key: cord-0704969-34gzbpnx authors: Haag, Christophe; Bellinghausen, Lisa; Jilinskaya-Pandey, Mariya title: QEPro: An ability measure of emotional intelligence for managers in a French cultural environment date: 2021-04-23 journal: Curr Psychol DOI: 10.1007/s12144-021-01715-6 sha: e1a6a0128d85f44fa60792e825806e3d80d174ad doc_id: 704969 cord_uid: 34gzbpnx Managers’ interest in the concept of emotional intelligence (EI) has grown steadily due to an accumulation of published articles and books touting EI’s benefits. For over thirty years, many researchers have used or designed tools for measuring EI, most of which raise important psychometric, cultural and contextual issues. The aim of this article is to address some of the main limitations observed in previous studies of EI. By developing and validating QEPro we propose a new performance-based measure of EI based on a modified version of Mayer and Salovey’s (1997) four-branch model. QEPro is an ability EI measure specifically dedicated to managers and business executives in a French cultural environment (N = 1035 managers and executives). In order to increase both the ecological and the face validity of the test for the target population we used the Situational Judgment Tests framework and a theory-based item development and scoring approach. For all items, correct and incorrect response options were developed using established theories from the emotion and management fields. Our study showed that QEPro has good psychometric qualities such as high measurement precision and internal consistency, an appropriate level of difficulty and a clear factorial structure. The tool also correlates in meaningful and theoretically congruent ways with general intelligence, Trait EI measures, the Big Five factors of personality, and the Affect measures used in this study. For all these reasons, QEPro is a promising tool for studying the role of EI competencies in managerial outcomes. For the past thirty years, an "affective revolution" (Barsade et al., 2003) has taken place in the workplace leading scholars around the world to study emotional intelligence (EI) in management. The intelligent use of emotions emerges as the new challenge for managers whose role is emotionally demanding (Ashkanasy et al., 2019; Ashkanasy & Daus, 2002; Côté, 2017; Haag and Getz, 2016, b; Humphrey et al., 2016) , even more today with the COVID-19 crisis (Brooks et al., 2020) . This worldwide health crisis -which is as much an economic and psychological crisis -has forced managers to be even more aware of their own emotions and the emotions of their subordinates, many of whom are anxious and struggle to regulate their stress at work (Serafini et al., 2020) . Managers with a high level of EI have proved to be more able to regulate disturbing emotions for self and others Farh, Seo, & Tesluk, 2012; Haag and Getz, 2016, b; Tse, Troth, Ashkanasy, & Collins, 2018) compared to those lacking such competence. Indeed, managers with a low level of EI, will need to develop their ability in order to successfully adapt -and help their team adapt -to this tense environment (Côté, 2017; McNulty & Marcus, 2020; Reiman et al., 2015; Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . As such, the development of accurate EI measures and development programs dedicated to managers, especially those lacking EI, becomes crucial for organizations. Therefore, the aim of this article is to propose and validate an ability-based measure of EI with theory-based scoring, dedicated to managers. Two approaches of EI coexist today: (a) EI as an "ability" (one form of intelligence among others; Farh et al., 2012; Haag and Getz, 2016, b; Tse et al., 2018) and (b) EI as a "personality trait" (a trait among others; Bar-On, 1997; Goleman, 1995; Petrides & Furnham, 2000 , 2003 . Despite their differences, these two approaches of EI -each with their own merits (Mikolajczak, 2010) -converge on at least two aspects. First, EI has a positive impact on various factors, such as health, performance in the workplace, and the level of well-being (Mikolajczak, 2010; Zeidner et al., 2012b, a) . Second, numerous studies have shown that these benefits are of interest to companies and their stakeholders (Côté, 2014) . "Personality trait" EI models depict a constellation of "traits". These models integrate aspects of personality, motivation, affective disposition and intelligence into the EI approach Roberts et al., 2008; Zeidner et al., 2004) . Trait models are strongly correlated to personality (Ciarrochi et al., 2001; Roberts et al., 2008) , but some researchers argue that "trait EI explains additional variance over and above related traits such as alexithymia or the Big Five" to predict different outcomes such as work performance (Mikolajczak et al., 2010, p. 26) . Despite the argument regarding Trait EI models having neurobiological correlates (Mikolajczak & Luminet, 2008) , they might not measure anything fundamentally different from the Big Five (Davis & Humphrey, 2014 ) and therefore would not constitute a new form of intelligence Zeidner et al., 2004) . In contrast, the "ability" approach considers emotional intelligence to be a set of cognitive skills. The underlying idea is that emotions are information that are captured and processed by human brain (John D. Mayer et al., 2004) . Ability approach of EI proposes a unique concept as it differs from analytical intelligence (Mayer et al., 2000a, b) and personality (Lopes et al., 2003) . This EI model combines key ideas from the fields of emotion and intelligence (Mayer et al., 2000a, b) . The arguments mentioned above led most researchers in the field of EI to consider the ability approach as the most promising one Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019; Zeidner et al., 2004) . This approach historically refers to Mayer & Salovey's (1997) fourbranch model presented below: (1) Branch 1 -Perception, Appraisal and Expression of Emotion: defined as the ability to accurately recognize emotions in self and others, to express emotions accurately and to discriminate between accurate and inaccurate / honest and dishonest expressions of feelings. (2) Branch 2 -Emotional Facilitation of Thinking: defined as the ability to orient attention to important information that helps in judgment and memory. This ability also encourages the change of individual perspective and supports the capacity to consider multiple point of views thanks to emotional mood swings. (3) Branch 3 -Understanding and Analyzing Emotions: defined as the ability to label emotions and recognize relationships among words and emotions themselves, to interpret the meaning of emotions, to understand complex feelings and to recognize transitions among emotions. Emotional and Intellectual Growth: defined as the ability to stay open to feelings, to reflectively engage or detach from emotions, to monitor and to manage emotions in relation to oneself and others. Three out of the four Mayer and Salovey's branches (1, 3 and 4) have shown to be useful for managers (Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . For example, some studies have revealed benefits of a well-developed ability to recognize emotions (Branch 1) for various management related tasks. This ability is associated with various outcomes such as receiving better ratings from subordinates (Brotheridge & Lee, 2008; Byron, 2008) improving effectiveness in negotiation (Elfenbein et al., 2007) emerging as a leader in the group (Walter et al., 2012) and behaving as a transformational leader (Rubin et al., 2005) . Researchers have also shown that decision-makers who are able to understand emotions (Branch 3) take more efficient decisions as they are less influenced by irrelevant feelingssuch as incidental anxiety -which are unrelated to the decision at hand (Yip & Côté, 2013) . More recently, these researchers have observed that decision makers with low EI tend to adopt maladaptive decision-making, due to their incorrect appraisal of intensity of physiological arousal (Yip et al., 2020) . Finally, series of studies have shown that managers who display a strong ability to regulate their own emotions (Branch 4) tend to have a positive influence on their team's performance (Lopes et al., 2006; Rice, 1999) , experience an improved quality of their social interactions with others (Lopes et al., 2004 (Lopes et al., , 2005 , and, overall, feel more comfortable with themselves (Côté, 2010; Haag & Getz, 2016) . Beyond the ability to regulate their own emotions, regulating the emotions of others (in particular those of their team members) is crucial for managers as these emotions are often important factors in team performance (Haag & Getz, 2016; Sy et al., 2005) . Researchers acknowledge that the best measurements of emotional competencies within the EI ability framework are performance-based tests (Petrides et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2010) . These types of tests are often opposed to self-report measures mostly used to measure "Trait EI". Self-report questionnaires are subject to key limitations such as social desirability (Day & Carroll, 2004; , lack of respondent's accuracy at estimating his/her own abilities (Brackett et al., 2006; Kruger & Dunning, 1999; Sheldon et al., 2014) . In addition, "Trait EI" measures "violate the first law of intelligence" (Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019, p. 560) because of their correlations with personality measures (Matthews et al., 2004, p. 225) and their lack of correlation with cognitive intelligence . In contrast, performance-based tests of EI measure individual performance in solving emotional problems and performing emotional tasks. There is only one correct answer, others being incorrect. These tests "measure maximum performance in that individuals know that they will be evaluated, are instructed to maximize their efforts, and are measured over a short period of time" (Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019, p. 560) . However, there are psychometric issues and limitations to current performance-based EI tests (Fiori & Antonakis, 2011; Roberts et al., 2010) . Two main criticisms are often addressed to Ability EI measures: psychometric issues and lack of workplace relevance of the items (Davies et al., 1998; Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . Several researchers have highlighted psychometric issues with ability measures of EI mainly consensus, expert based scoring and their reliability and validity Maul, 2012; O'Connor et al., 2019) . Performance tasks involve right and wrong answers. However, determining the "right answer" for