key: cord-0858998-adcvzi44 authors: Hagihara, Hiromichi; Yamamoto, Nozomi; Meng, Xianwei; Sakata, Chifumi; Wang, Jue; Watanabe, Ryoichi; Moriguchi, Yusuke title: COVID-19 school and kindergarten closure relates to children's social relationships: a longitudinal study in Japan date: 2022-01-24 journal: Sci Rep DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-04944-2 sha: 2d6eacc522bd082d0babc676b12e84dae8c347d9 doc_id: 858998 cord_uid: adcvzi44 The COVID-19 pandemic has led children to experience school closures. Although increasing evidence suggests that such intense social quarantine influences children’s social relationships with others, longitudinal studies are limited. Using longitudinal data collected during (T1) and after (T2) intensive school closure and home confinement, this study investigated the impacts of social quarantine on children’s social relationships. Japanese parents of children aged 0–9 years (n = 425) completed an online questionnaire that examined children’s socio-emotional behavior and perceived proximity to parents or others. The results demonstrated that social quarantine was not significantly related to children’s socio-emotional behavior across all age groups. However, changes in children’s perceived proximity varied depending on certain age-related factors: elementary schoolers’ perceived closeness to parents significantly decreased after the reopening of schools, whereas that to others, such as peers, increased. Such effects were not observed in infants and preschoolers. The follow-up survey 9-month after the reopening of schools (T3; n = 130) did not detect significant differences in both children’s socio-emotional behavior and perceived proximity from that after the intense quarantine. These findings suggest that school closure and home confinement may have influenced children’s social development differently across their age, and its effects were larger in perceived closeness rather than social behavior. The worldwide spread of the novel coronavirus, SARS-CoV-2, has considerably affected our everyday lives. Although children accounted for only a small proportion of the infected population and they likely showed less severe symptoms compared to adults [1] [2] [3] , children seem to be vulnerable to sudden changes in their physical and social environment: their health-related behaviors such as physical activities, sleep patterns, eating habits, and psychological responses have been negatively influenced by the pandemic 4, 5 . A major change in children's social life might be that, as the result of kindergarten and/or school closure, children had to be confined at home for a certain period in many countries, including Japan, to prevent the virus from rapidly spreading. This school closure has impacted the learning environment of students all over the world 6 . More importantly, it could have influenced children's social development [7] [8] [9] . Nevertheless, although studies have examined children's mental health, it is not well known how school and kindergarten closure owing to the pandemic affects social relationships between children and others, such as parents and friends. The change in social relationships between children and family or non-family members (e.g., peers) may be a crucial factor when we consider the mechanism of the negative impact of school closure on children's social development. Generally, children's social relationships expand from family members to others as they grow and develop. Parent-child relationships are assumed to provide a fundamental basis for the development of social interactions with others, such as relationships with peers [10] [11] [12] . Relationships with friends also influence children's social and emotional development 13 , such that the quality of friendship predicts improvements in school attitudes 14 . School closure and home confinement caused by the pandemic directly affected children's face-to-face contact with others in social settings, which may have affected their social relationships. For instance, school Method Study design and situation in Japan. We asked the primary caregivers of children aged 0-9 years to answer the questionnaire about their children's social relationships at two-time points: during the intense quarantine imposed by the Japanese government's declaration of a state of emergency (T1) and 6 months after when the number of COVID-19 cases was relatively declining and children were no longer quaratined (T2). Additionally, as a follow-up survey, we collected the data for the third time when the government declared a state of emergency again but schools and kindergartens were not closed (T3). The Japanese government declared the first state of emergency for 7 among 47 prefectures on April 7th, 2020, and it was extended to the entire country on April 16th, 2020 29 . Roughly speaking, approximately 70% of kindergartens and 90% of elementary schools were closed during this period 30 . From May 14th to 25th, 2020, the state of emergency was gradually relaxed. Accordingly, kindergartens and schools were re-opened 31 32 . The second state of emergency ended on March 21st, 2021. Since the Japanese fiscal year starts in April and ends in March, the school years of the participants in this study remained the same for all the time points ranging from T1 to T3. The correspondence between our data collection and the situation in Japan is briefly described in Fig. 1 . Participants. The sample comprised children aged 0-9 years who were randomly selected from the population of a database (Cross Marketing Inc., Tokyo, Japan). As proxies, their primary caregivers answered the questionnaire. During the first data point, which