key: cord-0713771-ql68fu52 authors: Goodwin, Robin; Ben-Ezra, Menachem; Takahashi, Masahito; Luu, Lan-Anh Nguyen; Borsfay, Krisztina; Kovács, Mónika; Hou, Wai Kai; Hamama-Raz, Yaira; Levin, Yafit title: Psychological factors underpinning vaccine willingness in Israel, Japan and Hungary date: 2022-01-10 journal: Sci Rep DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-03986-2 sha: 6e487316c1915dd43a674672627cfb7c3a579272 doc_id: 713771 cord_uid: ql68fu52 The spread of SARS-CoV-2 led to rapid vaccine development. However, there remains considerable vaccine hesitancy in some countries. We investigate vaccine willingness in three nations with very different vaccine histories: Israel, Japan and Hungary. Employing an ecological-systems approach we analyse associations between health status, individual cognitions, norms, trust in government, COVID-19 myths and willingness to be vaccinated, with data from three nationally representative samples (Israel, Jan. 2021, N = 1011; Japan, Feb. 2021, N = 997; Hungary, April 2021, N = 1130). Vaccine willingness was higher in Israel (74%) than Japan (51%) or Hungary (31%). In all three countries vaccine willingness was greatest amongst who would regret not being vaccinated and respondents who trusted their government. Multi-group latent class analysis identified three groups of COVID myths, with particular concern about alteration of DNA (Israel), allergies (Hungary) and infection from the vaccine (Japan). Intervention campaigns should address such cultural myths while emphasising both individual and social benefits of vaccination. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ is anticipated to be positively related to both threat appraisal and the ability to confront this; specifically, perceived susceptibility of contracting COVID-19, severity of COVID-19, benefits of vaccination, and anticipated regret if not vaccinated 11, 14 . Normative pressures to vaccinate are also anticipated to encourage willingness to vaccinate, while low trust, and a willingness to accept misbeliefs about vaccination, are expected to be negatively associated with willingness to vaccinate 14 . We control in these analyses for the key demographic variables of age (positively associated with vaccine willingness 12, 14 , education (positively associated with uptake 13 ) and sex (with men more willing to vaccine 11, 12 ) . In Israel only we also include religiosity, anticipated to be negatively associated with vaccine willingness 12 . Trust in government authorities is often negatively associated with conspiracy or false beliefs about a pandemic [18] [19] [20] . Misbeliefs, which often focus on the 'response cost' of vaccinations 21 , are particularly significant given the novelty of the vaccines developed 18, 22 , with vaccine side-effects the most prominent concern 20 . While these misbeliefs tend to correlate significantly with each other 19, 20 , they may also be divided along dimensions 18 . In addition to testing our ecological model we examine the clustering of these myths in each country via multi-group latent class analysis (LCA), associating each class in each country with willingness to vaccinate. This paper seeks to address three objectives. First, we compare rates of vaccine willingness across Israel, Japan and Hungary (Objective 1). Second, we examine the relative weight of each of the predictor variables in our three-layer ecological model by conducting a multigroup path analysis in each culture (Objective 2). Finally, (Objective 3), we conduct a sensitivity analysis examining specific cluster structure of myths about COVID-19, and the impact of these on willingness to vaccinate in each country. Objective 1: vaccine willingness in Israel, Japan and Hungary. Table 1 presents differences in the distribution of background and study variables between the three samples. Willingness to vaccinate was higher in Israel (74.1%) than in Japan (51.1%), or Hungary (31%) (χ 2 (2) = 397.86, P = 0.001). Objective 2: testing the ecological model. In Israel ( Table 2) , men were more willing to vaccinate (β = 0.05, p = 0.018). Those with higher subjective rated health were less willing to vaccinate (β = −0.05, p = 0.045). There were positive associations between willingness to vaccinate and the cognitive factors of benefits of vaccine (β = 0.17, p < 0.001), anticipated regret (β = 0.28, p < 0.001), and subjective norms (β = 0.08, p < 0.003) while barriers to vaccination were associated with reluctance to vaccinate (β = −0.13 p < 0.001). Trust in government was positively associated with willingness to vaccinate (β = 0.06, p = 0.012), and false beliefs about COVID-19 significantly associated with unwillingness to vaccinate (β = −0.16, p < 0.001). In Japan, education was positively associated with willingness to vaccinate (β = 0.06, p = 0.036). There were positive associations between willingness to vaccinate and the cognitive factor of anticipated regret if not vaccinated (β = 0.16, p < 0.001) while barriers to vaccination were associated with reluctance to vaccinate (β = −0.40 p < 0.001). Subjective norms