key: cord-0311552-9f53qkvi authors: Sommer, Verena R.; Mount, Luzie; Weigelt, Sarah; Werkle-Bergner, Markus; Sander, Myriam C. title: Memory specificity is linked to repetition effects in event-related potentials across the lifespan date: 2020-09-14 journal: bioRxiv DOI: 10.1101/2020.09.14.295972 sha: 34f740910752315eb1f8c469817889f4d162d959 doc_id: 311552 cord_uid: 9f53qkvi Our memories depend on our brain’s ability to form internal representations of relevant aspects of the world that can later be retrieved. The specificity with which past experiences can be remembered varies across the lifespan, possibly due to differences in how precisely information is encoded. This memory formation can be investigated through repetition effects, the common finding that neural activity is altered (suppressed or enhanced) when stimuli are repeated. However, whether differences in this indirect measure of memory formation relate to lifespan age differences in memory specificity has not yet been established. In the present study, we examined repetition effects in event-related potentials and their relation to recognition specificity. During incidental encoding, children (aged 7–9 years), young adults (18–30 years), and older adults (65–76 years) viewed repeated object images from different categories. During subsequent recognition, old, similar, and new objects were presented, allowing for a differentiation of memory for the specific item versus the general category. We identified neural repetition suppression effects in all age groups, and repetition enhancement for adults. Furthermore, individual item recognition performance comprising lure discrimination was positively associated with the magnitude of the neural repetition effects. These brain–behavior associations did not differ between age groups, indicating common neural mechanisms of memory formation. In sum, our findings demonstrate that neural repetition effects reflect encoding mechanisms that facilitate the formation of highly specific memory representations and highlight their significance as a neural indicator of individual differences in episodic memory encoding across the lifespan. Research Highlights We show reliable repetition effects in event-related potentials during object encoding for children, young adults, and older adults Inter-individual differences in the magnitudes of the repetition effects were positively related to highly specific item memory, including lure discrimination, but not mere category memory The association of repetition effects and memory performance did not differ between age groups, indicating similar neural mechanisms of memory encoding across the lifespan Thus, we demonstrate a common, age-independent neural indicator of the formation of high-fidelity memory representations, enabling precise episodic memory across the lifespan Memories of past events vary in their degree of specificity from very unique and detailed to more general and gist-like. Variability in memory specificity partly reflects how precisely incoming information has been encoded (McClelland and Rumelhart, 1985; Robin and Moscovitch, 2017) . Forming precise memory representations is critical in many situations as it enables remembering specific details and avoiding confusion with similar information. This ability changes across lifespan development (cf. Ofen and Shing, 2013) . Understanding how memory representations are formed is therefore crucial in the study of memory, particularly with regard to differences in memory competences across the lifespan. One way to study the formation of neural representations is to examine brain activity in response to repeated stimulus input (Barron et al., 2016a; Grill-Spector et al., 2006) . If the neural response differs between first and repeated encounters, this indicates that an internal representation of the repeated stimulus has been formed (Rugg and Doyle, 1994) . Indeed, such repetition effects are widely observed: Repetition suppression describes the finding that neural activity evoked by a repeated stimulus is reduced (Barron et al., 2016a; Grill-Spector et al., 2006) . In addition, increased activity (repetition enhancement) has also been reported, especially in the event-related potential (ERP) literature (Desimone, 1996; Doniger et al., 2001; Nagy and Rugg, 1989; Rugg et al., 1995) , but also in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; for a review, see Segaert et al., 2013) . Repetition effects can be leveraged to investigate particular properties of neural memory representations, for example, their specificity and overlap (cf. Barron et al., 2016b; Koolschijn et al., 2019; Schacter et al., 2004) . For instance, Jiang et al. (2006) modulated the similarity of face images by morphing and showed that moderately similar faces elicited smaller fMRI repetition suppression effects than identical faces, which was related to face discrimination performance. These findings suggest that repetition effects can track the specificity of neural representations (Gilaie-Dotan and Malach, 2007; Grill-Spector et al., 2006; Lueschow et al., 1994) . Although the repetition effect (primarily suppression) has been mainly implicated as the neural correlate of implicit memory, namely repetition priming (Doniger et al., 2001; Gotts et al., 2012; Henson, 2003; Wiggs and Martin, 1998) , it has also been associated with explicit memory (cf. Nagy and Rugg, 1989) . To date, however, this relationship has not been established fully. On the within-person level, i.e., in terms of 'subsequent memory effects' (Paller and Wagner, 2002) , Turk-Browne et al. (2006) showed that subsequently remembered scenes elicited greater fMRI repetition suppression and behavioral priming than scenes that were not remembered, thus linking repetition effects to both explicit and implicit memory. However, other studies found no relation (Rugg, 1990; Ward et al., 2013) or even a negative relation (Wagner et al., 2000; Xue et al., 2011) between neural repetition effects and explicit memory. On the between-person level, individual differences in fMRI repetition suppression were shown to be related to individual differences in memory performance and thus linked to memory deficits in particular groups only, such as in participants with autism spectrum conditions (Ewbank et al., 2017) , or mild cognitive impairments and Alzheimer's disease (Pihlajamäki et al., 2011) . Given that previous findings are mixed, it is an open question whether repetition effects can be leveraged as a useful indicator for successful episodic memory formation within and across different populations, for example, age groups. Memory abilities vary across the lifespan, especially the ability to remember specific details versus the general gist of an episode (Graf and Ohta, 2002; Hultsch et al., 1998) . The balance of being able to discriminate among similar events (pattern separation) and the ability to abstract and generalize between events (pattern completion) is critical for successful and adaptive behavior (McClelland and Rumelhart, 1985; Wiltgen and Silva, 2007; Xu and Südhof, 2013) . Compared with adults, children's memories have shown to rely more on perceptual properties than on abstract/semantic knowledge that is still developing, making them less susceptible to confuse similar information (Brainerd et al., 2002 (Brainerd et al., , 2008 Ofen and Shing, 2013; Sloutsky and Fisher, 2004 ; but see Keresztes et al., 2018; Ngo et al., 2018) . The opposite is observed at the other end of the lifespan: Older adults rely more on general concepts and similarities, leading to more false memories (Fandakova et al., 2018; Koutstaal and Schacter, 1997; Schacter et al., 1997) . Developmental differences in memory abilities are accompanied by age-dependent changes in the underlying brain structures and functions during maturation and senescence (Li et al., 2004; Ofen and Shing, 2013; Sander et al., 2020; Shing et al., 2010; Van Petten, 2004) . Specifically, differences in the neural encoding of memory representations may influence the specificity of mnemonic contents and may thus account for performance differences across the lifespan (Bowman et al., 2019; Fandakova et al., 2019; Morcom, 2015; Park