key: cord-0717380-26oj1mch authors: Duplantier, Sally C.; Gardner, Christopher D. title: A Critical Review of the Study of Neuroprotective Diets to Reduce Cognitive Decline date: 2021-06-30 journal: Nutrients DOI: 10.3390/nu13072264 sha: a6d73bc12d9981afbdfac81a5ecc495a14b30996 doc_id: 717380 cord_uid: 26oj1mch Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and other dementias are now the seventh leading cause of death in the world and are projected to affect 115.4 million people by 2050. Delaying the onset of AD by just five years is estimated to reduce the cost and prevalence of the disease by half. There is no cure for AD nor any drug therapies to halt its progression once the disease begins. Lifestyle choices including diet are being seen as a viable complementary therapy to reduce cognitive decline, the hallmark of AD. Mediterranean, DASH (Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension), and MIND (Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay) diets have biological mechanisms supporting their potential neuroprotective benefits, but the findings of study outcomes about these benefits have been inconsistent. This paper analyzed five Randomized Clinical Trials (RCTs) (from 2000 to 2021) and 27 observational studies (from 2010 to 2021) focused on the link between cognitive health and the Mediterranean/DASH/MIND diets to identify gaps and challenges that could lead to inconsistent results. These include a lack of accuracy in assessing food intake, multiple dietary pattern scoring systems, a shifting metric among studies focused on the Mediterranean diet, a lack of standards in the tools used to assess cognitive decline, and studies that were underpowered or had follow-up periods too short to detect cognitive change. Insights from these gaps and challenges are summarized in recommendations for future RCTs, including both pragmatic and explanatory RCTs. Alzheimer's disease (AD) and other dementias are now the seventh leading cause of death in the world [1] . AD is a disease with no cures or therapies to reduce its progression and symptoms once they start [2] . Currently, more than six million Americans are living with AD, and this number is expected to reach nearly 13 million by 2050 at an annual cost of $1.1 trillion [3] . Globally, AD and other dementias are projected to affect over 115 million people by 2050 [4] . There have been no new drug therapies for AD in more than a decade, and current therapies only manage disease symptoms but do not halt, slow, or reverse the disease itself [5] . Lifestyle choices, including diet and nutrition, are being examined as a complementary method to delay the onset of AD. Early therapeutic action is needed to address biological processes that may already be compromised, and before AD progresses from a state of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to cognitive failure [6] . By 2050, a five-year delay in the onset of AD could reduce the prevalence and cost of this disease by 50% and dramatically decrease the burden of AD on patients, their families, and the health care system [7] . AD involves the intraneuronal formation of abnormal tau protein and extracellular deposition of beta-amyloid protein in the central nervous system [8] . The pathology is a Red Wine (Medi, MIND diets) [29, 46, 49, [51] [52] [53] X X "X" indicates association with the neuroprotective benefit listed. Based on current research, it is unclear whether the Medi, DASH, or MIND diet is superior for preventing or reducing cognitive decline due to several factors. Most studies to date have been observational, lacking the rigor of RCTs [58] (p. 5). Within these studies, there is lack of consistency in the measures related both to dietary intake and cognitive change [41] . Despite the limitations in measurement tools and the inconsistencies in study outcomes mentioned, there is sufficient evidence of the potential neuroprotective benefits of all three dietary patterns [59] to warrant further investigation, especially given that the risks are so low and the stakes are so high. A treatment that delays the onset or progression of AD would have a significant impact on improving the quality of life for individuals and their families while simultaneously reducing caregiver burden and healthcare costs [3] . This paper provides a critical review of the current literature related to neuroprotective dietary patterns and cognitive decline. It identifies gaps in the design of current studies which have led to inconsistent findings within the literature reviewed and challenges in filling those gaps. This paper also recommends solutions for more robust study designs in the future. These solutions include gold-standard explanatory Randomized Clinical Trials which demonstrate efficacy, as well as pragmatic RCTs which demonstrate the effectiveness of interventions in real-life environments such as clinical practices or community settings. For this review, we searched the PubMed and InfoPsych databases using the key search terms "Mediterranean or DASH or MIND and cognitive decline or cognition or Alzheimer's or dementia". To narrow the volume of publications, the terms "and systematic review or meta-analysis" were added. The search yielded three systematic reviews [2, 30, 59] . Combined, these three systematic reviews provided 140 papers that were identified from these databases: PubMed, Medline, Cochrane Library, Embase, Google Scholar, Web of Science, and Scopus. An additional search performed to find