key: cord-0775734-hsgy6iwo authors: Brainard, Julii; Hunter, Paul R title: Misinformation making a disease outbreak worse: outcomes compared for influenza, monkeypox, and norovirus date: 2020-04-03 journal: Simulation DOI: 10.1177/0037549719885021 sha: bebf2f9c399db669bf5c8a75e65f92219fa80583 doc_id: 775734 cord_uid: hsgy6iwo Health misinformation can exacerbate infectious disease outbreaks. Especially pernicious advice could be classified as “fake news”: manufactured with no respect for accuracy and often integrated with emotive or conspiracy-framed narratives. We built an agent-based model that simulated separate but linked circulating contagious disease and sharing of health advice (classified as useful or harmful). Such advice has potential to influence human risk-taking behavior and therefore the risk of acquiring infection, especially as people are more likely in observed social networks to share bad advice. We test strategies proposed in the recent literature for countering misinformation. Reducing harmful advice from 50% to 40% of circulating information, or making at least 20% of the population unable to share or believe harmful advice, mitigated the influence of bad advice in the disease outbreak outcomes. How feasible it is to try to make people “immune” to misinformation or control spread of harmful advice should be explored. Previously we constructed an agent-based model (ABM) 1, 2 about what could happen during a norovirus outbreak exacerbated by circulating misinformation (which we call ''bad advice''). Bad advice is relevant when it changes human behavior to be riskier (that is, a higher risk of getting disease). Examples of possible risky behavior can include not washing hands, sharing food with ill people, not disinfecting potentially contaminated surfaces or fomites, sneezing, coughing, or vomiting in places where there is a high risk of spread onto food or surfaces, lack of disinfection, and other unprotected physical contact with infectious persons or their bodily fluids. Some people in disease outbreak situations take few precautions to avoid getting disease, even when awareness of the outbreak is widespread and many official sources are widely disseminating information about how to avoid illness. Compliance with quarantine protocols during a 2003 outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS)-coronavirus was found to be uneven. Compliance related to risk perception, which in turn related to how trustworthy and credible sources of health advice were perceived to be, as well as difficulties respondents had in accepting parts of the situation they could not be in control of. 3 During a large and well publicized norovirus outbreak at a Canadian university, 25% of symptomatic students were observed to not avoid contacts, while 17% of the observed cohort did not comply with recommended handwashing practices. 4 In a 2019 survey in the UK, 5 14% of 2000 surveyed parents reported sending a child to school with symptoms of contagious chickenpox, violating school policies and official advice to quarantine such children. That misinformation can be linked to taking fewer measures to effectively prevent disease transmission is especially well documented with respect to Ebola viral disease (EVD). For instance, persons affected by the West Africa outbreak (2013-2016) who believed other types of misinformation about EVD (e.g., that it can be airborne or transmitted via mosquito bites) were more likely to report practicing unsafe burial practices. 6 Low trust in institutions and more belief in EVD misinformation were associated with fewer preventive behaviors in the Congolese outbreak that started in 2018. 7 Following ineffective advice or disbelieving official recommendations often led to pursuing other self-care actions first and thus delayed actions that could reduce transmission (such as formally getting tested for EVD). 8 Most sources that have studied disease-precaution behavior, especially vaccine refusal (all diseases), note that cultural identity outside perceived mainstream society is strongly positively linked to the tendency to reject expectations of ''good citizen behavior,'' 9-12 including disease avoidance actions recommended by public health authorities. Our objective was to model possible interactions between misinformation spread and disease outcomes. The modeling was done within an agent-based modeling 13 environment, using Netlogo software. 14 Most model parameters were available from sources reporting actual human behavior, such as about the speed and frequency of social media and real life information sharing. An ABM environment was attractive because it afforded many opportunities to include complex aspects of behavior and response that might vary individually. We use the term complex to distinctively mean processes that are thought to be inherently unpredictable and have uncertain outcomes (as opposed to the often misused near synonym complicated, which can be better interpreted as describing events that can be modeled using consistent albeit possibly very multi-faceted decision trees). Complexity in our models meant that, for instance, clusters of individuals with frequent contact with each other (physical or information sharing contact) might have very different traits from the population averages. However, these differences could not be consistently predicted; nor could location or encounters with other agents. Location and chances of getting disease or encountering misinformation were best estimated for our purposes using reiterative modeling and probabilistic distribution of relevant attributes. These skewed traits could lead to localized hotspots or low