key: cord-0831094-bottaq7z authors: Leibovitz, Talia; Shamblaw, Amanda L.; Rumas, Rachel; Best, Michael W. title: COVID-19 Conspiracy Beliefs: Relations with Anxiety, Quality of Life, and Schemas date: 2021-01-28 journal: Pers Individ Dif DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110704 sha: 053bae534259b856e590da354d1343e96f6706cd doc_id: 831094 cord_uid: bottaq7z Background The COVID-19 pandemic has produced a worldwide mental health crisis. Conspiracy beliefs regarding the origin of COVID-19 are prevalent, however, mental health consequences and factors associated with the likelihood of endorsing COVID-19 conspiracy theories have not yet been examined. The current study examined predictors and mental health consequences of conspiracy beliefs. Methods Participants in Canada and the United States were surveyed via Amazon Mechanical Turk in April 2020 (N= 797), approximately one month after the WHO declared COVID-19 a pandemic, and again in May 2020 (N= 395). Results Approximately half of the sample (49.7%) believed at least one conspiracy theory. Greater Covid-19 conspiracy beliefs were associated with more anxiety at follow up but not quality of life. Religiosity/spirituality, not knowing someone at high-risk for COVID-19, and non-white ethnicity were associated with greater conspiracy beliefs. Lower positive other-schemas were associated with greater conspiracy beliefs, only at low and moderate levels of positive self-schemas. Conclusions There is substantial conspiracy belief endorsement during the COVID-19 pandemic and conspiracy beliefs are associated with anxiety, but not quality of life. Positive self-schemas protect against believing conspiracy theories and interventions to increase positive self-schemas may be effective to reduce the negative effects of conspiracy beliefs. . While conspiracy theories are not necessarily incorrect, they are typically not evidence-based and are resistant to contradictory evidence (Wood et al., 2012) . Conspiracy theories often develop when individuals are experiencing an existential threat (van Prooijen, 2019; van Prooijen & Douglas, 2017) and in situations characterized by increased uncertainty, anxiety, and perceived lack of control (Bruder et al., 2013; Grzesiak-Feldman, 2013; van Prooijen & Acker, 2015; van Prooijen & Jostmann, 2013; Whitson et al., 2015) . Although conspiracy beliefs often develop as a method of increasing a sense of control and certainty (Douglas et al., 2017) , it is also theorized that any benefits to conspiracy thinking are likely to be short-lived with longer-term negative effects (Freeman & Bentall, 2017) . Conspiracy thinking has been associated with negative emotions (Freeman & Bentall, 2017) , increased social isolation (Freeman & Bentall, 2017) , and anxiety (van Prooijen & van Vugt, 2018; van Prooijen & Douglas, 2017) . The COVID-19 pandemic has created a global context that is ideal for the development of conspiracy theories. Uncertainty regarding the severity and duration of the pandemic, sustained threat with little control over the outcome, and governments enforcing widespread restrictions make it unsurprising that conspiracy theories about COVID-19 have circulated. Reports of COVID-19 conspiracy belief prevalence have been mixed early in the pandemic with some studies suggesting that approximately 50% of the population believe conspiracy theories (Freeman et al., 2020) , while others have suggested the true prevalence is likely less than 25% (McManus et al., 2020; Sutton & Douglas, 2020) . However, most of these studies have been conducted in the United Kingdom. Most of the research on conspiracy beliefs during the pandemic has focused on the effect that conspiracy beliefs have on compliance with governmental policies to reduce the spread of COVID-19, however, preliminary cross-sectional evidence has suggested that conspiracy beliefs also have a personal impact on the individual holding the belief and is associated with greater mental distress and anxiety (Chen et al., 2020) . Quality of life is an indicator of a person"s satisfaction with their everyday life that has not yet been examined with relation to COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. Although conspiracy beliefs appear to be prominent during the pandemic, few investigations have examined who is most likely to believe COVID-19 conspiracy theories. A report from the United Kingdom suggested that younger participants, participants of non-white ethnicity, and participants with less education were more likely to believe COVID-19 conspiracy theories (Freeman et al., 2020) . A report using a global sample, found similarly more conspiracy beliefs among individuals with lower education, and also reported fewer conspiracy beliefs in countries that had been most substantially affected by COVID-19 (Georgiou et al., 2020) . In addition to demographic factors, psychological models of conspiracy theories emphasize social cognitive factors in the development of conspiracy beliefs (Douglas et al., 2017) . Schemas are social cognitive constructs, often representing views of the self or other people, that influence how information is processed. Individuals who find their positive self-schemas threatened are more likely to endorse conspiracy beliefs (Cichocka et al., 2016) , as are individuals who feel that