key: cord-0024621-082jwvu5 authors: Sinclair, Chelsea S.; Lisa, Suriya F.; Pischedda, Alison title: Does sexual experience affect the strength of male mate choice for high‐quality females in Drosophila melanogaster? date: 2021-11-10 journal: Ecol Evol DOI: 10.1002/ece3.8334 sha: 952f327a519e495ef976e78ccf031536da16bf3b doc_id: 24621 cord_uid: 082jwvu5 Although females are traditionally thought of as the choosy sex, there is increasing evidence in many species that males will preferentially court or mate with certain females over others when given a choice. In the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, males discriminate between potential mating partners based on a number of female traits, including species, mating history, age, and condition. Interestingly, many of these male preferences are affected by the male's previous sexual experiences, such that males increase courtship toward types of females that they have previously mated with and decrease courtship toward types of females that have previously rejected them. D. melanogaster males also show courtship and mating preferences for larger females over smaller females, likely because larger females have higher fecundity. It is unknown, however, whether this preference shows behavioral plasticity based on the male's sexual history as we see for other male preferences. Here, we manipulate the sexual experience of D. melanogaster males and test whether this manipulation has any effect on the strength of male mate choice for large females. We find that sexually inexperienced males have a robust courtship preference for large females that is unaffected by previous experience mating with, or being rejected by, females of differing sizes. Given that female body size is one of the most common targets of male mate choice across insect species, our experiments with D. melanogaster may provide insight into how these preferences develop and evolve. offspring than others (Edward & Chapman, 2011) . When females differ in quality and the costs associated with courtship are high, males may increase their fitness by directing courtship and mating efforts toward higher quality females (e.g., Arbuthnott et al., 2017; Edward & Chapman, 2012) . In the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, male mate choice has been found in response to several female traits. D. melanogaster males are more likely to court or mate with healthy females compared with females infected with a parasite or pathogen (Khan & Prasad, 2013; Wittman & Fedorka, 2015) , younger females compared with older females (Nandy et al., 2012) , virgin females compared with mated females (Baxter et al., 2015; Cook & Cook, 1975; Siegel & Hall, 1979) , and conspecific compared with heterospecific females (Dukas & Baxter, 2014; Shahandeh et al., 2020) . Additionally, multiple studies have shown male mate choice based on female body size, with males preferentially courting or mating with larger females compared with smaller females (Baxter et al., 2015; Byrne & Rice, 2006; Long et al., 2009 ). This preference for large females is likely adaptive, as body size is positively correlated with fecundity in D. melanogaster (Lefranc & Bundgaard, 2000; Long et al., 2009) , so males that mate with larger females may father more offspring. Interestingly, many male courtship preferences are modified by the male's previous sexual experiences. For example, D. melanogaster males that had been rejected by a mated female subsequently reduced courtship toward mated females, as did males that had previously mated with a virgin female (Dukas & Dukas, 2012 ). In contrast, males that had mated with a simulated-mated female (i.e., a virgin female treated with the pheromones of mated females) subsequently increased their courtship toward mated females (Dukas & Dukas, 2012) . Males can similarly use pheromones to discriminate between D. melanogaster females of different ages and will suppress courtship toward the type of female that had previously rejected them (Balaban-Feld & Valone, 2018; Ejima et al., 2005) . Additionally, D. melanogaster males that had been rejected by a female from the closely related species D. simulans subsequently courted D. simulans females less than inexperienced males (Dukas, 2004; Dukas & Dukas, 2012) , as did D. melanogaster males that had previously mated with a conspecific female (Dukas & Dukas, 2012) . These courtship modifications can occur rapidly, with male courtship behavior changing significantly after only a single mating (Dukas & Dukas, 2012) or a single rejection episode lasting 10 min (Balaban-Feld & Valone, 2018) , 30 min (Dukas & Dukas, 2012) , or 60 min (Dukas, 2004) . Taken together, these results indicate that male fruit flies can readily refine their courtship preferences in response to a previous successful or unsuccessful courtship encounter. While it is clear that past sexual experiences can affect male mate choice for some female traits, it is unclear whether this is the case for preferences based on female body size. Female body size is one of the most common targets of male mate choice in insects (Bonduriansky, 2001) , so experiments with D. melanogaster provide the opportunity to investigate how these preferences develop. Although mating with a large female instead of a small female could potentially increase male fitness due