key: cord-0683798-276ywhy7 authors: Betsch, Cornelia; Korn, Lars; Burgard, Tanja; Gaissmaier, Wolfgang; Felgendreff, Lisa; Eitze, Sarah; Sprengholz, Philipp; Böhm, Robert; Stollorz, Volker; Ramharter, Michael; Promies, Nikolai; De Bock, Freia; Schmid, Philipp; Renner, Britta; Wieler, Lothar H; Bosnjak, Michael title: The four weeks before lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany: a weekly serial cross-sectional survey on risk perceptions, knowledge, public trust and behaviour, 3 to 25 March 2020 date: 2021-10-21 journal: Euro Surveill DOI: 10.2807/1560-7917.es.2021.26.42.2001900 sha: 0e8212243ac6bd26efe8048c98bf567822b7573b doc_id: 683798 cord_uid: 276ywhy7 BACKGROUND: During the COVID-19 pandemic, public perceptions and behaviours have had to adapt rapidly to new risk scenarios and radical behavioural restrictions. AIM: To identify major drivers of acceptance of protective behaviours during the 4-week transition from virtually no COVID-19 cases to the nationwide lockdown in Germany (3–25 March 2020). METHODS: A serial cross-sectional online survey was administered weekly to ca 1,000 unique individuals for four data collection rounds in March 2020 using non-probability quota samples, representative of the German adult population between 18 and 74 years in terms of age × sex and federal state (n = 3,910). Acceptance of restrictions was regressed on sociodemographic variables, time and psychological variables, e.g. trust, risk perceptions, self-efficacy. Extraction of homogenous clusters was based on knowledge and behaviour. RESULTS: Acceptance of restrictive policies increased with participants’ age and employment in the healthcare sector; cognitive and particularly affective risk perceptions were further significant predictors. Acceptance increased over time, as trust in institutions became more relevant and trust in media became less relevant. The cluster analysis further indicated that having a higher education increased the gap between knowledge and behaviour. Trust in institutions was related to conversion of knowledge into action. CONCLUSION: Identifying relevant principles that increase acceptance will remain crucial to the development of strategies that help adjust behaviour to control the pandemic, possibly for years to come. Based on our findings, we provide operational recommendations for health authorities regarding data collection, health communication and outreach. Even before the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak was officially declared a pandemic in March 2020, several countries had experienced high numbers of cases and deaths, necessitating the introduction of strict measures to prevent physical contact in an effort to slow transmission of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) [1, 2] . Nonpharmaceutical interventions were implemented in the absence of specific treatments or preventive vaccines. Responsible authorities had to enact measures and rely on citizens' trust and compliance, while only able to provide uncertain information, e.g. about the likelihood of contracting COVID-19 or its potential severity [3] . The public was forced to rapidly adapt opinions and perceptions to new risk scenarios and radical behavioural changes. Thus, this early COVID-19 outbreak period is of particular interest from a public health perspective, as it may serve as an exemplary case for the ability of the public to adapt to an emerging infectious disease that is rapidly spreading worldwide. Many studies were conducted during that time to assess individuals' risk perceptions, behaviours and trust in information sources, e.g. in the United States [3] [4] [5] [6] [7] [8] , China [9] [10] [11] , Vietnam [12] , Bangladesh [13] , Indonesia [14] , South Africa [15] , Nigeria [16] , Sierra Leone [17] , Australia [18] , Italy [19, 20] , Israel [21] , Spain [22] , Egypt [23] and Japan [24] , as well as cross-national comparisons [25] [26] [27] . Most analyses relied on crosssectional, single data collections that were not always representative of the respective country. The results were used to support crisis communication, prepare key messages [28] and facilitate implementation of rational, adaptive and protective behaviour [29] . A few studies used a longitudinal design with two data collections [30] [31] [32] ; however, they used convenience and snowball sampling, which can increase self-selection bias and does not allow extrapolation to populations. From these studies, we can conclude that risk perceptions changed considerably over time. Thus, building pandemic response strategies based on a single time period may miss prominent changes and trajectories. However, knowledge remains limited about how risk perceptions and acceptance of measures developed from the outset of the pandemic, given the complex and dynamic interplay among changing epidemiology, news media attention and pandemic control measures [33] . Through the COVID-19 Snapshot Monitoring (COSMO) [34, 35] , weekly data on public opinion and perceptions have been collected since 3 March 2020, and have provided continuous