relevant papers published since 2019 yielded one additional RCT [2] , which is still underway. Included are all peer reviewed RCTs from 2000 and all observational studies from 2010 on the Mediterranean, DASH, and/or MIND diets. Excluded are studies on similar dietary patterns such as the Anti-Inflammatory Diet or the Prudent Healthy Diet because data are more limited for these [41] . From the studies that remained, 18 duplicates were eliminated, yielding a total of five RCTs and 27 observational studies. Six of the 27 observational studies included more than one dietary pattern (e.g., Mediterranean, DASH, and/or MIND diets were compared in one paper). This created a total of 35 dietary analyses based on observational studies, in addition to the five RCTS, for a total of 40 dietary analyses. The analysis of outcomes is based on our understanding of the primary hypothesis of each study. This paper has identified the potential ways in which neuroprotective diets may contribute to cognitive health, while recognizing that research to date has been inconsistent, and in some cases, conflicting. As examples, this paper highlights the 32 studies reviewed on the association between the Medi/DASH/MIND dietary patterns and cognition or cognitive decline. These papers cover five RCTs and 27 observational studies ( Figure 1 ). Because some studies included more than one dietary pattern, the review includes results for a total of 40 dietary analyses. Of the five RCTs, four included a Medi diet arm, with two of these reporting a positive impact between the Medi diet and cognitive health [33, 34] and one reporting a null effect [60] . The fourth Medi RCT [61] reported mixed results; there were no differences between either of the two intervention groups or the control group on three measures of cognitive function. For a fourth measure, which involved the task with the greatest processing load, the two intervention groups adhering to the Medi diet did significantly worse (p < 0.001) than the control group. The fifth RCT, testing a DASH diet [62] , also had mixed results. The intervention with the DASH diet alone (without exer-cise) showed improvement in psychomotor speed but not in executive function-memory learning. Within the observational studies representing 35 analyses of Medi/DASH/MIND, there were 21 positive associations between diet and cognition, nine null findings, and five studies with mixed outcomes. Details about the RCTs and observational studies can be found in Table 3. two of these reporting a positive impact between the Medi diet and cognitive health [33, 34] and one reporting a null effect [60] . The fourth Medi RCT [61] reported mixed results; there were no differences between either of the two intervention groups or the control group on three measures of cognitive function. For a fourth measure, which involved the task with the greatest processing load, the two intervention groups adhering to the Medi diet did significantly worse (p < 0.001) than the control group. The fifth RCT, testing a DASH diet [62] , also had mixed results. The intervention with the DASH diet alone (without exercise) showed improvement in psychomotor speed but not in executive function-memory learning. Within the observational studies representing 35 analyses of Medi/DASH/MIND, there were 21 positive associations between diet and cognition, nine null findings, and five studies with mixed outcomes. Details about the RCTs and observational studies can be found in Table 3 This section addresses the gaps and challenges of identifying the link between dietary patterns and cognitive change. These gaps and challenges contribute to the inconsistencies of study results to date. Some of these challenges are unique to the study of diet and cognition, and other challenges are common to any clinical trial or observational study but play a significant role in accurately assessing cognitive decline. The key to analyzing the link between neuroprotective diets and cognitive health is the accuracy of dietary intake. Inaccuracy here can lead to inconsistencies in results across studies. Common tools include food frequency questionnaires (FFQs), 24-h dietary recalls (24HRs), or food diaries. Dietary intake measures varied widely in the five RCTs we reviewed. One study used an FFQ, two studies used a combination of an FFQ and questionnaire, one study used dietary diaries, and the final RCT did not specify the dietary intake method. Within the observational studies, most (N = 23) incorporated FFQs or semi-quantitative FFQs, several (N = 3) used 24HRs, and one used food diaries. There were no between-group differences on three measures of cognitive function, but for a fourth measure, which involved the task with the greatest processing load, the two intervention groups did significantly worse (p < 0.001) than the control group. The change in performance was correlated with the change in total serum cholesterol level (p = 0.01). Low consumption of meat and meat products was linked to a better performance on the seven-minute screening (7MS) test (p = 0.001) and greater total brain volume (i.e., the sum of white and gray matter, p = 0.03) when controlling for potential confounders (e.g., BMI). Integrating all dietary features into the total modified Medi diet score explained less variance in cognitive functioning and brain volumes than the single dietary component meat intake. All dietary intake tools that involve self-report have inherent limitations which contribute to inaccuracies. For