key: cord-0253062-kzn2du87 authors: Bachman, Shelby L.; Nashiro, Kaoru; Yoo, Hyunjoo; Wang, Diana; Thayer, Julian F.; Mather, Mara title: Associations between locus coeruleus MRI contrast and physiological responses to acute stress in younger and older adults date: 2022-03-14 journal: bioRxiv DOI: 10.1101/2022.03.12.484104 sha: b2aef46d8314fc56f1ce32d55a7552fba9d5116d doc_id: 253062 cord_uid: kzn2du87 Acute stress robustly activates the brain’s noradrenergic system, the hub of which is the locus coeruleus (LC). Recent studies have indicated that structural integrity of the LC, assessed through magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), is associated with better cognitive outcomes in later life. However, no studies have examined whether MRI-assessed LC integrity is related to arousal responses to acute stress in either younger or older adults, despite the LC’s documented role in promoting physiological arousal as part of the acute stress response. In this study, 102 younger and 51 older adults completed an acute stress induction task while we assessed multiple measures of physiological arousal (heart rate, breathing rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, sympathetic tone, and heart rate variability, HRV). We used turbo spin echo MRI scans to quantify LC MRI contrast as a measure of LC integrity. Using univariate and multivariate approaches, we assessed how LC MRI contrast was associated with arousal at rest and during acute stress reactivity and recovery. We found that in older adults, having higher caudal LC MRI contrast was associated with greater stress-related increases in systolic blood pressure and decreases in HRV, as well as lower HRV during recovery from acute stress. These results suggest that having higher caudal LC integrity in older adulthood is associated with more pronounced physiological responses to acute stress. Further work is needed to confirm these patterns in larger samples of older adults. Acute stress is a recurring feature of daily life, occurring in response to various psychosocial and environmental stressors. The nervous system's highly-conserved response to acute stress is designed to promote behaviors and processes that facilitate survival in the face of such stressors (Johnson et al., 1992; Monaghan & Spencer, 2014) . At the same time, there is substantial variability in how individuals respond to stress (Rab & Admon, 2021; Sapolsky, 2015; Zänkert et al., 2019) , with exaggerated or prolonged stress responses associated with adverse health outcomes. Individuals with excessive responses to and impaired recovery from acute stress are at elevated risk for atherosclerosis, hypertension, myocardial infarction, and cardiovascular disease mortality (Chida & Steptoe, 2010; Panaite et al., 2015; Treiber et al., 2003) . Furthermore, stress is a documented contributor to dementia risk (Justice, 2018; Lyons & Bartolomucci, 2020; Yuede et al., 2018) , which may in part be mediated by noradrenergic modulation of β-amyloid and tau production and clearance (Mather, 2021) . Thus, characterizing factors that may protect against stress vulnerability across the adult lifespan is an important aim of psychophysiological research. Acute stress responses engage both the hypothalamic-pituitary-axis and the brain's noradrenergic system, the hub of which is the locus coeruleus. The locus coeruleus (LC) is a nucleus within the pons of the brainstem that releases norepinephrine throughout the brain and spinal cord (Dahlström & Fuxe, 1964; Schwarz & Luo, 2015) . Noradrenergic projections from the LC reach cortical regions implicated in attention, learning and memory (Sara, 2009 ), but the LC also sends projections to preganglionic sympathetic neurons in the spinal cord, which coordinate peripheral arousal responses (Jones & Yang, 1985) . Besides releasing norepinephrine to the brain and spinal cord, the LC is innervated by brain regions including the central nucleus of the amygdala, the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus, and the nucleus paragigantocellularis (Aston- Jones et al., 1986; Curtis et al., 2002; Mather, 2020; Samuels & Szabadi, 2008; Van Bockstaele & Aston-Jones, 1995) . These inputs provide visceral feedback signals which the LC integrates to adaptively regulate norepinephrine release and, in turn, arousal levels (Morris et al., 2020a) . As an arousal hub region within the nervous system, the LC is robustly activated in response to diverse stressors (Morilak, 2007; Valentino & Van Bockstaele, 2008) . During acute stress, in tandem with hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activation, corticotropin-releasing factor is released on the LC by the paraventricular nucleus, the central nucleus of the amygdala, and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Johnson et al., 1992; Valentino & Van Bockstaele, 2005) . Corticotropin-releasing factor increases the rate of tonic, or basal, norepinephrine discharge by LC neurons while decreasing the frequency of phasic, stimulus-evoked responses (Curtis et al., 1997; Valentino & Foote, 1988) . A shift to higher tonic LC activity increases cortical levels of norepinephrine (Kawahara et al., 2000) and promotes adaptive behavioral and physiological shifts that subserve threat detection and avoidance, such as the reorienting of attention and cardiovascular reactivity (Bremner et al., 1996; Sara & Bouret, 2012; Wood & Valentino, 2017) . The LC's short-term response to acute stress is adaptive, promoting behaviors that complete the stress cycle. Yet stress experienced over the longer term may have maladaptive consequences for the LC. Corticotropin-releasing factor exposure due to chronic stress causes morphological changes to LC neurons, increasing both dendritic arborization and the number of primary processes (Borodovitsyna et al., 2018) . Stress also affects the activity of LC neurons, with LC neurons from rodents exposed to chronic stress exhibiting higher excitability and sensitivity relative to those from controls, as well as anxiety-like behaviors (Jedema & Grace, 2003; Mana & Grace, 1997; McCall et al., 2015) . Together, these findings suggest that structure of the LC may be closely intertwined with physiological responses to acute stress. Despite the LC's involvement in the stress response, little is known about how LC structural integrity is related to physiological responses in humans. This may be due in part to limitations of studying the LC in vivo in humans due to the LC's small size and location. Recently, the development of specialized MRI protocols, including high-resolution turbo spin echo (TSE) and magnetization transfer sequences (Betts et al., 2019b; Sasaki et al., 2006) , has made quantifying LC structure in vivo possible. In these sequences, the LC appears as hyperintense regions bordering the fourth ventricle, with signal intensity of the LC relative to that of surrounding pontine tissue thought to reflect LC structural integrity (Keren et al., 2009) . A recent study using such a protocol found that LC volume was higher in younger adults with anxiety disorders relative to healthy controls, and that, across the sample of younger adults, LC volume was positively correlated with levels of anxious arousal and general distress (Morris et al., 2020b) . In separate studies, younger and older adults with higher LC integrity had lower heart rate variability (HRV), a measure of parasympathetic control over heart rate, during a fear conditioning task (Mather et al., 2017) , and younger adults with higher integrity of the LC's caudal region had lower average cortical