is referred to as T1 (i.e., "during quarantine"), the sample completed the online survey between April 28 and 30, 2020, when the government declared a state of emergency covering all regions in Japan and most of the kindergartens and schools in all prefectures were closed 30 . Based on their ages, the children were classified into ten groups, and 70 parents were selected as participants in each age group (resulting in a total of 700 participants). During T2 (i.e., "after quarantine"), which was 6 months after the first data point, when the number of COVID-19 cases was relatively declining and children did not experience quarantine, the same participants were recruited again. The sample completed the survey for T2 between October 6 to 26, 2020. The second survey took a longer duration for completion as some of the participants did not respond immediately. The final sample included 425 participants from the original sample. The sample characteristics for the final and dropout samples are presented in Table 1 . No differences were found between Figure 1 . Correspondence between the spread of COVID-19 in Japan and time points of data collection in this study. Many schools and kindergartens were closed during the 1st state of emergency, but not during the 2nd time. Data of daily cases of infected were obtained from 39 . T1 to T3 indicate the time points of the data collection. See the main text for a more detailed description. . Although most of these basic characteristics did not differ in the final and dropped samples, our data might have been possibly biased as we collected the data via a somewhat handy resource, and caregivers, not children themselves, answered the questionnaire. For the analyses, children were categorized into infants (0-3 years of age, n = 165), preschoolers (4-6 years of age, n = 88), and schoolers (6-9 years of age, n = 172) based on responses to questions regarding their age and schooling. Informed consent was obtained from all parents prior to their children's involvement in the study, which was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Psychological Science Unit, Kyoto University (No. 2-P-1). For the follow-up survey, the data was collected 4 months after T2 between February 3 to 9, 2021, when kindergartens and schools generally remained open despite the second state of emergency 32 , which is referred to as T3. Due to various limitations in terms of time, labor, and budget, we focused only on preschoolers and schoolers, and not infants. Of the 260 participants from preschooler and schooler groups, the remaining sample included 130 participants whereas the dropout sample included 130 participants (remaining rate = 50%). The remaining sample comprised 47 preschoolers and 83 schoolers. Again, there were no differences between the remaining and dropout samples at T2 in Materials and procedure. Background information. The parents answered questions seeking their background information. The questionnaire included items pertaining to parental age, parental gender, parental education level, family size, children's age, and children's gender. Parental education level was assigned a value from 1 to 5 (1 = less than high school, 2 = high school, 3 = some college, 4 = undergraduate degree, 5 = graduate level). Children's social behavior. Parents of preschoolers and schoolers (4-9 years of age) answered questions regarding their children's socio-emotional behaviors. This study adopted the SDQ 16,17 as a measurement of children's socio-emotional behaviors in daily contexts. The SDQ is a screening assessment of social, emotional, and behavioral functioning and consists of 25-items. These items are divided into five subscales, each including five items: peer problems (e.g. "Generally liked by other children"), prosocial behavior (e.g. "Considerate of other people's feelings"), conduct problems (e.g. "Often fights with other children or bullies them"), emotional symptoms (e.g. "Often unhappy, depressed or tearful"), and hyperactivity (e.g. "Restless, overactive, cannot stay still for long"). Parents answered whether each item applied to a child on a three-point scale from 0 (not true) to 1 (somewhat true) to 2 (certainly true). Note that parents of infants did not answer the SDQ because they did not meet the questionnaire's target age range. Children's perceived proximity to others. All parents answered questions about their children's perceived selfparent and self-other (e.g., peers) boundary overlap based on the their own impression of their children. This study adopted the IOS scale 27, 28 . The IOS is a measure to assess the level of overlap in boundaries between oneself and another (e.g., oneself and their parent) by asking participants to choose a pair of circles with different levels of overlap. Both children's perceived self-parent and self-other proximity were assigned a value from 1 to 7 with respect to the levels of overlap. A higher number (e.g., a value of 7 which represents the most overlap) indicates that the children felt closer to others. We adopted parents as informers because our target participants included infants. Although children did not answer this measure by themselves and thus the data were obtained in an indirect way, the parental impression was expected to reflect children's perception towards others to some extent, as the SDQ are usually filled in by the proxies. Children's social lives. To confirm the degree of kindergarten and school closure and home confinement, parents answered the number of days that children spent at home per week (e.g., 3 days) at T1 and T2. We asked parents to answer this item at each time point. Additionally, we collected supplementary measures