FFQs, used predominantly in large-scale epidemiologic studies, these limitations include inaccuracy related to portion size [83] , lack of generalizability to multi-racial/ethnic cohorts [84] , and the addition of non-standard items unique to the Medi, DASH, or MIND diets which may not exist on the FFQ and contribute to misclassification [85] . Limitations to 24HRs are the cost and logistics of the data [86] and the inability to account for day-to-day variation in diet. Limitations to food diaries include cost, logistics, and high respondent burden [86, 87] . To improve the accuracy of estimated individual intake, research supports the use of a combination of tools such as the FFQ and multiple 24HRs [88] . A second challenge in accurately assessing dietary intake is that multiple tools have been created for scoring adherence to the same dietary pattern. Radd-Vagenas et al. [89] identify 30 different Mediterranean Diet scoring (MDS) systems, and there are at least 10 different DASH scoring systems [85] . The two MDS systems most frequently used are the original scoring system by Trichopoulou et al. [54] and an alternate by Panagiotakos et al. [90] . The Trichopoulou et al. [54] score developed in 2003 ranges from 0 (minimum adherence) to 9 (maximum adherence) based on sex-specific median intakes of nine components. The Panagiotakos et al. [90] score ranges from 0 to 55 based on 11 components. None of the four Medi RCTs reviewed here used either the original or alternate scoring system. Two of the Medi RCTs used their own 14 item questionnaire to score adherence [33, 34] . One Medi RCT created their own scoring system for an Australian population based on the food content of 15 Medi diets and the nutrient content of eight [60] . The final Medi RCT did not identify a specific Medi scoring system but instead provided participants with guidelines to increase vegetables, fruits, and oily fish, reduce dietary fat to 30% of energy, and substitute predominantly monounsaturated fats for saturated fats [61] . Within the Medi observational studies reviewed, most used the original method (N = 18), some used the alternate method (N = 5), and one study used both. The MDS scoring metrics are uniquely problematic as they are typically based on median cut points for intake of food groups, which differ from one population to another [91] . For example, in Greece, the median intake of vegetables is six servings a day, and the median intake of fruits is three servings [55, 91] . An individual will receive a 0 or 1 score based on whether they were below or above the median. In contrast, the US median intake of vegetables is three servings a day and the median intake of fruits is two servings [92] . This creates a floating metric of intake across studies. Because the intake of vegetables is higher in Mediterranean countries, there is the potential that studies conducted here will show more positive associations with this dietary pattern and cognitive health. In the four Medi diet RCTs we analyzed, the two conducted in Mediterranean countries showed positive results, and the two in non-Mediterranean countries showed null results. Within the Medi diet observational studies, the three conducted in Mediterranean countries showed positive results. Within non-Mediterranean countries, 10 had positive results, eight had null results, and three had mixed results. An additional challenge within non-Mediterranean countries is the country-specific interpretation of the traditional scoring systems. To reflect the eating pattern of Swedes, the ULSAM observational study of the Medi diet replaced monounsaturated fatty acids with polyunsaturated fatty acids, excluded nuts and seeds, and combined potatoes with whole grains, even though it used the 0-9 MDS scoring system [73] . Similar adjustments were made in observational studies for Chinese populations [37] , Australian populations [67, 68] , and Nordic populations [76] . The DASH scoring metrics also vary widely, with more than 10 being used [85] , although the most common were developed by Fung et al. [56] and Folsom et al. [93] . The Fung et al. [56] score is based on eight dietary components, each of which receives a score of 0 to 1 based on quintile intake, with a total score between 8-40. The Folsom et al. [93] score is based on pre-defined cut-offs of 11 components, with a total score between 0-11. Both were used in the observational studies that were analyzed (Fung et al. score N = 3, Folsom et al. score N = 1). The dietary pattern in the one RCT focused on the DASH diet did not specify either of these dietary pattern scores but measured changes according to the Healthy Eating Index [62] . In contrast to the challenges with scoring the Mediterranean diet, which is based on median intake, or the multiple scoring systems for the DASH diet, the MIND diet is easier to score. Currently, there is only one system, and it is based on absolute vs. relative levels of intake [91] . It assigns one point for the specified consumption per day or week of 10 brain healthy foods (whole grains, leafy greens, other vegetables, berries, fish, poultry, beans, olive oil, nuts, and wine) and assigns one point for consuming less than the maximum specific portion per day or week of five unhealthy brain foods (red meat, fast/fried food, butter/margarine, cheese, and pastries/sweets [39] . The 2015 MAP observational study, Table 1 [17] provides an example of how all three dietary patterns may be scored in a single study. In the