et al., 2013) . In line with this, it has been suggested that neural representations become less specific in old age, which may be linked to aging-related cognitive decline (Carp et al., 2011; Li and Sikström, 2002; Li et al., 2001; Park et al., 2004 Park et al., , 2010 . Indeed, a positive relation between the specificity of the neural activity patterns during encoding and memory performance could be demonstrated for young and older adults (Kobelt et al., 2020; Koen et al., 2019) . In children (7-12 years), higher neural specificity of scene representations was associated with better memory performance, too (Fandakova et al., 2019) . In addition to measuring neural specificity as the discriminability of neural activation patterns (Fandakova et al., 2019; Koen et al., 2019) , the stimulus-specificity of neural repetition effects can indicate how precisely information is represented (see above; Jiang et al., 2006) and how these representations may vary across the lifespan (cf. Nordt et al., 2016) . With this approach it has been shown that the specificity with which faces of modified similarity were represented in face-selective brain regions was lower in children (5-12 years) than young adults (Natu et al., 2016) , and lower in older adults (61-72 years) than in young adults (Goh et al., 2010) , which was associated with differences in face discrimination performance. These results suggest a behaviorally relevant improvement of how precisely information is encoded from childhood to adulthood followed by a decline during aging. All in all, crucial aspects of neurocognitive lifespan development including representational specificity have been linked to repetition-related changes in neural activity. However, age-comparative studies directly testing the association between repetition effects and memory abilities, particularly memory specificity, are lacking. In the current EEG study, we therefore investigated whether neural repetition effects were directly associated with the specificity of later recognition by studying ERP repetition effects in response to repeated object images in children (7-9 years), young adults (18-30 years), and older adults (65-76 years). We investigated how individual differences in repetition effects were related to the ability to recognize the presented objects. During recognition, exact repetitions, similar lures, and entirely new items were presented (cf. Stark et al., 2019) , allowing for a dissociation of mere category memory from precise exemplar memory. This enabled us to investigate whether repetition effects reflected general concept or detailed item-specific encoding. A total of 23 healthy children, 41 healthy young adults, and 61 healthy older adults participated in the EEG study. Further collection of children's data had to be aborted due to the COVID-19 pandemic. We excluded one child and three older adults due to low performance in the vigilance task during encoding (see Behavioral performance) and one child, two young adults, and two older adults due to missing or noisy EEG data. One additional older adult was excluded based on low performance in the dementia screening (see below). The remaining 21 children (age: M(SD) = 8.38(0.74), 7-9 years, 15 female, 6 male), 39 young adults (age: M(SD) = 24.77(3.30) years, 18-30 years, 23 female, 16 male), and 55 older adults (age: M(SD) = 70.15(3.39) years, 65-76 years, 27 female, 28 male) are included in the analyses presented here. Children and adults were recruited at different sites. Children were recruited from a participant database at the Developmental Neuropsychology Lab at the Ruhr-Universität Bochum (RUB), Germany. Adults were recruited from a participant database at the Max Planck Institute for Human Development in Berlin, Germany, and through advertisements. All participants were native German speakers, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no history of psychiatric or neurological disease, and no use of psychiatric medication. We screened older adults with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) . One had a score below the threshold of 24 points (namely 21) and was excluded. The remaining older adults reached a mean score of 28.95 (SD = 1.21, 25-30 points). To control for the different task versions (see below), behavioral performance of 23 additional young adults on the shorter children's task was assessed at RUB. All experiments were approved by the respective local 65-76 committee. All adult participants and the children's parents gave written informed consent to taking part in the experiment and all participants and/or their parents could abort the study at any time without giving reasons. Data for this study were collected at two different sites. Children were tested at the Developmental Neuropsychology Lab at the Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Germany, and adults were tested at the Center for Lifespan Psychology at the Max Planck Institute for Human Development in Berlin, Germany. Some experimental procedures, such as collected covariates, also differed for children and adults. However, the same visual object encoding and recognition tasks were at the core of the study (see Figure 1A) , with children performing a shorter version of the tasks, namely only the first half. Other than that, the selection and order of stimuli in both tasks were identical for all participants. During encoding and recognition, participants viewed color photographs of objects from different categories (e.g., hats, trees, guitars). The pictures were partly selected from a published stimulus set ("Massive Memory" Object Categories; https://konklab.fas.harvard.edu; Konkle et al., 2010) and partly obtained from the internet. The stimuli were scaled to 500 × 500 pixels and all text was removed using Adobe Photoshop CC 2017. Stimuli were presented using Psychtoolbox (Psychophysics Toolbox, RRID: SCR_002881) for MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA; RRID: SCR_001622). During both tasks, EEG was acquired. In the following, we describe the task paradigm for the adults, followed by the specific differences for the children's sample. During the encoding task (see Figure 1 ), adult participants viewed objects from 80 different categories. Participants were not instructed to memorize the stimuli and did not know that their memory would be tested lateron. Stimuli were presented successively on a white background on the center of the screen. A central fixation cross was superimposed on the objects and remained on the screen throughout the task. Stimulus duration was 1500 ms with an inter-stimulus-interval jittered between 1500 and 2000 ms. Subjects were instructed to attend to the objects but to fixate on the cross in order to minimize eye movements. To ensure that participants attended to each trial, they performed a target detection task in which they were asked to press a button whenever the fixation cross changed its color from black to magenta. The target trials appeared in approximately 7% of all trials, that is 54 times in total, with 1-25 (on average 14.7) trials between two target trials. The color change occurred 100-500 ms after onset of the stimulus. The target trials showed objects from additional categories and were excluded from further analyses. After the first half of the trials, adult participants could take a voluntary break. To maintain motivation for the following trials, participants received visual feedback on how many color changes they had successfully detected so far. The two halves of the encoding task were independent of each other in that categories from the first half were not part of the second half. Overview of the experimental paradigm. In the encoding task, objects were sequentially presented, and subjects were asked to press a button whenever the fixation cross changed color. Stimulus presentation lasted 1.5 s and inter-stimulus intervals (ISI) were jittered between 1.5 s and 2 s. Items were repeated either 2 or 4 times (see B), with 3 to 10 items from other categories shown between repetitions. Either 2 or 4 exemplars from each object category were presented (see B), with at least 5 trials in-between. The task consisted of 780 trials for adults and