key: cord-0314680-q0igwb7t authors: Prsa, Mario; Kilicel, Deniz; Nourizonoz, Ali; Lee, Kuo-Sheng; Huber, Daniel title: Common computational principle for vibro-tactile pitch perception in mouse and human date: 2020-12-03 journal: bioRxiv DOI: 10.1101/2020.12.02.406272 sha: 29abd1d781e066a2c568a0ff2a07e080a61f11fe doc_id: 314680 cord_uid: q0igwb7t We live surrounded by vibrations generated by moving objects. These oscillatory stimuli can produce sound (i.e. airborne waves) and propagate through solid substrates. Pitch is the main perceptual characteristic of sound, and a similar perceptual attribute seems to exist in the case of substrate vibrations: vibro-tactile pitch. Here, we establish a mechanistic relationship between vibro-tactile pitch perception and the actual physical properties of vibrations using behavioral tasks, in which vibratory stimuli were delivered to the human fingertip or the mouse forelimb. The resulting perceptual reports were analyzed with a model demonstrating that physically different combinations of vibration frequencies and amplitudes can produce equal pitch perception. We found that the perceptually indistinguishable but physically different stimuli follow a common computational principle in mouse and human. It dictates that vibro-tactile pitch perception is shifted with increases in amplitude toward the frequency of highest vibrotactile sensitivity. These findings suggest the existence of a fundamental relationship between the seemingly unrelated concepts of spectral sensitivity and pitch perception. Pallesthesia is the clinical term to designate the sense of vibrations. In clinical practice, physicians test pallesthesia in their patients by applying a vibrating tuning fork against bones of lower and upper limbs. Indeed, Pacinian corpuscles, the mechanoreceptors specialized in transducing high frequency (>100 Hz) vibrations, can be found deep inside the forearm adjacent to joints and bones (Fleming and Luo, 2013; Prsa et al., 2019) . In turn, their innervating primary afferent neurons, located in the dorsal root ganglia, transmit the information along the ascending neuraxis to the somatosensory cortex, allowing us to consciously perceive properties of the vibratory stimulus. In the auditory system, the main property of airborne vibrations (i.e. sound) is pitch perception, which makes it possible to distinguish for example high from low notes or voices. It is quantified on a frequency scale but is a function of several physical properties of sound (Yost, 2009) . Similarly, vibro-tactile pitch perception is perhaps what allows one to identify the source of a nearby movement, such as a large or small object, a conspecific, a predator or a prey (Hager and Krausa, 2019; Hill, 2008; Mortimer et al., 2018; Narins et al., 2018) . Despite its importance, a systemic quantitative assessment of this percept is currently lacking in the somatosensory literature. On the one hand, standard V-shaped sensitivity curves have been established in humans and non-human primates (Brisben et al., 1999; Mountcastle et al., 1972) , and show that maximal vibration sensitivity occurs around 240 Hz. On the other, some evidence exists that vibro-tactile pitch is a complex function of multiple physical stimulus attributes, such as frequency and amplitude (Morley and Rowe, 1990; Prsa et al., 2019) . Can this function be precisely quantified, is it universal across species and is it in any way related to the spectral sensitivity curve? To answer these questions, mice and humans would ideally be trained in a frequency discrimination task at multiple spectral locations and tested if and how changes in vibration amplitude affect their perceptual responses. In a recent study (Prsa et al., 2019) we trained mice, using a go/no-go task design (Fig. 1A) , to discriminate 4 high frequencies (go response) from 4 low frequencies (no-go response) uniformly distributed around 450 Hz (pure sinusoidal vibrations). Mice were able to learn the discrimination task and perceived the stimuli on a continuum, as evidenced by the psychometric curve fits to their perceptual responses (Fig. 1B, black traces) . We then reasoned that if pitch perception depends exclusively on vibration frequency, their responses should not be affected when vibration amplitude is changed. To test the effect of amplitude change, after being trained on the frequency discrimination task at a fixed reference amplitude (5.6 μm) for 12 consecutive days, we introduced 5 different probe amplitudes on 30% of the trials on 5 separate days. This study revealed that amplitude change consistently shifted the psychometric curves: an amplitude increase required a decrease in stimulus frequency, and vice versa, in order to evoke the same perceptual response ( (Prsa et al., 2019) , Fig. 1B , colored traces). By fitting the frequency shift ratio as a function of the amplitude change factor (ACF), we identified that vibrotactile pitch is