key: cord-0046652-77zq31tw authors: D’Acci, Luca S. title: Urbanicity mental costs valuation: a review and urban-societal planning consideration date: 2020-06-30 journal: Mind Soc DOI: 10.1007/s11299-020-00235-3 sha: b1f615e3a10b2b1ea7a081b3701d2794451798c5 doc_id: 46652 cord_uid: 77zq31tw Living in cities has numerous comparative advantages than living in the countryside or in small villages and towns, most notably better access to education, services and jobs. However, it is also associated with a roughly twofold increase in some mental disorders rate incidence compared with living in rural areas. Economic assessments reported a forecasted loss of more than 19 trillion dollars in global GDP between 2011 and 2030 and of around 7 trillion for the year 2030 alone when measured by the human capital method. If we exclude self-selection processes and make the hypothesis to be able to level down the mental illness rate incidence in urban areas to these of the rural by better urban-societal planning, around € 1.2 trillion could be saved yearly worldwide. Even a reduction of only 20% in urban mental illness rate would save around 250 billion dollars yearly. disorders are also the primary cause of Disability-Adjusted Life Years worldwide (Bloom et al. 2012) . Decades of empirical research shows an association between mental health and urbanicity, especially for the individuals genetically more inclined and those who lived in cities during their early life. Links, often proven to be causal by longitudinal and dose-response analysis, between urbanicity and mental illness have been greatly reported such as in these 89 studies: Coid et al. 2020; Evans et al. 2020; Vargas et al. 2020; Sampson et al. 2020; Lecic-Tosevski 2019; Reed et al. 2018; Evans et al. 2018; Castillejos et al. 2018; Kirkbride et al. 2006 Kirkbride et al. , 2018 Cooper et al. 2017; Besteher et al. 2017; Gruebner et al. 2017; Krzywicka and Byrka 2017; Vassos et al. 2016; Newbury et al. 2016; Brockmeyer and D'Angiulli 2016; Adli et al. 2016; Freeman et al. 2015 , Wilker et al. 2015 Peterson et al. 2015; Haddad et al. 2015; Vaessen et al. 2015; Steinheuser et al. 2014; Haluza et al. 2014; Krabbendam et al. 2014; Streit et al. 2014; Calderón-Garcidueñas et al. 2013; Heinz et al. 2013; Bedrosian and Nelson 2013; Stevens et al. 2013; Tandon et al. 2012; Lederbogen et al. 2011; Fonken et al. 2011; Larson et al. 2011; Galea et al. 2011; McClung 2007 McClung , 2011 Meyer-Lindenberg 2010; Park et al. 2010; Bowler et al. 2010; Kelly et al. 2010; Mortensen et al. 2010; Levesque et al. 2011; Gwang-Won et al. 2010; Tae-Hoon et al. 2010; Peen et al. 2007 Peen et al. , 2010 van Os et al. 2010; Kennedy et al. 2009; Bentall et al. 2008; March et al. 2008; Joens-Matre et al. 2008; Fuller et al. 2007; Graziano and Cooke 2006; Maas et al. 2006; Mortensen 2001a, b, 2006a, b; Weich et al. 2006; Krabbendam and van Os 2005; Tsunetsugu and Miyazaki 2005; Wang 2004; Sundquist et al. 2004; van Os 2004; van Os et al. 2004; McGrath et al. 2004; Harrison et al. 2003; Caspi et al. 2003; Frumkin 2001; Allardyce et al. 2001; Haukka et al. 2001; Torrey et al. 2001; van Os et al. 2001; Eaton et al. 2000; Schelin et al. 2000; Marcelis et al. 1999; Mortensen et al. 1999; Marcelis et al. 1998; Thornicroft et al. 1993; Lewis et al. 1992; Cohen 1982; Eaton 1974; Christmas 1973; Faris and Dunham 1939; White 1903 . Paykel et al. (2000 , analysing data from almost ten thousand individuals (Household Survey of the National Morbidity Survey of Great Britain) via a logistic regression, reported "a considerable British urban-rural differences in mental health, which may largely be attributable to more adverse urban social environments". According to Vassos et al. (2016) , the rate of incidence of nine types of psychiatric disorders is in average 1.6 times higher in the capital city than in the rural areas, with 'schizophrenia and related disorders' even almost double (1.83), while the review of McGrath et al. (2004) of 68 studies found a schizophrenia incidence rate 2 times higher in urban areas than in mixed rural/urban areas; a rate that rises up to a 2.75 times greater risk of schizophrenia when one has lived 15 years of her early life in a capital city rather than a rural area (Pedersen and Mortensen 2001a) . Peen et al. (2007) reported an odds-ratio for mental disorders in very highly urbanized areas of 1.6 related to non-urbanized (1.8 when unadjusted by control variables). An approximatively twofold increase in psychosis risk associated with urbanicity is also confirmed in the following empirical studies: Marcelis et al. (1998 Marcelis et al. ( , 1999 , Mortensen et al. (1999) , Schelin et al. (2000) , Allardyce et al. (2001) , Pedersen and Mortensen (2001a, b) , van Os et al. (2001 van Os et al. ( , 2004 , Harrison et al. (2003 ), Sundquist et al. (2004 , Pedersen and Mortensen (2006a, b) , Kirkbride et al. (2006) , Haukka et al. (2001) and Torrey et al. (2001) . An increase as high as fourfold was found in Eaton et al. (2000) . A meta-analysis review summarised that urban dwellers have a 1.4 times greater risk of mood disorders than non-urban (Peen et al. 2010) . Due to the type of the