thickness . Both HRV and cortical thickness are lower in individuals with stress-and anxiety-related disorders relative to healthy controls (Chalmers et al., 2014; Molent et al., 2018) , and dysregulation of noradrenergic signaling is feature of such disorders (Hendrickson & Raskind, 2016; Ressler & Nemeroff, 2000) . Together, this evidence suggests that in younger adults, structural integrity of the LC may be associated with poorer stress-and anxiety-related outcomes. Yet there has been little work directly examining associations between LC integrity and comprehensive measures of physiological arousal during acute stress, despite the LC's projections to and innervation from sympathetic and parasympathetic arousal centers. In contrast to the reports described above, studies of older adults have indicated that having higher MRI-assessed LC integrity is associated with better cognitive performance across domains (Dahl et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2020) , higher cortical thickness , and lower risk of developing mild cognitive impairment (Elman et al., 2021a) in older adulthood. The LC is the first brain region where tau pathology accumulates in the progression of Alzheimer's disease (Braak et al., 2011) , and older adults with Alzheimer's disease have lower LC integrity relative to healthy controls (Betts et al., 2019a; Takahashi et al., 2015) . On the surface, these seemingly discrepant findings -LC integrity being associated with better outcomes later in adulthood but poorer outcomes earlier in adulthood -suggest that LC structure may be more influenced by stress in younger adults and more by neurodegeneration in older adults. However, just as for younger adults, no studies to date have assessed whether LC structure in older adulthood is related to aspects of the parasympathetic and sympathetic response to stress. In the present study, we attempted to fill these gaps in the literature by examining how physiological responses to acute stress were related to LC MRI contrast in a sample of 102 younger and 51 older adults. We assessed multiple measures of physiological arousal during rest, acute stress, and acute stress recovery, and we used TSE MRI scans to assess LC contrast, an in vivo measure of LC structural integrity, along the LC's rostrocaudal extent. Pairwise correlation and partial least squares correlation analyses were applied to assess how LC contrast was associated with multiple measures of physiological arousal in each age group. In line with previous findings of LC volume being positively correlated with levels of anxious arousal in younger adults and LC contrast being associated with lower parasympathetic control over heart rate (Mather et al., 2017; Morris et al., 2020b) , we predicted that younger adults with higher LC contrast would have higher-magnitude responses to acute stress. In terms of predictions for older adults, in line with studies linking MRI-assessed LC integrity to better cognitive and neural outcomes in aging (Dahl et al., 2019; Elman et al., 2021a; Liu et al., 2020) , we originally expected that older adults with higher LC contrast would have lower-magnitude acute stress responses, reflecting reduced potential impacts of stress. However, another perspective is that acute stress reactivity in aging is beneficial, reflecting a responsive, flexible autonomic system in the context of normal sympathetic tone. From this perspective, a competing possibility was that older adults with higher LC contrast would have larger-magnitude acute stress responses. Data were collected as part of a clinical trial testing the effects of heart rate variability biofeedback training on emotion regulation brain networks (Nashiro et al., 2021) . For the present analyses, only data from the pre-intervention measurement timepoint -that is, before participants learned about or started the intervention -were considered. Specifically, we considered data from all participants who completed an MRI session including an TSE scan and an acute stress induction task at the pre-intervention timepoint. This included 115 younger and 59 older participants (data collection for the older cohort was terminated prematurely due to the COVID-19 pandemic). These participants were MRI-eligible individuals without major medical, neurological, psychiatric, or cardiac conditions. Individuals who engaged in regular relaxation, biofeedback or breathing techniques were excluded from participation, as were individuals taking psychoactive medications. Individuals taking antidepressants or anti-anxiety medications were eligible to participate so long as the medication had been taken for at least three months prior to study participation. Older adults who scored lower than 16 on the TELE, a brief cognitive assessment administered over the telephone (Gatz et al., 1995) , were excluded from participation for possible dementia. Of those who completed the acute stress induction task and an MRI session, scans from 18 participants were excluded from LC delineation due to severe motion artifact (n = 15), susceptibility artifact overlapping the LC or pons (n = 2), and incorrect scan resolution (n = 1). Following LC delineation, 1 older participant was excluded from analysis due to incorrect placement of the LC search space, and 2 participants (1 younger, 1 older) were missing complete physiological recordings from the stress task due to recording errors and were therefore excluded from analysis. The final sample for analysis included 153 participants (102 younger, 51 older). The study protocol was approved by the University of Southern California Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written, informed consent and received monetary compensation for their participation. Note. Age and education are presented in years. During their lab visits for the acute stress induction protocol and MRI session, participants completed the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) and Trait Anxiety Inventory (TAI; Spielberger et al., 1983) . A single score for each participant was calculated by averaging over their scores from both visits. Comparison p-values reflect results of independent samples Welch's t-tests and, for comparing the proportion of females in each age group, a chi-squared test. During the first week of the study, participants completed a computerized acute stress induction procedure based on a standardized protocol known to elicit a robust acute physiological stress response (Crowley et al., 2011) . The task consisted of a 4-minute baseline resting phase, a cognitive challenge phase, and a 4-minute recovery resting phase ( Figure 1 ). During the baseline phase, participants sat with their feet resting flat on the ground and their hands resting in a supine position on a flat tabletop. Participants viewed a black screen with a white fixation cross centered on the screen and were instructed to refrain from talking and to breathe normally during this phase. Following the baseline phase, participants completed a challenge phase which consisted of two cognitive tasks for younger adults and one task for older adults (piloting indicated that the first task was excessively frustrating for older participants). To increase the socially evaluative nature of the challenge phase, participants were told that their performance would be evaluated by the experimenter and compared with that of other participants. The first task, completed only by younger adults, was a computerized version of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT; Figure 1B ; Tombaugh, 2006) in which participants were