activity zones of disease and/or information transmission. Multiple types of feedback loops could be in operation that affected behavior choices. These feedback processes could be modeled at individual level using an ABM. At the same time, many aspects of behavior and response seemed likely to be probabilistic but perhaps on uncertain distributions or with unclear central tendency points. The iterative nature of ABMs meant that we could not only try to use real world data to describe some behavior aspects in the models, but also use iterations to estimate what some central values for other behavior tendencies might be. In the ABM that we constructed, disease spread was via direct or indirect physical contact, while misinformation spread was via social contact, particularly within social groups (''bubbles''). These social groups were constructed to have relatively similar susceptibility for believing the misinformation. Members of one's bubble were often the same persons that were physically encountered, so these could be the same people with whom it was likely that the disease could be exchanged. Our initial work focused on norovirus because, although very common, gastrointestinal illness is rarely modeled in individual infectious disease models. 15 Norovirus also had the advantage of being unlikely to cause flight or death (so those things could be defensibly not included in the model). The incubation period is relatively short so travel outside the residence area was excluded. The purpose of this study is to adapt our misinformation (agent-based) model to other communicable diseases and outbreak conditions. We compare results for norovirus, influenza, and monkeypox (Orthopoxvirus). The latter diseases can be substantially different from norovirus in many respects (such as the mean incubation period) and are often not previously vaccinated for. Influenza is an important communicable respiratory illness globally, while monkeypox is an emerging disease of high concern that has triggered biosecurity concerns. 16, 17 These pathogens gave good opportunities to show that our modeling approach could be adapted to multiple types of disease and transmission risks. Table 1 shows model assumptions and targets, separated by disease. Model assumptions and design features, such as the rate of information injections, are described in greater detail elsewhere. 2 The underlying Netlogo model code is available from the authors upon request. At least 100 iterations were run for the most likely candidate thresholds or parameters to verify their reliability. Extra simulations (above 100 minimum) were run until we found that additional model runs did not produce mean or median estimates that were any closer to the target conditions (i.e., a monotonic state in target outputs had been achieved). A baseline stage 1 model was constructed for each circulating disease. Target basic reproduction numbers (r0) were determined from consulting relevant literature for norovirus and influenza, and a likely maximum number of generations during a monkeypox outbreak (see Table 1 ). The stage 1 models were designed to achieve the target r0 or number of generations. Real life transmission of disease depends on multi-faceted factors: biological, social, structural, and behavioral. 18 We conceptually break the risk of transmission down into three components: the probability of (1) infectious persons or (2) susceptible persons taking adequate precautions to avoid catching disease, as well as (3) viral shedding (which can be linked to pathogen and illness characteristics, not truly under individual control). The involuntary shedding is separated from behavioral risks for many reasons. It makes sense that a small part of the risk is purely biological not behavioral. Separation lets individual behavior be fully separated from involuntary shedding, while involuntary shedding may be adjusted to vary over the course of illness in future more sophisticated models (the amount of virus shed does varies with the stage of illness in real infections 19 ). In a model run, during each time step and in a random direction, well agents move one step. Well agents move five times further than ill agents (who move 0.2 steps). Each time step, susceptible agents near infectious agents were tested for possible disease transmission. Disease was transmitted if neither side took sufficient precautions and viral shedding was sufficient for transmission. Individuals had baseline take-precautions (TP)% values assigned at the start of model runs. TP is the percentage of the time in which agents took effective precautions against catching disease. TP did not vary in the stage 1 mode, but was important in stage 2 and 3 models when it could change in response to circulating (mis)information. To establish the initial (baseline) stage 1 models, an exercise (described in the next paragraph) was undertaken to estimate the proportion of risk that could be attributed to and reserved for just viral shedding. TP was assigned to each individual agent, generated stochastically, and assumed to have a normal distribution of values around the population mean, which was preset to 50% for several reasons. Constraining TP to a range only between 0 and 1.0 (but with a fixed central population mean) gave TP the maximum room for change. Also, 65 but there is a lack of data from highincome country settings. The model assumes no shortage of vaccines (once available, there is always enough to meet demand). Delay in influenza is for production timeline; delay in monkeypox vaccine is for time to procure supply and to recognize need. prespecifying TP allowed us to estimate and specify separately the