to the larger female's higher fecundity, individual D. melanogaster males court large females with varying intensity and a substantial number court or mate with small females even when large females are present (e.g., Byrne & Rice, 2006; Long et al., 2009) . The sexual history of experimental males is often not controlled, so this variation might reflect the experiencebased plasticity we see for other male preferences. Consistent with this hypothesis, D. melanogaster males that had recently been sperm/ejaculate-depleted by mating multiple times had a stronger preference for large females compared with control males that did not have their mating history manipulated (Byrne & Rice, 2006) . This increased male choosiness in sperm-depleted males was attributed to the higher costs of mating associated with diminished resources (Byrne & Rice, 2006) , but it is possible that the different mating histories of experimental and control males also contributed. In this study, we altered the sexual experiences of D. melanogaster males and tested whether this affected the strength of male mate choice for larger females. We compared the courtship and mating preferences of sexually inexperienced males with males that had their sexual experience manipulated in one of four ways: (a) males that had mated with random-size females 2 days before male mate choice trials, (b) males that had mated with small or large females 2 days before male mate choice trials, (c) males that had mated with a small or large female immediately before male mate choice trials, and (d) males that had been rejected by a small or large female immediately before male mate choice trials. A single mating is sufficient to significantly reduce seminal fluid protein amounts in males, but these levels are readily restored following a period of sexual inactivity (Sirot et al., 2009) . As a result, the first two experiments gave males a 2-day recovery period between the experience phase and the male mate choice trials to minimize any potential confounding effects of sperm and/or ejaculate depletion in mated males that could affect the strength of male mate choice (Byrne & Rice, 2006) . The second set of experiments had the male mate choice trials immediately following the experience phase to control for the possibility that past experiences may not continue to affect male courtship behavior 2 days later (e.g., Siegel & Hall, 1979) . Regardless of the experiment, sexually inexperienced virgin males consistently showed robust courtship preferences for large females, and the strength of male mate choice did not change with any of our sexual experience manipulations. The D. melanogaster flies used in this study came from the LH M population. LH M is a large, wild-type, outbred population of D. melanogaster that had been held under identical conditions in the lab for over 700 generations at the time of our experiments (Rice et al., 2005) . The population is reared on a 2-week, discrete generation lifecycle in 25 mm diameter vials containing 5-10 ml of cornmeal/ molasses/yeast medium. Each generation on day 12 post-egg ~1800 breeding individuals are lightly anesthetized using CO 2 and are then distributed among 56 vials seeded with 6.4 mg of additional live yeast. 2.5 days later, the adult flies are transferred into fresh, unyeasted vials for 18 h, and the eggs that are produced are manually culled to a standard density of 150-200 eggs per vial. All flies were maintained at 25°C and 50-70% humidity on a 12-h light:12-h dark cycle. We created small and large females for male mate choice trials by manipulating larval density using the methods developed by Byrne and Rice (2006) . We placed 16 pairs of sexually mature male and female fruit flies from the LH M population into embryo collection cages (3.75 cm diameter by 5.8 cm high; Genesee Scientific) fitted with 35 mm petri dishes containing food medium for females to oviposit on for approximately 18 h. To create large females, exactly 50 eggs were collected from a petri dish and placed into a vial containing 10 ml of food. To create small females, we collected 100 eggs from a petri dish and placed them into a vial containing 1 ml of food. This protocol for altering female body size creates large females that are approximately twice the size of small females (Byrne & Rice, 2006) and thus are visually distinguishable without magnification. Flies in the small treatment developed slower than flies in the large treatment, so the small female vials were set up 1 day before the large female vials to ensure that the large and small female flies were the same age post-eclosion for all experiments. 