key: cord-0823012-kvxs7glz authors: Valtorta, Roberta Rosa; Baldissarri, Cristina; Volpato, Chiara; Andrighetto, Luca title: Intergroup biologization and outgroup prejudice in the time of COVID‐19 date: 2021-08-30 journal: J Appl Soc Psychol DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12831 sha: 661cb6580618af7e3677efdcc14e59b93b7ef1be doc_id: 823012 cord_uid: kvxs7glz Through two studies (N = 602) conducted in Italy between February and March 2020, we examined the impact of the COVID‐19 emergency on biologization—a form of dehumanization that involves the perception of others as contagious entities—and outgroup prejudice. Overall, results showed that higher emergency perception was associated with greater biologization toward the groups most affected by the virus, namely the Chinese outgroup and the Italian ingroup. In turn, biologization toward the outgroup increased prejudice against that group. We also found that when the pandemic hit Italy, the greater emergency perception was associated with increased emotional closeness with Chinese people, resulting in reduced biologization and prejudice toward them. However, these results held true only for Italian respondents who reported higher levels of ingroup biologization. Taken together, our findings contribute to the knowledge gaps of biologization and prejudice by also providing relevant insights into the ongoing health emergency. In line with the above evidence, research highlights the key role of pathogen-avoidant responses and disease rhetoric in attitudes formation and in the stigmatization of those who appear diseased (Crandall & Moriarty, 1995; Schaller & Neuberg, 2012) . In addition, by integrating evolutionary theory and metaphorical language's effects, Brown and colleagues (2019) demonstrated how pathogen-avoidant motives interact with figurative language to influence attitudes. More specifically, they found that the use of disease metaphors increases negative perceptions and anti-immigration sentiments, especially among individuals with high levels of pathogen-avoidance motivations. Starting from these considerations, the present research aimed to extend the literature in this field by demonstrating that pathogen stress may elicit outgroup prejudice through specific dehumanizing perceptions. Indeed, as stated by Markel (1999) , the threat of infectious diseases plays a crucial role in eliciting negative evaluations of others as a result of dehumanization against them. According to the author, during quarantine, the human beings who have been affected by the virus become the "enemy" in the same way as the virus itself. In this respect, the scholar argued that a common symptom of the quarantine mentality is to do everything possible to prevent the spread of an epidemic, often denying humanity to those who have encountered the disease and expressing negative feelings toward the potential sources of contagion. Despite the relevance of this topic, to date, research has not provided clear evidence of the relationship between pathogen threat and dehumanizing representations. As a matter of fact, previous studies have mainly focused on the association between contagious diseases and prejudiced attitudes (e.g., Faulkner et al., 2004; Ji et al., 2019) . To address this gap, through two studies conducted among Italian citizens in February 2020-when the COVID-19 pandemic was not widespread in Italy-and in March 2020-when the pandemic had already broken out in this country-we intended to investigate the impact of the ongoing epidemiological situation caused by the novel coronavirus pandemic on outgroup prejudice via biologization, namely a dehumanizing process that involves the perception of individuals or groups as disease organisms (Douglas, 1966; Sontag, 2002; Volpato & Andrighetto, 2015) . Biologization is a form of dehumanization that employs metaphors linked to disease and has been theoretically examined within conflicting intergroup relations (Savage, 2007) . In particular, several authors (e.g., Douglas, 1966; Hirsch & Smith, 1991; Sontag, 2002) have revealed that biological rhetoric has been widely used in the political domain and in relation to aggressive episodes. For example, along with all the other dehumanizing representations, Hitler's Mein Kampf also included the conceptualization of Jews as harmful bacilli (Musolff, 2007) . In a different context, Steuter and Wills (2010) analyzed the language used by Western mass media and revealed that biological metaphors such as cancer, metastasis, or viruses are often adopted for describing terrorist enemies. More recently, Moullagaliev and Khismatullina (2017) found that many conventional metaphors deployed in media discourse on migration derive from the area of "diagnosis of disease", which frames outgroup members as dangerous health threats. Crucially for the main goal of the present research, some studies have emphasized the key role of the salience of contagious diseases in people's dehumanizing perceptions of others. For instance, Dalsklev and Kunst (2015) showed that exposing participants to a text focusing on the allegedly low hygienic standards of a minority group and their potential pathogen threat increased feelings of disgust and dehumanization, which, in turn, led to higher support of deportation. Importantly, Lawson et al. (2008) found that participants exposed to an editorial cartoon in which outgroup members were associated with disease subsequently dehumanized them more than participants who saw the same cartoon without reference to disease. Further, Valtorta, Baldissarri and colleagues (2019) focused on biologization in the work domain and found that the salience of dirty and unhealthy work environments characterizing certain lowstatus occupations (i.e., garbage collectors and janitors) increased the association of these workers with biological metaphors. Overall, this literature suggests that biological dehumanization is triggered by categories of people who are perceived as more likely to carry pathogens, which pose an acute threat to well-being or who are associated with specific types