only the first 390 trials for children. During recognition, objects were sequentially presented, and subjects were asked to decide for each object whether it was old, similar, or new. Stimulus presentation lasted 1.5 s, response time was self-paced but limited to 2 s, and ISI were jittered between 1.5 s and 2 s. Objects were either already shown during encoding (old targets), or from categories that were shown during encoding but new exemplars (similar lures), or from novel categories (new foils). Each item was shown only once. The recognition task consisted of 480 trials for adults and only the first 240 trials for children. B. Encoding conditions. The number of item repetitions (2 or 4) and category exemplars (2 or 4) were manipulated during encoding, resulting in 4 encoding conditions: Baseline (BL) with 2 repetitions of 2 exemplars, High-Repetition (HR) with 4 repetitions of 2 exemplars, High-Exemplar (HE) with 2 repetitions of 4 exemplars, and High-Repetition-and-Exemplar (HRE) with 4 repetitions of 4 exemplars. Each condition comprised 20 object categories for adults, and 10 object categories for children. One sample category per condition is shown for illustration. C. Recognition. From each category presented during encoding, 2 exemplars were presented as old target items plus 2 new exemplars as similar lures. Not depicted: 2 exemplars from novel categories were presented as new items. The 80 object categories were equally divided into four conditions that differed with respect to (a) the number of different presented exemplars from one category (either two or four), and (b) the number of exemplar repetitions (either two or four times). This resulted in four encoding conditions: (1) Baseline (BL) condition with two exemplars per category that were each presented twice, (2) High-Repetition (HR) condition with two exemplars per category that were each presented four times, (3) High-Exemplar (HE) condition with four exemplars per category that were each presented twice, (4) High-Repetition-and-Exemplar (HRE) condition with four exemplars per category that were each presented four times (see Figure 1B ). Twenty categories were randomly selected for each condition. For those conditions with two exemplars per category (BL and HR), two of the four available exemplars were randomly selected for presentation. The stimulus order was pseudorandomized with the restriction that 3-10 stimuli (from other categories) appeared between repetitions of the same item, with the same mean distance for all conditions. Furthermore, at least five other items were presented after the last repetition of a category exemplar before the next exemplar of that category was shown. In this way, exemplars from the same category were not presented interleaved. Since the number of exemplars and repetitions differed between conditions, the number of trials in each condition varied accordingly: 80 in the BL condition, 160 in the HR condition, 160 in the HE condition, and 320 trials in the HRE condition. This adds up to a total of 720 experimental trials. In addition to these experimental trials, 54 target trials (see above) and 6 filler trials were presented. Filler trials were needed when the distance restrictions of the pseudorandomization could not be met otherwise. Like target trials, filler trials showed objects from additional categories and they were excluded from further EEG analyses. A shorter version of the task was used for the children. They performed only the first half of the adult task, comprising 40 distinct object categories, i.e., 10 in each encoding condition, which results in 360 experimental trials (40 BL, 80 HR, 80 HE, 160 HRE), plus 27 target trials, and 3 filler trials. In contrast to the adults, children had more breaks, namely every 85 trials (approximately every 4 minutes). The breaks were voluntary and self-paced. After a 15-minute break, a surprise recognition task followed (see Figure 1A ). Again, object images were presented successively on a white screen and with a black fixation cross on top. The presented objects were either "old," i.e., identical to an item of the encoding task (targets), "similar," i.e., of a category that was already presented in the encoding task but a different exemplar (lures), or "new," i.e., of a novel category (foils). Stimulus duration was again 1500 ms. After each stimulus presentation, participants were asked to indicate whether the object was "old," "similar," or "new" by pressing one of three buttons on a button box. The response was self-paced but limited to 2000 ms. Thereupon, the fixation cross was shown for 1500-2000 ms before the next stimulus item appeared. For adults, the task consisted of 160 targets, 160 lures, and 160 foils, resulting in a total of 480 trials and a chance level of 33%. The targets contained the same 80 object categories that were presented in the encoding task, but for each category, only two exemplars were presented in the recognition task (see Figure 1C ). For those categories that belonged to an encoding condition with four exemplars (HE and HRE), two of these were randomly selected to be tested in the recognition task. Two more novel exemplars from the same categories were presented as similar lures. The foils consisted of two exemplars from each of the 80 novel object categories. The stimuli were pseudorandomized with the restriction that there were at least three trials between items from the same object category, and maximally four stimuli with the same correct response ("old," "similar," "new") were presented in a row. Targets that were presented in the first/second half of the encoding task were also tested in the first/second half of the recognition task, respectively. The children only performed the first half of the recognition task, i.e., the first 240 trials, containing 80 targets, 80 lures, and 80 foils. In contrast to the adults who had no breaks in the recognition task, the children could again take a self-paced break every 85 trials. Encoding task Performance in the encoding task was measured as the proportion of correctly detected color changes of the fixation cross. In the recognition task, participants were asked to indicate for each presented object whether it was "old" (target), "similar" (lure), or "new" (foil). One way of quantifying memory performance is to calculate the proportion of correct answers: Pr("old" | target) + Pr("similar" | lure) + Pr("new" | foil). However, the two respective incorrect responses for each item class can be evaluated as not equally incorrect but as reflecting the level of memory specificity. For example, incorrectly responding "similar" to a target is less precise than responding "old," but it shows that the general category has been correctly identified as known, whereas a "new" response would reflect no memory for either the item or category (cf. familiarity versus recollection; Mandler, 1980; Yonelinas, 2002) . On this basis, we computed performance measures that focus on different levels of specificity, namely on precise item memory and more general category memory (see Figure 2 ). These measures are equivalent to the classic performance measure d' in old/new recognition tasks that assess correctly recognized old items (hits) corrected for falsely recognized new items or the tendency to respond "old" (false alarms). The current recognition task comprised three response options with graded precision. Thus, different levels of memory specificity can be measured by scoring different responses as correct versus incorrect. Item memory (IM): Only if specific details of the objects were encoded, were participants able to distinguish previously presented targets from similar lures and vice versa. In this measure, only responses of correct target memory and lure detection were scored as accurate, corrected for both unspecific category memory (see below) as well as the general tendency to respond "old" or "similar." In terms of probability, since IM is corrected for four incorrect response combinations while only two combinations are counted as correct, it is more likely to give an incorrect than a correct response. Because of this imbalance of correct and incorrect response probabilities, the chance level