presented with a series of digits and instructed to add each digit to the digit that came directly before it. Participants were instructed to enter the resulting sum on the keyboard using their dominant hand, and potential responses were never greater than 20. The task consisted of 30 trials in which participants had 3 seconds to respond to each digit; the task lasted approximately 160 seconds. The second task, completed by both younger and older adults, was a Stroop color-word matching task ( Figure 1C ; MacLeod, 1991) , in which a color word (`RED`, `BLUE`, or GREEN`) was presented on a computer screen in a color incongruent with its meaning (either red, green or blue). Participants were instructed to use their dominant hand to press a key corresponding to either the color in which the word was presented, or the meaning of the word, based on an instruction which appeared directly before the word. The Stroop task consisted of 20 trials and lasted approximately 120 seconds. During both tasks, auditory feedback was provided to participants on each trial: A bell sound was played in response to correct responses, whereas a buzzer sound was played after missing or incorrect responses to increase the socially-evaluative nature of the tasks (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004) . Before beginning the tasks, participants were provided with instructions and practice trials for both tasks. Following the cognitive challenge phase, participants completed a 4-minute recovery resting phase that was identical to the baseline resting phase. Physiological signals were recorded throughout the acute stress induction protocol at a sampling rate of 2KHz using a BIOPAC MP160 system (Goleta, CA). Electrocardiogram (ECG) signals were collected with a standard Lead II configuration with disposable, pre-gelled Ag/AgCl electrodes (EL501) and transmitted using a wireless BioNomadix transmitter system. Respiration was measured with the Biopac Respiratory Effort Transducer, which involved a belt being placed around the lower rib cage to measure changes in chest circumference, and signals were transmitted using the BioNomadix system. Continuous blood pressure was recorded on each participant's non-dominant arm using a BIOPAC noninvasive blood pressure monitoring system (NIBP100D). Approximately one week after completing the stress induction task, participants returned to the laboratory for an MRI session. Sequences of interest for the present analyses included a three-dimensional, T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo ( Hz/pixel, voxel size = 0.43 x 0.43 x 2.5 mm, gap between slices = 1.0mm, 11 axial slices. Physiological signals collected during the acute stress induction protocol were first split into segments for each participant, with segments corresponding to the various parts of the protocol: baseline, PASAT, Stroop task, and recovery phase. Quality control checks and preprocessing were performed for each segment separately, using the steps described below. For 5 older participants, we included only baseline segments for preprocessing and analysis, because these participants completed a pilot version of the protocol that included 2 cognitive challenge tasks. Processing steps described in this section were performed using MATLAB (Version R2021b). ECG signals. To remove baseline wander and high-frequency noise, ECG segments were filtered with a finite impulse response (FIR) bandpass filter with a passband between 0.5 and 40 Hz. ECG signals were assessed for quality in two steps. ECG segments were first visually inspected for signal quality and noise; 12.3% (n = 68) of all segments demonstrating excessive noise or abnormalities such that QRS-complexes were not discernible were excluded from analyses of heart rate and HRV. Second, during r-peak delineation and HRV analysis (see Section 2.3.2), an average signal quality index from 0-1 reflecting a comparison between r-peak annotations performed by two algorithms, jqrs and wqrs Johnson et al., 2014) , was calculated for each segment. We excluded an additional 5.0% (n = 24) of ECG segments for having an average signal quality index of below 0.7. Further segments were excluded for atrial fibrillation being detected (6.4%, n = 31) and having more than 20% of peaks missing (0.8%, n = 4). Respiration segments were resampled to 50 Hz and filtered with an FIR filter with a passband between 0.05 and 1 Hz. Preprocessed segments were then visually inspected for signal quality and were overlaid with detected peaks corresponding to inhalations, for visual inspection of peak detection accuracy. Respiration segments with poor quality and/or inaccurate peak detection in the majority of the segment were excluded from all analyses. This led to 6.4% (n = 35) of respiration segments being excluded from analysis of breathing rate. Continuous blood pressure signals. For removal of high-frequency noise from continuous arterial blood pressure segments, we applied a FIR lowpass filter with a cutoff frequency of 40 Hz. Raw segments were visually inspected for abnormalities; those in which regular systolic peaks were not detectable were excluded from analyses of systolic blood pressure. This led to 3.6% (n = 20) of segments being excluded from analysis. For another 37 segments, the blood pressure monitor re-calibrated mid-way through data collection; these segments were also excluded from analysis. Preprocessed physiological data segments were next used to compute segment-wise measures of physiological arousal. We first calculated average values for each arousal measure across the baseline, challenge and recovery phases for each participant, for the purposes of visualization and for testing whether the acute stress induction task effectively modulated arousal in each age group (see Section 2.5.2). Then, to quantify the greatest magnitude of physiological reactivity to the stressor for each measure, we performed an additional set of calculations. Specifically, we combined segments from PASAT and Stroop for younger adults into a single challenge segment (older adults' challenge phase segment reflected only the Stroop phase) and then computed the rolling average of each arousal measure across the challenge phase, with a window size of 20 seconds and step size of 1 second. We then calculated the maximum (or, for RMSSD, the minimum) of the rolling average for each measure. These peak (or minimum) values were used for subsequent calculations of stress reactivity as described in Section 2.5.3 . Calculation of arousal measures was performed using MATLAB (Version R2021b). Heart rate and heart rate variability. The PhysioNet Cardiovascular Signal Toolbox (Version 1.0.2, Vest et al., 2018) , an open-source toolbox designed to address issues of validation, standardization and reproducibility in HRV signal processing, was used for QRS-complex detection and to calculate time-and frequency-domain measures of HRV from preprocessed ECG signals. QRS detection was performed with the jqrs beat detector . Parameters for HRV calculation are described in the Supplementary Methods (Section 1). Based on delineated r-peaks, we calculated a value of mean heart rate for baseline and recovery segments. For challenge phase segments, we computed a 20-second rolling average of heart rate and extracted the peak value across the entire phase. 