key: cord-0746893-4kbve16c authors: Bhatt, Mayank; Mahana, Bhuvan; Ko, Ji Hyun; Kolesar, Tiffany A.; Kanitkar, Anuprita; Szturm, Tony title: Computerized Dual-Task Testing of Gait Visuomotor and Cognitive Functions in Parkinson’s Disease: Test-Retest Reliability and Validity date: 2021-07-15 journal: Front Hum Neurosci DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.706230 sha: c81db1981b5c5df3bed3057e154cbfc03ecf44b5 doc_id: 746893 cord_uid: 4kbve16c BACKGROUND: Mobility and cognitive impairments in Parkinson’s disease (PD) often coexist and are prognostic of adverse health events. Consequently, assessment and training that simultaneously address both gait function and cognition are important to consider in rehabilitation and promotion of healthy aging. For this purpose, a computer game-based rehabilitation treadmill platform (GRP) was developed for dual-task (DT) assessment and training. OBJECTIVE: The first objective was to establish the test-retest reliability of the GRP assessment protocol for DT gait, visuomotor and executive cognitive function in PD patients. The second objective was to examine the effect of task condition [single task (ST) vs. DT] and disease severity (stage 2 vs. stage 3) on gait, visuomotor and cognitive function. METHODS: Thirty individuals aged 55 to 70 years, diagnosed with PD; 15 each at Hoehn and Yahr scale stage 2 (PD-2) and 3 (PD-3) performed a series of computerized visuomotor and cognitive game tasks while sitting (ST) and during treadmill walking (DT). A treadmill instrumented with a pressure mat was used to record center of foot pressure and compute the average and coefficient of variation (COV) of step time, step length, and drift during 1-min, speed-controlled intervals. Visuomotor and cognitive game performance measures were quantified using custom software. Testing was conducted on two occasions, 1 week apart. RESULTS: With few exceptions, the assessment protocol showed moderate to high intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) values under both ST and DT conditions for the spatio-temporal gait measures (average and COV), as well as the visuomotor tracking and cognitive game performance measures. A significant decline in gait, visuomotor, and cognitive game performance measures was observed during DT compared to ST conditions, and in the PD-3 compared to PD-2 groups. CONCLUSION: The high to moderate ICC values along with the lack of systematic errors in the measures indicate that this tool has the ability to repeatedly record reliable DT interference (DTI) effects over time. The use of interactive digital media provides a flexible method to produce and evaluate DTI for a wide range of executive cognitive activities. This also proves to be a sensitive tool for tracking disease progression. CLINICAL TRIAL REGISTRATION: www.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier NCT03232996. Safe, independent community walking requires both mobility skills and cognitive flexibility to manage complex terrains, for navigation in busy environments, to identify and track relevant visual targets, reading, and for processing of what is being seen. Mobility limitations (balance and gait impairments; Rochester et al., 2004; Kang et al., 2005) and decline in executive cognitive function (Olchik et al., 2017; Roheger et al., 2018) common to Parkinson's disease (PD) often coexist (Plotnik et al., 2011; Schneider et al., 2015) and are prognostic of adverse health events, including falls (Mak et al., 2014; Paul et al., 2014) . Over 60% of individuals with PD fall each year and a significant portion of those who fall will experience multiple falls (Pickering et al., 2007; Lord et al., 2017; Creaby and Cole, 2018) . The consequences of falls are often severe, leading to disability, loss of independence, and social isolation. The concept of motor automaticity has an important implication in the pathophysiology of PD related to mobility limitations and increased fall risk (Wu et al., 2015; Gilat et al., 2017) . One model of executive cognitive functions presents the supervisory attentional system, which distinguishes between processing of non-routine, attentionally demanding activities vs. routine, automated tasks (Dirnberger and Jahanshahi, 2013) . The role of the striatum has been equated with that of routine tasks, such as locomotion, that are usually performed automatically (Gilat et al., 2017) . The information processing demands of community ambulation are presumed to affect the already limited locomotor control of PD patients (Rochester et al., 2004; Kang et al., 2005) , and this may result in greater gait instability and a higher risk of falls (Heinzel et al., 2016; Penko et al., 2018) . Consequently, dual-task (DT) assessment and training programs that address both mobility and cognition are important to consider in the prevention and rehabilitation of mobility and cognitive decline. Impairments in gait (Rosenberg-Katz et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2015; Gilat et al., 2017) , and in executive functions (Monchi et al., 2001 (Monchi et al., , 2004 are associated with disruption of frontostriatal circuits in PD. This is consistent with findings of studies which have evaluated frontal lobe activity during DT-walking using functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Several fNIRS studies have reported increased activation levels of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) during DT-walking as compared to walk-only trials in healthy able-bodied adults (Fraser et al., 