they have been victimized (Bilewicz et al., 2013) . Thus, conspiracy beliefs may serve as a defensive response when positive self-schemas are threatened. Additionally, conspiracy beliefs have been linked to negative views of other people (Imhoff & Bruder, 2014; Kofta & Sedek, 2005) , suggesting that negative schemas about others likely contribute to conspiracy beliefs. Despite theories that conspiracy beliefs develop when individuals hold negative views of both J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Journal Pre-proof themselves and other people (Cichocka et al., 2016) , the interaction between self-and otherschemas has never been examined. Additionally, the role of schemas in COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs have not yet been examined, despite presenting a potential point of intervention. The current study had three primary aims. First, we examined the prevalence of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs in a North American sample and longitudinal stability of conspiracy beliefs. Second, we examined the personal consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs on anxiety and quality of life. Lastly, we examined demographic and cognitive factors associated with greater likelihood of believing COVID-19 conspiracy theories. 2. Method One-thousand participants were recruited via Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), which has been validated for psychological research (Clifford et al., 2015) and results in samples that are more representative of community demographics than other sampling approaches (Cheung et al., 2017) . Participants could access the survey if they had a Canadian or U.S.-based Internet protocol (IP) address and a 99% approval rating for prior MTurk tasks. Based on past research, it was expected that 20-25% of responses would be excluded due to insufficient effort on effort testing questions. An attrition rate of 50% was also expected between the baseline and followup surveys based on previous MTurk studies. Therefore, a baseline sample of 750 to 800 participants (after excluding participants with low effort) and an overall longitudinal sample of approximately 400 participants were expected, which is sufficient to detect small to moderate moderation effects based on simulation studies (Fritz & MacKinnon, 2007) . The baseline survey was administered via MTurk between April 21 st and 25 th , 2020, approximately one month after the WHO declared COVID-19 a pandemic (WHO, 2020) and the follow up survey was administered one month later between May 21 st and 27 th , 2020. Participants were compensated with $2 (USD) per survey and an additional $2 (USD) if participants passed the effort checks on both baseline and follow up surveys. Each participant gave written informed consent and all procedures were approved by the University of Toronto Research Ethics Board. In order to control for exposure to the pandemic, a proxy variable of COVID-19 density was calculated as the number of confirmed COVID-19 cases per 1 million population. Density was calculated for each state (US) and province/territory (Canada) on April 23, 2020 (mid-point of the baseline survey). Participants were presented with a list of 30 common physical health diagnoses and 6 common mental health diagnoses and asked to indicate whether they had been diagnosed with any of the conditions. Participants who indicated they had been diagnosed with two or more conditions in either category were considered at "high risk" for either physical health or mental health consequences from the pandemic. The same questions were asked regarding whether participants had a friend or family member who had been diagnosed with the listed conditions. In order to examine rates of conspiracy beliefs specifically about the cause of COVID-19, we developed a series of questions relating to commonly held COVID-19 conspiracy theories that were circulating at the beginning of the pandemic (April 2020). This self-report measure consists of 8 items (Table 1) . Each item states a belief regarding the COVID-19 pandemic that was circulating on social media platforms at the time of the survey. Items are rated on a 5-point bipolar rating scale ranging from "0" (Disagree) to "4" (Agree). Items were examined individually and not combined into a total score, thus no measure of internal consistency was calculated. ' 'somewhat disagree,' or 'neutral' to the Conspiracy Belief item. Group means and standard deviations are presented for both the GAD-7 and WHOQOL-BREF. GAD-7= Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 Item Scale; WHO-QOL-BREF= World Health Organization Quality of Life -BREF. *p< .006 (family wise correction), **p<.001 (Wood, 2016 ) is a 5-item self-report measure assessing conspiracy beliefs around any specific topic of interest. For the WHO-QOL-BREF COVID-19: Agree n (%) Agree M (SD) Disagree M (SD) t-stat Agree M (SD) Disagree M (SD) t-stat 1. Journal Pre-proof current study, questions were phrased with reference to the COVID-19 pandemic. The FICS was included as a validated measure that could be applied to COVID-19, however, the flexibility of the scale means that the items are less specifically related to popular COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale from "1" (strongly disagree) to "5" (strongly agree) with