small proportion of risk of transmission that could be attributed to viral shedding alone (meant to be pathogen-specific and due to the presence of infection, not the risk of transmission due to agents' good or poor behavior choices). Designating a small separate viral shed risk from behavior seemed desirable because only with fairly extreme precautions (such as wearing personal protection equipment) could the risk of disease transmission be truly reduced to effectively zero. The mechanism of disease transfer was as follows: transmission could happen when susceptible and infectious agents were in close proximity and neither took high enough precautions to avoid transmission. A small proportion of the risk was also linked to viral shedding for each disease. The variations in how disease was transmitted (with and without a separate viral shedding risk) is helpful in showing that our modeling approach can be flexible and adapted for different disease or outbreak conditions. Table 1 lists other model parameters and assumptions. Between 1% and 2% of agents were infected at simulation start time. Thereafter, agents moved around and tests were made about potentially transmitted disease each time step (1 hour). The model starts at 7am, and agents return ''home'' to the same location each evening, which means the highest disease transmission risk is with others near their home location. Agent density, grid size, and movement rules were designed such that, in the absence of any disease, the daily contact rate with other agents averaged very close to 11.74 unique others/day (a target drawn from published UK contact rates in non-epidemic situations 20 ). Empirically, we found that 1600 agents achieved the target contact rate, on a torus world shape (e.g., going off the bottom meant re-entry at the top), with a visible area measuring 88 3 90 patches that agents could move around on. Incubation periods, assumptions about shedding before or after illness, the duration of active illness and the infectious period, and case fatality rates were drawn from the relevant literature. In Table 1 , chances of hospitalization or fatality were plausible, not meant to be definitive. Because health care in the UK is free at the point of use to and urgent care facilities are widely available, we assume that all very ill individuals will seek medical advice. We assume that only very ill individuals will die from disease. Therefore, the models assume that only hospitalized cases ever died and that no transmission occurred after someone was hospitalized due to effective infection control measures. Vaccine efficacy was assumed to be 100%, but vaccine availability varied by disease. Uptake was assumed to be high but not universal (75% of those who could benefit). Hospitalization is the only form of quarantine considered. The stage 2 model used the same disease and information circulating assumptions as the stage 1 model, but with exacerbation due to spread of misinformation that often reduced taking effective precautions (TP). Recall that the mean TP value was assigned randomly on a normal distribution with a preset mean of 50% at the start. In stage 2, each exposure to misinformation changed individual TP, with limits at 0% and 100%. The possible change in TP (DTP) was determined using repeated model runs to achieve a 40% worse (higher) r0 for norovirus or influenza, or more generations of transmission (from target 4 to target 7 in 75% of simulations) for monkeypox. The magnitude of change in response to advice was equal whether good or bad advice. [21] [22] [23] DTP is the key response that individuals have to circulating information (or misinformation). Exposure to ''good'' advice increases taking precautions; exposure to bad advice decreases taking precautions. Each agent had a list of other agents that they might share information with (their own unique ''information bubble''). There were typically 80-230 members of this ''bubble'' (mean number = 150, to conform with estimates of significant friendship circle size, Dunbar numbers 24,25 ). The list of social contacts was created such that (on average) two-thirds of the social contacts had similar propensity (randomly assigned around population mean = 38.9%) to believe misinformation (from experimental and observation data that typically members of the British public believe on an average 38.9% of conspiracy theories that they are exposed to 26 ). About 20% of social contacts were located near the agent's home location; physical proximity made it more likely that disease would be shared with these specific same social contacts with whom (mis)information was shared. In every model run, agents moved around, potentially transmitted disease, and made decisions about whether to share information they had been exposed to. Susceptible agents with lower TP values were at highest risk of acquiring infection. A total of 138 times per hour a single agent chosen at random was exposed to a piece of information and made a decision (stochastically) whether to share the information onward to a small percentage (2.5%) of their social contacts. The cascades of resulting information (number of times the information passed through unique sharers) were monitored and the model performance in this regard is documented elsewhere. 2 The frequency of relevant information sharing and the likelihood of sharing advice were both determined empirically so that the resulting information cascades would conform with information sharing patterns reported recently on Twitter for true and false stories. 