evidence to Germany's government and crisis managers, as well as health communicators and news media. The distributions of age, sex and federal state in each sample of this serial crosssectional survey matched the German population for each respective variable. The protocol attracted substantial interest as identified by download statistics at psycharchives.org and built the basis of a study protocol suggested by the World Health Organization (WHO) in July 2020 [36, 37] . However, to our knowledge, this study is the only one of its kind that has assessed data from a country that was relatively unaffected at the outset of the study period until the lockdown was imposed, affecting large parts of society and economy. Here, we present data from Germany during this crucial period March 2020), which investigates the interplay between disease dynamics, political measures, news media, resulting knowledge and public opinion. These analyses can serve as a blueprint for other studies using a similar protocol and provide recommendations on implementing such survey systems and on how to learn from the data. Extant research on public health and social measures indicates that topic knowledge, trust in the government and risk perceptions are relevant to explain the acceptance and uptake of such measures [38] [39] [40] . This is supported by ample theoretical approaches to understanding preventive behaviour that point in similar directions [41, 42] , e.g. the Confidence and Cooperation model [43] , the protection motivation theory [44, 45] , the Health Belief Model [46] and the health action process approach [44] . Therefore, our main objective was to identify changes in risk perceptions and behaviours over time and answer the following research questions: (i) How did people perceive the disease and the pandemic situation from the outset of the pandemic? (ii) What did people know about COVID-19 and how quickly were they acquiring knowledge? (iii) How much did they trust institutions and media? (iv) How well did they accept imposed safety measures, and which factors affected acceptance? Since translating knowledge into preventive health behaviour is a major challenge in health promotion [47] , we also assessed which groups were compliant and which had difficulties following safety measures. Such knowledge from this early stage of the pandemic can help steer policies, public health measures and communication strategies in future waves or local outbreaks by identifying relevant principles that should be considered. With such a dynamic situation, fast and effective action may prevent larger outbreaks, whereas ineffective strategies may cause irreversible damage, e.g. avoidable deaths, misinformation or complacency. Thus, identifying factors that facilitate acceptance and compliance early on is crucial [48] . During the study period March 2020), confirmed COVID-19 cases in Germany rose from 145 to 39,441 [49] , and the situation transitioned from an issue threatening only other countries, such as China or Italy, into a severe and real threat in Germany. Pandemic control measures shifted drastically from none at all to a strict contact ban, wherein citizens were permitted to meet only one other person, at a distance of at least 1.5 m. The first COVID-19-associated deaths occurred in Germany on 8 March, just before the second week of data collection. The government reacted by closing non-essential shops and schools on 13 March, the day before the third round of data collection. A strict contact ban was enacted on 22 March, 2 days before the fourth round of data collection (Supplementary Figure S1 : Timeline of relevant events). This study was designed as a serial, cross-sectional online survey with ca 1,000 unique individuals participating in each of the four rounds of data collection. Care was taken that participants did not participate more than once. Data were collected on 3-4 (round 1), 10-11 (round 2), 17-18 (round 3) and 24-25 March 2020 (round 4). Non-probability quota samples were used, matching the German adult population in terms of age × sex and federal state, according to the German census. Study participants were members of an ISO 26362:2009-compliant online panel [50] . The externally contracted company responsible for data collection financially compensated participants. The questionnaire was programmed by the COSMO research group. All individuals between the ages of 18 and 74 years who completed the survey were eligible for inclusion in the analyses. Participants were admitted to the survey or excluded based on quotas. Response rates, defined as the number of people who participated in relation to the number of people invited, ranged between 15.1% (third data collection round) and 20.2% (second data collection round). We included a total of 3,910 individuals, with n round 1 = 973, n round 2 = 966, n round 3 = 1,015 and n round 4 = 956 for each of the four collection rounds. During the third and fourth data collection rounds, one participant was excluded because of previous participation. At the outset of each of the four data collection