2016; Maidan et al., 2016) . Similar fNIRS studies evaluating PD patients have reported no difference in PFC activation levels for DT-walking trials as compared to walk-only trials (Holtzer et al., 2015; Maidan et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2021) . A recent positron emission tomography pilot study of DT-walking evaluated brain glucose metabolism in PD (Hoehn and Yahr stages 2 and 3) in ten patients who rested and five who conducted a DT treadmill/cognitive video game task during the uptake period (Szturm et al., 2020) . DT interference (DTI) was consistently shown for gait and cognitive game performance measures for the DT-walking group. As expected, glucose metabolism was significantly increased in several brain regions of the patients in the DT group, compared to the resting group, including the primary visual/sensorimotor areas, thalamus, superior colliculus, and cerebellum. Within the DT group, the three patients in the earlier stage of PD (Hoehn and Yahr stage 2) showed increased glucose uptake in the PFC during DT treadmill walking compared to the resting group, while the two patients at Hoehn and Yahr stage 3 did not. These behavioral and neuroimaging results suggest that simultaneous tasks may be accomplished via different mechanisms as neurodegeneration progresses in PD. A clinically valid and reliable treatment to assess and improve DT performance may translate to improved or slowed decline of cognitive performance and mobility. Most DT gait studies are performed overground and all overground studies report a significant decrease in gait speed during the DT-walking condition as compared to walk only trials (Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Rochester et al., 2014; Stegemöller et al., 2014; Salazar et al., 2017; Raffegeau et al., 2019) . Besides gait speed, many DT gait studies examine how cognitive demand affects gait rhythm and stability (i.e., recording spatiotemporal gait variables; Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2005; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012; Nankar et al., 2017) . However, gait speed is a confounding variable as spatiotemporal and other kinematic gait variables are sensitive to changes in gait speed (Jordan et al., 2007; Chung and Wang, 2010; Keene et al., 2016; Cole et al., 2017) . Most of the overground DT-walking studies use an instrumented walkway, which records only 4-6 consecutive steps (Bruijn et al., 2009; Hollman et al., 2010) . It has been shown that using a continuous walking protocol instead of short intermittent walks, and collecting more than 30 consecutive steps improved reliability, in particular for measures of gait variability (Bruijn et al., 2009; Hollman et al., 2010; Galna et al., 2013) . Furthermore, there is a limited choice of executive cognitive tasks that can be completed during overground walking, and for the short time period to walk a few meters. For example, common cognitive tasks used during overground walking include recall of names/words, serial subtraction, or auditory Stroop tests (Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Raffegeau et al., 2019) . Walking outdoors requires visual attention, search, and processing of what is being seen (i.e., relevant visual information). Cortical areas devoted to visuospatial processing of behaviorally significant information, such as the PFC may be particularly susceptible to the neurodegenerative process in PD (Klencklen et al., 2012; Battisto et al., 2018; Lally et al., 2020) . To overcome these limitations, Szturm et al. (2017 Szturm et al. ( , 2020 ) developed a computer game-based rehabilitation treadmill platform (GRP; Mahana et al., 2021) which provides an integrated approach to assess and treat the decline in balance, gait, visuomotor, and executive cognitive functions. The GRP consists of a standard treadmill instrumented with a pressure mapping system used to compute spatiotemporal gait variables and gait stability measures Ahmadi et al., 2018 Ahmadi et al., , 2019 ) and a computer display placed at the front of the treadmill for participants to view and interact with computer-generated activities while walking. The GRP includes an interactive computer game application for DT which allows for recording and analyzing participant's actions and choices while walking and playing targeted cognitive games. Therefore, gait and cognitive performance can be measured synchronously during continuous, speed-controlled, steady state gait trials, lasting minutes. In a previous study , reliability of the GRP assessment tool has been established in older adults with fall histories. The present study aims to establish the reliability of the GRP assessment protocol in individuals with PD, and to examine the influence that visuospatial cognitive tasks have on gait performance in Hoehn and Yahr stage 2 (PD-2) vs. stage 3 (PD-3) patients. The first objective of the present study was to establish test-retest reliability and the minimal detectable change (MDC) of spatio-temporal gait variables, as well as visuomotor and cognitive performance measures examined during single task (ST; i.e., walking only or playing video games while sitting) and DT (i.e., walking while playing video games) conditions. The second objective was to examine the effects of task condition (ST vs. DT conditions) and disease severity (stage 2 vs. 3) on gait, visuomotor and cognitive performance measures. This objective addressed three hypotheses, which are as follows: (1) increased visuospatial cognitive loads (ST-walking