27 Distinguishing true from false (good and bad advice in our model) was important, because false stories were observed to be four times more likely to be shared in the Twitter study. In stage 2, the ratio of good:bad advice exposed to agents was 50:50. Proposed strategies to fight fake news from the previous literature include the following: 1) provide counter-information that is equally or better evidenced, or more persuasive [28] [29] [30] [31] [32] [33] [34] ; 2) tax the advertising or tax the profits of products sold via misinformation 35 ; 3) drown bad info with good information 35 ; 4) regulate information 33 and possibly impose civil or criminal liabilities, 29 which could lead to explicit censorship 29,33 ; 5) revise financial models available to fake news disseminators (incentives) to stop encouraging the production and sharing of false (or even just very salaciously written) stories over truth and accuracy 27,34,36-39 ; 6) labeling (reliability rating or counter-arguments provided) by the news provider 27, 29, 33, 34 ; 7) encourage individuals to actively strive to make their own social filter bubbles more diverse 33 ' 8) ''immunize'' recipients to disregard fake news (education-based strategy). 36, 40 We do not model the effects of intervention strategy 1 because the results are predictable; any changes will be linear responses if good advice increases without a reduction in bad advice or if good and bad advice are equally contagious. Therefore, in stage 3, two strategies for reducing misinformation impacts were tested because their impacts could not be easily foreseen: (1) increasing the proportion of ''good advice'' that encourages more protective behaviors; and (2) ''immunizing'' individuals so that they do not respond to or share bad advice. Possible thresholds (to reduce the r0 from stage 2 to stage 1 values, or even lower) were explored both for the proportion misinformation adjustment and ''immunization'' strategies. Stage 3 models were run under stage 2 conditions but with the following modifications and objectives. Stage 3.1: reduce bad advice injections from 50% to a value that achieved conditions similar to our stage 1 models (stage 1 target r0 or number of generations), as well as the stage 3.2 test of what happened if only 10% of circulating advice is bad advice. ''Immunize'' against bad information (but not immunized against the virus, and still able to react positively to good advice): a percentage of randomly selected agents were selected to be fully resistant (''immunized'') to bad advice. The exact percentage was found empirically, such that (stage 3.3) the stage 1 r0 or number of generations was achieved. We also tested (stage 3.4) if ''immunizing'' 90% of agents could reduce r0 below 1.0. ''Immunization'' also meant no sharing of bad advice. This strategy simulated approaches that were based on education or diversifying social contacts (social filter bubbles). Statistical differences in the outcomes under each set of modeling assumptions were calculated using Wilcoxon rank sum tests using Stata v. 16.0 software (stage 1 was the reference condition). Table 2 shows estimated viral shed risk values found to consistently get the closest to target r0s in stage 1 models, when mean TP = 50%, and the change in TP (DTP) required upon each information exposure to increase r0 by 40% (stage 2 models). Table 3 shows model run results (r0, duration of outbreak, final attack rate, and prevalence of disease at peak) for each separate disease. Supplemental files S1-S3 show additional results for alternative model parameters. For all diseases, reducing bad advice from 50% to 40% of all relevant circulating advice changed Table 1 ). Values in () are range of r0 or CFR, 5-95th percentiles, over ø 100 simulations. More detailed results are in the supplemental information. stage 2 outbreak conditions to stage 1 levels. Similarly, making 20-25% of individuals ''immune'' to believing or sharing bad advice changed stage 2 outbreak outcomes back to stage 1 levels (with respect to r0 or number of generations of transmission, duration of outbreak, peak attack rate, case fatality rate, or final total attack rate). We find it interesting to note that even if the strategies are applied relatively drastically and effectively (i.e., quite large reductions in proportion of bad advice circulating or high percentage ''immunized'' against bad advice), disease spread was not stopped. The results in Table 3 suggest that even if bad advice were only 10% of circulating advice (stage 3.2), r0 for norovirus and influenza may reach 1.0, while the number of generations of transmission for monkeypox will be ø 3 in at least 25% of simulations. Similarly, even if 90% of agents are ''immune'' to bad advice (stage 3.4), outbreak r0 (norovirus and influenza) will tend to be ø 1.0 and at least 25% of outbreaks will have ø 3 generations of transmission. The Wilcoxon rank sum tests (Table 3) show that, broadly, outcomes were Table 3 . Stage 1 (no sharing), stage 2 (outbreak exacerbated by bad advice), and stage 3 (results with intervention strategies). Mean values for given outbreak characteristics, with 5-95th percentiles to indicate range without the most extreme values. No previous studies have integrated information spread with disease spread to the level of sophistication that we have done. Prior models often considered information spread in disease outbreak development, but information awareness was typically equally available to all agents, and benign at worst. Thus, information spread in the models nearly always led to greater protective measures (such as increasing vaccine uptake or decreasing contact rates [41] [42] [43] [44] [45] [46] [47] [48] [49] [50] ). Most previous similar disease and awareness spread models had awareness increases that could only happen following physical contact or as a result of global conditions. 