of infectious diseases. In other words, as reported by several authors (e.g., Dalsklev & Kunst, 2015; Esses et al., 2013) , seeing individuals or groups as potential sources of disease transmission might be likely to lead to their dehumanization. Building from these arguments, we assumed that the groups most affected by the novel coronavirus would be associated with an increase in dehumanizing perceptions in terms of biologization. More specifically, by integrating the abovementioned research on biological dehumanization (e.g., Savage, 2007; Valtorta, Baldissarri, et al., 2019) with that concerning the behavioral immune system theory (e.g., Murray & Schaller, 2016; Schaller & Park, 2011) , we hypothesized that the perception of COVID-19 emergency might lead to biologization toward the groups most afflicted by the virus. In addition, we assumed that biological dehumanization due to the health emergency should, in turn, be associated with increased prejudice. In the following section, we provide the rationale for our hypotheses. A large number of studies (e.g., Dixon & Levine, 2012; Wilde et al., 2014) has shown that dehumanization and traditional prejudice are two different processes and dehumanizing an outgroup paves the way for negative treatments and prejudice toward that group (e.g., Costello & Hodson, 2010; Goff et al., 2008; Hodson & Costello, 2007) . For example, Greenhalgh and Watt (2015) showed that Australians' perception of value dissimilarity with refugees and asylum seekers was associated with greater prejudice toward them, and that this effect was mediated by dehumanizing perceptions. These findings echo those of Esses and colleagues (2008) , who found that individuals who are higher in social dominance orientation, an ideology involving preferences for social hierarchy, were especially likely to dehumanize refugees by perceiving them as less characterized by human qualities of morality. As a result, they expressed more negative emotions and prejudice toward refugees. Thus, the authors demonstrated that once the process of dehumanization has begun, whether because of pre-existing levels of social dominance orientation or due to information presented about a group, overall negative attitudes, prejudice, and a desire to exclude the group are likely to emerge. Furthermore, Costello and Hodson (2014) examined laypeople's beliefs about the causes of and solutions to outgroup dehumanization and prejudice by concluding that dehumanization can be considered an important but largely unrecognized prejudice precursor. Congruent with this previous research, we assumed that the emergency perception due to the COVID-19 contagion would affect outgroup prejudice through increased dehumanizing representations. Importantly, as we will discuss below, we also assumed that the current COVID-19 emergency would be associated with greater emotional closeness among the groups most impacted by the epidemic. In turn, such increased closeness should be associated with a decrease in the considered negative intergroup evaluations (i.e., biologization and prejudice). A growing body of research (e.g., Andrighetto et al., 2016; Eranen & Liebkind, 1993; Kaniasty & Norris, 2004) has demonstrated that peculiar circumstances, such as disaster exposure and human tragedies, may enhance prosocial attitudes and behaviors. In this regard, some scholars have written about the emergence of an "altruistic community" in the aftermath of hurricanes, floods, or earthquakes, characterized by high levels of solidarity and fellowship. For example, in research conducted in the United States after September 11, 2001, participants who reported higher suffering also reported emotional support, donating, and volunteering more than those who reported less suffering (Schuster et al., 2001) . According to these results, Staub (2003 Staub ( , 2005 introduced the expression "altruism born of suffering" to indicate that individuals' sufferings may enhance positive attitudes and motivation to help others. Several authors (e.g., Aron et al., 1992; Jacob et al., 2008; Vollhardt, 2009 ) argued that the "altruism born of suffering" could be explained by the fact that experiencing natural disasters may create the perception of a shared fate among individuals who suffered, or may change the relations with other individuals, such that one becomes emotionally closer to these others. Moreover, as reported by Mawson (2007) , acts of solidarity are often accompanied by feelings of closeness with people in the same situation. Relevant to the present research, the scholar argued that during natural disasters or other crises, affiliative behaviors are directed at people who shared the same psychological experiences. Thus, a state of heightened empathy and emotional closeness prevails, exemplified by increased mutual liking and "we-feeling". Consistently, Aron and colleagues (2004) stated that under some conditions such as human tragedies, when someone in the ingroup perceives to have something in common with an outgroup person, the effect is that, to some extent, ingroup members begin to see the outgroup as a part of their own group. Therefore, in these situations, the ingroup-outgroup distinction, vital to produce negative intergroup attitudes, is directly diminished by the outgroup member's connection to an ingroup member. Furthermore, negative attitudes toward the outgroup are reduced and positive attitudes are enhanced by this closeness (and inclusion) of the outgroup with (in) the self. Starting from these considerations, after examining the impact of the current health emergency on biologization and prejudice toward the group who first has encountered the novel coronavirus outbreak, namely Chinese people (Study 1), we aimed to investigate the potential role of emotional closeness in reducing negative perceptions against this outgroup (Study 2). In particular, we assumed that the ongoing COVID-19 emergency in Italy would be associated with greater Italians' emotional closeness with Chinese people. In turn, such increased closeness should be associated with decreased biologization