vs. DT-walking) will have a significant effect on gait performance measures in both PD-2 and PD-3 patients, and vice versa: increased physical demands (sitting vs. walking) will have a significant effect on visuomotor and cognitive performance measures (ST-gaming vs. DT-gaming); (2) increased cognitive loads (ST-walking vs. DTwalking) have a greater influence in gait performance measures in PD-3 patients as compared to PD-2 patients; and (3) increased physical demands (sitting vs. walking) will have a greater influence on visuomotor and cognitive performance measures in PD-3 patients as compared to PD-2 patients. Thirty PD patients were recruited for this study. The inclusion criteria included: (a) 50-75 years of age, (b) clinical diagnosis of PD (Hoehn and Yahr stages 2 and 3) made by a movement disorders specialist based upon the United Kingdom PD Society Brain Bank diagnostic criteria (Hughes et al., 1992) , (c) treated with antiparkinsonian medications, (d) able to walk at least 50 m without any assistance, and (e) adequate hearing and vision to perform the computer game task, i.e., while viewing an 80 cm monitor at a distance of 1 m. The exclusion criterions included (a) any significant cognitive impairment [Montreal cognitive assessment scores (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005) <25], (b) any other neurological disorder except PD, (c) with any musculoskeletal impairment or uncontrolled cardio-vascular condition that might prevent participants from walking on a treadmill for 2-4 min. The Human Research Ethics Board at the University of Manitoba approved the study, and the study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov. The Protocol Registration System number is NCT03232996. All participants provided informed consent. The participants were tested on two separate days, 1 week apart, and at the same time of day, during the ON medication phase. The Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS; Fahn et al., 1987) was completed in the first test session. Participants were instructed to take their medications at the same time prior to testing. Figure 1 presents the components of the treadmill platform. Participants stood on a treadmill at a viewing distance of 100 cm from an 80 cm computer monitor. The treadmill is instrumented with a pressure mat (Vista Medical, Ca) which was used to record vertical foot contact forces and to compute spatiotemporal gait variables (Rochester et al., 2014) . A miniature, wireless inertialbased (IB) mouse Scoop Pointer Remote (Model: RXR1000-0302E; Hillcrest labs) was used to control and interact with two computer applications described below. The IB mouse contains a 2-axis gyroscope and 2-axis accelerometer and firmware that provides an instantaneous angular position signal, and emulates a plug-n-play standard optical computer mouse. Velcro was used to secure the wireless motion mouse to a sports cap or plastic head band allowing head rotation to be used to point the device to control the position and motion of a computer cursor or game sprite. Therefore, a hands-free computer game controller was used to interact with various computer applications (cognitive activities) while treadmill walking. Head pointing movements are among the most natural and can easily be performed with minimal instruction. Two computer applications were developed for DT balance assessment. Details of the applications and protocols can be found in Szturm et al. (2017) . First, visuomotor tracking (VMT), as shown in Figures 1B,E , involves tracking a visual target that moves horizontally left and right on a computer display for several cycles. Two cursors of different shapes appear on the monitor. The target was a circle, its motion was computer controlled and moved at a predetermined frequency of 0.5 Hz with an amplitude of 80% of the monitor width. The second cursor, a rectangle, was slaved to head rotation via the IB mouse. Participants were instructed to move and overlap the rectangle head-controlled cursor with the target circle for several cycles. The second computer application involved a visuomotor cognitive game (VCG), as shown in Figures 1C,F ,G. The goal of this task is to move a game paddle (via head rotation) horizontally to interact with the moving game objects. The game objects are categorized as designated targets or designated distractors. The game objects appear at random locations at the top of the display every 2 s and move to the bottom of the display. The game objects move in a diagonal path from top to bottom. In response to each game event, participants produced a head rotation (i.e., rotation of the IB mouse) to move the game paddle to catch the target objects while avoiding the distractors. A logged data file is generated that synchronously records (100 Hz sampling rate) the coordinates of the game paddle and the game objects (tracking cursor, target, and distractor objects). These tasks were performed while walking on a treadmill at 0.9 m/s for 1 min. This speed was well tolerated by the PD-3 group in pilot-testing, allowing these patients to walk without falling or needing to lean on the treadmill supports. Four conditions were included: (a) walk only ST condition (WO), (b) DT-walk while performing a VMT task, (c) DT-walk while performing a VCG task, and (d) a ST gaming condition while seated (VMT and VCG games). Participants were not allowed to lean on the treadmill structure during testing. Prior to testing, the participants were allowed to play the games while seated for a few minutes to become