higher scores indicating greater conspiracy beliefs surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic. Internal consistency was α = 0.91 at baseline and α = 0.92 at follow-up. The GCB-15 (Brotherton et al., 2013 ) is a 15-item self-report measure assessing general conspiracy beliefs. The GCB-15 was included to measure individuals' general tendency to endorse conspiracy theories. Items are rated on a 5point Likert-type scale with responses ranging from "1" (definitely not true) to "5" (definitely true), with higher scores indicating greater conspiracy beliefs. Internal consistency was α = .94 at baseline and α = .95 at follow-up. The WHOQOL-BREF (WHOQOL Group, 1998) is a 26-item self-report measure assessing quality of life across four domains: physical health (7 items), psychological (6 items), social relationships (3 items), and environment (8 items). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale with higher scores indicating higher quality of life. Items within each domain are averaged and the mean score is multiplied by 4 so that domain scores have a maximum score of 20. A total score is created by summing the domain scores. Internal consistency ranged from α = .91 to α = .93. The GAD-7 (Spitzer et al., 2006 ) is a 7-item selfreport measure assessing symptoms of generalized anxiety. Items are rated on a 4-point Likerttype scale ranging from "0" (not at all) to "3" (nearly every day) with higher scores indicating greater anxiety. Internal consistency was α = .92 at baseline and α = .93 at follow-up. The BCSS (Fowler et al., 2006 ) is a 24-item self-report measure assessing core schemas about the self and others and includes four domains: negative beliefs about the self, positive beliefs about the self, negative beliefs about others, and positive beliefs about others. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale from "0" (do not believe it) to "4" (believe it totally), with higher scores indicating stronger beliefs. Internal consistency for the four scales ranged from α = .89 to .95 at baseline and α = .89 to .96 at follow-up. Missing data within scales were interpolated using participant mean replacement. Prevalence of the COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs at baseline were calculated as the percentage of participants who agreed with the belief (responses "agree" or "somewhat agree"). Stability of conspiracy belief endorsement across time points was examined in the completers sample with a McNemar test. Anxiety and QOL were compared between participants who endorsed each COVID-19 conspiracy belief and those who did not endorse the belief using t-tests. To investigate whether conspiracy beliefs are associated with QOL, a hierarchical linear regression was conducted at baseline. Age, Covid-19 density, and gender were entered in the first step to control for differences by age, gender, and exposure to COVID-19; the FICS and GCB were entered in the second step to examine the effect of conspiracy beliefs on QOL. This same analysis was conducted with baseline variables predicting QOL at follow-up to assess longitudinal J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Journal Pre-proof associations. To examine the direction of the relation between conspiracy beliefs and anxiety, a cross-lagged panel analysis was conducted with correlation coefficients presented for each relationship. Relations between specific conspiracy beliefs on the FICS and general conspiracy beliefs on the GCB were examined with demographic characteristics using Pearson correlations and t-tests. To examine the relation between positive and negative self-and other-schemas with COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, moderated regression analyses were conducted with schemas at baseline predicting FICS at both baseline and follow-up. Positive and negative schemas were examined independently in two separate regression analyses. The first analysis examined positive otherschemas as the independent variable (IV) and positive self-schemas as the moderator. The second analysis examined negative other-schemas as the IV and negative self-schemas as the moderator. 3. Results One-thousand participants completed the baseline survey. 203 (20.3%) participants were excluded for answering more than one effort question incorrectly, resulting in a sample size of 797. Of these, 408 participants (51%) completed the survey again one month later. At followup, 13 individuals (3%) were excluded for incorrectly answering more than one effort question, resulting in a follow-up sample of 395. Two participants were missing data for baseline WHO-QOL-BREF and two participants did not indicate state of residence required to calculate COVID-19 density. Participants were retained in analyses for which they had complete data for. A participant flow diagram is presented in Figure 1 and demographic characteristics of the baseline and longitudinal sample are presented in Table 2 . Table 3 . Conspiracy belief endorsement was stable across the one-month follow-up period with none of the COVID-19 beliefs demonstrating any significant change from baseline to follow-up (Table 3) . Additionally, total score on the FICS was stable from baseline (M = 13.02, SD = 5.34) to follow-up (M = 12.93, SD = 5.53), t(394) = .50, p = .619. At baseline, the GCB and FICS demonstrated