that may reflect the social environment children experienced during and after home confinement. Specifically, we asked for average hours per day that children spent playing outside; experiencing screen time such as TV, DVD, and digital games; and taking lessons or classes other than formal schooling. Data analysis. Data were classified into three sub-categories depending on children's age: infants, preschoolers, and schoolers. Children who were less than 4 years at T1 were assigned to the infants group. Of the remaining children, those who did not enter elementary school at T1 were assigned to the preschoolers group (usually less than or equal to 6 years of age), whereas those who entered elementary school were assigned to the schoolers group. First of all, we assessed changes in children's social lives during and after kindergarten and school closure (i.e., at T1 and T2). We fitted the data to a linear mixed model while regarding the number of days children spent at home as dependent variables, and the time points, age groups, and their interactions as independent variables. Similar supplementary analyses were conducted for other dependent variables, including hours children spent playing outside, experiencing screen time, and taking lessons or informal classes. Specifically, we wanted to know whether there was a significant difference between each time point for each age group. These analyses included random effects of participants to take within-participant effects into account. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Next, changes in the SDQ and IOS scores for each time point were assessed. We ran linear mixed modeling for the primary data (n = 425, T1 to T2) and the follow-up data (n = 130, T2 to T3) separately due to different properties of the data. For the changes in the SDQ score, this study adopted the Total Difficulties Score (which is calculated by summing the scores of each item on four subcategories, i.e., peer problems, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, and hyperactivity in SDQ) as dependent variables; time points, age group, and their interactions as independent variables; and participants as random effects. Additionally, a similar analysis was conducted using each subcategory score in SDQ as dependent variables; and period, types of subcategories, children's age group, and their interactions as independent variables. This analysis included random effects of participants, interaction of participants and subcategories, and that of participants and time points to account for within-participant effects with reference to Kuznetsova et al. 33 . Note that for the analyses using SDQ, only preschoolers and schoolers (not infants) were included because of the target age range of the SDQ. For the changes in children's perceived proximity to others (i.e., IOS scores), this study adopted IOS scores as dependent variables; and time points, types of relationships (parents or others), children's age group, and their interactions as independent variables. Random effects were also included, as in the analysis of the SDQ subcategory scores. Note that for the follow-up data, the participants included only parents of preschoolers and schoolers because we did not collect data from parents of infants at T3. All linear mixed modeling was conducted using R software (version 4.0.4) and lme4 package 34 . For post hoc analyses to examine single main effects, Bonferroni method was adopted to adjust p values. Changes in children's social lives during and after home confinement. The degree of school closure and home confinement was assessed using the longitudinal data. The overall mean days of children's home confinement per week were 6.11 (SD = 1.84, n = 346) days during quarantine and 2.04 (SD = 0.65, n = 346) days after quarantine, and they were consistent regardless of children's age group. There were significant main effects of time (F(1,354.53) = 1370.43, p < 0.0001), age group (F(2,354.29) = 4.21, p = 0.016), and their interaction (F(2,353,31) = 9.08, p = 0.00014). Since our aim here was to confirm whether children experienced home confinement during T1 and not T2, post hoc tests were conducted with a focus on the single main effects of time in each age group. As expected, children in all age groups spent a significantly longer time at home during T1 compared with T2 (Infants estimate ± SE = 3.62 ± 0.20, t(376) = 18.06, p < 0.0001; Preschoolers estimate ± SE = 3.65 ± 0.20, t(343) = 18.15, p < 0.0001; Schoolers estimate ± SE = 4.49 ± 0.14, t(337) = 31.52, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2a) . Evidently, children of all age groups spent time mostly at home during quarantine while they went to daycare, kindergartens, or schools after quarantine as they did before. To further explore changes in children's social lives, we also assessed time spent playing outside, experienc- Post hoc tests revealed that preschoolers and schoolers experienced screen time for significantly longer at T1 than T2 (Preschoolers estimate ± SE = 0.54 ± 0.19, t(422) = 2.83, p = 0.044; Schoolers estimate ± SE = 0.97 ± 0.14, t(422) = 7.07, p < 0.0001) but infants did not (estimate ± SE = 0.20 ± 0.14, t(422) = 1.45, p = 1.0; Fig. 2c ). Thus, children older than 4 years spent approximately 30-60 min more watching screens during kindergarten and school closure compared with after that. For lessons other than formal schooling, the overall mean hours spent by children were 0.14 (SD = 0.32, n = 425) and 0.17 (SD = 0.33, n = 425) at T1 and T2, respectively. There were significant main effect of age group (F(2,422) = 29.79, p < 0.0001), but not for time (F(1,422) = 1.29, p < 0.26) and interaction between