key: cord-0896368-6mnjt0t8 authors: Pedersen, Mogens Jin; Favero, Nathan title: Social Distancing During the COVID‐19 Pandemic: Who Are the Present and Future Non‐compliers? date: 2020-05-22 journal: Public Adm Rev DOI: 10.1111/puar.13240 sha: b829908050753db3ef2871e90ed10f85323eb441 doc_id: 896368 cord_uid: 6mnjt0t8 Social distancing is an effective means for containing the spread of COVID‐19, but only so if we all participate. Who are the individuals who are the least likely to adhere to social distancing recommendations, presently and in the long‐term? Such knowledge is important for policymakers looking to sustain the public's buy‐in to social distancing. Using survey data from sample U.S. residents (n = 1,449), we show that some demographic factors (gender, age, race, political party) help predict intent to adhere to social distancing. Yet demographic factors are relatively poor predictors compared to individual attitudes and media diets. We argue that public officials should make efforts to inform and persuade the public of the importance of social distancing, targeting mediums like TV and radio where audiences are less likely to currently engage in social distancing or are less likely to envision themselves sustaining strict social distancing for several weeks or months. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. The coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic presents a major challenge to all societies. For curbing the spread of the virus, governments have enacted various policies aimed at regulating peoples' behavior and social habits. In particular, citizens across the globe are intensely encouraged to engage in "social distancing" (also referred to as "physical distancing") (Briscese et al. 2020; Merelli 2020; Paun et al. 2020) . Health authorities and experts enforce how maintaining a physical distance from others and avoiding crowds is crucial for mitigating both the extent and speed of the COVID-19 spread (Ahmed, Zviedrite, and Uzicanin 2018; Chen et al. 2020; ECDC 2020; Fong et al. 2020; Rashid et al. 2015; WHO 2020; Zhang et al. 2020) . Complementing other containment measures, such as hand hygiene and widespread testing, social distancing is key for reducing excessive demands on intensive health care services-and, thus, for ensuring the effective treatment of all who become infected. Given clear externalities created when individuals ignore social distancing guidelines, there is a compelling case for governmental regulation of such behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Both the costs and the benefits of social distancing are widely distributed throughout the public (rather than concentrated), which indicates a case of "majoritarian politics" under James Q. Wilson's typology of the politics of regulation (see Wilson 1995) . Yet because widespread, aggressive enforcement of social distancing rules may be impractical or perceived as unduly invasive, governments largely rely on residents to voluntarily comply with social distancing rules and guidelines. Americans in general appear to be widely engaged in social distancing (Cortez 2020; Fetzer et al. 2020; Resnick 2020) . However, notable variation is observed, suggesting that some individuals are far less compliant than others-a fact highlighted prominently by recent protests of stay-at-home orders in many U.S. state capitals (Bosman, Tavernise, and Baker 2020) . A Stanford-led study (Moore et al. 2020 ) finds that roughly 4 in 10 Americans were This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. Accepted Article not complying with social distancing recommendations in mid-March, 2020. Although the most common reason for not following orders was work requirements for non-essential industries (28.2%), other explanations include worries about mental and physical well-being (20.3%), the belief that other precautions, such as hand-washing, are sufficient (18.8%), a wish to continue everyday activities (13.9%), and the belief that society is overreacting (12.7%). Similarly, a Gallup poll from April 6 (McCarthy 2020) shows that a non-negligible minority of Americans are still going to public places (11%) and are neither avoiding small gatherings (16%) nor travel by plane, bus, subway, or train (22%). Findings such as these raise important questions. Who are these non-compliers? What characterizes the people least inclined to adhere to social distancing recommendations, presently and in the long-term? Using survey data from a sample of 1,449 U.S. residents collected on April 3, 2020, this research note offers insights into the individual-level predictors of citizens' willingness to social distance. We examine various potential predictors of one's willingness to social distance, including not only standard demographic characteristics and political identities but also measures of prosocial motivation, empathy, and news consumption. The role of news consumption in particular may be important given political communication research emphasizing news consumption as a key means through which