key: cord-0077431-roolfn1u authors: Schrock, Joshua M; Sugiyama, Lawrence S; Naidoo, Nirmala; Kowal, Paul; Snodgrass, J Josh title: Greater chronic morbidity is associated with greater fatigue in six countries: A case of evolutionary mismatch? date: 2022-04-11 journal: Evol Med Public Health DOI: 10.1093/emph/eoac011 sha: 1a6e25d9ac11f5d5924fa8e137f75a7300a1460c doc_id: 77431 cord_uid: roolfn1u BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Human susceptibility to chronic non-communicable disease may be explained, in part, by mismatches between our evolved biology and contemporary environmental conditions. Disease-induced fatigue may function to reduce physical activity during acute infection, thereby making more energy available to mount an effective immune response. However, fatigue in the context of chronic disease may be maladaptive because long-term reductions in physical activity increase risks of disease progression and the acquisition of additional morbidities. Here, we test whether cumulative chronic morbidity is associated with subjective fatigue. METHODOLOGY: We constructed a cumulative chronic morbidity score using self-reported diagnoses and algorithm-based assessments, and a subjective fatigue score based on four questionnaire items using cross-sectional survey data from the Study on global AGEing and adult health, which features large samples of adults from six countries (China, Ghana, India, Mexico, Russia and South Africa). RESULTS: In a mixed-effects linear model with participants nested in countries (N = 32 455), greater cumulative chronic morbidity is associated with greater subjective fatigue (β = 0.34, SE = 0.005, P < 2e−16). This association replicates within each country and is robust to adjustment for key sociodemographic and physical covariates (sex, age, household wealth, physical function score, habitual physical activity, BMI and BMI(2)). CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS: Fatigue is a common but perhaps maladaptive neuropsychological response to chronic morbidity. Disease-induced fatigue may mediate a self-perpetuating cycle, in which chronic morbidity reduces physical activity, and less physical activity increases cumulative chronic morbidity. Longitudinal research is needed to test whether chronic morbidity, fatigue and physical activity form a cyclical feedback loop. Lay Summary: Fatigue during acute illness may promote recovery, but persistent fatigue in the context of chronic disease may make matters worse. We present evidence from six countries that more chronic disease is associated with more fatigue. This fatigue may reduce physical activity, which increases risks of acquiring additional chronic health problems. that have experienced evolutionarily novel increases in the average life expectancy, there is considerable variability in healthy aging [18] . Some individuals experience multiple decades of disabilityfree life in older adulthood, while other individuals experience rapid decline in functional status with age. Research is needed to identify processes across the lifecourse that explain this variation in healthy aging. Given the rapid, recent rise of chronic non-communicable diseases as a major source of morbidity and mortality, our evolved brains and bodies may be poorly equipped to deal with these diseases [19] . Much of our susceptibility to chronic disease may be explained by mismatches between our evolved biology and contemporary environmental conditions [4] . For example, humans have a propensity to generate large fat reserves when it is nutritionally feasible [20] . In the environments typical of our evolutionary history, the capacity to form large fat deposits provided a mechanism to maintain a stable energy supply for funding the high metabolic costs of maintaining our large brains, supporting multiple dependent offspring, foraging and lactating during extended periods of negative energy balance [21, 22] . In contemporary environments characterized by calorie-dense foods and sedentary lifestyles, our propensity for adiposity makes us vulnerable to obesity. Along the same lines, human immune systems seem to be mismatched with contemporary environments that are low in microbial diversity and feature low rates of exposure to infectious pathogens that were common for most of our evolutionary history (e.g. parasitic worms) [23, 24] . This lack of exposure to our long-time microbial and macroparasitic co-evolutionary companions influences the development of our immune systems, making us vulnerable to allergies, autoimmune diseases and inflammation-related disorders [24, 25] . In this article, we consider another feature of our evolved biology that appears to be mismatched with contemporary environments-disease-induced fatigue. Multiple lines of evidence suggest that disease-induced fatigue evolved to promote host survival during acute infection. But like chronic inflammation, fatigue may be counterproductive when it is deployed chronically in response to chronic noncommunicable diseases. Like other vertebrates, humans exhibit a typical neuropsychological response to internal immunological cues of infection or somatic damage [26, 27] . This response includes increased lethargy, social withdrawal, reduced appetite and increased pain sensitivity [26] . Animal behaviorists and psychoneuroimmunologists refer to these changes as sickness behavior [26, 28] . These changes are hypothesized to be adaptive adjustments