key: cord-0755823-pmy2e997 authors: Resta, Elena; Mula, Silvana; Baldner, Conrad; Di Santo, Daniela; Agostini, Maximilian; Bélanger, Jocelyn J.; Gützkow, Ben; Kreienkamp, Jannis; Abakoumkin, Georgios; Khaiyom, Jamilah Hanum Abdul; Ahmedi, Vjollca; Akkas, Handan; Almenara, Carlos A.; Atta, Mohsin; Bagci, Sabahat Cigdem; Basel, Sima; Kida, Edona Berisha; Bernardo, Allan B. I.; Buttrick, Nicholas R.; Chobthamkit, Phatthanakit; Choi, Hoon‐Seok; Cristea, Mioara; Csaba, Sara; Damnjanović, Kaja; Danyliuk, Ivan; Dash, Arobindu; Douglas, Karen M.; Enea, Violeta; Faller, Daiane Gracieli; Fitzsimons, Gavan J.; Gheorghiu, Alexandra; Gómez, Ángel; Hamaidia, Ali; Han, Qing; Helmy, Mai; Hudiyana, Joevarian; Jeronimus, Bertus F.; Jiang, Ding‐Yu; Jovanović, Veljko; Kamenov, Zeljka; Kende, Anna; Keng, Shian‐Ling; Kieu, Tra Thi Thanh; Koc, Yasin; Kovyazina, Kamila; Kozytska, Inna; Krause, Joshua; Kruglanski, Arie W.; Kurapov, Anton; Kutlaca, Maja; Lantos, Nóra Anna; Lemay, Edward P.; Lesmana, Cokorda Bagus J.; Louis, Winnifred R.; Lueders, Adrian; Malik, Najma Iqbal; Martinez, Anton P.; McCabe, Kira O.; Mehulić, Jasmina; Milla, Mirra Noor; Mohammed, Idris; Molinario, Erica; Moyano, Manuel; Muhammad, Hayat; Muluk, Hamdi; Myroniuk, Solomiia; Najafi, Reza; Nisa, Claudia F.; Nyúl, Boglárka; O'Keefe, Paul A.; Osuna, Jose Javier Olivas; Osin, Evgeny N.; Park, Joonha; Pica, Gennaro; Pierro, Antonio; Rees, Jonas H.; Reitsema, Anne Margit; Rullo, Marika; Ryan, Michelle K.; Samekin, Adil; Santtila, Pekka; Sasin, Edyta; Schumpe, Birga M.; Selim, Heyla A.; Stanton, Michael Vicente; Stroebe, Wolfgang; Sultana, Samiah; Sutton, Robbie M.; Tseliou, Eleftheria; Utsugi, Akira; van Breen, Jolien A.; van Lissa, Caspar J.; van Veen, Kees; van Dellen, Michelle R.; Vázquez, Alexandra; Wollast, Robin; Yeung, Victoria Wai‐lan; Zand, Somayeh; Žeželj, Iris Lav; Zheng, Bang; Zick, Andreas; Zúñiga, Claudia; Leander, N. Pontus title: ‘We are all in the same boat’: How societal discontent affects intention to help during the COVID‐19 pandemic date: 2021-10-08 journal: J Community Appl Soc Psychol DOI: 10.1002/casp.2572 sha: 69e87dab212c0fc4aec433f26763cf98d0554756 doc_id: 755823 cord_uid: pmy2e997 The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID‐19) pandemic has caused a global health crisis. Consequently, many countries have adopted restrictive measures that caused a substantial change in society. Within this framework, it is reasonable to suppose that a sentiment of societal discontent, defined as generalized concern about the precarious state of society, has arisen. Literature shows that collectively experienced situations can motivate people to help each other. Since societal discontent is conceptualized as a collective phenomenon, we argue that it could influence intention to help others, particularly those who suffer from coronavirus. Thus, in the present study, we aimed (a) to explore the relationship between societal discontent and intention to help at the individual level and (b) to investigate a possible moderating effect of societal discontent at the country level on this relationship. To fulfil our purposes, we used data collected in 42 countries (N = 61,734) from the PsyCorona Survey, a cross‐national longitudinal study. Results of multilevel analysis showed that, when societal discontent is experienced by the entire community, individuals dissatisfied with society are more prone to help others. Testing the model with longitudinal data (N = 3,817) confirmed our results. Implications for those findings are discussed in relation to crisis management. Please refer to the Supplementary Material section to find this article's Community and Social Impact Statement. suppose that a sentiment of societal discontent, defined as generalized concern about the precarious state of society, has arisen. Literature shows that collectively experienced situations can motivate people to help each other. Since societal discontent is conceptualized as a collective phenomenon, we argue that it could influence intention to help others, particularly those who suffer from coronavirus. Thus, in the present study, we aimed (a) to explore the relationship between soci- The year 2020 saw one of the largest global health crises (Van Bavel et al., 2020) . Starting in December 2019, cases of a new form of pneumonia, coronavirus disease 2019 , surged to the point that on March 11, 2020, the World Health Organization (WHO) declared the outbreak, a pandemic. To deal with the rapid spread of the disease, different countries have adopted restrictive measures, such as lockdowns, social distancing and quarantines (Benke, Autenrieth, Asselmann, & Pané-Farré, 2020) . Embracing those preventive behaviours had, among others, economic and social consequences. For instance, the pandemic has caused a de-globalization, obligating the closure of national and sub-national borders, decreasing the demand for manufactured products and increasing that of medical