expressed as the product of vibration frequency (f) and a power function of vibration amplitude (A), two independent physical attributes (Fig. 1C) . The A k x f curve, with k=0.32 (fit to the data of 4 mice), represents all amplitude/frequency pairs that evoke the same pitch percept as a 450 Hz vibration at 5.6 μm. Here, we therefore first asked whether the same rule governs vibrotactile pitch perception in humans. Participants were instructed to compare the perceived frequency (and ignore the amplitude) of two consecutive vibrations (a test and a standard) delivered to the fingertip of their index finger in a two-alternative forced choice task design (Fig. 1D , see Methods for details). The standard stimulus was a 440 Hz vibration, and the frequency of the test stimuli uniformly distributed around the standard. Test stimuli were always presented at the same amplitude of 11.8 μm and the standard was presented at seven different reference amplitudes. As in the mouse data, changing vibration amplitude consistently shifted the psychometric curves so that pitch can be expressed as the A k x f product (Fig. 1E ,F). However, surprisingly the fitted k exponent was negative (k=-0.24, fit to the median of 9 subjects), meaning that a relative amplitude increase (of the test relative to the standard, ACF>1) required an increase of vibration frequency in order to evoke the same percept. Why does, in the case of a 440/450 Hz vibration, the A k x f equal pitch curve slope negatively in mice and positively in humans? To answer this question, we repeated the same experiments with a broader range of center/standard frequencies: 1000 Hz and 1600 Hz in mice and 160 Hz, 200 Hz, 280 Hz and 480 Hz in humans. We found that in the mouse experiments, changing the amplitude did not affect frequency discrimination for the 1000 Hz vibration (k not significantly different from 0) and yielded a negative k exponent (k=-0.044) for the 1600 Hz vibration ( Fig. 2A) . In human experiments, we found that the equal pitch curves sloped negatively (k>0) for 160 Hz and 200 Hz vibrations, and positively (k<0) for the 280 Hz and 480 Hz vibrations (Fig. 2B) . Therefore, in both species, the k exponent changes from positive to negative as we move higher in the vibration spectrum. The transition seems to occur at 1000 Hz in mice and ≈240 Hz in humans. To understand the significance of these transition points, we sought to establish their respective V-shaped sensitivity curves. Both mice and humans were trained for this purpose in a twoalternative forced choice task. Mice had to identify the presence or absence of a vibrotactile stimulation by licking either toward a left or right reward spout, and humans had to report in which of two successive intervals a vibratory stimulus was present (see Methods for details). The detection tasks yielded comprehensive sensitivity curves (Fig. 2C) , which revealed that the 1000 Hz and ≈240 Hz transition points are also the frequencies of highest vibrotactile sensitivity in the mouse and human, respectively. Therefore, the difference in pitch perception of a 440/450 Hz vibration between mice and humans is relatable to this frequency being in the lower end of the perceptual range of mice and in the higher end of that of humans. Finally, because the perceived intensity of a vibration also depends on both amplitude and frequency, it is important to disentangle equal pitch from equal intensity perception. To this end, we conducted the converse experiment, using the same task design, in which participants were instructed to compare the amplitude (and ignore the frequency) of a standard and a test vibration. The standard stimulus was this time always at a fixed amplitude (6 µm, 8 µm, 10 µm or 12 µm tested in different sessions) and the amplitude of the test stimuli uniformly distributed around this standard value. Within each session, we probed seven different reference frequencies for the standard vibration whereas the test stimuli were presented at the same 200 Hz frequency. As previously, by quantifying the shift in the psychometric fits (along the amplitude axis) caused by frequency changes of the standard yielded equal intensity curves (Fig. 3A) . The amplitude/frequency pairs falling on each curve are perceived to be equally intense as the reference 200 Hz vibration at the corresponding standard amplitude (black squares in Fig. 3A) . The minima at ≈250 Hz confirm this to be the frequency of maximal vibrotactile sensitivity in humans and an overlay with equal pitch curves ( A k x f equal pitch curve fits (red lines) to vibration amplitude as a function of the frequency shift ratio (normalized to the center frequency, black square, see Methods for details) for 450Hz (N=4 mice), 1000 Hz (N=4 mice) and 1600 Hz (N=6 mice) center frequencies (colored symbols). B: A k x f equal pitch curve fits (blue lines) to vibration amplitude as a function of the median (± quartiles, colored symbols) frequency