key: cord-0910277-9s5un3sp authors: Novick, Andrew M.; Stoddard, Joel; Johnson, Rachel L.; Sammel, Mary D.; Berkowitz, Lily; Epperson, C. Neill title: Adapting psychiatric research in the age of COVID-19: role of online studies date: 2021-09-24 journal: BJPsych Open DOI: 10.1192/bjo.2021.1015 sha: 9973e1db82021021d0a8e84710077507c7d1c8ad doc_id: 910277 cord_uid: 9s5un3sp Virtual platforms can provide a socially distanced mechanism by which to promote ongoing research progress in the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) era and may change our approach to online research in the future. Understanding how to best utilise online research represents an important task for our field. Although the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic has prompted an increase in research related to the SARs-COV-2 virus itself, it simultaneously has placed significant barriers on research in other areas. In mid-March 2020 many institutions, including the University of Colorado-Anschutz Medical Campus, halted most clinical research projects that did not have a clear benefit to human participants. Although research with human participants has resumed at many institutions under carefully orchestrated 'ramp ups', the disruption has been significant. The disruption in research activity has been particularly detrimental to early career investigators in psychiatry, 1 for whom progress with research is essential to establishing a track record of productivity and independence from one's mentors. Women in academia are especially hard-hit by the pandemic as evidenced by a decrease in female-authored publications since March 2020. 2 Acquiring data and moving one's research programme forward requires adaptations of protocols and tools for data collection that allow for flexibility to balance the serious challenges imposed by the pandemic. Prior to March 2020 and institution closure to all but life-preserving research, our group was investigating the effects of oral contraceptive pills on reward processing in women. By early April 2020, it became obvious that our plans to do in-person assessments, including neuroimaging during a reward task, would no longer be viable for an unforeseen time. Instead of delaying our investigation, we pursued online research methods that did not require in-person contact and that would further advance our knowledge regarding steroid contraceptive effects on reward. In sharing our experience with other researchers, 3 we were met with considerable interest and curiosity. This suggested that the psychiatric community would benefit from learning more about conducting online research and evaluating its role even beyond the pandemic. Of course, online research in psychiatry is not necessarily new: literature on the implementation and evaluation of online psychological interventions dates back over 20 years. 4 Thus, although much has been written about online research, 5,6 here we aim to give a brief description of our experience and discuss the advantages of online research, available tools and potential limitations, such that others might consider its implementation. For those interested in exploring online research methods, Sauter et al 6 recently published a brief yet thorough guide to the necessary steps, tools and considerations for running online behavioural experiments. We designed our study in REDCap, 7 a platform for research database management, which allows for participantfacing surveys and task participation via the web. Importantly, we adapted the reward task that was originally planned for use during functional magnetic resonance scanning. This task sought to measure both reward anticipation and initial response to reward for various types of images (erotic images, non-erotic pleasant images such as puppies and scenery, as well as neutral images such as furniture). REDCap's interface was well suited for this task, allowing for stimulus presentation along with user input of image ratings. A number of platforms for delivering novel, online tasks are availablefrom extensions of lab-based software such as PsychoPy and E-Prime to online-only platforms such as Testable -REDCap was sufficient for our needs and we were already familiar with using it. A list of various resources that can assist in online study design and implementation can be found in the Appendix. Recruitment of participants occurred through various internet media, including university listservs, social media and the use of established platforms, such as ResearchMatch that connect researchers with potential volunteers and allow targeted advertising to specific volunteers (Appendix). Over a period 2.5 months, we recruited n = 1000 eligible participants, half were current users of hormonal contraceptives and half who did not use hormonal contraceptives. This provided us with a large data-set to apply multivariable statistical and reinforcement learning models in order to evaluate the relationship between contraception use and reward processing. Furthermore, the data-set serves as preliminary results that will hone our original hypotheses and design of an in-person, neuroimaging experiment with the most robust stimuli possible to distinguish differences between groups. We found that one of the biggest advantages of online research is that it removes geographic barriers allowing researchers to recruit participants nationally and internationally as well as individuals from more isolated regions. The ability to complete studies from home may also eliminate issues of stigma that prevent individuals participating in research. 8 It also allows for the rapid accrual of large amounts of data. And because it requires minimal equipment and less investment on the part of the participant in terms of time and travel, it is also an economic research strategy that can be used by early career investigators and others who have yet to acquire significant funding. Although online research has the obvious benefit of feasibility during the pandemic, it has known limitations and disadvantages. 8 The requirement for internet access and necessary computer skills is biased against low-income individuals with less education. 9 As all eligibility information is gathered by self-report with minimal interaction with research staff, there is increased risk of inadvertent erroneous reporting or frank deceptive practices. Some individuals may rush through study questions with little thought to their responses whereas others may even set up automated methods or 'bots' in order to redeem payment with minimal effort. These risks can be minimised by putting in quality control measures, such as using online CAPTCHA (Completely Automated Public Turing test to tell Computers and Humans Apart) tools and interspersing questions that deliberately ask for a certain response (i.e. 'Please select option 4'). Even with quality assurance strategies, there are differences in the environment in an online versus lab setting for cognitive tasking that need to be better understood. Whereas one participant might be participating on their computer in a quiet environment, another might be on their phone while watching television. Such variation might be particularly relevant when asking participants to complete tasks with greater cognitive demands. 10 However, researchers considering online experiments should be reassured by data suggesting that attention to tasks 11 and data quality 12, 13 are similar between online and lab participants. Furthermore, anonymous, online tasking might promote more naturalistic behaviour, when the respondent is less influenced by an unfamiliar environment or the experimenter. The latter is known to lead to social desirability or demand effects, which may be particularly important in experiments like ours, in which we ask individuals to rate their experience of erotica. 14 In any event, as the pandemic has promoted a shift for online clinical and research assessment, our field will need to better understand its implications for study design, analysis and interpretation. Thus, ongoing comparisons of similar methods that are conducted in laboratory environments versus at home via the internet will be necessary. The COVID-19 pandemic has created both challenges and opportunities for psychiatric research. Here we described how utilising an online research strategy enabled data collection and progress to inform our future work when other methods were not feasible. As opposed to a replacement for laboratory-controlled experiments, we discovered how online research could complement and inform our in-person experimental designs. During this time of unpredictability, we suggest this to others as a strategy for ongoing research progress. As patients and clinicians are adapting towards a responsible use of telehealth that will outlast the pandemic, so too should clinical researchers shift towards a better understanding and use of online sampling. Conducting eating disorders research in the time of COVID-19: a survey of researchers in the field COVID-19 medical papers have fewer women first authors than expected When SARS CoV-2 Gives Your Research Career Lemons: Make Lemonade. Annual Conference of the National Network of Depression Centers Internet-delivered psychological treatments: from innovation to implementation The SAGE Handbook of Online Research Methods hosting and recruiting: a brief introduction to running behavioral experiments online The REDCap consortium: building an international community of software platform partners They know what they are getting into:" researchers confront the benefits and challenges of online recruitment for HIV research Digital divide: marked disparities in computer and broadband internet use and associated health inequalities in the United States Computerized neuropsychological assessment devices: joint position paper of the American academy of clinical neuropsychology and the national academy of neuropsychology Is There a cost to convenience? An experimental comparison of data quality in laboratory and online studies Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon's MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing Comparing online and lab methods in a problem-solving experiment I can see you": the impact of implied social presence on visual attention to erotic and neutral stimuli in men and women