1 We also computed stress reactivity using average values of each measure during the challenge phase and examined resulting associations with LC contrast. The pattern of results was very similar as when we used peak metrics, with the following differences: the positive correlation between caudal LC contrast and systolic blood pressure reactivity was no longer significant, r(30) = 0.31, p = .088; RMSSD reactivity was no longer a stable contributor to the caudal LC-arousal PLS latent variable (bootstrap ratio = -1.96). Time-domain HRV analysis was performed on resulting rr-intervals, yielding a measure of root mean square of the successive differences (RMSSD) for each segment. For baseline and recovery segments, this was performed across the entire segment, but for challenge phase segments, RMSSD was computed over 20-second intervals, and the minimum RMSSD value across the challenge phase was saved (Munoz et al., 2015) . Frequency-domain analysis was performed using the Lomb periodogram method for generating power spectral density, which is the default in PhysioNet because it can handle losses of up to 20% of data (Clifford, 2002) . This yielded measures of low-frequency (LF) and high-frequency (HF) spectral power for each segment. For challenge phase segments, LF and HF power were not computed with a 20-second rolling average but rather calculated for the entire challenge phase. Frequency bands of 0.04 -0.15 and 0.15 and 0.4 Hz were used for calculating LF and HF power, respectively. Breathing rate. Peaks corresponding to inhalations were identified on resampled, filtered respiration signals using MATLAB's `findpeaks` function with a minimum peak width of 500 milliseconds. Identified peaks were used to calculate a single value of breathing rate for baseline and recovery segments. For challenge phase segments, we calculated a 20-second rolling average of breathing rate and extracted the peak value across the entire challenge phase. Systolic and diastolic blood pressure. Beat-to-beat systolic and diastolic blood pressure were extracted from preprocessed continuous blood pressure segments using the algorithm from the CRSIDLab toolbox (da Silva & Oliveira, 2020). This method uses rr-intervals in the corresponding ECG segment to identify systolic peaks and dicrotic notches within each cardiac cycle (Parati et al., 1995) . For this method, a systolic/diastolic threshold of 80 was specified (the default in CRSIDLab). When rr-intervals were unavailable due to a low-quality ECG segment, an alternative algorithm based on the continuous blood pressure waveform was used (Li et al., 2010) . For both methods, we specified that successive maxima and successive minima could be a minimum of 0.375 and a maximum of 2 seconds apart. Identified peaks were used to calculate a value of mean systolic and diastolic blood pressure for each baseline and recovery segment. For challenge phase segments, systolic and diastolic blood pressure were calculated with a 20-second rolling average, and the peak value across windows was selected. Sympathetic tone. We assessed sympathetic tone using the neuECG method, an approach to quantify skin sympathetic nerve activity from ECG signals (Kusayama et al., 2020) . In this approach, raw ECG segments were first high-pass filtered with an FIR filter with cutoff frequency of 500 Hz. Filtered ECG signals were then full-wave rectified and integrated with a leaky integrator (time constant: 0.1 seconds). We then computed the average voltage of the resulting signal across each segment (aSKNA), a measure that has been shown to increase during sympathetic-activating manipulations (Kusayama et al., 2020) . For baseline and recovery segments, we computed aSKNA across entire segments, but for challenge segments, we computed a 20-second rolling average of aSKNA and identified the peak value. We used a validated, semi-automated approach to delineate the LC on TSE scans ( Figure 2 ). The method is fully described in Bachman et al. (2021) Avants et al., 2011) , and visualization steps were performed using ITK-SNAP (Version 3.6.0, Yushkevich et al., 2006) . Contrast ratios for left and right LC were averaged within each z-slice, and in a final step, the peak ratio across all z-slices was used for statistical analysis (these are henceforth referred to as peak LC contrast values). Based on evidence that rostral LC exhibits greater neuronal loss in aging and Alzheimer's disease relative to caudal LC (Manaye et al. 1995; Zarow et al., 2003) , as well as reports of spatially confined associations with contrast along the LC's rostrocaudal axis Dahl et al., 2019) , we also calculated rostral and caudal LC contrast values for each participant. This entailed first calculating percentiles of slices along the LC's rostrocaudal axis where we previously found age differences in LC contrast and associations with cortical thickness in younger versus older adults . These percentiles were applied to the z-range of slices included in the LC meta-map (z = 85-112) to identify ranges of slices corresponding to rostral and caudal LC (rostral: MNI z = 87-95; caudal: MNI z = 101-104). For each participant, contrast ratios were then averaged across the caudal and rostral clusters of slices to obtain values of rostral and caudal LC contrast, respectively. RMSSD, LF power and HF power values were determined to exhibit severe non-normality and were therefore log-transformed prior to analysis. Outliers for each arousal measure were then identified using the mean absolute deviation-median rule for each age group separately and treated as missing values for all analyses (Wilcox, 2011) . Outlier detection was performed prior to the detection of peak (or minima) arousal metrics for each challenge segment. A summary of identified outliers for each measure is included in the Supplementary Methods (Section 3). For peak, rostral and caudal LC contrast values, we tested for outliers for younger and older adults separately according to the mean absolute deviation-median rule. One older participant was an outlier for peak LC ratios and excluded from relevant analyses. To assess whether the acute stress induction protocol was effective at modulating each measure of physiological arousal in younger and older adults, we fit a series of linear mixed effects models. These models tested the fixed effects of phase (baseline/challenge/recovery), age group, and their interaction on each arousal measure (heart rate, breathing rate, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, sympathetic tone, RMSSD, LF power and HF power), using a separate model for each measure. For these analyses, we used average values of each measure from each phase. We used a repeated contrast coding scheme for the phase factor to test two contrasts of interest: challenge vs. baseline and recovery vs. challenge (Schad et al., 2020) . Age group was sum coded (younger = 0.5, older = -0.5). Models were fit with the `lmer4` R package We also examined performance on the PASAT and Stroop tasks by computing each participant's mean accuracy and reaction time on the tasks. As only younger adults completed the PASAT, we reported PASAT performance as the mean and standard deviation of accuracy and reaction time across participants. For the Stroop task, we used independent-samples Welch's t-tests to compare accuracy and reaction times by age group. We then calculated measures of acute stress reactivity by computing the change in each physiological measure from baseline to the challenge phase (Llabre et al., 1991) . For these calculations, we used peak values of each measure from the challenge phase: To calculate measures of acute stress during recovery, we likewise computed the difference in each measure from baseline to the recovery phase: Larger-magnitude values of reactivity were therefore expected to reflect greater stress reactivity, whereas