emotional intelligence tests is not as obvious as for other ability measures. Unlike conventional performance tasks, there is not a single answer to an item. Thus, "correct" answers have to be determined in different ways. Different methods have been identified (MacCann et al., 2004) : researchers have set up a multi scoring method, thus contrasting with the scoring method used in traditional intelligence tests. Most frequently used methods to establish individual scores are "expert" scoring method and "consensus" scoring. The MSCEIT (John D. Mayer, 2002) and the STEU (MacCann & both use these scoring methods. The consensus criterion is the most widely used scoring principle (MacCann et al., 2004; John D. Mayer et al., 1999) . It consists in taking the modal score for an item as the best (correct) response to the item. Distribution of scores of the experimental group is used to constitute the percentage grid to determine the attribution of individual scores. To properly discriminate scores, proportions of responses for each response modality are taken into account. Individual responses are coded into frequencies of responses given for each item by the entire sample. This method is often criticized because it assimilates the majority's opinion to the right answer (Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . Consensus can help assess whether a response is incorrect, but it lacks the power to discriminate sensitive issues raised by social relationships. Thus, this method does not discriminate participants satisfyingly and a tendency towards right-asymmetric distributions is often observed (MacCann et al., 2004; Roberts et al., 2001) . With the expert criterion scoring method, experts first complete the questionnaire as participants. Once the responses are collected, the experts' scores are transformed into frequencies. Then, participants' scores are calibrated according to experts' response frequencies (MacCann et al., 2004) . It should be noted that expert scoring methods also present limits (Conte, 2005; Maul, 2012; O'Connor et al., 2019; Roberts et al., 2001) especially for abstract dimensions as regulation of emotions . In sum, these two methods both have strong limitations and despite their differences produce comparable results (MacCann et al., 2004; John D. Mayer et al., 1999) . New perspectives on scoring of EI ability measures have emerged in order to address those limitations such as theorybased item development and scoring. In this approach theories are used to define the characteristics representing high and low ability levels for each measured competence. These characteristics are then included as response options for each test items. The response option containing the characteristics associated with high ability levels "as defined by a given established theory, is specified as the objectively correct response and is expected to be chosen with a higher probability by individuals with a higher ability EI level" (Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019, p. 561) . For example, GECo (Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) and STEM (MacCann & Roberts, 2008) are based on this scoring method. To anchor QEPro in the theory based-item development framework, each dimension was defined theoretically and then operationalized in reference to Management and Affective Sciences. Correct answers as well as distractors were created based on findings from experimental studies in the field of emotion and emotional regulation. Those studies combine findings from general as well as managementspecific contexts. Following Ashkanasy and Daus's (2005) classification of EI measures, QEPro thus belongs to the category of ability measures based on Mayer and Salovey's model: maximum performance test with correct and incorrect answers. As such, all QEPro items are scored by standards based on emotion research / theory-based scoring rather than by consensus or expert rating (Kasten & Freund, 2015; MacCann et al., 2011) . A typical situational judgment test consists of a series of scenarios with a set of multiple-choice answers. SJTs are measurement methods that present applicants with job-related situations and possible responses to these situations. Applicants have to indicate which response alternative they would choose in real life. Therefore, the strengths of SJTs are that they show criterion-related validity and incremental validity above cognitive ability and personality tests (Lievens et al., 2008) . For example, Situational Test of Emotional Understanding (STEU, MacCann & Roberts, 2008) uses SJTs framework, but with scenarios unrelated to work. This test evaluates only one subdimension of EI, which is Understanding Emotions. QEPro was conceived in the SJTs Framework. As such, common emotional situations encountered in managerial practices were collected from different occupational settings (pharmaceutical organizations, banking and finance, HRM...). This exploration of emotional situations led to the construction of vignettes (descriptions of situations familiar to managers) that are likely to occur in different managerial contexts. In this framework, the ecological validity of the assessment (i.e. the predictive relationship between one's performance on a set of tasks and one's actual behavior in a variety of realworld settings) is improved thanks to the increased verisimilitude of the test items and real-world situations (Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996) . Indeed, as the verisimilitude of the items refers to the "the similarity between the task demands of the test and the demands imposed in the everyday environment" (Spooner & Pachana, 2006, p. 328) , the SJT anchors the vignette's context in the managerial environment where they usually appear. Similarly, the face validity of a test is defined by Streiner et al. (2015) as a subjective judgment, made by the target population, whether on the face of it, the instrument appears to be assessing the desired qualities. Although face validity is additional to more important psychometric qualities (such as criterion-related, content or construct validity) for psychological or educational tools intended for practical use, it is an important psychometric quality (Anastasi & Urbina, 1996; Brown, 1983; Cronbach, 1970) . Indeed, a test with high face validity may have a better chance of inducing cooperation, reducing dissatisfaction and feeling of injustice among low scorers as well as increasing motivation during the test taking process (Nevo, 1985) . SJT framework and construction of QEPro items with a theory-driven scoring approach will increase both the ecological validity Orchard et al., 2009) and the face validity of the test for the target population (Holden, 2010) . Mayer and Salovey's (1997) model was used as a framework for the construction of QEPro. We selected the three most robust branches of their model: Emotion Identification, Emotion Understanding and Emotion Management branches. We excluded the Emotional Facilitation branch because it did not load as a distinct factor in factor analyses and structural models (Ciarrochi et al., 2000; MacCann et al., 2014; Palmer et al., 2005; Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . The three selected dimensions were conceived at an ability level, as such, they were operationalized through a second level of emotional competencies which can be observed at the manager's behavioral level. The following section describes QEPro's three abilities with their corresponding emotional competencies (Table 1) . IE refers to the ability to accurately identify emotions in self and in others. To identify emotions, one has to be able to gather, combine and process different types of emotional information: facial, postural and physiological cues, behavioral and cognitive manifestations, and triggers of emotions (e.g. Ekman, 1994; Ekman & Friesen, 1978; Scherer, 2000) . (1) Scanning Physiological Manifestations. This competence refers to an individual's ability to identify her/his own emotions according to an introspective analysis of the physical sensations experienced. Nummenmaa et al. (2014) mapped bodily sensations associated with different kinds of emotions. Each emotion was associated with a unique map. Scanning one's body can therefore help recognize the type of emotion currently being experienced. Emotions can activate specific parts of the body (e.g. increased heat, tensed muscles) or deactivate them (e.g. numbness), accelerate rhythms of the body (e.g. heart rate, respiration) or slow them down (e.g. decreased heart rate) (James, 1922; Levenson, 2003; Nummenmaa et al., 2014) . Noticing these changes and associating them with specific emotions creates a valuable source of information for better understanding and decoding one's environment (Damasio, 1996) . Similarly, this competence also serves to identify emotions of others. The physiological components of emotional responses of others can be both observed (e.g. accelerated heart rate, breathing, muscle tension…) as well as experienced in self through an emotional contagion process (Hatfield et al., 1993) . (2) Interpreting Emotional Cues. In addition to the physiological level, emotions can also be identified through their cognitive manifestations; behavioral action tendencies; vocal, postural and facial cues; and the associated subjective-experiential component (Frijda, 1986; Luminet, 2008; Scherer, 1984) . These cues differ in intensity and are not always easy to recognize in self or others. For instance, the cue may consist of only weak signals, such as a slight smirk, a furtive look, or a slightly raised eyebrow. (3) Identifying Emotional Triggers. In addition to their manifestations, emotions are also associated with specific triggers (Basch & Fisher, 1998; . Those triggers indicate that something occurred in one's environment (e.g. danger, a loss, etc.), which can have a positive or negative impact on self and others. Therefore, this third dimension refers to the competence of individuals to identify the specific triggers of their own emotional state and that of others. Identifying the causes of emotions is important as it allows to complete the identification process and initiate the understanding of what to do in order to better adapt to a situation . The ability to use those three competencies in a concomitant and combined manner increases the accuracy and the efficiency of the identification process. As such, the ability to identify emotions can be seen as a meta-competence. UE refers to the ability to accurately appreciate the intensity level of a given emotional state and to anticipate its evolution over time and its consequences on self and others (John D. Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . (1) Understanding Emotional Timelines. This competence allows an individual to assess the intensity of her/ his emotional state (and that of others) and to anticipate its evolution over time. Emotions of the same category will logically follow one another along an intensity continuum over time (e.g. before feeling anger or becoming enraged, an individual will experience different, less intense emotional states; Plutchik, 1984) . Knowing this sequencing not only allows one to estimate the precise intensity level currently being experienced, but also to make predictions regarding likely future emotional states, for both self and others. (2) Anticipating Emotional Outcomes. This competence allows an individual to anticipate the positive and negative consequences of an emotion. Each emotion can be associated with a specific adaptive role Damasio, 1994; Darwin, 1872; Fredrickson, 2002) . The outcomes of emotions (e.g. behaviors, action tendencies, cognitive patterns…) are associated with both positive and negative implications (Tran, 2007) . For example, anger, on the one hand, can enhance aggressiveness which could nurture conflict and potential fighting while, on the other hand, anger can also help to gain self-confidence and the right amount of energy to achieve one's goal (Tran, 2007) . As such, any emotion, be it pleasant or unpleasant, has implications on the self, others, and groups. These consequences are both negative and positive and thus can facilitate (or hamper) one's performance in a given situation. These competencies are essential in order to help prepare an efficient and strategic management of emotions to adapt to a situation. Strategic Management of Emotions (SME) SME refers to the ability to reflectively manage emotions, to influence emotions one experiences, to choose when to experience them and how to feel and express them, which is in line with Gross' (2013) model of Emotion Regulation. In QEPro's model, this ability is operationalized with two emotional competencies: the competence to define an emotional goal or target emotional state (Gross, 2013; Mikolajczak & Desseilles, 2012) and the competence to implement emotional regulation strategies in order to achieve the emotional goal (Gross, 2013) . (1) Selecting the Target Emotional State. This competence enables individuals to choose the target emotional state that best fits the situation in order to enhance performance and well-being for self and others. In order to select the right target emotional state, individuals have to consider three parameters (Gross & Thompson, 2007; Mikolajczak et al., 2014 ) (a) Duration of the emotion: they can choose to extend or shorten their current emotional state (or that of others), (b) level of intensity: they can increase or decrease the level of intensity of their current emotional state (and that of others), (c) nature of the emotion: they can choose to switch to a different emotional state that better fits the situation. In addition, our clinical experience led us to add a fourth parameter (d) complementary emotion(s). An individual can also choose to activate one or a combination of additional emotions, emotions that are complementary to the experienced emotional state. These emotions, so-named resource emotions, are selected based on their potential positive outcomes in a given situation. (2) Emotion Regulation. This competence enables to choose the best possible emotional regulation strategy to achieve a given target emotional state. Gross (1999) defines two types of regulation strategies: antecedentfocused strategies (selection or modification of the situation; attention deployment; cognitive reappraisal) and response-focused strategies (modulation of the emotional response). These emotion regulation strategies can be used in order to influence one's own emotion or that of others. SME involves the ability to combine those two competencies in order to adapt efficiently to the workplace and enhance well-being. Test development and validation is a continuous process of collecting evidence related to reliability and validity of test scores according to different criteria (e.g. convergent and divergent measures related to ability EI; Nunnally, 1994) and thereby improving quality of items (Downing, 2006) in order to reach sufficient standards for psychometric ability-based measurement. This main objective is further divided into sub-objectives related to different stages of test development and validation. First, we present a summary of the test development process. Then, we present the dimensions of the final version of QEPro. Furthermore, we describe the participants and procedure as well as the material and measures used in the validation study. The test was developed within a Multiple-Choice-Questions with Single correct Answer (MCQ-SA) framework in an online format. MCQ-SA format was selected due to its ease of administration and better measurement properties over other formats (Bible et al., 2008; Simkin et al., 2011) . For each of the items -in addition to the correct answer -4 to 5 distractors were retained in the final version as recommended by literature in order to maximize psychometric qualities of the questionnaire (Dickes et al., 1994; Nunnally, 1994) . For the first version of the QEPro Questionnaire, the authors generated 8 to 15 theory-based items per dimension (total of 70 items) as it is advised to develop an initial pool of twice the final number of items (Dickes et al., 1994) . Previous studies have demonstrated that systematic item review by experts has a positive impact on test validity (Downing & Haladyna, 1997) . Thus, the first version of QEPro was submitted to a group of experts in the fields of management and emotions. Based on guidelines established by previous studies on expertise, experts were selected on the following criteria (a) presence of initial training in their field (Chi, 2006) , (b) at least 10 years of experience in their field (Ericsson, 1999; Ericsson et al., 1993; Howe, 2001; Simon & Chase, 1973) , (c) pursuing continuous education and training in their field of expertise (Ericsson et al., 1993) , and (d) being recognized by peers and professional associations as an expert (Chi, 2006) . Fulfilling all of the above criteria, 25 experts were identified (9 professional coaches and 16 senior managers). Their qualitative feedback was highly encouraging as the experts were able to identify the correct answer for every item in the first three subscales. Two items were discarded from the Understanding Emotional Timelines subscale and four, from the Anticipating Emotional Outcomes subscale due to wording of the items. The last two subscales required more adjustments. For the subscale Selecting the Target Emotional State five items, for which the experts could not identify the correct answer, were discarded. Finally, for the subscale Emotion Regulation one situation was discarded. Overall, the experts judged QEPro useful and of interest for improving management outcomes. The group highlighted the potential benefits of the tool for recruitment and the development of managers' and executives' emotional skills to promote well-being and performance at the workplace. This qualitative feedback lead to select 58 items for the quantitative pre-test (Table 2 ). The quantitative pre-test aimed to assess the difficulty and the discriminant power of the 58 items. It was administered to a sample extracted from the target population, namely managers and executives. The sample was composed of 466 managers (284 men and 182 women) with an average age of 26.6 years (SD = 6.65). Most of the participants had significant managerial experience (25% and 67% of the participants had, respectively, between 3 to 10 years, and over 10 years of managerial experience). Statistical item difficulty as well as discriminant analysis were conducted. Only the items meeting the following criteria were kept for the final version of QEPro (a) the discrimination index of the item was superior to .20 and inferior to .80, (b) no single distractor was chosen more often than the right answer, and (c) each distractor was selected as the right answer by some participants. This analysis led us to discard 22 items. The final version was composed of 36 items organized in seven subscales (Table 2) . QEPro was administered online using Qualtrics platform without limited time to complete the test. The subscales, along with an example of items, are detailed below. Scanning Physiological Manifestations This subtest measures the test-taker's ability to accurately identify physiological changes associated with a specific emotional state. The subtest is composed of four items. The test taker has to associate a specific emotion with one of the three bodies presented in the item. Each of the bodies reflects a different emotional state as mapped by Nummenmaa et al. (2014) . E.g. "Among a set of three bodily maps of emotions each with distinct topographical bodily sensations, identify the bodily map corresponding to the emotion stated in the question." Interpreting Emotional Cues This subtest measures the test taker's competence to accurately identify an emotional state based on different emotional cues or manifestations as described by Scherer (1984) . The subtest is composed of five items. Each item describes an emotion based on three to four emotional cues. The test takers' task is to select which emotion (among the 6 options proposed) is described in the item. This subtest measures the test taker's ability to associate a trigger with the corresponding emotion. The subtest is composed of five items. Each item describes an emotional trigger, the test-taker's task is to select among the 6 answer options, the emotion corresponding to the described trigger. E.g. "A situation where your boundaries are offended can lead to:" (a) Disgust; (b) Fear; (c) Anger; (d) Guilt; (e) Envy; (f) Sadness. Understanding Emotional Timelines This subtest measures the test taker's ability to group emotion words per family and place them correctly on an emotional intensity continuum in line with Plutchik's Wheel of Emotions (1984) . This subtest is composed of six items. Each item presents an emotional intensity continuum graph (low intensity, medium intensity and high intensity). Two of the emotional words are missing and the test taker is asked to select the appropriate words to complete the graph from 6 answer options. E.g. "Among the list presented below identify the word best corresponding