supplies and food (e.g., Nicola et al., 2020; Van Bavel, Baicker, Boggio, et al., 2020) . These economic changes and a reduction in activity, across all economic sectors, have resulted in the widespread loss of jobs and rise of financial problems. Furthermore, some restrictions and preventive behaviours, such as wearing masks or avoiding crowded spaces, caused a reduction of individuals' freedom and a substantial change in everyone's routine (Tisdell, 2020) . Within this frame, it is reasonable to assume that people have begun to worry about the present and future of society. Uncertainty about the future and many unexpected changes in the community could reasonably have induced the perception that society is in the process of deterioration. This would reflect the perception of societal discontent, defined by Taggart (2004) as a sense of crisis, which includes the perception that various relevant aspects of society are about to break down. Accordingly, Steenvoorden (2015) described it as citizens' concern about the precarious state of society. Literature reports societal discontent as a collective phenomenon in which citizens of the entire community share a tacit understanding that society is in a predicament (e.g., van der Bles, 2018). In a similar vein, Steenvoorden and Harteveld (2018) found that societal unease, a specific conceptualization of concern about the state of society, is strongly associated with broader societal pessimism. The main characteristic of societal discontent is that it concerns a generalized perception of the community, and, thus, it can be defined as a negative Zeitgeist: a pervasive, collective-level evaluation that society is in decline (e.g., Gootjes, Kuppens, Postmes, & Gordijn, 2021; van der Bles, Postmes, & Meijer, 2015) . Interestingly, collective judgements about the present and the future of society can be independent from individual perception of the same societal issues (van der Bles et al., 2015) . Thus, a generalized discontent can be completely distinct from individuals' personal problems or well-being. In this vein, van der Bles, Postmes, LeKander-Kanis, and Otjes (2018) reported that even if people thought their country was going in the wrong direction, they were satisfied with their personal lives. Societal discontent concerns the sense of 'how we are doing' in the context of society, and, therefore, it deals with the perception of the situation the entire community is experiencing (van der Bles et al., 2018) . Similarly, the term 'common fate' refers to the same situation experienced collectively which, by being common, elicits a sense of 'we-ness' (Vollhardt, 2009) . Literature investigated common fate mainly in context of threat and adversity, reporting that it motivates prosocial behaviour (Dovidio et al., 1997) . Dovidio and Morris (1975) found that common fate, particularly of stressful situations, increases helping others. In a similar vein, Richardson and Maninger (2016) reported a 'we were all in the same boat' effect: citizens who shared the traumatic experience of Hurricane Ike helped each other to recover from the natural disaster. Thus, a stressful event, when perceived by the entire community, can motivate people to increase prosocial behaviour. In fact, drawing from empathy-altruism hypothesis of helping behaviour (Batson & Oleson, 1991) , similarity to other people, has been demonstrated to increase helping behaviour (e.g., Dovidio, 1984) . In a similar vein, literature reported of different events in which individuals who felt 'in the same boat' after an adverse event were prone to help each other. For instance, Hernandez (2001) reported that experiences of adversity motivated Colombians to engage in activism on the side of the community. Furthermore, common fate has been used to explain the increasing of prosocial behaviour, such as donating money, comforting or volunteering, during stressful and traumatic events experienced collectively (e.g., Yum & Schenck-Hamlin, 2005) . In line with this reasoning, since societal discontent is conceptualized as a sentiment experienced by the whole country, we argue that it may influence helping behaviour. However, no previous research has investigated the effect of dissatisfaction with society on prosocial behaviour. Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, no study has yet explored the cross-level interaction effect between societal discontent at the individual level and societal discontent at the country level. In fact, perceptions at the individual level can be discrepant from those at the collective level (Postmes, Branscombe, Spears, & Young, 1999) . Thus, we can find not only different predictive effects of concern about society at the individual or collective level, as agued by van der Bles et al. (2018) , but also interesting findings when those two levels of analysis combine with each other. Our aim was to investigate the relationship between societal discontent and the intention to help others, in the context of adversity due to the COVID-19 pandemic. The spread of the disease has affected society worldwide and has produced consequences at the individual level, such as reduction in personal freedom, but also at the collective level, such as economic crisis. It seems reasonable that this scenario could have induced a general discontent, which, in turn, may affect the tendency to help each other. To deal with the consequences of COVID-19, an increasing number of fundraisings were created (Rajwa et al., 2020) . Thus, in the unusual circumstances of the pandemic, it is meaningful to understand if societal discontent can influence the willingness to help others, particularly those who suffer physically and/or economically from coronavirus. Drawn from these premises, the present research had two main purposes. First, to explore the relationship between societal discontent and intention to help at the individual level with the hypothesis that concern about society (i.e., societal discontent) could influence citizens' intent to help those who suffer from coronavirus. Second, given that societal discontent is conceptualized as a collective phenomenon, we were particularly interested in investigating whether and how societal discontent at the country level modulates this relationship. Specifically, the aim of the study was exploratory. However, the literature we reviewed above, especially that about common fate, suggests that events experienced by the entire community would have a positive impact on helping behaviour. Thus, we argue that individuals dissatisfied with society would be more inclined to help those who suffer from COVID-19 when this sentiment of dissatisfaction is experienced by the whole country. In addition, to investigate if the effects of societal discontent at the individual and country level, and their interaction, had influenced intention to help over time, we tested a longitudinal model. This study employed data from the PsyCorona Survey, a cross-national longitudinal study aimed to investigate people's responses to the COVID-19 pandemic, both at the individual and country levels (cfr. PsyCorona Project: https://psycorona.org). The study was approved by the institutional review board at New York University Abu Dhabi (protocol HRPP-2020-42) and the Ethics Committee of Psychology at Groningen University (protocol PSY-1920-S-0390). Each participant gave informed consent before beginning the survey. Data were collected online through Qualtrics' panel management service between 19 March 2020 and 13 March 2021. The PsyCorona Survey, available in 30 languages, was distributed in 115 countries through a combination of convenience sampling, snowball sampling and paid procedures, recruiting over 60,000 participants. After completing this initial survey, respondents who provided contact information were invited to participate in subsequent follow-up surveys, for a total of 20 follow-up assessments. For the purposes of the present study, we used data collected at the time of the baseline and at the seventh follow-up assessment (May 16th, 2020). Precisely, we used the measures of societal discontent and intention to help collected at the baseline and the measure of intention to help collected at the seventh follow-up, which was assessed with only two items (out of eight) already measured at the baseline. Additionally, we decided to include only countries that had more than 150 participants, in order to obtain an average degree of reliability for a multilevel analysis (Kline, 2016) . Thus, the final sample of the cross-sectional study included 61,734 participants (61.1% female) from 42 countries, aged from 18 to over 75 years old. In the final sample, 23.5% of participants had a higher education, 30.4% had a bachelor's degree, 16.1% had a master's degree and 5.1% had a PhD degree (see Tables 1 and 2 T A B L E 1 Descriptive statistics of participants included in the analyses: country, N per country, gender and age. N.B. Total percentages may not reach 100% due to missing data that were not included in the In order to investigate our hypotheses, we focused on measures of societal discontent and COVID-related intention to help. Societal discontent was assessed through a subscale from Gootjes et al. (2020) , which previously showed good reliability. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed with the following sentences: (1) I fear that things will go wrong in society, (2) I feel concerned when I think about the future of society and (3) I am satisfied with society (reverse). Items were responded on a five-point scale (À2 = 'Strongly disagree'; +2 = 'Strongly agree') and were averaged to form a single individual societal discontent score (Cronbach's alpha = 0.69). Since societal discontent is conceptualized as a collective phenomenon in literature, we also considered discontent at the group level. Thus, even if societal discontent was measured at the individual level, we aggregated its perception to the country level, using the within-group average for each group as a whole. To justify the aggregation of societal discontent at the group level, we previously demonstrated high within-country agreement (r wg [j] ; James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1993) . In this study, r wg(j) for societal discontent was 0.78, providing adequate support for variable aggregation (James et al., 1993) . In addition, the ICC(1) value exceeded the recommended cutoff of 0.06 [ICC (1) = 0.09], indicating that 9% of the variance in societal discontent was explained by country and that the societal discontent scale had high inter-rater reliability. In addition, the ICC(2) value exceeded the recommended cutoff of 0.70 [ICC(2) = 0.99], thus indicating that the country-level mean scores of societal discontent scores were highly reliable. Participants' intention to help others who suffer from coronavirus was measured through two different sets of items. The first set aimed to investigate to what extent people were willing to help others that suffer from COVID-19, for example, by making donations or personal sacrifices (4 items, for example, 'I am willing to make personal sacrifices to prevent the spread of coronavirus'). The second set of items aimed to investigate to what extent participants were willing to help with the economic and financial consequences of coronavirus (4 items, for example, 'To help with the economic and financial consequences of coronavirus, I am willing to make donations to help others that suffer from such consequences'). Both sets of items were answered using a seven-point scale (À3 = 'Strongly disagree'; +3 = 'Strongly agree') and were averaged to form a single intention to help score (Cronbach's alpha = 0.89 Participants were asked to indicate their age, gender and level of education. Since Romano et al. (2021) found a significant effect of age and gender on intention to help, we used these variables, as well as education, as covariates. We conducted the analyses using SPSS Statistic version 25.0. Before testing our hypotheses, we investigated differences between countries in societal discontent and intention to help. We conducted two analyses of vari- and Ukraine were the least motivated to help others whereas those in Philippines, Bangladesh and Pakistan were the most motivated. Details regarding the mean and standard deviation for each country can be found in the Data S1. To test the effect of societal discontent at the individual level on the intention to help others and the possible moderating role of societal discontent at the country level on this relationship, we used multilevel modelling, treating participants as nested within countries. Specifically, to test the cross-level interaction effect on intention to help, we entered in the model societal discontent at the individual and country levels, and their interaction, as fixed effects. Subsequently, we ran a second model to verify whether results of the first model remain the same while controlling for covariates, that is, gender, age and education. We ran the models using maximum likelihood (ML) estimation. In the two models, only the intercept was a random effect, entered at the country level. To remove between-country variability in the individual-level discontent, we decided to center societal discontent at the individual level to the mean of each country (Enders & Tofighi, 2007) . Furthermore, to facilitate the interpretation of the effects, we centered societal discontent at the group level to the grand mean (Kenny & Garcia, 2010) . Table 3 summarizes the results obtained. We found a significant negative main effect of societal discontent at the individual level on intention to help (b = À0.023; p < .001). The main effect of societal discontent at the country level on intention to help was not significant (b = À0.012; p = .958). Interestingly, when testing the cross-level hypothesis, we found a significant positive effect of the interaction on intention to help (b = 0.202; p < .001). Specifically, the relationship between societal discontent at the individual level and intention to help others tended to be more strongly positive for higher levels of societal discontent at the country level. To examine the interaction effect, we conducted simple slopes analysis using the web page 'Simple intercepts, simple slopes and regions of significance in HLM 2-way interactions' (http://www.quantpsy.org/ interact/hlm2.htm). When societal discontent at the country level was low (1 SD below the mean), the relationship between societal discontent at the individual level and intention to help was negative (b = À0.2241, SE = 0.0254, p < .001), whereas when societal discontent at the country level was high (1 SD above the mean), this relationship was positive (b = 0.1719, SE = 0.0261, p < .001). Controlling for age, gender and education did not change these patterns. To