familiar with each task. The following outcome measures were computed from the recorded pressure mat data; (a) average and coefficient of variation (COV) were computed over 30 consecutive steps, for right and left step length (SL) and swing time (SwT; 44), and (b) the location of all heel contacts in the mediolateral (ML) axis during each walk trial were determined, and the dispersion of heel contact locations were computed as the COV, and reported as ML-drift (Ahmadi et al., 2018) . Since the statistical analysis showed no significant difference in means of right and left gait variables, only the right gait variables are presented. Figure 1E presents synchronous plots of the computer-controlled target motion (circle cursor) and participants head rotation (rectangle cursor) for a typical VMT task. The total residual error (TRE) was determined by computing the difference between the trajectories of the target and head cursor motions for each sample period . The first two cycles of the VMT tasks were excluded to allow the participants time to acquire the moving target and begin tracking. Figure 1F presents the trajectory of an individual game movement response. The duration of each VCG trial was 60 s and each game event was 2 s. Therefore, 30 game movement responses were made, half in each direction. Based on the time indexes of each target appearance and disappearance, the software segments each individual game movement response and sorts them by direction. Figure 1G presents overlay plots of all game movement responses in one direction . The following variables were determined from the game movement responses: (a) average response time (Avg. RT), i.e., the time from target appearance to the start of the game paddle (head rotation), (b) success rate (SR) determined as the percentage of target objects that were caught, and (c) movement variance (MV). The individual movement traces for each direction in one game session were averaged and the standard deviation (SD) computed for each sampled data point. The total SD over all sampled data points was taken as movement variation. The sample size for this study was computed using Table 1 , of Zou (2012) . Thirty participants were required for an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) value of 0.8, assurance of 70%, and class interval half-width of 0.15. Normal distribution of the data was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk test, and Mauchly's test of sphericity was used to test the equality of variance for comparison between different task conditions and stages of PD. The gait, VMT and VCG performance measures satisfied the assumptions of normality and equal variance. Relative reliability was assessed using a two-way random model ICC. The ICC scores were considered to be high when equal to or greater than 0.70, moderate between 0.50 and 0.69, and low when less than 0.50 (Lexell and Downham, 2005) . MDC UPDRS Total (mean ± SD) 28.6 ± 9.9 43.6 ± 9.6 <0.001 UPDRS Motor (mean ± SD) 20.9 ± 7.7 30.8 ± 6.6 <0.001 MoCA (mean ± SD) 29.0 ± 1.0 28.7 ± 1.0 0.418 was computed according to a published guideline (Lexell and Downham, 2005) . Absolute reliability was analyzed using MDC (Messick, 1995; Haley and Fragala-Pinkham, 2006) . Systematic errors between the test periods were evaluated using a paired t-test. A repeated measures ANOVA was used to evaluate the effect of task condition (ST vs. DT), disease severity (Hoehn and Yahr stage 2 vs. 3) and the interaction of task and disease stage. The significance level was α set at p < 0.05. Effect size was calculated (Cohen's d); values less than 0.20 were considered negligible, values between 0.20 and 0.50 were considered small, between 0.50 and 0.80 were medium, and greater than 0.80 was considered large (Schäfer and Schwarz, 2019) . All statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS (v.22, SPSS Science, Chicago, IL, United States, RRID:SCR_019096). Table 1 presents the demographic and clinical data for PD-2 and PD-3 participants. The male to female ratios were similar in the two PD groups. There was a slight age difference between the two groups but this was not statistically different [t(28) = 1.678, p = 0.105]. Test scores for the MoCA were similar between groups (p > 0.7). The UPDRS scores (Total and Motor) were significantly greater in the PD-3 group as compared to the PD-2 group (p < 0.001). Presented are the group mean (standard deviation, SD) for test 1 and test 2 (occurring 1 week later), intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) values (95% confidence interval, CI), and minimal detectable change (MDC% as a percentage of the group mean). The values are for single-task walking only (WO), dual-task visuomotor cognitive game (VCG), and dual-task visuomotor tracking (VMT) tasks. Statistical results of the effect of task condition (WO vs. VMT/VCG), disease stage (PD-2 vs. PD-3), and task × stage interaction on the gait variables are presented in Table 4 . There was a significant main effect of task condition on all gait performance measures (p < 0.05) except for ML-drift between WO and VMT conditions (p = 0.1). There was a significant main effect of disease stage on all gait outcome measures (p < 0.05) except for Avg-SwT (p = 0.1). With one exception (COV of SwT for the VCG task, p = 0.008), there was no interaction effect (task × stage) on any of the gait outcome measures (p > 0.1). Group means and SD of the spatiotemporal gait variables by task condition and disease stage are presented in Figure 2 . As compared to the WO condition, the