shift ratio (normalized to the reference frequency, black square, see Methods for details) of n=9 subjects, for 160 Hz, 200 Hz, 280 Hz, 440 Hz and 480 Hz reference frequencies. C: V-shaped perceptual sensitivity curves (amplitude thresholds as a function of vibration frequency) for mouse (shaded lines: individual mice, symbols: mean) and human (mean ± s.e.m.). The equal pitch curves for all tested center/reference frequencies are replotted in the bottom panel illustrating that vibratory pitch perception shifts, with increases in amplitude, toward the frequency of highest vibrotactile sensitivity in both mouse (red) and human (blue). We conclude that vibrotactile pitch perception follows a common computational principle across different mammalian species in spite of fundamentally different anatomical distribution of Pacinian corpuscles between primate and mouse hands (Kumamoto et al., 1993; Prsa et al., 2019) . This perceptual quantity is expressed in terms of a vibration's physical attributes, frequency and amplitude, as A k x f. The latter product represents perceptual constancy or metamers, that is, equal pitch curves composed of physically different stimuli. The k exponent is adjusted so that the equal pitch curves always slope towards the frequency of maximal sensitivity (Fig. 2C) . In other words, if the amplitude of a vibration is changed by a factor N, its frequency must be shifted by a factor of (1/N) k in order to maintain the same pitch percept. The k exponent for a given equal pitch curve is such that decreases in amplitude always require a shift along the frequency axis toward the center of the perceptual range. If the frequency is however kept constant, perception will move to a new iso-pitch curve that is closer to the range center in the case of an amplitude increase, and further from the center in the case of an amplitude decrease. . Equal intensity and equal pitch curves quantify two different perceptual phenomena. A: equal intensity curves as cubic spline interpolations (green lines) of vibration frequency as a function of the mean (± SEM, colored symbols) amplitude shift ratio (normalized to the reference amplitude, black square, see Methods for details) of n=10 subjects, for 6 µm, 8 µm, 10 µm or 12 µm reference amplitudes tested in different sessions (the four panels). B: Overlay of equal intensity curves (from A) and equal pitch curves (from Fig. 2B) show that perceptual constancy relative to a reference vibration (square symbols) follows a different rule for intensity and pitch. Previous behavioral studies also reported that both humans (Harris et al., 2006; Morley and Rowe, 1990 ) and rodents (Adibi et al., 2012) might be "blind" to the physical attributes A and f of a vibration but instead perceive a composite feature. The feature was identified as the product A x f when rats were trained to discriminate a 37.5 Hz from a 75 Hz whisker vibration at two different amplitudes (Adibi et al., 2012) . This is consistent with our model of a A k x f iso-pitch curve given that the value of the k exponent increases as we move lower in the vibration spectrum ( Fig. 2A,B) and might thus approach unity below 100 Hz. This study however concluded that vibrations are also sensed as the A x f product when the rats were first trained to discriminate between the two different amplitudes instead of frequencies. It might in fact be impossible to disentangle pitch from intensity perception when testing very low frequencies (Fig. 3B ), but the distinction becomes clear closer to the center of the vibrotactile spectrum. In contrast to these earlier reports, our psychometric approach not only allowed us to obtain a precise quantification of vibrotactile pitch perception across the whole physiological spectral range, but also reveal its underlying computational principle by linking it to the spectral sensitivity of the somatosensory system. A similar principle seems to apply to auditory stimuli (i.e. airborne vibrations) as well. Indeed, Stevens described that changes in sound amplitude affect how high or low the pitch of a tone is perceived (Cohen, 1961; Stevens, 1935) . His classical work on this psychoacoustic effect also shows that iso-pitch curves slope toward the center of the hearing range. Recently, neural recordings revealed that in both somatosensory (Prsa et al., 2019) and auditory (Tao et al., 2017) cortex, frequency-tuned neuronal response curves shift with changes in stimulus amplitude according to the same computational principle. This rule seems to originate from the sensory periphery. On the one hand, the location of cochlear maximum excitation has been reported to shift with sound level (Zwislocki and Nguyen, 1999) , and on the other, in rapidly adapting afferents innervating the hand, the vibration frequency that entrains the maximal number of spikes is observed to become higher for smaller amplitudes (Johansson et al., 1982) . The idea that a common