analysis conducted, the causality (rather than a reverse causation) of the nature of this link, emphasising that urbanicity has an etiological effect on mental health, has been underlined, among many, by March et al. (2008 and Lederbogen et al. (2011) . If we shift our attention to people's preferences toward places to live their lives, "many surveys about quality of life in cities invariably suggest that it is in smaller cities that the highest quality of life is achieved" (Batty 2018, p. 95) . Similarly, to European surveys, 44% of Americans voted small towns/rural environments as the best kind of places to live and only roughly one in five (20%) voted cities (Knox and Pinch 2006) . Another questionnaire (D'Acci 2020) reported that only 32% of respondents prefers to live in a city rather than (ceteris paribus) in a natural environment (36%), in a town/village (24%), in a suburb (6%), while 2% of them were indifferent. In line with these stated residential preferences, happiness seems to decrease when urbanicity levels increase (Sander 2011; Lawless and Lucas 2010) , and studies about self-declared life satisfaction, psychological well-being in rich countries systematically show lower levels of life satisfaction in urban areas compared to the rural or less urban areas (Viganò et al. 2019; Easterlin et al. 2011; Gilbert et al. 2016; Helliwell et al. 2018; Fassio et al. 2013 This discrepancy between rural and urban environments' influence on mental health, life satisfaction and happiness suggest that by re-organizing our socioeconomic urban daily life and the physical urban-regional structure itself, there would be a potential margin of reduction in the urban mental illness rates and an increase of life satisfaction and daily mood of urban dwellers. To convince governments, urban and regional planners, stakeholders and the ordinary population about the relevance of the issue, an economic translation of the costs that psychological effects that cities have to us, might help to make the topic more tangible. Mental disorder costs go far beyond the direct costs (diagnostic and treatment); their economic costs assessment for the society as a whole should monetarily translate also the following indirect factors: increased chance of leaving school early, lower likelihood of achieving good and full-time employment, reduced quality of life for the individual and her loved ones. The monetary quantification of indirect costs on health usually follows the human capital method which measures the personal direct costs plus the amount of discounted earnings from lost productivity due to several reasons such as those listed above (Doran and Kinchin 2017; Gustavsson et al. 2011) . Early commencement mental disorders result to be statistically significantly associated with the interruption of secondary education (Leach and Butterworth 2012) , which in turn means less likelihood to be employed in higher skilled professions (Schofield et al. 2011) . As expected, psychiatric disorders between the ages of 18 and 25, after controlling for confounding variables, was statistically significantly (p value < 0.05) negatively linked with workforce participation, income and economic living standards at age 30, and, more generally, cumulative episodes of psychiatric disorders negatively affect life outcomes (Gibb et al. 2010 ). However, a bi-directional causality might appear between mental health and labour force participations as once workforce participation is being affected, a dangerous positive feedback loop could start: you get mentally ill then you work less, and the more excluded from work the more mentally ill you might be (Laplagne et al. 2007 ). This unemployment rate within the mentally ill population has being quantified to be as high as four times more than the healthy population, and when they work they are more inclined both to presenteeism (work with low productivity) and absenteeism (more leave for illness) (Schofield et al. 2011 ). This psychological distress cost related to lower productivity has been estimated in 2010 to be A$ 5.9 billion (equivalent to roughly A$ 7.5 billion in 2019) per annum in Australia (Hilton et al. 2010) , and the individuals' loss due to depression has been assessed as a 73% lower income than their full-time counterparts, while those deciding to retire early because of their mental health issues have 78% lower incomes, which at a national aggregate level means US$ 407 million in transfer payments, $ 278 million in lost income taxation revenue, and almost $ 2 billion in GDB, just in 2009 (Schofield et al. 2011) . Reports for Canada (Smetanin et al. 2011 ) assessed that in 30 years (2011-2041) the cumulative costs related to mental illness could be around US$ 3 trillion (based on US$ 2019)-even if underestimated for the lack of some types of cost and of mental illness-and in 2010-2011 Australia spent A$ 6.9 billion (7.7% of all government health outlay) in mental health services by governments and health insurers. 