larger-magnitude values of recovery would reflect higher arousal during stress recovery. Prior to testing associations between LC contrast and arousal, we examined whether peak LC contrast differed in younger and older adults using an independent-samples Welch's t-test. Based on previous findings of age differences in contrast according to LC topography (Dahl et al., 2019) , we also performed a separate 2x2 mixed-design ANOVA, implemented with the R package `afex` (Version 1.0-1; Singmann et al., 2021) , testing the effects of age group (younger, older) and topography (rostral, caudal) on LC contrast. We first assessed associations between LC contrast and physiological arousal by computing, separately for younger and older participants, a Pearson correlation matrix reflecting pairwise correlations between all measures of arousal (baseline, reactivity and recovery) and all measures of LC contrast (peak, rostral and caudal). For this step, all available pairwise observations were used. To further probe associations between LC contrast and arousal using a multivariate framework, we then used a series of partial least squares (PLS) correlation analyses. The aim of PLS is to identify latent variables that express a maximal amount of covariance between a set of predictors and an outcome variable (Krishnan et al., 2011; Mcintosh et al., 1996; McIntosh & Lobaugh, 2004) . In this case, our goal was to identify patterns of arousal measures whose relation with LC contrast differed across age groups. Because PLS required the data to be restricted to complete cases, we removed breathing rates from this set of analyses to boost the number of available complete cases to reflect 53 younger and 23 older participants. Then, all measures were centered and normalized. Physiological arousal measures reflecting rest, stress reactivity and stress recovery were stored in a matrix , with rows reflecting individual participants and columns containing the various measures. LC contrast values were stored in a single-column matrix , with rows reflecting individual participants. The cross-correlation map was computed for each age group = and after arranging the maps in a matrix, the matrix was subjected to singular value decomposition: . The resulting left singular vectors ( ) reflect the LC contrast profiles = that best characterized the correlation matrix (also termed "LC saliences"), the right singular vectors ( ) reflect the physiological profiles that best characterized the correlation matrix (also termed "physiological saliences"), and is a matrix of singular values. The original data and were then projected onto their respective singular vectors, yielding latent variables of ("physiological scores"; ) and latent variables of ("LC scores"; ) for each = = participant. To test the reliability of identified latent variable(s), a permutation test with 10,000 samples was conducted. This entailed randomly shuffling the rows of but not and using the distribution of singular values from all permutation samples for testing the null hypothesis of no reliable latent variables (Krishnan et al., 2011; McIntosh & Lobaugh, 2004) . Latent variable (s) identified as reliable were then tested for stability through bootstrapping (Krishnan et al., 2011) . Specifically, for each of 10,000 bootstrap samples, and were sampled with replacement, and standard errors were calculated based on physiological saliences across all bootstrap samples. Physiological saliences were divided by their standard errors, yielding a bootstrap ratio for each physiological arousal measure, with each ratio reflecting how much the given arousal measure showed a stable association with LC contrast in the latent variable of interest. Bootstrap ratios with absolute values greater than 2 were considered significantly stable (Krishnan et al., 2011) . The procedure described above was performed three times, once with each LC contrast measure (peak, rostral or caudal) comprising . PLS correlation analyses were performed in MATLAB using the `PLScmd` toolbox (Mcintosh et al., 1996) . All other analyses were performed in R (Version 4.0.4; R Core Team, 2021). Effect sizes for analyses other than PLS were calculated using the R package `effectsize` (Version 0.5; Ben-Shachar et al., 2020) and reported as partial r. Average measures of arousal during each phase of the stress induction task are presented in Figure 3 . Linear mixed-effects analyses were used to assess whether during the acute stress induction protocol, average measures of arousal differed during the baseline and challenge phases, and during the challenge and recovery phases ( Table 2) . Results of all planned, pairwise comparisons of each measure for each phase contrast and age group are presented in the Supplementary Results (Section 1). For heart rate, breathing rate, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood pressure, we found significant elevations from the baseline to the challenge phase (ps <= 0.012; Table 2 ), and significant decreases from the challenge to the recovery phase (ps < .001; Table 2 ). Sympathetic tone did not increase significantly from baseline to the challenge phase (p = .244; Table 2 ) but decreased significantly from challenge to recovery (p = .002; Table 2 ). For systolic blood pressure, we found a significant phase (recovery-challenge) x age group interaction (p = .006), which was driven by greater challenge-to-recovery decreases in blood pressure for older compared to younger participants, although challenge-to-recovery changes were significant in both age groups (Supplementary Results, Section 1). Examining measures of HRV during the stress task, we found that RMSSD decreased significantly from the baseline to the challenge phase (p = .034; Table 3 ) and increased significantly from the challenge to the recovery phase (p = .017; Table 3 ). Although both HF and LF power exhibited the same numeric pattern, the only significant phase contrast was an increase in LF power from challenge to recovery (p < .001; Table 3 ). For LF power, we also found a significant phase (challenge-baseline) x age group interaction (p = .012; Table 3 ) and a marginally significant phase (recovery-challenge) x age group interaction effect (p = .066; Table 3 ); pairwise comparisons indicated significant baseline-to-challenge decreases and challenge-to-recovery increases in LF power for younger but not older participants (Supplementary Results, Section 1). Note. Models tested the fixed effects of each phase contrast of interest (Challenge -Baseline, Recovery -Challenge), age group and their interaction effects. All models included random intercepts for participants. CI = confidence interval; SE = standard error. Note. Models tested the fixed effects of each phase contrast of interest (Challenge -Baseline, Recovery -Challenge), age group and their interaction effects. All models included random intercepts for participants. CI = confidence interval; LF = low-frequency; HF = high-frequency; RMSSD = root mean square of the successive differences; SE = standard error. In terms of performance on the cognitive challenge tasks, accuracy and reaction times on Peak, rostral and caudal LC contrast values in the sample are presented in Figure 4 Visualizations As a multivariate approach to quantify the associations between LC contrast and arousal during the stress induction task, we performed a series of PLS correlation analyses. We note that these analyses were performed with only participants with no missing values (53 younger, 23 older), whereas the correlations presented above reflected all available