recent book published by the National Academy of Sciences, Preventing Cognitive Decline: A Way Forward, the authors identify a lack of consistent cognitive outcome measures across current and recent trials to be a major challenge in understanding the effects of lifestyle interventions such as diet on cognition, making it impossible to pool results across studies [58] (pp. [84] [85] . For meaningful outcomes, it is recommended that the measures used to assess cognitive decline reflect the latest diagnostic guidelines for assessing AD. However, those guidelines are shifting, and research methods have not kept pace. The Alzheimer's Disease Diagnostic Guidelines, established more than 30 years ago [94] , were updated in 2011 [95] to better assess the disease at its earliest stages. Assessment now requires a battery of tests across cognitive domains (e.g., working memory, semantic memory, visuospatial ability, and perceptual speed) in addition to episodic memory, where the first major outward symptoms of dementia are seen [95] . In addition, the new guidelines recommend that biomarkers such as structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) be used in research-based studies to identify disease presence, even though an MRI cannot confirm dementia with certainty [95, 96] . Within the RCTs reviewed here, four used a battery of neuropsychological tests, but the 2013 PREDIMED-NAVARRA trial [33] used only the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and Clock Drawing Test (CDT). Both are considered crude measures of assessing cognitive decline which lack sensitivity to small changes [97] with the potential to under-report cognitive change. In addition, the 2013 PREDIMED trial failed to administer these tests at baseline, so it could only analyze cognitive function, not change in cognition, over the 6.5 years of the study [33] . Within the observational studies that were reviewed, researchers used a wide range and number of neurological assessments (N = 1-21). Similar to the RCTs, some (N = 5) used only the MMSE to assess cognitive change. Others used simplified measures such as the Subjective Cognitive Function (SCF) test, with only six yes/no questions based upon participants' perception of their current cognitive functioning [64] or the Six Item Screener, with just six questions that result in a rating of "impairment likely" or "impairment less likely" [81] . These assessments fall short in analyzing the complexity of cognitive change, making it more difficult to interpret results across studies. In contrast, a battery of neurological assessments provides a more complete picture of the effects of diet on specific cognitive functions, although a downside to testing batteries is that they often provide mixed results, leading to challenges in interpretation of overall findings. These mixed results occurred in two of the RCTs studied and four of the observational studies. Examples of commonly used tests in neuropsychological test batteries are found in Table 4 . Even though biomarkers such as structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) are now part of the standard guidelines for diagnosing AD [95] , only the PIVUS observational study in Sweden [80] used brain imaging in addition to neuropsychological tests. For a study to generate substantive findings, it must be long enough to capture at least a minimum number of events or changes in measures that involve continuous outcomes. There is no gold standard for the number of years required to measure cognitive decline, but it is difficult to detect in periods of less than a year [58] (p. 81). Within the five RCTs reviewed, three had durations of less than a year. The MedLey study (null finding) was six months [60] ; the ENCORE study (mixed findings) was four months [62] ; and the Wardle et al. study (mixed findings) was 12 weeks [61] . Insufficient follow-up periods lead to spurious results. A challenge in longer studies is participant attrition. The two PREDIMED-NAVARRA RCT studies [33, 34] had longer follow-up periods than the three already cited (6.5 and 4.1 years, respectively). Both found a positive link between the Medi diet and cognition, although these studies suffered from a common challenge of longer trials: high attrition. Because of high drop-out rates in both studies, the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality considered both to be at high risk for survivor bias [58] (p. 113). Ideally, studies are designed to detect a meaningful minimum difference (if there is one), and to provide confidence that a null finding is due to a lack of an effect when that is the true relationship, and not an error in study design (e.g., inadequate sample size, high attrition). The statistical "power" of a study is largely determined by the variance in study variables and sample size. Sample size is especially important in the study of cognitive change because the subset of participants who develop cognitive decline is relatively small and drawing conclusions from a small subset can be misleading. This was the challenge of the PATH Through Life observational study [65] which analyzed the impact of the Medi diet on the cognition of 1528 men for four years and found that monounsaturated fatty acids (MUFAs), combined with excessive calorie intake, were predictive of MCI. This finding on monounsaturated fats is the oppositive of dozens of studies reporting neuroprotective benefits of MUFAs [29, 33, 34] . However, the conclusions of the PATH Through