and outgroup prejudice. Importantly, we investigated whether this pattern emerges specifically among Italian participants who reported higher levels of ingroup biologization. Indeed, Bastian and Crimston (2014) have demonstrated that people who experience self-dehumanization in response to exceptional situations are more likely to report positive evaluations and engage in prosocial behaviors. In line with these findings, Bastian and colleagues (2013; see also Jordon et al., 2011; Zhong & Liljenquist, 2006) stated that when people feel that they have lost their humanity, they may be motivated to engage in a positive or prosocial way with others, thereby reconnecting them back into their human community and re-establishing their moral status. Thus, in some circumstances, self-perceiving as losing one's own humanity may be an important step toward ending-rather than perpetuating-the cycle of inhuman behaviors. In this sense, it is plausible to think that experiencing higher ingroup biologization would lead Italian participants to report emotional closeness and positive responses toward Chinese people, in an effort to regain the lost humanity. In addition, it is possible that people who dehumanize Italians and Chinese might perceive these groups as more akin to each other than people who do not dehumanize their ingroup. This similar perception might wash away part of the ingroup-outgroup separation, by thus strengthening the associations among emotional closeness, dehumanization, and prejudice toward Chinese people. Study 1 was conducted in February 2020, immediately after the announcement by the World Health Organization of the novel coronavirus pneumonia of China as a Public Health Emergency of International Concern and just before that this pneumonia massively targeted also the Italian population. This study aimed to assess whether the Italians' emergency perception due to the COVID-19 contagion affected biologization and prejudice toward Chinese people, namely the group who first had to struggle with a massive outbreak of the virus (79,824 confirmed cases and 2,870 deaths as of February 29, 2020). More specifically, we assumed that the peculiar conditions characterizing the novel coronavirus epidemic would affect prejudice toward Chinese people via biological dehumanization. Indeed, as reported above, dehumanization is an extreme response to extraordinary situations that usually facilitates negative judgments and treatments against others. Furthermore, to verify the specificity of this link toward the Chinese outgroup, a comparison target group was included. In particular, we decided to consider North African people because, in our research context (i.e., Italy), this group has usually been subject to harshness and hostility (Caricati et al., 2017; Kirchler & Zani, 1995; . We supposed indeed that the health emergency period for epidemic contagion from COVID-19 would promote prejudice via biological dehumanizing perceptions toward Chinese people but not against a negatively perceived group that is not affected by the epidemic to such a great extent (i.e., North African people; 2 confirmed cases and 0 deaths as of February 29, 2020). 2 We recruited Italian participants using Prolific Academic, which allowed us to obtain data from a heterogeneous sample. Given the correlational nature of our study, we aimed at collecting data on a large sample (i.e., N > 250) that would guarantee the stability of the tested correlations (Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013 ) and a power of 0.80 for correlation as low as 0.17, as determined by a priori power analysis conducted using G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) . Therefore, we considered an initial sample of 326 respondents. In order to obtain a reliable sample of respondents and to identify inattentive respondents, we included two attentional check items in our survey (e.g., "Please answer 3 to this item"; see Oppenheimer et al., 2009 ). Twenty-six participants failed these items and were removed from the analyses. Thus, the final sample was composed of 300 Italian participants (124 females, 173 males; M age = 27.69, SD = 8.48; age range: 18-60). The following scales were used to measure participants' perceptions during the emergency period due to the COVID-19 spread. The order of presentation of the scales was randomly varied. The target groups to whom participants were asked to respond were Chinese people, North African people, and Germans. This last group was included as filler to mask the primary groups of interest: Chinese and North African people. After fulfilling the scales described below, participants were asked to indicate some demographic information about themselves. They were then debriefed and thanked for their participation. Participants rated their emergency perception by answering the question: "What level of emergency would you attribute to the current situation related to coronavirus?" on a scale from 0 (low-level emergency) to 100 (high-level emergency). Biologization was measured using seven disease-related nouns (i.e., To compare the biologization and prejudice score across the considered groups, we performed two repeated measures ANOVAs (group: Chinese people, North African people, Germans). Regarding biological dehumanization, Mauchly's test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ 2 (2) = 28.94, p < .001; therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.92). The results showed a main 2 Confirmed cases and deaths in North Africa were computed by summing the statistics concerning Egypt, Tunisia, Morocco, Libya, and Algeria. 