addition of the VMT and VCG tasks resulted in (a) a significant decrease in Avg-SwT and Avg-SL, and (b) a significant increase in COV-SL, COV-SwT, and ML-drift. As compared to the PD-2 group, the PD-3 group had significantly shorter Avg-SwT, larger COV values, and greater ML-drift. This was the case for WO, VMT, and VCG walk trials. Effect sizes for task condition and disease stage were similar, ranging from small to medium (0.20 to 0.52). The magnitude of DTI (i.e., effects of task condition) of cognitive demands on gait performance measures was similar in both PD-2 and PD-3 groups. The one exception was a greater magnitude of DTI on COV-SwT for the PD-3 group as compared to the PD-2 group. Effect sizes for task × stage interaction were negligible, less than 0.20. Statistical results of the effect of task condition and disease stage on VMT and VCG performance measures are presented in Table 5 . There was a significant effect due to task condition and disease stage on TRE (p < 0.001), but there was no significant task × stage interaction effect for TRE (p = 0.2). As evident in Figure 3 , TRE was significantly greater during DT-walking as compared to sitting, and was significantly greater in the PD-3 group compared to the PD-2 group. This was the case when tested in sitting and during DT-walking. There was a significant effect due to VCG task condition on movement onset time, success rate, and movement variation ( Table 5) . There was a significant effect due to disease stage on success rate but not on movement onset time or movement variation. There was no significant task × stage interaction effect on any of the three VCG outcome measures. As evident in Figure 3 , there was a significant decrease in success rate and a significant increase in movement onset time and movement variation when tested during DT-walking as compared to sitting. Also, success rate was significantly higher in the PD-2 group compared to the PD-3 group when tested in sitting and during DT-walking. Medium effect sizes for TRE (d = 0.56), success rate (d = 0.58), and movement variation (0.52) were observed for task condition. Small effects sizes for TRE (0.47) and success rate (0.20) were observed for disease stage. Effect sizes for task × stage interaction were negligible, less than 0.20. Overall, with a few exceptions, the computer game-based DT treadmill walking assessment protocol showed moderate to high test-retest reliability for the spatio-temporal gait measures, and VMT and VCG performance measures, under the DT condition. DTI was observed for all outcome measures of gait, VMT, and VCG with one exception: ML drift was not significantly different between WO and VMT trials. The limitation of a treadmill is the relatively narrow belt width, which does limit the degree to which a participant can drift leftward and rightward before stepping off the moving belt and falling. This constraint may have influenced the amount of ML drift during the VMT task. However, it is not clear why there was a significant increase in ML drift during the VCG tasks. The present reliability findings are consistent with the results of Strouwen et al. (2016) who examined test-retest reliability for average SL and average time during overground DT-walking in individuals with PD (Strouwen et al., 2016) . Participants walked on a GAITRite instrumented walkway of 4 m while performing backward-digit counting and an auditory Stroop test. Average SL and SwT and the cognitive performance measures (number of errors), demonstrated moderate test-retest reliability during DTwalking. However, it is not known whether gait speed was the same for task conditions and test periods in this study (Strouwen et al., 2016) . The present analysis extends these results to include measures of variation: COV-SL, COV-SwT, and ML-drift. In addition, in the present study, participants performed the VMT and VCG tasks for 60 s while walking (DT); the same steady state velocity was used for both DT and ST (walk only) conditions and test periods. Therefore, the present analysis was based on averages and variance of 30 movement cycles (VMT), 30 game movement responses, and on average, 30 consecutive right and left steps. In regard to absolute reliability, MDC% values for the average SL and SwT gait variables for WO, VMT and VCG walking trials were in the range of 15-20%. Values of MDC less than 20% would be considered an acceptable level for use in clinical trials (Koo and Li, 2016) . The COV and ML-drift outcome measures had high MDC% values, with a range of 52-75%. When evaluating the effectiveness of treatment programs, outcome measures having high MDC% values would require a greater amount of change from pre-to post-intervention in order to be considered significant. Thus, caution needs to be taken when using gait variability measures to examine change due to some intervention. In this regard, inclusion criteria may need to be restricted (for example to include only Stage 2 or Stage 3 PD patients) as to minimize subject variability. A significant reduction in average SL, and average SwT was observed when the VMT and VCG tasks were added. This was the case for both PD-2 and PD-3 groups. These findings for average gait variables would indicate a significant effect of visuospatial processing load on locomotor rhythm and pacing. There was also a significant increase in COV of SL, SwT, and ML-drift when the visuospatial processing loads were increased. Gait variability measures are important outcome measures as they reflect gait stability (Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2005; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012 Ahmadi et al., 2019) and are independent predictors of falls (Callisaya et al., 2011; Heinzel et al., 2016) . In addition, previous studies have reported increased ML excursions of the body during treadmill walking when exposed to moving visual scenes (Bauby and Kuo, 2000; O'Connor and Kuo, 2009) . Taken together, the present findings for COV-SL/SwT and ML-drift would indicate a main effect of visuospatial processing load on gait stability in PD. Most studies that examined DT-walking in PD patients have used cognitive tasks such as counting backward tasks, animal/word enumeration tasks, and auditory Stroop tests (Rochester et al., 2014; Stegemöller et al., 2014; Salazar et al., 2017; Raffegeau et al., 2019) . The present study extends these tasks to include VMT and VCG tasks, which require participants to actively rotate their heads in order to track (VMT) and to interact (VCG) with moving target objects and avoid distractor objects. Many different types of similar head pointing movements and gaze fixations occur during our daily activities such as walking and navigating through busy and complex environments, avoiding people and obstacles, shopping and searching for specific items, or during many recreational activities. A significant decline in performance of all visuospatial processing tasks was observed when tested during DT-walking, as compared to sitting. This was the case for both PD-2 and PD-3 groups. Schlachetzki et al. (2017) showed a significant decrease in gait speed, and SL of stage 3 participants as compared to stage 2. Hass et al. (2012) also reported a significant reduction in average SL, and average SwT of stage 3 participants compared to stage 2. The present study extends these findings and demonstrates a significant decline in gait, VMT and VCG performance measures for the PD-3 group compared to the PD-2 group during both ST and DT treadmill walking conditions. However, there was no significant difference in the magnitude of DTI (task × stage interaction) of cognitive demands on gait performance measures between the PD-2 and PD-3 groups. During the WO trials, SL was significantly less and gait variation and ML-drift were significantly greater in the PD-3 group compared to the PD-2 group. This may indicate that the PD-3 group is near the threshold level (capacity) beyond which a fall or loss of balance would occur, i.e., if the magnitude of DTI was higher in PD-3 patients, they would have fallen. For this reason, a greater increase of DTI in the PD-3 group would not be possible. Following the principle of neural overlap (Wollesen et al., 2016; Crockett et al., 2017) , DTI should be greatest when the cognitive and motor tasks engage the same neural circuits and processing resources, e.g., visual-spatial processing. Walking and maintaining balance on a moving treadmill requires attentional resources for continuous monitoring of rhythm, pacing, spatial orientation relative to borders of the treadmill belt, and to generate timely corrective responses to counter drift. This requires processing and organization of spatial information from multiple sensory systems, including visual spatial information. The VMT and VCG tasks used in the present study require sustained visual attention, visual search, cognitive inhibition and spatial processing of the moving targets, i.e., executive cognitive functions. The increased attentional demands and processing of spatial information to prevent drifting and, more seriously, falling, would compete for resources required to perform the VMT and VCG tasks, and vice versa. One limitation of the present study which is true of all DT studies relates to how individuals spontaneously prioritize their attention between the walking and the tracking/cognitive tasks. It is possible that walking was prioritized and participants did not attend to the tracking/cognitive activities with 100% effort. Alternatively, the information provided during the VMT and VCG tasks was received and processed and the performance was affected due to DTI. Modest performance levels were observed in the present study for both VMT and VCG tasks during walking. This would indicate the participants were attending to and processing the information they saw on the display. However, they may have stopped intermittently for a few seconds and prioritized locomotor processing (i.e., to correct rhythm, pacing and balance). Another limitation is that treadmill and overground are different types of walking tasks. However, treadmill walking involves repetitive stepping and dynamic stability requirements that are comparable to the demands of overground walking, as well the kinematics and kinetics of gait during treadmill and overground walking have very similar patterns and degree of variation (Watt et al., 2010; Hollman et al., 2016; Yao et al., 2019) . In the present assessment, treadmill was used to ensure a steady state gait at a controlled speed and within a narrow spatial range (width/length of treadmill walking surface). Also important is that treadmills can be easily equipped with a computer monitor to access digital media. This provides standardized repeatable computer tracking and cognitive game activities and thus presents the same information to all participants on both test days, and for all test conditions (sitting and during walking). In addition, the gait and cognitive outcome measures are quantified not just for a few seconds, but for durations of minutes. The