investigate the cross-level hypothesis over time, we used the measure of COVID-related intention to help at the seventh follow-up, which was composed by two items, already measured at the baseline ('I am willing to help others who suffer from coronavirus', 'I am willing to protect vulnerable groups from coronavirus even at my own expense'; Cronbach's alpha = 0.82). We tested the longitudinal model using data from 12 countries, specifically from Canada, France, Germany, Greece, Italy, the Netherlands, Republic of Serbia, Romania, Spain, Ukraine, the United Kingdom and the United States of America. Using the same analysis strategy of cross-sectional effects, we examined the predictive effects of societal discontent at the individual level, societal discontent at the country level and their interaction, at the baseline, on the intention to help others, measured at the seventh follow-up. Table 4 summarizes the results obtained in the longitudinal model. We found a significant negative main effect of societal discontent at the individual level on intention to help (b = À0.054; p < .05). The main effect of societal discontent at the country level on intention to help was not significant (b = À0.07; p = .753). However, for the purpose of the present study, the most important result was that relating to the cross-level hypothesis. We found a significant positive effect of the interaction between societal discontent at the individual level and societal discontent at the country level on the intention to help those who suffer from coronavirus at the seventh follow-up (b = 0.289; p < .05). Thus, the relationship between societal discontent at the individual level and intention to help at the seventh follow-up tended to be more strongly positive for higher levels of societal discontent at the country level. All results were obtained controlling for outcome variable measured at the baseline. To examine the interaction effect, we conducted simple slope analysis. When societal discontent at the country level was low (1 SD below the mean), the relationship between societal discontent at the individual level and intention to help at seventh follow-up was negative (b = À0.3434, SE = 0.1229, p = .0052), whereas when societal discontent at the country level was high (1 SD above the mean), this relationship was positive (b = 0.2355, SE = 0.1204, p = .0505). Controlling for age, gender and education did not change these patterns. Within the framework of the COVID-19 pandemic, the main purposes of the present research were to investigate the cross-sectional effects of societal discontent at the individual level, societal discontent at the country level and, particularly, their interaction, on COVID-related intention to help. Given the correlational nature of the data, we also tested the model with longitudinal data to verify the effect of the cross-level interaction on intention to help over time. Before testing our hypotheses, we investigated differences between countries in societal discontent and intention to help. The results showed that participants from Hong Kong S.A.R. reported the lowest level of satisfaction, while those from China reported the highest level of satisfaction with society. Furthermore, participants from Russia reported the lowest level of intention to help whereas those from Philippines reported the highest level of intention to help. However, it is important to note that responses to both constructs' variables may have been affected by social desirability. In fact, to maintain a socially favourable self-image, 'participants tend to underreport socially undesirable behavior and overreport socially desirable behavior' (Krumpal, 2013 (Krumpal, p. 2028 . Moreover, literature suggests that social desirability can vary across cultures. Lalwani, Shavitt, and Johnson (2006) reported that collectivistic individuals tend to appear more normatively appropriate, whereas individualistic people tend to emphasize personal skills and abilities. Concerning the two main aims of the present study, we wanted to examine the influence of societal discontent at the individual level on willingness to help and a hypothetical moderating effect of societal discontent at the country level on this relationship. We used multilevel modelling and found a negative main effect of societal discontent at the individual level on intention to help. Thus, the more individuals were dissatisfied with society, the less they were willing to help others who suffer from coronavirus. This result is in line with research showing that individuals dissatisfied with society (i.e., low political trust) are less prone to act pro-socially (Mariën & Hooghe, 2011) . Similarly, a work by Tyler (2006) proposed that distrust in societal structures is related to uncooperativeness (i.e., intention to help). Moreover, we did not find a predictive effect of societal discontent at the country level on intention to help. As argued by Postmes