3 To further examine the relationships among the emergency perception due to the COVID-19 disease, biologization, and outgroup prejudice, we measured this latter variable also by using the feeling thermometer (see Nelson, 2008 than Germans, all ps < .001. Furthermore, despite a non-significant difference (p = .156), a trend in the results showed that participants biologized more Chinese than North African people (see Table 1 ). For prejudice, Mauchly's test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ 2 (2) = 21.58, p < .001; therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.94). The analysis showed a main effect of group, F(1.88, 562.34) = 99.34, p < .001, 2 p = 0.25: participants exhibited more prejudice toward Chinese and North African people than Germans, all ps < .001. Furthermore, the prejudice score toward North African people was higher than the score reported against Chinese people, p = .001 (see Table 1 ). Table 1 , emergency perception was positively correlated with biologization toward both our target groups (i.e., Chinese and North African people), whereas it was unrelated to prejudice. Further, the prejudice score was positively associated with biological dehumanization. To verify the prediction that the health emergency period would promote prejudice toward Chinese people (but not against North African people) via biologization, we tested two models using Hayes' (2017) PROCESS macro (Model 4) and the bootstrapping method (5,000 resamples; see Figure 1 ). Table 2 , analyses indicated that health emergency perception was a significant predictor of biologization toward Chinese people. Moreover, biological dehumanization was positively associated with prejudice against this group. The direct effect of health emergency on prejudice was not significant; crucially, the The same model was tested for North African people, and as reported in Table 3 , analyses showed a similar pattern of results. North African people. In turn, biologization was significantly related M age = 28.82, SD = 12.14; age range: 18-62). As in the previous study, the order of presentation of the following scales was randomly varied. Regarding emergency perception, we ad-hoc created a more accurate six-item scale (e.g., "The coronavirus emergency will affect my life also in the future") instead of using the single-item measure employed in Study 1. Participants were asked to express their agreement on a 7point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 7 = extremely). Scores were combined to yield an overall emergency perception score (α = 0.71); higher scores denote greater emergency perception due to the COVID-19 disease. 5 The Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS; Aron et al., 1992) Scale was used to examine whether Italians perceived a higher overlap between their own self and Chinese people rather than between their own self and North African people. More specifically, participants were asked to choose among seven pictures the one that best represented their relationship in terms of emotional closeness with Chinese and North African people. Each image showed two circles (labeled "self" and "Chinese people" in one question, and "self" and "North African people" in the other) with varying degrees of overlap, from non-overlapping (i.e., 1) to almost completely overlapping (i.e., 7). After fulfilling the scales described above, participants were asked to indicate some demographic information about themselves. They were then debriefed and thanked for their participation. To compare the biologization score across the considered groups, we performed a repeated-measures ANOVA (group: Chinese people, North African people, Italians). Mauchly's test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ 2 (2) = 19.97, p < .001; therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh-Feldt estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.95). The results showed a main effect of group, F(1.89, 569.01) = 60.64, p < .001, 2 p = 0.17, indicating that the most biologized group was the ingroup one (i.e., Italians), all ps < .001. Furthermore, Chinese people were more biologized than North African people, p < .001 (see Table 4 ). Two repeated-measures ANOVAs (group: Chinese people, North African people) were then conducted to ascertain whether there were significant differences in prejudice and emotional closeness scores toward the two target groups. As for prejudice, the analysis showed a significant effect of group, F(1, 301) = 7.91, p = .005, 2 p = 0.03, indicating that participants exhibited more prejudice against North African than Chinese people (see Table 4 ). Regarding emotional closeness, in line with our assumption, results showed that Italian participants reported more closeness with Chinese people than with North African people, F(1, 301) = 42.76, p < .001, 2 p = 0.12 (see Table 4 ). As shown in Table 4 , the emergency perception was positively correlated with biologization toward both our target groups (i.e., Italians and Chinese people) but not with biological dehumanization against North African people. Moreover, in line with the results of Study 1, the emergency perception due to the COVID-19 epidemic was unrelated to prejudice. Importantly, Italians' emergency perception was positively associated with emotional closeness with Chinese people. This latter variable was negatively correlated with biologization and prejudice against Chinese people. To examine the role of ingroup biologization and the relationships among emergency perception, emotional closeness, and negative perceptions toward both Chinese and North African people, we tested two conditional process models. As reported in Figure 2 , we considered participants' emergency perception as the predictor variable, emotional closeness as the first-level mediator, biologization as the second-level mediator, and ingroup biologization as the moderator of the relationship between health emergency and emotional closeness. Finally, prejudice was entered as the outcome variable (Model 83 of the PROCESS macro for SPSS with 5,000 bootstrapping samples; Hayes, 2017). Table 5 , for the moderated path from emergency perception to emotional closeness with Chinese people, results showed a non-significant effect of biologization toward Italians. Crucially, emergency perception and the two-way interaction Emergency perception × Biologization toward Italians were positively associated with emotional closeness. As expected, decomposition of this interaction revealed that emergency perception was positively related with