to X and the word best corresponding to Y. X (Low intensity) -Anger -Y (High Intensity)". Anticipating Emotional Outcomes This subtest measures the test taker's ability to identify possible positive and negative implications of emotions on self, others and the group. This subtest is composed of six items. Each item describes a situation with a positive or a negative outcome linked to a specific emotion. The test taker has to select among the 6 answer options the emotion which could lead to such outcomes. E.g. "I strengthen the sense of belonging to the group, increase self-confidence, enhance and drive task engagement." Selecting the Target Emotional State This subtest measures the test taker's ability to select the most suitable and efficient emotional state for a goal in a given situation. This subtest is composed of five vignettes. These vignettes describe managerial situations that are likely to occur in organizations. In order to achieve the situational goal mentioned in the vignette, the test-takers are required to select the most suitable emotion to experience among six answer options. The correct answer in the following example is based on emotional recall and mood congruent memory's studies (e.g. Gilligan & Bower, 1983 ). E.g. "You have misplaced an important document. You misplaced it the other day while coming out of a meeting which was especially annoying. To increase your chances of finding the file quickly, what emotion should you activate in yourself?" Pride; (f) Sadness Emotion Regulation This subtest measures the test taker's ability to select the most efficient emotion regulation strategy to achieve a specific emotional goal. This subtest is composed of five vignettes. Each vignette describes a managerial situation along with an emotional goal. The test takers are asked to select the regulation strategy they would most likely use in real life to achieve the emotional goal mentioned in the vignette among five answer options. Each of the answer options were generated within the emotion regulation framework defined by Gross (1999) and corresponds to one of the emotional regulation strategies he identified (e.g. situation selection; cognitive reappraisal; modulation of the emotional re-sponse…). The correct answer was defined as the strategy which allows to attain the emotional goal mentioned in the vignette, at the right intensity level. E.g. "Bob, one of your employees, who is rather shy and quiet, has exceeded his target despite a difficult context (lack of means and strong pressure). You want to motivate him further, and want to make him feel proud. Select the strategy which you would use in real life to make him feel proud?" (a) You remind him that the team has been of an invaluable help to him in carrying out this project. (b) You ask him to stand up and lift his chin up towards the sky. (c) You congratulate him during the weekly meeting, in front of the whole department and ask his colleagues to applaud. (d) You encourage him to list the targets he wants to achieve in the next semester. A total of 1035 managers and business executives (535 men, 500 women) with 1 to 25+ years of experience in management participated in the validation study. The average age was 43.9 years (SD = 8.25). Three levels of management were identified: front line management (N = 400), middle management (N = 347), and top management (N = 288). Most of the managers held a graduate degree (N = 684). The managers belonged to different divisions within their companies (Table 3) . The most represented divisions were general management (20.5% of the sample), sales (18.2%), and human resources (11.7%). All participants were recruited via email through a variety of sources (alumni directory of a top French business school, online professional networking sites...) and voluntarily participated in the study without financial incentives and being aware of the confidentiality of their answers. All participants completed QEPro (seven subscales) along with other tests and questionnaires online (via the Qualtrics software package). The constructs measured by all these tests can be categorized in three broad areas: Ability, Personality and Trait EI & Affective Measures (Table 4 ). The test administration extended over a period of six weeks in late 2016 and all participants took the tests in the same order. The Advanced Progressive Matrices -Short Form (APM-SF; Arthur Jr & Day, 1994) measures general cognitive ability. This test is composed of 20 items to be solved in a maximum of 20 min. Each item consists of a matrix of nine boxes (3 × 3) one of which is left blank. The participants are required to choose the correct response among the eight alternatives presented below the matrix. The internal consistency and the testretest reliability of the APM-SF are good (Cronbach's α = .72; test-retest reliability r = .75). The Big Five Inventory (BFI-FR; Plaisant et al., 2010 ) was used to measure personality. BFI-FR is a 45-item measure of personality. The five factors of personality measured are: extraversion (E = 8 items), agreeableness (A = 10 items), conscientiousness (C = 9 items), neuroticism (N = 8 items), and openness (O = 10 items). Each item was rated on a fivepoint Likert-type scale from "strongly disapprove" to "strongly approve." The reliability of the scale is good (Cronbach's α: E = .82; A = .75; C = .80; N = .82; and O = .74). The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue-SF; Mikolajczak et al., 2007a, b, c) . This self-report measure is composed of 30-items rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from "Completely Disagree" to "Completely Agree". The TEIQue-SF provides a general assessment of Trait EI. TEIQque-SF scales have good internal consistency (Cronbach's α between .71 and .91). The Emotional Intelligence Scale (EIS; Schutte et al., 1998) . This self-report measure is composed of 33 items (three of which are reversed) to be evaluated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from "Strongly Disagree" to "Strongly Agree". The EIS provides a general assessment of Trait Emotional Intelligence based on the earlier EI model of Salovey and Mayer (1990) . The EIS has a good reliability (Cronbach's α = .87; Stability at 2 weeks, r = .78). The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Loas et al., 1996) is a 20-item measure of Alexithymia. Alexithymia is a personality construct which reflects a significant deficit in experiencing, expressing and regulating emotions. The TAS-20 is composed of 20 items to be rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale. It consists of three factor scores measuring: difficulty in identifying one's feelings (7 items), difficulty in describing one's feelings (5 items), and externally-oriented thinking (8 items). The reliability of the scale is good (Cronbach's α = .81; Test re-test at 3 weeks, r = .77). It has demonstrated convergent and discriminant validity, and scores show high agreement with observer ratings of alexithymia (Parker et al., 1993) . The Maslach Burn-Out Inventory (MBI-GS; Dion & Tessier, 1994) evaluates burnout in general terms (not specific to any particular profession). MBI-GS assesses the psychological impact of the emotional and affective demands of intense involvement and investment in one's work. MBI-GS is composed of 16 items to be rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from "Never" to "Always". The MBI-GS provides three scales: Exhaustion (5 items), Cynicism (5 items), and Loss of Professional Efficacy (6 items). The tool's validity has been shown to be satisfactory (Aguayo et al., 2011; Langballe et al., 2006; Schutte et al., 1998) . The Basic Empathy Scale for Adults (BES-A; Carré et al., 2013) is composed of 20 items to be rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from "Strongly Disagree" to" Strongly Agree." The BES-A provides three scores: emotional contagion, cognitive empathy, and emotional disconnection. The factors are defined by Carré et al. (2013) as follows (a) Emotional Contagion refers to "a persons' ability to automatically replicate another person's emotion", (b) Cognitive Empathy corresponds to "a persons' ability to understand and to metalize another person's emotions", (c) Emotional Disconnection is defined as a "regulatory factor that involves self-protection against distress, pain, and extreme emotional impact" (Carré et al., 2013, p. 681). BES-A subscales have good reliability (Cronbach's α between .69 and .82). The Consideration of Future Consequences Scale -French version (CFC-14; Strathman et al., 1994 ) measures the consideration of future consequences. This dimension is conceived as a stable trait describing, at one end, individuals who prefer to rely on immediate consequences or the satisfaction of immediate goals and, at the other end, those who prefer to defer the satisfaction of their immediate needs to take care of their overall well-being. The CFC-14 is composed of 14 items to be rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from "Extremely Uncharacteristic" to "Extremely Characteristic." The reliability of the scale is good (Cronbach's α between .80 and .86 depending on the sample; Stability at two weeks r = .76 and Stability at five weeks r = .72). First, we examined the reliability of QEPro: (a) sensitivity and discriminating power of QEPro items, (b) structure of the questionnaire through confirmatory factor analysis at both the item level and the dimension level and finally, (c) stability of the questionnaire over time (test-retest reliability). Second, we assessed the criterion validity using a Multi Trait -Multi Method analysis with reference to four categories of measures (a) Demographic Data, (b) Ability Measure, (c) Personality and Trait EI, and (d) Affect Measures. The initial step of this analysis enabled us to select items with a satisfactory discriminant power to use in the final version of the questionnaire. We used three criteria to judge the quality each item: Difficulty Index, Discrimination Index and quality of distractors (Dickes et al., 1994) . The Difficulty Index is the ratio of respondents who correctly answered the item to the total number of people in the sample. The index ranges from 0 to 1 where 0 indicates that no one identified the correct answer and 1 indicates that everyone identified the correct answer. The lower the index score for the item, the more difficult the item. To select the most suitable items for the test, we followed Crocker and Algina's (1986) criteria. They suggested an acceptable range of .20 to .80 for a four-choice item if those intend to measure a range of ability levels. This