42, 45, 48, [50] [51] [52] [53] [54] [55] Our modeling is unusual because information spread was individual and separated from the physical interactions that could transmit disease. Our model is unique and original in attempting to consider the potentially deleterious role of information sharing with stochastic and individually assigned elements. The need for research such as ours has been recognized before. 17, 56 More sophisticated information sharing networks than we tried to create could make these models more credible. There exist more sophisticated models on rumor spread that we could possibly replicate for the information spreading process, [57] [58] [59] and simultaneously merge with existing sophisticated disease spread models. More ambitious models than ours would describe more agents and more complicated movement patterns, such as including flight as a behavior option. Many rumor spreading models have borrowed ideas and methods from epidemiological models, 60, 61 but not many (if any) previous models have integrated both rumor and disease spread as separate but interacting processes into one unified probabilistic model. This study describes the spread of three viral diseases; misinformation affecting the spread of bacterial diseases could be modeled equally well. The ideas could be applied to non-communicable diseases and health outcomes, but it would be necessary to change the time scale to be much longer to model chronic and lifestyle diseases and how their incidence might change in response to circulating misinformation. A much longer time scale would mean incorporating many other lifestyle factors into the models. Model construction relied heavily on a small number of existing studies about such factors as number of contact rates, social contacts (i.e.., Dunbar numbers), how much bad or good advice can change behavior, and the propensity to believe in misinformation (the finding that on average, British people believe in 38.9% of conspiracy theories that they are exposed to). More reliably estimating any of these and many of the other factors would also increase the credibility of our results. Our threshold for a ''worse'' outbreak situation was r0 being 40% worse or the number of generations of disease transmission increased from 4 to 7; these thresholds were decided for convenience in this set of demonstration models. Given our definition of stage 2 as an outbreak ''made worse by circulating misinformation,'' stage 3.1 modeling concluded for all three diseases that a ratio of about 60:40 good:bad advice circulating would reduce the stage 2 conditions to those of stage 1. The models also suggested that ''immunizing'' about 20% of the population against misinformation was likely to revert stage 2 to stage 1 conditions (for all diseases, stage 3.3). Since these apparent consistencies could be artefacts of shared model design, tests to explore the true consistency of these findings for multiple diseases would be worthwhile. It is possible that more sophisticated, detailed, or larger models or more flexible modeling software 62 would facilitate better insights into risk distributions and behavior choices. There is uncertainty in the reliability of these findings because the models are experimental and have not been tested in real world situations. There is a general lack of reliable quantification for how much misinformation spread impacts real life risk-taking behavior with regard to communicable diseases. We applied three stages of modeling (1 = no misinformation spread, 2 = misinformation making outbreaks worse, and 3 = strategies to reduce the influence of misinformation). Our modeling approach and design is adaptable to many different types of diseases. Controlling spread of misinformation or susceptibility to it could reduce communicable disease burdens. Our stage 3.1 modeling found that a ratio of about 60:40 good:bad circulating advice reduced stage 2 conditions to those of stage 1 in three types of disease. ''Immunizing'' about 20% of the population against misinformation (stage 3.3) was likely to revert stage 2 to stage 1 conditions (for all diseases). The feasibility of implementing these types of strategies (''immunization'' or changing the proportions of types of advice in circulation) should be explored. The efficacy of implementing such strategies to fight ''fake news'' needs to be tested in real world settings, with costs and benefits ideally compared with real world disease reduction. the National Health Service, the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR), the Department of Health, or PHE. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript and associated documents. An agent-based model about the effects of fake news on a norovirus outbreak An agent-based model about the effects of fake news on a norovirus outbreak (under review Risk perception and compliance with quarantine during the SARS outbreak University students' hand hygiene practice during a gastrointestinal outbreak in residence: what they say they do and what they actually do Millions of children are sent to school or nursery when ill, poll claims. The Independent Effect of knowledge and perceptions of risks on Ebola-preventive behaviours in Ghana Institutional trust and misinformation in the response to the 2018-19 Ebola outbreak in North Kivu, DR Congo: a population-based survey Fears and misperceptions of the Ebola response system during the 2014-2015 outbreak in Sierra Leone Anti-vaccination movements and their interpretations Trusting blindly can be the biggest risk of all': organised resistance