Life study are based on a small subset of the study population who developed MCI or dementia: 10 participants were diagnosed with MCI (0.6%); 19 had a Clinical Dementia Rating of 0.5 (1.2%); and 37 were diagnosed with mild cognitive disorder (2.4%) [65] . While past studies have limitations, it is important to learn from them and take advantage of new tools and more robust design methods so that these neuroprotective dietary patterns can be introduced to more individuals and a broader array of researchers. This section provides two distinct ways to accelerate future research through RCTs in the form of both pragmatic trials and explanatory trials. Clinical trials, the gold standard within the medical sciences community to test and evaluate interventions, fall into two broad categories: explanatory and pragmatic. Many trials have features of both designs [99, 100] . Explanatory trials are designed to test whether an intervention works under optimal conditions (efficacy), while a pragmatic trial evaluates the effectiveness of an intervention in real-life practical conditions, such as a clinical care setting. Explanatory trials carefully control the inclusion and exclusion criteria of participants while pragmatic trials have broader inclusion criteria that is more representative of the general population [101] . In its recommendations for public health interventions to prevent or delay cognitive impairment, the NAS identifies pragmatic trials as one of the top methodological strategies to get dementia-related interventions into broad, routine clinical practices or community settings [58] (p. 88 ). An example of a pragmatic trial is AgeWell.de, a WW FINGERS trial funded by the Alzheimer's Association [100] . A major goal of the WW FINGERS research is to get interventions to as large of a population as possible in real-world settings so that they can be understood, refined, and applied by practitioners [102, 103] . The AgeWell.de study is being conducted by primary care physicians within their offices and includes a wide range of older adults who are part of their physician's general practice. This section provides specific recommendations for future explanatory RCTs, drawing upon an example of an RCT underway at Rush Medical Center [2] . The study is a three-year RCT designed to test the effects of the MIND dietary pattern on the cognitive function of 604 men and women, 65-84 years old, cognitively unimpaired, and overweight, with suboptimal diets. Both the intervention group and control group will be prescribed mild caloric restriction for weight loss. Use a combination of 24HRs and FFQs. The Rush RCT [2] combines both measures and applies these at baseline and at months 6, 12, 24, and 36. This combination of both measures across all test intervals is designed to allow for more accurate intake of dietary components. In the studies examined in this paper, the FFQs and semi-quantitative FFQs varied widely across the 32 studies reviewed in this paper. Researchers should select an FFQ that has been validated within the population to be studied. For RCTs with insufficient funding for staff administered 24-h recalls, an option is the Automated Self-Administered 24-h (ASA24 ® ) Dietary Assessment Tool [104] . This is a free, web-based tool developed in 2009 in conjunction with the NIH. It enables multiple, automatically coded, self-administered 24-h diet recalls and/or single or multi-day food records and has been used in more than 6000 studies as of January 2020. Measure absolute rather than relative intake levels. There are advantages to scoring dietary patterns on an absolute intake of food, which is the norm for the MIND diet, rather than on relative cutoffs based on within population levels of intake, as in the Medi diet. Absolute intake makes it easier to interpret results across different studies because they share a common metric. It is also easier to understand when presented as servings per day [91] . All three dietary patterns discussed can be scored in absolute values vs. relative (Table 4 ). To pool results across studies, the servings per day should be consistent across populations, not altered for median intake. Use a neuropsychological test battery combined with a biomarker test (at least in a subset of the study population). These recommendations reflect the latest Alzheimer's Disease Diagnostic Guidelines [95] and take advantage of new tools to measure cognitive change. A recommended tool for screening is the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [37] . Compared to the MMSE, which has been cited in many of the studies reviewed here, the MoCA shows less susceptibility to both cultural and educational biases and is free for clinical use [90] . The MoCA also has the advantage of testing multiple cognitive domains with an easy and single scoring system [96] . For the neuropsychological testing, an option is the NIH Toolbox (nihtoolbox.org). This set of instruments was developed within the past decade and designed to establish a standard approach to measuring cognitive, emotional, sensory, and motor function, allowing data across research studies to be pooled. Additionally, at least a subset of participants should have MRI scans at the beginning and end of the intervention period to assess change in measures of total brain volume along with other measures of brain macro-and micro-structural integrity [2] . Longer is better but allow more than one year. As stated earlier, there is no gold standard for the number of