time and age group (F(2,422) = 1.31, p < 0.27). No significant differences between T1 and T2 were found for any age groups (Fig. 2d) . Changes in children's social behavior. For SDQ scores in the primary survey, the Total Difficulties Score was calculated (Fig. 3) . Preschoolers exhibited descriptively more problematic behavior during quarantine (Mean = 11.09, SD = 5.54) than after quarantine (Mean = 10.61, SD = 6.13), and so did schoolers www.nature.com/scientificreports/ were significantly higher than those in peer problems (estimate ± SE = 3.48 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 20.35, p < 0.0001), conduct problems (estimate ± SE = 3.67 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 21.50, p < 0.0001), emotional symptoms (estimate ± SE = 3.90 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 22.86, p < 0.0001), and hyperactivity (estimate ± SE = 2.00 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 11.73, p < 0.0001). Also, scores in hyperactivity were significantly higher than those in peer problems (estimate ± SE = 1.47 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 8.62, p < 0.0001), conduct problems (estimate ± SE = 1.67 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 9.77, p < 0.0001) and emotional symptoms (estimate ± SE = 1.90 ± 0.17, t(1032) = 11.13, p < 0.0001). However, no differences were found for each subcategory score between T1 and T2. For the follow-up survey from T2 to T3, preschoolers descriptively seemed to exhibit more problematic social behavior in terms of the Total Difficulties Score at T2 (Mean = 10.55, SD = 6.44) than at T3 (Mean = 9.68, SD = 6.04). The scores for schoolers were also higher at T2 (Mean = 10.17, SD = 5.74) than T3 (Mean = 9.57, SD = 5.86). However, no significant main effects were found for time (F(1,128) Post-hoc multiple comparison of subcategory scores demonstrated that the scores in prosocial behavior were significantly higher than those in peer problems (estimate ± SE = 3.74 ± 0.23, t(512) = 16.30, p < 0.0001), conduct problems (estimate ± SE = 4.16 ± 0.23, t(512) = 18.11, p < 0.0001), emotional symptoms (estimate ± SE = 4.23 ± 0.23, t(512) = 18.43, p < 0.0001), and hyperactivity (estimate ± SE = 2.43 ± 0.23, t(512) = 10.58, p < 0.0001). Hyperactivity scores were also significantly higher than those in peer problems (estimate ± SE = 1.31 ± 0.23, t(512) = 5.72, p < 0.0001), conduct problems (estimate ± SE = 1.73 ± 0.23, t(512) = 7.53, p < 0.0001) and emotional symptoms (estimate ± SE = 1.80 ± 0.23, t(512) = 7.84, p < 0.0001). The averaged scores over subcategory and age group were significantly higher at T2 compared with T3 (estimate ± SE = 0.16 ± 0.07, t(128) = 2.12, p = 0.36). However, no specific significant differences were found for each subcategory score from T2 to T3. The descriptive statistics for each subcategory at each time point were shown in Table 2 . Changes in children's perceived closeness to others. The IOS scores of child-parent relationships in the primary survey were higher at T1 than T2 for infants (Mean = 5.32, SD = 1.69 at T1; Mean = 4.99, SD = 1.90 The primary purpose of this study was to investigate changes in children's social relationships during and after quarantine using the SDQ and the IOS. Previous studies have shown that children's and adolescents' physical and mental health-related behaviors, such as physical activities, can be affected by sudden changes in their physical and social environment during the COVID-19 pandemic 4,5 . Kindergarten/school closure and the lack of interaction with peers may affect physical activities in children 35 . However, few studies examined the impact of quarantine on children's social relationships with others. Using the SDQ measure, our longitudinal data revealed that quarantine was not related to children's social behavior for all age groups. These results can be interpreted with two possible explanations. First, although school closure may have influenced children's social relationships, it was not severe enough to change children's behavior towards others. Second, the present study might have failed to detect changes in children's social behavior due to methodological factors, as parental report measures could provide only coarse data and were more likely to overlook children's problems than detailed measures 36 . It is also plausible that parents were unable to grasp children's behavioral change precisely because during home confinement, it was relatively difficult to observe children's interaction with peers. Although it is acknowledged that a sophisticated follow-up study is necessary, the findings drawn from this longitudinal data would be rather plausible and convincing as these previous studies used only cross-sectional data. Besides, it may also be the case that children could socially interact with peers even during home confinement, which led to fewer changes in their social behavior, given that children spent more time playing outside and using screens at T1 compared with T2. This may be due to that the national lock down was not introduced in Japan in contrast with other countries, where children had higher socio-emotional problems during the pandemic compared to before 21, 22 . Such changes in their social lives may have compensated for the quantity and quality of social relationships that can be maintained in formal schooling. However, the amount of time when they did not go to school was considerably huge (several days per week), while the increased time of playing outside and using screens was still less (several hours per week). Hence, children's social lives must have been affected by daycare, kindergarten, and school closure at