that help organisms fight acute infection and recover from acute somatic damage [26, 27] . Depending on various contextual cues, sickness behavior includes: (i) increased fatigue to reduce physical activity, thereby making more energy available for the immune system; (ii) increased sensitivity to nausea and pain to reduce the risk of acquiring additional infections or injuries that would compound the immune system's workload; (iii) changes in temperature perception to promote thermoregulatory behaviors that reduce the cost of maintaining or increasing body temperature; (iv) changes in appetite to promote consumption behaviors (or lack thereof ) that support the fight against infection; and (v) detectable changes in facial expressions, body language and social behavior that signal to our social allies that we need help and support [29] . Acute infection, injury and chronic degenerative disease are all associated with increases in pro-inflammatory immune activity [30, 31] . Studies with human participants and animal models demonstrate that experimentally induced increases in pro-inflammatory cytokine production can initiate the psychological and behavioral changes characteristic of sickness behavior [26, 27] . During acute infection or injury, increased fatigue reduces physical activity, thereby making more energy available to mount an effective immune response [29, 32, 33] . During chronic disease, however, physical inactivity is an important causal factor driving disease progression and the acquisition of additional morbidities [34, 35] . In some cases, physical exercise can even reverse chronic non-communicable diseases, such as type 2 diabetes, though the doses of physical activity needed to reverse type 2 diabetes are much higher than the doses of physical exercise typically prescribed [36] . During acute infection, systemic inflammation is one of the main mechanistic cues that activates the neural mechanisms that regulate sickness behavior [29] . Systemic inflammation is often also elevated in people with chronic diseases, and some cases of chronic disease appear to induce a chronic version of sickness behavior [37, 38] . Along the same lines, greater systemic inflammation has been linked to greater risks for chronic fatigue and depression [39, 40] . Physical activity reduces inflammation through the action of myokines produced by active skeletal muscle [41] . Thus, physical inactivity can further exacerbate inflammation stemming from chronic diseases by failing to activate a major regulatory pathway that normally down-regulates inflammation. Thus, chronic morbidity may initiate a vicious self-perpetuating cycle, in which increased chronic morbidity triggers increased chronic fatigue, and greater chronic fatigue leads to even greater chronic morbidity by reducing levels of physical activity. Besides inflammation, there are other mechanisms that may contribute to fatigue during chronic disease [37, 42] . For example, disrupted insulin regulation in the context of diabetes may interfere with one's ability to mobilize metabolic resources, thereby inducing fatigue. Reduced oxygen supply may contribute to fatigue in people with chronic lung disease. The higher energy costs of movement may increase fatigue for people with high levels of adiposity. Cardiovascular damage may increase fatigue by limiting one's ability to mount sufficient cardiac output. The discomfort of movement may exacerbate fatigue in arthritis. Recent research has demonstrated that total energy expenditure progressively declines in humans after the age of 60 in contemporary populations, so some cases of apparent chronic disease-related fatigue may simply reflect age-related declines in total energy expenditure [43] . For most of our evolutionary history, fatigue may have been an adaptive response when experiencing physiological cues of infection, metabolic depletion, tissue damage, hypoxia or prolonged pain [29] . In the context of chronic aging-related diseases, fatigue may be a maladaptive response that exacerbates the underlying chronic condition by reducing physical activity. This feedback loop connecting chronic disease, fatigue and physical activity may play a role in generating the global pandemic of chronic non-communicable disease. One prediction arising from this model is that greater cumulative chronic morbidity is associated with greater subjective fatigue. We test this prediction using large cross-sectional samples of adults from six culturally distinct countries. We used data from the World Health Organization's (WHO) Study on global AGEing and adult health (SAGE), Wave 1, which collected cross-sectional data on aging-related health among adults in six middle-income countries (China, Ghana, India, Mexico, Russia and South Africa). In our analyses, we include all adults (ages 18þ) from the Wave 1 dataset who have complete data for all variables included in our statistical models. Wave 1 data collection began in 2007 and concluded in 2010. Data collection protocols for SAGE are described in detail elsewhere [44] . All SAGE study protocols were approved by WHO's Research Ethics Review Committee and by the ethical review organization with jurisdiction in each country. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. We created a cumulative chronic morbidity variable by summing the number of chronic conditions reported by each participant. Data were available on seven chronic conditions: arthritis, stroke, angina, diabetes, chronic lung disease, asthma and hypertension. For arthritis, angina, chronic lung disease and asthma, we followed previously published protocols for coding each condition as either present or absent [45] . A participant was coded as having the condition if they reported having been diagnosed with it, or if their responses to a symptombased diagnostic algorithm indicated that they had the condition (for angina and stroke). Participants were coded as having hypertension if they reported ever having been diagnosed with it or if their measured resting blood pressure (average of three trials, except for the Mexico data, where an average of the two available trials was used) was greater than systolic ¼140 mmHg or diastolic ¼90 mmHg. Participants were coded as having diabetes if they reported having been diagnosed with it. Summing the number of chronic conditions for each individual generated a count variable ranging from 0 to 7. We calculated a subjective fatigue score by summing responses to four questions. Question 1 asked, 'Overall in the last 30 days, how much difficulty did you have in vigorous activities (vigorous activities require hard physical effort and cause large increases in breathing or heart rate)?' Question 2 asked, 'Overall in the last 30 days, how much of a problem did you have due to not feeling rested or refreshed during the day?' Question 3 asked, 'Do you have enough energy for everyday life?' Question 4 asked, 'During the last 12 months, have you had a period lasting several days when you have been feeling your energy decreased or that you are tired all the time?' Questions 1-3 were answered on a 5-point ordinal scale, with higher values indicating greater subjective fatigue. Question 4 was a yes-or-no question, so we coded the response 'no' as 1 and the response 'yes' as 5 so that this item would have an influence on the subjective fatigue score equivalent to the other three items. The resulting subjective fatigue score ranged from 4 to 20. Age was reported as chronological age in years. Sex was reported as male or female. Household wealth was a composite measure based on possession of durable goods, dwelling characteristics and access to services [44] . Responses to 21 items were coded as '1' (denoting possession or access to the item) or '0' (denoting a lack of possession or access to the item). In a reshaped dataset, each response item was then treated as a separate observation for wealth in a pure random effects model, which produced indicator-specific thresholds for a latent wealth scale. Households were then arranged into a country-specific asset ladder using an empirical Bayes postestimation method. The value of this asset ladder was assigned to individuals as their wealth score. A physical function score was calculated by combining performance on two different timed walk tasks and grip strength for each hand. One timed walk task involved walking 4 m at a normal pace. The other timed walk task involved walking 4 m as quickly as possible. Grip strength was measured using a dynamometer with two trials for each hand. We averaged the two trials in each hand to create an average for the dominant and non-dominant hands. If no dominant hand was reported, the right hand was coded as dominant. Normal walk time and rapid walk time were each standardized by stature separately for men and women. Dominant grip strength and nondominant grip strength were each standardized by body weight separately for men and women. These standardized scores were combined to generate an overall physical function score. To remove implausible values and extreme outliers, we dropped cases for which the physical performance score was more than four standard deviations from the mean. Following the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire analysis guide [46] , we calculated a habitual physical activity variable that multiplies the minutes per week spent in each category of activity by the metabolic equivalent (MET) for that activity (the estimated ratio of total energy expenditure required to perform the task to the energy expenditure required for just resting). The resulting MET-minutes variable integrates both the quantity and intensity of an individual's physical activity levels in a typical week. Body mass index (BMI, kg/m 2 ) was calculated from stature and weight measured using standard protocols. In analyses with all countries combined, we specified mixedeffects linear models with participants nested in countries (i.e. with a random effect for country) in the R package 'lme4' (version 1. [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] [18] [19] . In analyses that considered each country separately, we specified ordinary least squares multiple regression models in base R (version 3.5.0). We created plots using the R package 'ggplot2' (version 3.1.0). In the first set of models, only cumulative chronic morbidity and an intercept term were included as predictors of subjective fatigue. In the second set of models, the sociodemographic covariates (age, sex and wealth) were included, in addition to the terms in the first set of models. In the third set of models, the physical covariates (physical function score, physical activity level, BMI and BMI 2 ) were included, in addition to the terms in the second set of models. Models with physical covariates included terms for both BMI and BMI 2 because we expected BMI to have both linear effects (greater body mass represents greater energetic reserves and, therefore, less subjective fatigue) and curvilinear effects (those in the underweight and obese extremes of BMI are expected to have greater subjective fatigue). All variables except age, sex and cumulative chronic conditions were standardized prior to analysis (mean ¼0, SD ¼ 1). Physical activity levels (MET-minutes) and subjective fatigue scores were positively skewed and were natural log-transformed prior to standardization. The distribution of age, sex and cumulative chronic morbidity by country is presented in Table 1 . All countries combined Model 1 included fixed effects for cumulative chronic morbidity, a random effect for country, and the model intercept as predictors of subjective fatigue. Greater cumulative chronic morbidity was associated with greater subjective fatigue (b ¼ 0.34, SE ¼ 0.005, P < 2e-16). Model 2 contained all the terms in Model 1 as well as fixed effects for age, sex and wealth. Greater cumulative chronic morbidity was also associated with greater subjective fatigue in Model 2 Model 3 contained all the terms in Model 2, as well as fixed effects for physical function score, physical activity levels, BMI and BMI 2 . Greater cumulative chronic morbidity was also associated with greater subjective fatigue in Model 3 (b ¼ 0.25, SE ¼ 0.005, P < 2e-16). In the mixed models combining data from all countries, the fully adjusted model (Model 3) exhibited the highest quality as indexed by both Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayes information criterion (BIC). Analyses stratified by country included the same set fixed effect terms as the pooled analyses with all countries combined. In Model 1, greater cumulative chronic morbidity was associated with greater subjective fatigue in all countries: In Model 2, greater cumulative chronic morbidity was also associated with greater subjective fatigue in all countries: China The same was true in Model 3: In within-country models, the fully adjusted model (Model 3) exhibited the highest quality in all countries as indexed by AIC. In all countries except the Russian Federation, the fully adjusted model (Model 3) exhibited the highest quality as indexed by BIC. In the Russian Federation, the model with only the intercept, cumulative chronic morbidity and sociodemographic covariates (Model 2) exhibited the highest quality as indexed by BIC. In Table 2 , we present coefficients, standard errors and P-values for all models. In Fig. 1 , we use violin plots and plotted lines representing marginal effects to visualize the relationship between cumulative chronic morbidity and subjective fatigue by country. We find that greater cumulative chronic morbidity is consistently associated with greater subjective fatigue. In the multilevel model including all participants, having four additional chronic conditions is associated with a full standard deviation increase in levels of subjective fatigue. This pattern is remarkably consistent across samples from six countries that are culturally and geographically distinct. Adding key physical variables to the model (physical function score, habitual physical activity, BMI and BMI 2 ) does not substantially diminish the association between cumulative chronic morbidity and subjective fatigue in any model, which suggests that this association is not mediated by declines in physical capacity. Plotting subjective fatigue by number of chronic conditions reveals a dose-response pattern-each additional chronic condition is associated with a higher level of subjective fatigue. This pattern suggests that subjective fatigue is actually associated with cumulative chronic morbidity, not just with one or two of the chronic conditions aggregated in the variable. In within-country analyses, the mean adjusted r 2 in models with only cumulative chronic disease burden as the independent variable was 0.15 (range: 0.08-0.26), indicating that cumulative chronic disease burden alone explained about 15% of the variation in subjective fatigue scores. The mean adjusted r 2 in the full model was 0.27 (range: 0.18-0.34). Cumulative chronic disease burden, along with other variables in the model, explained a substantial proportion of the variation in subjective fatigue scores. But there is also considerable remaining variation that remains unexplained in these models, reflecting the complex etiology of subjective fatigue. Our findings suggest that subjective fatigue may be useful as a low-cost, non-invasive marker of cumulative pathology across a variety of physiological systems. Our findings dovetail with a previous study of the general UK population, which found that greater fatigue predicted higher hazards of all-cause and cardiovascular disease-related mortality, even after adjusting for a variety of potential confounders [47] . Along the same lines, a study of older US adults found that a single-item measure of fatigue at baseline ('do you feel tired most of the time?') predicted mortality rates 10 years later [48] . In this study, we utilize relatively low-resolution measures of cumulative chronic morbidity and subjective fatigue. Even so, we find that greater cumulative chronic disease burden exhibits consistent associations with greater subjective fatigue. Using measures that capture a wider range and finer grain of variation in these variables might yield even stronger associations between cumulative chronic morbidity and fatigue. A