key: cord-0746591-d78h3zkk authors: Kiviniemi, Marc T.; Orom, Heather; Hay, Jennifer L.; Waters, Erika A. title: Prevention is political: political party affiliation predicts perceived risk and prevention behaviors for COVID-19 date: 2022-02-14 journal: BMC Public Health DOI: 10.1186/s12889-022-12649-4 sha: 459641e28bf85135a4e1a527f7f15c54cf338003 doc_id: 746591 cord_uid: d78h3zkk BACKGROUND: Many US politicians have provided mixed messages about the risks posed by SARS-CoV-2/COVID-19 and whether and to what extent prevention practices should be put in place to prevent transmission. This politicization of the virus and pandemic may affect individuals’ risk perceptions and willingness to take precautions. We examined how political party affiliation relates to risk perception for one’s own and other people’s likelihood of SARS-CoV-2 infection/COVID-19 illness. METHODS: We surveyed members of a nationally-representative, probability-sampling based survey panel (N = 410) to examine their risk perceptions, precautionary behaviors, and political party affiliation. RESULTS: The more strongly one identified as a Republican, the less risk one perceived to oneself from SARS-CoV-2/COVID-19 and the less risk one perceived other people faced. Moreover, those identifying as more strongly Republican engaged in fewer preventive behaviors. CONCLUSIONS: This differential response may affect virus transmission patterns and poses a considerable challenge for health communications efforts. As of November 23, 2021, there were over 43.6 million SARS-CoV-2 infections in the US [1, 2] and more than 700,000 COVID-19 deaths [1, 3] . Both the infection and the death rates likely substantially underestimate the true population impact given that there are nearly 300,000 excess deaths in the US since the start of the pandemic [4, 5] . In addition to the mortality burden, there are longterm cardiac, respiratory, and other health consequences for COVID-19 patients [6, 7] . In many areas of the US, infection rates have shifted over time with changes in policy-mandated preventive strategies, decreasing when prevention-oriented policies were in place and then increasing again as prevention strategies were eased [8] . A centerpiece of public health strategies to control infectious disease spread is either recommending or mandating that members of the public engage in protective behavior to prevent disease transmission. In the case of COVID-19, staying 6 ft apart, wearing masks, working remotely when possible, avoiding public gatherings, and other strategies have been endorsed and communicated to the public by CDC, FEMA, WHO, and other national and international public health agencies [9] [10] [11] . These preventive strategies are effective at slowing the rate of COVID-19 infection [12] [13] [14] . However, for infectious disease prevention behaviors to be effective, they must be undertaken consistently by a sufficient proportion of the population to slow transmission [15] [16] [17] . Most theoretical and empirical treatments of risk perception focus on individual cognitive and reasoning processes as the key determinant of a person's risk perceptions [18, 19] . In contrast, the social amplification of risk framework [20, 21] describes the process by which scientific evidence, the ways in which people obtain information (e.g., news media), and political and cultural forces shape how individuals interpret and prioritize health risk information. According to this framework, perceptions of risk can be amplified or attenuated through social processes that influence: 1) the availability of risk information (e.g., via media and political sources) and 2) society's response to the information (e.g., discourse about the veracity of the risk information in the media and interactions with cultural and peer groups [20] ). Influential communicators such as social/activist organizations and opinion leaders among social groups or organizations are key sources of information and can influence discourse surrounding risk information [21] . For example, political groups, parties, and leaders are prominent examples of influential communicators who may shape how their members and affiliates interpret risk information. These amplification and attenuation processes through influential communicators likely contribute to an alignment between, on one hand, people's political affiliation and their underlying values and, on the other hand, their perceived risk. During the pandemic, politicians in multiple countries have sought to control the amount and kind of information the public received about COVID-19 risk as well as actively disputing scientific discourse about the risk. Residents of several countries received messages from political leaders that minimized risk and raised doubt about preventive strategies. For example, the Prime Minister of Great Britain, Boris Johnson, publicly announced that he would not engage in social distancing and, specifically, would continue to shake hands [22] less than a week before the British government began to plan policy strategies to prevent transmission [23] . In Brazil, President Jair Bolsonaro made public statements that minimized the perception that the virus posed a risk, comparing it to the flu [24] and actively arguing against preventive policy strategies that were being put in place in Brazilian cities [25, 26] . Similar minimizing and contradictory statements can be found from the leaders of other countries [27] . In the United States, the country of focus in the current study, then President Donald Trump regularly made public statements that downplayed the threat posed by the virus [28, 29] in terms of both severity, comparing it to the flu [30] , and the number of cases and deaths [31] . President Trump also downplayed the importance of preventive actions to protect against transmission, including arguing against mask mandates [32] , pushing for early easing of stay at home and business closure orders [33] , and holding public gatherings in spite of social distancing policies in place [34] . The President admitted to being motivated to downplay the risk despite receiving clear warnings about the severity and seriousness of the spread of COVID-19 ([35] p. xviii). In addition to risk perception, individuals' decision making about COVID-19 prevention behaviors has taken place in the context of a complex, saturated, fast-moving information environment, with multiple and sometimes contradictory messages from traditional media, social media, and government messaging [36] . There is evidence that political messaging has an influence on behavior for individuals who support the politician conveying the messaging. Specifically, a study of the impact of President Trump's anti-vaccination messaging found that the exposure to the messages negatively impact vaccination engagement intentions, but only on the part of voters who voted for him [37] . Based on the social amplification of risk framework, one would expect that the selective communication of risk information by politicians and the conflating of politics and public health in media messaging would influence the public's perceptions of risk and their decision making about behavioral strategies to mitigate risk. Moreover, given the current US phenomenon where some news media outlets provide partisan lenses on issues, one would expect this effect to be exacerbated as news media "amplify" the messaging about risk perception [38] . Thus, one would predict that political affiliation would, by affecting exposure, attention, processing, and response to SARS-CoV-2/COVID-19 messages, influence risk perception and behavior. Given the empirical evidence and plausible, theoryderived mechanisms for the role of social amplification in risk perception and decision making about risk reduction messages, we hypothesize that political party affiliation will relate to engagement in preventive measures for SARS-CoV-2/COVID-19, such that -given the predominant minimization themes in President Trump's statements, those who identify more strongly as Republicans will be less likely to engage in preventive measures and will perceive less risk relative to those who identify more strongly as Democrats. We examined whether Americans' political party identification is related to perceived risk of SARS-CoV-2 infection, the severity of COVID-19 illness, and engagement in a range of preventive behaviors. Although there have been examinations of how political affiliation relates to support for preventive measures against COVID-19 (e.g., policy measures to restrict gatherings, mask wearing), our approach adds to this literature in two ways. First, we utilize a nuanced, quasi-continuous assessment of strength of party affiliation, allowing for an understanding of how strength of political beliefs affect responses to COVID-19. Second, we examine the effects of political partisanship on risk perceptions for COVID-19 infection in addition to support for preventive measures, adding to the understanding of how politicians' risk minimizing messages might affect individuals' perceptions of the risks posed by COVID-19. We conducted a cross-sectional, population-representative national survey of US adults. The study was reviewed and determined exempt by the University of Kentucky Institutional Review Board. A random sample of 410 members of the Ipsos Knowl-edgePanel [39] were surveyed by Ipsos. KnowledgePanel is a survey panel recruited using probability-based sampling techniques. The sampling frame for the panel is therefore population representative of US adults aged 18 and older and non-institutionalized. The survey was conducted over 6 days in mid-June 2020. Given the novelty of COVID-19 risk perceptions and behaviors at the time, there were not existing data on which to base an effect size estimate. Therefore, the necessary sample size was calculated to allow for an 80% power to detect a "small effect" (Cohen's f 2 > = 0.02) for the relation between party affiliation an outcomes. Political affiliation was assessed using the methods of the American National Election Studies [40] . Participants were first asked to indicate whether they identified as a Democrat, Republican, Independent, another party, or no preference. Those indicating Democrat or Republican were then asked a follow-up question in which they were asked whether their party affiliation was "strong" or "not very strong". Those indicating independent were then asked whether they more strongly identified with the Republican or the Democratic party. These two questions were used to create a 6-point party affiliation measure ranging with levels of Strong Republican, Weak Republican, Independent-Lean Republican, Independent Lean Democrat, Weak Democrat, and Strong Democrat. Only 11 of the 410 respondents reported no preference or another party affiliation (2.6% of the sample, a percentage consistent with other nationally-representative assessments of party affiliation [41] ). Given their very small number, these respondents were not included in the political affiliation analyses. We assessed risk perception for two different referents -participants' perception of their own risk for SARS-CoV-2 infection and their perception of the risk for an average person in the US (other risk). We asked about four different components of risk perception that have all been shown to be important in behavioral adoption. First, we asked participants to report perceptions of the absolute risk for infection using a measure modified from the Health Information National Trends Survey [42, 43] ("How likely are you to get COVID-19 in the next 6 months?"). Second, we asked participants to report the how much fear they experienced concerning the virus [44] ("How afraid are you of getting COVID-19 in the next 6 months?"). Third, we asked participants to provide a gist-based assessment of how likely they felt infection was to happen to them [45, 46] ("How easily do you feel you could get COVID-19 in the next 6 months?"). Fourth and finally, we asked participants to report how severe infection would be were it to occur to them (Modified from [47] ; "If you were to get COVID-19 in the next 6 months, how serious would it be?"). Each risk perception question was assessed with a four-point scale, with a score of 1 indicating the lowest level of perceived risk and a score of 4 indicating the highest level of perceived risk. Participants reported their engagement in 10 preventive behaviors over the previous week. The ten behaviors were drawn from public health guidance at the time of the survey and included behaviors related to sanitization (e.g., wash hands), transmission control (e.g., wear mask), and social distancing (e.g., avoid visiting with others in person). The full list of behaviors can be found in Table 1 . For each behavior, participants were asked if they had done each behavior in the past 7 days (yes/no). Analyses include both dichotomous individual behavior measures for each individual behavior as well as a count of the number of the ten behaviors in which participants engaged. The IPSOS KnowledgePanel survey team calculated adjusted design weights to address potential differential non-response to the survey. These adjusted design weights were calculated by assessing the distribution of US adults from the most recent fielding of the Current Population Survey. All reported analyses were conducted using STATA version 16.1 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX) and incorporated design weights to provide population representative descriptive and inferential estimates. Given that political affiliation is not independent of demographic characteristics, all reported analyses control for age, education, ethnicity, gender, income, and rural/urban residence. To examine the relation between political affiliation and engagement in preventive behaviors, we estimated linear regression models for the overall number of preventive behaviors participants reported. The six-level party identification variable was used as a continuous predictor variable, following the ANES recommendations, and the number of behaviors reported was modelled as a continuous outcome variable. We then estimated a separate logistic regression model for each of the individual behaviors, with behavioral engagement as a dichotomous (no, yes) outcome variable and party identification as a continuous predictor variable. To identify the relation between political affiliation and risk perception, we estimated linear regression models for each component of perceived risk and for each referent target. In each model, perceived risk (e.g., absolute risk for the average person in the us) was modelled as a continuous outcome variable and political affiliation was modelled as a continuous predictor variable. Six hundred eighty-three adults drawn from a representative sample of the Knowledge Panel were invited to participate. The survey was completed by 410 participants (completion rate of 60% [48] ). The weighted demographics of the sample, per sampling design, mirror those of the US population. Of particular importance for this paper, 17% of the population identified as strongly Republican, 10% as weakly Republican, 20% as Independent/Lean Republican, 15% as Independent/Lean Democrat, 15% as weakly Democrat, and 23% as strongly Democrat. Individuals' perceptions of their own personal risk, their degree of feeling fear about the possibility of infection, and their feelings of risk are reported in Table 2 . As indicated by the positive slopes for the relation of political affiliation (in which higher numbers indicate more strongly Republican affiliation) and risk (where higher numbers indicate greater risk), for all three categories of risk, the sense of personal risk of infection rises as one shifts from more strongly Republican to more strongly Democratic Party identification. Also reported in Table 2 are individuals' perceptions of the average US adult's risk, their degree of feeling fear about the possibility of infection for the average adult, and their feelings of risk for the average adult. As with personal risk, the perception of the average US adult risk of infection rises steadily as one shifts from more strongly Republican to more strongly Democratic Party identification. Table 2 contains associations between political affiliation and perceptions of how serious/severe an infection would be for both the individual respondent and the average US adult. As with risk perception, perceptions of both own and other severity increase as party identification moves from more strongly Republican to more strongly Democratic. The number of preventive behaviors participants reported engaging in as a function of party identification is presented in Fig. 1 . Although respondents of all political party identifications reported some preventive behaviors, as can be seen in the figure, the number of preventive behaviors increases as party identification shifts from more strongly Republican to more strongly Democratic; linear trend b = 0.30, t(396) = 3.59, p < .001 (95% CI 0.14, 0.47). In followup analyses, both the independent-leaning Democrat and the strong Democrat categories were significantly different than the strongly Republican (both t(396) > 3.06, both p < 0.01). Table 1 reports the percentage of respondents reporting engaging in each individual behavior in the past 7 days, separated by political party identification. For six of the ten preventive behaviors, engagement in the behavior became significantly more likely as one shifted from stronger identification with the Republican Party to stronger identification with the Democratic Party (e,g, 80% of strong Democrats reported avoiding in person visits, whereas only 48% of strong Republicans reported the same behavior; ORs range from 1.18 to 1.53; the measure of party affiliation ranges from strongly Republican to strongly Democratic, so ORs above 1 indicate that behavior becomes more likely as one moves toward being more strongly Democratic). The only opposite effect was for hand washing, where likelihood became significantly lower with a shift from Republican to Democratic identification (OR 0.53). There was not a significant difference for disinfecting surfaces, avoiding touching face, or use of hand sanitizers. Note that the analysis for handwashing did NOT include demographic covariates, because so few participants reported not handwashing that there were multiple missing cells for demographics/behavior combinations. Political party affiliation was consistently related to the degree of risk people perceived from SARS-CoV-2 infection and COVID-19 disease. This finding was consistent across multiple components of risk perception and for both perceptions of the person's own risk and perceptions of risk to other people. Moreover, party affiliation was associated with the number of preventive behaviors a person engaged in and, for most the individual preventive behaviors, with the probability that a given person engaged in each individual behavior. These findings are consistent with both laypeople's observations of differences in behavior and with press reports over the course of the pandemic. In addition, they mirror non peer-reviewed reports [49] . There has been parallel work at a community/geographical level of analysis showing that the overall political makeup of counties in the US influences the degree to which residents restricted mobility during stay at home orders [50] and engaged in social distancing [51] . Importantly, to our knowledge this paper represents the first report from a nationally-representative, probability-based sample to examine both risk perceptions and preventive behaviors, thus providing strong descriptions of the effects of partisanship on risk perceptions and preventive behaviors among the American public. In addition, the finding that partisanship affects both beliefs about risk and actions that are intended to mitigate risk highlights that complex and multifaceted ways in which public responses to the pandemic have become politicized. Also, we find that our effects extend past perceptions of one's own risk to the little studied but, in the context of infectious disease, equally important construct of perceived risk to others. Finally, our work extends beyond previous studies focusing on single behavioral responses (e.g., social distancing [51] , vaccination [52] to demonstrate a remarkably consistent partisanship effect across a range of preventive behaviors with very different levels of effort, social involvement, etc. Each of these characteristics increases confidence in the conclusion that politicization of the coronavirus pandemic and of preventive measures influences the way in which the American public's responses to the pandemic have played out over the past several months. Finally, although our findings are framed in the context of SARS-CoV-2/COVID-19, they are likely to extend to other scientific and public health issues that become politicized. As described in the introduction, President Trump's anti-vaccination Twitter messages have had a demonstrable effect on childhood vaccination attitudes of his supporters [37] . Politicization of public health has occurred in the past in response to such now commonplace measures as motorcycle helmet laws, milk pasteurization, water fluoridation, and mandatory seat belt use in automobiles [53] [54] [55] . Thus, the implications of these findings extend past the current pandemic to the more general environment around science, public health, public policy, and community action to protect health and well-being. The current SARS-CoV-2/COVID-19 public health emergency provides a clear illustration that social factors, including political party identification and the messaging and activities of politicians and leaders from those parties, influence risk perceptions and preventive behavior. There are, of course, multiple plausible mechanisms that might explain the effect. First, as discussed in the introduction, President Trump and other politicians directly provided messaging about the pandemic. The President's messaging overwhelmingly focused on minimizing perceptions of risk and in some cases directly undermining public health messaging about precautionary behaviors [35] . Partisan messaging from both politicians and the media includes false risk dichotomies. For example, politicians downplayed the risk of the virus to the public amplified their negative economic impact as summarized by the President's tweet that "WE CANNOT LET THE CURE BE WORSE THAN THE PROBLEM ITSELF". Such sentiment was echoed in the statements of governors who resisted shutdowns even in the face of rapidly increasing virus spread. Analysis of politicians' messaging showed that Democratic members of Congress communicated about the health-related and riskrelated aspects of the pandemic both earlier and more than their Republican counterparts, who were more likely to focus on business and economy messaging [56] . Because source credibility is a key factor in interpretation of information [57, 58] , the matching of more health and risk focused messaging from Democratic leaders being seen as credible by those affiliating with the Democratic party versus risk minimizing messaging from those seen as credible by those identifying as Republicans could account for the findings we report here. Consistent with this argument, recent work using statements by then candidate Trump showed that regardless of the truth or falsehood of the statement, Trump supporters more strongly believed in the statement when it was attributed to the president than when it was not [59] . When media outlets and politicians selectively communicate risk information this is likely to attenuate perceptions of risk to a greater degree among those who most trust these sources -in this case Republicans. In addition, direct spreading of misinformation about SARS-CoV-2 infection prevention as well as COVID-19 severity and treatment has been a prominent feature of the media environment [60, 61] . Some of this has resulted in illness, injury, or death to individuals who acted based upon it [62, 63] . In the early stages of the pandemic, media outlets with a more right-wing/conservative leaning orientation discussed COVID-19 misinformation more frequently than did those outlets that were more left-wing/liberal leaning [38] . President Trump was a key spreader of this misinformation, both through his Twitter account and through amplification in traditional medial sources. The President himself was cited as an information source by 37.9% of media articles containing misinformation [64] . Such scientific misinformation is likely to undermine trust in science, and trust in science is associated with following recommendations for disease prevention [65] . Susceptibility to misinformation may be even higher when it is presented by a communicator who is aligned with the recipient's partisan position, as is the case for Republicans for communications from President Trump, since accepting misinformation is more likely when the recipient attends to ancillary cues (like partisanship) rather than focusing on judging the accuracy of the message [66] . Finally, ideological differences or other factors may underlie both political party affiliations and risk perceptions and behavioral responses. Public health interventions are shaped and constrained by the tension between individuals' rights and community-level goals of protecting the public health [53, 67, 68] . Ideological and political arguments between individual rights and community safety have impacted implementation of public health actions [53, 69] . In the US, more politically conservative individuals expressed lower willingness to vaccinate, a relation that is partly related to lower trust in government medical officials [70] . This tension has been seen in early responses to COVID-19, with some state and national leaders opposing public health mandates on personal liberty grounds [29] . Similar to the COVID-19 opposition, there has been a trend in anti-vaccination messaging towards more policy-focused arguments [71] . In addition, factors such as health literacy, general perceptions of health, or broader beliefs about the nature of health might differ across party lines. Opposition to public health mandates are problematic during pandemics because policy level interventions, many of which mandate engagement in or avoidance of particular behaviors [72] , have profound effects on behavior. In the context of COVID-19, key policies that may affect transmission include mandated mask wearing, closure of non-essential businesses and activities, social distancing requirements, screening for disease, and testing. The polarization of people's beliefs about these threats resulting from politicized messaging creates barriers to coordinated public health responses to mitigate the pandemic. COVID-19) Cases in the US Coronavirus Locations: COVID-19 Map by County and State Provisional Death Counts for Coronavirus Disease 2019 -Daily Updates of Totals by Week and State Estimation of excess deaths associated with the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States Excess Deaths Associated with COVID-19, by Age and Race and Ethnicity -United States Long-term health consequences of COVID-19 As their numbers grow, COVID-19 "long haulers" stump experts How to prepare and protect yourself Kentucky Public Health. Team Kentucky Coronavirus Home Page Association of Public Health Interventions with the epidemiology of the COVID-19 outbreak in Wuhan, China An investigation of transmission control measures during the first 50 days of the COVID-19 epidemic in China Reducing transmission of SARS-CoV-2 To mask or not to mask: modeling the potential for face mask use by the general public to curtail the COVID-19 pandemic Community use of face masks and COVID-19: evidence from a natural experiment of state mandates in the US Dynamic models of meningococcal carriage, disease, and the impact of