years needed to assess cognitive change although various sources offer a range. The NAS acknowledges that one year is too short of a duration and recommends 5 to 10 years [58] (p. 82). A systematic review relating to change point studies on accelerated decline in cognitive and neurological outcomes found that change points related to MCI were 3-7 years prior to diagnosis, and change points related to AD and other forms of dementia were 1-11 years prior to diagnosis [105] . The two Medi diet RCTs with positive findings were 6.5 years and 4.1 years, respectively [33, 34] . The Rush RCT [2] is designed to include a follow-up of more than three years, although the study authors recognize this relatively short timeframe may be a limitation. Establish measures to achieve and assess adherence. Most RCTs have questionable long-term adherence to the dietary intervention selected [106] . To address this, there are two separate components to consider: achieving adherence and assessing adherence. In many RCTs, these aspects of study design and conduct are overlooked or minimized, or adherence is confused with attending a class vs. eating the prescribed foods. The Rush RCT [2] applies a rigorous and consistent method to support dietary adherence. This includes an initial three-to four-week run-in period to assess compliance, combined with frequent telephone/email communications with a nutritional case manager, personalized diet plans, compliance aids including a food tracking phone app, group cooking classes and buddy systems, and frequent weight monitoring and check-ins. Assessment of adherence in that study is conducted using both FFQs and 24HRs, as described previously. These approaches are aligned with maximizing both the level of adherence achieved and the ability to report adherence achievement to allow for more effectively critical evaluation. Help participants feel valued and maximize their engagement. Selective attrition is a potential problem in longer-running RCTs (as well as longitudinal studies). If the people who continue to participate differ from their drop-out counterparts with respect to key characteristics, the remaining sample becomes less representative [107] . In cognitive studies, participants who remain are often characterized by better levels of education and health, potentially skewing the results to show less cognitive decline [108] . To minimize attrition, it is important to make all study participants feel valued as "citizen scientists" [109] and not merely research subjects. As shown in the Rush RCT [2] , equal time, attention, and engagement must be given to participants in all study arms so that there is equipoise between them. Ensure the study is powered to detect the meaningful minimum difference that has been identified as the study's primary outcome. In the Rush RCT [2] , the study is powered to test the effect of the MIND intervention over the control intervention based on the annual rate of change in the global cognitive score (a composite of the 12 neuropsychological tests). Based on a sample size of 300 participants in each of the two groups, the MIND trial has >85% power to detect a between-group difference of 2% in the annual rate of cognitive decline (two-sided p = 0.05) considering a possible 5% drop-out at each visit. Begin testing populations at an earlier age. A potential limitation of the Rush RCT [2] is its focus on older adults aged 65-84 years. While the study of dementia in older adults is the norm, the recommendation of the Alzheimer's Association and the NAS is to begin studies at an earlier age, since changes in brain physiology and cognitive abilities may occur years before symptoms appear, and lifestyle interventions may have a more significant impact if started at an earlier life-stage [3, 58] (pp. [81] [82] . The challenge of studying a younger population is that there is less cognitive decline, so these populations need to be studied for a longer duration to determine if dietary patterns have an effect. Include the intake of anticholinergic drugs, including antihistamines, and benzodiazepines as potential confounders of study outcomes. These drugs are associated with adverse cognitive effects in older adults [110, 111] and may lead to spurious results if they are not analyzed as part of the study. Only one RCT analyzed [34] included anticholinergic drugs as a potential confounder. Alzheimer's disease is a critical public health issue given the societal, financial, individual, and family burden. Projected numbers in the next three decades may increase beyond the current staggering estimates based on the unknown and potentially lasting effects of social isolation due to COVID-19. The traditional pharmaceutical approach has not demonstrated a cure or reduction in symptoms over the long term, while lifestyle approaches, including dietary patterns, provide a low-risk, low-cost alternative that may dramatically change the trajectory of this disease. More RCTs are needed so that dietary patterns with an evidence base for effectiveness can be identified and can reach a broader population more quickly. The design of future RCTs should address gaps and challenges that may have led to inconsistent study findings in the past including inaccuracy in assessing food intake, multiple dietary scoring systems, a shifting metric among studies focused on the Mediterranean diet, a lack of standards in the tools used to assess cognitive decline, and studies that were underpowered or followup periods that