of IM is below 0, namely -0.22. To have all measures on the same scale, we adjusted IM such that it denotes the deviation from its chance level (see Figure 3 ). Category memory only (CMO): This performance measure captures the ability to successfully remember the general category of presented objects but a failure to distinguish individual exemplars, corrected for the general response bias. These performance measures emphasize different degrees of memory specificity. For CMO, participants show only a sense of familiarity for the object categories, whereas high IM performance requires remembering specific object details in order to recognize the exact exemplar and to reject a similar lure. Lure discrimination index (LDI): One component of item memory is lure discrimination. The ability to discriminate lures has been assessed separately in a number of studies and associated with hippocampal pattern separation/completion and age differences therein (e.g., Keresztes et al., 2017; Morcom, 2015; Ngo et al., 2017; Stark et al., 2013; Toner et al., 2009; Yassa et al., 2011) . The measure reported here computes the difference between correct lure detection and the susceptibility to mistake lures as old (cf. Ngo et al., 2017; Toner et al., 2009) . between identifying lures as similar and mistaking them as old is the lure discrimination index (LDI; yellow). The measures of IM and CMO are furthermore corrected for the tendency to respond "old" or "similar" (not depicted). EEG was recorded during the object encoding and recognition tasks. In adults, EEG was recorded continuously with BrainVision amplifiers (BrainVision Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) from 61 Ag/AgCl electrodes embedded in an elastic cap (EASYCAP GmbH, Herrsching, Germany). Two additional electrodes were placed at the outer canthi and one below the left eye to monitor eye movements (horizontal and vertical EOG). During recording, all electrodes were referenced to the right mastoid electrode, and the left mastoid electrode was recorded as an additional channel. Electrode AFz served as ground. The EEG was recorded with a pass-band of 0.1 to 250 Hz and digitized with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. During preparation, electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. In children, EEG was recorded continuously with BrainVision amplifiers (BrainVision Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) from 64 active Ag/AgCl electrodes in an ActiCAP cap (EASYCAP GmbH, Herrsching, Germany). Electrode FCz served as reference and AFz as ground. The sampling rate was 1000 Hz and electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. For all age groups, the same EEG data preprocessing pipeline was used, except that adults' EEG and eye-tracking data were first integrated using the Eye-EEG toolbox (Dimigen et al., 2011; RRID: SCR 012903 Hz; fourth order Butterworth). Data were visually screened for excessive muscle artifacts, followed by an independent component analysis (ICA) to identify components related to eye, muscle, and cardiac artifacts (Jung et al., 2000) . Microsaccadic artifacts were detected by using an algorithm for the correction of saccade-related transient spike potentials (COSTRAP; Hassler et al., 2011) . Artifact components were automatically detected, visually checked, and removed from the data. Following the FASTER procedure (Nolan et al., 2010) , automatic artifact correction was then performed for any residual artifacts. Channels excluded prior to ICA were interpolated after artifact correction with spherical splines (Perrin et al., 1989) . Finally, 10% of the remaining trials were again visually screened to determine successful cleansing. For further analysis, data epochs of 5.5 s were extracted from -2 s to 3.5 s with respect to the onset of the object presentation. The current paper only covers analysis of the EEG recorded during encoding. Furthermore, since there were no interactions between age and item manipulations during encoding, we largely disregard the encoding conditions and do not separate the trials accordingly in the following analyses (see Supplements). We computed ERPs by averaging the time-locked EEG data over the respective trials, separately for each electrode (see Supplementary Figure S1 for ERPs at selected electrodes). For ERP group comparison, we applied an absolute pre-stimulus baseline correction (200 ms before onset). To examine repetition effects, we calculated ERPs separately for the first and second presentation of the object items. For each age group, we tested whether participants performed better than chance in the recognition task by contrasting the proportion of correct responses to chance level (0.33) (one-sample t-tests). We tested for age differences in all reported behavioral measures by conducting one-way ANOVAs contrasting children, young adults, and older adults, followed by post-hoc t-tests. On the first (within-subject) level, we compared the ERPs of every encoding trial in which an object item was presented for the first time with the ERPs of the respective second presentation using two-sided paired sample t-tests. On the second (between-subject) level, the individual t-maps of the first-level analysis were contrasted against zero using two-sided independent samples t-tests. We controlled for multiple comparison by conducting non-parametric, cluster-based, random permutation tests (Fieldtrip toolbox; RRID: SCR 004849; Maris and Oostenveld, 2007; Oostenveld et al., 2011 ; see also Fields and Kuperberg, 2019) . All scalp electrodes (60 for adults, 64 for children) and the trial time points from -0.1 to 1.6 s relative to stimulus onset were included in the analysis. Clusters were formed by grouping neighboring channel × time samples with a p-value below 0.05 (spatially and temporally). The respective test statistic was then determined as the sum of all t-values within a cluster. The Monte Carlo method was used to compute the reference distribution for the summed cluster-level t-values. Samples were repeatedly (10,000 times) assigned into two classes and the differences between these random classes were contrasted with the differences between the actual two conditions (e.g., first and second presentation). For every permutation the t-statistic was computed, and the t-values summed for each cluster. This was done separately for each age group to reveal the group-specific repetition effects. On the third (between-group) level, we tested whether the identified effects differed between age groups. For this, we extracted and averaged the individual t-values from the first-level analysis within the time-electrode clusters identified in the second-level analysis. For all group comparisons, t-values were transformed to z-values. For each repetition effect, these values were entered into a one-way ANOVA contrasting children, young adults, and older adults, or underwent a two-sample t-test if only two groups were compared. We tested whether the identified clusters may be different reflections of the same repetition suppression/enhancement effect by correlating the individual effect sizes, i.e., the extracted mean t-values, across subjects (Pearson correlation). If clusters correlated highly (r > 0.7), we pooled them together as one composite effect by averaging across the individual mean t-values. We tested for a relationship between ERP repetition effects (first versus second presentation) and memory performance by modelling their correlations using maximum likelihood estimations (in Ωnyx 1.0-1010; von Oertzen et al., 2015) . Specifically, the correlations of the individual mean z-transformed t-values extracted from the identified time-electrode clusters (see above) and the individual measures of memory performance were modelled (a) separately for each age group, and (b) across all age groups. Since we observed overall age differences in both effect sizes and memory performance, we standardized both measures within groups in order to test for the brain-behavior association independent of overall age differences. We tested whether the models (a and b) showed significantly different model fits using likelihood-ratio (chi-squared) tests. Significant differences