rounds, participants received a link to the online questionnaire, which took between 15 and 25 min to complete. During each data collection round, the same core questions assessing demographics and psychological variables were included in the survey. Each questionnaire contained unique questions not reported here. The original questionnaires are accessible online [35] . In the survey, participants provided sociodemographic information on their age, sex, education (low: up to 9 years of schooling; medium: at least 10 years of schooling without university entrance qualification; high: at least 10 years of schooling with university entrance qualification), whether or not they worked in the healthcare sector and the size of their community (small: < 20,000 inhabitants; medium: 20,000-100,000 inhabitants; large: > 100,000 inhabitants). All variables are described in detail in the Supplementary Methods [51] . The questionnaire comprised relevant theoretical concepts, as detailed above, to (i) assess trust in governmental and health institutions and media (one item each, using ratings on Likert-type scales ranging from 1 to 7); (ii) knowledge about SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19 (three right/ wrong items); (iii) knowledge about several protective behaviours (summary score); (iv) cognitive risk perception, e.g. susceptibility to SARS-CoV-2 infection (ratings on Likert-type scales ranging from 1 to 7 [52] ); (v) affective risk perception, e.g. feeling fear, worry or thinking about the coronavirus all the time [53] (mean score across three Likert-type scales ranging from 1 to 7); (vi) self-efficacy in protecting oneself against the disease (one item, Likert-type scale ranging from 1 to 7); (vii) perceiving the outbreak as media hype (one item; Likert-type scale ranging from 1 to 7); (viii) protective behaviours practised (yes/no) and (ix) acceptance of restrictions of freedom, as a proxy for acceptance of safety measures (one Likert-type scale ranging from 1 to 7). We included time as a between-subjects variable to assess whether the dependent variables changed over time. Google Trends data indicate how often certain keywords are searched on the internet and serve as a proxy for information demand and attention paid to a topic. Data on the Google search volume were downloaded from Google Trends [54] . In contrast to common words used in the scientific literature, news outlets and laypeople in Germany used the term 'corona' much more frequently than COVID-19 or SARS-CoV-2; therefore, we focussed on the search term 'corona' only. Thus, the data represent only the search interest regarding 'corona', relative to the maximum interest (number of searches) for Germany between 25 February and 25 March 2020. As provided by Google Trends, a value of 100 indicated peak popularity for the term, 50 indicated moderate popularity and zero indicated that there was not enough data for this term. WHO noted that, along with the COVID-19 pandemic, there has also been an 'infodemic', i.e. an abundance of information that includes misinformation [55] . Considering that news media are an important source of health information [56] , we used the frequency with which news outlets reported about the COVID-19 pandemic as an indicator of information density and availability. To study the amount of newspaper reporting in Germany during the study period and the week prior (25 February-25 March 2020), we collected data from the Wiso press database (https://www.wiso-net.de) using the filter 'Presse Deutschland' to exclude articles from Switzerland and Austria. Altogether, 158 newspapers, magazines and online news portals generated hits within the search strings for all texts containing the keywords 'corona' or 'covid'. We chose a large sample size to detect small effects and to increase the probability of congruence between the distribution of the demographics in the sample and the German population (regarding age × sex and federal state), comprising approximately 1,000 individuals per data collection round [57] . Deviations occurred because of decreased or increased invitations to participants in the face of open quotas. Given a sensitivity power analysis for zero-order correlations (p = 0.05), a sample size of 1,000 is sufficient to detect correlation coefficients of (at least) r = 0.08 with sufficient power of 0.8 in each survey. We used unweighted [58] , stepwise linear regression analyses with backward elimination as follows: (i) first step: demographic variables and time, (ii) second step: psychological variables and (iii) third step: interactions between psychological variables and time. Statistical requirements were tested and fulfilled (see the Supplement). To identify which variables affected acceptance of restrictive measures, we used ordinary least squares regressions (packages lm, glm) in the statistical software R (R Foundation, Vienna, Austria) plus the modern applied statistics with S (MASS) package [59] . We further examined acceptance of new behavioural rules over time and factors influencing this acceptance by using a