9-10 days after the egg setup (for the large and small females, respectively), the females were collected as virgins within 6 h of eclosion using light CO 2 anesthesia. Large and small females were held separately in vials containing food medium at a density of 10 females per vial until the male mate choice trials. Before the experiments began, the vials were checked for larvae to ensure female virginity. Males for each experimental trial were randomly collected from vials reared under standard LH M conditions at a density of 150-200 eggs per vial in vials containing 5-10 ml of food medium. Nine days after the egg setup, males were collected as virgins within 6 h of eclosion under light CO 2 anesthesia and placed in groups of five in vials containing a small amount of food medium. Males were collected at the same time as the large and small virgin females to ensure that all flies were same post-eclosion age for each experiment. To determine whether the strength of male mate choice for large females was affected by the male's past sexual experience, we altered the sexual history of our experimental males in four separate experiments. This sexual experience phase was conducted either 2 days before the male mate choice trials, when flies were 3 days old post-eclosion (Experiments 1 and 2), or immediately before the male mate choice trials (Experiments 3 and 4). After the sexual experience phase, we conducted all male mate choice trials when the experimental flies were 5 days old post-eclosion. In reference to the normal LH M lifecycle, the male sexual experience phase at 3 days post-eclosion (12 days post-egg) corresponds to the time at which these males would be transferred from their developmental vial into fresh vials with females. Similarly, the male mate choice trials at 5 days post-eclosion (14 days post-egg) occur within the last 24 h of the lifecycle, at a time when mating rates are elevated . As a result, the sexual experience phase and the male mate choice trials both occur at biologically relevant times within the LH M lifecycle. 2.4.1 | Experiment 1: Mating with random-sized females 2 days before male mate choice trials Theory predicts that males should use their past encounters with females to evaluate their own attractiveness and modify their degree of choosiness accordingly, such that successful mating experiences lead to males with stronger courtship and mating preferences (Fawcett & Bleay, 2009) . As a result, we first tested whether male mating status (virgin or mated) affects the strength of male mate choice for large females. For this experiment, we mated males to random-sized females 2 days before our male mate choice trials to ensure the males were not sperm/ejaculate depleted during our trials. We collected random-sized females as virgins from the same vials as our experimental males, under standard LH M culture conditions (150-200 eggs in vials containing 5-10 ml of food). To create "mated" males, five virgin males were placed with 10 random-sized virgin females into vials containing food medium when the incubator lights came on and these flies were left for 1 h at 25°C. We used a mass-mating protocol for the experience phase instead of directly observing matings because this less labor-intensive approach allowed us to increase the sample size of males available for the experimental trials. Additionally, our preliminary work indicated that all of the males successfully mated at least one time in 28/30 trials under these conditions using a 30-min interaction period, so here we increased the interaction time to 1 h to ensure that all (or nearly all) males mated. While it is possible that some males mated with more than one female during this time, the 2-day recovery period allows time for any multiply-mated males to restore their sperm and/ or ejaculate supplies (Sirot et al., 2009) . We also concurrently set up a virgin male treatment as a control by placing five virgin males into a vial containing food medium, but no females, for 1 h at 25°C. At the end of the hour, the mated male vials were lightly anesthetized using CO 2 , the females were discarded, and the males were returned to the vials in groups of five. Flies from the virgin male treatment were also lightly anesthetized with CO 2 at this time. All males were then held at 25°C for 2 days until the male mate choice trials (see below). In total, we set up 46-47 mate choice trials per treatment for Experiment 1. We next tested whether the phenotype of the female that the male had previously mated with (i.e., large or small) affects the strength of male mate choice for large females 2 days later. With the exception of the size of the females used for the mating experience, Experiment 2 was conducted identically to Experiment 1. To mate our experimental males, five virgin males were placed with either 10 large or 10 small virgin females in vials containing food medium when the incubator lights came on and were left for 1 h at 25°C to create "large-mated" and "small-mated" male treatments. We do not expect any difference between the two treatments in the number of males that mated during this experience phase, as past work in the LH M population has shown that large and small virgin females are equally receptive to mating (Byrne & Rice, 2006) , which is consistent with data collected in the present study (see Experiment 3 below). Although some males might have mated with more than one female during this experience phase, the mated females would have been consistent in phenotype (i.e., both large females or both small females), and the 2-day recovery period provides time for any multiply mated males to restore their sperm and/or ejaculate supplies (Sirot et al., 2009) . We also set up a virgin male treatment concurrently, as in Experiment 1. The females were discarded after this experience phase and the males were held in groups of five at 25°C for 2 days until the male mate choice trials. In total, we set up 54-57 male mate choice trials per treatment for Experiment 2. To control for the