voters learn information, with different types of news sources potentially promoting different reactions and degrees of learning (Kruikemeier, Lecheler, and Boyer 2018; Peterson forthcoming; Sobieraj and Berry 2011; Strömbäck, Falasca, and Kruikemeier 2018) . In terms of outcomes, our analysis focuses on two distinct social distancing measures: (a) an index of social distancing behavior and (b) the duration (number of weeks) that people say they can see themselves staying at home and avoiding social contact. The former measure ('social distancing behavior') captures a set of key responses representing behavioral compliance with social distancing recommendations. Other COVID-19-related work uses similar item measures in order to, for example, evaluate different means of promoting social distancing (Lunn et al. 2020; Pfattheicher et al. 2020; Utych and Fowler 2020 ). Yet research is needed that directly and more robustly substantiates knowledge about the types of people who are not adhering to social distancing. Our second outcome measure ('social distancing anticipated duration') taps into an equally important but understudied issue directly relevant to efforts at mitigating the spread of COVID-19 in the long-term. To what extent are people mentally prepared to follow stayat-home orders potentially lasting many weeks or months? Who are the individuals least inclined to sustain social distancing for longer periods, even if they might have been okay with initially staying at home and avoiding social contact? Identifying the subpopulations of individuals who are the most likely social distancing non-compliers, presently and in the long-term, is important for public officials who are tasked with safeguarding the public welfare. While public health experts, medical doctors, and virologists must provide policy-makers with the information they need to make wellinformed decisions, social scientists should provide insights regarding how to best account for the human behavioral aspects of pandemic response that are so crucial for effectively curbing the COVID-19 spread. Whether at the municipal, state, or federal level, governments face a regulatory task that requires the use of not just formal sanctions (e.g., fines) for noncompliance but also persuasion and positive inducements (e.g., enhanced unemployment benefits that encourage staying at home). Regulation-"the public administrative policing of a private activity with respect to a rule prescribed in the public interest" (Mitnick 1980: 7)is one of the core functions of government (Christensen 2011). Thus, enforcing and regulating social distancing during COVID-19 is a public task and responsibility that rests, first and foremost, with government and the public administration. Public interventions specifically targeting both present and future non-compliers will be helpful-if not essential-for achieving control over the spread of COVID-19 (i.e., maintaining a so-called "R0" of one or less; see Eisenberg 2020). While forecasting is difficult, policy enforcing or incentivizing people anew to continued social distancing may assume prominent importance presently or in the near future. For example, a recent study published in Science suggests that prolonged or intermittent social distancing may be necessary into 2022 (Kissler et al. 2020) , and much research supports the concept of "behavioral fatigue" for pandemic prevention measures. Several U.S. states have recently seen public protests against government stay-at-home orders (Bosman, Tavernise, and Baker 2020) . As noted by Bell (2020) To measure American's intentions and attitudes regarding social distancing, we fielded a short online survey on April 3, 2020. By this date, 42 states had issued stay-at-home orders, including high-profile states (e.g., Florida, Georgia) where governors had resisted calls for such orders until that week (Fernandez 2020). We recruited 1,502 paid survey respondents (1,449 after dropping observations with unusable data) using Prolific, a professional crowdworking platform designed for academic and marketing researchers. Prolific offers a profiled participant pool comprising more than 15,800 U.S. nationals, and research suggests that " [Prolific] provides data quality that is comparable or not significantly different than MTurk's, and [Prolific's] participants seem to be more naïve to common experimental research tasks, and offer a more diverse population in terms of geographical location, ethnicity, etc." (Peer et al. 2017: 161 ; see also Palan and Schitter 2018). Nevertheless, as with all non-probability samples, we recognize how our respondents are not fully representative of the general U.S. population. Indeed, certain demographic groups are overrepresented in our sample (i.e., young people, Democrats, Asians, well-educated people). We have applied weights to our data to improve the generalizability of our findings in terms of demographics. Still, weighting may only enhance representativeness on observable characteristics, and even after our reweighting, our sample is