classification-type approach to extract profiles from homogenous groups of those who complied or did not comply with the requested behaviour. For extraction of homogenous groups, we used a k-means cluster analysis and multinomial logistic regression to examine group members' characteristics. Cases with missing data for any of the predictors were excluded from the analyses, which were all repeated using multiple imputation, eliciting no substantial differences in results compared with listwise deletion, as documented in the Supplement (Sensitivity of linear regression results). Data and analysis codes are available online [51] . The University of Erfurt's institutional review board approved this study (Number 20200501), which is in line with the 2013 Declaration of Helsinki. Participants provided informed consent before participating in the study. An overview of the study participants across the four rounds of data collection compared to respective distributions of demographic variables in the German population is presented (Table 1 ). We found an increasingly active search for information based on Google Trends data and an increasing availability of information in the news media ( Figure 1A) . Despite the steep increase in news media reporting, the situation was viewed as less of a news media-hyped story as time passed, and trust in media increased correspondingly. SARS-CoV-2and COVID-19-related knowledge was high during these 4 weeks, and increased between the first and last round of our survey [60] . Knowledge about the lack of medical treatments and vaccines, in particular, increased between the second and third round (Supplementary Table S1 : Participants' characteristics across the time points), the period during which the largest increase in information searches and availability occurred. Interestingly, Google searches peaked relatively soon after the first COVID-19-associated death, reaching further local maxima when new regulations took effect. The cognitive risk perceptions, e.g. regarding susceptibility, increased very slowly ( Figure 1B ), but affective risk perceptions increased sharply between the first COVID-19-associated death and the closing of non-essential shops and schools. As affective risk perceptions increased, acceptance of restrictive measures increased to a similar extent. Trust in institutions remained high, even after strict measures were implemented, potentially because of the high acceptance of strict measures at that time. Suggested protective behaviours increased over time ( Figure 1C ). When predicting the acceptance of restrictive policies, the addition of psychological variables to the demographics increased the amount of explained variance from 9% to 19% ( Table 2) . Acceptance of restrictive policies increased with age and employment in the healthcare sector. Both age and employment in the health sector are risk factors, as age increases the likelihood for severe disease and being employed in the health sector increases the risk of being exposed to the virus. Regarding psychological dimensions, both cognitive and affective risk perceptions were significant predictors of acceptance. Generally, acceptance of restrictive measures increased over time and was higher among those with greater trust in institutions and media. However, these effects changed over time, as indicated by several interaction effects. Trust in institutions became more relevant over time (Figure 2A ), whereas trust in media became somewhat less relevant over time ( Figure 2B ). Greater knowledge of COVID-19 was associated with lower acceptance of restrictions early in the outbreak, but this association diminished over time ( Figure 2C ). Knowledge about effective preventive behaviour is not a guarantee of compliance [47] ; therefore, we extracted profiles of homogenous groups of people to examine knowledge and compliance with requested behaviour. On the basis of eight items on pandemic-related knowledge, e.g. knowing that handwashing is an effective preventive measure, and eight items on various protective and crisis-related behaviours, e.g. cancelling planned travels (see the Supplement), four distinct clusters were identified based on results from all four data collection rounds (see Figure 3 ). Participants in Cluster 1 are considered 'informed compliant' because they score high on both knowledge and behaviour ( Figure 3A ). Those grouped in Cluster 2, defined as 'complacent', can be characterised as wellinformed, but falling short in acting on their knowledge about COVID-19. Participants in Cluster 3 are regarded as 'non-compliant', which features those practising little or no protective behaviours. This cluster also contains the largest share of participants with very low knowledge levels. Participants in Cluster 4, classed as 'ignorant but compliant', scored low in knowledge, but nevertheless took appropriate actions to prevent the spread of the virus. At the beginning of the pandemic, more than half the participants belonged to the two clusters scoring low in knowledge (Clusters 3: noncompliant and 4: ignorant but compliant) ( Figure 