possibility that past mating experience may not continue to affect male behavior after 2 days (e.g., Siegel & Hall, 1979) , our third experiment mated males to either small or large females immediately before the male mate choice trials. Because males would not have time to replenish their sperm and/or ejaculatory reserves before the male mate choice trials, we directly observed flies during the experience phase to ensure that the males only mated a single time. As soon as the incubator lights came on, virgin males were aspirated individually into a vial containing food medium and either a single large or a single small virgin female that had been aspirated into the vial the previous day. Foam plugs were pushed down into the vials to create approximately 1-2 cm of interaction space. We observed these vials for 30 min at room temperature to ensure that the males mated; any males that did not mate in this time were discarded. There was no significant difference between the number of males that mated in the large experience treatment (60/64) and the small experience treatment (57/65; Chi-square test: χ 2 = 1.403, p = .24), or in the time it took males to mate with the large or small females (large females: mean = 9.82 min, 95% confidence interval = 8.40-11.23 min; small females: mean = 11.23 min, 95% confidence interval = 9.77-12.68 min; t-test: t = 1.377, df = 115, p = .17). Males were allowed to finish mating uninterrupted, and the females were then removed from the vials using an aspirator and discarded. We also set up a virgin male treatment at the same time by aspirating males individually into vials containing food medium, pushing foam plugs down into the vials to create 1-2 cm of space, and leaving the males at room temperature while the large-mated and small-mated sexual experience occurred. All male mate choice trials using these virgin, small-mated, and large-mated males began within 30 min of the completion of the mating experience phase. In total, we set up 36-37 male mate choice trials per treatment for Experiment 3. To test for the effect of an unsuccessful sexual experience, our last experiment measured the strength of male mate choice in males that had been rejected by either a small or large female immediately before. We used nonvirgin, and thus sexually unreceptive, large and small female flies as our "rejecting females" for this sexual experience phase. To obtain these nonvirgin small and large females, we set up additional vials with manipulated egg densities at the same time that we set up the vials for our experimental small and large females. However, instead of collecting the rejecting females as virgins, we allowed them to eclose and mate within their developmental vial. We transferred all of the eclosed flies into new vials containing fresh food medium 2 days before our male mate choice trials to ensure the adults had adequate food resources. Past work in LH M indicates that 96-99% of the large and small rejecting females will have mated in the developmental vial at this time . The morning of our experiment (and about 1 h before the incubator lights came on), we collected small and large rejecting females individually into vials containing a small amount of food medium under light CO 2 anesthesia. These females were given 1 h to recover at room temperature before the rejection sexual experience phase began. At this time, a single virgin male was added to each vial using a mouth aspirator, such that each male was placed with either an individual large rejecting female or an individual small rejecting female. Foam plugs were pushed down into the vials to create 1-2 cm of interaction space and we observed these vials for 30 min at room temperature to ensure that the males courted, but did not mate, the rejecting females. None of the males mated with the rejecting female during this experience phase (N = 50 per treatment) and we only saved males that courted the female at least 10% of the time (≥3 min). The number of males that met this threshold did not differ significantly between the large (38/50) and small (40/50) treatments (Chi-square test: χ 2 = 0.233, p = .63). At the end of the rejection phase, the rejecting females were removed from the vials using an aspirator and discarded. We also set up an inexperienced virgin male treatment at the same time using the protocol for the virgin treatment in Experiment 3. All male mate choice trials using these inexperienced, small-rejected, and large-rejected males were started no later than 30 min after the completion of the rejection experience phase. In total, we set up 30-31 male mate choice trials per treatment for Experiment 4. All male mate choice trials were conducted when the experimental males and females were 5 days old post-eclosion. The evening before each mating observation, one large virgin female and one small virgin female were aspirated into a vial containing a small amount of food medium and returned to the 25°C incubator overnight. Once the incubator lights had turned on the next morning, we aspirated a single male (from one of the treatments described above) into each vial containing one large and one small female and pushed a foam plug down into the vial to create an interaction space of approximately 1-2 cm. For each experiment, males from the different treatments were distributed evenly among