were associated with willingness to vaccinate (β = 0.11 p = 0.001) as was trust in government (β = 0.13, p < 0.001). Finally, in Hungary, men were more willing to vaccinate (β = 0.11, p < 0.001), and there were positive associations between willingness to vaccinate and benefits of vaccine (β = 0.22, p < 0.001), anticipated regret (β = 0.22, P < 0.001) and Across the world there is evidence of continued vaccine unease, with vaccine resistance identified as a top ten threat to global health even before COVID-19 26 . Countries however have fared differently in the availability of vaccines and the trust of their populations towards a growing range of possible vaccines, with a nested set of factors influencing uptake. In Israel, a well-established community-based health service, large-scale public health campaigns and the early purchase of a large number of vaccines helped the country achieve a rapid and comprehensive roll-out of the Pfizer-BioNtec vaccine 27, 28 . Unsurprisingly therefore almost three-quarters of our sample in this country demonstrated willingness to be vaccinated. In contrast, our Hungarian and Japanese respondents were less willing to vaccinate. In our sample only just over half (51%) of Japanese respondents indicated such willingness, higher than the 45% reported by the Imperial College COVID-19 tracker in January 2021 but lower than the 62% indicated by a cross-sectional survey also conducted that month 11 . In Hungary, initial delays in accessing some vaccines, and the politicisation of the vaccine roll-out 7 , (including disputes over the use of vaccines not approved by the EU 8 ), contributed to high national rates of scepticism about efficacy, with only 31% of our national sample expressing willingness to vaccinate, and a further 21% uncertain. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ In competitive regressions demographic factors, and personal or family experience with COVID-19, had only a small association with vaccine willingness, although men were significantly more willing to vaccinate in Israel and Hungary. However, those who were more likely to regret not vaccinating were more likely to indicate vaccine willingness. There were only small and culturally variable associations between perceived likelihood or severity of infection and vaccine intention, indicating only a weak association between perceived threat and vaccination. This may be because while infection likelihood and severity are closely associated with viral threat, benefits and regrets may be more proximally associated with actual vaccine behaviour. Subjective pressure to vaccinate was significantly higher in Israel and Japan compared to Hungary and associated with vaccine uptake in just these first two countries. This suggests that the importance of friends, families and others for vaccine willingness may be particularly significant where important others are prepared to be vaccinated. Trust in government emerged in all three countries as a significant contributor to vaccine willingness, as elsewhere 22 . This association was strongest in Hungary, where vaccine uptake, and choice of vaccine, has been particularly politically contentious. False information about the virus, most likely to emerge from social media, has been shown to be negatively associated with COVID-19 health protective behaviour 23 , including vaccine willingness 12 . In Israel, where we included also included religion, additional analyses found false beliefs to be strongest in the Ultra-Orthodox population, lowest in secular respondents (F (3, 1003) = 4.68 P = 0.003). As reported elsewhere 19, 30 , misbeliefs were significantly associated with low trust in governmental authorities (rs −0.25,−0.24 and −0.32 for Israel, Japan and Hungary respectively, P = 0.001) but only the association in Israel between false beliefs and vaccine willingness survived the competitive regression models. In our samples, misbeliefs about the vaccine correlated with each other, supporting the idea of a 'monological belief system' 20, 31 . However, in Israel the belief that COVID-19 can alter your DNA (held by 62% of those in the second category) was distinctive as a predictor of vaccine (un)willingness. Contrastingly, in Hungary it was the association between allergies and the vaccine (held by 77% of those in class 2) that distinguished a grouping of respondents reluctant to take the vaccine. In Japan, the largest latent class (43% of respondents) indicated their belief that the vaccine can give you COVID-19. Notably, 61.5% of Japanese respondents who believed the vaccine can give you COVID in Japan were unwilling to vaccinate, compared to 48.6% Israel and 14.0% in Hungary, suggesting the particular significance of this misbelief in Japan for vaccine intentions. Limitations. Our studies benefitted by including national samples from three very different cultures, with different histories of vaccine uptake. However, we recognise a number of limitations to our survey. First, samples were cross-sectional, and were therefore not able to assess predictors of vaccine willingness over