limitation of this study is its reliance on self-report measures of physical activity and subjective fatigue. The limitations of self-reported physical activity are well documented-there is imprecise correspondence with objective measures and participants generally overestimate physical activity [49] . The limitations of self-reported fatigue are not precisely analogous to those of self-reported physical activity. For retrospectively selfreported physical activity, participants must attempt to recall and mentally aggregate behaviors across a specified timeframe. For subjective fatigue, the perception of fatigue is itself the target construct. Nonetheless, there are other ways of conceptualizing and measuring fatigue or fatigability, such as measuring the rate of decline in performance on standardized tasks [42] . Further research is needed to test whether our results replicate when using objective measures of physical activity and fatigue. Another limitation of this study is that our measures of physical function were not comprehensive. Future studies should test whether our results replicate when controlling for more comprehensive indices of physical function, including measures of cardiorespiratory fitness, such as maximal oxygen consumption. A third limitation of this article is that it does not identify the mechanisms linking chronic morbidity and fatigue. Given the analogous role of acute inflammation in generating fatigue during acute infection, inflammation is one possible mechanism linking chronic disease and fatigue [32] . However, fatigue in the context of chronic non-communicable diseases may have different mechanistic origins than fatigue during acute infection [42] . There are other possible physiological cues that could induce fatigue in the context of chronic non-communicable diseases, including reduced energy accessibility due to metabolic dysregulation, reduced oxygen availability, inability to mount In the dataset with all countries combined, each additional chronic condition is associated with an increase in subjective fatigue score. These patterns are broadly similar across countries. The plotted line represents the marginal effects from a fully adjusted regression model. Subjective fatigue scores were natural log-transformed and standardized (mean ¼0, SD ¼ 1) prior to plotting sufficient cardiac output, tissue damage and increased discomfort when moving. Further research is needed to identify mechanisms linking chronic morbidity and fatigue. Our model proposes a cyclical relationship between chronic morbidity and fatigue-greater morbidity induces greater fatigue, and greater fatigue increases risks for additional morbidity by reducing long-term levels of physical activity (Fig. 2) . A limitation of this study is that our cross-sectional study design cannot determine the temporal order of cumulative chronic morbidity and subjective fatigue. The cross-sectional associations, we observed in this study could be explained by chronic morbidity causing fatigue, fatigue causing chronic morbidity through reduced physical activity, or some combination of both. Longitudinal research is needed to determine whether increases in cumulative chronic morbidity predict subsequent increases in subjective fatigue. Clinical research is also needed to test whether interventions that reduce chronic diseaserelated fatigue are also successful in reducing subsequent morbidity and mortality. There are specific features of many contemporary environments that may contribute to chronic disease-induced fatigue. Low microbial diversity, low rates of exposure to ancestrally common pathogens and obesity are all thought to play a role in the etiology of chronic inflammation [23, 25] . Experimental studies have demonstrated that inflammatory processes play a key mechanistic role in inducing disease-related changes in behavior and psychology, including increased fatigue [30] . Thus, chronic inflammation may initiate the self-reinforcing feedback cycle of declining physical activity and increasing chronic morbidity, even before the appearance of clinically identifiable chronic disease. In contemporary medicine, individuals diagnosed with chronic non-communicable diseases are often prescribed exercise (discretionary physical activity undertaken for the sake of health and fitness) [22] . The literature has demonstrated that adherence to prescribed exercise is often low in people with chronic non-communicable diseases. Another potential issue is that fatigue may lead people with chronic diseases to inadvertently 'compensate' for bouts of exercise by reducing physical activity in other domains of life (e.g. work and recreation). Research is needed to evaluate the role of chronic disease-related fatigue in shaping adherence to prescribed exercise. It is worth noting that we chose not to include depressive symptoms or self-reported diagnosis with a depressive disorder as a covariate in our statistical models because depression is likely a collider variable for cumulative chronic disease and persistent fatigue. Chronic physical disease predicts greater risks for depression [38] , and persistent fatigue is a symptom that can contribute to classification as having a depressive disorder [31] . Thus, depression diagnoses may be causally 'downstream' from both cumulative chronic disease and persistent fatigue. Controlling for variables that are causally downstream from the independent and dependent variables introduces collider bias, which can produce misleading results [50] . Future studies aimed at investigating the interplay between cumulative chronic disease, fatigue and other symptoms of depression should utilize statistical approaches designed for simultaneously examining multiple outcome variables (e.g. regularized partial correlation networks and structural equation modeling). In this article, we show that greater cumulative chronic morbidity is associated with greater subjective fatigue in six culturally diverse countries. Prior research has demonstrated that some parts of our evolved biology, such as our propensity for adiposity and our immune systems, are mismatched with contemporary environments where diets are calorie-dense, occupations are sedentary, microbial diversity is low and ancestrally common pathogens are rare. During acute infection, disease-induced fatigue may improve host survival by reducing physical activity, thereby making more energy available to mount an immune response. In the context of chronic non-communicable diseases, fatigue and reduced physical activity may increase risks of disease progression and acquisition of additional morbidities. Disease-induced fatigue may be another example of an evolved mechanism that is mismatched with many of the environments that humans currently inhabit. The authors thank the SAGE participants and research teams for making this study possible. Global, regional, and national agesex-specific mortality for 282 causes of death in 195 countries and territories, 1980-2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990-2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study Evolutionary, historical and political economic perspectives on health and disease The Story of the Human Body: Evolution, Health, and Disease Coronary atherosclerosis in indigenous South American Tsimane: a cross-sectional cohort study Hunter-gatherers as models in public health Antiquity of postreproductive life: are there modern impacts on hunter-gatherer postreproductive life spans? Population Dynamics of a Philippine Rain Forest People: The San Ildefonso Agta High adult mortality among Hiwi huntergatherers: implications for human evolution The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People Demography of the Hadza, an increasing and high density population of savanna foragers EMERGING AND RE-EMERGING INFECTIOUS DISEASES: the third epidemiologic transition Major infectious diseases: key messages from disease control priorities The International Bank for Reconstruction and Development/The World Bank The global epidemic of obesity: an overview Obesity, SES, and economic development: a test of the reversal hypothesis Socioeconomic status and obesity in adult populations of developing countries: a review Heterogeneity in healthy aging Living with the past: evolution, development, and patterns of disease The Evolutionary Biology of Human Body Fatness: Thrift and Control Metabolic correlates of hominid brain evolution Exercised: Why Something We Never Evolved to Do is Healthy and Rewarding The hygiene hypothesis in autoimmunity: the role of pathogens and commensals Evolution, human-microbe interactions, and life history plasticity Evolutionary biology and anthropology suggest biome reconstitution as a necessary approach toward dealing with immune disorders Vertebrate sickness behaviors: adaptive and integrated neuroendocrine immune responses Human sickness behavior: ultimate and proximate explanations Behavioral adaptations to pathogens and parasites: five strategies Lassitude: the emotion of being sick Immune-neural connections: how the immune system's response to infectious agents influences behavior Depression and sickness behavior are Janus-faced responses to shared inflammatory pathways Fatigue and sleepiness responses to experimental inflammation and exploratory analysis of the effect of baseline inflammation in healthy humans Increased activity correlates with reduced ability to mount immune defenses to endotoxin in zebra finches Lack of exercise is a major cause of chronic diseases Benefits of exercise therapy for chronic diseases Dose-response effects of exercise on glucose-lowering medications for type 2 diabetes: a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial Cancer-related fatigue-mechanisms, risk factors, and treatments Epidemiology and treatment of depression in patients with chronic medical illness The role of inflammation in depression and fatigue Traumatic events and mental health: the amplifying effects of pre-trauma systemic inflammation Muscle-organ crosstalk: the emerging roles of myokines The Psychology of Fatigue: Work, Effort and Control IAEA DLW Database Consortium. Daily energy expenditure through the human life course Data resource profile: the World Health Organization Study on global AGEing and adult health (SAGE) Chronic noncommunicable diseases in 6 low-and middle-income countries: findings from wave 1 of the World Health Organization's study on global Ageing and adult health (SAGE) Development of the World Health Organization Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAQ) Fatigue is associated with excess mortality in the general population: results from the EPIC-Norfolk study Fatigue predicts mortality among older adults A systematic literature review of reviews on techniques for physical activity measurement in adults: a DEDIPAC study Illustrating bias due to conditioning on a collider