observers who were blind to the treatments, with each individual observing 10 vials at a time. The vials were observed for 30 min, and minuteby-minute courtship data were collected. Specifically, courtship was recorded for each minute in which the male performed a courtship song (in which the male extends and vibrates one or both wings while in proximity to a female) or an attempted copulation (in which the male curls his abdomen toward a female but is unsuccessful at copulating), and we noted which female these courtship efforts were directed toward. When a male courted both the large and the small female within the same minute, both courtship instances were recorded for that minute. We focused on singing and attempted copulation because these forms of courtship were easier to track during our observations compared with other courtship components like chasing, licking, and tapping (Spieth, 1974) . If the male mated a female during the observation period, we recorded any courtship that occurred up to and including the minute the mating began; if the male did not mate, we recorded any courtship that occurred throughout the 30-min observation period. All observations were completed within 2 h of the incubator lights turning on. All four of our experiments used the male mate choice trial methods described above, with a few exceptions. Because Experiments 1 and 2 did not have a sexual experience phase immediately before the male mate choice trials, we were able to conduct two or three replicates back-to-back within 2 h of the incubator lights turning on. The different male treatments were evenly split between replicates and the flies for later replicates were held at 25°C until their male mate choice trials began. We found no significant effect of replicate on male courtship preferences (see below) in either experiment (Wilcoxon/Kruskal-Wallis tests; all p > .11). Additionally, due to laboratory restrictions imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic, a subset of the data for Experiments 3 and 4 were obtained from video recordings of male mate choice trials. These recordings were made using an iPhone 11 (Apple) held horizontally over the vials by an Arkon Pro Phone Stand (model number HD8RV29). The male mate choice trials that we recorded were set up identically to the trials that we observed in person, with the exception that each recording could only fit six vials in the frame of the camera (compared with 10 vials that were observed simultaneously by each in-person observer). We found no significant difference in male courtship preferences (see below) between recorded or in-person observations for either experiment (Wilcoxon tests; all p > .05). As such, we pooled all observations, irrespective of replicate or observation method, for analysis. We used the courtship data collected above to calculate a "preference index" for each male that measures the strength of the courtship preference for the large female. The preference index (PI) for each male was calculated as: The PI ranges from −1 to +1, with a PI greater than 0 indicating a male preference for the large female (i.e., the male spent more time courting the large female), a PI less than 0 indicating a preference for the small female (i.e., the male spent more time courting the small female), and a PI = 0 indicating no preference (i.e., the male courted the small and large female equally). PI also measures the strength of male mate choice for a specific female: a PI with a higher absolute value indicates a stronger preference for the large female (if PI is positive) or small female (if PI is negative) compared with a PI with a lower absolute value. We performed two analyses using PI for each experiment. We first tested whether or not PIs within each treatment differed significantly from 0 using one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank tests followed by sequential Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons (Holm, 1979) . We then tested for a significant difference in PI between treatments using a Wilcoxon test (for Experiment 1) or a Kruskal-Wallis test (for Experiments 2 through 4). Because PI calculates the excess courtship directed towards the large female, we feel it is the most appropriate metric of male mate choice. However, it is possible that males with different sexual histories modified the absolute amount of courtship they performed, which cannot be captured with a preference index. As such, we also calculated the courtship effort directed toward the large female for each male as: For males that did not mate during the male mate choice trials, the total number of courtship minutes possible was 30, and for males that did mate it was the number of minutes until mating began. We tested for significant differences in the large female courtship effort between treatments for each experiment using Wilcoxon/Kruskal- We focus on courtship metrics (PI and large female courtship effort) as our main measures of male mate choice because they can quantify the effort that males invest in each female. Metrics that involve mating (such as mating success and/or mating latency with large females) might be more strongly influenced by female mate choice. This is particularly relevant for our study, as D. melanogaster females have been shown to discriminate between males with different