to childhood vaccination in the UK If you ask them, will they come? Predictors of quarantine compliance during a hypothetical avian influenza pandemic: results from a statewide survey Attitudes toward the use of quarantine in a public health emergency in four countries: the experiences of Hong Kong Putting the agent in agent-based modeling Netlogo epidem basic model Lessons from a decade of individual-based models for infectious disease transmission: a systematic review Federal Select Agent Program. Select agents regulations Comparison and analysis of biological agent category lists based on biosafety and biodefense Wildlife trade and the emergence of infectious diseases Viral shedding and clinical illness in naturally acquired influenza virus infections Social contacts and mixing patterns relevant to the spread of infectious diseases Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review The effects of gain-versus loss-framed messages following health risk information on physical activity in individuals with multiple sclerosis The advantages of compliance or the disadvantages of noncompliance? A meta-analytic review of the relative persuasive effectiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages The structure of online social networks mirrors those in the offline world Calling Dunbar's numbers Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: the generic conspiracist beliefs scale The spread of true and false news online How social media is transforming the spreading of knowledge: Implications for our perceptions concerning vaccinations and migrant health The science of fake news Twitter, and misinformation: a dangerous combination The effectiveness of flood risk communication strategies and the influence of social networks-insights from an agent-based model What makes online content viral? The ''echo chamber'' distraction: disinformation campaigns are the problem, not audience fragmentation Replacing disputed flags with related articles Regulating misinformation Finding Foothold in the Age of Trump'' Television broadcast on10 Social media and fake news in the 2016 election Post-truth: how bullshit conquered the world From headline to photograph, a fake news masterpiece. The New York Times Fake news: belief in post-truth Disease interventions can interfere with one another through disease-behaviour interactions Modeling interaction between individuals, social networks and public policy to support public health epidemiology Public avoidance and epidemics: insights from an economic model Information-related changes in contact patterns may trigger oscillations in the endemic prevalence of infectious diseases Incorporating individual health-protective decisions into disease transmission models: a mathematical framework Multi-scale modeling for the transmission of influenza and the evaluation of interventions toward it Multiple sources and routes of information transmission: implications for epidemic dynamics Predicting self-initiated preventive behavior against epidemics with an agent-based relative agreement model Modeling influenza by modulating flu awareness Impact of asymptomatic infection on coupled disease-behavior dynamics in complex networks Coupled contagion dynamics of fear and disease: mathematical and computational explorations Adaptive human behavior in epidemiological models Imitation dynamics of vaccination behaviour on social networks A rule-based model of a hypothetical zombie outbreak: Insights on the role of emotional factors during behavioral adaptation of an artificial population Coevolution of risk perception, sexual behaviour, and HIV transmission in an agentbased model Computational modeling of spontaneous behavior changes and infectious disease spread Point process modelling of rumour dynamics in social media A novel rumour propagation model on social networks Theory of rumour spreading in complex social networks The exact solution of the general stochastic rumour Analyzing the dynamics of a rumor transmission model with incubation EpiModel: an R package for mathematical modeling of infectious disease over networks Norovirus transmission dynamics: a modelling review Estimates of the reproduction number for seasonal, pandemic, and zoonotic influenza: a systematic review of the literature Inference of R0 and transmission heterogeneity from the size distribution of stuttering chains Emergence of monkeypox as the most important orthopoxvirus Infection in humans Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Norovirus in healthcare facilities fact sheet Incubation periods of viral gastroenteritis: a systematic review Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Clinical signs and symptoms of influenza Human monkeypox Association of host, agent and environment characteristics and the duration of incubation and symptomatic periods of norovirus gastroenteritis A foodborne outbreak of norovirus gastroenteritis associated with a Christmas dinner in Porto, Portugal Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. The symptoms of norovirus Case fatality rates based on population estimates of influenza-like illness due to novel H1N1 influenza The annual production cycle for influenza vaccine Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Key facts about seasonal flu vaccine Julii Brainard is a Senior Research Associate at Norwich Medical School, UK. Her interests include evidence evaluation, modeling, and emergency preparedness Paul Hunter is Professor of Health Protection at Norwich Medical School. His main interests are in epidemiology, especially waterborne and gastrointestinal diseases Supplemental material for this article is available online.