sets of pairwise observations. PLS analyses examining associations of peak and rostral LC contrast, respectively, with arousal yielded no reliable latent variables. The final PLS analysis, examining associations between caudal LC contrast and arousal, indicated 1 marginally reliable latent variable (p = .053). Bootstrap ratios reflecting the contribution of each arousal measure to this latent variable, as well as the correlation between physiological scores for this latent variable and peak LC contrast values, are shown in Figure 6 . Physiological scores on this latent variable were highly correlated with caudal LC contrast in older participants, r(21) = 0.66, p < .001, but not in younger participants, r(51) = 0.04, p = .781. Furthermore, higher physiological scores for this latent variable reflected greater systolic blood pressure increases and RMSSD decreases during stress reactivity, higher systolic blood pressure during stress recovery, and lower RMSSD and HF power during stress recovery. Figure 6 . Results of partial least squares (PLS) correlation analyses examining the association between caudal LC contrast and physiological arousal during the stress induction task. These analyses indicated a marginally reliable latent variable reflecting an association between caudal LC contrast and arousal for older participants. The left panels depict bootstrap ratios which reflect how much each arousal measure contributed to the latent variable (bootstrap ratios with absolute value greater than 2, indicated in red, were considered stable contributors). Right panels depict associations between physiological scores -reflecting the projection of each respective latent variable onto the original physiological arousal data -and LC contrast values. As an arousal hub region in the brain, the LC plays a major role in the central nervous system's response to acute stress, releasing norepinephrine throughout the brain and spinal cord to promote behaviors that facilitate stressor avoidance or elimination (Bremner et al., 1996; Sara & Bouret, 2012; Wood & Valentino, 2017) . Studies using MRI to assess the LC's structural integrity in vivo have suggested that having a more structurally intact LC in later adulthood is associated with better cognitive outcomes and reduced risk of cognitive decline (Elman et al., 2021a; Liu et al., 2020) , but it is unclear whether LC MRI contrast is related to acute stress responses. Limited evidence in small studies of mostly younger adults suggests that higher LC contrast is associated with greater arousal levels, with anxious arousal being positively correlated with LC volume (Morris et al., 2020b) and LC contrast being negatively correlated with parasympathetic control over heart rate (Mather et al., 2017) . Here, we tested how LC MRI contrast was associated with arousal at rest, during reactivity to acute stress, and during recovery from acute stress in both younger and older adults. Across univariate and multivariate analyses, we found that for older adults, having higher caudal LC contrast was associated with higher stress-related increases in systolic blood pressure and lower HRV during stress recovery. Together, these findings suggest that having a more structurally intact caudal LC in older adulthood is associated with more pronounced physiological responses to acute stress. In response to acute psychosocial stressors, sympathetic arousal increases and HRV generally decreases, reflecting parasympathetic withdrawal (Rab & Admon, 2021) . Aging is associated with changes to the autonomic system that may impact acute stress responses (Kaye & Esler, 2008) . In general, older adults exhibit higher mean levels of cortisol than younger adults, reflecting higher tonic activation of the HPA axis in later adulthood (Lupien et al., 2009 ). Sympathetic nervous system activity tends to also increase in aging (Fagius & Wallin, 1993; Seals & Esler, 2000) , and consistent with this pattern, we found that older participants had higher values of a measure of skin sympathetic nerve activity quantified from ECG signals, relative to younger participants. In addition to older age being associated with elevated sympathetic tone, vagal control of heart rate and HRV decline with age (Jandackova et al., 2016) . Evidence for age changes in parasympathetic responses to stress is scarce, but here, we found that older participants had significantly smaller decreases in LF power relative to younger participants during acute stress. Thus an autonomic system that is relatively less affected by age-related dysregulation might be expected to feature more dynamic responses to acute stress -specifically, greater sympathetic increases and greater parasympathetic withdrawal in response to stress. We found that older participants with more pronounced physiological responses to acute stress -that is, greater sympathetic increases and parasympathetic withdrawal in response to stress -had higher caudal LC contrast. The LC is a component of the central autonomic network, the set of brain regions that coordinates neuroendocrine, visceromotor and behavioral responses to situational demands such as acute stress (Benarroch, 1993) . In particular, the LC contains excitatory projections to the rostral ventrolateral medulla, inhibitory projections to parasympathetic nuclei, and bidirectional connections with C1 neurons that coordinate cardiovascular responses (Lamotte et al., 2021) . According to the neurovisceral integration model, the ability to modulate heart rate on a moment-to-moment basis reflects the central autonomic network's capacity for brain-heart integration (Thayer & Lane, 2000) . With MRI-assessed LC integrity thought to reflect neurodegeneration in aging (Betts et al., 2019b) , our results suggest that the structural integrity of the caudal LC may be important for allowing messages from the brain to reach the heart and coordinate effective physiological responses. These findings add to a growing body of literature linking higher LC integrity to better cognitive and neural outcomes in aging Dahl et al., 2019; Elman et al., 2021b; Liu et al., 2020) . Furthermore, current research suggests a role of the LC in the progression of Alzheimer's disease (Jacobs et al., 2021) , with lower MRI-indexed LC integrity being associated with elevated risk for mild cognitive impairment (Elman et al., 2021a) and LC integrity being lower in individuals with Alzheimer's disease relative to healthy controls (Betts et al., 2019a; Takahashi et al., 2015) . Mild cognitive impairment and dementia are characterized by autonomic dysfunction, including HPA axis hyperactivation (Justice, 2018) and reductions in heart rate variability (Collins et al., 2012; da Silva et al., 2017) . Our findings provide novel evidence that greater neurodegeneration of the caudal LC in aging may be associated with dysregulated physiological responses to stress. Our results furthermore highlight a potential role of the LC's caudal aspect in neurovisceral integration. We previously found that in aging, associations with episodic memory and gray matter integrity were greater for rostral compared to caudal LC contrast Dahl et al., 2019) . Consistent with the rostral LC being important for age-related outcomes, the rostral LC undergoes relatively more cell loss than the caudal LC in aging (Manaye et al., 1995) and Alzheimer's disease (Zarow et al., 2003) . So why might the caudal LC be more relevant for physiological arousal in aging? Hirschberg et al. (2017) identified two populations of LC neurons: one population consisting of more rostrally-originating neurons