1 Studies also estimate that personal family costs and lost productivity for businesses and other non-government organisation costs, equal, or even surpass, the total government expenditures (Degney et al. 2012; Hilton et al. 2010; Jacobs et al. 2010) . The 2018 OECD report estimates as more than 4% of GDP (around € 600 billion) the costs due to mental illness across Europe (OECD 2018), while Gustavsson et al. (2011) estimated it to be around € 800 billion for the 2010, including Norway, Iceland and Switzerland to the 28 European countries. A team of members from the World Economic Forum and the Harvard School of Public Health (Bloom et al. 2012 ) used different methods (although non comparable among each other) to estimate mental disorders costs: (1) direct and indirect costs by human capital approach (the standard cost-of-illness method), (2) impact on economic growth (macroeconomic simulation), and (3) value of statistical life (willingness to pay). Each method has a different approach: personal versus social, private versus public, yearly costs versus multiple years' cumulative costs. The human capital approach (1), as we anticipated earlier, considers personal costs such as medical costs, transportation, care, income losses (related also to education loss due to illness), and sometimes it can also add non-personal costs such as public health education campaigns and research. The economic growth method (2), also called the value of lost output, considers how the investigated diseases diminish labour, capital and any other factors involved in the GDP formation at the country level, focusing on the illness related mortality rates impact on GDP. The value of statistical life method (3) is based on the people's willingness to pay (a kind of trade off) to lessen the risk of disability or death connected with the analysed illness, therefore by attaching an economic value to health/life itself it goes beyond the practical impact on GDP alone. The quantification is done either by observed trade-offs (e.g. in the labour market the wage premium a worker is willing to receive to take a job with a high injury-death risk, or the extra amount of money an individual spends for healthier food) and hypothetical trade-offs (surveys asking people how much they would pay to elude a risk or how much they would ask to take that risk). By method (1) the team (Bloom et al. 2012 ) estimated a world cost for mental illness of US$ 2.5 trillion for the year 2010 alone, and US$ 6 trillion for the year 2030 alone, two-thirds of which for indirect costs. By method (2) they estimated a world cumulative GDP loss of US$ 16.3 trillion (USA dollars 2010) due to mental health alone over 20 years (2011) (2012) (2013) (2014) (2015) (2016) (2017) (2018) (2019) (2020) (2021) (2022) (2023) (2024) (2025) (2026) (2027) (2028) (2029) (2030) . By method (3) they estimated a world output loss of US$ 8.5 trillion in 2010, and US$ 16.1 trillion in 2030. Converted into US$ for the year 2019, they resulted approximatively US$ 19.2 trillion of GDP loss during the 20 years between 2011 and 2030; US$ 7.1 trillion of human capital loss for the year 2030 alone; and US$ 18.9 trillion the willingness to pay for the year 2030 alone. All these estimates, even if already showing impressively high economic loss translations, are very likely underestimated (Whiteford et al. 2016 ). It seems clear from decades of a reasonable amount of mutually confirming independent research that urban life has unfavourable (often hidden) effects on our psyche, especially for those genetically susceptible and for those exposed to urban contexts during their juvenile years when the brain is still developing, whose causality has been proven by longitudinal and dose-response studies. Most people may not be aware about this psychological damage as it might be that the harm does not reach a sufficient entity to become visible, and that would remain below a certain level implying a manifested invisibility. Yet, individuals might still suffer some kind of psychological uncomfortable feeling even without being able to define it, or, if so, to establish the direct link with their urban life. If it is indeed true that it is not 'only' a small percentage of genetically susceptible urban dwellers targeted by statistically significantly higher psychosis risks, but a larger urban population, although with consistent variability in magnitude, we need to include this type of mental costs within any cost-benefit alike analysis. Territorial and urban planners cannot ignore the negative consequences that cities and territories have on our psychological well-being and mental health when poorly planned, designed and managed (e.g. endless cementification, lack of daily natural contact, congestion, lack of sky view, crowding, visually and socially boring dormitory areas extended for hectares, …). The same is valid for actions enabling us to change our socio-economic systems toward a more liveable scenario: just to cite an example, teleservices and teleworking (i.e. working remotely from home or wherever), a practice more and more in use 2 and even becoming law (since July 2015, first case in Europe and probably in the world) in the Netherlands if the worker wishes, would dramatically improve quality of life, free time, work efficiency and productivity, and enormously reduce