et al. (1999) , perceptions at the individual level can be discrepant from those at the collective level. In fact, individual and group dimensions represent different facets of personal identity (Turner, 1987) . This implies that personal-level judgements are different from group-level judgements (Major, 1994) . Thus, not finding a predictive effect of societal discontent at the country level, yet finding it at the individual level, is theoretically consistent (van der Bles et al., 2018). Finally, we found a moderating effect of societal discontent at the country level on the relationship between societal discontent at the individual level and intention to help. When people are dissatisfied with society, but this sentiment is not perceived by the community, the willingness to help decreases. However, when people are concerned about the state of society and, at the same time, this sentiment is experienced by the whole country, individuals are prone to act pro-socially. We found the same results when testing the model with longitudinal data. Our findings are consistent with research reporting that the experience of 'being in the same boat' increases helping behaviour (e.g., Richardson & Maninger, 2016) . The most relevant examples of such attempts have been investigated during natural disasters, wars or fight against chronic illness, situations in which different individuals encounter specific negative events (e.g., Coyne & Smith, 1994; Kaniasty & Norris, 1993; Khalaf, 2002) . Under those circumstances, being all affected by the same adversity increases the perception that the problem is common to everyone, and that people should stay together to cope with it, rather than facing the event alone (Afifi, Hutchinson, & Krouse, 2006) . Therefore, the perception of shared adversity motivates individuals to act prosocially in order to alleviate negative outcomes. In fact, as suggested by Midlarsky (1991) , helping each other can be an effective coping strategy for people under a stressful situation (i.e., concern and dissatisfaction with the state of society). Similarly, a study conducted by Piferi, Jobe, and Jones (2006) , reported that the exposure to a collective stressful event increased helping behaviour, such as donating money. As literature suggests, shared experience, or a common fate affecting individuals, indeed plays a key role in promoting mutual aid. In this regard, Zhang (2019) demonstrated that shared experience of risk promotes more cooperation in public goods provision compared to individual experience of risk. Thus, an adverse event, that is also perceived by the community, can motivate people to increase prosocial behaviour. In line with this reasoning, our findings show that individuals dissatisfied with society, who lived in a country with high levels of discontent, were more prone to help others who suffered from coronavirus. As with every research, this study has limitations. In the present research, we measured the intention to help others rather than the actual behaviour. Thus, future studies should address this issue by considering real helping behaviour. Moreover, our data come from a cross-sectional study, which is susceptible to common method/sources biases. However, common method/sources bias can inflate the entity of relationships between variables (i.e., 'main effects'), but simulation studies suggest that it may lead to an underestimation of interaction effects (Evans, 1985; McClelland & Judd, 1993) . This evidence reduces concerns about the possibility that the results for the interactive effect of societal discontent at the individual level and societal discontent at the country level may be explained by common method variance. Furthermore, in our analyses, we aggregated the perception of societal discontent to the country level, thus, reducing possible biases at the individual level, such as previous experiences, personality characteristics and personal background (Seibert, Silver, & Randolph, 2004) . In addition, the confirmation of our results with longitudinal data should be considered a further evidence that common method variance has unlikely explained our findings (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003) . Another limitation we might consider concerns the representativeness of the countries we included in the present study. In fact, in the cross-sectional analysis, only 20 out of 42 countries had a representative sample, while in the longitudinal analysis, none of the countries had a representative sample. However, following Kline's (2016) guidelines, both in cross-sectional and longitudinal models, we included only countries that had more than 150 participants, in order to guarantee an average degree of reliability for multilevel analysis. Following this reasoning, in the longitudinal analysis, we considered only 12 countries. Thus, the results of the longitudinal model should be referred just to those countries and cannot be generalized