projecting to the prefrontal cortex and another clustered in the caudal LC and projecting primarily to the spinal cord. At the spinal cord, noradrenergic neurons from the LC synapse onto sympathetic preganglionic neurons, promoting downstream peripheral arousal responses (Clark & Proudfit, 1991) . Based on current evidence, it is unclear whether medullary and parasympathetic projections from the LC also originate predominantly in the LC's caudal aspect. Yet our results suggest that the caudal LC may play a role in the pathway linking neural appraisals of the world to physiological arousal responses. We found associations between LC contrast and patterns of acute stress responding in older participants, but we did not find the expected relationships between LC contrast and arousal in younger participants. Specifically, neither PLS nor pairwise correlation analyses indicated a consistent pattern: We found that peak LC contrast was positively correlated with lower breathing rate during stress recovery, while rostral LC contrast was negatively correlated with sympathetic tone during stress recovery. Although few studies have investigated associations with LC structure in younger adults, the largely null findings in our younger sample are inconsistent with reports of higher LC volume being associated with higher anxious arousal (Morris et al., 2020b) and of caudal LC contrast being negatively associated with cortical thickness in younger adults. We previously reported that LC contrast was negatively correlated with HRV during a fear conditioning task in both younger and older adults (Mather et al., 2017) . Our current findings offer an alternative explanation for the previous findings in older adults (more pronounced acute stress responses in individuals with higher LC contrast), but they are inconsistent with the previous findings in younger adults. Based on these and other previous findings linking LC structure to arousal in younger adults, we predicted that LC contrast would be associated with larger physiological arousal responses to acute stress among younger participants. In this case, a potential reason that we did not observe expected associations in younger adults is that the LC contrast measure does not reflect functional acute stress responses in younger adults. Related to this, it is possible that the imaging method used may be less reliable in younger than older adults (Hämmerer et al., 2018) . A different explanation, and another limitation of the study, is that the cognitive challenge tasks may not have induced reliably acute stress for younger participants. Qualitatively, older participants reported the Stroop task to be very challenging, whereas this was not common feedback from younger participants. Heart rate, breathing rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure and sympathetic tone all increased reliably for younger participants during the challenge phase. However, because we did not measure salivary cortisol levels throughout the acute stress induction task, we cannot be sure that this task elicited an acute stress response in participants or simply cognitive load. Further research will be needed to examine whether robust acute stress responses in younger and older adults are indeed related to MRI-indexed LC integrity. Another important limitation of this study is that the PLS analyses used only cases with all available physiological measures, constraining the number of participants included for these analyses. (The older sample available for analysis was also smaller in size than the younger sample, due to early termination of data collection at the start of the COVID-19 pandemic.) To combat this limitation associated with PLS, we also reported pairwise correlations between measures of LC contrast and physiological arousal and considered whether the results were consistent with the pattern of associations that emerged using PLS. The latent variable reflecting associations between caudal LC contrast and arousal indicated that participants with higher caudal LC contrast had higher systolic blood pressure increases and RMSSD decreases in response to stress, higher systolic blood pressure during stress recovery, and lower RMSSD and HF power during stress recovery. Pairwise correlation analyses likewise indicated that older participants with higher caudal LC contrast had greater systolic blood pressure increases in response to stress, the pattern of associations between caudal LC contrast and HRV during recovery was also negative, albeit not significant. These findings suggest the interesting possibility that older adults with caudal LC contrast have more pronounced physiological responses to acute stress, but further work is needed to replicate these findings in a larger sample of older adults. To conclude, we examined how LC MRI contrast, an in vivo measure of LC structural integrity, was related to physiological arousal at rest, during acute stress reactivity, and during recovery from acute stress. In younger participants, LC contrast was largely unrelated to physiological arousal, although this may be explained by the task being challenging, but not reliably stressful, for younger participants. In older participants, caudal LC contrast was associated with greater stress-related increases in systolic blood pressure and decreases in HRV, as well as lower HRV during stress recovery. These results suggest that caudal LC integrity is associated with more pronounced stress responses in aging and implicate the caudal LC in neurovisceral integration. The brain nucleus locus coeruleus: Restricted afferent control of a broad efferent network A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration Locus coeruleus MRI contrast is associated with cortical thickness in older adults Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 A Comparison of Single Channel Fetal ECG Extraction Methods The Central Autonomic Network: Functional Organization, Dysfunction, and Perspective Effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters Locus coeruleus MRI contrast is reduced in alzheimer's disease dementia and correlates with CSF amyloid-beta levels Locus coeruleus imaging as a biomarker for noradrenergic dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases Persistent Stress-Induced Neuroplastic Changes in the Locus Coeruleus/Norepinephrine System Stages of the pathologic process in alzheimer disease: Age categories from 1 to 100 years Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and anxiety: I. preclinical studies Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: A meta-analysis Greater Cardiovascular Responses to Laboratory Mental Stress Are Associated With Poor Subsequent Cardiovascular Risk Status: A Meta-Analysis of Prospective Evidence The projection of locus coeruleus neurons to the spinal cord in the rat determined by anterograde tracing combined with immunocytochemistry Signal processing methods for heart rate variability Parasympathetic autonomic dysfunction is common in mild cognitive impairment The interactive effect of change in perceived stress and trait anxiety on vagal recovery from cognitive challenge Corticotropin-releasing factor neurones of the central nucleus of the amygdala mediate locus coeruleus activation by cardiovascular stress Activation of the locus coeruleus noradrenergic system by intracoerulear microinfusion of corticotropin-releasing factor: effects on discharge rate, cortical norepinephrine levels and cortical electroencephalographic activity Rostral locus coeruleus integrity is associated with better memory performance in older