least to some extent. Using the IOS measure, elementary school children's perceived proximity to parents was found to have significantly decreased from during to after school closure (i.e. between T1 and T2), while that to others, such as peers, showed the opposite pattern. These results suggest that, as posited, the pandemic-led school closure and home confinement may have been related to the psychological distance that children at school age felt toward others. Entry into formal schooling largely affects children's peer experiences 37 , and children spend more time with their classmates (and teachers) during the day than their family members. School closure directly transformed their usual style of social contact, which would have led to changes in their feelings toward others. Meanwhile, once schools reopened, children's perceived mental closeness seemed to have recovered to some extent, although this is not conclusive because this study does not have data on the same participants' IOS scores before the pandemic. If this was the case, perceived social relationships during middle childhood would be subject to not only vulnerability but also a certain amount of resilience. The results from the follow-up survey support this possibility to some extent as the IOS scores did not differ between after quarantine and 6 months after the reopening of kindergartens and schools (i.e., T2 to T3). Thus, it would be plausible to think that kindergarten/school closure significantly influenced the way children perceived closeness to others, rather than to think that tense critical situations such as the declaration of a state of emergency itself affected it. Unlike school-age children, this study did not find significant changes in infants' and preschoolers' perceived closeness to both parents and others, although the overall trends were similar to those of older participants. Compatible with the study's prediction, social quarantine was less related to these children's social relationships. Parent-child interactions play an important role in children's social development [10] [11] [12] ; hence, for younger children, it is likely that perceived proximity remains somewhat robust, as long as there are no circumstances that would significantly alter ways of parent-child relationships (e.g., parent-child permanent separation). Meanwhile, when family coexistence was challenged during quarantine, problems pertaining to children became more serious 8 . The present study has theoretical and practical implications for children's social development. Theoretically, our results showed that changes in the social environment can affect children's social relationships measured by IOS. However, the Japanese children's social behaviors measured by SDQ were not related to the kindergarten and school closure led by COVID-19 pandemic, which is inconsistent with the previous studies about children's socio-emoitonal behaviors in countries with the national lock down 21, 22 . Taken together, the strict lockdown, but not school closure, can affect children's social behaviors, perhaps because parents may have felt strong stress under the national lockdown, which may lead to children's social behaviors. Moreover, our results were also inconsitent the Great East Japan Earthquake, tsunami, and radiation disaster 23, 24 . The inconsistency may be because the earthquake could have been more stressful than COVID-19 pandemic for children. Children under the earthquake may have experienced the loss of their family or friends. Such stressful events can have stronger impacts on children's social behaviors. Practically, at least from the results of this study, it could be said that parents do not have to seriously worry about children's social development in terms of intense quarantine. However, previous studies showed that parents' mental problems can affect children's mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic 38 . Moreover, it is possible that children in an adverse situation (e.g., low-socioeconomic status family) may have suffered more problems than other children during the COVID-19 pandemic. We need to examine the issues in future studies. This study has several limitations. First, the exact perceived proximity to peers is unclear since the question was asked in reference to non-family members, with peers raised just as an example. Second, it is possible that the data collected in this study were biased or distorted because parents, instead of children, answered the questionnaire indirectly. Specifically, the IOS is generally used to evaluate participants' own psychological closeness to others and parental report based on their observations on their children's behavior is not the conventional way www.nature.com/scientificreports/ of using IOS 27, 28 . We adopted parents as informers because our target participants included infants. In future studies, we need to devise a methodology, such as observing infants' and children's own behaviors online, to evaluate their psychological closeness to others. Additionally, our data might have been possibly biased as we collected the data via a handy resource of an online survey. Third, although only limited effects of quarantine were found on children's social relationships, the extent to which the findings can be generalized to other countries is not clear. The characteristics of quarantine may have differed across countries. When faced with the COVID-19 pandemic, several countries such as China, UK, India, Spain, and others imposed strict lockdown, but in Japan, activity restrictions were imposed in the form of self-restraints rather than a lockdown. In other words, Japanese people had more mobility and