to countries worldwide. In conclusion, our study showed that societal discontent at the individual level decreases the willingness to help others. However, when the societal discontent is experienced by the entire community, citizens dissatisfied with society are more prone to act pro-socially. Those are interesting results for policies aimed to manage situation of social crisis, such that of the COVID-19 pandemic, that can induce societal discontent. When citizens are dissatisfied with the state of society, highlighting the fact that 'we are all in the same boat' can promote helping behaviour. This research received support from the New York University Abu Dhabi (VCDSF/75-71,015), the University of Groningen (Sustainable Society & Ubbo Emmius Fund) and the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (COV20/00086). The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at New York University Abu Dhabi (protocol HRPP-2020-42) and the Ethics Committee of Psychology at Groningen University (protocol PSY-1920-S-0390). The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Toward a theoretical model of communal coping in postdivorce families and other naturally occurring groups Current status of the empathy-altruism hypothesis Lockdown, quarantine measures, and social distancing: Associations with depression, anxiety and distress at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic among adults from Germany Couples coping with a myocardial infarction: Contextual perspective on patient selfefficacy Helping behavior and altruism: An empirical and conceptual overview Extending the benefits of recategorization: Evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping Effects of stress and commonality of fate on helping behavior Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in moderated multiple regression analysis Disentangling Societal Discontent and Intergroup Threat: Explaining Actions Towards Refugees and Towards the State Trust in government regarding COVID-19 pandemic and its associations with preventive health behaviour and prosocial behaviour during the pandemic: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study A personal dimension of human rights activism: Narratives of trauma, resilience and solidarity rwg: An assessment of within-group interrater agreement Intergenerational conflicts of interest and prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic A test of the social support deterioration model in the context of natural disasters Web appendix for using the actor-partner interdependence model to study the effects of group composition. Unpublished manuscript Civil and uncivil violence in Lebanon: A history of the internationalization of communal conflict Principles and practice of structural equation modeling Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: a literature review What is the relation between cultural orientation and socially desirable responding? From social inequality to personal entitlement: The role of social comparisons, legitimacy appraisals, and group membership Does political trust matter? An empirical investigation into the relation between political trust and support for law compliance: does political trust matter? Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects Helping as coping The socio-economic implications of the coronavirus pandemic (COVID-19): A review Giving to others during national tragedy: The effects of altruistic and egoistic motivations on long-term giving Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies Comparative processes in personal and group judgments: Resolving the discrepancy Online Crowdfunding Response to Coronavirus Disease We were all in the same boat": An exploratory study of communal coping in disaster recovery Cooperation and trust across societies during the COVID-19 pandemic Taking empowerment to the next level: A multiple-level model of empowerment, performance, and satisfaction The appeal of nostalgia: The influence of societal pessimism on support for populist radical right parties A general discontent disentangled: A conceptual and empirical framework for societal unease Populism and representative politics in contemporary Europe Economic, social and political issues raised by the COVID-19 pandemic Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory Psychological Perspectives on Legitimacy and Legitimation The consequences of collective discontent: A new measure of zeitgeist predicts voting for extreme parties Understanding collective discontents: A psychological approach to measuring zeitgeist Altruism born of suffering and prosocial behavior following adverse life events: A review and conceptualization Reactions to 9/11 as a function of terror management and perspective taking Common fate motivates cooperation: The influence of risks on contributions to public goods Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of the article at the publisher's website.