3B ). The share of people in these two clusters decreased over time. The share of the 'informed compliant' cluster increased significantly over time, while the share of the 'non-compliant' cluster decreased sharply. At the end of data collection, around 60% of the participants were in the two clusters scoring high on knowledge. Even so, the clusters that were not fully compliant with appropriate measures still comprised around half of all participants. It is noteworthy that among those who did not comply, the share of those who also lacked knowledge was relatively small. In the next step, we examined which variables explained cluster membership using a multinomial logistic regression approach. We used time (round), sociodemographic and psychological factors as sets of predictors in a full model (entering all sets simultaneously) and in single models (entering only one set) ( Table 3 ). The analysis revealed that being older increased preventive behaviours and high knowledge levels, i.e. the likelihood of being in the 'informed compliant' cluster. Notably, having a higher education level increased the gap between knowledge and behaviour, in that higher education was related positively to being in the 'non-compliant' or 'ignorant but compliant' cluster. Cognitive and affective risk perceptions were related to showing more protective behaviour, which is indicated by the negative associations with belonging to the 'non-compliant' cluster or the 'complacent' cluster. Trust in institutions was related to turning knowledge into action, as trust was related negatively with belonging to the 'non-compliant' cluster. The results indicate that trust in institutions (government, health authorities) became more important over time for the acceptance of restrictive measures (panel A), while the importance of trust in media declined over time (panel B). Though greater knowledge decreased compliance at the beginning of the German epidemic, knowledge was rather unrelated with compliance a month later (panel C). Round denotes the round of data collection; ratings were collected on Likert-type scales ranging from 1 to 7. Published studies on protective behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic have demonstrated that motivation and knowledge are relevant drivers [61] . However, most of these studies took place at a later stage in the pandemic and collected data only once, sometimes twice, which does not allow understanding of change over time . As early action is critical, given uncertain and potentially drastic consequences [48] , it is of utmost importance to learn how people adapt to such a situation so that policymakers are more informed regarding how to guide people's behaviour effectively. The data reported here are unique, as they were collected weekly during the 4-week period from the report of the first COVID-19 cases in Germany to the first national lockdown. Our data indicated that public perceptions and opinions adapted rapidly to the new threat and to the need for radical behavioural changes, which were partially regulated by strict policies mandated by the German government. The dynamic spread of COVID-19 was mirrored by affective risk data; feelings of fear and being at risk increased sharply at the outset and were more closely related to acceptance of strict measures rather than perceived susceptibility to contracting the disease. Other published studies from the same time period also indicate that perceiving governmental actions as non-efficacious was related to increased worry and fear [26] . Thus, while facing drastic measures may be a cue that elicits fear, the lockdown also may have led to a containment of fear [31] and an increase in trust. As the pandemic progresses further, it will be relevant to assess how adaptation to the threat and decreasing risk perceptions affect the acceptance of strict measures and trust in government authorities. Our data reveal that trust in institutions played an increasing role in the acceptance of measures as cases increased and more restrictive measures were implemented. Thus, at these critical points in time, German health authorities and the government seem to have acted in ways that strengthened trust. Not only did the relationship between trust and acceptance of measures increase, but so did average trust itself. This suggests that trust will remain a crucial factor as the pandemic continues. Trust can be achieved through transparency and the use of available evidence [62] . However, evidence changes quickly during such a dynamic situation [63] . Meta-communication about how knowledge changes and how this knowledge is still used to update policies may increase trust in acting authorities. News media have been an important source of information, especially at the beginning of the pandemic and when new restrictions first came into effect. As data from Italy indicate, consumption of more media reports was also related to higher risk perceptions [30] . In addition, although news media are an important knowledge source, nearly half the study's participants were not fully compliant when the lockdown was implemented, despite high knowledge levels. Interestingly, participants with higher education levels fell into the two clusters in which knowledge and behaviour were not related, e.g. 