emotional closeness with Chinese people for Italian participants who reported higher levels of ingroup biologization, b = 0.44, As for North African people, in line with our expectations, health emergency did not predict emotional closeness (b = 0.12, p = .189) and biological dehumanization (b = 0.07, p = .304). In the same vein, the two-way interaction Emergency perception ×Biologization toward Italians was not associated with emotional closeness (b = 0.07, p = .151). Thus, the tested model was not supported. As found in Study 1, results showed that the emergency perception due to the COVID-19 spread was positively associated with biological dehumanization but not with outgroup prejudice. It is noteworthy that this pattern emerged not only when participants assessed Chinese people but also when Italians were asked to evaluate their own group. Importantly, in line with the previous study, our results revealed that higher levels of emergency perception increased biologization toward Chinese people (but not North African people) that in turn increased prejudice against them. In addition, we found that because of the similar health emergency that both Chinese and Italian people struggled with, this latter group perceived a higher emotional closeness with Chinese people, and this perception, in turn, reduced negative evaluations against them. However, this pattern of results emerged only for Italian participants that tended to perceive their own group in biological terms. The main aim of this research was to investigate the intergroup con- Schematic representation of the model tested in Study 2 Biologization Prejudice Emotional closeness Ingroup biologization we included in the study, namely North African people. In addition, the same pattern of relationships among emergency perception, biologization, and prejudice that emerged for Chinese people was also found for North African people. These unexpected results can be explained in the light of the literature on dehumanizing perceptions and prejudice, according to which linguistic metaphors related to the disease are often used to describe the negative impact of immigrants on Italian society by thus increasing negative views of foreigners and outgroup members. Several research projects (e.g., European Commission against Racism and Intolerance (ECRI), 2016; Valtorta, Signorato, et al., 2019; indicated indeed a widespread perception among the Italian population that immigrants and Black people represent a threat to security and well-being. In this sense, it is plausible to think that the salience of contagious disease due to the initial phase of the COVID-19 spread may have reactivated the link between North African people, dehumanization, and prejudice, even if this group was one of the less affected by the epidemic on the date of the data collection. The impact of the COVID-19 emergency on several negative outcomes in the field of intergroup relations was further investigated in Study 2, in which the first western population hit by the novel coronavirus, namely Italians, was added as a new target group. In particular, Study 2 showed a significant association between the health emergency perception and biological dehumanization addressed to the groups most affected by the contagion when the study was conducted (i.e., Italians and Chinese people versus North African people). In this regard, it is noteworthy that Italians were more biologized than Chinese people and North African people. In other words, participants reported more dehumanizing perceptions toward their ingroup than toward outgroups. This result can be interpreted as further evidence of the key role played by the current peculiar health emergency circumstances in eliciting biologization, also going beyond the traditional research on the intergroup dehumanization dynamics (e.g., Costello & Hodson, 2014; Leyens et al., 2000) , which so far conceived dehumanization and its different forms especially as a means of denigrating outgroup members. In this regard, we believe that our findings complement research about self-dehumanization. For example, the internalization of dehumanizing acts and perceptions emerged in the analysis conducted by Volpato and Contarello (1999) of Primo Levi's If This Is a Man (1958) , one of the most valuable testimonies of the Holocaust. According to Levi, dehumanization affected both victims and oppressors. In Levi's eyes, "The personages in these pages are not men. Their humanity is buried, or they themselves have buried it" (p. 127). As reported by the authors, the text contains a great deal of self-dehumanizing metaphors, especially in terms of animal imagery. The most exhausted prisoners remind Levi of "sled-dogs in London's books, who slave until the last breath and die on the track" (p. 49). On the other hand, the toughest prisoners have "the rudimentary astuteness of a draught-horse, which stops pulling a little before it reaches exhaustion" (p. 48). In addition, one set of studies conducted by Bastian and Haslam (2010) linked experiences of social exclusion with self-dehumanization by revealing that social exclusion heightened targets' viewing themselves as having fewer human characteristics. Different factors (e.g., human tragedies, experiences of social exclusion) can therefore promote the internalization of dehumanizing traits by thus modifying how we see and perceive ourselves. The present research adds a tile to this picture by demonstrating that in peculiar circumstances such as an epidemic, biological dehumanizing representations can be adopted to describe one's own group membership. Relevant to our findings, several authors (e.g., Hu et al., 2018; Wu et al., 2015 Wu et al., , 2019 suggested that the mechanism of ingroup derogation is related to the evolutive response of the behavioral immune system, and it is specifically triggered when dealing with a peculiar ecological condition in which greater threat of diseases is incurred by ingroup members. In this regard, studies (see Fincher & Thornill, 2012) have shown that, in some areas (e.g., Africa), the correlations between parasite