mating histories (Markow et al., 1978; Saleem et al., 2014) . Nevertheless, mating is at least partly determined by male mate choice, so we included two mating parameters to complement our courtship analyses: female mated and courtship threshold to mate. First, we tested whether males within each treatment had a mating bias toward large or small females using binomial tests followed by sequential Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons (Holm, 1979 ). Next, we tested for an association between sexual experience treatment and whether the male mated with the large female versus the small female using Pearson's chi-square tests. Finally, we compared the courtship threshold to mate (measured as the number of minutes a male spent courting a female before mating with her) for both large and small females between treatments using a t-test (for Experiment 1) or one-way ANOVA (for Experiments 2 through 4). All analyses were performed using JMP 14. We found significantly positive median PIs for virgin males and males that had mated 2 days before male mate choice trials When we tested for an effect of mating immediately before male mate choice trials, we again found that all male treatments had sig- (Table 1) , nor were there any differences between treatments in the courtship thresholds to mate with the large female or the small female (Table 2) . PIs for Experiment 1, in which virgin males were compared with males that had mated with random-sized females 2 days prior. Both male treatments showed a significantly positive PI (both p < .002; N = 46-47), indicating that the majority of males spent more time courting the large female compared with the small female, and there was no difference in PI between treatments (p = .91). (b) PIs for Experiment 2, in which virgin males were compared with males that had mated with small females or large females 2 days prior. All male treatments showed a significantly positive PI (all p < .05; N = 54-57) and there were no differences in PIs between treatments (p = .88). (c) PIs for Experiment 3, in which virgin males were compared with males that had mated with a small female or a large female immediately before the trials. All male treatments showed a significantly positive PI (all p < .02; N = 36-37) and there were no differences in PIs between treatments (p = .72). (d) PIs for Experiment 4, in which inexperienced virgin males were compared with males that had been rejected by a small female or a large female immediately before the trials. All male treatments showed a significantly positive PI (all p < .02; N = 30-31) and there were no differences in PIs between treatments (p = .91). The more than the small female (all p-values significant after sequential Bonferroni correction). We again found that sexual experience was not associated with whether the male mated with the large or small female (Table 1) , and the courtship thresholds to mate with the large female or the small female did not differ between treatments (Table 2) . Large female courtship efforts for males with different sexual histories. (a) Large female courtship efforts for Experiment 1, in which virgin males were compared with males that had mated with random-sized females 2 days prior. There was no difference in courtship effort with large females between treatments (p = .46). (b) Large female courtship efforts for Experiment 2, in which virgin males were compared with males that had mated with small females or large females 2 days prior. There were no differences in courtship efforts with large females between treatments (p = .72). (c) Large female courtship efforts for Experiment 3, in which virgin males were compared with males that had mated with small females or large females immediately before the trials. There were no differences in courtship efforts with large females between treatments (p = .39). (d) Large female courtship efforts for Experiment 4, in which inexperienced virgin males were compared with males that had been rejected by small females or large females immediately before the trials. There were no differences in courtship efforts with large females between treatments (p = .58) In this study, we investigated the potential impact that previous sexual experiences may have on the strength of D. melanogaster male mate choice for large-bodied females. We found that sexually inexperienced males had a robust courtship preference for large females in all four experiments, consistent with past work using a different population of D. melanogaster (Baxter et al., 2015) . Despite this courtship preference, virgin males only mated with significantly more large females than small females in Experiment 1 ( Table 1 ). The inconsistency of these mating data might reflect variation in female mate choice between experiments, which is beyond the scope of this study. Nevertheless, virgin males show similar preferences for larger females in other insect species, including lesser wax moths (Goubault & Burlaud, 2018) , seaweed flies (Pitafi et al., 1990) , stink bugs (Capone, 1995) , and bedbugs (Kaufmann & Otti, 2019) . In D. melanogaster, sexually inexperienced males also exhibit courtship or mating preferences based on female fecundity (Edward & Chapman, 2012) , infection status (Khan & Prasad, 2013) , mating history (Siegel & Hall, 1979) , and species (Shahandeh