'non-compliant' and 'ignorant but compliant'. More highly educated individuals may have more freedom to choose whether they comply with rules, possibly because they were less likely to work in frontline jobs involving situations with increased risk of SARS-CoV-2 infection [64, 65] . Furthermore, we found that women's display of protective behaviours was unrelated to knowledge, and those who felt they were at lower risk practised less protective behaviours, regardless of knowledge. Men tended to be non-compliant, regardless of knowledge. Findings from other countries also suggest that men were a main target group for interventions as they showed less preventive behaviours [31, 66] . Younger people were also an important group to assess because they were less likely to be in the 'informed compliant' Cluster 1, possibly because of high knowledge but lower risk perceptions and thus lower motivation to protect themselves. The four clusters described can be used to identify target groups for further interventions. Inspecting potential levers suggests that strategies addressing affect may be useful because feeling at risk was a relevant difference between those categorised as 'informed compliant' and the other clusters. However, it is important to note that these findings do not mean that appeals to fear are the solution to promoting compliance [67] . Eliciting strong fear may erode trust and create reactance, i.e. a state of anger and counter-reaction [68] . Strategies that increase self-efficacy in complacent people could increase protective behaviour and help turn knowledge into action. Recommendations from the analyses are summarised in the Box. This study has some limitations. Firstly, our estimates of article output containing 'corona' did not exclude double mentions, which may be due to different editions of local newspapers. However, the data provide an estimate of the topic's presence in news media. Secondly, the survey data were collected online. We refrained from including people over 75 years of age because coverage bias for online surveys in this age group may be particularly prominent [69] , and confounders that might affect health could come into play, e.g. more active lifestyles in older adults who use the internet vs non-users [70] . We also refrained from including people under 18 years of age, as parental consent would have been necessary. Thirdly, other work has documented psychological strain and the consequences of the pandemic for children and adolescents [71, 72] , but this was beyond the scope of this study. Fourthly, our study also relied on self-reporting, which may be subject to social desirability bias [73] , considering that individuals with high social desirability may not only report more compliant behaviour, but may also Data collection • Install a representative population panel that enables rapid surveillance of psychological factors that can affect acceptance of measures at the early stages of a pandemic (or other major health crisis) to identify target groups for health communication and other interventions. Health communication • Install a mechanism through which to communicate results to different audiences in a timely and understandable fashion, e.g. to national and local crisis teams, politicians, news media and the general public. • Provide information, e.g. about how a pathogen is transmitted, to allow for informed compliance, e.g. protective behaviour. Delivering relevant knowledge is necessary, but insufficient to elicit desired behaviour. • Increase awareness of the social impact of non-compliance in order to increase the motivation of well-informed but noncompliant individuals. Although feeling at least somewhat at risk is necessary, communicating daily case numbers may not be sufficient, as perceived susceptibility was less important. Be wary of using strong fear appeals, as they can backfire and increase opposition to measures. If risks are pointed out, provide information to increase self-efficacy, i.e. empower people to protect themselves and others. • Knowledge-behaviour gaps may be particularly high among the well-educated. Identify target groups and whether these groups lack knowledge or motivation to comply. • High trust can turn knowledge into action, but trust may change over time, particularly during crises with long durations. Thus, maintaining trust is vital. Changing policies can increase trust if risk perceptions are high, and the measures are viewed as appropriate. However, changing policies can also create challenges in preserving trust. Highlighting that decisions are evidence-based, and that evidence changes often, can be helpful. • Find ways to boost compliance. For example, if women comply regardless of knowledge, encourage women in campaigns to help their family and friends. If elderly people comply based on high knowledge, motivate them to explain their