mechanism governs the pitch perception of sound and substrate vibrations is intriguing given that the two emerge from fundamentally different sensory receptors (hair cells vs. lamellar corpuscles). Actually, it has been proposed that communication via airborne sounds might have evolved from the more ancient precursor modality based on substrate-borne vibration signaling (Hill, 2008) . Many insect species communicate exclusively by emitting and sensing substrate vibrations (Cocroft and Rodríguez, 2005) while in others, the same sensory organ, such as the Johnston's organ in drosophila, is used to detect both sound and touch (Azevedo and Wilson, 2017) . Vestiges of this modality seem to be still present in rodents, given that Ehrenberg's mole-rats vibrate their subterranean tunnels to communicate with conspecifics (Heth et al., 1987; Rado et al., 1987) , and might explain the parallels between pitch perception in auditory and somatosensory systems. All experiments were conducted with male and female C57BL/6 (Charles River Laboratory) mice, 10 to 20 weeks old at the start of behavioral training. They were first prepared for head-fixation under general isoflurane anaesthesia (1.5 to 2%) as previously described (Prsa et al., 2019) . Briefly, a custom made titanium head bar was fixed on the skull with a cyanoacrylate adhesive (ergo 5011, IBZ Industrie) and dental cement to allow for subsequent head fixation. They were housed in an animal facility, maintained on a 12:12 light/dark cycle and were placed under a water restriction regime (1 ml/day) 1 week before the start of experiments. The experiments were performed during the light phase of the cycle. The animals did not undergo any previous surgery, drug administration or experiments and were housed in groups of maximum 5 animals per cage. All procedures complied with and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Geneva and Geneva veterinary offices. The cohort included 19 participants aged between 21 and 48 years ( mean ± s.d. = 30.21 ± 8.38, 9 females) with no history of somatosensory injury or disease, no psychiatric disorder and no substance abuse. Prior to study participation, all gave informed consent and received a 20 CHF/h monetary retribution at their last session. All experimental procedures were approved by the ethics commission of the Geneva canton (CCER). Vibrotactile stimulation was delivered with piezoelectric stack actuators (P-841.3 for mouse and P-841K191 for human experiments, Physik Instrumente). The stimulation endpoint was a metal rod (2 mm diameter) mounted either vertically (for human fingertip stimulation) or horizontally (for mouse forepaw stimulation) on the actuator with an M3 screw. Actuator position was monitored with a strain gauge sensor and the actuator and sensor controllers (E-504 and E-509.S3 for mouse, E-618.1 and E-509.S1 for human experiments, Physik Instrumente) operated either in closed loop (450 Hz center frequency experiment in mice) or open loop (all other experiments) modes. Operating in open loop mode was necessary in order to produce the full range of frequencies tested in the study. The recorded sensor signals were analyzed offline in temporal and spectral domains and revealed that open loop operation did not compromise the integrity of the vibratory stimuli. The stimuli were pure sinusoids (250 or 500 ms duration, 25 ms or 50 ms linear onset/offset ramps) sampled at either 10, 20 or 30 kHz (USB-6353, National Instruments). Although naturally occurring vibrations are non-stationary and typically have a broad spectrum, pure sinusoidal stimuli can be used to better quantify perceptual responses. The amplitude of the sinusoids was calibrated based on sensor measurements in order to produce the required actuator displacements. Recalibration was performed regularly to guarantee stimulus consistency over time. Mouse behavior was controlled with real-time routines running on Linux (BControl, brodylab.princeton.edu/bcontrol) and interfaced with Matlab (Mathworks) running on a separate PC. Human behavior was controlled with custom routines programmed in Matlab. We used a go/no-go task to train mice to discriminate frequencies of vibrotactile stimuli with their forepaw. They were head-fixed and positioned inside a tube (25 mm inner diameter) such that their right forepaw held the stimulator to maintain balance, while their left forelimb was blocked from protruding outside the tube. The trial started with a 1 s period requiring continuous holding of the stimulator followed by stimulus delivery. The hold interval was reset upon every paw release. A white noise sound was played over loud speakers at the moment of vibratory stimulation, thereby acting simultaneously as an auditory mask and as a stimulus cue. Following stimulus presentation, mice had to initiate licking of a water spout for Go frequencies and refrain from licking for No-go frequencies, within a 2 s period. Hit trials (licking for go stimuli) were rewarded by a drop of water, misses (no licking for go stimuli) and false alarms (licking for no-go stimuli) were punished by a 1 to 6 s timeout. Correct rejections (no licking for no-go stimuli) were not rewarded nor punished. A new trial was initiated after licking ceased for a minimum of 2 s. To minimize licking response bias, one of two strategies was used. In the first, a minimum of three consecutive correct rejection responses were required before a go trial was presented. In the second, the probability of a go trial (P go ) was determined according to the double sigmoidal model: Where S1 and S2 are the slopes at the chosen inflection points τ1=-0.5 and τ2=0.5, respectively. The steepness of the slopes was arbitrarily chosen to be S1=16 and S2=2.7xS1. The bias value was defined as the difference in the fraction of correct responses between go and no-go trials in the last 20 trials. We tested three different frequency ranges with three groups of mice: a low range (4 mice; center frequency: 450 Hz; no-go stimuli: 310 Hz, 345 Hz, 380 Hz and 415 Hz; go-stimuli: 485 Hz, 520 Hz, 555 Hz and 590 Hz), a middle range (4 mice; center frequency: 1000 Hz; no-go stimuli: 900 Hz, 925 Hz, 950 Hz and 975 Hz; go-stimuli: 1025 Hz, 1050 Hz, 1075 Hz and 1100 Hz) and a high range (6 mice; center frequency: 1600 Hz; no-go stimuli: 1500 Hz, 1525 Hz, 1550 Hz and 1575 go-stimuli: 1625 Hz, 1650 Hz, 1675 Hz and 1700 . The 4 mice tested on the middle range were also part of the high range group. The two ranges were tested more than two weeks apart. In the first 7 to 14 sessions, the mice performed the task at a fixed reference amplitude (5.6 µm for the low range and 3.8 µm for the middle and high ranges). In the last 6 sessions (5 for the low range), non-trained probe amplitudes were introduced in 30% of the trials to test the effect of amplitude change on frequency discrimination. The probe trials occurred pseudorandomly and followed the same go/no-go rules as the 70% of trials delivered at the trained reference amplitude. A single probe amplitude was tested in each session (8.6 µm, 7.1 µm, 4.1 µm, 2.6 µm or 1.1 µm for the low range; 6.9 µm, 5.4 µm, 4.8 µm, 2.7 µm, 2.1 µm or 1.8 µm for the middle and high ranges). Each stimulus frequency-amplitude pair was repeated at least 10 times in a single session. In the low range group, one last session consisted of a control experiment where the paw was restrained and not in contact with the stimulator. Performance consisted of zero fraction of lick responses across all tested frequencies (data not shown) confirming that mice could not use auditory cues to perform the discrimination task. The data from the low range group has been previously published by our group (Prsa et al., 2019) . We used a two-alternative forced choice task to test vibrotactile pitch perception in 9 healthy human participants (age mean ± s.d. = 27.56 ± 5.03; 5 females). An additional 6 subjects performed the task but were excluded from the analysis after realizing the actuator failed to generate vibrations at one of the amplitudes due to a coding error. Participants sat comfortably in a dark room and positioned their right forearm on a vibration isolation table. The stimulator endpoint (a punctuate probe of 3 mm diameter mounted on the piezo stack) was placed in contact with the fingertip of their index finger, with their hand either in the palm down (4 subjects) or palm up (5 subjects) position. We did not control for the contact force as it was previously reported to play no role in behavioral performance (Brisben et al., 1999) . The participants wore noise canceling headphones (3M Peltor WorkTunes Pro HRXS220A) and masking white noise was played throughout the session. The task was guided with visual cues displayed on a 60 inch monitor viewed at a 140 cm distance. Each trial started with a 0.5 s pre-stimulus interval during which a red fixation dot was displayed, followed by a 2.5 s stimulus interval cued with the fixation dot turning green. During this interval, two successive vibrations (0.5 s duration each) were delivered to the fingertip, preceded, separated and followed by a 0.5 s silent period. A non-timed answer period followed in which the words 'First' and 'Second' appeared on the screen. The participants were instructed to select whether the first or second vibration had a higher frequency with the push of a button (Stream Deck Mini) held in their left hand. The instruction was to focus on the frequency and ignore the amplitude; the two terms were clearly explained to the participants prior to experiment start. One of the two stimuli (the standard) was always at the same reference frequency f REF and the other at a changing test frequency f TEST = f REF ± Δ. The order of the standard and test was randomized, but the comparison of the test relative to the standard was measured during analysis. The amplitude of the test stimuli was kept constant