between the model fits (H1) would show that the model allowing for group-specific correlations (a) explains the data better, indicating that the correlations differed between groups, which would then require follow-up analyses of the pairwise differences. In turn, if the model fits did not differ significantly (H0), this would indicate that the brain-behavior relationship was not different between groups. During encoding, the participants' task was to detect when the fixation cross changed its color. We expected close-to-ceiling performance when participants attended to the stimuli and therefore excluded everyone who detected less than 80% of the respective trials. One child (0.67 hit rate) and two older adults (0.72 and 0.50 hit rate, respectively) were excluded based on this criterion. The remaining participants had a mean (±SD) hit rate of 0.98 (0.03) (children), 0.99 (0.02) (young adults), and 0.96 (0.04) (older adults). In the recognition task, participants were asked to decide for each presented object whether it was "old" Age differences in memory specificity. Highly specific item memory (IM; Eq. (1); Figure 3A ) showed significant age differences (F(2,112) = 7.76, p < 0.001; one-way ANOVA). Post-hoc t-tests revealed higher performance of children compared with young adults (t(58) = 3.27, p = 0.002) as well as older adults (t(74) = 3.81, p < 0.001), but no differences between the adult groups (t(92) = 0.78, p = 0.435). In contrast, correct category in combination with incorrect item memory (CMO; Eq. (2); Figure 3B ), showed no age differences (F(2,112) = 1.21, p = 0.301). That is, the age groups differed in their specific exemplar memory but not mere category memory. One aspect of IM is lure discrimination (LDI; e.g., Ngo et al., 2017; Toner et al., 2009 ; Figure 3C ). A high LDI is achieved by a large proportion of trials in which lures could be identified as similar and a low proportion of trials in which lures were mistaken as old, indicating pattern separation. A lower score, in contrast, indicates greater generalization or a bias towards pattern completion (cf. Keresztes et al., 2017) . A one-way ANOVA contrasting all groups revealed significant age differences (F(2,112) = 19.86, p < 0.001). Post-hoc t-tests showed no significant differences between children and young adults (t(58) = 0.82, p = 0.414) whereas both children (t(74) = 4.76, p < 0.001) and young adults (t(92) = 5.49, p < 0.001) showed a higher LDI than older adults did. Thus, whereas all groups generally exhibited a bias towards pattern separation (i.e., positive values), older adults were driven more towards pattern completion than children and young adults. To control for the different task versions (see Experimental design), 23 additional young adults performed the shorter children's task. In the shorter task, children and young adults did not differ in either IM (t(42) = -1.12, p = 0.267), CMO (t(42) = -1.15, p = 0.257), or LDI (t(42) = -1.58, p = 0.121), whereas young adults with the shorter task performed better than young adults with the longer task (IM: t(60) = 3.94, p < 0.001; CMO: t(60) = 2.96, p = 0.004; LDI: t(60) = 3.14, p = 0.003), indicating that children's high performance was largely due to lower task difficulty. As there were no interactions between age and item manipulations, i.e., the number of repetitions and exemplars (see Supplements), we largely disregard the encoding conditions and do not separate the trials accordingly in the following analyses. Using paired sample t-tests (first level) and non-parametric cluster-based permutation analysis (second level), we derived group-specific time-electrode clusters that indicate the trial time points and electrodes in which first and second object presentations showed reliable differences. We considered clusters whose test statistic exceeded the 97.5th percentile for its respective reference distribution as showing reliable repetition effects. With this procedure, we identified two clusters in children, three in younger adults, and four in older adults (all clusters p ≤ 0.002) with broadly overlapping topography and latency (see Figure 4 for details). For all age groups, there was a cluster over posterior electrodes showing lower positive amplitudes for the second versus first stimulus presentation (i.e., repetition suppression; RS1; Figure 4A ). This cluster appeared earliest in children (380-868 ms after stimulus onset), slightly later and with shorter duration in young adults (492-840 ms), and latest and with the shortest duration in older adults (568-884 ms). Within similar time windows another repetition suppression cluster (RS2) was identified at frontal and central electrodes which showed reduced negativity for repeated stimuli ( Figure 4B ). This cluster also appeared earliest in children (192-864 ms) , followed by young adults (224-756 ms), and latest, as well as with much shorter duration in older adults (676-848 ms). For all age groups, both of these suppression effects occurred mainly while the ERP deflections came back to baseline, starting right at or after the peak in children and starting only shortly before returning to baseline in older adults. As the effects appeared at similar times but at different locations, and with opposite polarity, they could be two reflections of the same repetition suppression effect (perhaps resulting from the two sides of the dipole). To test this, we assessed whether the magnitudes of the effects correlated across subjects (see below). Furthermore, we identified a repetition enhancement effect (RE1) for both young and older adults that showed stronger (more negative) activity for the second than for the first object presentations over mainly frontal and temporal electrode sites (young adults: 244-368 ms; older adults: 308-548 ms; Figure 4C ). An opposite enhancement effect (RE2) was only identified for older adults over centro-parietal regions at 308-640 ms after stimulus onset ( Figure 4D ). In analogy to the two suppression effects, the opposite enhancement clusters identified in older adults could be two reflections of the same effect, which would be indicated by a high correlation between the effect sizes. To test for correlations between the repetition effects, we extracted the individual t-values (first-level analysis) from the identified time-electrode clusters (second-level analysis) and averaged them across the respective time points and channels for each subject. These t-values were then correlated across subjects. High correlations would indicate that participants who showed large effect sizes in, for example, RS1, also showed a large RS2 effect, indicating that they are not two independent effects potentially serving different functions. Indeed, for all age groups as well as across groups, the magnitudes of the RS1 and RS2 effects correlated strongly (children: r = 0.86, p < 0.001; young adults: r = 0.72, p < 0.001; older adults: r = 0.81, p < 0.001; across groups: r = 0.81, p < 0.001; Pearson correlation). Likewise, the two enhancement effects of the older adults were highly correlated (r = 0.72, p < 0.001). Hence, we pooled the repetition effects into composite RS and RE effects by averaging the respective effect sizes for each subject. RS and RE effects did not correlate, potentially indicating two functionally different effects. Repetition effects were identified for all age groups (see Figure 4 ). In a next step, we examined whether the size of the effects differed between age groups (see Figure 5 left). Because of overall age differences in EEG amplitudes (see Figure 5 ), rather than computing and comparing the raw amplitude difference between the ERPs to first and second presentations, it is more appropriate to compare the effect sizes, i.e., the We examined the relationship between the individual mean effect sizes of the composite repetition effects (see above) and the individual performance measures (see Behavioral analysis) by modelling their correlations using maximum likelihood estimates, (a) separately for each age group, and (b) across all groups. Because there were overall age differences in both ERP effects and memory performance (see above), we standardized both measures within groups to test for