somewhat younger, better-educated, and more Democratic-leaning on average than the broader U.S. public. Our primary interest is to specify the individual characteristics that can help predict social distancing intentions and attitudes during COVID-19. We believe our sample is suitable for this purpose, though our results most directly describe these patterns among a set of people who signed up to participate in occasional online surveys. Inferences should be made with care and consideration of our sampling method. Relatedly, and though recent COVID-19 research shows that self-reported measures of social distancing track actual behavior both at the individual and at the U.S. state level (Gollwitzer et al. 2020), our results may be susceptible to common method bias (Favero and Bullock 2014). More details on our methodology-including similar results obtained from analyses without survey weights-are contained in the appendix. Our respondents generally indicate an intention to engage in strict social distancing. On a scale ranging from 0 to 100, where 0 indicates 'no intention to socially distance' and 100 indicates 'maximum social distancing,' the average response is 87.8. This scale was constructed based on responses to five different statements about their behavior in the next few weeks. Table 1 shows the five statements. Variation is greatest for a statement about intentions to meet with friends or relatives. However, even for this statement, the average response (1.56 out of 10) indicates a general adherence to social distancing recommendations. [ Table 1 about here] We also asked respondents to report the maximum duration (number of weeks) for which they could see themselves maintaining social distancing, if officials advised it ('social distancing anticipated duration'). Again, we see a relatively high willingness to socially distance. The average respondent can see herself avoiding social contact as much as possible for about 17 weeks (the median is 10 weeks). Still, we observe substantial variation: 30% of respondents indicate 6 weeks or fewer, while 17% indicate half a year (26 weeks) or longer. The final five items in Table 1 capture COVID-19 news attentiveness as well as four measures of attitudes and beliefs about COVID-19. Most respondents indicate they are following news about COVID-19 quite closely (average is .81 out of 1). We see strong agreement with the belief that COVID-19 is "the single biggest threat to society" (average is .71 out of 1). The next two items indicate that respondents generally believe that prioritizing saving lives over economic concerns is important. Finally, we see substantial support for regulatory policies requiring non-essential businesses to shut down for a time (average is .85 out of 1). In sum, we observe a public that is generally highly concerned about the dangers posed by COVID-19 and that is generally highly willing to engage in social distancing, irrespective of the economic costs. Despite broad agreement on these matters, some respondents diverge in their attitudes and intended behaviors. We also detect considerable variation in how long individuals can see themselves remaining at home and avoiding social contact. We now turn our attention to the individual-level factors that may help to explain the variation we observe both in social distancing behavior and in the duration for which people can see themselves maintaining social distancing. We ran a set of OLS regressions, which can be found in the appendix. Figure 1 plots the main results. [ Figure 1 about here] For social distancing behavior, women report higher levels of social distancing than men. This finding is in line with research suggesting that women are socialized toward a gender role that is more passive, rule-abiding, and compassionate (Coffe and Bolzendahl 2010; Granié 2007), and that women tend to exhibit higher levels of organizational rule abidance than men (Portillo and DeHart-Davis 2009). Conversely, we find some indication that men can see themselves remaining in isolation for more weeks than women. A possible explanation is that men tend to care less about interpersonal relationships than women (Yang and Girgus 2019), or that men tend to have smaller and less varied social networks with fewer friends and less contact frequency (Psylla et al. 2017; Shye et al. 1995) . We see no substantial associations for age in Figure 1 . However, in line with research observing a positive relation between age in adulthood and law-abiding orientation (Fine et al. 2020), older respondents (age 45+) do indicate more social distancing in regression models with no or fewer independent, attitudinal variables (see appendix, Table A -2, models based differences in terms of COVID-19 news attentiveness and COVID-19-related attitudes and beliefs. Black individuals appear to socially distance somewhat less than others do, and Asians appear to socially distance slightly more. Education appears to have little association with social distancing. As one might expect, essential workers-who are less able