congestion, daily car use, pollution, car parkstreet space, carbon emissions, and so on. Similar effects would be induced by flexible personalized working times (following personal biological circadian rhythmsessential for health and productivity-and private life schedules) and reduction of national daily working hours from, e.g. from 8 to 6 h: equivalent or probably even higher productivity thanks to more efficient use of working time, concentration, positive mood and an overall physical and psychologically healthier population. Probably a substantial help will come from medical genetics, pharmaceutics and psychologic-psychiatric progress regarding non-modifiable risk factors such as age, sex and genetic make-up, and from urban-territorial planning and governance, politics and education regarding the modifiable risk factors such as environment (e.g. greener and less crowded-polluted cities) and life style (diet, sport, sleeping, hobbies, sociality, daily natural contact) part of it linked with the environment where one lives. According to the large amount of empirical research evidence we saw, we can quite confidently say that urbanicity determines an approximately twofold increase in psychosis risk. Let's speculate that by planning better structural-infrastructural urban environments and forms (D'Acci 2020) and their socio-economic systems/life styles, (eliminating crowd-congestion, pollution, greenless, noise, crime, overwork, stress, over-pace…) of our current cities we are also able to entirely reduce their extra psychosis incidences and then levelling the urban psychosis rate to the rural one. If we refer to the cost-of-illness method (human capital) which directly involves money actually spent in mental illness issues, US$ 2.7 trillion 3 would be use for mental urban costs in 1 year alone (2030, when, according to UN 2019, a 60.4% of urban population is expected). If we prefer to avoid forecasts so far away in time (2030) and refer our thoughts only to real data from the past, in 2010 the money actually spent for mental illness was US$ 2.9 trillion worldwide (2019 dollars). An amount also in line with the 4% of GDP costs for mental illness assessed by the 2018 OECD regarding Europe: in fact if we use this GDP percentage at the world level, the world GDP in 2010 was around (in current dollars) US$ 66.037 trillion, 4 whose 4% means US$ 2.64 trillion, namely around US$ 2.9 trillion in 2019 dollars. By following the previous reasoning about levelling the urban psychosis incidence to the non-urban one thanks to better urban planning and socio-economic life styles, the share of world urban population in 2010 was around 51.6%, 5 therefore US$ 1 trillion 6 could have been saved in urban mental illness costs in that year alone. In 2018 the world urban population was around 55.3% and the world GDP around US$ 85.9 trillion 7 (current dollars), meaning roughly US$ 87.4 trillion today, whose 4% is around US$ 3.5 trillion which, following the same approximate reasoning (and assuming a similar percentage of GDP use) means that roughly US$ 1.24 trillion 8 could have been not spent in mental illness due to urbanicity issues. If we assume a reduction of "only" 20% of urban mental illness rate, we would still save around 250 billion dollars yearly. To put these trillions in context, the entire Apollo Space Program (1961) (1962) (1963) (1964) (1965) (1966) (1967) (1968) (1969) (1970) (1971) (1972) (1973) , including the 1969 walks on the Moon, still one of the major humanity achievements, costed only around US$ 175 billion 9 (in 2019 dollars); almost 6 times less than what can be saved in only 1 year in mental illness due to urban life. An equivalent program but on Mars (sending nine crews), could cost around US$ 1.5 trillion, 10 while the Mars 2020 rover mission costs 'only' between 2 and 3 US$ trillions. Another colossal human achievement, the 13 year Human Genome Project costed 'just' US$ 2.7 billion (1991 adjusted into 2018 dollars 11 ). Cities are a potentially great place to live and achieve our life's goals and progress, both as individual and as a species; however, it has some mental costs for the most susceptible. By planning better cities, territories and socio-economic daily life styles such as teleworking plus flexible working times, weekly working hour national reductions, greening cities and radically transforming the physical structures, forms and functioning of our current urban environments it would have an enormous potential economic impact environmentally, infrastructurally, but also psychologically speaking. From the academic literature we can say that the urban impact on mental health implies an approximately twofold increase in the rate of mental issues compared to the rural environment. Reports estimated a US$ 2.9 trillion global expense for mental disorders in 2010 alone and forecasted to become as high as US$ 7.1 trillion in the year 2030 alone. If we make a rather imaginative, although not that unrealistic, hypothesis to level the mental illness rate incidence of urban areas to these of the rural by better urban-societal planning, an astonishing amount of more than one trillion US dollars