et al., 2020) . Although experimentally elevating male costs (e.g., through sperm/ejaculate depletion, Byrne & Rice, 2006; Nandy et al., 2012) (Dukas & Dukas, 2012) , and species (Dukas, 2004; Dukas & Dukas, 2012) . Despite this, past rejection by a small or large female did not affect any metric of male mate choice for large females in our study. It is worth noting that we used nonvirgin small and large females as our "rejecting females," which likely exposed males to multiple female cues during the experience phase (i.e., the mating status and body size of the rejecting female). Nevertheless, this is a realistic rejection experience, as non-receptive D. melanogaster females are usually those that have previously mated (Wolfner, 1997) . Additionally, our results are comparable to work from Balaban-Feld and Valone (2018), who used large or small decapitated virgin females as rejecting females, but still found no effect of this rejection on male courtship persistence with subsequent large or small females. The finding that a single mating or rejection experience did not affect the strength of male mate choice for large females was surprising given the plasticity based on sexual experience that has been reported for other male preferences in this species (Balaban-Feld & Valone, 2018; Dukas, 2004; Dukas & Dukas, 2012; Ejima et al., 2005) . One possibility is that the preference seen in virgin males is too strong to be altered by sexual experience. The strength of male mate choice correlates positively with the difference in female fecundity in D. melanogaster (Edward & Chapman, 2012; Nandy et al., 2012) , and the large females used here are approximately twice as fecund as the small females (Byrne & Rice, 2006) . However, work by Edward and Chapman (2012) Dukas, 2004; Dukas & Dukas, 2012; Ejima et al., 2005) . The mechanisms underlying these differences in plasticity remain unclear, signifying that we still have much to learn about how male preferences develop, the degree to which they are adaptive, and their overall contribution to sexual selection within a population. We are grateful to Abigail Gutierrez, The authors do not have any conflicts of interest to declare. Data referenced in this study are available through Dryad: https:// doi.org/10.5061/dryad.1g1jw stxq. Alison Pischedda https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9684-3736 Why are female birds ornamented? Sexual selection Mate choice in fruit flies is rational and adaptive Changes in courtship behaviour following rejection: The influence of female phenotype in Drosophila melanogaster Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila Effects of age and experience on male mate choosiness The evolution of male mate choice in insects: A synthesis of ideas and evidence Evidence for adaptive male mate choice in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster Mutual preference for large mates in green stink bugs The attractiveness to males of female Drosophila melanogaster: Effects of mating, age and diet Male fruit flies learn to avoid interspecific courtship Mate choosiness in young male fruit flies Learning about prospective mates in male fruit flies: Effects of acceptance and rejection The evolution and significance of male mate choice Measuring the fitness benefits of male mate choice in Drosophila melanogaster Sequential learning of pheromonal cues modulates memory consolidation in trainer-specific associative courtship conditioning Previous experiences shape adaptive mate preferences Do males choose their mates in the lekking moth Achroia grisella? Influence of female body mass and male reproductive status on male mate choice A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure Males increase their fitness by choosing large females in the common bedbug Cimex lectularius Male Drosophila melanogaster show adaptive mating bias in response to female infection status The influence of male and female body size on copulation duration and fecundity in Drosophila melanogaster The timing of mating influences reproductive success in Drosophila melanogaster: Implications for sexual conflict Remating in Drosophila melanogaster: Are indirect benefits condition dependent? Evolution A cost of sexual attractiveness to high-fitness females Male mating experience and competitive courtship success in Drosophila melanogaster Degree of adaptive male mate choice is positively correlated with female quality variance Adult size and mate choice in seaweed flies (Coelopa frigida) Inter-locus antagonistic coevolution as an engine of speciation: Assessment with hemiclonal analysis Sexual experience enhances Drosophila melanogaster male mating behavior and success The genetics of male pheromone preference difference between Drosophila melanogaster and Drosophila simulans Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and mutant Drosophila Seminal fluid protein depletion and replenishment in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster: An ELISA-based method for tracking individual ejaculates Courtship behavior in Drosophila Male mate choice for unparasitized females in Drosophila melanogaster Tokens of love: Functions and regulation of drosophila male accessory gland products Does sexual experience affect the strength of male mate choice for high-quality females in Drosophila melanogaster?