the brain-behavior association independent of age differences. The resulting estimated correlation coefficients are presented in Table 1 . Across groups, the composite RS effect correlated significantly with IM (see Figure 5A right) in the direction that larger RS were associated with better memory performance. The RE effect that only occurred in adults also showed a positive association to IM across young and older adults ( Figure 5B right). In contrast, CMO and LDI did not show significant brain-behavior correlations across groups (see Table 1 ). We tested whether the models (a and b) differed in their model fits using likelihood ratio (LR) tests. For all repetition effects (RS, RE) and performance measures (IM, CMO, LDI), the model fits were not significantly different from each other (see Table 1 ). That is, the model that allowed age group-specific brain-behavior relations did not explain the data significantly better than the model with fixed correlations across groups. This indicates that the brain-behavior associations did not differ across the age groups. Therefore, in the following we do not interpret the group-specific but only the cross-group brain-behavior correlations. Table 1 . memory performance measures (IM, CMO, LDI; standardized) for all age groups separately as well as across groups (All). Correlations for item memory (IM) are also plotted in Figure 5 . Likelihood ratio (LR) tests were used to compare the models of group-specific versus group-independent brain-behavior correlations. Significant LR would indicate that correlations differed between age groups. The present study investigated whether processes of memory formation, as reflected in neural repetition effects, are associated with inter-individual differences in item and category memory. Specifically, we were interested whether age group differences in memory performance would show as age-differential associations between repetition effects and memory specificity in a lifespan sample. For this, we compared repetition-related differences in ERPs during incidental object encoding in groups of 7-9-year-old children, 18-30-year-old young adults, and 65-76-year-old older adults in relation to their later recognition performance. We distinguished between memory for specific items and for the general stimulus category only. We identified reliable ERP repetition suppression effects for all age groups and repetition enhancement among adults. Across groups, item-specific memory performance was positively associated with the magnitude of repetition suppression and enhancement. In sum, we demonstrate common neural mechanisms of memory encoding across the lifespan. Overall, we found little evidence for age differences in memory performance. While children showed better item-specific memory compared with adults who performed a longer task, there were no performance differences between children and a control sample of young adults who also performed the children's task version. With regard to simple recognition, this is in accordance with previous findings also showing no age differences in object recognition between children, adolescents, and young adults (Golarai et al., 2007) . However, with regard to memory specificity, some previous work suggested that pre-school children are less susceptible for false memories than adults (Brainerd et al., 2002 (Brainerd et al., , 2008 , while other studies showed higher false alarm rates for foils and, in particular, similar lures, in children below the age of 6 compared to older children and adults . In line with the finding of improving lure discrimination abilities across childhood and adolescence (Keresztes et al., 2017; Ngo et al., 2018; Rollins and Cloude, 2018) , our current findings substantiate the evidence that older children (here, 7-9-year-olds) have already reached adult-like item-specific memory and lure discrimination abilities, which indicates advanced hippocampal maturation (Keresztes et al., 2017 (Keresztes et al., , 2018 . At the other end of the lifespan, also younger and older adults did not differ with respect to recognition memory, which is in accordance with previous work showing much less pronounced or no age differences for item compared to associative memory, especially under incidental encoding conditions (Old and Naveh-Benjamin, 2008) . However, one might have expected that older adults would tend to generalize more and thus show worse item-specific memory than young adults, perhaps accompanied by greater reliance on category memory Tun et al., 1998) . Although this was not the case in these aggregated performance assessments, evidence in this direction is nevertheless provided by age-related differences in lure discrimination. The ability to recognize lures as "similar" and not "old" is only achievable if the original targets have been encoded with high specificity. Here, young adults outperformed older adults, which corroborates previous findings Stark and Stark, 2017; Stark et al., 2013 Stark et al., , 2015 and has been shown to be related to age-associated disruptions of the intra-hippocampal circuitry (Shing et al., 2011; Wilson et al., 2006; Yassa et al., 2011) . All in all, the age pattern shows that 7-9-year-old children already show adult-like levels of itemspecific recognition and lure discrimination. Further, aggregated performance measures did not show differences between young and older adults while individual elements of the measures (e.g., lure discrimination) did, indicating aging-related behavioral disparity in specific memory aspects. With regard to our ERP results, we showed that all age groups exhibited repetition-related changes in the neural responses during encoding, suggesting common neural mechanisms of memory formation overall. We identified repetition suppression (for all age groups) and repetition enhancement (only for adults) with overlapping topographies and time windows across age groups but age-related differences with regard to the magnitudes of the effects. The topography and timing of the observed repetition effects are in line with the effects described in the ERP literature that have been observed in adult samples (cf. Henson et al., 2004; Lawson et al., 2007; Penney et al., 2001; Rugg and Doyle, 1994; Stefanics et al., 2018) . The repetition suppression effects may be linked to modulations in the P600 ERP component, which is sensitive to word frequency and familiarity (Rugg and Doyle, 1994; Rugg 1990) , and in FN400, which is also associated with familiarity (Curran, 2004; Friedman and Johnson, 2000; Mecklinger, 2000) . Thus, modulations in these components may suggest differences in stimulus familiarity that are certainly to be expected when items are repeated. Since we observed these modulations in all age groups, item familiarity during the second item presentation may be related to suppressed neural activity for children, young, and older adults alike, which is in line with their comparable item recognition abilities. The repetition enhancement effect that we observed only for adults partially resembles the original ERP repetition effect described in early studies (Rugg and Doyle, 1994) . It has been argued that this reflects modulations in several ERP components, mainly a decrease in N400, which is related to priming and contextual integration, and an increase in P300, which is related to working memory and stimulus categorization (Friedman, 2000; Lawson et al., 2007; Rugg and Doyle, 1994; Van Petten and Senkfor, 1996) . The observation that the ERPs of repeated stimuli show changes in these components may indicate successful integration of the items into internal object representations. The finding that children did not show such an effect, while young and older adults did not differ in their enhancement effects, may suggest that the neural mechanisms of forming representations was not yet fully mature in children, but still intact in older adults. This would be largely consistent with previous studies showing developmental differences in context integration and binding in general (Káldy and Kovács, 2003; Raj and Bell, 2010) and also in relation to working memory (Fandakova et al., 2014) . However, this contrasts with the suggestion by