analysis also helped us organize the items within each subscale: we placed the easiest items at the beginning of each subscale and the most difficult question at the end. The Discrimination Index assesses the discriminating power of an item, i.e. the ability to distinguish individuals belonging to the high-performers group from those belonging to the low-performers group as clearly and precisely as possible. This index is defined as the difference between the proportion of correct answers to an item among the highest scoring individuals, and the proportion of correct answers to the item among lowest scoring individuals (Bond & Fox 2001) . To calculate this index (D), we divided the sample into three groups based on their total score: the 27% highest scorers (high performers), the 27% lowest scorers (low performers) and the 46% with middle scores. Only the two extreme groups (high and low performers) were used to calculate the index. Within each group we calculated the proportion of success in answering the item correctly. This proportion will be referred to as RHigh and RLow for high-and low-performers groups respectively and represents the ratio between the number of individuals who successfully answered the items and the total number of individuals. A Discrimination Index (Eq. 1) was computed for all the items of the seven subscales. The minimum accepted level of item discrimination was set at .20 because it provides a satisfactory level of discrimination as suggested by Nunnally and Bernstein (1967) . Before computing the discrimination index for an item, the item's score was subtracted from the total score, thereby correcting for spuriousness. The analysis of the distractors allowed to identify distractors that were not performing correctly, either because they are infrequently chosen (which would indicate that the distractor is too unlikely), or because conversely, they are chosen too often (which would indicate that the distractor is far too close to the correct answer for it to be considered incorrect). Each item was expected to have a roughly equal distribution of responses across all the distractors. In order to represent the full spectrum of emotions experienced in the workplace we aimed to keep a balance between the number of items on pleasant and unpleasant emotions within each subscale. The 36 items of QEPro's final version presented satisfactory discriminant qualities (.20 < D < .80). The items were consistent and allowed for a precise positioning of individuals in different ability groups with distractors that were neither over-selected nor under-selected by respondents. After studying the discriminating power of the items, we moved on to examine the structure of QEPro by using Confirmatory Factor Analysis. We examined the structure of the questionnaire at both the item and the dimension levels. Item Level. In order to test that the seven-dimensional model can explain the relationship among the items, the structure of the questionnaire was studied using CFA. The items were postulated to load on one factor only with no cross-loadings, while the seven factors were postulated to be inter-correlated. Three covariances were added. Within the Scanning Physiological Manifestations dimension, one covariance was added between the only two items dealing with emotions that elicit high activation (as opposed to a high deactivation) in the body. Within the Understanding Emotional Timelines dimension, one covariance was added between the only two dimensions dealing with pleasant emotions. Within the Selecting the Target Emotional State dimension, one covariance was added between two items. Those two share the same strategic process (re-activating the same state in which an event occurred) as far as defining the target emotional state is concerned which differs from all the other items of the scale. The final model is presented in Fig. 1 . Recent studies and simulations (Glockner-Rist & Hoijtink, 2003; Savalei et al., 2015) have argued that using Robust Weighted Least Squares (WLSMV) as the extraction method (as opposed to Maximum Likelihood) is appropriate for dichotomous variables (L. K. Muthén & Muthén, 1998) . This will estimate the appropriate matrix for the factor extraction, based on tetrachoric correlations (Barendse et al., 2015; Flora & Curran, 2004) . MPLUS (Version 6.12. [Computer Software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén) was used to estimate the parameters of the model. Although the test of exact fit proved to be significant (χ 2 = 631.413, df = 536), the test of close fit (RMSEA = .013) is inferior to the minimum of .05 required to prove an acceptable adjustment (Browne & Cudeck, 1993) . This hypothesis can be accepted at a high probability threshold (p RMSEA <= .05 > .99). In his "Guidelines Concerning the Modelling of Traits and Abilities in Test Construction", Schweizer (2010) advises on the reporting of CFI (Comparative Fit Index; Bentler, 1990) and SRMR (Standardized Root Mean Square Residual) in addition to the χ 2 and the RMSEA. As our data are categorical, the WRMSR (Weighted Root Mean Square Residual) will be reported instead of the SRMR (DiStefano et al., 2018; Flora & Curran, 2004; B. Muthén, 1978) . The Comparative Fit Index is acceptable (CFI = 0.958) and is superior to the .95 limit (Hu & Bentler, 1999) . The WRMSR is acceptable as it is inferior to the cut-off score of 1 (WRMSR = .978; DiStefano et al., 2018) . Dimension Level A CFA conducted with MPLUS (Version 6.12 [Computer Software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén) confirmed that the correlated multi-dimensional structure of QEPro presents a better fit than a hierarchical structure with a general second order factor saturating the seven firstorder factors (one for each dimension of QEPro). Indeed, QEPro measurement model indicates a good fit of the data to the correlated multidimensional model (Barrett, 2007; Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2005) . For the dimension Identifying Emotions we observe that the sub-scale Interpreting Emotional Cues is loading less strongly, which can be explained by the inherent specificity of this competence. Indeed, compared to the two others sub-scales of this dimension, Interpreting Emotional Cues is more oriented towards the identification of emotions in others as opposed to the identification of emotions in oneself. The measurement model and corresponding estimates (standardized factor loading values and latent correlations) are presented in Fig. 2 . It is necessary that assessments remain stable over time (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1967) . However, international standards on test development recommend different levels depending on the type of measurement (Chan, 2014) . In our case, stability over time was analyzed on a subsample of managers (N = 108) who answered the questionnaire a second time after a 6-week interval. The analysis indicates satisfactory indices for the Global EI (GEI) score as well as for the meta-competencies. The test-retest correlation for GEI was of .73 (p < .01), a correlation of .67 (p < .01) was observed for IE, and .55 (p < .01) for UE and .60 (p < .01) for the SME .64 (p < .01). These coefficients are similar to those found in the literature for measures of ability EI, such as the MSCEIT (Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test) total score stability coefficient at 3-weeks is r = .86 (Brackett & Mayer, 2003) . We observe that women have significantly higher scores than men for GEI (t = −4089, df = 1033; p < .001), UE (t = −3278, df = 1033; p < .001) and SME (t = −4,9, df = 1033; p < .001). These findings are in line with the results reported in the literature showing that women have greater ability EI than men (Brackett & Mayer, 2003; Ciarrochi et al., 2000; Day & Carroll, 2004; Extremera et al., 2006; Farrelly & Austin, 2007; Goldenberg et al., 2006; John D. Mayer et al., 1999; McIntyre, 2010; Palmer et al., 2005) . In our sample, no significant gender differences have been found for IE (t = −0.234, df = 1033; NS) ( Table 5 ). This result is in line with some studies showing that there are no differences between male and female participants when recognizing facial highly expressive emotions (Fischer et al., 2018; Hoffmann et al., 2008) . Indeed, in QEPro the items measuring this meta-component use descriptions corresponding to a moderate/high intensity level of emotional experiences. More research is needed, on how male and female managers exactly differ in their identification of subtle emotion cues. We observe that age is fairly independent from GEI as well as the meta-competencies (Table 6) . No relation was found between age and IE (r = − .01) and a weak and negative relation (r = −.06 to r = −.08) was observed with the other abilities and GEI. These results are in line with, on the one hand, studies reporting low negative correlations (Cabello et al., 2014; Day & Carroll, 2004; Palmer et al., 2008) and, on the other hand, with those reporting no relationship between EI and age (Farrelly & Austin, 2007; Webb et al., 2014) . With regard to educational levels, we observe a moderate positive correlation between EI and educational levels (ranging from r = .09 for IE to r = .24 for UE and the GEI). It highlights that education and initial experience are more related than age to EI (GEI, IE, UE & SME) in our sample of adults. This supports the idea that EI develops with experience and education more than as the result of biological ageing alone. EI and the cognitive ability (APM-SF) present moderate positive correlations ranging from r = .14 for IE and SME to r = .28 for GEI (Table 6 ). This suggests some commonalities but mostly that the EI scales do measure a unique, different construct from general intelligence, as measured by the APM-SF. These results are in line with those observed for the MSCEIT (Fabio, 2015, p. 59 ; r = .19 with APM) but differ from those reported by Schlegel and Mortillaro (2019, p. 573) for GECo (r = .60 with the Cultural Fair Intelligence Test Scale 2). This questions the relationship between other ability based EI measures and the fluid component of intelligence (Côté, 2010, p. 129) . We observe weak-to-low correlations between ability EI measures (GEI, IE, UE & SME) and the EI trait measures (r = −.07 to r = .06). These results suggest that QEPro measures a different construct, or at least different aspects of the larger EI construct. This is in line with results reported by Mayer et al. (1999, b) and Brackett and Salovey (2004) concerning the MSCEIT. We observe low correlations between EI (GEI, IE, UE & SME) and Big Five Personality dimensions as measured by the BFI-FR (r = −.09 to r = .07), indicating global independence between ability EI and trait personality measures . In contrast, we observe moderateto-high correlations (r = −.56 to r = .37) between Trait EI measures (TEIQue & EIS) and the Big Five Personality dimensions (BFI-FR). This is in line with results reported by Mikolajczak et al. (2007a, b, c) for TEIQue and BFI. These results suggest that ability EI and trait EI correspond to fundamentally different constructs (Table 8) . We observe that GEI is negatively related to Alexithymia (correlation for subscales of TAS-20 range from r = −.06 to −.16). These results are similar to those of Palmer et al. (2008) . Among the subscales of TAS-20 we observe a negative relation between the Difficulty of Describing Feelings subscale and UE (r = −.09). This suggests that the ability to describe feelings is a central component of understanding emotions. Indeed, the ability to understand emotions partially depends on the ability of the individual to put emotions into words and to describe and understand them (Table 9) . We observe a moderate negative correlation between the subscale Externally-Oriented Thinking and UE (r = −.16). This indicates that the more externally oriented an individual's thoughts, the less he/she will be able to understand the emotions. This result suggests that the ability to understand emotions relies partially on the ability to orient one's thoughts. Similarly, we observe a negative correlation between this subscale and SME (r = −.13). This suggests that the more externally oriented an individual's thoughts, the less he/she will be able to strategically manage emotions. This aspect is of special interest for training and development of EI as it could be interesting to train people to internally orient their thinking in order to develop both abilities: understanding and managing emotions. Regarding burnout (MBI-GS), we observe a negative correlation between GEI and the Loss of Efficiency subscale of Burnout (r = −.12). Indeed, part of the burnout is associated with a lack of ability to recognize early signs and indicators of the premises of burnout (Mikolajczak et al., 2007a, b, c) . As such, if the identifying emotion step is not precisely performed, understanding and managing emotions cannot be engaged. We observe interesting patterns between QEPro and Empathy (BES-A). The ability to deeply connect on an emotional level with another person (Emotional Contagion subscale) is related to GEI (r = .18), IE (r = .14) and SME (r = .13). UE is less related to the Emotional Contagion subscale (r = .08). This indicates that both the ability to identify emotions in self and others as well as the ability to manage emotions prevent from being overpowered and "hijacked" by emotions of others, therefore allowing a deeper emotional connection. Indeed, empathy is defined as the ability to understand another person's views and his/her feelings (Rogers, 1951) . This definition highlights again the crucial role of IE and SME meta-competencies: to be fully empathic supposes to be able to maintain a certain distance and a distinction between "the self" and "the other," and not to engage in a complete process of identification (Carré et al., 2013) . For the Emotional Disconnection subscale (BES-A) we observe a low negative correlation with SME (r = −.18). As this subscale of Empathy is associated with self-protection against 'extreme unpleasant emotions' (Batson et al., 1987; Lamm et al., 2007) it is so forth negatively linked to SME which involves, among others, the ability to be receptive to emotional experiences, even unpleasant ones, in order to manage them properly. Furthermore, the Orientation Towards Future Consequences subscale of the CFC-14 was positively related to UE and SME (r = .07 and r = .10 respectively). This relationship is in line with the definition of these meta-competencies. Indeed, UE and SME help individuals to channel the emotions which arise, whenever one chooses to forego immediate gratification for greater long-term benefits. In this article we validated a new ability-based measure of EI dedicated to managers. In line with the EI model proposed by Schlegel and Mortillaro (2019) , QEPro model is composed of three branches excluding Mayer and Salovey's Facilitation of Thought branch which is problematic (MacCann et al., 2014; Roberts et al., 2008; Schlegel & Mortillaro, 2019) . Furthermore, QEPro model integrates fundamental characteristics of emotions and the research on emotional regulation. In line with Gross (2007) , we defined the ability to strategically manage emotions as composed of two competencies: the ability to identify and select the appropriate emotional state in a given situation (Selecting the Target Emotional State) and the ability to implement the accurate emotion regulation strategy (Emotion Regulation) to reach the target emotional state. In our approach, target emotional state is defined as the most efficient emotional state to reach a given operational goal (e.g. enhance group creativity, face to face negotiation, decision making process). In order to address critics related to EI ability measures, QEPro's items and their scoring method were developed based on theory. Additionally, to enhance ecological and face validity for the target population, the vignettes used to assess the Strategic Management of Emotions (SME) dimension were designed within the Situational Judgement Tests framework (SJT). Different studies were conducted to assess the psychometric qualities of QEPro. Results yielded preliminary support for the validity of QEPro in a sample of French managers and business executives. Results indicated a good fit both at the item and the factor levels. QEPro also showed good stability over time. To assess convergent and divergent validity, we explored the links between GEI, the three metacompetencies (IE, UE, SME) of QEPro and demographic characteristics and psychological measures. Regarding demographic data, age had a minimum effect in this sample of adults, but respondent's education level was found to influence QEPro results, suggesting that emotional competencies develop more through experience and education than through biological ageing in this sample of adults. Furthermore, QEPro results varied by gender, which suggests that separated norms for men and women would be useful. QEPro correlated in meaningful and theoretically congruent ways with general intelligence, Trait EI measures, the Big Five factors of personality, and the Affect measures used in this study. The validation process being a continuous process (DeVellis, 2011), we aim at gathering more evidence on the validity of QEPro. In this vein, our research agenda includes further validity investigations (a) exploring the predictive validity of QEPro on managerial outcomes, (b) adapting and validating the QEPro to other populations and cultures, (b) measuring the link between QEPro and the toxicity level of managers, (c) assessing the effect of developmental programs based on the QEPro model. We discuss each in turn below. Studies exploring the link between the seven competencies of QEPro and a range of specific managerial outcomes such as decision-making process, negotiation, team management, conflict management or crisis management should be conducted. To assess the impact of EI on managerial performance qualitative, quantitative, laboratory and biometric studies could be used. We aim to explore the predictive power of QEPro through field studies in partnership with French companies from different sectors. These companies are currently collecting key managerial outcomes indicators such as subordinate's satisfaction level, performance beyond expectations, leadership style, subordinate's attrition, financial performance, number of reported conflicts in the team. Getting access to such data will not only allow to demonstrate the relations between the QEPro subscales and those outcome variables, but also increase the ecological validity of our results. As a further step, we also aim to conduct laboratory studies exploring the link between the three meta-competences of QEPro and stress as it has been shown that EI has a moderating impact on cortisol response to stress (Mikolajczak et al., 2007a) and may even work as a "stress buffer" (Lea et al., 2019) . Such study would allow to explore the predictive validity of QEPro in relation to stress-induced outcomes (e.g. hormonal levels, skin conduction, heart rate)as stress management is a crucial competence for managers especially during these difficult times (Hagger et al., 2020; Knight, 2020; Serafini et al., 2020) . Even though QEPro was specifically designed for managers, it could be adapted to other populations working in emotionally demanding environments (e.g. sales, military, nursery; Hochschild, 2012) by recontextualizing the vignettes of the Strategic Management of Emotions dimension (Schmitt & Chan, 2006) . In order to capture other professional contexts and to facilitate the test takers ability to identify with the described situations, methods such as focus groups, identification of critical incidents (Flanagan, 1954) and expert survey should be used. This would allow to develop context-parallel versions that would be based on the same scoring method, the validation of those new versions would then follow guidelines used for cultural and linguistic test adaptations (Iliescu, 2017) . Future research on QEPro should investigate its cross-cultural validity on two levels. On the one hand, the robustness of the model can be explored by examining its factorial invariance across cultures, as it has been done for the MSCEIT (Karim & Weisz, 2010) . On the other hand, the universality of QEPro competencies should be examined (Hambleton & Kanjee, 1995) . Indeed, according to past cross-cultural studies on emotion recognition (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Jack et al., 2009 ) and on emotion regulation (Matsumoto et al., 2008) , we hypothesize that two of the meta-competencies of QEPro -Identifying Emotion (IE) and Strategic Management of Emotions (SME) -would be more dependent on cultural context. Understanding the intercultural functioning of these dimensions might benefit from a differential item functioning exploration (Borsa et al., 2012) . For the past decade, researchers have been actively studying abusive and toxic behaviors (e.g. aggressiveness, rudeness, manipulativeness) of individuals in the workplace and their effects on psychological health (e.g. depression) and on performance of employees (Forsyth et al., 2012; Krasikova et al., 2013; LeBreton et al., 2018; Spain et al., 2014; Tepper, 2000) . Most of these studies have focused on the so-called Dark Triad (DT; Adrian Furnham et al., 2013) , namely the three dark personality traits of Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and Narcissism. Studies showed that these three personality traits are aversive (toxic) and distinct, although they share a number of factors in common (Paulhus & Williams, 2002) . Some studies have focused on a particular type of company employee, namely the managers, and showed that possessing DT traits predicts destructive leadership (Forsyth et al., 2012; Krasikova et al., 2013; Spain et al., 2014) . In recent years, on the basis of an emerging academic literature, a debate has risen about the association between EI and the DT (Jauk et al., 2016) . Studies tend to show a negative association between EI and some DTs (Miao et al., 2019) . Other studies, which are rarer but have received considerable media coverage (Bariso, 2018; Cummins, 2014) , have revealed a positive link between EI and some DTs, often focusing on one or more subdimensions of EI, for instance the ability to regulate emotions (Côté et al., 2011; Davis & Nichols, 2016) . Such studies conclude by claiming that individuals who are able to regulate their emotions and those of others, will take advantage of this ability to manipulate the other preferring to serve their own interests in certain situations (Côté et al., 2011; Davis & Nichols, 2016) . The vast majority of studies focusing relationships between EI and the DT have focused on investigating the "trait" approach to emotional intelligence (see the literature review in Jauk et al., 2016 ; although see Côté et al., 2011 for a notable exception). Only a few studies about the DT have used ability EI tests (Jauk et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2015) , and none to our knowledge has done so on a population of real-world business managers. Thus, we propose to explore the relation between QEPro and Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and Narcissism, using a sample of real-world managers. Recent studies revealed the existence of emotional plasticity (Davidson et al., 2000; Kotsou et al., 2011; Lepousez et al., 2015) legitimating the development of training programs based on QEPro. To assess the effectiveness of QEPro based training programs we propose to follow Kirkpatrick and Kirkpatrick (2016) guidelines. The authors recommend a longitudinal design with a four-stage evaluation system: (a) Reaction assessment: assessment of the emotional reactions and judgments about the usefulness of the training, (b) Learning evaluation: assessment of the competence development at four different points in time (before the training, directly after the training, six months later, and one year later), (c) Behavior assessment: assessment of whether or not a transfer of skills occurred, and (d) Results: assessment of the impact of the training on organizational performance criteria. Such an assessment would allow to identify the conditions most conducive for the development of the seven emotional competencies measured by QEPro. To conclude, the present research offers a fine-grained approach to ability measurement of EI in the workplace. QEPro, which offers advantages over existing ability EI measures, may be especially useful in studies and practices aiming to link EI to management. We believe that the future of EI, both academic and practical, lies in the development of such approaches which tried to address common theoretical and psychometric criticisms of EI. Funding The tool (QE Pro) was developed with funding from Badenoch & Clark, a subsidiary of The Adecco Group, as part of an academic research project. Neither the test nor the data collected to calibrate the tool belong to the company that funded the research project. Data Availability The data that supports the findings of the study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Conflict of Interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest. The tool which was used to test the Ability EI model (QEPro) was developed with funding from Badenoch & Clark, a subsidiary of The Adecco Group, as part of an academic research project. Neither the test, nor the data used in this research to validate the tool, belong to the company that funded the research project. Ethical Approval Our study involved data from human participants. The Scientific Committee of emlyon business school decided in its meeting February 17th, 2019 to approve our study retrospectively. The Scientific Committee does not believe that the nature of the research design needs to go to a second stage and to be reviewed by a formal ethics committee. The Scientific Committee believes we have followed the standards for good and ethical research applied in our field. We, the authors, certify that the study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Potential Harm and Risks (Economic, Emotional, Legal, Physical, Political, Psychological, Social) to Participants We believe our study involves very minimal or no risk. Participants were informed, as an introduction to data collection, of the length of the questionnaires administered (1.30 h), the aim of the study (to validate a new EI tool dedicated to managers), that no EI scores will be provided once the questionnaires were completed (as it is the construction phase of our tool). One of the authors offered a webinar and a conference (at emlyon) to debrief some initial results of the study with participants who wanted to. Also, a few weeks after the questionnaire was completed, all participants got access to a free video providing some tricks to help them regulate their emotions in management situations. Issues with Privacy and Confidentiality Before filling the questionnaires, participants were informed that their data will be treated confidentially. Names, phone numbers, credit cards numbers, social security numbers were NOT asked. Gender, age, mother tongue, types of diploma, length of Professional experience, the size and activity sector of the company they work for, seniority in their company and seniority at their current position, the number of persons they supervise directly and indirectly, the type and length of skills trainings they followed, if they were/ are coached, were asked. The email address the participants provided was only used to send them the URL access to the tips and tricks videos. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. A metaanalytic reliability generalization study of the Maslach burnout inventory Psychological Testing Development of a short form for the raven advanced progressive matrices test Emotion in the workplace: The new challenge for managers Rumors of the death of emotional intelligence in organizational behavior are vastly exaggerated Prelims The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure Using exploratory factor analysis to determine the dimensionality of discrete responses There'sa dark side to emotional intelligence. Here's how to protect yourself BarOn emotional quotient inventory. Multi-health systems Structural equation modelling: Adjudging model fit Organizational behavior: The state of the science Affective events-emotions matrix: A classification of work events and associated emotions Adults' emotional reactions to the distress of others. Empathy and Its Development Comparative fit indexes in structural models Using multiplechoice tests to evaluate students' understanding of accounting. Accounting Education Applying the Rasch Model: Fundamental measurement in the human sciences Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of psychological instruments: Some considerations Convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence Measuring emotional intelligence with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet The emotions of managing: an introduction to the special issue Principles of educational and psychological testing Alternative ways of assessing model fit Differential effects of male and female managers' nonverbal emotional skills on employees' ratings Ability of university-level education to prevent age-related decline in emotional intelligence The basic empathy scale in adults (BES-A): Factor structure of a revised form The emotionally intelligent manager: How to develop and use the four key emotional skills of leadership Standards and guidelines for validation practices: Development and evaluation of measurement instruments Laboratory methods for assessing experts' and novices' knowledge A critical evaluation of the emotional intelligence construct Measuring emotional intelligence in adolescents A review and critique of emotional intelligence measures Taking the "intelligence" in emotional intelligence seriously Emotional intelligence in organizations Enhancing managerial effectiveness via four core facets of emotional intelligence The Jekyll and Hyde of emotional intelligence: Emotionregulation knowledge facilitates both prosocial and interpersonally deviant behavior Introduction to classical and modern test theory The dark side of emotional intelligence Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex The expression of emotions in animals and man Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation: Perspectives from affective neuroscience Emotional intelligence: In search of an elusive construct Ability versus trait emotional intelligence Does emotional intelligence have a "dark" side? A review of the literature Using an ability-based measure of emotional intelligence to predict individual performance, group performance, and group citizenship behaviours Scale development: Theory and applications La psychométrie: Théories et méthodes de la mesure en psychologie Validation de la traduction de l'Inventaire d'épuisement professionnel de Maslach et Jackson Examination of the weighted root mean square residual: Evidence for trustworthiness? Structural Equation Modeling: A Test item development: Validity evidence from quality assurance procedures Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell's mistaken critique Facial action coding systems Is there an in-group advantage in emotion recognition? Reading your counterpart: The benefit of emotion recognition accuracy for effectiveness in negotiation Creative expertise as superior reproducible performance: Innovative and flexible aspects of expert performance The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance Spanish version of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT). Version 2.0: Reliabilities, age and gender differences Beyond fluid intelligence and personality traits in social support: The role of ability based emotional intelligence Emotional intelligence, teamwork effectiveness, and job performance: The moderating role of job context Ability EI as an intelligence? Associations of the MSCEIT with performance on emotion processing and social tasks and with cognitive ability The ability model of emotional intelligence: Searching for valid measures Gender differences in emotion perception and self-reported emotional intelligence: A test of the emotion sensitivity hypothesis The critical incident technique An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data A meta-analysis of the dark triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective Conceptual foundations of ecological validity in neuropsychological assessment Positive emotions. Handbook of Positive Psychology Trait emotional intelligence and happiness The dark triad of personality: A 10 year review Reminding and mood-congruent memory The best of both worlds: Factor analysis of dichotomous data using item response theory and structural equation modeling The assessment of emotional intelligence: A comparison of performance-based and self-report methodologies Emotion regulation: Past, present, future Handbook of emotion regulation Handbook of emotion regulation Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations The InBoard emotional contagion model Managing stress during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic and beyond: Reappraisal and mindset approaches Increasing the validity of crosscultural assessments: Use of improved methods for test adaptations Emotional contagion The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling Sex differences with the recognition of emotion: The small difference-women recognise subtly expressed emotions better than men Face validity Genius explained Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives The benefits of merging leadership research and emotions research Adapting tests in linguistic and cultural situations Cultural confusions show that facial expressions are not universal The emotions The dark triad and trait versus ability emotional intelligence The big five personality inventory. Theoretical Perspectives Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale Cross-cultural research on the reliability and validity of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT). Cross-Cultural Research A meta-analytical multilevel reliability generalization of Situational Judgment Tests (SJTs) Kirkpatrick's four levels of training evaluation. Association for Talent Development Principles and practice of structural equation modeling How to handle the pressure of being a manager right now Emotional plasticity: Conditions and effects of improving emotional competence in adulthood Destructive leadership: A theoretical review, integration, and future research agenda Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal The factorial validity of the Maslach burnout inventory-general survey in representative samples of eight different occupational groups Does emotional intelligence buffer the effects of acute stress? A Systematic Review The dark triad and workplace behavior Adult neurogenesis and the future of the rejuvenating brain circuits Blood, sweat, and fears: The autonomic architecture of emotion Situational judgment tests: A review of recent research Factor analysis of the French version of the 20-ltem Toronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20) Emotional intelligence, personality, and the perceived quality of social relationships Emotional intelligence and social interaction Emotion regulation abilities and the quality of social interaction An ability model of emotional intelligence: Implications for assessment and training Psychologie des émotions: Confrontation et évitement New paradigms for assessing emotional intelligence: Theory and data Consensus scoring and empirical option weighting of performance-based emotional intelligence (EI) tests Emotional intelligence as assessed by situational judgment and emotion recognition tests: Building the nomological net Emotional intelligence is a second-stratum factor of intelligence: Evidence from hierarchical and bifactor models MBI: Maslach burnout inventory Mapping expressive differences around the world: The relationship between emotional display rules and individualism versus collectivism Traits, states and the trilogy of mind: An adaptive perspective on intellectual functioning Emotional intelligence: Science and myth The validity of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT) as a measure of emotional intelligence MSCEIT: Mayer-salovey-caruso emotional intelligence test. Multi-Health Systems What is emotional intelligence. Emotional development and emotional intelligence Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory, development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace Models of emotional intelligence Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications Gender differences in the nature and linkage of higher-order personality factors to trait and ability emotional intelligence leading-throughthe-crisis-or-managing-the-response?ab= hero-subleft-3 The relationship between emotional intelligence and the dark triad personality traits: A meta-analytic review Integrating ability and trait perspectives in the prediction and management of stress Traité de régulation des émotions Trait emotional intelligence and the cognitive appraisal of stressful events: An exploratory study Psychometric properties of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue; Petrides & Furnham, 2003): Factor structure, reliability, construct, and incremental validity in a French-speaking population Explaining the protective effect of trait emotional intelligence regarding occupational stress: Exploration of emotional labour processes The moderating impact of emotional intelligence on free cortisol responses to stress Association between frontal EEG asymmetries and emotional intelligence among adults Les compétences émotionnelles. Dunod Contributions to factor analysis of dichotomous variables Mplus user's guide sixth edition Face validity revisited Bodily maps of emotions Psychometric theory 3E. Tata McGraw-hill education Psychometric theory The Measurement of Emotional Intelligence: A Critical Review of the Literature and Recommendations for Researchers and Practitioners A psychometric evaluation of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test version 2.0. Intelligence A comprehensive framework for emotional intelligence Factorial validity of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy Psychometric properties of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue) On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioural validation in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction Trait emotional intelligence: Moving forward in the field of EI Validation par analyse factorielle du Big Five Inventory français (BFI-Fr). Analyse convergente avec le NEO-PI-R Emotions: A general psychoevolutionary theory. Approaches to Emotion Principles of adaptive management in complex safety-critical organizations A quantitative study of emotional intelligence and its impact on team performance Does emotional intelligence meet traditional standards for an intelligence? Some new data and conclusions The measurement of emotional intelligence: A decade of progress. The Sage Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment Emotional intelligence: Toward a consensus of models and measures Client-centered counseling Leading from within: The effects of emotion recognition and personality on transformational leadership behavior Emotional intelligence. Imagination CFA with binary variables in small samples: A comparison of two methods On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach Psychological models of emotion The Geneva emotional competence test (GECo): An ability measure of workplace emotional intelligence Situational judgment tests: Method or construct Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence Some guidelines concerning the modeling of traits and abilities in test construction The psychological impact of COVID-19 on the mental health in the general population Emotionally unskilled, unaware, and uninterested in learning more: Reactions to feedback about deficits in emotional intelligence Why use multiple-choice questions on accounting certification examinations? Skill in chess The dark side of personality at work Ecological validity in neuropsychological assessment: A case for greater consideration in research with neurologically intact populations The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior Health measurement scales: A practical guide to their development and use The contagious leader: Impact of the leader's mood on the mood of group members, group affective tone, and group processes Consequences of abusive supervision Chapter 2 the use, overuse, and misuse of affect, mood, and emotion in organizational research Affect and leader-member exchange in the new millennium: a state-of-art review and guiding framework Emotion recognition and emergent leadership: Unraveling mediating mechanisms and boundary conditions The role of cognitive versus emotional intelligence in Iowa gambling task performance: What's emotion got to do with it? Intelligence The emotionally intelligent decision maker: Emotion-understanding ability reduces the effect of incidental anxiety on risk taking Follow your gut? Emotional intelligence moderates the association between physiologically measured somatic markers and risk-taking Emotional intelligence in the workplace: A critical review The emotional intelligence, health, and well-being nexus: What have we learned and what have we missed? Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being What we know about emotional intelligence: How it affects learning, work, relationships, and our mental health The role of the dark triad traits and two constructs of emotional intelligence on loneliness in adolescents Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations Acknowledgements We thank emlyon business school and The Adecco Group for their invaluable help in data collection. We also thank Olivier Le Courtois for his relevant feedback on the draft of this paper and Kevin Charras and Nicolas de Zamaroczy for the copy-editing. Finally, we thank the editor and the reviewers for their suggestions that have been very helpful in improving the manuscript.