key: cord-0071272-kwxyej4j authors: Barcelos, Ana Maria; Kargas, Niko; Maltby, John; Hall, Sophie; Assheton, Phil; Mills, Daniel S. title: Theoretical Foundations to the Impact of Dog-Related Activities on Human Hedonic Well-Being, Life Satisfaction and Eudaimonic Well-Being date: 2021-11-25 journal: Int J Environ Res Public Health DOI: 10.3390/ijerph182312382 sha: ece2be99ec37d8ced7ac3cd569ce0eac37cd0259 doc_id: 71272 cord_uid: kwxyej4j Cross-sectional comparisons of well-being between dog owners and non-owners commonly generate inconsistent results. Focusing on the uniqueness of the relationship might help address this issue and provide a stronger foundation for dog-related psychotherapeutic interventions. This study aims to evaluate the impact of dog-related activities (e.g., exercising the dog) on owner hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being. It was also hypothesised that psychological closeness to the dog would affect these well-being outcomes. For this study, 1030 dog owners aged over 18 years old answered an online questionnaire about the impact of 15 groups of dog-related activities on their well-being. Ordinal regressions were used to estimate the mean response (and its uncertainty) for each outcome, while conditioning for psychological closeness to the dog and controlling for several key covariates. Tactile interactions and dog playing were significantly more beneficial than other activities for hedonic well-being, and dog training and dog presence for eudaimonic well-being. In contrast, dog health issues and behavioural problems were linked to decrements in these well-being outcomes. Higher psychological closeness to the dog predicted greater improvement in well-being in positive dog-related activities. Our quantitative study validates the general findings of previous qualitative work and lays the groundwork for future longitudinal studies. Despite frequent reports of the benefits of pet ownership to human well-being based on qualitative studies, quantitative findings based largely on cross-sectional comparisons between owners and non-owners are inconsistent [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] . These mixed results highlight not only the complexity of the pet-human relationship but also the need for theoretical frameworks that account for the diversity of the relationship [4] . For example, although McNicholas et al. [6] have proposed a range of mechanisms whereby pet ownership might have a direct, indirect (via enhancement of social interactions) or no effect at all on human well-being, this is clearly not a comprehensive list. By contrast, Gee et al. [7] have suggested that a biopsychological model may explain the benefits of dog-human interaction. However, while this might seem more comprehensive, it lacks detail concerning the specific behaviours associated with ownership that impact human well-being. For example, are the benefits related to dog walking, tactile interactions, the routine of looking after the animal, or simply to the animal's presence? Similar questions are being increasingly asked, not only in studies of pet ownership [8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] , but also in experiments featuring human-animal interaction [14] [15] [16] [17] . By addressing the specific activities/situations associated with pet ownership, we can begin to objectively address the psychological basis to any benefits. Barcelos et al. [18] have recently proposed a framework that describes 58 dog-related activities/situations (e.g., dog walking, feeding the dog and dog aggression), organised into 15 themes (e.g., exercise, providing for the dog and unwanted behaviours), and their impact on hedonic well-being (positive and negative affect) [19, 20] , life satisfaction [21] and eudaimonic well-being (i.e., six areas of life functioning: autonomy, purpose in life, personal growth, control over own environment, self-acceptance and positive relations with others) [22, 23] of UK dog owners. This thematic framework has also been shown to be relatively robust in other cultures (e.g., Brazilian dog owners) [24] , amongst a more neurodiverse population (e.g., autistic adults dog owners) [25] , and even in other species (e.g., cat owners) [26] , illustrating how pet ownership can be deconstructed into a range of related activities that might positively or negatively impact human well-being. Besides accommodating the uniqueness of the pet-owner relationship, this approach, unlike many others, considers both hedonia and eudaimonia, as recommended for investigations of wellbeing, given their complementary psychological functions. Hedonic pursuit is associated more with immediate well-being (e.g., everyday affect), while eudaimonic is more closely associated with longer-term well-being (e.g., life engagement) [27] . Findings of other studies also indicate that the psychological closeness between owner and dog could be an important moderator of owner well-being outcomes [28, 29] . The latter found a positive correlation with well-being (i.e., life satisfaction and happiness) and a negative correlation with psychological distress, and in the study by Cavanaugh et al. [28] , owners who reported higher psychological closeness to their dog tended to be more satisfied with their lives. Having used qualitative methods to build a framework of dog owner interactions which impact on their well-being [18] , the aim of the current study was to use quantitative methods to deduce the significance of the reported relationships between types of dog owner activity and their impact on owner well-being in a wider population [30] . Furthermore, given the potential importance of psychological closeness on the proposed effects, we included a measure of this in order to illustrate the importance of psychological closeness in relation to different types of activity. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Lincoln (protocol code 2021_0503). Electronic informed consent was obtained from participants prior to their participation. Participants completed an online questionnaire (QualtricsTM) divided into two sections: (1) general questions about the participant and their dog, used as covariates in the statistical analysis, and (2) questions about the impact of 15 groups of dog-related activities on participants' hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being. A total of eleven aspects of well-being were self-reported in relation to these activities: life satisfaction [21] , four elements of hedonic well-being [31] [32] [33] and six elements of eudaimonic well-being [22] . Most questions of the survey were not mobile friendly. Therefore, participants were advised at the introduction of the survey to answer the survey from a desktop and not from their phones. The questionnaire is available in the Supplementary Material. The dog-related activities (Table 1) were based on our previous framework [18, 24, 25] . To decrease potential order effects, the order of the questions about dog-related activities (for example, how does 'dog playing' impact on your satisfaction with your own life?) was randomised for each participant. The impact of each of these activities on the 11 well-being outcomes was assessed using a 7-point Likert response (−3 worsen a lot, −2 worsen moderately, −1 worsen a bit, 0 no impact, +1 improve a bit, +2 improve moderately, +3 improve a lot). Life satisfaction was referred to as 'satisfaction with your own life'. The four aspects of hedonic well-being were described in terms of states: (1) 'positive affect of high arousal (e.g., happiness, excitement, joy, fun, activation, etc)', (2) 'positive affect of low arousal (e.g., calmness, relaxation, serenity, peacefulness, etc)', (3) 'negative affect of high arousal (e.g., stress, annoyance, worry, frustration, anxiety, anger, etc)', and (4) 'negative affect of low arousal (e.g., sadness, tiredness, unhappiness, guilt, feeling low, grief, etc)'. The six elements of eudaimonia considered were: (1) 'autonomy (your independence, freedom from others' approval)', (2) 'your control over situations/events in your life or in your surroundings', (3) 'personal growth (your growth or achievement of your potentials)', (4) 'positive relations with others (good social relations with other people)', (5) 'purpose in life (having aims/goals in life)', and (6) 'self-acceptance (accepting yourself, the good and bad in you)'. Finally, the level of psychological closeness to the dog was measured with an adapted version of the Inclusion of Other in the Self scale [34] , a visual scale, ranging from 1 to 7, originally developed to assess interpersonal closeness. A power analysis was conducted prior to data collection. Aiming for 99% chance of reaching narrow confidence intervals equivalent to ≤20% of the one-point distance between the options given to participants (e.g., improve moderately and improve a lot) in each activity, a sample of 1000 dog owners was estimated. The power analysis was based on the estimated marginal means of an ordinal regression analysis applied to each of the 1000 simulations. Each simulation assumed weak correlations (R = 0.1) between all pairs of covariates and moderate correlations (R = 0.3) between the DV and three of the covariates (zero correlation between DV and the remaining covariates). The model was further challenged in the power analysis by capturing an unequal spread of responses across the seven ordinal response categories (the seven categories were chosen to have marginal probabilities of selection of 0.01, 0.01, 0.01, 0.07, 0.1, 0.2 and 0.6, respectively). Using convenience and snowball sampling, volunteer participants were recruited via social media (i.e., Facebook, Twitter, Quora, Reddit, WhatsApp) and local media (i.e., University of Lincoln News). To be included, they needed to be at least 18 years old and own a dog. Estimated means were calculated using ordinal probit regression models on the R packages 'ordinal' (v.2019.12-10) and 'emmeans' (v.1.6.1). The following participants' characteristics were included as covariates in the statistical models, as they could influence owners' well-being: age (continuous variable [35] ), gender (female, male, non-binary [36] ), country (UK, US, other [37] ), living alone (yes/no [38] ), on the autism spectrum (yes/no [39] ), diagnosed or experiencing a mental health problem (yes/no [40] ), and level of psychological closeness to the dog (1 to 7 [29] ). Dogs' characteristics were also controlled in the models, as they might influence the performance of dog-related activities and/or human well-being (e.g., dog size can affect dog walking performance [41] ): sex of the dog(s) (female, male, mixed-e.g., one female and one male in the household was coded as mixed), young dogunder one year old (yes/no), senior dog-10 years old or more (yes/no), reproductive status (intact, neutered/spayed, mixed-i.e., having both intact and neutered dogs in the household was coded as mixed), very small/small dog (yes/no), and very large/giant dog (yes/no). Given the focus of this study on how dog-related activities differ in their impact on well-being, we have used a graphical approach to present our results (Figures 1-11 presented later in the manuscript). We show the estimated means for each pair of activity and well-being outcome (e.g., life satisfaction rating due to exercise with the dog versus life satisfaction due to dog playing). Such an approach facilitates the visualisation of statistical differences between dog-related activities. Plots (Figures 1-11) show, through error bars (99% CI), the regressed well-being outcomes of each dog-related activity for both owners with lower (score 2-lower quartile) and higher (score 6-upper quartile) psychological closeness to their dogs. Plots were created using the R package 'ggplot2' (v.3.3.3). Due to the large number of ordinal regressions performed, a more conservative confidence interval (i.e., 99%) has been used in the error bars in Figures 1-11 to provide some mitigation of Type I errors. Of the 2041 people who started the survey, 1030 met the inclusion criteria and completed the entire survey (50.5%). Respondents (Table S1) (Table S1 ). Of those respondents who did not complete the questionnaire, 598 had similar demographics to the actual participants of the study: 18-79 years old (mean 38.9, sd = 13.4), mostly female (79.9%), and mostly living in the US (38.3%) or UK (35.6%). Demographic information of the remaining 413 respondents is not available, as those individuals either did not meet the inclusion criteria, being redirected to the end of the survey, or stopped at the introductory stage due to a warning message telling them to answer the survey from a desktop and not from their phones Nine dog-related activities tended to be reported as predominantly positive for wellbeing across the aspects of well-being assessed (Figures 1-11 ): exercise with the dog (e.g., walking); the presence of the dog; tactile interactions with the dog; other close interactions (e.g., dog greeting the owner); training the dog; looking after the dog (e.g., feeding); dog playing; talking to others because of the dog; simply having the dog. These activities will be referred to as 'positive activities' from hereon. In contrast, five activities were mainly detrimental to participant well-being: dog health problems (e.g., dog being sick); sensory behaviour problems (e.g., dog barking); lacking control over the dog (e.g., dog pulling on the lead); dog aggressive behaviours; other dog unwanted behaviours. These will be referred to as 'negative activities' from hereon. One activity (maintenance of the dog, e.g., taking dog to the vet) mainly had no to little impact across participants' well-being. In Figures 1-11 , the non-overlap of error bars between different dog-related activities indicates that the activities compared are significantly different (p < 0.01). For example, in terms of the 'positive affect of high arousal' (Figure 1 ), dog playing increases happiness significantly more than looking after the dog in both owners with higher and lower closeness to their dogs. To avoid repetition of information provided in the figures, only the most relevant activities-highest/lowest score in our sample and overlapping the least with other activities-are reported in the text of each figure. However, this does not imply that this activity is significantly different to all of the others, as it could still overlap with other activities in the figure. The overlap of activities indicates that they might have a similar impact on the respective well-being outcome. Finally, the estimated means for each pair of dog-related activity and well-being outcome is available in the Supplementary Material (Tables S2-S12). On average, participants reported moderate-high level of psychological closeness to their dogs (mean = 4.82, sd = 1.89). The effect of closeness to the dog is illustrated in Figures 1 and 2 . Asterisks next to the error bars indicate the level of significance of this variable. Overall, when rating 'positive activities' or 'maintenance of the dog', owners who were closer to their dogs reported greater improvement in well-being outcomes than owners with lower closeness. By contrast, in relation to 'negative activities', the difference in well-being between owners with higher or lower closeness to their dogs were generally not significant. The exceptions to this were in relation to 'health problems', being more negatively rated for its impact on 'positive affect of high arousal' and 'life satisfaction' by owners who were closer to their dogs (Figures 1 and 5) ; and in relation to 'other unwanted behaviours', being more detrimental for 'positive affect of high arousal' in owners with higher closeness to their dogs ( Figure 1 ). Full statistical details for the independent variable 'closeness to the dog' and the confidence intervals of well-being outcomes at lower and higher closeness to the dog are available in the Supplementary Material (Tables S13-S33). 'Tactile interactions' and, secondly, 'dog playing' and 'other close interactions' were reported to improve most the negative affect of high arousal (Figure 3) . 'Health problems' and 'dog aggression' were, again, the worst for this aspect of well-being. 'Tactile interactions' and, secondly, 'dog playing' and 'other close interactions' were reported to improve most the negative affect of high arousal (Figure 3) . 'Health problems' and 'dog aggression' were, again, the worst for this aspect of well-being. Similar to what has been described in terms of the negative affect of high arousal, 'tactile interactions', 'other close interactions' and 'dog playing' were the most positive for negative affect of low arousal ( Figure 4) . Again, 'health problems', followed by 'dog aggression', were the worst for this aspect of owners' well-being. Similar to what has been described in terms of the negative affect of high arousal, 'tactile interactions', 'other close interactions' and 'dog playing' were the most positive for negative affect of low arousal ( Figure 4) . Again, 'health problems', followed by 'dog aggression', were the worst for this aspect of owners' well-being. Similar to what has been described in terms of the negative affect of high arousal, 'tactile interactions', 'other close interactions' and 'dog playing' were the most positive for negative affect of low arousal ( Figure 4) . Again, 'health problems', followed by 'dog aggression', were the worst for this aspect of owners' well-being. Similar to positive affect of high arousal, 'simply having the dog' and 'dog presence' were the activities that improved most the owners' life satisfaction ( Figure 5 ), whereas 'health problems' was the activity that worsened this well-being outcome the most. Similar to positive affect of high arousal, 'simply having the dog' and 'dog presence' were the activities that improved most the owners' life satisfaction ( Figure 5 ), whereas 'health problems' was the activity that worsened this well-being outcome the most. All 'positive activities' were rated as having a very similar positive impact on owners' autonomy. However, 'exercising' with the dog seems to be slightly better than other activities ( Figure 6 ). The 'negative activities' were also rated similarly, but 'dog aggression' had the most negative effect, particularly among owners with higher closeness to their dogs. All 'positive activities' were rated as having a very similar positive impact on owners' autonomy. However, 'exercising' with the dog seems to be slightly better than other activities ( Figure 6 ). The 'negative activities' were also rated similarly, but 'dog aggression' had the most negative effect, particularly among owners with higher closeness to their dogs. Similar to positive affect of high arousal, 'simply having the dog' and 'dog presence' were the activities that improved most the owners' life satisfaction ( Figure 5 ), whereas 'health problems' was the activity that worsened this well-being outcome the most. All 'positive activities' were rated as having a very similar positive impact on owners' autonomy. However, 'exercising' with the dog seems to be slightly better than other activities ( Figure 6 ). The 'negative activities' were also rated similarly, but 'dog aggression' had the most negative effect, particularly among owners with higher closeness to their dogs. 'Training the dog' was the activity reported to most improve an owner's sense of environmental mastery (Figure 7) . In contrast, 'health problems' and 'dog aggression' were the worst for environmental mastery. 'Training the dog' was the activity reported to most improve an owner's sense of environmental mastery (Figure 7) . In contrast, 'health problems' and 'dog aggression' were the worst for environmental mastery. According to owner ratings, 'training the dog' and 'simply having the dog' were the most beneficial activities for personal growth (Figure 8 ). Although the negative activities did not seem to have a considerable impact on personal growth, 'health problems' was the most detrimental for this well-being outcome, particularly for owners with higher closeness to their dogs. According to owner ratings, 'training the dog' and 'simply having the dog' were the most beneficial activities for personal growth (Figure 8 ). Although the negative activities did not seem to have a considerable impact on personal growth, 'health problems' was the most detrimental for this well-being outcome, particularly for owners with higher closeness to their dogs. 'Training the dog' was the activity reported to most improve an owner's sense of environmental mastery (Figure 7) . In contrast, 'health problems' and 'dog aggression' were the worst for environmental mastery. According to owner ratings, 'training the dog' and 'simply having the dog' were the most beneficial activities for personal growth (Figure 8 ). Although the negative activities did not seem to have a considerable impact on personal growth, 'health problems' was the most detrimental for this well-being outcome, particularly for owners with higher closeness to their dogs. Positive relations with others was reported to improve most due to 'talk to others because of the dog', followed by 'simply having the dog', 'dog presence', 'dog training', 'dog playing' and 'exercise' (Figure 9 ). As with autonomy, 'dog aggression' was the most detrimental event impacting on this aspect of well-being. Positive relations with others was reported to improve most due to 'talk to others because of the dog', followed by 'simply having the dog', 'dog presence', 'dog training', 'dog playing' and 'exercise' (Figure 9 ). As with autonomy, 'dog aggression' was the most detrimental event impacting on this aspect of well-being. Positive relations with others was reported to improve most due to 'talk to others because of the dog', followed by 'simply having the dog', 'dog presence', 'dog training', 'dog playing' and 'exercise' (Figure 9 ). As with autonomy, 'dog aggression' was the most detrimental event impacting on this aspect of well-being. As with autonomy, the ratings of the 'positive activities' were very similar to each other. Still, 'dog presence', 'tactile interactions' and 'simply having the dog' seem to be the most beneficial for self-acceptance. 'Dog aggression' was the only activity significantly detrimental for self-acceptance independently of level of closeness to the dog (Figure 11 ), although high closeness made 'health problems' more likely to be associated with a negative impact. As with autonomy, the ratings of the 'positive activities' were very similar to each other. Still, 'dog presence', 'tactile interactions' and 'simply having the dog' seem to be the most beneficial for self-acceptance. 'Dog aggression' was the only activity significantly detrimental for self-acceptance independently of level of closeness to the dog (Figure 11 ), although high closeness made 'health problems' more likely to be associated with a negative impact. The findings indicate that different dog-related activities have varying impacts on human hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being, as suggested in our previous qualitative work [18, 24, 25] . These specific findings have the potential to lay the foundation for more targeted psychotherapeutic interventions using owners' own pet dogs and, possibly, also assistance animals in AAI (e.g., positive tactile interaction with a dog to help decrease negative affect of high arousal in a patient). We suggest that specific aspects of human well-being might be more effectively boosted using a more tailored activity-based approach, based on the leading activities in Figures 1-11 . Further these results provide important insight into why the reported benefits of pet ownership are so inconsistent [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] ; i.e., different owners will engage in different activities and thus receive varying amounts of benefit or cost to diverse types of well-being. The activities' high variability in their level of impact across different well-being outcomes also highlights the importance of a comprehensive assessment of well-being in this type of work [27] . In this way, the potential benefits or costs associated with a specific activity are less likely to be missed. Another important finding was the potential moderating role of the closeness of the relationship between the owner and dog, on many of the potential effects. A higher level of closeness to the dog was associated with greater perceived benefits from all 'positive activities' and 'maintenance of the dog' in all aspects of well-being, which might explain why owners who are closer to their dogs have greater well-being [28, 29] and are more satisfied with their relationship with their dogs than other owners [42] . In contrast, 'negative activities' were generally scored similarly by owners regardless of their closeness. In other words, dog behavioural and health problems generally worsen The findings indicate that different dog-related activities have varying impacts on human hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being, as suggested in our previous qualitative work [18, 24, 25] . These specific findings have the potential to lay the foundation for more targeted psychotherapeutic interventions using owners' own pet dogs and, possibly, also assistance animals in AAI (e.g., positive tactile interaction with a dog to help decrease negative affect of high arousal in a patient). We suggest that specific aspects of human well-being might be more effectively boosted using a more tailored activity-based approach, based on the leading activities in Figures 1-11 . Further these results provide important insight into why the reported benefits of pet ownership are so inconsistent [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] ; i.e., different owners will engage in different activities and thus receive varying amounts of benefit or cost to diverse types of well-being. The activities' high variability in their level of impact across different well-being outcomes also highlights the importance of a comprehensive assessment of well-being in this type of work [27] . In this way, the potential benefits or costs associated with a specific activity are less likely to be missed. Another important finding was the potential moderating role of the closeness of the relationship between the owner and dog, on many of the potential effects. A higher level of closeness to the dog was associated with greater perceived benefits from all 'positive activities' and 'maintenance of the dog' in all aspects of well-being, which might explain why owners who are closer to their dogs have greater well-being [28, 29] and are more satisfied with their relationship with their dogs than other owners [42] . In contrast, 'negative activities' were generally scored similarly by owners regardless of their closeness. In other words, dog behavioural and health problems generally worsen the well-being of a dog owner to a similar degree. These findings have important implications for the management of people's mental health: since closer owners seem to benefit more from 'positive activities', increased frequency/duration of those activities (e.g., walking the dog more often) are likely to be much more efficacious for them. It is also worth speculating that when these activities are used within an AAI context, their impact may be less than for an owner, until a close bond is formed. Thus, focusing on building emotional closeness between a client and therapeutic animal may be important to the efficacy of a proposed activity. By contrast, psychological interventions focused on decreasing 'negative activities' (e.g., dog behaving less aggressively) appear to have a similar effect on the owner regardless of their emotional closeness. Positive interactions with the dog (e.g., touching, being greeted by the animal) and routine-like activities (e.g., walking, feeding) were rated as beneficial to owners' hedonic well-being, which is consistent with the findings of Barcelos et al. [18, 25] and Corrêa et al. [24] . Tactile interactions, in particular, led the rating score in three aspects of hedonic well-being, increasing owners' calmness and decreasing both stress and sadness. Indeed, it has been suggested that human-dog tactile interactions are a major component of the 'pet effect' [15] , being associated with lower human cortisol [43] [44] [45] , blood pressure, heart rate [15] , and higher oxytocin [43, 44] . Playing with the dog or watching the animal play were also very important to three aspects of hedonic well-being, improving owners' happiness, stress and sadness. Horowitz and Hecht [46] reported that, from their observations, 61% of dog-human playful interactions appear to elevate owners' positive affect. Human cortisol has also been reported to decrease after 'low-key playful interactions' with dogs [44] and qualitative studies have consistently reported the hedonic benefits of dog play (e.g., is enjoyable/fun, improves sadness, stress [18, 24, 25, 47, 48] . However, in a cross-sectional study, owners' reported frequency of dog playing was not correlated with well-being scores (e.g., stress, psychosomatic symptoms) [49] , but this may reflect the issue of less precise measurement of well-being in many studies, that we commented on in the introduction to this discussion. Life satisfaction was rarely mentioned by participants in our previous qualitative studies [18, 24, 25] , however all 'positive activities' were rated here as positive to life satisfaction. Indeed, simply having a dog seems to improve life satisfaction [50] , even if this is not something that often comes to mind in interviews or focus groups. In the current study, the mere 'presence of the dog' led the rating scores for life satisfaction and positive affect of high arousal. The benefits of the dog's presence for humans is one area of humananimal interaction where the scientific results are fairly consistent: being associated not only with beneficial neurophysiological changes [16, 51] but also with improvements in self-reported well-being [8, 11, 14, 18, 24, 25] . By contrast, dog health problems (e.g., disease, injury) and behavioural problems, particularly dog aggression, were reported to worsen owners' hedonic well-being and life satisfaction. Caring for sick animals is a burden and source of stress for pet owners [52] [53] [54] [55] [56] ; likewise, pet behaviour problems undermine owner well-being [48, 57, 58] and increase dissatisfaction with their animal [42] . Thus, interventional strategies that minimise detrimental activities and augment positive ones will likely improve owners' hedonic well-being and life satisfaction. Exercising the dog (e.g., walking), looking after the animal (e.g., feeding) and the presence of the dog were positive across all elements of eudaimonic well-being. Dog walking has been reported to give purpose in life and facilitate positive social relationships [9, 59, 60] . Caring for an animal can give owners routine, life structure and purpose [59, 61, 62] , even potentially helping in suicide prevention [25, 63] . Dog presence/company is an important social lubricant [60, [64] [65] [66] and is potentially a source of social support [45] . Interestingly, dog training led the rating score of most eudaimonic elements (e.g., environmental mastery, personal growth). Positive effects from dog training on eudaimonia have been reported in qualitative studies of owners (e.g., [18, 24, 25] ), and in relation to formal dog training programs (e.g., with prisoners, or soldiers with post-traumatic stress disorder). This might be because improving participants' skills, autonomy, trust in others and emotional control might give them a sense of achievement and self-development [67] [68] [69] [70] [71] . Interestingly, while positive activities were universally beneficial, negative events (e.g., health problems, dog aggression) had little to no impact on personal growth, purpose in life and self-acceptance; the significant detrimental effects were mostly related to autonomy, environmental mastery and positive relations with others. The latter negative results might therefore be linked largely to the stress and burden of these activities in the lives of pet owners in terms of owners being unable to make independent choices (autonomy), manage everyday affairs (environmental mastery), and due to interpersonal conflicts (relation with others), e.g., frequent disputes with other people because of dog showing aggression [48, [52] [53] [54] [55] [56] [57] [58] . The strengths of this study include its large sample size and the comprehensiveness of the dog-related activities and well-being outcomes evaluated, providing rich findings that can be empirically tested in future studies. This study had a very solid foundation, building on the consistency of our previous qualitative investigations [18, 24, 25] . Two main limitations of the study are its cross-sectional methodology, which does not permit inferences of causality, and the large proportion of female participants (83.9%), a common issue in studies of human-animal bond (see [72] ), such as those about dog ownership [73] . Another limitation of the study is the lack of assessment of additional covariates that could have impacted how owners perceive the interactions with their dogs, such as participants' socioeconomic status [74] and personality traits [75] . Utz [76] , for example, found that socioeconomic status is an important confounder in the association between pet ownership and human physical health. Despite these limitations, our study controls for several important covariates (e.g., age, gender, country, age of the dog, size of the dog, etc.) which is not always the case in studies on pet ownership [77] . Future investigations could test the impact of fluctuations in dog-related activities on the well-being of dog owners longitudinally, as a way to assess causality. Additionally, the same methodology could be extended to other pets and non-pet animals, such as those studies which have already been initiated with cats [26] . To increase male participation, targeted recruitment strategies can be applied, such as using more male dog owner pictures in advertisement materials, asking participants to invite male friends to the study, targeting social media groups/venues more populated by men. By employing a wider spectrum of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being herein, we may have begun to specify where the greatest impact of various human-dog interactions lies. Overall, different dog-related activities were found to have specific effects on owner well-being. Accordingly, we caution against over-simplified generalisations of a 'pet effect' or recommendations of pet acquisition as a psychological aid. A more individualised approach, based on increments or decrements in dog-related activities aiming to improve specific aspects of one's well-being is likely to result in more predictable outcomes and thus be more fruitful. Attention should also be given to the level of closeness between the person and the dog, as this may have significant moderating effects. Future psychotherapeutic interventions with pet dogs or studies about dog ownership and human well-being could use the findings presented here to provide guidance for their hypotheses and therapeutic goals. We hope that greater understanding of the complexity of the dog-human relationship and the impact that dogs have on human mental health will not only help shape a better human-dog relationship, but also a better society for all. The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/ 10.3390/ijerph182312382/s1, Questionnaire, Table S1 : Characteristics of participants (n = 1030) of the study and of their dogs, Tables S2-S12: Estimated means and sd for each pair of activity and well-being outcome, Tables S13-S23: Statistics of the independent variable 'closeness to the dog' in the ordinal regressions, Tables S24-S33: Confidence intervals of each activity at lower and higher 'closeness to the dog'. Institutional Review Board Statement: The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Lincoln (protocol code 2021_0503, approved on 12 April 2021). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/n6m2k/?view_only=3e70af2059564120a0383f1ed8937b30, accessed on 20 November 2021. Associations Between Pet Ownership and Attitudes Toward Pets With Youth Socioemotional Outcomes The Impact of Pets on Human Health and Psychological Well-Being Cat and dog companionship and well-being: A systematic review A Systematic Review of Research on Pet Ownership and Animal Interactions among Older Adults The power of support from companion animals for people living with mental health problems: A systematic review and narrative synthesis of the evidence Pet ownership and human health: A brief review of evidence and issues Dogs Supporting Human Health and Well-Being: A An Experience Sampling Approach to Investigating Associations between Pet Presence and Indicators of Psychological Wellbeing and Mood in Older Australians Dog Walking and the Social Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Loneliness in Older Adults Longitudinal evidence of the impact of dog ownership and dog walking on mental health The Pet-Effect in Daily Life: An Experience Sampling Study on Emotional Wellbeing in Pet Owners Pets enhance antidepressant pharmacotherapy effects in patients with treatment resistant major depressive disorder A study protocol for a randomised controlled trial to evaluate the effectiveness of a dog-facilitated physical activity minimal intervention on young children's physical activity, health and development: The PLAYCE PAWS trial How the presence of a dog reduces mental distress after social exclusion Cardiovascular effects of human-pet dog interactions Cardiovascular reactivity and the presence of pets, friends, and spouses: The truth about cats and dogs Oxytocin and cortisol levels in dog owners and their dogs are associated with behavioral patterns: An exploratory study A framework for understanding how activities associated with dog ownership relate to human well-being Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction Investigating the relationships between subjective well-being and psychological well-being over two decades Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being Psychological Well-Being Revisited: Advances in the science and practice of Eudaimonia Dog-related activities and human well-being in Brazilian dog owners: A framework and cross-cultural comparison with a British study Understanding the impact of dog ownership on autistic adults: Implications for mental health and suicide prevention Cat-human related activities associated with human well-being An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts A tail of two personalities: How canine companions shape relationships and well-being The benefits of giving as well as receiving need support in human-pet relations Strengths and limitations of qualitative and quantitative research methods What are emotions? And how can they be measured? A 12-point circumplex structure of core affect Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness Factors related to type of companion pet owned by older women The association between dog walking, physical activity and owner's perceptions of safety: Cross-sectional evidence from the US and Australia Cross-cultural difference in subjective wellbeing: Cultural response bias as an explanation Pet ownership, other domestic relationships, and satisfaction with life among seniors: Results from a Canadian national survey Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders Depression and life satisfaction in Nepal and Australia Perkins, E. I walk my dog because it makes me happy: A qualitative study to under-stand why dogs motivate walking and improved health Dog ownership satisfaction determinants in the owner-dog relationship and the dog's behaviour Short-term tnteraction between dogs and their owners: Effects on oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate-an exploratory study Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs Effects of social support by a dog on stress modulation in male children with insecure attachment Examining dog-human play: The characteristics, affect, and vocalizations of a unique interspecific interaction Fagerberg, I. Can therapy dogs evoke awareness of one's past and present life in persons with Alzheimer s disease? Eight domains of pet-owner wellness: Valuing the owner-pet relationship in the counseling process Benefits of dog ownership: Comparative study of equivalent samples Pets and happiness: Examining the association between pet ownership and wellbeing The presence of a dog attenuates cortisol and heart rate in the Trier Social Stress Test compared to human friends Evaluation of the impacts of epilepsy in dogs on their caregivers We have a ticking time bomb": A qualitative exploration of the impact of canine epilepsy on dog owners living in England Caregiver burden in owners of a sick companion animal: A cross-sectional observational study Caregiver burden and veterinary client well-Being Looking after chronically ill dogs: Impacts on the caregiver's life. Anthrozoös Living with and loving a pet with behavioral problems: Pet owners' experiences An exploratory model of dog disciplining Exploring the meaning and experience of chronic pain with people who live with a dog: A qualitative study Dogs as catalysts for social interactions: Robustness of the effect The Dog Narratives: Benefits of the human-animal bond for women with HIV Meaning in life and meaning of life in mental health care: An integrative literature review Best friends come in all breeds: The role of pets in suicidality Current challenges to research on animal-assisted interventions The facilitation of social interactions by domestic dogs Domestic dogs as facilitators in social interaction: An evaluation of helping and courtship behaviors Measuring the psychological impacts of prison-based Dog training programs and in-prison outcomes for inmates You think you're helping them, but they're helping you too": Experiences of Scottish male young offenders participating in a dog training program The role of service dog training in the treatment of combat-related PTSD The effects of dog-training programs: Experiences of incarcerated females Dog training programs in Pennsylvania's Department of Corrections Women Dominate Research on the Human-Animal Bond Sociodemographic correlates of prospective dog owners' intentions to participate in controlled trials of dog ownership and human health Debt, income and mental disorder in the general population Is Happiness Good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the Big Five with subjective well-being Walking the dog: The effect of pet ownership on human health and health behaviors Demographic and contextual factors as moderators of the relationship between pet ownership and health. Health Psychol The authors would like to thank all the participants of the study. The authors declare no conflict of interest.