absence of a healthy age-matched control group is another limitation. This analysis would provide added information as to differences in DTI due to age effects and disease stage. A study is in progress (paused due to to compare the effects of VMT and VCG on gait function (and vice versa) between age-matched able-bodied participants and stage 2 and 3 PD participants. Finally, the VMT and VCG tasks used in the present study involve both head rotation and information processing, and at this point we cannot rule out any intersegmental mechanical effect of the head rotation as a cause of the gait changes observed between the WO and the DT walk trials. Head rotations required for the VCG tasks were relatively small head rotations of between 20-40 degrees, and of 300-500 ms in duration. These small ramp head rotations about the vertical axis would not likely cause any deviation in position of the body center of mass in ML or AP directions, i.e., passive mechanical disturbance to gait. Duysens et al. (2008) examined center of pressure migration during an open-loop tracking task (up to 30 degrees of visual target motion) while treadmill walking. Three tasks were performed: tracking with eye movements only (head stationary), tracking by rotating the head in synchrony with the moving visual target (open-loop tracking task), and tracking while rotating the trunk in synchrony with the moving visual target. The results demonstrated no significant deviation of the center of pressure migration when participants performed the tracking task with eye or head rotation, whereas, trunk rotations led to a doubling of ML-center of pressure deviation. The mechanical effect of head rotation on gait rhythm pacing and variation will receive further investigation. The high to moderate ICC values along with the lack of systematic errors in the measures indicate that this tool has the ability to repeatedly record reliable DTI effects over time. This also proves to be a sensitive tool for tracking disease progression. A comprehensive analysis of spatial and temporal features of steady state gait has a greater validity to measure gait performance (rhythm, pacing, and stability) as compared to gait speed alone. The use of standardized, interactive computer applications provides a flexible method to produce and evaluate a wide range of executive cognitive activities while performing complex motor behaviors such as walking. Objective evaluation of VMT and VCG computer tasks provide important information about different aspects of information processing. While the focus of the present work is on assessment of DTI in PD, the GRP platform is directly applicable to other diseases that affect gait and cognition (e.g., cognitive vascular impairment, Alzheimer's disease, and aging). Future studies will examine the type and levels of DTI that are prognostic of falls and adverse health events. A randomized clinical trial is also planned to examine the feasibility and effectiveness of a computer gamebased DT training program for PD patients. Many designed cognitive games, as well as many engaging and inexpensive common and modern computer games can be used with the GRP to challenge a broad range of visuomotor and executive cognitive functions . The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation. The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Human Research Ethics Board at the University of Manitoba. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. MB, BM, and TS: study concept, design, and acquisition. MB, BM, JK, TK, AK, and TS: analysis, interpretation of data, and preparation of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version. The funding for this study was provided by the University of Manitoba's University Collaborative Research Program (Fund #: 50667), as well as the Weston Brain Institute (Fund #: TR193095), and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) postdoctoral fellowship (PDF-545615-2020). Comparison of selected measures of gait stability derived from center of pressure displacement signal during single and dual-task treadmill walking The effects of aging and dual tasking on human gait complexity during treadmill walking: a comparative study using quantized dynamical entropy and sample entropy Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis The body position spatial task, a test of whole-body spatial cognition: comparison between adults with and without Parkinson disease Active control of lateral balance in human walking Statistical precision and sensitivity of measures of dynamic gait stability Gait, gait variability and the risk of multiple incident falls in older people: a population-based study The change of gait parameters during walking at different percentage of preferred walking speed for healthy adults aged 20-60 years Imposed faster and slower walking speeds influence gait stability differently in Parkinson fallers Gait characteristics and falls in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis Resting state default mode network connectivity, dual task performance, gait speed, and postural sway in older adults with mild cognitive impairment Executive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease: a review How trunk turns affect locomotion when you are not looking where you go Unified Parkinson's disease rating scale Comparable cerebral oxygenation patterns in younger and older adults during dual-task walking with increasing load Effect of gait speed on gait rhythmicity in Parkinson's disease: variability of stride