reasons for compliance to their families. • Cluster analyses can be used to identify important sources of information to ensure optimal outreach from communication campaigns. Recommendations for healthcare authorities actually be more compliant when it is currently socially desirable to do so. Finally, the statistical analyses are unweighted [58] . As the sample's characteristics deviated from the general population in terms of education and age, this potentially inflates the sizes of the 'noncompliant' and 'ignorant but compliant' clusters. In general, the repeated cross-sectional data do not permit causal inferences, but they still permit important insights into changes in average perceptions and behaviour over time, given that participants in each group were sampled randomly from the same population. Moreover, non-probability online surveys were the only viable method available during the lockdown period for gathering data in a timely manner; personal interviews were precluded by the risk of transmission, phone studios for computer-assisted telephone interviewing surveys were closed and available probabilitybased survey infrastructures either were restricted to certain research groups or did not provide sufficient capacities to accommodate this study. Multiple reasons may explain the response rates of 15-20% for contacted individuals. A large number of participants matching the quotas had to be recruited during a very short period of time. This required inviting larger groups, as people may not have seen the invitation in their email inbox or could not find time to answer within the very short time frame. Moreover, response rates decreased with age and were lower in small federal states [74], which may have lowered the overall response rate. It remains possible that individuals with particular interest in the topic are overrepresented, but this limitation pertains to all survey methods. As the COVID-19 pandemic continues, knowledge about acceptance factors that drive individual protective behaviour, such as physical distancing, is crucial to further support strategies that help adjust behaviour to control the pandemic. It is important to note that such data always will be a snapshot with many confounding factors, such as news media reports, political discussions and decisions, case numbers and deaths. This naturally impedes causal interpretations of changes over time. If resources allow, a complementary longitudinal study design could add important insights. Nevertheless, the current study provides colleagues in other countries with a blueprint for analysing, interpreting and using the data collected in other countries (Box). This may help identify cross-national commonalities and differences relevant to public health measures aimed at controlling this pandemic, as well as future outbreaks of new pathogens. Ranking the effectiveness of worldwide COVID-19 government interventions An investigation of transmission control measures during the first 50 days of the COVID-19 epidemic in China Age differences in COVID-19 risk perceptions and mental health: evidence from a national U.S. survey conducted in Relationships between initial Covid-19 risk perceptions and protective health behaviors: a national survey Social distancing in response to the novel coronavirus (COVID-19) in the United States Perceptions of the adult US population regarding the novel coronavirus outbreak Public intent to comply with covid-19 public health recommendations Know your epidemic, know your response: Early perceptions of COVID-19 and self-reported social distancing in the United States Government intervention, risk perception, and the adoption of protective action recommendations: evidence from the covid-19 prevention and control experience of China Risk perception and depression in public health crises: evidence from the covid-19 crisis in China Analysis of risk perceptions and related factors concerning covid-19 epidemic in Chongqing Data for understanding the risk perception of COVID-19 from Vietnamese sample Psychosocial and socio-economic crisis in Bangladesh due to covid-19 pandemic: a perception-based assessment. Front Public Health Perceived risk of being infected with SARS-CoV-2: a perspective from Indonesia South Africans' understanding of and response to the COVID-19 outbreak: an online survey Survey data of COVID-19-related knowledge, risk perceptions and precautionary behavior among Nigerians. Data Brief Community knowledge, perceptions and practices around COVID-19 in Sierra Leone: a nationwide, cross-sectional survey COVID-19 is rapidly changing: Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to this evolving pandemic Emerging adults and covid-19: the role of individualism-collectivism on perceived risks and psychological maladjustment A preliminary evaluation of the public risk perception related to the covid-19 health emergency in Italy Analysis of public perception of the Israeli government's early emergency instructions regarding covid-19: online survey study We are staying at home: association of self-perceptions of aging, personal