stress and ingroup sociality were reported to be negative rather than positive. Overall, it seems robust that the pathogen stress significantly shapes attitudes and ingroup perceptions. For the first time in the literature, we provided preliminary evidence of this association by considering one of the most derogating form of social perception, namely dehumanization. According to Study 1, results of Study 2 revealed that higher levels of emergency perception increased biologization toward Chinese people (but not toward North African people), which in turn increased prejudice and negative attitudes against them. In line with these findings, previous literature (e.g., see Schaller et al., 2015) revealed that emergency situations in which individuals are-or merely perceive themselves to be-more vulnerable because of a diseaserelated condition, as the current pandemic, heighten protective behaviors by thus triggering stigmatization of others. As stated by Clissold and colleagues (2020) , emergency situations characterized by outbreaks of infectious disease represent a fertile breeding ground for unveiling existing negative perceptions which are often driven by collective fear. Indeed, infectious disease and its correlates are seen as a threat, and therefore attempts are made to "other" this threat (Nelkin & Gilman, 1988; Reny & Barreto, 2020) . These attempts at "othering" are commonly reinforced by a desire to assign blame and responsibility for the dangerous situation to make sense of such adversity. Evidence of these speculations was provided by Washer (2004) , who conducted a study looking at the British media's response to the 2002 SARS outbreak. The scholar found the development of a discourse suggesting that British citizens were protected from SARS and its detrimental consequences because they were "different" from the Asian citizens, who were initially affected by such an outbreak. Our results seem to be consistent with this literature and demonstrate that the current health emergency together with all its repercussions have a propensity to incite negative intergroup attitudes and perceptions. Furthermore, we found that Italian participants perceived higher emotional closeness with Chinese people (vs. North African people). Importantly, this emotional closeness reduced biologization and prejudice toward this outgroup. Therefore, by expanding the behavioral immune system literature (e.g., Murray & Schaller, 2016; Schaller & Park, 2011) , not only did we confirm the relationship between the salience of contagious disease and negative perceptions against outgroup members, but we also demonstrated that sharing the experience of a viral epidemic could reduce the negative effects of the pathogen stress via the increased emotional closeness among the involved groups. In this regard, it is important to note that this pattern of results emerged only among Italian respondents who reported higher levels of ingroup biologization. Of particular relevance to these findings, several studies (for a review, see Bastian & Crimston, 2014) provided an interesting twist on dehumanization by showing that dehumanizing the self or the ingroup in response to unethical behaviors or exceptional situations motivates a tendency to report and engage in prosocial responses. It is plausible to think that the underlying motivation to report positive attitudes toward the outgroup may represent an attempt to restore humanity within self-perception. Possessing humanity is indeed a basic feature for our identity, and when it is lost, this would be expected to motivate attempts to restore it. In the case of the present research, the perception of "being the virus" because of the COVID-19 epidemic, and the resulting self-biologization, may have led Italian participants to report positive responses toward Chinese people (i.e., decrease in their biologization and prejudice) in an attempt to regain humanity lost. Through the present studies, by integrating previous empirical findings concerning the behavioral immune system theory (e.g., Murray & Schaller, 2016; Park et al., 2007; Thornhill & Fincher, 2014) with the theoretical insights regarding biological dehumanization and prejudice (e.g., Costello & Hodson, 2010; Savage, 2007; Volpato & Andrighetto, 2015) , we demonstrated that the exceptional epidemiological situation due to the COVID-19 spread has the capacity to directly foster social-cognitive biologization of both outgroup and ingroup, but not other forms of outgroup prejudice. In line with these findings, Wilde and colleagues (2014) analyzed dehumanizing processes by distinguishing them from negative attitudes and evaluations. In particular, the authors suggested that dehumanization is a distinct category rather than simply an extreme form of prejudice. Furthermore, a large number of studies (Costello & Hodson, 2010; Goff et al., 2008; Hodson & Costello, 2007; Leyens et al., 2000) have demonstrated that outgroup dehumanization can be considered a robust predictor of prejudice in intergroup contexts. These considerations are particularly relevant for the results that emerged in the present studies, according to which the health emergency perception affected prejudice toward outgroups via biological dehumanization. Our findings seem to confirm previous research on the crucial role of dehumanizing perceptions in shaping outgroup prejudice (e.g., Costello & Hodson, 2012; Goff et al., 2008) , by also providing the first evidence of this relationship in such an exceptional situation as a pandemic. It is noteworthy that although several researchers demonstrated that increased outgroup prejudice may stem from pathogen threat, some recent works showed discrepant findings regarding this specific link. For example, results by van Leeuwen and Peterson (2018) were inconsistent with the view that the behavioral immune system motivates outgroup prejudice. Instead, their findings suggested that this system "simply" motivates the avoidance of any infected individual, regardless of their group membership. In this sense, biologization could be considered a relevant