freedom compared to people in other countries where people's actions were strictly regulated. Thus, our results may not be generalized to children in different situations. Fourth, the effect of non-face-to-face communication (e.g., online live interactions) was only partially explored. Even during intensive quarantine, children's perceived proximity to others may have been improved by enhancing online live interactions with peers. These limitations should be addressed in future studies. Nevertheless, the current study could contribute to a better understanding of how physical and social quarantine from others affects children's social bonding and could possibly inform better policy-making. In middle childhood, when peer relationships are intimately formed, quarantine is a factor that psychologically distanced children from others. Although the present study did not observe the actual differences in children's social behavior during and after quarantine, using the IOS measure it was detected that children's social development can potentially be altered. Hence, the change in children's perceived proximity to others can negatively influence children's social development and well-being, even if not serious enough to increase problematic behavior. Measures that enable children to feel closer to others are necessary (e.g., online environment, safe in-person meeting) to enable or even promote children's healthy development during this difficult time. The datasets and codes used in this study are available in the GitHub repository: https:// github. com/ hagi-hara/ COVID-19_ onlin esurv ey. Received: 10 August 2021; Accepted: 4 January 2022 Age-related morbidity and mortality among patients with COVID-19 COVID-19 in children: Analysis of the first pandemic peak in England Clinical characteristics of a case series of children with coronavirus disease 2019 Potential health-related behaviors for pre-school and school-aged children during COVID-19 lockdown: A narrative review Health-related behaviors among school-aged children and adolescents during the Spanish Covid-19 confinement. Front. Pediatr. 8, 573 UNESCO rallies international organizations, civil society and private sector partners in a broad coalition to ensure #LearningNeverStops Vulnerability and resilience in children during the COVID-19 pandemic Immediate psychological effects of the COVID-19 quarantine in youth from Italy and Spain Implications of COVID-19-induced nationwide lockdown on children's behaviour in Punjab, India. Child Care Health Dev Attachment security and adjustment to school in middle childhood The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview Parents of aggressive and withdrawn children in Handbook of Parenting Children's friendship relations: A meta-analytic review Friendship quality as a predictor of young children's early school adjustment Mitigate the effects of home confinement on children during the COVID-19 outbreak The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note Scale properties of the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): A study of infant and school children in community samples The prevalence of behavioral problems among school-aged children in home quarantine during the COVID-19 pandemic in China Immediate impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the socio-emotional and digital skills of Japanese children Testing the effects of COVID-19 confinement in Spanish children: The role of parents' distress, emotional problems and specific parenting Young people's mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic Risk of behavioural and emotional disorders in lithuanian school aged children as assessed with SDQ during the second lockdown due to COVID-19 Assessing the mental health impact of the 2011 great Japan earthquake, tsunami, and radiation disaster on elementary and middle school children in the Fukushima Prefecture of Japan Prosocial behaviors during school activities among child survivors after the 2011 earthquake and tsunami in Japan: A retrospective observational study Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness Sharing distress increases helping and contact intentions via social identification and inclusion of the other in the self: Children's prosocial behaviour after an earthquake Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness Measuring the closeness of relationships: A comprehensive evaluation of the "inclusion of the other in the self " scale Nihon kokunai no kansenshasu (NHK matome) [The number of people infected in Japan Shingata korona uirusu kansensho Taisaku no tameno gakko ni okeru rinjikyugyo no jisshijokyo ni tsuite Shingata korona uirusu kansensho ni kansuru gakko no saikaijokyo ni tsuite [Status of the re-opening of schools in response to COVID-19 infections Japan Broadcasting Corporation (NHK) ImerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 Cardiorespiratory fitness in adolescents before and after the COVID-19 confinement: A prospective cohort study The genetic relationship between social cognition and conduct problems Peer relationships and developmental psychopathology Caregivers' mental distress and child health during the COVID-19 outbreak in Japan Labour and Welfare. Visualizing the data: Information on COVID-19 infections Y.M. developed the study concept. All authors contributed to the study design. Funds for a grant from JSPS (18H01083), a grant for research related to the COVID-19 by the Japanese Psychological Association, and a grant from Maekawa Foundation. The authors declare no competing interests. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to Y.M.Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.