time. Data was first collected in early January, at the start of the first major vaccine roll-out, meaning that we did not include later misbeliefs that emerged in subsequent months. Emergent concerns over vaccine safety (such as worries about blood clotting following the AstraZeneca vaccine 32 ) may serve to directly inhibit uptake and perpetuate further new misbeliefs and distrust. Second, because of the speed of the evolving vaccination situation in both www.nature.com/scientificreports/ countries our survey companies expedited data collection within a short time period. Although widely used, and particularly particularly for the collection of time-sensitive data during a vaccination campaign, we recognise that the quota sampling has important limitations in ascertaining accurate response rates 12, 33 . In Hungary in particular the low willingness to vaccinate may have been impacted by the omission in our data of those already vaccinated. Third, we assessed only three countries; future work should expand the testing of nested models across settings. We were not able to assess income or family structure across all countries, so did not include this in our analyses: both factors might be valuable further predictors of vaccine willingness. Finally, we measure only behavioural intentions rather than actual vaccination behaviour. Although the link between the two has been well established 34 we recognise that attitudes towards any vaccination are likely to vary as populations acquire further experience with the vaccination rollout. Implications for vaccine drives. Despite these limitations we believe our findings have important implications as other nations strive to accelerate their vaccination drives. Vaccine campaigns may need not focus on disease threat: instead, these initiatives would better focus on the effectiveness of the vaccine, confront misinformation, and seek to emphasise the trustworthiness of key actors, such as national health services. Those vaccinated should be encouraged to inform close others, in order to emphasise the normative nature of this activity. Public health agencies need to reach people beyond remote media campaigns and be present where individuals shop and work 35 ; doctors have been widely reported to be important in addressing myths 36 with pharmacists in Hungary significant for encouraging influenza vaccination in that country 29 . Our analyses suggested that, despite some similarities in belief structure, there were distinctive beliefs in each culture important for understanding vaccine willingness. Finally, within country, group factors may be also particularly important, including culturally variable myths about COVID-19 37 . We note that in some countries (e.g. Israel), uptake has been greater in settlements with higher socio-economic status, despite the greater morbidity from COVID-19 amongst poorer communities 38 . To address these variations in uptake the specific concerns of religious and other social groups need to be considered, with community leaders actively engaged through culturally appropriate conversations in order to allay fears, address specific myths and thus help further facilitate a successful vaccine campaign 39 . Participants and procedure. Data were from nationally representative samples of adult populations collected in Israel (N = 1011), Japan (N = 997) and Hungary (N = 1130), using large panel survey companies in each country (iPanel for Israel, Quesant! for Japan, Medián Opinion and Market Research in Hungary). Eighty respondents in Japan did not respond to the vaccination willingness item but provided other data (e.g. COVID-19 myths). In each country quota sampling was used to ensure the sample: participant sex, age (Israel), sex, age and geographical region (Japan, Hungary) were chosen to match population parameters in each country. Data in Israel were collected from 31.12.2020 to 11.01.2021 i.e. early on during the first vaccination drive. At that time the percentage of those receiving their first vaccination doubled, from 11 to 22% of the population 2 . In Hungary, data were obtained from 8.4.21 to 16.4.21, during which 27.8% (then 32.0%) had received at least one vaccine 2 . In Japan data were collected prior to the start of the vaccination campaign (between 15.2.2021 and 16.2.2021). For each sample participants were contacted as part of cloud panels administered by the survey company and asked to participate via email. They were then reimbursed by the companies for their participation. All participants provided informed consent before proceeding with the study. In each country inclusion criteria required participants be the approved minimal age set by ethical requirements (Israel, Hungary-18, Japan-20), and to successfully pass validation checks (both specific items and timing of responses) to ensure participant attention and accuracy. All respondents were fluent in the relevant national language. Ethical approval was from Ariel University's Institutional Review Board (No. AU-SOC-MBE-20201224), the Yamaguchi University Review Committee for Non-Medical