process in explaining the differences concerning the literature on this topic. Indeed, our results are consistent with some scholars who speculated on the relationship between the behavioral immune system theory and dehumanization. For example, Prażmo and Augustyn (2020) stated that the metaphorical notion of a social and bio parasite to describe immigrants is strongly related to the activation of the behavioral immune system, whose aim is to protect us from having any other close contact with potentially contaminated. According to the authors, it may also affect our reasoning and decision-making in relation to political inclinations and attitudes toward others. Thus, activating the parasite imagery inevitably leads to elicit defensive reactions and negative perceptions. Finally, it is noteworthy that we found a non-significant difference between biologization of Chinese and North African people in Study 1, whereas results of Study 2 showed that the North African group was significantly less biologized than the Chinese one. Furthermore, biologization of both the target groups was higher in Study 2 than in the first study (see Table 1 and Table 4 ). In other words, when Italy became the first western country hit by the novel coronavirus and Study 2 was conducted (i.e., March 2020), biologization became more severe, and we found a higher biologization score for Chinese than North African people. In this regard, it is plausible to think that Italian participants were still not quite as concerned in February 2020. Thus, as the pandemic progressed and broke out in this country, Italians Despite the novelty of our studies, it is important to acknowledge that our research has some limitations that may restrict its 6 Given that we used different scales for emergency perception (from 0 to 100 in Study 1; from 1 to 7 in Study 2), before conducting the independent samples t-test, we rescaled the measures to be on the same scale (i.e., 7-point Likert scale; for more details, see the IBM SPSS Statistics support page on how to transform different scales to a common scale). generalizability. Although the associations we observed among variables are consistent with previous findings on dehumanization and prejudice, the correlational nature of the current data does not allow us to draw any causal inferences. It is likely indeed that the relationships between some of our constructs are bidirectional and dynamic. Experimental or longitudinal studies would be an important next step toward determining the direction of these paths. Moreover, it is important to note that in Study 1 and Study 2, participants were asked to think about Chinese and North African people, without providing other information about the groups. Therefore, it is not possible to know whether participants thought of the two target groups as immigrants in Italy or as people living in their respective countries. Given that the migration background might have been a confounding, further studies should deepen our findings by controlling for this aspect. Finally, it is noteworthy that in both studies, the mean ratings of biological dehumanization were low, indicating a weak association of the targets with virus-related words. However, it should be noted that our measure assessed the association between the target and dehumanized perceptions using a self-report measure, which may have been affected by the participants' desirability concerns (e.g., Crowne & Marlowe, 1964; Nederhof, 1985) . Greater associations with biological metaphors may emerge in studies using a subtler measure of dehumanization and implicit techniques, which are less susceptible to motivated responding (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006) and less explicitly related to the current pandemic. Related to this last issue, it is important to note that the indirect effects of the tested models were weak in both our studies. Further research is needed to corroborate our findings and the relationships among emergency perception, dehumanizing processes, and prejudicial attitudes. Our findings reveal how being threatened with disease, as in the case of the COVID-19 outbreak, is an important source of biologization. Crucially, we demonstrated the relevance of this dehumanizing perception in promoting prejudice against the potential sources of contagion. At the same time, we also showed that such negative effects could be reduced when ingroup members experience the same distressing situation, by virtue of the increasing emotional closeness among them. In addition, our results shed light on a new facet of biological dehumanization that, in such an exceptional situation, can become a relevant component not only for the way we see others but also for the way we perceive our own group and identity. We hope that this work will advance knowledge about biologization and prejudice and it will help us better understand the conditions that affect people's perceptions toward both outgroup and ingroup members. The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest. https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0565-5463 Inside the earthquake: Perceived disaster exposure and helping intentions among Italian and immigrant victims of the 2012 Italian earthquakes Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness Including others in the self Self-dehumanization. TPM, Testing, Psychometrics Excluded from humanity: The dehumanizing effects of social ostracism Losing our humanity: The self-dehumanizing consequences of social ostracism Is the cure a wall? Behavioral immune system responses to a disease metaphor for immigration The role of ingroup threat and conservative ideologies on prejudice against immigrants in two samples of Italian adults Anti-Chinese sentiment during the 2019-nCoV outbreak Pandemics and prejudice Exploring the roots of dehumanization: The role of animal-human similarity in promoting immigrant humanization Explaining dehumanization among children: The interspecies model of prejudice Lay beliefs about the causes of and solutions to dehumanization and prejudice: Do non-experts