were too short to detect cognitive change. Because lifestyle interventions such as diet may have greater impact at earlier life stages, studies should begin to include younger populations. It is vital to accelerate the study of these neuroprotective diets through explanatory and pragmatic trials to potentially mitigate the devastating effects of a disease that is projected to affect more than 100 million people in the next 30 years. World Health Organization. The Top 10 Causes of Death Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) study: Rationale, design and baseline characteristics of a randomized control trial of the MIND diet on cognitive decline Alzheimer's Facts and Figures Dementia: A Public Health Priority Treatment Combinations for Alzheimer's Disease: Current and Future Pharmacotherapy Options When Does Alzheimer's Disease Really Start? The Role of Biomarkers Changing the Trajectory of Alzheimer's Disease Report The Natural History of Cognitive Decline in Alzheimer's Disease Nutrition and the Risk of Alzheimer's Disease Modulating Effect of Diet on Alzheimer's Disease. Diseases Autophagy and Alzheimer's Disease Reduction of SorLA/LR11, a Sorting Protein Limiting β-Amyloid Production Dietary DHA and health: Cognitive function ageing Omega 3 fatty acid for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia Understanding the Role of Systemic Inflammation in Alzheimer's Disease Involvement of Oxidative Stress in Alzheimer Disease MIND diet associated with reduced incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement Western Diet Induces Impairment of Liver-Brain Axis Accelerating Neuroinflammation and Amyloid Pathology in Alzheimer's Disease. Front Mediterranean diet: The role of long-chain ω-3 fatty acids in fish tea, cacao and wine; probiotics and vitamins in prevention of stroke, age-related cognitive decline, and Alzheimer disease Cohort Profile: Design and methods of the PREDIMED study DASH Dietary Pattern and Cardiometabolic Outcomes: An Umbrella Review of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Dietary Patterns and Cardiometabolic Outcomes in Diabetes: A Summary of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses The role of diet for prevention and management of hypertension The influence of smoking, sedentary lifestyle and obesity on cognitive impairment-free life expectancy Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective Association Between Midlife Vascular Risk Factors and Estimated Brain Amyloid Deposition The overlap between vascular disease and Alzheimer's disease-Lessons from pathology Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission Cognitive decline, dietary factors and gut-brain interactions Relationships of dietary patterns, foods, and micro-and macronutrients with Alzheimer's disease and late-life cognitive disorders: A systematic review Can nutrition support healthy cognitive ageing and reduce dementia risk? Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline over time in an elderly Mediterranean population Mediterranean diet improves cognition: The PREDIMED-NAVARRA randomised trial Mediterranean Diet and Age-Related Cognitive Decline: A Randomized Clinical Trial Prospective study of Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension-and Mediterranean-style dietary patterns and age-related cognitive change: The Cache County Study on Memory, Health and Aging DASH and Mediterranean-Type Dietary Patterns to Maintain Cognitive Health Dietary Patterns and Cognitive Decline Among Chinese Older Adults The Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Diet, Cognitive Function, and Cognitive Decline in American Older Women MIND diet slows cognitive decline with aging MIND not Mediterranean diet related to 12-year incidence of cognitive impairment in an Australian longitudinal cohort study Dietary Patterns and Cognitive Health in Older Adults: A Systematic Review Nutrition, Inflammation, and Cognitive Function Association between dietary fiber and markers of systemic inflammation in the Women's Heath Initiative Observational Study Formation of short chain fatty acids by the gut microbiota and their impact on human metabolism Effect of plant-based diets on obesity-related inflammatory profiles: A systematic review and meta-analysis of intervention trials Vascular action of polyphenols Neuroprotective Potential of Ellagic Acid: A Critical Review Associations between Dietary Pulses Alone or with Other Legumes and Cardiometabolic Disease Outcomes: An Umbrella Review and Updated Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Prospective Cohort Studies Olive Oil and Red Wine Antioxidant Polyphenols Inhibit Endothelial Activation: Antiatherogenic Properties of Mediterranean Diet Phytochemicals López de las Hazas, M.d.C.; et al. Virgin Olive Oil Enriched with Its Own Phenols or Complemented with Thyme Phenols Improves DNA Protection against Oxidation and Antioxidant Enzyme Activity in Hyperlipidemic Subjects Red wine antioxidants protect hippocampal neurons against ethanol-induced damage: A biochemical, morphological and behavioral study Red wine consumption increases antioxidant status and decreases oxidative stress in the circulation of both young and old humans Adherence to a Mediterranean Diet and Survival in a Greek Population The Development of the Mediterranean-Style Dietary Pattern Score and Its Application to the American Diet in the Framingham Offspring Cohort1-3 Adherence to a DASH-Style Diet and Risk of Coronary Heart Disease