and family resources Relationship between perception and anxiety about COVID-19 infection and risk behaviors for spreading infection: A national survey in Japan Use of rapid online surveys to assess people's perceptions during infectious disease outbreaks: a cross-sectional survey on COVID-19 Perceived efficacy of COVID-19 restrictions, reactions and their impact on mental health during the early phase of the outbreak in six countries Public perceptions, anxiety and the perceived efficacy of health-protective behaviours to mitigate the spread of the SARS-Cov-2/ COVID-19 pandemic Crisis and emergency risk communication as an integrative model The CDC field epidemiology manual Environmental and psychological variables influencing reactions to the COVID-19 outbreak Factors associated with the perception of risk and knowledge of contracting the SARS-Cov-2 among adults in Bangladesh: analysis of online surveys Looking out for myself: Exploring the relationship between conspiracy mentality, perceived personal risk, and COVID-19 prevention measures Pandemic public health paradox": time series analysis of the 2009/10 influenza A/H1N1 epidemiology, media attention, risk perception and public reactions in 5 European countries Monitoring behavioural insights related to COVID-19 Covid-19 snapshot monitoring. Trier: PsychArchives COVID-19 Snapshot Monitoring (COSMO Standard): Monitoring knowledge, risk perceptions, preventive behaviours, and public trust in the current coronavirus outbreak -WHO standard protocol. Copenhagen: WHO/ Euro Survey tool and guidance: rapid, simple, flexible behavioural insights on COVID-19: 29 Socio-psychological determinants of public acceptance of technologies: A review Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey Regulation and compliance motivations: examining different approaches Vom sicheren Umgang mit Unsicherheit: Was wir von der pandemischen Influenza (H1N1) 2009 lernen können Risk perception and self-protective behavior Risk Perception and the TGC Model of Cooperation Framework for a collaborative process to increase preparation for drought on U.S. public rangelands A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change1 The health belief model and sick role behavior Closing the knowledge-behavior gap in health promotion: the mediating role of self-efficacy Immediate action is the best strategy when facing uncertain climate change Access panels in market, opinion and social research -vocabulary and service requirements Supplementary materials to: The four weeks before lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential Mountain View: Google Leveraging media and health communication strategies to overcome the COVID-19 Determining sample size: how to calculate survey sample size Struggles with survey weighting and regression modeling R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Core Team Inference by eye: reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals Capability, opportunity, and motivation to enact hygienic practices in the early stages of the COVID-19 outbreak in the United Kingdom Improving government policy on risk: eight key principles The ethics of uncertainty. In the light of possible dangers, research becomes a moral duty Non-compliance with COVID-19-related public health measures among young adults in Switzerland: Insights from a longitudinal cohort study How to improve adherence with quarantine: rapid review of the evidence Sociodemographic and psychological correlates of compliance with the covid-19 public health measures in France Sixty years of fear appeal research: current state of the evidence Control, its loss, and psychological reactance Establishing an open probability-based mixedmode panel of the general population in Germany: the GESIS panel Lifestyle segmentation to explain the online health information-seeking behavior of older adults: representative telephone survey Mental health and clinical psychological science in the time of COVID-19: challenges, opportunities, and a call to action Psychological Symptoms and Behavioral Changes in Children and Adolescents During the Early Phase of COVID-19 Quarantine in Three European Countries. Front Psychiatry The role of constructs in psychological and education measurement Germany's COVID- 19 None declared. CB: study design, data collection, verified the underlying data, data analysis, data interpretation, writing of first draft. LK: study design, data collection, verified the underlying data, data analysis, data interpretation, writing, review of final version of manuscript. LF: data collection, data analysis, verified the underlying data, data interpretation, writing, review of final version of manuscript. SE: data collection, data analysis, verified the underlying data, data interpretation, This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) Licence. You may share and adapt the material, but must give appropriate credit to the source, provide a link to the licence and indicate if changes were made.Any supplementary material referenced in the article can be found in the online version.