at A TEST = 11.8 μm and the amplitude of the standard was consistently changed between seven different values A REF = 7.4 μm, 8.4 μm, 9.8 μm, 11.8 μm, 14.2 μm, 16 .5 μm and 18.9 μm. Before the start of each session, participants received training trials with Δ = Δ MAX , repeated until they performed 10 correct answers in a row for each A REF . During the training trials, feedback about correct performance was given by highlighting in green (for correct) or red (incorrect) the selected response. The purpose of these training trials was to ensure that the subjects understood the instructions and were repeated until they performed close to 100% correct for the easiest comparisons (i.e. f TEST = f REF ± Δ MAX ). No feedback was given on the subsequent test trials. The test stimuli were presented using a custom staircase adaptive procedure. For each A REF , test stimuli started with Δ = Δ MAX . After each correct or incorrect answer, Δ was lowered or increased by dΔ (its rate of change), respectively. After three successive changes in the same direction, dΔ was doubled and after each change direction reversal, dΔ was halved. These adjustments were made independently for f TEST The participants were given an option to take a break after every block of ten trials. In order to determine their perceptual thresholds, we used a two-alternative forced choice task to train 4 mice in a vibrotactile detection task. Mice were trained to lick, in the response period, toward either a right or left reward spout if a vibrotactile stimulus was present or absent during the preceding stimulus period, respectively. All other experimental conditions were as described above in the frequency discrimination task. Correct responses were rewarded with a drop of water at the corresponding spout and incorrect responses were not punished by a timeout. Trials without a response were neither rewarded nor punished and occurred on <5% of trials. To minimize a direction bias, the trial type was chosen pseudorandomly by allowing a maximum of 2 trials of the same type in a row (50% chance of occurrence for each otherwise). We tested the perceptual thresholds at 7 different frequencies (200 Hz, 450 Hz, 700 Hz, 1000 Hz, 1300 Hz, 1600 Hz and 2000 in separate sessions and in randomized order. Between 1 and 3 sessions were tested in a single day and the same session (i.e. frequency) was repeated up to 5 times on separate days per mouse. Prior to testing, the mice were first trained on all frequencies at the largest possible amplitude that the actuator could produce at each frequency. This value ranged from 10 µm (at 200 Hz) to 1 µm (at 2000 Hz). The training lasted 10 days, followed by a two month break (COVID-19) and a second training period of 10 to 12 days. Testing of each frequency started at the largest possible amplitude and was progressively attenuated in -4 dB steps after every 6 vibration trials (total of ≈12 trials) if the proportion of correct responses exceeded 70 %. The amplitude was increased by 4 dB if the proportion of correct responses decreased below 60 % after every ≈12 trials (including at least 6 vibration trials). To determine the perceptual threshold at each frequency, we compared the ratio of correct responses for each bout of trials at a given amplitude to chance (i.e. 0.5) using the one-sided binomial test. The threshold was the lowest amplitude of the session for which the test yielded a significance level of <0.05. The thresholds of repeated sessions were averaged and allowed establishing the V-shaped vibrotactile sensitivity curves (Fig. 2C) . We used a two-alternative forced choice task to determine the perceptual thresholds across a wide range of vibration frequencies in 19 healthy human participants including the same 9 participants from the previous task. Each trial started with a 0.5 s pre-stimulus interval (red fixation dot) followed by a 3.25 s stimulus interval. The stimulus interval consisted of two successive 1.5 s active periods (cued by green dots on the display) separated by a 0.25 s passive period (red dot on display). A 0.5 s vibratory stimulus was delivered at a random time either during the first or the second active period. The participants were instructed to answer in which of the two periods the stimulus was present by either selecting 'First' or 'Second' on the display with the push of a button. We tested 14 different vibration frequencies (10 Hz, 25 Hz, 50 Hz, 75 Hz, 100 Hz, 200 Hz, 300 Hz, 400 Hz, 500 Hz, 600 Hz, 700 Hz, 800 Hz, 900 Hz and 1000 Hz) in separate blocks and in randomized order. To determine the perceptual threshold for each, we used a 3-down, 1-up adaptive staircase procedure. For each frequency, the vibration amplitude started at its maximal value (i.e. the maximal travel range of the piezo stack at that frequency) and was decreased by Δ dB after 3 successive correct answers and increased by Δ dB after 1 incorrect answer. Δ started at 12 dB and was