Research Involving Human Participants (2020-004-01) and the Eötvös Loránd University ethical review panel (PPK KEB 2021/130-2). All data collection was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the relevant guidelines and regulations in each country. Demographics. Participants in each country indicated their age, sex and education. Because demographic information procedures vary across countries we obtained slight variations in each country, in common with other such cross-cultural comparisons 12 , while retaining the core ecosystems model variables in each country for comparative analysis. In Japan and Israel respondents identified whether they completed only high school prior to University or were currently a student/had graduated; in Hungary students indicated their level of schooling. Education was then recoded to academic vs. non-academic education. Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for each country. Health conditions. Respondents indicated their risk group membership using the US CDC risk group memberships (e.g. hypertension, diabetes). Participants also indicated whether they had been formally diagnosed with COVID-19 (yes, no), whether someone from their social circle had been thus diagnosed (yes, no), and their self-rated health (from bad (1) to excellent (4)). Vaccine acceptance. Participants were asked Would you be willing to accept a vaccine approved safe and effective by the government? (strongly disagree to strongly agree)). Because we questioned respondents at the start of actual vaccine campaigns, rather than about a hypothetical vaccine, and were cautious about both the translation of 'uncertain/undecided' and the use of intermediate responses in some cultural contexts 40 we treat this as a binary response, in line with much previous cross-national vaccine research 22 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ ('completely agree' or 'somewhat agree') vs. 0 (undecided, unwilling, very unwilling) 3, 22 . We provide individual scores on the five-point scale by country in the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Table S2 ). Predictors of vaccine willingness. We included eight potential predictors of vaccine willingness drawn from three major theoretical perspectives previously used to assess vaccine uptake: the Health Belief Model (HBM), the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) and the Theory of Protection Motivation (TPM) 13, 14, 34 , as well as subsequent work on the influence of false beliefs and trust in authorities on vaccine willingness. Full items and response categories are reported in the Online Supplementary File (Supplementary Table S1 ), scale alphas in Table 1 . Analytic procedure. Since samples differed on some important demographic characteristics, we tested if oneto-one propensity-score matching should be used between cultural groups and between the willing and unwilling to vaccinate [41] [42] [43] . To estimate the propensity score, a multinomial and logistic regression model was used to predict culture (three groups) and the willingness to vaccinate (willing/unwilling) with the following covariate variables: age, sex, education and subjective rated health, using the Matching package for R. After the propensity score was estimated, we examined the extent to which matching produced balance 44 Table S3 ). Mean differences were 73.28 and 74.14 for matched and unmatched samples.. As we would need to limit our analyses by reducing sample size substantially in order to use matching samples we used instead residuals from regressions for the purpose of controlling for unwanted effects in multivariable datasets and to produce unbiased parameter estimates 45 . Residual variables were saved in the regression procedure, to "clean" the variables from the effects of the covariates. In order to calculate residual scores, we conducted a logistic regression predicting willingness to vaccinate by age, gender, education and subjective rated health, as well as the interaction between them. We report residuals that control for the variance related to covariates in Supplementary Table S4 . We utilized layered multi-group logistic regression analysis using MPlus 8.1 46 to test the ecological model. Data were analysed using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) to handle nonnormal distributions. We used multi-group analyses 47 to test if paths from the covariates, demographic factors and health status, individual cognitions, normative pressures, trust in government, belief in COVID-19 myths and willingness to be vaccinated varied by culture. We included participant information on their own (or social network's) positive COVID-19 diagnoses. Missing data due to nonresponse ranged from 0.9% to 4%. To analyse the structure of misbeliefs about the vaccine we employed Multi-Group Latent Class Analysis (MLCA) to identify subgroups within the three samples (Israel, Japan and Hungary). We use binary indicators of false beliefs. We specified models with one to four classes and compared the models to determine the optimal number of classes. Models