and Stroke in Women DASH diet and prevalent metabolic syndrome in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos Preventing Cognitive Decline and Dementia: A Way Forward Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH), and Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) Diets Are Associated with Less Cognitive Decline and a Lower Risk of Alzheimer's Disease-A Review The Mediterranean Diet and Cognitive Function among Healthy Older Adults in a 6-Month Randomised Controlled Trial: The MedLey Study Randomized trial of the effects of cholesterol-lowering dietary treatment on psychological function Effects of the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Diet, Exercise, and Caloric Restriction on Neurocognition in Overweight Adults with High Blood Pressure Association of long-term adherence to the mind diet with cognitive function and cognitive decline in American women Adherence to Mediterranean diet and subjective cognitive function in men The Mediterranean Diet Is Not Related to Cognitive Change in a Large Prospective Investigation: The PATH Through Life Study Mediterranean diet and cognitive function: The SUN project Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and Alzheimer's disease risk in an Australian population Dietary patterns and cognitive decline in an Australian study of ageing Inflammatory and Metabolic Biomarkers, and Risk of Alzheimer's Disease. J. Alzheimer's Dis No Association between Dietary Patterns and Risk for Cognitive Decline in Older Women with 9-Year Follow-Up: Data from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study Mediterranean diet and cognitive function: A French study Association between the mediterranean diet and cognitive decline in a biracial population Dietary patterns and cognitive dysfunction in a 12-year follow-up study of 70 year old men Unsaturated Fats, Moderate Alcohol Intake, and Mild Cognitive Impairment Long-Term Adherence to the Mediterranean Diet Is Associated with Overall Cognitive Status, but Not Cognitive Decline, in Women The Nordic Prudent Diet Reduces Risk of Cognitive Decline in the Swedish Older Adults: A Population-Based Cohort Study Adherence to a Mediterranean Diet Protects from Cognitive Decline in the Invecchiare in Chianti Study of Aging Adherence to a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and cognitive decline in a community population Relation of DASH-and Mediterranean-like dietary patterns to cognitive decline in older persons Mediterranean diet habits in older individuals: Associations with cognitive functioning and brain volumes Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and risk of incident cognitive impairment Mediterranean Diet and Cognitive Decline in Women with Cardiovascular Disease or Risk Factors Best Practice Guidelines for dietary assessment in health research Racial Differences in Dietary Relations to Cognitive Decline and Alzheimer's Disease Risk: Do We Know Enough? Comparison of three DASH scoring paradigms and prevalent hypertension among older Hispanics Taking Advantage of the Strengths of 2 Different Dietary Assessment Instruments to Improve Intake Estimates for Nutritional Epidemiology Dietary assessment methods in epidemiologic studies Combining a Food Frequency Questionnaire with 24-Hour Recalls to Increase the Precision of Estimation of Usual Dietary Intakes-Evidence From the Validation Studies Pooling Project Evolution of Mediterranean diets and cuisine: Concepts and definitions Dietary patterns: A Mediterranean diet score and its relation to clinical and biological markers of cardiovascular disease risk Nutrition and risk of dementia: Overview and methodological issues: Nutrition and dementia Adults Meeting Fruit and Vegetable Intake Recommendations-United States Degree of Concordance with DASH Diet Guidelines and Incidence of Hypertension and Fatal Cardiovascular Disease Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease Advances in screening instruments for Alzheimer's disease Brief cognitive screening instruments for early detection of Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review Overview and findings from the rush Memory and Aging Project Why are so few randomized trials useful, and what can we do about it? Recruitment and Baseline Characteristics of Participants in the AgeWell.de Study-A Pragmatic Cluster-Randomized Controlled Lifestyle Trial against Cognitive Decline A pragmatic view on pragmatic trials World-Wide FINGERS Network: A global approach to risk reduction and prevention of dementia de-Study protocol of a pragmatic multi-center cluster-randomized controlled prevention trial against cognitive decline in older primary care patients Automated Self-Administered 24-Hour (ASA24 ® ) Dietary Assessment Tool When Does Cognitive Decline Begin? A Systematic Review of Change Point Studies on Accelerated Decline in Cognitive and Neurological Outcomes Preceding Mild Cognitive Impairment, Dementia, and Death Challenges in Designing and Delivering Diets and Assessing Adherence: A Randomized Controlled Trial Evaluating the 2010 Dietary Guidelines for Americans Selectivity of Attrition in Longitudinal Studies of Cognitive Functioning Predictors of attrition in a longitudinal cognitive aging study: The Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS) Maximizing the promise of citizen science to advance health and prevent disease National Prescribing Trends for High-Risk Anticholinergic Medications in Older Adults Benzodiazepine use among older adults