a high correlation, r(795) = .61, p < .001, suggesting that both measure conspiracy beliefs but that generic and COVID-19-specific beliefs also have distinguishable variance. Stability of COVID-19 Beliefs from Baseline to Follow-up Note. Agree includes responses "agree" and "somewhat agree" to the item. None of the specific COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs were associated with QOL (Table 1) . After controlling for age, gender, and density of COVID-19 cases, GCB was associated with QOL at baseline, ß = -.12, t = -.273, p = .007 however, FICS was not, ß = -.03, t = -0.57, p = .570. In the longitudinal model, neither GCB, ß = -.05, t = -1.15, p = .249, nor FICS, ß = -.02, t = -0.51, p = .609, at baseline were associated with QOL at follow-up. More severe anxiety symptoms were associated with believing that COVID-19 is a message from God, and a conspiracy (Table 1) . A cross-lagged panel analysis is presented in Figure 2 . GAD-7 and FICS were correlated with each other at baseline but not at follow-up. Additionally, baseline FICS was associated with GAD-7 at follow-up but not vice-versa, suggesting a directional relationship such that greater conspiracy belief endorsement leads to more severe anxiety. Note. Cross-lagged panel analysis of GAD-7 and FICS at baseline and one-month follow-up. Pearson correlation coefficients are presented. GAD-7= Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 Item Scale; FICS= Flexible Inventory of Conspiracy Suspicions. *p<.05, **p<.01 Age was not significantly correlated with FICS, r(795) = -.04, p = .274, but did have a significant negative correlation with GCB, r(793) = -.08, p = -.021, such that older age was associated with less endorsement of general conspiracy beliefs. Importance of religion/spirituality had a significant positive correlation with both FICS, r(795) = .36, p < .001, and GCB, r(793) = .18, p < .001, such that greater religiosity/spirituality was associated with greater endorsement of both general and COVID-specific conspiracy beliefs. There was also a significant difference in FICS score, t(795) = 5.46, p < .001, and GCB score, t(793) = 4.23, p < .001, between individuals who The present study examined prevalence of conspiracy beliefs relating to COVID-19, mental health consequences, and factors associated with believing conspiracy theories. This is the first study that we know of to examine this question in a North American sample, and to examine COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs longitudinally. Approximately half (49.7%) of the current sample believed at least one COVID-19 conspiracy theory, which is consistent with other reports during the COVID-19 pandemic (Freeman et al., 2020) . Sutton & Douglas (2020) criticized the use of a unipolar scale by Freeman et al. (2020) and found that when a bipolar scale was used with equal numbers of positive and negative response options, estimates of conspiracy belief endorsement were similar to that reported prior to the pandemic (Freeman & Bentall, 2017) . Despite using bipolar scaling, we found comparable rates of conspiracy belief endorsement to Freeman et al., (2020) . The time at which the study was conducted may have determined the extent of conspiracy belief endorsement. Our study (baseline late April 2020) was conducted at a similar time as Freeman et al., (2020) (May 2020) whereas Sutton & Douglas (2020) collected data in late June. It is possible that endorsement was greatest early in the pandemic and has since decreased. Although, our longitudinal data suggests that conspiracy belief endorsement was stable over the month of May 2020, it is possible that longer term follow-up might reveal a decreasing trend for conspiracy beliefs. The current results also provide evidence for negative mental health consequences associated with conspiracy beliefs. Conspiracy beliefs at baseline were associated with anxiety at follow-up, but not vice-versa, suggesting that believing conspiracy theories may lead to increased anxiety. This is consistent with previous ideas that although conspiracy beliefs may develop as a mechanism to reduce anxiety about the unknown, the content of the conspiracy belief itself may increase anxiety (Douglas et al., 2017 ). It appears that conspiracy theories surrounding COVID-19 may be exacerbating the anxiety that a pandemic is naturally expected to elicit (Holmes et al., 2020) . The substantial proportion of the population endorsing conspiracy beliefs (~50%) is also concerning, given the effects that conspiracy beliefs have on mental health. Future research could examine methods to effectively reduce conspiracy beliefs at both the societal and individual levels. COVID-19 specific conspiracy beliefs did not predict poorer QOL during the pandemic. General conspiracy beliefs were associated with QOL at baseline (but not longitudinally), suggesting a relationship between the general propensity to endorse conspiracy beliefs and QOL, however, the direction of this relationship remains unclear. The lack of relationship between COVID-19 specific conspiracy beliefs and QOL, may suggest that while believing COVID-19 conspiracy theories increases anxiety, it does not translate to changes in QOL. However, future studies should examine this relationship with a longer-term follow-up. Individuals who reported