with lower values on the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), and sample-size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (aBIC) were prioritized. Entropy values and average latent class probabilities indicated classification accuracy, with preference for models with entropy values and probabilities of correct class assignment closer to 1.00 48 . Once optimal number of classes was determined we computed sample percentages assigned to each class and conditional probabilities by class, with labels for latent classes based on patterns of probabilities for endorsing each false belief. Datasets are available in the Open Science Framework (OSF) repository: https:// osf. io/ dm587/. Received: 22 July 2021; Accepted: 8 December 2021 Mathematical assessment of the roles of vaccination and non-pharmaceutical interventions on COVID-19 dynamics: a multigroup modeling approach COVID-19 vaccination doses per 100 people and vaccine acceptance rates Covid-19 Behaviour Tracker Mapping global trends in vaccine confidence and investigating barriers to vaccine uptake: a large-scale retrospective temporal modelling study Expectations and Anxieties About the COVID-19 Vaccine in Japan Now is the opposition really anti-vaccination? Telex Willingness to Use an Approved COVID-19 Vaccine: Cross-national Evidence on Levels and Individuallevel Predictors I took the trouble to make inquiries, so i refuse to accept your instructions": Religious authority and vaccine hesitancy among ultra-orthodox jewish mothers in Israel Vaccine hesitancy as self-determination: An Israeli perspective Acceptance of a COVID-19 Vaccine in Japan during the COVID-19 pandemic Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom Using a theoretical framework to determine adults' intention to vaccinate against pandemic swine flu in priority groups in the UK COVID-19 vaccination intention in the UK: Results from the COVID-19 vaccination acceptability study (CoVAccS), a nationally representative cross-sectional survey Barriers of influenza vaccination intention and behaviour-A systematic review of influenza vaccine hesitancy Factors associated with uptake of vaccination against pandemic influenza: A systematic review The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England Are COVID-19 Conspiracies a Threat to Public Health? Psychological Characteristics and Health Protective Behaviours of Believers Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions A global survey of potential acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency Comparing typological structures across cultures by multigroup latent class analysis: A primer Bayesian model selection in social research Top ten threats to global health in 2019 Israel's rapid rollout of vaccinations for COVID-19 Israel's vaccination rollout: short term success, but questions for the long run Determinants of influenza vaccine uptake and willingness to be vaccinated by pharmacists among the active adult population in Hungary: A cross-sectional exploratory study Systematic literature review on the spread of health related misinformation on social media Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories Thromboembolism and the Oxford-AstraZeneca vaccine Summary report of the AAPOR task force on non-probability sampling Predicting intention to receive a seasonal influenza vaccination using Protection Motivation Officials gird for a war on vaccine misinformation COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the Segmentation of the number of vaccinators by age and locality Cultural competence in covid-19 vaccine rollout Response style and cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales among East Asian and North American students Constructing a control group using multivariate matched sampling methods that incorporate the propensity score Propensity score adjustment with multilevel data: setting your sites on decreasing selection bias A critical appraisal of propensity-score matching in the medical literature between Multivariate and propensity score matching software with automated balance optimization: The matching package for On the misuse of residuals in ecology: Regression of residuals vs. multiple regression Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables: User's Guide Version 8 Principles and practice of structural equation modeling 4th edn Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study Writing-original draft preparation Funding for the study in Israel was awarded to Prof. Ben-Ezra from Ariel University No. RA2000000522 in Japan to Professor Masahito Takahashi (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 18K01963) and in Hungary to Drs. Lan Anh Nguyen Lu and Mónika Kovács (National Excellence Programme 2018-1.2.1-NKP-2018-00006). The authors declare no competing interests. The online version contains supplementary material available at https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s41598-021-03986-2.Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to R.G.Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.