halved after each direction reversal, but maintained at a minimum of 3 dB. The testing stopped after 5 direction reversals and the detection threshold was taken as the mean amplitude of the last 10 trials. All other experimental conditions were as described above in the frequency discrimination task. We used a two-alternative forced choice task to test vibrotactile intensity perception in 10 healthy human participants (age mean ± s.d. = 27.2 ± 5.39; 5 females). All experimental details were as described above for the frequency discrimination task. The participants were instructed to select whether the first or second vibration had higher amplitude. The instruction was to focus on the amplitude and ignore the frequency. One of the two stimuli (the standard) was always at the same reference amplitude A REF and the other at a changing test amplitude A TEST = A REF ± Δ. The frequency of the test stimuli was kept constant at f TEST = 200 Hz and the frequency of the standard was consistently changed between seven different values f REF = 75 Hz, 100 Hz, 150 Hz, 200 Hz, 266 Hz, 400 Hz and 534 Hz. Each participant repeated the experiment four times, each time with a different A REF , in separate sessions. The task structure and testing procedure were analogous to those described above in the frequency discrimination task. The four tested A REF were 6 µm, 8 µm, 10 µm and 12 µm. Their respective Δ MAX were different for A TEST > A REF than for A TEST < A REF due to the amplitude limitations imposed by the hardware. For A TEST > A REF , the respective Δ MAX were 12 µm, 10 µm, 8 µm and 6 µm, and for A TEST < A REF , the Δ MAX were 5 µm, 7 µm, 9 µm and 11 µm. The minimum rate of change dΔ was 1 µm, and the maximum rate of change was 3 µm for all A REF . Psychometric curve fitting In the frequency discrimination tasks, we analyzed the fraction of lick responses in mice and the fraction of test stimuli reported to be higher relative to the standard in humans, as a function of vibration frequency. The data was fit with a sigmoid function (i.e. a cumulative Gaussian) assuming equal asymptotes, using the psignfit Matlab toolbox (Schutt et al., 2016) . Only for the middle range data in mice could we not assume equal asymptotes and therefore fitted in addition the lapse rate and guess rate parameters. We identified that pitch perception can be expressed as A k x f by fitting the frequency shift ratio μ/μ REF Fig. 1C ,F and Fig. 2 A,B were plotted by multiplying the frequency shift ratio values by the center/reference frequency and the amplitude change factor values by the reference amplitude. The A k x f fit was deemed significant when the 95% confidence intervals of the fitted k parameter did not include zero. For non-significant fits, k was made equal to zero. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample size. No randomization was required as our study did not involve separating subjects into control and experimental groups. Analyses of data comparing different experimental conditions in the same subjects were performed by blinded researchers. All data analyses were performed with custom written routines in Matlab (Mathworks). Behavioral study of whisker-mediated vibration sensation in rats Active Mechanisms of Vibration Encoding and Frequency Filtering in Central Mechanosensory Neurons Detection of vibration transmitted through an object grasped in the hand The Behavioral Ecology of Insect Vibrational Communication Further Investigation of the Effects of Intensity upon the Pitch of Pure Tones The anatomy, function, and development of mammalian Abeta low-threshold mechanoreceptors Acacia Ants Respond to Plant-Borne Vibrations Caused by Mammalian Browsers Factors Affecting Frequency Discrimination of Vibrotactile Stimuli: Implications for Cortical Encoding Vibrational Communication in Subterranean Mole Rats (Spalax ehrenbergi) Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements Distribution of pacinian corpuscles in the hand of the monkey, Macaca fuscata Perceived pitch of vibrotactile stimuli: effects of vibration amplitude, and implications for vibration frequency coding Classifying elephant behaviour through seismic vibrations Detection thresholds for stimuli in humans and monkeys: comparison with threshold events in mechanoreceptive afferent nerve fibers innervating the monkey hand Plant-borne vibrations modulate calling behaviour in a tropical amphibian Feature-selective encoding of substrate vibrations in the forelimb somatosensory cortex Seismic signalling as a means of communication in a subterranean mammal Painfree and accurate Bayesian estimation of psychometric functions for (potentially) overdispersed data The relation of pitch to intensity Bidirectional Shifting Effects of the Sound Intensity on the Best Frequency in the Rat Auditory Cortex. Scientific reports 7 Pitch perception. Attention, perception & psychophysics 71 Place code for pitch: a necessary revision