to socially distance because of their jobs-report adhering to socially distancing behaviors less, and they also see themselves isolating for a fewer number of weeks. Political partisanship appears to have little association with social distancing. However, as with age, regression models with fewer independent, attitudinal variables reveal a significant association. Specifically, before we include controls for COVID-19 news attentiveness and COVID-19-related attitudes and beliefs, Democrats report doing more social distancing than either Republicans or those who identify with neither of the major U.S. political parties (see appendix, Table A People with high levels of prosocial motivation appear to engage in more social distancing, and there is some indication that people with high levels of empathy can see themselves isolating for longer. Media consumption habits are associated with social distancing and anticipated duration of isolation in interesting ways. Those who primarily get their news from radio or social media tend to report slightly less social distancing, while those who primarily get their news from websites tend to report more. People who get their news from magazines can see themselves avoiding social contact for longer, while those who get their news from TV or radio indicate a fewer number of weeks for which they can see themselves isolating. We recognize that each of these news media types are marked by within-type variation in political ideology (e.g., different TV channels are reflective of different political views). However, as we control for political partisanship, our findings indicate the predictive value of This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. Accepted Article knowing that someone does or does not consume each type of media source while holding his/her partisanship constant. Finally, those who indicate they follow news about COVID-19 more closely appear more engaged in social distancing. Many of the strongest predictors of social distancing behavior are COVID-19-related attitudes and beliefs. This is perhaps unsurprising given the costs of social distancing-costs that people may be unwilling to bear absent a strong belief in the importance of addressing the health issues caused by the coronavirus. Those who believe that COVID-19 is a monumental threat to society tend to socially distance more, and those who believe that government should require non-essential businesses to close for a time tend to socially distance much more. Those who think we must keep the economy going even if it means more COVID-19 deaths report behavior less consistent with social distancing guidelines. Finally, those who believe in temporarily closing businesses also tend to say they can see themselves avoiding social contact for much longer. What characterizes the individuals who are the least inclined to adhere to social distancing recommendations, presently and in the long-term? We find survey evidence (n = 1,449) suggesting that Americans' willingness to socially distance is associated with sociodemographic, behavioral, and attitudinal characteristics. While we do find that some demographic characteristics (gender, age, race, political party) can meaningfully predict social distancing intentions, the explanatory power of such variables is relatively modest. Furthermore, some demographic variables (age, political party) lose their predictive power once we account for attentiveness to COVID-19 news and COVID-19-related attitudes and beliefs. Thus, older age and Democratic partisanship are likely associated "indirectly" with This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. Accepted Article social distancing compliance; these groups appear to exhibit a greater concern about the COVID-19 situation and associated health risks, factors which are in turn strongly associated with social distancing behaviors. While we do find evidence that partisanship matters to some extent when predicting social distancing behavior, our simple three-category (Democrat, Republic, Other) (see Baker and Renno 2019) measure of partisanship does not allow us to consider whether even greater differences emerge once one distinguishes between weak and strong partisans. Future research should consider whether, for example, strong supporters of President Trump behave differently than those who somewhat reluctantly support him. We also note that education levels do not significantly predict social distancing outcomes in any of our models-despite the emerging importance of education as a social cleavage in recent years (see Schaffner, MacWilliams, and Nteta 2018). Social distancing behaviors also appear to be higher on average among those who have strong prosocial motivation and among those who get their news from websites rather than radio or social media. The significance of prosocial motivation suggests that people are motivated in part to social distance because of concern for the wellbeing of others. We are somewhat surprised by the positive association between social distancing and