Henson et al. (2000) that repetition enhancement may reflect the formation of new representations of previously unfamiliar stimuli, whereas suppression may reflect the strengthening of already existing representations (see also Grossmann et al., 2009 ). According to this interpretation, children may have had less experience with the presented objects (cf. Brod et al., 2013) and therefore should show more enhancement than adults, who presumably had previously well-established representations of all the objects. The absence of repetition enhancement in children may also be due to deficient statistical power as the children's group was smaller and they had fewer trials than the adult groups. Since other studies on repetition effects in children of this age are lacking, we can only speculate on whether children may indeed exhibit no enhancement. If this is the case, it may indicate that the underlying neural mechanisms related to context integration and working memory are not yet fully developed, which, however, did not appear to have impaired children's memory performance. While similar patterns of suppression effects occurred in all three age groups, we observed differences in the magnitudes of the effects between groups. Comparing the composite suppression effect between age groups yielded the largest effect sizes for the children's group. Young and older adults differed in neither the magnitudes of suppression nor enhancement, which is generally in line with previous studies (e.g., Hamberger and Friedman, 1992 ; but see Friedman et al., 1993) . On this between-group level, the cooccurrence of children's strong memory performance and larger repetition effects while the adult groups differed in neither, may suggest that memory success is associated to strong repetition suppression, in line with the hypothesis that repetition effects reflect successful memory formation. To expand on this proposition, especially with regard to the degree of memory specificity, we further examined the relation of individual differences in repetition effects and memory on the item-and category-level both within and across age groups. The central finding of the present study is the observed positive relationship of the extent to which neural activity is altered in response to repeated input and individual explicit memory performance. In line with the hypothesis that these repetition-related activation differences reflect the formation of memory representations, across groups, individuals exhibiting larger repetition effects during encoding showed better item recognition memory than individuals with smaller repetition effects did. Importantly, this was not due to overall age group differences, such as children showing both larger effects and better performance (see above), but the association remained when overall age differences were eliminated by standardizing the measures within groups. Furthermore, the estimated brain-behavior associations did not significantly differ between the groups, suggesting largely common neural correlates of successful memory encoding across the lifespan. The finding that the repetition effects were linked to specific item memory and not to mere category memory furthermore suggests that repetition-sensitive neural responses reflect highly detailed memory encoding. Moreover, repetition effects were not related to mere lure discrimination but specific item memory, including correct target recognition, lure detection, and foil rejection, indicating that the LDI alone may provide an incomplete picture with regard to the abilities reflecting highly specific memory representations. All in all, we show that ERP repetition effects provide an age-independent indicator of individual differences in successful encoding of item-specific details. To conclude, we found that memory formation processes that manifest in repetition-sensitive ERP components are related to individual differences in item recognition performance across the lifespan. The effects did not differ between age groups, indicating age-independent reflections of memory encoding that have not been shown before. These novel findings highlight the significance of encoding mechanisms that facilitate the formation of high-quality memory representations and enable highly specific recognition and item distinction. In the following, we asked whether item and category repetitions might affect memory performance differentially in children, younger adults, and older adults. We initially expected better item-specific memory performance when items were repeated more often and worse performance when more exemplars were presented during encoding. This was based on the hypothesis that more repetitions would provide more opportunity to encode item-specific details leading to both better target recognition as well as rejection of similar lures (Benjamin, 2001 ; but see Reagh and Yassa, 2014) , whereas more exemplars would create stronger interference, making it harder to distinguish between similar items (Anderson, 2015) . The latter is supported by previous findings showing that increasing the number of exemplars from the same object category, reduced participants' ability to discriminate between lures and targets from that category (Gallo, 2004; Konkle et al., 2010; Omohundro, 1981; Poch et al., 2019) . Alternatively, interference from similar memories can also trigger a repulsion of memory representations, suggested by the findings of less overlapping activation patterns in the hippocampus (pattern separation), which was associated with less memory interference, i.e., better performance (Chanales et al., 2017 (Chanales et al., , 2020 Favila et al., 2016) . That is, both scenarios -better or worse item memory when more exemplars were presented -are conceivable. To examine these predictions, we conducted a three-way mixed ANOVA with age group as the betweensubjects factor, number of repetitions and number of exemplars as within-subjects factors, and item memory performance as the dependent variable. The results revealed a main effect of age (F(2,109) = 7.48, p < 0.001) with the same pattern of age differences as reported above, and a main effect of the number of exemplars (F(1,109) = 8.53, p = 0.004). Post-hoc t-tests demonstrated better item memory when four rather than two exemplars were presented (t(111) = 3.58, p < 0.001), revealing a beneficial effect of a higher number of category exemplars. This is in line with the repulsion prediction (see above), whereas the number of repetitions did not influence item memory (F(1,109) = 0.99, p = 0.321). To better disentangle the effect of repetitions and exemplars for item-specific memory, we further examined the potential effect of repetitions by directly contrasting memory performance for items from the HR and BL condition, i.e., independently of the exemplar manipulation. Across age groups, this revealed better item memory for the HR condition (t(111) = 2.72, p = 0.008; paired-sample t-test). Since there were no interactions between age and item manipulations, we largely disregard the encoding conditions and do not separate the trials accordingly in the analyses presented here. The ERPs of children, young adults, and older adults at selected representative electrode sites are shown in Figure S1 . Children exhibited overall higher amplitudes than adults, which is consistent with previous findings in the literature (Coch et al., 2005; Dustman and Beck, 1969; Mueller et al., 2008) . This could, for example, be due to differences in skull thickness (Frodl et al., 2001 ) but in the current study it may also be a result of the different EEG systems and laboratories used to test the children and adults. Raincloud plots: a multiplatform tool for robust data visualization Incidental forgetting Repetition suppression: a means to index neural representations using BOLD? Unmasking Latent Inhibitory Connections in Human Cortex to Reveal Dormant Cortical Memories On the dual effects of repetition on false recognition Sensory representations supporting memory specificity: Age effects on behavioral and neural discriminability Are young children susceptible to the falsememory illusion? Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory The influence of prior knowledge on memory: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective Age differences in neural distinctiveness revealed by multi-voxel pattern analysis Overlap among spatial memories triggers repulsion of hippocampal representations Adaptive repulsion of long-term memory representations is triggered by event similarity Auditory and visual refractory period effects in children and adults: an ERP study Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention Coregistration of eye movements and EEG in natural reading: analyses and review Visual perceptual learning in human object recognition areas: A repetition priming study using highdensity electrical mapping The effects of maturation and aging on the wave form of visually evoked potentials Repetition suppression and memory for faces is reduced in adults with autism spectrum conditions Age differences in shortterm memory binding are related to working memory performance across the lifespan Age differences in false memory: The importance of retrieval monitoring processes and their modulation by memory quality Neural specificity of scene representations is related to memory performance in childhood Experience-dependent hippocampal pattern differentiation prevents interference during subsequent learning Having your cake and eating it too: Flexibility and power with mass univariate statistics for ERP data Mini-mental state": A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician Event-related brain potential investigations of memory and aging Event-related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review Event-related potentials as indicators of repetition priming in young and older adults: Amplitude, duration, and scalp distribution The effect of the skull on event-related P300 Using recall to reduce false recognition: Diagnostic and disqualifying monitoring Sub-exemplar shape tuning in human face-related areas Reduced neural selectivity increases fMRI adaptation with age during face discrimination Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with categoryspecific recognition memory Repetition priming and repetition suppression: A case for enhanced efficiency through neural synchronization Lifespan development of human memory Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects The neural basis of perceptual category learning in human infants Event-related potential correlates of rpetition priming and stimulus classification in young, middle-aged, and older Adults Induced gamma band responses in human EEG after the control of miniature saccadic artifacts Neuroimaging studies of priming The effect of repetition lag on electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of visual object priming Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming Memory Change in the Aged Evaluation of a shape-based model of human face discrimination using fMRI and behavioral techniques Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation Visual context integration is not fully developed in 4-year-old children Hippocampal maturity promotes memory distinctiveness in childhood and adolescence Hippocampal maturation drives memory from generalization to specificity Tracking age differences in neural distinctiveness across representational levels The relationship between age, neural differentiation, and memory performance Conceptual distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real-world objects The hippocampus and neocortical inhibitory engrams protect against memory interference Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults Age effects on brain activity during repetition priming of targets and distracters Integrative neurocomputational perspectives on cognitive aging, neuromodulation, and representation Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span Inferior temporal mechanisms for invariant object recognition Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information Interfacing mind and brain: A neurocognitive model of recognition memory Resisting false recognition: An ERP study of lure discrimination Electrophysiological correlates of selective attention: A lifespan comparison Modulation of event-related potentials by word repetition: The effects of inter-item lag Development of neural sensitivity to face identity correlates with perceptual discriminability The ontogeny of relational memory and pattern separation FASTER: Fully automated statistical thresholding for EEG artifact rejection The use of repetition suppression paradigms in developmental cognitive neuroscience Structural equation modeling with Ωnyx From perception to memory: Changes in memory systems across the lifespan Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis Recognition vs. classification of ill-defined category exemplars FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data Observing the transformation of experience into memory Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral visual cortex An fMRI study of episodic encoding across the lifespan: Changes in subsequent memory effects are evident by middleage Neural specificity predicts fluid processing ability in older adults Repetition related ERP effects in a visual object target detection task Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping Failure of repetition suppression and memory encoding in aging and Alzheimer's disease The impact of increasing similar interfering experiences on mnemonic discrimination: Electrophysiological evidence Cognitive processes supporting episodic memory formation in childhood: The role of source memory, binding, and executive functioning Repetition strengthens target recognition but impairs similar lure discrimination: evidence for trace competition Details, gist and schema: hippocampal-neocortical interactions underlying recent and remote episodic and spatial memory Development of mnemonic discrimination during childhood The recall of repeated and unrepeated words: An ERP analysis Event-related potentials and stimulus repetition in direct and indirect tests of memory Modulation of event-related potentials by the repetition of drawings of novel objects Oscillatory mechanisms of successful memory formation in younger and older adults are related to structural integrity False memories and aging Specificity of priming: a cognitive neuroscience perspective The suppression of repetition enhancement: a review of fMRI studies Episodic memory across the lifespan: the contributions of associative and strategic components Hippocampal subfield volumes: Age, vascular risk, and correlation with associative memory Induction and categorization in young children: A similaritybased model Age-related deficits in the mnemonic similarity task for objects and scenes A task to assess behavioral pattern separation (BPS) in humans: Data from healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment Stability of age-related deficits in the mnemonic similarity task across task variations Mnemonic similarity task: A tool for assessing hippocampal integrity Timing of repetition suppression of event-related potentials to unattended objects Visual object pattern separation deficits in nondemented older adults Response latencies for false memories: gist-based processes in normal aging Linking implicit and explicit memory: Common encoding factors and shared representations Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis Memory for words and novel visual patterns: repetition, recognition, and encoding effects in the event-related brain potential Interactions between forms of memory: When priming hinders new episodic learning Repetition suppression and multi-voxel pattern similarity differentially track implicit and explicit visual memory Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming Neurocognitive aging: prior memories hinder new hippocampal encoding Memory for context becomes less specific with time A neural circuit for memory specificity and generalization Spaced learning enhances subsequent recognition memory by reducing neural repetition suppression Age-related memory deficits linked to circuit-specific disruptions in the hippocampus The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research