recognize the role of human-animal relations Physical illness stigma and social rejection The approval motive: Studies in evaluative dependence The effect of disgust-eliciting media portrayals on outgroup dehumanization and support of deportation in a Norwegian sample Beyond prejudice: Extending the social psychology of conflict, inequality and social change Purity and danger: An analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo Coping with disaster: The helping behavior of communities and individuals Uncertainty, threat, and the role of the media in promoting the dehumanization of immigrants and refugees Justice, morality, and the dehumanization of refugees Fifth report on Italy G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes Parasite-stress theory may be a general theory of culture and sociality Parasite-stress promotes ingroup assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/ collectivism Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences Preference for consistency and value dissimilarities in dehumanization and prejudice toward asylum seekers in Australia Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A 436 regression-based approach The language of extermination in genocide Interpersonal disgust, ideological orientations, and dehumanization as predictors of intergroup attitudes Pathogen load and the nature of ingroup derogation: Evolution of ingroup derogation based on the sexual Penna model Immunizing against prejudice: Effects of disease protection on attitudes toward out-groups Disaster mythology and fact: Hurricane Katrina and social attachment Generalized or origin-specific out-group prejudice?: The role of temporary and chronic pathogenavoidance motivation in intergroup relations Striving for the moral self: The effects of recalling past moral actions on future moral behavior Social support in the aftermath of disasters, catastrophes, and acts of terrorism: Altruistic, overwhelmed, uncertain, antagonistic, and patriotic communities Bioterrorism: Psychological and public health interventions Why don't they stay at home? Prejudices against ethnic minorities in Italy Thou shall not kill… your brother: Victimperpetrator cultural closeness and moral disapproval of Polish atrocities against Jews after the Holocaust The competitive consequences of the association of immigrants with disease. Paper presented at the bi-annual meeting of the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues When thinking of my death leads to thinking of others' deaths: The effect of collectivism, psychological closeness, and mortality salience on prosocial behavioral intentions in the Sewol ferry disaster The emotional side of prejudice: The attribution of secondary emotions to ingroups and outgroups Social desirability of subtle and blatant prejudice scales When germs travel Mass panic and social attachment: The dynamics of human behavior Metaphors in media discourse on migration The behavioral immune system: Implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence What role do metaphors play in racial prejudice? The function of anti-Semitic imagery in Hitler's Mein Kampf Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes Elevated ethnocentrism in the first trimester of pregnancy Methods of coping with social desirability bias: A review Placing blame for devastating disease Feeling thermometer Human threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people Subtle and blatant prejudice in Western Europe Parasites, herbivores and dried fish: Dehumanising metaphorical blends in Japanese Xenophobia in the time of pandemic: Othering, anti-Asian attitudes, and COVID-19. Politics, Groups, and Identities Disease Incarnate": Biopolitical discourse and genocidal dehumanisation in the age of modernity Implications of the behavioural immune system for social behaviour and human health in the modern world Danger, disease, and the nature of prejudice(s) The behavioral immune system (and why it matters) Disgust as embodied loss aversion At what sample size do correlations stabilize A national survey of stress reactions after the Illness as metaphor and AIDS and its metaphors The psychology of good and evil: Why children, adults, and groups help and harm others The roots of goodness: The fulfillment of basic human needs and the development of caring, helping and nonaggression, inclusive caring, moral courage, active bystandership, and altruism born of suffering The vermin have struck again": Dehumanizing the enemy in post 9/11 media representations. Media, War & Conflict The parasite-stress theory of values and sociality: Infectious disease, history and human values worldwide The dirty side of work: Biologization of physically tainted workers Le immagini come armi di propaganda. La delegittimazione nei manifesti di Forza Nuova (Pictures as weapons. Delegitimization strategies in the visual propaganda of Forza Nuova) The body and soul emotion The behavioral immune system is designed to avoid infected individuals, not outgroups. Evolution and Human Behavior Altruism born of suffering and prosocial behavior following adverse life events: A review and conceptualization Dehumanization Towards a social psychology of extreme situations: Primo Levi's If This is a Man and social identity theory Tra paternalismo e disprezzo: Gli stereotipi dei gruppi immigrati in Italia L'incontro interculturale. Forme, processi, percorsi Picturing the other: Targets of delegitimization across time Representations of SARS in the British newspapers Historical linkages: Epidemic threat, economic risk, and xenophobia. The Lancet Dehumanization as a distinct form of prejudice. TPM, Testing, Psychometrics The social impact of pathogen threat: How disease salience influences conformity Behavioral immune system and ingroup derogation: The effects of infectious diseases on ingroup derogation attitudes Disease threat and the functional flexibility of ingroup derogation Contemporary parasite stress curvilinearly correlates with outgroup trust: Cross-country evidence from Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing