key: cord-0726202-ex32j5pk authors: Wong, Alex H.K.; Glück, Valentina M.; Boschet, Juliane M.; Engelke, Paula title: Generalization of extinction with a generalization stimulus is determined by learnt threat beliefs date: 2020-10-22 journal: Behav Res Ther DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103755 sha: ac6f371a99aa8589786033f27a6d6db64120d8a0 doc_id: 726202 cord_uid: ex32j5pk Expectancy violation refers to the mismatch between an expected and the actual outcome. Maximizing expectancy violation is crucial for exposure-based treatment. Since the original stimulus of fear acquisition (CS+) is rarely available, stimuli that resemble the CS+ (generalization stimuli; GSs) are presented during treatment. A given GS may evoke either strong or weak generalized fear depending on an individual's threat beliefs. Presenting this GS in extinction would then evoke different levels of expectancy violation, which determines the strength of the subsequent generalization of extinction to other stimuli, including the CS+. After differential fear conditioning, participants exhibited discrete generalization gradients depending on their inferred relational rules (Linear vs Similarity). Crucially, the Linear group showed strong generalized fear to the GS used in extinction. This strong expectancy violation led to enhanced extinction learning and subsequently to strong generalization of extinction as characterized by a flat generalization gradient, and reduced conditioned fear to the CS+. In contrast, the Similarity group showed weak generalized fear to the same GS in extinction, and limited generalization of extinction. These results corroborate the importance of expectancy violation in exposure-based treatment, and suggest that exposure sessions designed to evoke strong threat beliefs may lead to better treatment outcome. Exposure-based therapy, a pillar of cognitive behavioural therapy, is considered one of the most effective treatments for anxiety disorders (Carpenter et al., 2018; Hofmann & Smits, 2008) . Exposure-based therapy involves repeatedly presenting fear-provoking stimuli without any aversive outcome, thereby disconfirming an individual's threat belief to these fear-related stimuli. Disconfirmation of threat beliefs involves expectancy violation through a mismatch between an expected outcome and the actual outcome . Expectancy violation has been proposed to be a major factor influencing exposure treatment outcome (Craske et al., 2014 , hence suggesting a positive association between challenging patients' threat beliefs and treatment outcome. In the laboratory, exposure-based therapy is modelled via fear extinction within a Pavlovian conditioning framework. Fear extinction refers to the repeated presentation of a conditioned stimulus (CS+) alone that previously signalled the occurrence of an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US). As a result of the CS -no US presentation, conditioned fear to the CS+ gradually decreases across extinction trials. The fear extinction model is proposed to be a valid laboratory model for exposure-based therapy (Scheveneels, Boddez, Vervliet & Hermans, 2016) . However, in exposure-based therapy, the exact same stimuli or situations at the time of fear acquisition (i.e., the original CS+) are unlikely to be reproduced. Therefore, cues that resemble the original fear-related stimuli are presented during exposure-based treatment. This use of extinction stimuli that are similar, but not identical to the original CS+, is equivalent to presenting a generalization stimulus (GS) that perceptually or conceptually resembles the CS+ during extinction in a laboratory model. Empirical studies have shown that extinction learning to a GS generalized weakly to the CS+ in test (e.g., Barry, Griffith, Vervliet & Hermans, 2016; Vervliet & Geens, 2014; Vervliet, Vansteenwegen, Baeyens, Hermans & Eelen, 2005; Vervliet, Vansteenwegen & Eelen, 2004; Vervoort, Vervliet, Bennett & Baeyens, 2004; Wong & Lovibond, 2020; Zbozinek & Craske, 2018) , or to J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f another novel GS in test (e.g., Vervliet et al., 2004; Vervoort et al., 2014; Wong & Lovibond, 2020; Zbozinek & Craske, 2018) , as indicated by little to no decrease in conditioned fear to the respective test stimulus. The weak generalization of GS extinction potentially results from weaker activation of conditioned fear to GSs than to a CS+, due to the CS+ not sharing all its excitatory elements with the GS (i.e., generalization decrement; Mackintosh, 1974) . Therefore, these unshared excitatory elements do not gain inhibitory strength during GS extinction. As a result, stimuli that consist of these unshared elements (the CS+ or other novel GSs) still being able to evoke fear after GS extinction. This renders a weak generalization of GS extinction effect (for details, see Blough, 1975; Pittig, Treanor, LeBeau & Craske, 2018) . Struyf, Hermans and Vervliet (2018) thus proposed that presenting a GS that activates at least as much conditioned fear as the CS+ in extinction may produce a strong generalization of GS extinction effect. In support of this notion, the use of an asymmetrical stimulus dimension (i.e., intensity of one end of the dimension is higher than the other end) often resulted in stronger responding to a GS that was more intense than the CS+ (intensity generalization; Hull, 1949; Razran, 1949; but see Pierrel & Sherman, 1960; Zielinski & Jakubowska, 1997) . For instance, responding was stronger to a tone GS with a higher decibel level than a tone CS+ (Huff, Sherman & Cohn, 1975) . Therefore, to examine whether a GS more intense than a CS+ (hence evokes more conditioned fear than a CS+ does) would lead to strong generalization of GS extinction, Struyf et al. (2018) used an intensity stimulus dimension of facial stimuli with increasingly fearful expressions. During differential Pavlovian fear acquisition, the stimulus with the least fearful expression served as the safety stimulus (CS-) whereas the stimulus in the middle of the dimension (i.e., moderately fearful expression) served as the CS+. In a subsequent extinction phase, one group received the GS with the highest intensity along the dimension (i.e., the most fearful expression at the opposite end of CS-along the stimulus dimension; J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f peak GS), one group received the CS+, and another group received a GS intermediate of the CS+ and the CS-(i.e., intensity of fearful expression weaker than the CS+; weak GS). Both the peak GS group and the CS+ group showed stronger conditioned fear to the extinction stimulus than the weak GS group in early extinction trials. This not only supports the notion that a GS more intense than the CS+ evokes at least as much conditioned fear as the CS+, but also suggests that the extinction stimulus in the peak GS group and the CS+ group led to higher levels of expectancy violation than the weak GS group. In a final test phase, all stimuli along the dimension were presented without an US. Both the peak GS group and the CS+ group showed a flatter generalization of extinction gradient than the weak GS group. More specifically, while both the peak GS group and CS+ group showed similarly low level of fear responding to the CS+, the peak GS group showed less fear responding to the peak GS than the CS+ group. This suggests that extinction learning to a GS that evoked stronger fear than the CS+ led to greater generalization of extinction across the test stimuli. The authors interpreted this pattern via a typicality asymmetry account, in which generalization from highly typical exemplars (e.g., bear) to other members of the same category (e.g., mammals) is stronger, whereas generalization from atypical exemplars (e.g., armadillo) to other category members is weaker (Dunsmoor & Murphy, 2014; Scheveneels et al., 2017) . Participants may have learnt that a fearful expression signalled a shock. Thereby, the stimulus with the strongest fearful expression (the peak GS) became a highly typical exemplar of the category of fearful faces. Presenting it in extinction maximized expectancy violation and hence resulted in strong generalization of GS extinction. The strong generalization of GS extinction found by Struyf et al. (2018) could, however, be interpreted alternatively with the formation of threat beliefs through rule-based generalization. Recent evidence demonstrated a diverse range of generalization gradients, and the shape of these gradients corresponded with participants' self-reported rules (Ahmed & J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Lee, Hayes & Lovibond, 2018; Wong & Lovibond, 2017; Wong & Lovibond, 2018) . For instance, participants that responded based on a linear rule (e.g., the further away the stimulus is positioned from the CS+ along the stimulus dimension in the opposite direction of the CS-, the higher the likelihood of a shock), exhibited a linear gradient. Individuals who responded based on a similarity rule (e.g., the more perceptually similar the stimulus is to the CS+, the higher the likelihood of a shock), exhibited a peaked, bell-shaped gradient with the highest fear responding to the CS+, with decreasing responding as stimuli became more dissimilar to the CS+. As a result, participants from these two rule groups would hold different threat beliefs. Specifically, they would show different generalized fear to the GS at the extreme end of the stimulus dimension in the direction opposite of the CS-: Participants who responded on a linear rule basis showed strong generalized fear to this GS, while those who responded based on a similarity rule showed weak generalized fear to the same GS. Evidence also suggests that participants are biased to respond on a linear-rule basis along an intensity stimulus dimension (e.g., Ahmed & Lovibond, 2019) . Considering that Struyf et al. (2018) used such an intensity stimulus dimension, most participants might have been encouraged to respond based on a linear rule (i.e., the more fearful the facial expression, the higher the likelihood of a shock), hence forming strong generalized fear to the peak GS. Therefore, presenting the peak GS in extinction may have had produced a high level of expectancy violation, resulting in a strong generalization of GS extinction. However, given that rule-based generalization was not assessed, this alternative explanation could not be tested. The current study therefore sought to systematically examine how different threat beliefs formed through rule-based generalization determine the magnitude of generalization of GS extinction. We used a differential conditioning procedure, with the stimulus intermediate of a non-intensity based stimulus dimension serving as the CS+ and the stimulus J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f at the extreme left end of the dimension serving as the CS-(Fear acquisition). The full range of stimuli along the dimension was then presented to assess the generalization gradients of fear acquisition (Fear generalization test). Next, participants were presented with the GS at the extreme right end of the dimension in extinction (GS extinction). In the final test phase, generalization of GS extinction was assessed by presenting selected GSs to the participants (Post-extinction generalization test). Based on previous studies (e.g., Lee et al., 2018; Wong & Lovibond, 2017) , we expected a majority of participants would form either a similarity-or a linear-based rule in the current design. This allowed us to compare how threat beliefs formed by qualitatively different generalization rules influence the effectiveness of generalization of GS extinction. We predicted that participants who responded with a linear rule would show strong generalized fear to the GS in extinction, as the GS was placed at the opposite end on the continuum to the CS-. This would lead to a high level of expectancy violation in the extinction phase, resulting in strong generalization of GS extinction. In contrast, participants who responded with a similarity rule would show weak generalized fear to the same GS in extinction; this would evoke little if any expectancy violation, thus resulting in little to no generalization of extinction effect. Individuals from the University of Würzburg and the general community were recruited as participants and received either partial course credit or 10€ for participation. We followed the sample size in previous rule-based generalization studies (e.g., Lee et al., 2018; Wong & Lovibond, 2017) , which was approximately 50 participants in total. To ensure appropriate sample sizes in both the similarity-based group (Similarity group) and the linearbased group (Linear group), we used a recruitment strategy to recruit an initial sample of 50 J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f participants. We then classified participants according to their reported rules (see Results for details) and checked if there were at least 20 participants in each of the two rule groups. If not, we continued recruiting until both groups reached at least 20 participants. This led to a total recruitment of 63 participants 1 . The Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology at the University of Würzburg approved the experimental procedure. The stimulus dimension consisted of 9 yellow squares with black outline (9.5cm x 9.5cm) containing a black dot that varied in position from the left to the right. These stimuli were labelled A (with the dot at the left-most) to I (with the dot at the right-most), by manipulating the dot position by 0.8cm from one stimulus to the next (see Fig. 1 ). Stimulus E and A always served as the CS+ and CS-, respectively. The position of the CS-was not counterbalanced given that the stimulus dimension was symmetrical and intensity bias was minimized (see Lee et al., 2018; Wong & Lovibond, 2017) . All stimuli were individually presented in the centre of a white screen. A computer equipped with Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Berkeley, CA, Version 20.1) presented all visual stimuli and recorded the US expectancy ratings. Another computer with BrainVision Recorder (Brain Products GmbH, Gliching, Germany) recorded the skin conductance data via two Ag/AgCl electrodes at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. A DS7A Digitimer stimulator generated an electric shock US. The electric shock consisted of 125 pulses separated by 5ms (i.e., US duration of 625ms), delivered through a bar electrode attached to participants' wrist. 1 Seventeen participants were recruited after the COVID-19 restrictions had been lifted in Germany (mid-June, 2020). For safety measures, participants cleaned their hands with soap followed by alcohol wipes, in addition to wearing a facial mask throughout the experiment. These safety measures did not affect the recording of SCR (see Supplementary Materials). J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f After providing written informed consent, participants were asked to fill in the German version of DASS-21 (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995; Nilges & Essau, 2015) . The DASS-21 is a short version of the original DASS (Depression Anxiety Stress Scale), which validly measures and discriminates three different constructs: depression, anxiety and stress (Antony, Bieing, Cox, Enns & Swinson, 1998; Brown, Chorpita, Korotitsch & Barlow, 1997; Henry & Crawford, 2005; Lovibond, 1998) . Next, the skin conductance and electric shock electrodes were attached to the participants' non-dominant hand. Skin conductance electrodes filled with isotonic gel were attached to the hypothenar muscles on the palm. Participants were then led through an electric shock US intensity calibration, in which the shock US intensity was gradually increased starting from 0.2mA until participants reached a level of shock US intensity that was perceived as 'definitely unpleasant but not painful'. The ensuing experiment consisted of four phases: fear acquisition, fear generalization test, GS extinction and post-extinction generalization test (see Table 1 ). Participants were instructed to indicate their shock expectancy ratings using a visual analogue scale (VAS) that appeared at the bottom of the screen during CS presentations. The expectancy VAS ranged from 0 to 100 in which 0 indicates certain no shock and 100 indicates certain shock. The fear acquisition phase was divided into two blocks: in each block, six trials of CS+ and six trials of CS-were presented, leading to twelve trials per block. The CS+ was reinforced at 75% and the CS-was never reinforced. The CS+ was not fully reinforced to allow room for stronger fear responding to stimuli beyond the CS+ in the opposite direction of the CS-, for instance a potential rule-based linear gradient. The CSs were presented alongside the shock expectancy VAS for 8s. The electric shock was delivered immediately after the offset of reinforced CS+ trials for 625ms. The presentation order was pseudo-randomized, so that the same trial type was not presented more than twice in a row, and the first and last CS+ presentations were always reinforced. The inter-trial interval (ITI) varied between 15 and 18s and was applied to all the following phases. Fear generalization test (shock electrodes disconnected). The experiment was paused and the experimenter disconnected the shock electrodes. Participants were informed that the electrodes were disconnected to examine if the disconnection would induce a change in J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f physiological responding, therefore setting up the cover story for disconnecting the shock electrodes. Given that no shock could be possibly administered anymore, participants were asked to continue providing their shock expectancy ratings, assuming hypothetically that it was still possible for them to receive a shock. This procedure was used to prevent the occurrence of extinction learning during the test phase, which reduces the probability of participants modifying their response strategy due to extinction learning (e.g., Wong & Lovibond, 2017) . Therefore, this phase allowed participants to reveal rule-based generalization without any interference from extinction learning. All nine stimuli along the dimension (stimuli A to I) were presented once each in a randomized order. Thus, the CSs and seven GSs of varying levels of similarity to the CS+ were presented. Each stimulus was presented for 8s and participants were prompted to rate their shock expectancies via the expectancy VAS. GS extinction (shock electrodes reconnected). The experiment was paused again and the experimenter reconnected the shock electrodes. Participants were told that it was again possible to receive an electric shock. Stimulus I was presented for 6 trials for 8s alongside the VAS without reinforcement. Post-extinction generalization test (shock electrodes connected). This phase continued seamlessly from the previous phase. Stimuli A, C, E, G and I were presented once each in a randomized order alongside the shock expectancy VAS for 8s. We only presented 5 selected stimuli instead of the whole dimension to minimize the effect of ongoing extinction in this phase. No electric shock was delivered in this phase. After the completion of the conditioning task, participants were asked to fill in a 2page questionnaire (see Supplementary Materials). On the first page, the expectancy ratings to stimuli A and I that the individual participant had made during the fear generalization J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f phase (when the shock electrodes were disconnected) were presented. Participants were prompted to explain why they had made these ratings, and to write down in detail any rules or strategies of responding they used. The second page was handed out to the participant only after the first page was completed, and consisted of five statements. Each statement described a potential relationship between the stimuli and the electric shock in terms of relational rules (linear right, linear left, similarity, no rule and other). Participants had to rate how much they considered each statement to be true on a scale of 0 to 100, with 0 being false and 100 being true. If participants perceived that none of the given statements matched their responding during the conditioning task, they were asked to write down their own description of the relationship between the stimuli and the shock US in the 'Other' section. Analysis of skin conductance data was based only on data collected when the shock electrodes were connected (i.e., during fear acquisition, GS extinction, and post-extinction generalization test). Given that a shock was expected to occur during these phases, anticipatory fear indexed by skin conductance could be measured. First, a 1 Hz high-pass filter and a 50 Hz notch filter were applied to the skin conductance data. Next, we calculated the SCR by finding the difference between the maximum response and the corresponding trough in the interval of 1s after stimulus onset to stimulus offset (see Pineles, Orr & Orr, 2009 ). This was first done by identifying the maximum and minimum responses in each stimulus interval automatically by BrainVision Analyzer. We then checked if the detected minimum response was located at the corresponding trough of the maximum response (i.e., a stimulus interval with multiple peaks); if not, we manually adjusted it accordingly. The SCRs were then square root transformed to reduce skewness (Boucsein et al., 2012) . Both expectancy ratings and SCR data were analysed by a set of planned contrasts, using a multivariate repeated measures model (O'Brien & Kaiser, 1985) . Planned contrasts compare specific levels of factors to test for a priori hypotheses, allowing for more targeted tests than an omnibus ANOVA (Bird, 2004) . Planned contrasts were used to assess acquisition, extinction and generalization gradients, and to compare these between the different rule groups for US expectancy ratings and SCRs. For fear acquisition, three orthogonal repeated measures contrasts were used. First, the averaged responding to the CS+ was compared to the CS-(main effect of CS type). Second, the second block was compared to the first block to examine whether there were any changes in responding across acquisition (main effect of Block). Third, the interaction of these two contrasts examined whether the changes in responding across blocks differed between the CSs (CS type x Block interaction). For GS extinction, a linear trend repeated measures contrast was used to assess whether responding to stimulus I decreased across extinction trials. For both fear generalization test and post-extinction generalization test, two orthogonal polynomial trends were used: A linear trend repeated measures contrast was used to capture linear gradients across the stimulus dimension, and a quadratic trend repeated measures contrast was used to assess peaked, bellshaped gradients. With an additional repeated measures contrast, we compared responding to the CS+ and the CS-in the post-extinction generalization test, to examine whether differential responding to the CSs persisted after GS extinction. We used between-group contrasts to compare the difference in responding between the rule groups. Finally, all interactions between the group and repeated measures contrasts were calculated to assess rule group differences in all phases. For the post-extinction generalization test, the additional contrast comparing responding to the CSs and the two polynomial contrasts were nonorthogonal. Therefore, we applied a Bonferroni correction for these three non-orthogonal repeated measures contrasts (α/3 = .05/3 = .0167) to control for family-wise-error rate. The Statistical analyses were restricted to participants who had acquired differential US expectancy ratings. This was defined by a difference of at least 50 points between the averaged expectancy ratings to the last four CS+ trials and the averaged expectancy ratings to the last four CS-trials in the fear acquisition phase, same as in our prior study (Wong & Lovibond, 2017) . Two participants were excluded on this basis. Two additional participants were excluded, one due to technical issues that prohibited the recording of skin conductance, and one for requesting to reduce shock US intensity during the experiment, leaving a total of 59 participants. Three raters, who were blind to the US expectancy and SCR data categorized Thirty-four participants were classified as having adopted a similarity rule, whereby they expected that stimuli that were more perceptually dissimilar to the CS+ were less likely to signal shock (Similarity group). Twenty participants were classified as having adopted a linear rule, whereby they expected that the more the dot position was to the right, the more likely it would signal shock (Linear group). Five participants reported not able to infer any clear rules (No-rule group) . Considering the small sample size of the No-rule group (and therefore the lack of statistical power), participants from the No rule group were excluded from statistical analyses. The final sample thereby comprised 54 participants (38 females) with a mean age of 26.1 years (SD = 5.4) and a mean electric shock intensity of 1.1 mA (SD = .5). Table 2 shows the demographic and DASS-21 data. No significant group differences emerged. In sum, both groups successfully acquired differential responding to the CSs in both US expectancy ratings and SCR. While there were no predisposing group differences in the acquisition of US expectancy ratings, the Linear group showed enhanced acquisition of skin conductance responding to the CS+. Fear generalization test Figure 3A shows the fear generalization gradients of US expectancy in the Similarity and the Linear group. The Similarity group was characterized by a peaked gradient with decreasing expectancy ratings to stimuli more different from the CS+ along the stimulus dimension, whereas the Linear group was characterized by a S-shaped gradient with low ratings to stimuli on the left of CS+ and high ratings to stimuli right of the CS+. The Similarity group showed a more peaked gradient than the Linear group, yielding a significant Figure 4A shows the US expectancy to the extinction stimulus (stimulus I) during GS extinction. Averaged across groups, expectancy ratings to the extinction stimulus decreased across trials, supported by a main effect of linear trend across trials, F(1,52) = 133.02, p < .001, η p 2 = 0.72. Crucially, the Linear group showed higher ratings than the Similarity group to the extinction stimulus in early extinction trials, followed by a more rapid and greater decrease in ratings to it. This pattern was supported by a significant interaction The SCR data showed a similar pattern (Fig. 4B) . Averaged across groups, responding to the extinction stimulus decreased across trials, yielding a significant linear trend across J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f trials, F(1,52) = 20.79, p < .001, η p 2 = 0.29. The Linear group descriptively showed stronger SCRs to the extinction stimulus early in extinction compared to the Similarity group. However, there was no statistical evidence to suggest stronger extinction learning to the extinction stimulus in the Linear group, as the interaction between Group and linear trend across trials did not reach significance, F(1,52) = 0.07, p = .788, η p 2 < 0.01. Figure 3B shows the US expectancy to the selected test stimuli along the stimulus dimension. Averaged across groups, we observed a peaked gradient with the highest ratings to the CS+, yielding a significant main effect of quadratic trend along the stimulus dimension, the Linear group showed less fear responding to the CS+ than the Similarity group, supported by a significant interaction between Group and differential responding to the CSs, F(1,52) = 6.50, p (.05/3) = .014, η p 2 = 0.11. Follow-up analyses confirmed that the Similarity group exhibited significant differential responding to the CSs, F(1,33) = 15.99, p (.05/3) < .01, η p 2 = 0.33. In contrast, there was no evidence of a persistence of differential responding to the CSs in the Linear group, F(1,19) = 0.38, p (.05/3) = .545, η p 2 = 0.02. In sum, the Linear group showed a flatter generalization gradient of GS extinction than the Similarity group. Although this pattern was significant in the expectancy ratings, it remained descriptive in the SCR measure. Furthermore, the Linear group showed less fear responding to the CS+ than the Similarity group in both expectancy ratings and SCR measure. The linearity of the generalization gradient decreased after GS extinction as compared with before GS extinction, presumably due to a decrease in expectancy ratings to the CS+ and stimuli right of the CS+. This decrease was more significant in the Linear group than the Similarity group. This pattern was supported by a 3-way interaction between phase, linear trend across stimuli and groups, F(1,52) = 46.71, p < .001, η p 2 = 0.41. Follow-up analyses confirmed that both groups showed a significant decrease in their generalization gradients' linearity (lowest F = 17.67, p < .001, η p 2 = 0.35). This pattern suggests that both groups showed generalization of GS extinction, and provides further evidence that this effect was larger in the Linear group than in the Similarity group. No interactions involving quadratic trend reached significance (highest F = 2.56, p = .116, η p 2 = 0.05). Using a differential conditioning procedure, the current study examined whether different learnt threat beliefs led to different magnitude of generalized fear to the same GS, thus presenting this GS in extinction would evoke different levels of expectancy violation. Importantly, we examined whether this difference in expectancy violation would determine the strength of generalization of GS extinction along a non-intensity based stimulus dimension. We identified rule groups that reported using either a linear or a similarity rule during generalization and examined group differences in generalization gradients of fear acquisition and GS extinction. The generalization gradients observed in the fear generalization test were highly consistent with participants' reported rules. Specifically, participants reporting a similarity rule showed a peaked generalization gradient, with the highest US expectancy ratings for the CS+ and a gradual decrease in expectancies to stimuli more different from the CS+. In contrast, participants who reported a linear rule showed increasing US expectancies from the CS-and reached an asymptotic level at the CS+, while expectancies for stimuli right of the CS+ slightly decreased and then increased back to the asymptotic level again. These patterns replicated the discrete generalization gradients between rule groups observed in past studies (Ahmed & Lovibond, 2019; Lee, Hayes & Lovibond, 2018; Wong & Lovibond, 2017 . In fact, rule-based generalization shares some similarities with studies of feature generalization (Vervliet et al., 2010; Vervliet & Geens, 2014; Ahmed & Lovibond, 2015a) . In these feature generalization studies, participants learnt that one feature of the CS+ (e.g., shape or color) was the predictor of an aversive US through either verbal instruction (Ahmed & Lovibond, 2015a; Vervliet et al., 2010) or direct experience (Ahmed & Lovibond, 2015b; Vervliet & Geens, 2014) before or during fear acquisition. In a subsequent generalization phase, participants showed more generalized fear to a GS that shared the predictive feature with the CS+ (e.g. the same color) than a GS that shared a non-predictive feature with the J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f CS+ (e.g., the same shape). That is, although the two GSs were equally similar to the CS+, participants exhibited selective fear generalization to the stimulus that shared the learnt predictive feature with the CS+, therefore resulting in participants holding different threat beliefs (e.g., novel stimuli that shared the same color with the CS+ predict an aversive outcome). The results from our study are thus in line with these findings and further corroborate the notion that threat beliefs formed through either feature learning of the CSs or rule formation guide fear generalization, indicating the importance of identifying individual threat beliefs. Consistent with our prediction, generalization rules differentially affected fear extinction learning. During GS extinction, where the GS at the far right on the stimulus dimension served as the extinction stimulus, the Linear group showed strong generalized fear to it, as evidenced by strong US expectancy ratings to the GS in both fear generalization test and GS extinction. The absence of an US therefore evoked a large amount of expectancy violation, resulting in strong extinction learning as indexed by a rapid decrease in US expectancy (Craske et al., 2014; . In contrast, the Similarity group showed weak generalized fear to the extinction stimulus, as shown by the low level of expectancy ratings to it in both fear generalization test and GS extinction. The absence of a shock US was thus largely consistent with participants' threat belief, hence little if any extinction learning took place. However, this pattern was only observed in the US expectancy ratings. A potential explanation for the lack of group differences in the extinction of SCR is presumably due to its high inter-individual variability (Lykken & Venables, 1971) . A major finding was that the Linear group showed a relatively flat generalization gradient of US expectancy ratings in the post-extinction generalization test. Even more critically, this flat gradient was characterized by a low level of fear responding to the CS+ in both US expectancies and SCR measures. In contrast, the Similarity group showed a sharp J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f peaked gradient comparable to the gradient before GS extinction, with strong fear responding to the CS+ compared to the Linear group. Combining these patterns with the findings observed in the GS extinction phase, the results strongly suggest that the magnitude of extinction outcome depends on the violation of one's threat expectancy to the extinction stimulus and thereby determines the degree of subsequent generalization of GS extinction. The current findings are highly consistent with the notion that the degree of expectancy violation strongly influences extinction learning (Craske et al., 2014 , and further illustrate how expectancy violation to a GS generalizes to other fear-related stimuli. Crucially, as the same GS was used in extinction across all participants, the observed group difference in the generalization gradient of GS extinction is due to the GS evoking expectancy violation to different extents. An alternative explanation for the current findings is that different degrees of expectancy violation led to the preservation or extinction of one's initial threat belief. The GS in extinction was largely consistent with the Similarity group's initial threat belief (i.e., the further away the dot is from the center of the box, the less likely shock occurs), therefore this threat belief was largely preserved. This was indicated by the similar peaked gradients before and after GS extinction in the Similarity group. In contrast, the GS in extinction violated the Linear group's initial threat belief (i.e., the further the dot is to the right, the more likely shock occurs), thus leading to the violation of this threat belief. Nonetheless, the current study could not distinguish whether the findings were purely driven by the generalization of GS extinction, the violation of one's initial threat belief, or potentially a combination of both. Future studies could assess to what extent participants continue to embrace their initial threat beliefs after GS extinction. Although the Linear group exhibited strong generalization of GS extinction, threat expectancy to the CS+ still persisted (i.e., differential expectancy ratings to the CS+ and the CS-remained after GS extinction). This apparent persistence of threat expectancy to the CS+ J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f could be a by-product of the stimulus dimension in use. Lee et al. (2018) One limitation of the current study is that we had no control over the sample size of the rule groups because we could not manipulate the rules inferred by participants. The unequal sample sizes in the rule groups may have reduced statistical power. However, we also perceive the self-generated rules as a strength of this study, considering that it parallels with clinically anxious individuals spontaneously forming their own threat beliefs. A second limitation was that the stimulus dimension in use might have encouraged the classification of stimuli into different categories, rendering the stimulus dimension non-continuous (see Lee et al., 2018) . This might have then limited the generalization of GS extinction to other stimuli, for instance, the CS+. A third limitation is that the Linear group showed stronger acquisition of SCR to the CS+ than the Similarity group. However, this group difference is unlikely to have confounded the current findings considering that the Linear group still exhibited less SCR and lower US expectancy ratings to the CS+ than the Similarity group after GS extinction. In conclusion, the current work replicated the discrete gradients formed by rule-based generalization (Ahmed & Lovibond, 2019; Lee et al., 2018; Wong & Lovibond, 2017) , suggesting the formation of different threat beliefs. As a result, the same GS elicited different levels of generalized fear between rule groups. Presenting this GS in extinction accordingly evoked different levels of expectancy violation and thereby different extinction learning outcomes. Participants for whom expectancy violation was maximized during extinction learning (i.e., Linear group) showed greater generalization of GS extinction than participants with little to no expectancy violation during extinction (i.e., Similarity group). The present J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f work emphasises the importance of identifying different threat beliefs in clinically anxious individuals, and suggests tackling the strongest threat belief to maximize the effectiveness of exposure-based treatments. Captions for the tables: We wish to confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest associated with this publication and there has been no significant financial support for this work that could have influenced its outcome. We confirm that the manuscript has been read and approved by all named authors and that there are no other persons who satisfied the criteria for authorship but are not listed. We further confirm that the order of authors listed in the manuscript has been approved by all of us. We confirm that we have given due consideration to the protection of intellectual property associated with this work and that there are no impediments to publication, including the timing of publication, with respect to intellectual property. In so doing we confirm that we have followed the regulations of our institutions concerning intellectual property. We further confirm that any aspect of the work covered in this manuscript that has involved either experimental animals or human patients has been conducted with the ethical approval of all relevant bodies and that such approvals are acknowledged within the manuscript. We understand that the Corresponding Author is the sole contact for the Editorial process (including Editorial Manager and direct communications with the office). He is responsible for communicating with the other authors about progress, submissions of revisions and final approval of proofs. We confirm that we have provided a current, correct email address which is accessible by the Corresponding Author and which has been configured to accept email from hon.wong@uni-wuerzburg.de Rule-based processes in generalisation and peak shift in human fear conditioning The impact of instructions on generalization of conditioned fear in humans The impact of previously learned feature-relevance on generalisation of conditioned fear in humans Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample The role of stimulus specificity and attention in the generalization of extinction Threat-related gaze fixation and its relationship with the speed and generalisability of extinction learning Analysis of variance via confidence intervals Steady state data and a quantitative model of operant generalization and discrimination Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements Psychometric properties of the depressiojn anxiety stress scales (DASS) in clinical samples Cognitive behavioural therapy for anxiety and related disorders: A metaanalysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach Phia: Post-hoc interaction analysis Stimulus typicality determines how broadly fear is generalized The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large nonclinical sample Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials Some effects of response-independent reinforcement in auditory generalization gradients Stimulus intensity dynamism (V) and stimulus generalization Peak shift and rules in human generalization Long-term stability of depression, anxiety, and stress syndromes Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales Direct measurement of skin conductance: A proposal for standardization The psychology of animal learning Human fear conditioning depends on stimulus contingency instructions Die depressions-angst-stress-skalen: Der DASS-ein screeningverfahren nicht nur fur schmerzpatienten [depression, anxiety and stress scales: DASS-A screening procedure not only for pain patients MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: An extensive primer Generalization of auditory intensity following discrimination training An alternative scoring method for skin conductance responding in a differential fear conditioning paradigm with a longstimulus The role of associative fear and avoidance learning in anxiety disorders: Gaps and directions for future research Avoidance and its bidirectional relationship with conditioned fear: Mechanisms, moderators, and clinical implications Generalization of extinguished fear to untreated fear stimuli after exposure R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing Stimulus generalisation of conditioned responses A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non reinforcement One for all: The effect of extinction stimulus typicality on return of fear The validity of laboratorybased treatment research: Bridging the gap between fear extinction and exposure treatment Afex: Analysis of factorial experiments. R package cersion 0 Maximizing the generalization of fear extinction: Exposures to a peak generalization stimulus Fear generalization in humans: Impact of feature learning on conditioning and extinction Fear generalization in humans: Impact of verbal instructions Return of fear in a human differential conditioning paradigm caused by a stimulus change after J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f extinction Generalization of extinguished skin conductance responding in human fear conditioning Generalization of human fear acquisition and extinction within a novel arbitrary stimulus category Rule-based generalization in single-cue and differential fear conditioning in humans Excessive generalisation of conditioned fear in trait anxious individuals under ambiguity Generalization of extinction of a generalization stimulus in fear learning Pavlovian extinction of fear with the original conditional stimulus, a generalization stimulus, or multiple generalization stimuli Auditory intensity generalization after CER differentiation training