time and swing time respond differently Is gait variability reliable in older adults and Parkinson's disease? Towards an optimal testing protocol Dopamine depletion impairs gait automaticity by altering cortico-striatal and cerebellar processing in Parkinson's disease Interpreting change scores of tests and measures used in physical therapy Quantitative normative gait data in a large cohort of ambulatory persons with Parkinson's disease Motor dual-tasking deficits predict falls in Parkinson's disease: a prospective study Number of strides required for reliable measurements of pace, rhythm and variability parameters of gait during normal and dual task walking in older individuals A comparison of variability in spatiotemporal gait parameters between treadmill and overground walking conditions Online fronto-cortical control of simple and attention-demanding locomotion in humans Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases Walking speed influences on gait cycle variability Clinical characteristics in early Parkinson's disease in a central California population-based study The application of multilevel modelling to account for the influence of walking speed in gait analysis What do we know about aging and spatial cognition? Reviews and perspectives A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research Association between motor subtype and visuospatial and executive function in mild-moderate Parkinson disease How to assess the reliability of measurements in rehabilitation Cognitive and motor dual task on multi-area brain activation and gait performance in individuals with Parkinson's disease. Res. Sq Natural history of falls in an incident cohort of Parkinson's disease: early evolution, risk and protective features Does use of dual-task cognitive game-assisted treadmill system improve balance and gait in Parkinson's disease? A feasibility study The role of the frontal lobe in complex walking among patients with Parkinson's disease and healthy older adults: an fNIRS study Impaired executive function can predict recurrent falls in Parkinson's disease Standards of validity and the validity standards in performance assessment Neural bases of set-shifting deficits in Parkinson's disease Wisconsin Card Sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging SYNERGIC TRIAL (SYNchronizing exercises, remedies in gait and cognition) a multi-centre randomized controlled double blind trial to improve gait and cognition in mild cognitive impairment Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking, and risk of falls The interacting effects of treadmill walking and different types of visuospatial cognitive task: discriminating dual task and age effects The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment Direction-dependent control of balance during walking and standing Comparison trail making test between individuals with Parkinson's disease and health controls: suggestions of cutoff point The relative contribution of physical and cognitive fall risk factors in people with Parkinson's disease: a large prospective cohort study Dual-task interference disrupts Parkinson's gait across multiple cognitive domains A meta-analysis of six prospective studies of falling in Parkinson's disease Effects of cognitive function on gait and dual tasking abilities in patients with Parkinson's disease suffering from motor response fluctuations A meta-analysis: Parkinson's disease and dual-task walking The nature of dualtask interference during gait in incident Parkinson's disease Attending to the task: interference effects of functional tasks on walking in Parkinson's disease and the roles of cognition, depression, fatigue, and balance Progression of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease Fall risk is associated with amplified functional connectivity of the central executive network in patients with Parkinson's disease Dual tasking in Parkinson's disease: cognitive consequences while walking The meaningfulness of effect sizes in psychological research: differences between sub-disciplines and the impact of potential biases Wearable sensors objectively measure gait parameters in Parkinson's disease Relationship between motor symptoms, cognition, and demographic characteristics in treated mild/moderate Parkinson's disease Associations between cognitive and gait performance during single-and dual-task walking in people with Parkinson disease Test-retest reliability of dual-task outcome measures in people with Parkinson disease Imaging cerebral glucose metabolism during dual-task walking in patients with Parkinson's disease Changes in metabolic activity and gait function by dualtask cognitive game-based treadmill system in Parkinson's disease: protocol of a randomized controlled trial Computerized dual-task testing of gait and visuospatial cognitive functions; test-retest reliability and validity A three-dimensional kinematic and kinetic comparison of overground and treadmill walking in healthy elderly subjects Influence of a visual-verbal Stroop test on standing and walking performance of older adults Motor automaticity in Parkinson's disease Lower limb joint motion and muscle force in treadmill and over-ground exercise Sample size formulas for estimating intraclass correlation coefficients with precision and assurance The authors would like to thank the funding agencies that helped make this work possible, including the University of Manitoba Collaborative Research Program, the Weston Brain Institute, and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.