use of websites because print media has traditionally been viewed as more conducive to learning than online media, although some recent studies have reached relatively optimistic conclusions about the ability of news websites to promote productive engagement of audiences with information (Kruikemeier, Lecheler, and Boyer 2018; Fletcher and Nielsen 2017). Our negative finding for radio could align with Sobieraj and Berry's (2011) work suggesting that political commentary on cable television and radio tends to be especially loaded with "outrage discourse," or rhetoric intended to elicit visceral reactions. Such appeals to outrage may effectively crowd out the very sorts of presentations of factual information that promote learning in other news mediums. To the extent that social media also disproportionately directs people to outrage discourse, this same mechanism could account for our negative finding for social media. Results for the duration (number of weeks) that people are willing to stay at home and avoiding social contact are estimated with less precision. However, we do find some indications that men and those who are more empathetic are more mentally prepared to follow stay-at-home orders lasting many weeks or months. COVID-19-related attitudes and beliefs appear to be the best predictors of this outcome, alongside choice of news sources. Individuals who primarily get their news from TV or radio can see themselves avoiding social contact for fewer weeks, whereas those who get their news from magazines can see themselves isolating for longer. These insights may help inform decision-making aimed at securing and maintaining social distancing compliance across a highly diverse and politically polarized society. A Gallup poll from April 17-19 suggests that Americans are as likely to say the coronavirus situation in the U.S. is getting better (41%) as to say that it is getting worse (39%) (Jones 2020). Only ten days prior, they were far more likely to say it was getting worse (62%). Trends such as this may soon bring about a critical need for new regulatory approaches, including communicative interventions and behavioral incentives directed at motivating people to maintain their social distancing behavior. Any positive signs in the containment of COVID-19 spread may quickly fade or dissipate in the face of a (too) rapid attenuation in social distancing caused by, for example, "behavioral fatigue" or perceptual underestimation of the actual risk (Bell 2020). One possible approach toward addressing concerns about non-compliance is to target messaging toward the demographic groups that are least willing or mentally prepared to engage in prolonged social distancing efforts. Yet our results suggest that demographic factors are not particularly strong predictors of social distancing compliance. Thus, while targeted messaging may be advisable, public officials should also take care to guard against stereotyping or exaggerating the extent to which certain groups are less likely to comply with social distancing. Attitudes are better predictors of social distancing behavior than demographic factors, and attitudes about COVID-19 are only weakly associated with demographics. Furthermore, apparent non-compliance may be driven by practical constraints, such as those faced by essential workers and communities of color; thus, public officials should consider not just how to discourage activities that increase infection rates but also how to empower all residents to socially distance to the greatest extent possible (see Barari et al. 2020 ). More broadly, policy-makers and administrators would do well to remember that effective and normatively laudable implementation of governmental regulatory functions requires an attention to social equity and due process considerations that can easily be lost amid the zeal to address pressing social ills (see Meier and Smith 1994; Haque 2002; Spicer 2002) . Given that attitudes appear to be primary drivers of social distancing behavior, public messaging may be particularly important for gaining compliance. While firmly-held attitudes are difficult to change, our results regarding media consumption are suggestive of potential means by which public officials might win over residents reluctant to uphold social distancing. Given that consumers of written news media and those who follow COVID-19 news very closely tend to report stronger social distancing behavioral intentions, perhaps public officials should devote more attention to engaging with TV and radio stations-media forms whose audiences appear less inclined to engage in social distancing presently or in the future. The U.S. public typically expresses greater trust in local than federal government (Cole and Kincaid 2000; see also Baker 1995) , so it may be especially important for residents to hear from local officials, even when such officials feel ill-prepared to go on live TV or radio. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. While public officials dealing with the coronavirus response are undoubtedly short on time right now, maintaining public support for compliance with public health guidelines is crucial. Dr. Anthony Fauci serves as an inspiring role model in this regard. Despite the current demands on his time, Dr. Fauci has sat for numerous interviews across a wide range of media platforms, allowing him to inform the public about the latest coronavirus developments and what we can do to mitigate its spread. The topic of public engagement is of particular importance to public administration since the populace may have greater trust in information coming from public servants-especially medical doctors and scientists-than in officials filling roles viewed as more political (Frewer et al. 1996) . This notion is directly connected to the classical politics-administration dichotomy (Gulick 1937; Wilson 1887 ). Yet, while a degree of separation between "administration" (scientifically-informed advice and implementation) and "politics" (resolving clashes over political values and norms) may be crucial for the effectiveness of public health messaging during COVID-19, complete segmentation of these tasks is impossible, perhaps more so than ever in the highly polarized When it comes to formulating content for effective public messaging, COVID-19related research supports the utility of appealing to peoples' prosocial motivation and empathy as means for promoting social distancing (Everett et al. 2020; Pfattheicher et al. 2020 ). However, research also emphasizes public messaging focused on ideas for how to ease This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. Accepted Article the burden of isolation, rather than a lopsided perspective on only the health risks and dangers of not adhering to social distancing (Barari et al. 2020; Favero and Pedersen 2020) . Moreover, use of charismatic leadership tactics-a set of specific rhetorical techniques in communication-may improve the effectiveness of both types of messaging (Jensen 2020). A mixed approach may be the way forward. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. 87.78 14.86 0-100 "I will meet with friends or relatives who live outside my own household"* 1.56 2.50 0-10 "I will make the fewest possible trips to the grocery store" 8.90 1.76 0-10 "I will be at places where other people will also be (café, restaurant, specialty shops, church, etc.)"* 1.13 2.17 0-10 "I will avoid all social gatherings (i.e., adhere to so-called 'social distancing')" 9.08 2.06 0-10 "I will strongly encourage others to avoid all social contact (i.e., adhere to so-called 'social distancing')" 8. Notes: * item was reversed to create the social distancing index. Notes: Plot of OLS regression estimates (Table A -2, models 3 and 7, in the appendix). Dots indicate the relationship between an independent variable (such as gender or age) and the social distancing outcome identified at the top of the graph (i.e., coefficient point estimates). Dots to the right of the 0-line indicate a positive association with the outcome, while dots to the left of the 0-line indicate a negative association with the outcome. The horizontal bands around each dot indicate statistical uncertainty about the estimated relationship for that variable (i.e., 95% and 90% confidence intervals shown by thin and thick bands, respectively). Many of the independent variables have statistically indeterminate associations with the outcomes, which is why their bands overlap with the 0-lines. On the left side of the figure, values in parentheses shows the comparison groups for categorical variables. Our survey sample comprises U.S. residents recruited via Prolific-a commercial crowdworking platform similar to Amazon's Mechanical Turk. In particular, participants were invited to complete a five-minute survey on "Coronavirus (COVID-19) Perceptions" in exchange for $0.55. Prolific has implemented extensive measures for preventing individuals from setting up multiple user accounts (Bradley 2018) , and research supports that Prolific has a diverse participant pool and is suitable for online social science experimentation (Palan and Survey weights were constructed using post-stratification raking based on gender, age, education level, and race. Prior to constructing weights, listwise deletion was used to eliminate any observations with missing values for variable used in this study (note: individuals who selected non-binary gender in our survey were dropped because population figures for non-binary gender identification are not available from the American Community Survey). Weights were constructed using the user-written Stata command "ipfraking" (after convergence, weights were trimmed to a minimum of .5 and a maximum of 2, in order to avoid extreme weighting of observations). Population figures were based on 2018 American Community Survey 1-year estimates, which indicated the following population proportions: Social distancing behavior was measured using an index comprising the five items appearing in the research note, Table 1 . We developed the items for the purpose of our survey. The five