greater religiosity/spirituality were more likely to endorse conspiracy beliefs, as were individuals who were of a non-white ethnicity. Future research should examine cultural factors associated with believing conspiracy theories. Individuals were also more likely to endorse COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs if they did not know anyone who was at high-risk for health complications from COVID-19. Knowing someone at high-risk may make COVID-19 a more legitimate threat, reducing the likelihood of believing conspiracy theories. From a cognitive perspective, the current results suggest that individuals are most likely to believe conspiracy theories when they hold negative schemas about both themselves and others. Positive self-schemas appear to mitigate the effects of schemas about other people, thus, one J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f therapeutic approach to working with people who hold distressing conspiracy beliefs may be to develop stronger positive self-schemas. Numerous therapeutic approaches, such as cognitivebehavioural therapy and compassion focused therapy, can effectively strengthen positive selfschemas and may be worth considering as individual intervention options. The current results should be interpreted with consideration of several limitations. Due to the requirement that participants be proficient in the use of technology to access the MTurk platform, the results may not generalize to people who are not technologically adept. However, several studies have also suggested that MTurk produces samples that are more representative of the general population than other comparable sampling approaches (Clifford et al., 2015; Cheung et al., 2017) , thus it may be a reasonable representation of the population. The online nature of this survey limited the types of measures that could be used to those that are self-reported, which are vulnerable to social desirability biases, demand characteristics, and effort. However, we used a rigorous approach of embedded effort-testing questions to ensure that participants were actively attending to questions. Additionally, the longitudinal nature of this study is a significant strength, however, the 1-month follow-up period may not have been long enough to detect substantial changes in the examined variables. Studies with longer follow-up may be necessary to appropriately characterize change in these factors during the pandemic. The current study found high rates of COVID-19 conspiracy belief endorsement in a North American sample. Conspiracy beliefs were associated with anxiety and greater endorsement of conspiracy beliefs at baseline were associated with greater anxiety at follow-up. Negative selfand other-schemas were associated with more conspiracy beliefs, and positive self-schemas were protective against believing conspiracy theories. Thus, conspiracy theories are prevalent during the COVID-19 pandemic and are associated with reduced well-being. Societal and individual methods of reducing belief in conspiracy theories may be helpful to improve wellbeing during the pandemic. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Journal Pre-proof Harmful ideas: The structure and consequences of anti-semitic beliefs in Poland Measuring Belief in Conspiracy Theories: The Generic Conspiracist Beliefs Scale Belief in a COVID-19 Conspiracy Theory as a Predictor of Mental Health and Well-Being of Health Care Workers in Ecuador: Cross-Sectional Survey Study Amazon Mechanical Turk in Organizational Psychology: An Evaluation and Practical Recommendations Does Self-Love or Self-Hate Predict Conspiracy Beliefs? Narcissism, Self-Esteem, and the Endorsement of Conspiracy Theories Are samples drawn from Mechanical Turk valid for research on political ideology? Research & Politics The Psychology of Conspiracy Theories The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS): Psychometric properties and associations with paranoia and grandiosity in nonclinical and psychosis samples The concomitants of conspiracy concerns Coronavirus Conspiracy Beliefs, Mistrust, and Compliance with Government Guidelines in England Required sample size to detect the mediated effect COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with perceived stress and pre-existing conspiracy beliefs Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalised political attitude Conspiracy stereotypes of Jews during systemic transformation in Poland Addicted to answers: Need for cognitive closure and the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs Covid conspiracies: misleading evidence can be more damaging than no evidence at all A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7 Agreeing to disagree: reports of the popularity of Covid-19 conspiracy theories are greatly exaggerated Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment Conspiracy suspicions as a proxy for beliefs in conspiracy theories: Implications for theory and measurement Dead and Alive: Beliefs in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories Timeline of WHO's response to COVID-19 TL, ALS, and MWB designed the study. RR was responsible for data collection. TL and MWB analyzed the data. TL wrote the first draft, while ALS, RR, and MWB provided comments. All authors approved the final draft of the manuscript.