could be saved yearly: we could live happier, and send nine crews to Mars every year. Neurourbanism-a joint methodological approach between urban planning and neurosciences Comparison of the incidence of schizophrenia in rural Dumfries and Galloway and urban Camberwell Influence of the modern light environment on mood Social predictors of psychotic experiences: specificity and psychological mechanisms Dissatisfaction with city life: a new look at some old questions An urban-rural happiness gradient Associations between urban upbringing and cortical thickness and gyrification The global economic burden of noncommunicable diseases A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments How air pollution alters brain development: the role of neuroinflammation Brain immune interactions and air pollution: macrophage inhibitory factor (MIF), prion cellular protein (PrPC), Interleukin-6 (IL-6), interleukin 1 receptor antagonist (IL-1Ra), and interleukin-2 (IL-2) in cerebrospinal fluid and MIF in serum differentiate urban children exposed to severe vs. low air pollution Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene A systematic review and meta-analysis of the incidence of psychotic disorders: the distribution of rates and the influence of gender, urbanicity, immigration and socio-economic level Psychological stresses of urban living: new direction for mental health services in the inner city Calculating people: the spread of numeracy in early America Urbanicity and psychosis in a chinese undergraduate population: preliminary findings Schizophrenia-risk and urban birth are associated with proteomic changes in neonatal dried blood spots Preferring or needing cities? Psychology, utility and disutility of urban life Counting the cost: the impact of young men's mental health on the Australian economy. Inspire Foundation and Ernst and Young, Canberra Doran CM, Kinchin I (2017) A review of the economic impact of mental illness The impact of modern economic growth on urban-rural differences in subjective well-being Residence, social class, and schizophrenia Obstetric factors, urbanization and psychosis Urbanicity is associated with behavioral and emotional problems in elementary school-aged children Urbanicity, biological stress system functioning and mental health in adolescents Health, quality of life and population density: a preliminary study on "contextualized" quality of life Air pollution impairs cognition, provokes depressive-like behaviors and alters hippocampal cytokine expression and morphology The stress of the street for patients with persecutory delusions: a test of the symptomatic and psychological effects of going outside into a busy urban area Beyond toxicity: human health and the natural environment Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with biodiversity The urban environment and mental disorders: epigenetic links Burden of psychiatric disorder in young adulthood and life outcomes at age 30 Are rural residents happier? A quantitative analysis of subjective wellbeing in Scotland Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior Cities and mental health Cost of disorders of the brain in Europe Functional neuroanatomy associated with natural and urban scenic views in the human brain: 3.0T functional MR imaging Brain structure correlates of urban upbringing: an environmental risk factor for schizophrenia Green perspectives for public health: a narrative review on the physiological effects of experiencing outdoor nature Association between psychotic disorder and urban place of birth is not mediated by obstetric complications or childhood socioeconomic position: a cohort study Regional variation in the incidence of schizophrenia in Finland: a study of birth cohorts born from 1950 to 1969 Urbanicity, social adversity and psychosis How happy are your neighbours? Variation in life satisfaction among 1200 Canadian neighbourhoods and communities Using the interaction of mental health symptoms and treatment status to estimate lost employee productivity The cost of mental health and substance abuse services in Canada: a report of the Mental Health Commission of Canada. Institute of Health Economics Schizophrenia and the city: a review of literature and prospective study of psychosis and urbanicity in Ireland Personal space regulation by the human amygdala Heterogeneity in incidence rates of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders: findings from the 3-center AESOP Study City living and psychotic disorders-implications of global heterogeneity for theory development Schizophrenia and urbanicity: a major environmental influence-conditional on genetic risk Neural effects of the social environment Restorative qualities of and preference for natural and urban soundscapes Effects of health and education on labour force participation What role can child-care settings play in obesity prevention? A review of the evidence and call for research efforts Predictors of regional well-being: a county level analysis The effect of early onset common mental disorders on educational attainment in Australia Is urban living good for mental health? City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans Air pollution and the brain: subchronic diesel exhaust exposure causes neuroinflammation and elevates early markers of neurodegenerative disease Schizophrenia and city life Green space, urbanity, and health: how strong is the relation? Urbanization and psychosis: a study of 1942-1978 birth cohorts in the Netherlands Urbanization and risk for schizophrenia: does the effect operate before or around the time of illness onset Psychosis and place Circadian genes, rhythms and the biology of mood disorders Circadian rhythms and mood regulation: insights from pre-clinical models A systematic review of the incidence of schizophrenia: the distribution of rates and the influence of sex, urbanicity, migrant status and methodology From maps to mechanisms through neuroimaging of schizophrenia Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia Psychiatric family history and schizophrenia risk in Denmark: which mental disorders are relevant Why are children in urban neighborhoods at increased risk for psychotic symptoms? Findings from a UK longitudinal cohort study Unhappy metropolis (when American city is too big) Urbanism and happiness: a test of Wirth's theory of urban life The physiological effects of Shinrinyoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan Urban-rural mental health differences in Great Britain: findings from the National Morbidity Survey Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk Family history, place and season of birth as risk factors for schizophrenia in Denmark: a replication and reanalysis Are the cause(s) responsible for urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk rooted in families or in individuals? Urbanization and traffic related exposures as risk factors for schizophrenia Is the prevalence of psychiatric disorders associated with urbanization? The current status of urban-rural differences in psychiatric disorders Effects of prenatal exposure to air pollutants (polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) on the development of brain white matter, cognition, and behavior in later childhood Interaction of childhood urbanicity and variation in dopamine genes alters adult prefrontal function as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Urbanization, urbanicity, and depression Location and happiness in the United States Regional differences in schizophrenia incidence in Denmark How depression and other mental health problems can affect future living standards of those out of the labour force The life and economic impact of major mental illnesses in Canada: 2011 to 2041. Risk Analytica, on behalf of the Mental Health Commission of Canada Rural-urban differences in life satisfaction: evidence from the European Union Stress and the city: impact of urban upbringing on the (re)activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis Adverse health effects of nighttime lighting: comments on American Medical Association policy statement A functional variant in the neuropeptide S receptor 1 gene moderates the influence of urban upbringing on stress processing in the amygdala Urbanisation and incidence of psychosis and depression Human brain activation in response to visual stimulation with rural and urban scenery pictures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study Frequency of parent-supervised outdoor play of US preschool-aged children Urban-rural differences in the associations between social deprivation and psychiatric service utilization in schizophrenia and all diagnoses: a caseregister study in Northern Italy Risk factors and confounders in the geographical clustering of schizophrenia Measurement of absolute hemoglobin concentrations of prefrontal region by near-infrared time-resolved spectroscopy: examples of experiments and prospects United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division The dopaminergic response to acute stress in health and psychopathology: a systematic review Does the urban environment cause psychosis? Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms Confirmation of synergy between urbanicity and familial liability in the causation of psychosis The environment and schizophrenia Chronic stress, structural exposures and neurobiological mechanisms: a stimulation, discrepancy and deprivation model of psychosis Urban-rural differences in incidence rates of psychiatric disorders in Denmark Urban-rural dwellers' well-being determinants: when the city size matters. The case of Italy. City Estimating the true global burden of mental illness Disease burden and government spending on mental, neurological, and substance use disorders, and self-harm: cross-sectional, ecological study of health system response in the Americas Rural-urban differences in the prevalence of major depression and associated impairment Rural/non-rural differences in rates of common mental disorders in Britain: prospective multilevel cohort study The geographical distribution of insanity in the United States Challenges to estimating the true global burden of mental disorders Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter, residential proximity to major roads and measures of brain structure