items were preceded by the following text: "To what extent do you agree with the following statements? During the next few weeks…" Item order was randomized, and item responses were captured on an 11-point Likert scale, anchored at 0 = 'Strongly disagree' and 10 = 'Strongly agree. The index was generated as a simple additive measure (Cronbach's α = .73), each items having equal weight, and rescaled to a theoretical range of 0 to 100 by multiplying the additive scale by 2 (0 = 'no intention to socially distance' and 100 = 'maximum social distancing'). Social distancing anticipated duration was measured using the following item: "What is the longest amount of time you could see yourself staying at home as much as possible and avoiding all social contact (i.e., adhering to so-called "social distancing")? If officials advised it, I could see myself generally staying at home and avoiding social contact for up to [text box requiring whole-number] weeks." We developed the item for the purpose of our survey. Responses were entered into an open textbox, because we did not want to prime respondents based on a set of pre-constructed response options. Both social distancing outcomes were preceded by a brief informational text passage involving experimental variation in degree of appeal to prosocial motivation and empathy (Favero and Pedersen 2020). Importantly, the minor difference in text did not appear to affect the survey responses in a meaningful way. ANOVA tests yield p-values indicating nothing close to a significant difference in means across experimental condition (p = .93 for social distancing behavior; p = .98 for anticipated duration). Still, we decided on a cautious approach and, therefore, conduct our analyses controlling for treatment assignment. As a robustness check, we run regressions without treatment dummies (see Table A -2). We also tried controlling for the geographic region of residence and found no meaningful change in our results (these models not shown). COVID-19 news consumption was measured using the following item: This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. 'Not at all closely' (0)). Responses were assigned the values listed in parentheses in order to make the scale range from 0 to 1. COVID-19-related attitudes and beliefs were measured by the four individual items appearing in the research note, Table 1 . The four items were preceded by the following text: "To what extent do you agree with the following statements?" Item order was randomized, and item responses were captured on an 11-point Likert scale, anchored at 0 = 'Strongly disagree' and 10 = 'Strongly agree. Our measure of each attitude was then divided by 10 to produce a range of 0 to 1. Political affiliation was measured using the following item: "What is your preferred political party?" Response options were: 'Republican,' 'Democratic,' 'Other' (in randomized order). Prosocial motivation was measured using a slightly adapted version of a four-item scale by Grant (2008) (Cronbach's α = .92), with four response options ("Very closely", "Fairly closely", "Not too closely", "Not at all closely"). An additive index was created and then rescaled to a theoretical range of 0 to 1, with a 1 indicating "very closely" for all four items. The prosocial motivation items were presented in random order in a batter that also included our empathy measure (which used the same response options). Empathy was measured using the Singe Item Trait Empathy Scale (SITES)-an item developed and validated by Konrath, Meier, and Bushman (2018)-and was also scaled to range from 0 to 1, with 1 indicating "very closely." Primary news source was measured using the following item: "Where do you usually get information about the news? (Select all that apply)." Response options were: Newspapers, Magazines, TV, Radio, Websites, Social media (e.g., Facebook, Twitter) (in randomized order). In terms of question ordering, the following items appeared prior to the social distancing items: gender, age, education, prosocial motivation, empathy, political affiliation, and news consumption. Information about race and "essential worker" status was collected at the end of the survey. 1449 Notes: + p<0.10, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01 (two-tailed). Standard errors in parentheses (robust option used for models 4 and 8). Comparison groups for categorical variables are: male, age <24, White, high school or less, not essential worker, Republican. Effectiveness of workplace social distancing measures in reducing influenza transmission: A systematic review Public response to hurricane probability forecasts Nonpartisans as false negatives: The mismeasurement of party identification in public opinion surveys Evaluating COVID-19 public health messaging in Italy: Self-reported compliance and growing mental health concerns Do we suffer 'behavioral fatigue' for pandemic prevention measures? Mind Hacks Why These Protesters Aren't Staying Home for Coronavirus Orders. The New York Times Bots and data quality on crowdsourcing platforms