serogroup C conjugate vaccination The theory of decision making Perceived risk, real risk: social science and the art of probabilistic risk assessment The social amplification of risk: a conceptual framework The social amplification of risk -theoretical foundations and empirical applications What's up, doc? The PM's still shaking The realisation has struck no 10 that Britain has lost control of Covid-19, but it should at least look as though it is doing something Brazil's leader dismisses 'the little flu' . In: Y; 2020 Dismissing health response as 'hysteria' is an insult Nimble to treat past health crises, Brazil stumbles over this one Brazil's president has aligned himself with 'ostrich' dictators by denying the risks his people face Bolsonaro cannot hide from Covid-19 for ever How Trump was out of step with the CDC during coronavirus response As coronavirus cases rise nationwide, public health experts urge caution Press Conference: Donald Trump Provides an Update on the Coronavirus Outbreak Remarks by president trump, vice president pence, and members of the coronavirus task force in press conference Donald Trump vows not to order Americans to wear masks Trump Says States Can Start Reopening While Acknowledging the Decision Is Theirs Trump rallies 2.0: behind the curtain at the president's campaign events in the COVID-19 era. USA Today New York: Simon & Schuster 676. • fast, convenient online submission • thorough peer review by experienced researchers in your field • rapid publication on acceptance • support for research data, including large and complex data types • gold Open Access which fosters wider collaboration and increased citations maximum visibility for your research: over 100M website views per year submit your research ? Choose BMC The psychological roots of antivaccination attitudes: a 24-nation investigation How right-leaning media coverage of COVID-19 facilitated the spread of misinformation in the early stages of the pandemic in the U.S. Can American National Election Studies. ANES question search Gallup Organization. Party affiliation The health information National Trends Survey (HINTS): development, design, and dissemination Associations of perceived risk and worry with cancer health-protective actions: data from the health information National Trends Survey (HINTS) Affective components of risk mediate the relation between cognitively-based perceived risk and colonoscopy intentions The importance of affectively-laden beliefs about health risks: the case of tobacco use and sun protection Physical activity: the relative associations with cognitive and affective risk beliefs Health beliefs and compliance with mammography-screening recommendations in asymptomatic women Computing response metrics for online panels Partisanship, health behavior, and policy attitudes in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic Political partisanship influences behavioral responses to governors' recommendations for COVID-19 prevention in the United States Polarization and public health: partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic A future vaccination campaign against COVID-19 at risk of vaccine hesitancy and politicisation Political ideology and tobacco control The fluoridation dialogue: is it an ideological conflict? A study of the anti-scientific attitude Elusive consensus: polarization in elite communication on the COVID-19 pandemic The persuasiveness of source credibility: a critical review of five Decades' evidence Theoretical foundations of campaigns Processing political misinformation: comprehending the trump phenomenon The lancet infectious D. the COVID-19 infodemic Deadly masks' claims debunked 2020 FDA cautions against use of hydroxychloroquine or chloroquine for COVID-19 outside of the hospital setting or a clinical trial due to risk of heart rhythm problems Knowledge and practices regarding safe household cleaning and disinfection for COVID-19 prevention -United States Coronavirus misinformation: quantifying sources and themes in the COVID-19 'infodemic' . Ithaca: The Cornell Alliance for Science Modeling compliance with covid-19 prevention guidelines: the critical role of trust in science Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention Public health and individual liberty On lobbies, liberty, and the public good Attitude roots and Jiu Jitsu persuasion: understanding and overcoming the motivated rejection of science The influence of political ideology and trust on willingness to vaccinate Facebook pages, the "Disneyland" measles outbreak, and promotion of vaccine refusal as a civil right For the Public's health: revitalizing law and policy to meet new challenges Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations Not applicable. All authors conceptualized the study. MTK coordinated data collection, conducted data analysis, prepared the manuscript, and provided funding. All authors reviewed the manuscript. The authors read and approved the final manuscript. Not applicable. Data collection was supported by the Development Dimensions International Endowed Professorship Fund (MTK). Data, syntax code, and survey items are available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author. Ethics approval and consent to participate All study activities were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the University of Kentucky institutional review board. Participants were part of a standing IPSOS survey panel. IPSOS obtained informed consent from study participants at the time of joining the panel. Not applicable. The authors declare that they have no competing interests.