adults Locus coeruleus integrity is related to tau burden and memory loss in autosomal-dominant Alzheimer's disease Evidence for the existence of monoamine-containing neurons in the central nervous system. Demonstration of monoamines in the cell bodies of brain stem neurons Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research MRI-assessed locus coeruleus integrity is heritable and associated with multiple cognitive domains, mild cognitive impairment, and daytime dysfunction Associations Between MRI-Assessed Locus Coeruleus Integrity and Cortical Gray Matter Microstructure Long-term variability and reproducibility of resting human muscle nerve sympathetic activity at rest, as reassessed after a decade An Empirical Test of Telephone Screening to Identify Potential Dementia Cases Locus coeruleus integrity in old age is selectively related to memories linked with salient negative events Noradrenergic dysregulation in the pathophysiology of PTSD Functional dichotomy in spinal-vs prefrontal-projecting locus coeruleus modules splits descending noradrenergic analgesia from ascending aversion and anxiety in rats In vivo and neuropathology data support locus coeruleus integrity as indicator of alzheimer's disease pathology and cognitive decline Are Changes in Heart Rate Variability in Middle-Aged and Older People Normative or Caused by Pathological Conditions? Findings From a Large Population-Based Longitudinal Cohort Study Chronic Exposure to Cold Stress Alters Electrophysiological Properties of Locus Coeruleus Neurons Recorded In Vitro R-peak estimation using multimodal lead switching Mechanisms of stress: A dynamic overview of hormonal and behavioral homeostasis The efferent projections from the reticular formation and the locus coeruleus studied by anterograde and retrograde axonal transport in the rat The relationship between stress and Alzheimer's disease The role of afferents to the locus coeruleus in the handling stress-induced increase in the release of noradrenaline in the medial prefrontal cortex: a dual-probe microdialysis study in the rat brain Autonomic Control of the Aging Heart Vivo Mapping of the Human Locus Coeruleus Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review Simultaneous noninvasive recording of electrocardiogram and skin sympathetic nerve activity (neuECG) lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models Stress and central autonomic network Emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means On an automatic delineator for arterial blood pressure waveforms Noradrenergic-dependent functions are associated with age-related locus coeruleus signal intensity differences Magnetic resonance imaging of the human locus coeruleus: A systematic review The Reliability and Specificity of Delta Versus Residualized Change as Measures of Cardiovascular Reactivity to Behavioral Challenges Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition Stress and Alzheimer's disease: A senescence link? Half a century of research on the stroop effect: An integrative review Chronic cold stress alters the basal and evoked electrophysiological activity of rat locus coeruleus neurons Locus coeruleus cell loss in the aging human brain: A non-random process The locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system role in cognition and how it changes with aging Noradrenaline in the aging brain: Promoting cognitive reserve or accelerating Alzheimer's disease? Higher locus coeruleus MRI contrast is associated with lower parasympathetic influence over heart rate variability Version 9 CRH Engagement of the Locus Coeruleus Noradrenergic System Mediates Stress-Induced Anxiety Spatial pattern analysis of functional brain images using partial least squares Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: Applications and advances Reduced cortical thickness and increased gyrification in generalized anxiety disorder: a 3 T MRI study Stress and life history Norepinephrine and stress The role of the locus coeruleus in the generation of pathological anxiety Sub-millimeter variation in human locus coeruleus is associated with dimensional measures of psychopathology: An in vivo ultra-high field 7-Tesla MRI study Validity of (Ultra-)Short Recordings for Heart Rate Variability Measurements Increasing coordination and responsivity of emotion-related brain regions with a heart rate variability biofeedback randomized trial Cardiovascular Recovery From Psychological and Physiological Challenge and Risk for Adverse Cardiovascular Outcomes and All-Cause Mortality Spectral Analysis of Blood Pressure and Heart Rate Variability in Evaluating Cardiovascular Regulation: A Critical Appraisal Parsing inter-and intra-individual variability in key nervous system mechanisms of stress responsivity and across functional domains The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population Role of serotonergic and noradrenergic systems in the pathophysiology of depression and anxiety disorders Functional Neuroanatomy of the Noradrenergic Locus Coeruleus: Its Roles in the Regulation of Arousal and Autonomic Function Part I: Principles of Functional Organisation Stress and the brain: individual variability and the inverted-U The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition Orienting and reorienting: The locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal Neuromelanin magnetic resonance imaging of locus ceruleus and substantia nigra in Parkinson's disease How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: A tutorial Organization of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system Human ageing and the sympathoadrenal system CRSIDLab: A toolbox for multivariate autonomic nervous system analysis using cardiorespiratory identification Heart Rate Variability Indexes in Dementia: A Systematic Review with a Quantitative Analysis Afex: Analysis of factorial experiments Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory Detection of changes in the locus coeruleus in patients with mild cognitive impairment and alzheimer's disease: High-resolution fast spin-echo T1-weighted imaging A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation A comprehensive review of the paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT) Cardiovascular Reactivity and Development of Preclinical and Clinical Disease States: Psychosomatic Medicine Corticotropin-releasing hormone increases tonic but not sensory-evoked activity of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons in unanesthetized rats Convergent regulation of locus coeruleus activity as an adaptive response to stress Functional interactions between stress neuromediators and the locus coeruleus norepinephrine system Integration in the ventral medulla and coordination of sympathetic, pain and arousal functions An open source benchmarked toolbox for cardiovascular waveform and interval analysis Introduction to robust estimation and hypothesis testing The brain norepinephrine system, stress and cardiovascular vulnerability An in vivo probabilistic atlas of the human locus coeruleus at ultra-high field Interactions between stress and physical activity on Alzheimer's disease pathology User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and reliability HPA axis responses to psychological challenge linking stress and disease: What do we know on sources of intraand interindividual variability? Neuronal Loss Is Greater in the Locus Coeruleus Than Nucleus Basalis and Substantia Nigra in Alzheimer and Parkinson Diseases We are grateful to the individuals who helped with recruitment and data collection for this