown. For example, the way people interpret emerging health threats might depend on the policy and health measures in place in a territory. Indeed, at the beginning of the study, the perceptions of the Quebecers and the French were more focused on their feelings (Study 1), and fear in particular. The feeling of fear has also been an important characteristic of previous epidemics (e.g., SARS) [17] , and our results seem to confirm this phenomenon. Indeed, the unprecedented nature of the COVID-19 pandemic confronts populations with the unknown, loss of control, and feelings of powerlessness [43] . As a result, the epidemic generates negative emotions such as fear and anxiety; fear is therefore a normal reaction to danger that helps guide our choices and actions by avoiding excessive risks to our health [44] . Fear is a fundamental adaptive defense mechanism for survival, and involves several biological processes in preparing a response to perceived threatening events [45] . However, the results of these studies raise questions about the way people conceptualize changes over time. Indeed, if the participants' perceptions were strongly marked by emotions during the first study, it seems that they then progressively de-centered themselves from their emotions. Thus, they mentioned the lockdown measures (Study 2), and then the time of lockdown seems to have allowed them to mention more elements related to the individual management (e.g., barrier gesture) of the COVID-19 (Study 3). These results could suggest that individuals had tempered their strong emotions by managing the risk more analytically [46] . Furthermore, these results are in line with work that has identified socio-cultural [47] and emotional [48] factors important for effective risk communication. Indeed, these studies suggest that individuals' perceptions and adoption of preventive behaviors are not simply associated with a set of medical or scientific information but also with a given time period and cultural characteristics. Thus, these studies draw attention to the role of perceptions in pandemic management, as already advocated by Van Bavel et al. [10] at the beginning of pandemics. The results of these studies show to policy makers that relatively similar communication strategies can have different effects depending on the psycho-sociological, behavioral, and cultural anchors of the individuals. However, these studies have some limitations. Firstly, the small sample size is one of the limits of these studies. This does not allow us to cover the wide diversity of perceptions that can exist within a territory. In addition, a larger sample could have allowed us to explore perceptions within each territory in order to assess the existence of differences (e.g., between town and country). Secondly, our method does not allow us to study "changes" over time. To do so, it would have been necessary to carry out repeated measurements on the same individuals. Nevertheless, our aim was not to study changes in perceptions of COVID-19, but rather to explore these perceptions in order to shed light on how individuals' perceptions may be structured, depending on the territory where they live. Future longitudinal research could study the evolution of individuals' psychological and behavioral responses to political and health decisions implemented in a territory. This research should also take into account the socio-demographic characteristics of the participants, as they may influence the perception of health risks (e.g., [49] ). Finally, if these studies suggest that the lockdown period was, for many, a time for reflection, introspection, and taking a step back from COVID-19, the negative impacts of the pandemic, particularly on the mental health of many people, should not be overlooked. Epidemic management is often conceptualized by policy makers through the notion of risk. Indeed, they measure risk as the probability of contracting a disease multiplied by the magnitude of its consequences [50] . However, these three studies suggest that perceptions of COVID-19 risk semantics may also be influenced by complex psychological, social and political processes. Faced with an epidemic threat such as COVID-19, it would be necessary to consider the evolution of people's perceptions in order to adapt communications and effectively mobilize experiential (official messages from the authorities at the beginning of a crisis) and/or rational (communication of epidemiological data during the epidemic) content. In addition to communication devices, it would be wise to restructure the environment (e.g., the nudge theory) in order to guide populations towards health behaviors that promote public health. Far from a representation that would oppose emotion against reason, this study suggests the development of prevention measures that properly integrate the study of perceptions to support the management of epidemics. Transmission dynamics of the COVID-19 outbreak and effectiveness of government interventions: A data-driven analysis Comorbidity and its impact on 1590 patients with COVID-19 in China: A Nationwide Analysis Economic Effects of Coronavirus Outbreak (COVID-19) on the World Economy. SSRN Electron The socio-economic implications of the coronavirus and COVID-19 pandemic: A review COVID-19: Facts, Cultural Considerations, and Risk of Stigmatization Indirect effects of COVID-19 on the environment The effect of COVID-19 lockdowns on political support: Some good news for democracy? The outbreak of COVID-19 coronavirus and its impact on global mental health The Global Macroeconomic Impacts of COVID-19: Seven Scenarios Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response National Identification, a Social Cure for COVID-19? Evidence from 67 Countries Is the lockdown important to prevent the COVID-19 pandemic? Effects on psychology, environment and economyperspective A nation-wide survey on emotional and psychological impacts of COVID-19 social distancing Fighting COVID-19 Misinformation on Social Media: Experimental Evidence for a Scalable Accuracy-Nudge Intervention Panic, Paranoia, and Public Health-The AIDS Epidemic's Lessons for Ebola Immediate Psychological Responses and Associated Factors during the Initial Stage of the 2019 Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Epidemic among the General Population in China The Impact of the SARS Epidemic on the Utilization of Medical Services: SARS and the Fear of SARS The ecology of human fear: Survival optimization and the nervous system Social representations, identity threat, and coping amid COVID-19 Understanding an Ebola outbreak: Social representations of emerging infectious diseases Emotion, Sex, Politics, and Science: Surveying the Risk-Assessment Battlefield Risk: From perception to social representation Le regard des représentations sociales La fabrique des risques Social, Psychological, and Philosophical Reflections on Pandemics and Beyond Strategic risk behaviour and its temporalities: Between risk propensity and decision context Representations of SARS in the British newspapers Mental health problems and social media exposure during COVID-19 outbreak Implications of the behavioural immune system for social behaviour and human health in the modern world Psychological adjustment during the global outbreak of COVID-19: A resilience perspective The trajectory of loneliness in response to COVID-19 Attitude, Perceptions and Practice towards COVID-19: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis 400 ans de Relations D'exception: 400 ans de Relations D'exception; Presses de l Understanding the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on career development: Insights from cultural psychology Les inégalités sociales à l'épreuve de la crise sanitaire: Un bilan du premier confinement Methods for Studying the Structure of Social Representations: A Critical Review and Agenda for Future Research: Methods and Structure of Social Representations Under threat. Lay thinking about terrorism and the three-dimensional model of personal involvement: A social psychological analysis Socialisation, perspectives temporelles et implication personnelle: Une étude dans le champ de l'environnement Analyse des correspondances et variations des contenus de représentations sociales Erès: Ramonville Saint The Psychology of Attitudes and Attitude Change Persuasion: Psychological Insights and Perspectives The Fear of COVID-19 Scale: Development and Initial Validation Pandemic fear" and COVID-19: Mental health burden and strategies The fear of COVID-19 and its role in preventive behaviors Risk as Analysis and Risk as Feelings: Some Thoughts about Affect, Reason, Risk, and Rationality Sociocultural vectors of effective risk communication Risk, communication and trust: Towards an emotional understanding of trust Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world Socio-demographic factors associated with self-protecting behavior during the Covid-19 pandemic In this section, you can acknowledge any support given which is not covered by the author contribution or funding sections. This may include administrative and technical support, or donations in kind (e.g., materials used for experiments). The authors declare no conflict of interest.